Você está na página 1de 4

AN IDEA WORTH RESEARCHING

Advancing Research on Cognitive Flexibility in Eating


Disorders: The Importance of Distinguishing Attentional
Set-Shifting and Reversal Learning
C 2014 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. (Int J Eat Disord 2014; 47:227–230)
Jennifer E. Wildes, PhD* V

Erika E. Forbes, PhD


Marsha D. Marcus, PhD

Introduction sity in the assessments that have been employed.1


For example, many studies have used perseverative
Cognitive inflexibility has received considerable errors on the WCST as an index of cognitive inflexi-
attention as a putative mechanism that might bility in EDs. However, because the WCST is a com-
increase the risk for eating disorder (ED) onset, and plex task that requires respondents to match cards
influence illness course and treatment response. under changing rules and feedback about perform-
However, research findings have been mixed and ance, perseverative errors indicating cognitive
have not led to advances in treatment outcome. inflexibility can reflect disruptions in two discrete
Using a framework informed by cognitive neuro- neurocognitive processes: (1) attentional set-
science, we contend that separating two discrete shifting, i.e., the ability to shift attention away from
facets of cognitive flexibility that are conflated in one abstract stimulus dimension (e.g., color)
measures currently employed in the EDs field (i.e., toward another (e.g., shape); and (2) reversal learn-
attentional set-shifting and reversal learning) will ing, i.e., the ability to override a recently acquired
help elucidate the mechanisms that underlie its stimulus-reinforcement association (e.g., matching
relation with ED symptoms. Moreover, we argue based on color) to apply a new stimulus-
that research that incorporates measures of atten- reinforcement association (e.g., matching based on
tional set-shifting and reversal learning will promote shape).2
understanding of the role of cognitive flexibility in Importantly, attentional set-shifting and reversal
the expression, course, and treatment of EDs. learning have distinct neural correlates (Table 1).3
For example, in animals, inactivations or lesions of
the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), but not the lateral
Defining Cognitive Flexibility: prefrontal cortex, influence reversal learning,
Attentional Set-Shifting and Reversal whereas inactivations or lesions of the lateral (pri-
Learning mate) or medial (rodent) prefrontal cortex, but not
the OFC, affect attentional set-shifting. In humans,
A critical step in studying cognitive flexibility is to
reversal learning has been linked to function in the
clarify the behavioral and neural dimensions that
OFC and ventral striatum (VS), while attentional
are most salient to EDs. This is challenging because
set-shifting has been related to the ventrolateral
research in EDs has relied primarily on multidi-
prefrontal cortex (VLPFC). Finally, there are dis-
mensional clinical neuropsychological measures
tinctions in the neurochemical correlates of these
such as the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST)
dimensions, with depletion of prefrontal cortical
and Brixton Test, and there is considerable diver-
dopamine (DA) and norepinephrine disrupting
attentional set-shifting, and striatal DA, in particu-
Accepted 31 October 2013
lar at DA D2 receptors, and serotonin in the OFC
*Correspondence to: Jennifer E. Wildes, PhD, Western Psychiatric influencing reversal learning. Thus, separating the
Institute and Clinic, 3811 O’Hara Street, Pittsburgh, PA 15213. behavioral components of attentional set-shifting
E-mail: wildesje@upmc.edu
and reversal learning, and their neural correlates, is
Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh School of
Medicine, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania crucial for advancing research on cognitive flexibil-
Published online 10 January 2014 in Wiley Online Library ity in EDs.
(wileyonlinelibrary.com). DOI: 10.1002/eat.22243
C 2014 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
V

International Journal of Eating Disorders 47:3 227–230 2014 227


WILDES ET AL.

TABLE 1. Selected neural correlates of attentional set-shifting and reversal learning


Behavior Mediating Neural Structures Modulating Neurotransmitters
Attentional set-shifting Ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC) Norepinephrine (NE) in the prefrontal cortex (PFC)
Anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) Dopamine (DA) in the PFC
Posterior parietal & temporal areas DA D1 receptors
Reversal learning Orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) Serotonin (5-HT) in the OFC
Ventral striatum (VS) DA in the striatum
DA D2 receptors

How Should Attentional Set-Shifting during the first stage of a two-stage probabilistic
and Reversal Learning be Discrimi- reversal learning task, respondents are rewarded
nated and Measured? for selecting stimulus 1 and punished for selecting
stimulus 2 on 80% of the trials; however, false feed-
The selection of behavioral tasks is critical for back is delivered on 20% of the trials such that
explicating the roles of attentional set-shifting and respondents are punished for selecting stimulus
reversal learning in EDs. Unlike clinical neuropsy- 1 and rewarded for selecting stimulus 2. In the sec-
chological measures that tap multiple neurocogni- ond stage of the task, the probabilities are
tive domains, experimental behavioral tasks are reversed. Probablistic reversal tasks are preferred
designed to assess specific cognitive processes. For over non-probabilistic tasks (e.g., reversal errors
example, the intradimensional/extradimensional on the intradimensional/extradimensional shift
shift task from the Cambridge Neuropsychological task) because they are more difficult and more
Test Automated Battery was developed as an exper- likely to encourage perseverative behavior after
imental analogue of the WCST to separate atten- contingency reversals. Probabilistic reversal learn-
tional set-shifting and reversal learning. During ing paradigms also have been developed for use in
this nine-stage task, respondents are presented functional imaging studies, and have been shown
with pairs of stimuli and must learn to select the to elicit responses in the VS and OFC.3 However,
correct stimulus based on feedback following each no study to our knowledge has used a probabilistic
trial. The rule for correct responding is modified at reversal learning task in a sample of individuals
the beginning of each stage to dissociate aspects of with EDs.
cognitive flexibility. For example, the intradimen-
sional shift stage examines rule generalization
when novel stimuli are introduced, whereas the
extradimensional shift stage examines the ability to Using Measures of Attentional Set-
inhibit or shift attention away from previous rele- Shifting and Reversal Learning to
vant stimulus dimensions (i.e., attentional set- Advance Research on Cognitive Flexi-
shifting). A functional imaging paradigm based on bility in EDs
the intradimensional/extradimensional shift task
has been developed, and has been shown to engage Future work is needed to clarify whether atten-
the VLPFC and related set-shifting circuitry.2 Extra- tional set-shifting is salient to EDs, or whether
dimensional shift errors on this task have been other forms of cognitive inflexibility, such as
linked to numerous psychiatric disorders.3 How- impaired reversal learning, are responsible for clin-
ever, research using the intradimensional/extradi- ical neuropsychological findings. Hypotheses war-
mensional shift task in ED patients has failed to ranting further exploration include the possibility
document impairments in extradimensional set- that attentional set-shifting is relevant only to par-
shifting,1 which might suggest that attentional set- ticular forms of ED, or that differences between ED
shifting is less salient to the expression of cognitive patients and controls in the neural correlates of
inflexibility than reversal learning. attentional set-shifting exist despite similar task
performance. Below we suggest three lines of
Studies that assess attentional set-shifting and
research that could be advanced through the use of
reversal learning separately within the same sam-
behavioral and neural measures of attentional set-
ple will help to clarify the relative roles of these
shifting and reversal learning.
processes in EDs. In humans, reversal learning
frequently is measured using probabilistic reversal
learning paradigms in which respondents learn to Phenotypic Heterogeneity in EDs
select the correct stimulus based on feedback, but Differences in the psychological and biological
some of the feedback is inaccurate. For example, correlates of anorexia nervosa-restricting type,

228 International Journal of Eating Disorders 47:3 227–230 2014


ATTENTIONAL SET-SHIFTING AND REVERSAL LEARNING

Figure 1. Schematic model linking variations in the salience of neural correlates of attentional set-shifting and reversal learning to phenotypic
heterogeneity across AN-R, AN-BP, and BN. Note. AN-R 5 anorexia nervosa, restricting type; AN-BP 5 anorexia nervosa, binge-eating/purging type;
BN 5 bulimia nervosa; VLPFC 5 ventrolateral prefrontal cortex; ACC 5 anterior cingulated cortex; OFC 5 orbitofrontal cortex; VS 5 ventral striatum;
NE 5 norepinephrine; DA 5 dopamine; 5-HT 5 serotonin.

anorexia nervosa-binge-eating/purging type, and alternative explanation for these findings is that indi-
bulimia nervosa raise the intriguing hypothesis viduals early in the course of an ED may be compen-
that these presentations might be differentially sating for disrupted processing during task
associated with attentional set-shifting and reversal performance by: (a) increasing the magnitude of
learning (Fig. 1).4,5 As such, administering behav- response in neural circuits underlying attentional
ioral measures of attentional set-shifting and rever- set-shifting and reversal learning in order to success-
sal learning to individuals across the ED spectrum, fully complete cognitive flexibility tasks; (b) recruit-
and conducting imaging studies using conceptu- ing additional neural structures to offset inadequate
ally relevant neurocognitive probes, could help activity in neural circuits underlying attentional set-
elucidate phenotypic heterogeneity in ED presen- shifting and reversal learning; or (c) both. In support
tation. Variations in the salience of attentional set- of this idea, research in non-ED-related psychiatric
shifting and reversal learning also might help to disorders and older adults has found alterations in
explicate other forms of systematic heterogeneity neural processing during cognitive tasks even when
in EDs, such as undercontrolled, overcontrolled, task performance has been equal in the patient and
and low psychopathology presentations. control groups.7 Thus, studying the neural correlates
of attentional set-shifting and reversal learning in
Cognitive Flexibility in Adolescents with Eating adolescents with EDs is crucial to explicating
Disorders the role of cognitive inflexibility in the early course
Cross-sectional studies using behavioral measures of these disorders. Using more precise behavioral
have failed to document cognitive inflexibility in tasks, as opposed to multidimensional clinical neu-
adolescents with EDs, leading some scholars to ropsychological measures, also will help to clarify
speculate that cognitive inflexibility is a conse- which aspects of cognitive inflexibility are most
quence of disordered eating.6 However, a compelling salient to adolescent EDs. Finally, examining

International Journal of Eating Disorders 47:3 227–230 2014 229


WILDES ET AL.

changes in behavioral and neural facets of atten- also can be used to help individuals accommo-
tional set-shifting and reversal learning over time date areas of dysfunction, even if underlying def-
will help elucidate the role of cognitive flexibility in icits do not change.5 Thus, clarifying the
the early progression of ED symptoms. processes that underlie cognitive inflexibility in
EDs will facilitate the development of novel cog-
Treatment Development nitive retraining strategies designed to target spe-
Ultimately, a better understanding of the cific mechanisms that have salience to particular
behavioral and neural facets of cognitive flexibil- ED symptoms.
ity with relevance to EDs will promote develop-
ment of novel interventions and prevention References
programs for high-risk groups. For example, sev-
eral medications have been shown to improve 1. Roberts ME, Tchanturia K, Stahl D, Southgate L, Treasure J. A systematic
attentional set-shifting in individuals with psy- review and meta-analysis of set-shifting ability in eating disorders. Psychol
Med 2007;37:1075–1084.
chiatric disorders, and animal research has found
2. Hampshire A, Owen AM. Fractionating attentional control using event-
that second-generation antipsychotic agents related fMRI. Cereb Cortex 2006;16:1679–1689.
attenuate reversal learning deficits.3 Finally, one 3. Fineberg NA, Potenza MN, Chamberlain SR, Berlin HA, Menzies L, Bechara
limitation of cognitive remediation therapy is A, et al. Probing compulsive and impulsive behaviors, from animal models
that it targets a broad range of neurocognitive to endophenotypes: A narrative review. Neuropsychopharmacology 2010;
35:591–604.
functions that may or may not be salient to the
4. Bailer UF, Kaye WH. Serotonin: imaging findings in eating disorders. Curr
expression or maintenance of EDs. In contrast, Top Behav Neurosci 2011;6:59–79.
cognitive retraining strategies that focus on spe- 5. Frank GK, Reynolds JR, Shott ME, Jappe L, Yang TT, Tregellas JR, et al. Ano-
cific processes that have relevance to particular rexia nervosa and obesity are associated with opposite brain reward
psychiatric presentations (e.g., attention bias response. Neuropsychopharmacology 2012;37:2031–2046.
6. Fitzpatrick KK, Darcy A, Colborn D, Gudorf C, Lock J. Set-shifting among ado-
modification for anxiety disorders) have
lescents with anorexia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord 2012;45:909–912.
been shown to produce significant decreases in 7. Bryck RL, Fisher PA. Training the brain: Practical applications of neural plas-
psychiatric symptoms when compared to control ticity from the intersection of cognitive neuroscience, developmental psy-
interventions. Cognitive retraining interventions chology, and prevention science. Am Psychol 2012;67:87–100.

230 International Journal of Eating Disorders 47:3 227–230 2014

Você também pode gostar