Você está na página 1de 7

Research

Original Investigation

Effect of Protruding Ears on Visual Fixation Time


and Perception of Personality
Ralph Litschel, MD; Juleke Majoor, MD; Abel-Jan Tasman, MD

Invited Commentary page 189


IMPORTANCE Protruding ears are often thought to be a stigma, supposedly drawing attention Journal Club Slides at
and negatively influencing the perception of personality. These purported negative effects jamafacialplasticsurgery.com
that may indicate corrective aesthetic otoplasty in patients too young to provide informed
CME Quiz at
consent have not been quantified.
jamanetworkcme.com and
CME Questions page 232
OBJECTIVE To quantify attention directed toward protruding ears and its effect on the
perception of selected personality traits.

DESIGN, SETTING, AND PARTICIPANTS In this observational study conducted from August 1,
2013, to October 31, 2013, visual scan paths were recorded of 20 lay observers looking at
photographs of faces of 20 children (age range, 5-19 years) with either protruding ears or ears
morphed via computer software to appear nonprotruding. Subsequently, the observers rated
10 perceived personality traits based on the same photographs.

MAIN OUTCOMES MEASURES Visual fixation time on protruding vs nonprotruding ears was
compared and correlated with observers’ scores for personality traits.

RESULTS Fixation time on protruding ears was significantly longer compared with that for
morphed nonprotruding ears (mean [SD], 9.6% [5.6%] vs 5.8% [3.2%] of total fixation time,
P = .04). The difference between the overall personality questionnaire scores and between
individual scores for assiduousness, intelligence, and likeability was not significant for
protruding and nonprotruding ears. Faces in which the protruding auricles received the
highest percentage of visual attention scored higher than average for the overall personality
scores (mean [SD], 66.09 [4.50] vs. 55.81 [13.36]) and for assiduousness (6.64 [0.74] vs. 5.59
[1.41]), intelligence (6.78 [0.74] vs. 5.83 [1.31]), and likeability (7.29 [0.47] vs. 6.28 [1.40]).

Author Affiliations: Department of


CONCLUSIONS AND RELEVANCE Protruding ears had the potential to draw viewers’ attention
Otorhinolaryngology–Head and Neck
but did not cause a negative perception of personality traits. This study therefore does not Surgery, Cantonal Hospital, St Gallen,
provide confirmatory evidence for the stigmatizing nature of protruding ears. Switzerland.
Corresponding Author: Abel-Jan
LEVEL OF EVIDENCE 3. Tasman, MD, Department of
Otorhinolaryngology–Head and Neck
Surgery, Cantonal Hospital,
JAMA Facial Plast Surg. 2015;17(3):183-189. doi:10.1001/jamafacial.2015.0078 Rorschacher Str 95, 9007 St Gallen,
Published online March 19, 2015. Switzerland
(abel-jan.tasman@kssg.ch).

P
rotruding ears continue to be perceived as a negative defined as “an attribute or characteristic that conveys a social
physical attribute in many cultural settings, with af- identity that is devalued in a particular social context,” which
fected children frequently being ridiculed and adoles- includes “being the target of negative stereotypes, being re-
cents experiencing reduced self-esteem.1 Protruding ears have jected socially, being discriminated against, and being eco-
been associated with inferior cognitive performance at school, nomically disadvantaged.”5(p505) Facial stigmata, such as scars,
immaturity, less favorable personality traits, diminished self- acne, strabismus, nasal deformities, and protruding ears are
confidence, and social avoidance.2,3 Media often select a per- the most common reasons for a request for surgical revision.
son with large, prominent, or oddly shaped ears when wish- Persons with these facial stigmata provoke a negative reac-
ing to depict an odd character or a less-intelligent individual. tion in the observer, with facial deformities having a negative
Few, if any, features are believed to elicit such negative re- effect on the perception of honesty, trustworthiness, and em-
sponses as prominent or overly large ears. Prominent ears have ployability and, thereby, social functionality.6 The relation-
been found to be significantly larger than normal ears.4 From ship between visual deformities and high psychological dis-
this perspective, protruding ears may be perceived as a stigma, tress was demonstrated to be close.7 Several studies have

jamafacialplasticsurgery.com (Reprinted) JAMA Facial Plastic Surgery May/June 2015 Volume 17, Number 3 183

Copyright 2015 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: on 09/02/2018


Research Original Investigation Protruding Ears

analyzed the psychological effect of microtia in children.8,9 Pa- reflecting the variability found in the consecutive sample.
tients with larger deformities were found to cope better with Outcome simulations were created as part of the routine ini-
the negative feelings of others (mostly pity and aversion) prob- tial consultation using Adobe Photoshop Elements (Adobe
ably owing to adaptation, unlike those with less-pronounced Systems) as described elsewhere. 17 Simulations of the
cosmetic deformities, who may be continuously uncertain intended outcome of otoplasty were used instead of the
about their ears potentially becoming subject to ridicule.10 Chil- actual postoperative outcome to eliminate inevitable con-
dren pay more attention than do adults to small differences founders in postoperative photographs, such as facial
in appearance between themselves and others.1 Measuring the expression or changed hair color, hairstyle, or clothing
effect of corrective otoplasty using tests, such as the Child Be- (Figure 1). The 20 preoperative photographs and the 20
havior Checklist, the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Chil- simulated outcome photographs were divided into 2 series
dren, and the Children’s Depression Inventory, revealed im- of 20 photographs each, containing either the preoperative
provements in almost all the assessed items after the or the simulated outcome photograph. These sets were pro-
procedure.10,11 Happiness and self-confidence increased, so- cessed for eye tracking by cropping, centering, and correct-
cial experience improved, and bullying was reduced. Oto- ing scales (resolution of 1024 × 1024 pixels) of the patient
plasty was also found to potentially significantly increase pa- images and by adding calibration images and relaxing photo-
tients’ health-related quality of life and lead to a high rate of graphs of pleasant landscapes between the patient photo-
patient satisfaction.12,13 It is estimated that roughly 10% of the graphs in a file (PowerPoint, Microsoft Office Professional
population bears some type of facial deformity, with protrud- 2010) for presentation. Presentation times of the faces and
ing ears having an incidence of 5%.14 Surprisingly, given the landscapes were 10 and 3 seconds, respectively.
high prevalence and psychological effect of facial deformi- Twenty participants were recruited from the outpatient
ties, data on the visual perception of faces with deformities are clinic at the Department of Otorhinolaryngology–Head and
sparse.15,16 Neck Surgery of Cantonal Hospital. Participants were pa-
To our knowledge, no attempt at quantifying the tients in good health presenting for a routine follow-up, as well
attention-drawing potential of protruding ears has been pub- as persons who were accompanying patients. All participants
lished. Also, the effect of auricular deformities on the per- (18 male and 2 female; mean age, 44.7 years [range, 21-75 years])
ception of personality traits has not been reported. The aim had normal, symmetric eye movements as a prerequisite for
of this study was therefore to quantify the visual attention of eye tracking. The participants were informed that they would
naive observers directed to protruding ears and to measure be asked questions about the personality of the persons whose
the effect of protruding ears on the perception of personality faces they were about to see on a screen after the having seen
traits. Our first hypothesis was that protruding ears unknow- the photographs. Participants provided written informed con-
ingly cause the observer to give them more visual attention sent that their eye movements would be recorded but they were
compared with nonprotruding ears; our second hypothesis not informed of the purpose of the study.
was that personality traits of faces with protruding ears are An eye tracker is a device used, especially in psychologi-
judged less favorably. cal research, to record eye movements (visual scan path) of a
study participant and measures the duration of the fixation in
specific regions of interest. This technique enables investiga-
tion of the spontaneous, initial visual assessment of a face. A
Methods visual scan path is composed of a sequence of fixations and
This observational study was conducted at the Department of saccades.12,18 Fixations contain a continuous series of gaze
Otorhinolaryngology–Head and Neck Surgery of Cantonal Hos- points on a segment of the visual field with a predefined du-
pital, St Gallen, Switzerland. Written informed consent for the ration that is set at 200 milliseconds.18
use of patients’ photographs was obtained from patients or The visual scan paths (Figure 2) were recorded using a To-
their legal guardians and ethical approval was granted by the bii X120 eye tracker (Tobii Technology AB) linked to a host com-
local Ethics Committee of the Canton St Gallen. The study con- puter and a second monitor with a 19-inch diagonal display fac-
sisted of 2 parts, reflecting both hypotheses. ing the seated participant. For the calibration of the eye tracker
and presentation of the photographs, Tobii Studio Software,
Visual Attention for Protruding and Nonprotruding Ears version 2.0.6 (Tobii Technology AB), was used, in which par-
In the first part, visual attention to the auricles was quanti- ticipants had to follow a red dot on the screen.
fied with an eye-tracking device. A sample of 20 preopera- In each group, there were 200 eye-tracker measurements
tive photographs of patients (14 male and 6 female; age and a total of 400 measurements. The power analysis was based
range, 5-19 years) who had requested an otoplasty and 20 on the data published by Ishii et al14 (mean [SD] fixation time
photographs of the simulated desired outcome of the proce- of the defect region of 1630 [1177] milliseconds). A total of 137
dure was selected from the database of the Department of viewed photographs per group or 247 photographs in total were
Otorhinolaryngology–Head and Neck Surgery of Cantonal required to detect a difference of 400 milliseconds with 80%
Hospital. All en face patient photographs were taken under power and a significance level of P < .05. These calculated num-
standardized conditions (distance, lighting, background, and bers of photographs were increased from 137 and 247 to 200
resolution). The degree of overall ear protrusion and sym- and 400, respectively, to improve power of the study and be-
metry of protrusion was heterogeneous among the patients, cause of anticipated withdrawals.

184 JAMA Facial Plastic Surgery May/June 2015 Volume 17, Number 3 (Reprinted) jamafacialplasticsurgery.com

Copyright 2015 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: on 09/02/2018


Protruding Ears Original Investigation Research

Figure 1. Preoperative and Simulated Postoperative Photographs of Patients With Protruding Ears

A B

C D

E F
A-F, Selection of the photographs of
6 of 20 patients who were included
in the study. The preoperative images
are on the left, the simulated
intended outcome of an otoplasty on
the right. These are the patients who
are referred to in Figure 5,
representing the orange, green, and
blue squares.

Personality Ratings for Faces With Protruding fixation time of both pinnae was also correlated with the sum
and Nonprotruding Ears score of the questionnaire, as well as with the scores for indi-
For the assessment of perceived personality traits, a question- vidual questions regarding likeability, intelligence, and as-
naire was delivered that included pairs of antipodes on 10 per- siduousness.
sonality traits (sociable–withdrawn, content–discontent, as-
siduous–lazy, intelligent–unintelligent, creative–uninspired,
friendly–unfriendly, successful–unsuccessful, exciting–
boring, accessible–inaccessible, and honest–dishonest). These
Results
items were extracted from the Neuroticism-Extraversion- Visual Attention for Protruding
Openness Personality Inventory and are said to represent the and Nonprotruding Ears
5 broad dimensions of traits that are used to describe human Data of all 20 observers on 20 patient photographs were ame-
personality: neuroticism, extraversion, openness, agreeable- nable for analysis of 400 eye-tracking data sets. Four ques-
ness, and conscientiousness.19 We added 4 more traits in ac- tionnaires had to be excluded owing to missing data.
cordance with previously published research: successful- Mean (SD) fixation time of both auricles was 9.6% (5.6%)
ness, boringness, accessibility, and honesty.20 Each trait was of total fixation time for protruding ears and 5.8% (3.2%) for
displayed on a visual analog scale from 0 to 10, with 0 indi- nonprotruding ears (Figure 4). This difference was signifi-
cating the lowest and 10 the highest positive score. These per- cant (P = .04).
sonality traits were rated by all participants immediately af-
ter they had completed the eye-tracking part of the study with Personality Ratings for Faces With Protruding
the image they had seen on the screen printed on the ques- and Nonprotruding Ears
tionnaire for recognition. Eye tracking and rating of person- Sixteen of the 20 observers returned the questionnaire fully
ality traits required between 25 and 35 minutes and was moni- completed, including 10 visual analog score personality trait
tored by one of us (J.M.). ratings for each patient image, totaling 200 individual rat-
ings by each of the 16 observers, for a sum of 3200 individual
Statistical Analysis ratings.
The auricles were selected as the area of interest in Tobii Stu- A significant correlation between the sum of the person-
dio and the fixation time of both pinnae was calculated as the ality questionnaire scores for faces with protruding and non-
percentage of total fixation time (Figure 3). The difference be- protruding ears was found (Pearson correlation; R, 0.73,
tween relative fixation times for protruding and nonprotrud- P < .001). The difference between the sum of the personality
ing ears was analyzed by the average fixation time for each questionnaire scores for faces with protruding and nonpro-
patient with protruding and nonprotruding ears with a non- truding ears was not significant (mean [SD], 55.8 [1.37] vs 53.6
parametric paired sample test (Wilcoxon signed rank test). The [1.25], P = .31).

jamafacialplasticsurgery.com (Reprinted) JAMA Facial Plastic Surgery May/June 2015 Volume 17, Number 3 185

Copyright 2015 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: on 09/02/2018


Research Original Investigation Protruding Ears

Figure 2. Eye-Tracker Recordings

A B

C D

A and B, The upper row shows


preoperative photographs. C and D,
The lower row shows simulated
postoperative photographs. Each dot
represents a fixation time of more
than 200 milliseconds; dot size
corresponds to visual fixation time.
Connecting lines represent the visual
scan path. Fixation times are
represented as heat maps (red
represents maximum cumulative
fixation time).
As for the sum of all personality traits, the difference be-
tween the mean (SD) scores for assiduousness, intelligence, and Discussion
likeability did not differ significantly depending on protru-
sion or deformity of the ears (5.59 [14.5] vs 5.24 [1.43], P = .22; Facial stigmata catch the eye because a negative stimulus re-
5.83 [1.34] vs 5.53 [1.42], P = .41; and 6.28 [1.44] vs 6.20 [1.26], ceives more visual attention than does a positive stimulus. Spi-
P = .83, respectively). A significant positive correlation be- ders or unhappy faces, for example, are fixated on longer than
tween the scores for faces with protruding and nonprotrud- butterflies or happy faces.21-23 One contributing factor to this
ing ears was found for likeability (R, 0.75, P < .001). phenomenon may be that an abnormality in the face is per-
Both the quartile of the 5 faces for which the protruding ceived as new and represents a novel stimulus. According to
auricles received most visual attention (patients 2, 3, 4, 5, and the novel stimulus theory, to become familiar with it, people
6, respectively; orange and green squares in Figure 5) and the stare at a stigma.24 Also, unexpected facial features, such as a
quartile of the 5 faces with the largest difference in fixation time crooked nose or floppy ears, may be helpful in the face recog-
between protruding and nonprotruding auricles (patients 1, 3, nition process and therefore receive more attention.7,25
4, 5, and 6, respectively; orange and blue squares in Figure 5) Research has shown a negative correlation between at-
scored higher than the average for overall personality scores tractiveness and the lesion size of a facial disfigurement but
(mean [SD], 66.09 [4.50] vs 55.81 [13.36]) and for individual not between attractiveness and the location of a facial
scores on assiduousness (6.64 [0.74] vs 5.59 [1.41]), likeability disfigurement.26 A person’s attractiveness is known to strongly
(7.29 [0.47] vs 6.28 [1.40]), and intelligence (6.78 [0.74] vs 5.83 influence the perception of personality by the viewer. This phe-
[1.31]). In the quartile of faces for which the protruding au- nomenon is often referred to as the attractiveness halo. Indi-
ricles received most visual attention, the sum of personality viduals who are considered attractive will more likely be seen
scores and the likeability score was lower for the modified pho- as nice, friendly, and intelligent. As a consequence of a nega-
tograph without protrusion of the auricle in 4 of 5 patients (pa- tive first impression, people with facial deformities will more
tients 3, 4, 5, and 6). The scores for assiduousness and intel- likely be treated harshly.27 Such a first impression can have a
ligence were lower for the modified images in 3 of 5 patients distressing effect and dramatic psychosocial influence on
(patients 3, 4, and 5). people with facial disfigurements. Dating agents ranked pro-

186 JAMA Facial Plastic Surgery May/June 2015 Volume 17, Number 3 (Reprinted) jamafacialplasticsurgery.com

Copyright 2015 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: on 09/02/2018


Protruding Ears Original Investigation Research

Figure 3. Marking the Area of Interest (AOI) for Both Ears Figure 4. Visual Fixation Time for Both Ears for Protruding
vs Nonprotruding Ears

30

Nonprotruding Ears, %
20

10

AQI-R-EAR 0
AQI-L-EAR
0 10 20 30
Protruding Ears, %

Visual fixation time is reported as a percentage of total fixation time.

Figure 5. Visual Fixation Time of Both Ears vs Personality Trait Total Score

A B

The right and left ear were individually marked on the patient photographs 80 80
using the eye tracker’s Tobii Studio Software. Personality Sum Score

Personality Sum Score


60 60
truding ears as having a large negative effect more in men than
in women.28 Of all visible disfigurements, those of the face
40 40
seem to be least tolerated and most likely to provoke anxiety
and aversion. Children with protruding ears had more prob-
20 20
lems regarding self-esteem compared with children with nor-
mal ears or even facial port-wine stains.29
Experience shows that the eyes of the viewer typically fol- 0 0
0 10 20 30 0 10 20 30
low a particular scan path in a face with no obvious facial de- Protruding Ears, % Nonprotruding Ears, %
formities, focusing on a central triangle, which includes the
mouth, nose, and eyes.14,30-32 Attention had been found to be A, Protruding ears. B, Nonprotruding ears. Visual fixation time is reported as a
redirected to obvious facial defects at the expense of time spent percentage of total fixation time. Four orange squares and 1 green square
represent the patient quartile with the longest fixation time on both ears. Orange
analyzing the central triangle.
squares and the blue square represent the patient quartile with the largest
Therefore, it was expected that the average visual fixa- differences between the fixation times of protruding and nonprotruding ears.
tion time on protruding ears would be longer than the atten-
tion directed to nonprotruding ears. Still, the difference that
was quantified in this study appeared smaller than the per-
ception of the clinical problem from the patient’s and physi- most attention scoring higher than the 3 quartiles with less no-
cian’s perspective might suggest. The experimental setup and ticeable ears. These faces also yielded higher scores than their
the cutoff setting for registration of visual fixation of 200 mil- counterpart with corrected auricles in 4 of 5 cases for the total
liseconds may have reduced an effect that could be more pro- personality sum score and likeability score and in 3 of 5 cases
nounced in personal encounters. The spectrum of auricular de- for assiduousness and intelligence scores. In light of these find-
formities included in this study was representative of the ings, the relevance of protruding ears as a negative stigma re-
patients requesting an otoplasty at a Swiss tertiary care cen- garding perceived personality traits may be considered less pro-
ter. Obviously, the degree of deformity varied considerably, ex- nounced than purported in the literature.
plaining part of the variation in attention directed toward the This study has several strengths. Use of preoperative origi-
ears. Only ears that protruded markedly or were conspicu- nal and morphed postoperative images in which the degree of
ously deformed caught the eye, as documented for patients protrusion and deformity was the only variable in otherwise
2, 3, 4, 5, and 6. identical images excluded potentially attention-drawing dif-
If noticeable protruding ears were to be perceived by the ferences between preoperative and real postoperative photo-
population at large as heralding less-favorable personality traits, graphs. In addition, lay individuals acting as observers in this
the experimental setup in this study should be expected to have study were naive to the objective of the study and were shown
reflected this phenomenon, at least in part, through lower either the original or the morphed image, excluding a poten-
scores given by the observers. Surprisingly, the opposite was tial recognition of faces or noticing of a difference between 2
found, with the quartile of faces with the ears attracting the versions of the same face as a confounder. Finally, the degree

jamafacialplasticsurgery.com (Reprinted) JAMA Facial Plastic Surgery May/June 2015 Volume 17, Number 3 187

Copyright 2015 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: on 09/02/2018


Research Original Investigation Protruding Ears

of protrusion was variable, further reducing the clues the ob- sibility, respecting the expected attention span of partici-
servers may have noticed regarding the end point of the study pants. The definition of 10 seconds’ projection time for each
while scanning the faces. face was arbitrary and an effect of this projection time cannot
Several limitations of this study require discussion. First, be ruled out. Finally, answering 20 sets of 10 questions each
observers who were initially naive regarding the end point of on personality was thought to be tedious by some partici-
the study may have noticed the high prevalence of protrud- pants and fatigue cannot be ruled out as a potential con-
ing ears (50%) while viewing the faces, which may explain founder. Still, with the sequence of the original or modified
relative fixation times that may exceed those reported in the patient image being identical in both sets, the effect of
literature. Reducing the likelihood of this confounder would fatigue should be expected to have been minor and evenly
have required a substantially larger sample of faces with distributed. Finally, the group of lay observers may be con-
inconspicuous ears. A preliminary study had revealed that a sidered to be representative of the Swiss adult population.
substantially larger sample would have strained the visual Still, the effect of a cultural bias on the perception of protrud-
attention span of observers to the point of acquiring invalid ing ears cannot be ruled out.
data. It also remains unclear to what extent looking at photo-
graphs of faces on a monitor relates to visual analysis of a
face during encounters in person. The faces included dis-
played a variable degree of protrusion or deformation of the
Conclusions
auricle, reflecting the deformities found in a consecutive To our knowledge, this is the first study quantifying the at-
sample of patients who requested an otoplasty in a Swiss ter- tention-drawing potential of protruding ears using an eye-
tiary care center. The simulated degree of correction was tracking device and the first report correlating this effect with
variable, mirroring the preoperatively agreed-upon objective perceived personality. The confirmation of the hypothesis that
of a planned otoplasty. The shape of the modified auricle was protruding ears catch the attention of observers was ex-
realistic as to the desired projection but less so for the antici- pected. Not finding support for the second hypothesis, that pro-
pated relative position of the antihelical fold and helix in truding ears negatively affect the perception of personality
some patients. This shortcoming being inherent to the traits, was surprising. It remains unclear to what extent local
imaging method used in this study may have had an effect on cultural factors may have influenced this outcome. These data
the fixation times of virtually corrected pinnae. Also, the eye- may be helpful for future reference and for comparison with
tracking segment of the study was designed for optimum fea- studies addressing other facial regions of interest.

ARTICLE INFORMATION 4. Driessen JP, Borgstein JA, Vuyk HD. Defining the 13. Hao W, Chorney JM, Bezuhly M, Wilson K,
Accepted for Publication: December 9, 2014. protruding ear. J Craniofac Surg. 2011;22(6): Hong P. Analysis of health-related quality-of-life
2102-2108. outcomes and their predictive factors in pediatric
Published Online: March 19, 2015. patients who undergo otoplasty. Plast Reconstr Surg.
doi:10.1001/jamafacial.2015.0078. 5. Crocker JMB, Steele C. Social stigma. In: Gilbert
DT, Fiske ST, Lindzey G, eds. The Handbook of Social 2013;132(5):811e-817e.
Author Contributions: Dr Tasman had full access Psychology. Vol 2. 4th ed. Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill; 14. Ishii L, Carey J, Byrne P, Zee DS, Ishii M.
to all the data in the study and takes responsibility 1998. Measuring attentional bias to peripheral facial
for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the deformities. Laryngoscope. 2009;119(3):459-465.
data analysis. 6. Rankin M, Borah GL. Perceived functional
Study concept and design: Litschel, Tasman. impact of abnormal facial appearance. Plast 15. Valente SM. Visual disfigurement and
Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: All Reconstr Surg. 2003;111(7):2140-2146. depression. Plast Surg Nurs. 2009;29(1):10-18.
authors. 7. Madera JM, Hebl MR. Discrimination against 16. McNeil ML, Aiken SJ, Bance M, Leadbetter JR,
Drafting of the manuscript: Litschel, Tasman. facially stigmatized applicants in interviews: an Hong P. Can otoplasty impact hearing? a
Critical revision of the manuscript for important eye-tracking and face-to-face investigation. J Appl prospective randomized controlled study
intellectual content: All authors. Psychol. 2012;97(2):317-330. examining the effects of pinna position on speech
Statistical analysis: Litschel, Tasman. 8. Okajima H, Suzuki K, Takeichi Y, Umeda K, reception and intelligibility. J Otolaryngol Head
Administrative, technical, or material support: All Baba S. Long-term results of otoplasty for microtia. Neck Surg. 2013;42:10.
authors. Acta Otolaryngol Suppl. 1996;525:25-29. 17. Palma P, Khodaei I, Tasman AJ. A guide to the
Study supervision: Litschel, Tasman. assessment and analysis of the rhinoplasty patient.
9. Steffen A, Meyer Zu Natrup C, König IR,
Conflict of Interest Disclosures: None reported. Frenzel H, Rotter N. Analysis of psychosocial Facial Plast Surg. 2011;27(2):146-159.
changes following ear reconstruction with rib 18. Toh WL, Rossell SL, Castle DJ. Current visual
REFERENCES cartilage—development of a short test [in German]. scanpath research: a review of investigations into
1. Ju DM. The psychological effect of protruding Laryngorhinootologie. 2009;88(4):247-252. the psychotic, anxiety, and mood disorders. Compr
ears. Plast Reconstr Surg. 1963;31:424-427. 10. Cooper-Hobson G, Jaffe W. The benefits of Psychiatry. 2011;52(6):567-579.
2. Braun T, Hainzinger T, Stelter K, Krause E, otoplasty for children: further evidence to satisfy 19. McCrae RR, John OP. An introduction to the
Berghaus A, Hempel JM. Health-related quality of the modern NHS. J Plast Reconstr Aesthet Surg. five-factor model and its applications. J Pers. 1992;
life, patient benefit, and clinical outcome after 2009;62(2):190-194. 60(2):175-215.
otoplasty using suture techniques in 62 children 11. Gasques JA, Pereira de Godoy JM, Cruz EM. 20. Gruendl M. Beautycheck: causes and
and adults. Plast Reconstr Surg. 2010;126(6): Psychosocial effects of otoplasty in children with consequences of human facial attractiveness.
2115-2124. prominent ears. Aesthetic Plast Surg. 2008;32(6): http://www.uni-regensburg.de/Fakultaeten/phil
3. Arnulf W. Personalauswahl: Anforderungsprofil, 910-914. _Fak_II/Psychologie/Psy_II/beautycheck/english
Bewerbersuche, Vorauswahl und 12. Noton D, Stark L. Eye movements and visual /zusammen/zusammen1.htm. Updated July 2011.
Vorstellungsgespräch. Wiesbaden, Germany: Springer perception. Sci Am. 1971;224(6):35-43. Accessed August 1, 2014.
Gabler; 2008.

188 JAMA Facial Plastic Surgery May/June 2015 Volume 17, Number 3 (Reprinted) jamafacialplasticsurgery.com

Copyright 2015 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: on 09/02/2018


Protruding Ears Original Investigation Research

21. Fox E, Russo R, Bowles R, Dutton K. Do 25. Tsao DY, Schweers N, Moeller S, Freiwald WA. stains and prominent ears. J Am Acad Child Adolesc
threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in Patches of face-selective cortex in the macaque Psychiatry. 1995;34(12):1637-1647.
subclinical anxiety? J Exp Psychol Gen. 2001;130(4): frontal lobe. Nat Neurosci. 2008;11(8):877-879. 30. Walker-Smith GJ, Gale AG, Findlay JM. Eye
681-700. 26. Godoy A, Ishii M, Byrne PJ, Boahene KD, movement strategies involved in face perception.
22. Rinck M, Becker ES. Spider fearful individuals Encarnacion CO, Ishii LE. The straight truth: Perception. 1977;6(3):313-326.
attend to threat, then quickly avoid it: evidence measuring observer attention to the crooked nose. 31. Phillips ML, David AS. Visual scan paths are
from eye movements. J Abnorm Psychol. 2006;115 Laryngoscope. 2011;121(5):937-941. abnormal in deluded schizophrenics.
(2):231-238. 27. Pertschuk MJ, Whitaker LA. Social and Neuropsychologia. 1997;35(1):99-105.
23. Mogg K, Millar N, Bradley BP. Biases in eye psychological effects of craniofacial deformity and 32. Groner R, Groner MT. Attention and eye
movements to threatening facial expressions in surgical reconstruction. Clin Plast Surg. 1982;9(3): movement control: an overview. Eur Arch
generalized anxiety disorder and depressive 297-306. Psychiatry Neurol Sci. 1989;239(1):9-16.
disorder. J Abnorm Psychol. 2000;109(4):695-704. 28. Mojon-Azzi SM, Potnik W, Mojon DS. Opinions
24. Langer EL, Fiske S, Taylor SE, Chanowitz B. of dating agents about strabismic subjects’ ability to
Stigma, staring, and discomfort: a novel-stimulus find a partner. Br J Ophthalmol. 2008;92(6):765-769.
hypothesis. J Exp Soc Psychol. 1976;12(5):451-463. 29. Sheerin D, MacLeod M, Kusumakar V.
doi:10.1016/0022-1031(76)90077-9. Psychosocial adjustment in children with port-wine

Invited Commentary

Moving Toward Objective Measurement of Facial Deformities


Exploring a Third Domain of Social Perception
Lisa E. Ishii, MD, MHS

In this issue, Litschel et al1 present novel data on the visual dis- Significant progress has been made in recent years in sub-
traction caused by protruding ears. They used an eye- jectively and/or objectively measuring the perception of the
tracking device for objective evaluation and correlated the eye- casual observer of facial deformities or abnormalities, includ-
tracking test findings with personality survey assessments. ing the “abnormality” of the aging face. In a 2013 survey study
These data represent an im- by Zimm et al,3 patients who had undergone surgical proce-
portant addition to a burgeon- dures for aging faces were perceived to be younger than their
Related article page 183 ing body of evidence on the actual age. An objective line of investigation using eye track-
effect of facial deformities. ing has developed based on the foundation of normal face view-
Until now, in facial plastic and reconstructive surgery, we have ing, first described by Walker-Smith et al4 in the 1970s. Eye
had a relatively limited understanding of the effect of facial movements are a surrogate for attention, so, by measuring eye
deformities and abnormalities on visual distraction and fa- movements, one has insight into where an observer is direct-
cial perception. Previously, we had accepted the general te- ing attention. In their seminal work, Walker-Smith et al dem-
net that faces with abnormalities are less attractive and less onstrated that casual observers gazing on novel faces do so in
“normal,” and that our surgical procedures restore attractive- a highly conserved manner, directing the majority of atten-
ness and normality; however, these ideas have been based on tion to the central triangle region in a manner measurable using
limited objective evidence. Furthermore, we have relied pri- objective eye tracking. Recent research has taken advantage
marily on the subjective perceptions of experts and the pa- of this foundation, using eye tracking to objectively measure
tients themselves to inform our ideas on these paradigms of how distracting facial deformities are by measuring how fa-
attractiveness and normalcy, inadequately assessing the per- cial gaze patterns deviate from the normal, highly conserved
ceptions of casual observers. pattern in the presence of deformities. This method has been
Only recently have we begun to formally attempt to measure used to quantify the visual distraction caused by crooked
the effect of facial deformities from the perspective of casual ob- noses,5 skin lesions, and facial paralysis.6 After objectively
servers. The importance of measuring this effect lies in part in showing the extent to which the gaze of naive observers is
the fact that, for many people, one of their greatest concerns is drawn to facial deformities, survey studies have demon-
how they think others, particularly strangers, are viewing them. strated how observers then perceive attractiveness,7 affect
In a survey of patients with head and neck cancer, patients noted display,8 and willingness to engage in conversation, based on
that of greatest concern to them was how they would be per- the distraction of the deformity. Furthermore, these findings
ceived by others once they had a deformity.2 Patients with new have been extended to show the effect of surgical reconstruc-
facial deformities or abnormalities report changing their social tion on these visual distractions and changes in perception.6
habits to avoid encountering strangers or even family and friends. In their novel application, Litschel et al1 first measure how
Furthermore, we facial plastic and reconstructive surgeons per- casual observers gaze on faces of children with protruding ears,
form cosmetic and reconstructive procedures intrinsically be- and then measure gaze patterns after a surgical simulation,
lieving, along with the patients, that our procedure will restore using software to morph protruding ears into normal-
a more attractive and normal appearance to our patients so they appearing ears. They correlate the changes in eye gaze by an
can engage more effectively in society. observer with changes in perception of the faces before and

jamafacialplasticsurgery.com (Reprinted) JAMA Facial Plastic Surgery May/June 2015 Volume 17, Number 3 189

Copyright 2015 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: on 09/02/2018

Você também pode gostar