Você está na página 1de 6

Personality and Individual Differences 121 (2018) 100–105

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

With risk may come reward: Sensation seeking supports resilience through MARK
effective coping☆
Samuel McKay⁎, Jason L. Skues, Ben J. Williams
Swinburne University of Technology, John Street, Hawthorn, Victoria 3122, Australia

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Sensation seeking is often considered a risk factor for negative psychosocial outcomes. Our research challenges
Sensation seeking this view, testing a model in which sensation seeking is associated with psychological resilience, with this re-
Resilience lationship mediated by coping. Two-hundred-and-sixty-eight respondents completed measures of sensation
Wellbeing seeking, experiences of trauma, coping, perceived resilience, subjective wellbeing, and perceived stress.
Risk taking
Sensation seeking was directly and indirectly positively associated with the psychological resilience markers of
Coping
life satisfaction, positive affect, and (lower) perceived stress through problem-focused coping in those who had
experienced trauma. Sensation seeking was also positively related to greater perceptions of resilience. Based on
the results it is argued that sensation seeking increases resilience by decreasing stress responses and increasing
individual's resources to manage adversity. Results highlight the need to consider sensation seeking as a mul-
tifaceted construct that can support healthy development through building resilience.

1. Introduction the Internet; buffering the impact of daily stressors leading to lower
psychological discomfort; decreased perceived time to recover from
Risk taking is often viewed as a negative or irrational behaviour stressful events, and; lower PTSD symptoms after war trauma
(e.g., Lupton & Tulloch, 2002). Research to understand and mitigate the (Jorgensen & Johnson, 1990; Misra & Stokols, 2012; Neria, Solomon,
risks of such behaviour has frequently used trait conceptualisations of Ginzburg, & Dekel, 2000; Solomon, Ginzburg, Neria, & Ohry, 1995).
sensation seeking to predict negative risky activities (Roberti, 2004). However, little research has looked at the processes underlying such
However, some research has found sensation seeking can be associated findings.
with positive outcomes (e.g., Ravert et al., 2013). We propose a model
in which sensation seeking promotes psychological resilience by pro- 1.2. Coping, resources, and self-perceived resilience
moting perceptions of potential stressors as challenges rather than
threats. This increases involvement in, and successful negotiation of Coping refers to behaviours and thoughts people use to manage
these stressors, thereby supporting the development of and belief in stress (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Appraisals are cognitive and emo-
individual's coping skills and ability to handle adversity, and supports tional processes individuals undertake to determine if a stressor has
long-term resilience. implications for their wellbeing. Primary appraisals involve assessing
whether a situation is positive, dangerous, or irrelevant for one's
1.1. Assessing sensation seeking and resilience wellbeing. When situations are appraised as dangerous, individuals
consider if the stressor could cause harm or loss, presents a threat for
Sensation seeking is most often operationalised using Zuckerman's losses or a challenge with potential gains or growth (Lazarus & Folkman,
general definition - “the seeking of varied, novel, complex and intense 1984). Secondary appraisals involve consideration of the resources such
sensations and experiences, and the willingness to take physical, social, as coping styles an individual has to manage the stressor. Coping styles
legal, and financial risks for the sake of such experience” (Zuckerman, are cognitive and behavioral strategies for managing stress. Many have
1994, p. 27). The few studies focusing on this “experiential” aspect of been identified, and are associated with different outcomes
sensation seeking found positive associations with resilience-related (Carver & Scheier, 1994). Active and problem-focused coping strategies
outcomes, including: lower perceived information overload when using generally result in positive adaptation and better long term functioning


The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Corresponding author.
E-mail address: smckay@swin.edu.au (S. McKay).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.09.030
Received 11 April 2017; Received in revised form 15 September 2017; Accepted 18 September 2017
Available online 23 September 2017
0191-8869/ © 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
S. McKay et al. Personality and Individual Differences 121 (2018) 100–105

while denial and avoidance (e.g., consuming alcohol to forget) are as- proposed relationships between isolated subsets of variables in our
sociated with longer-term dysfunction (Campbell-Sills, Cohan, & Stein, model, but to our knowledge, this mediation model is the first to in-
2006). An individual's resources and the stressor's nature influence the tegrate all of these components. We test a mediation model where
coping strategy selection, however, individuals tend to have “disposi- sensation seeking is related to increased psychological resilience (op-
tional” coping strategies they use more frequently (Bouchard, erationalised as life satisfaction, positive and negative affect, and per-
Guillemette, & Landry-Léger, 2004), and these can predict longer-term ceived stress) through the mediating factors of coping and perceived
adjustment and resilience (Litman & Lunsford, 2009). resilience.
Another important predictor that influences secondary appraisals is We hypothesized that sensation seeking would moderate the re-
an individual's expectancy of their capacities to manage adversity. Self- lationship between cumulative lifetime adversity and psychological
perceived resilience, belief in one's ability to bounce back from stress, resilience. We further hypothesized that the relationship between sen-
has been linked to positive outcomes despite adversity (Smith et al., sation seeking and psychological resilience would be mediated by ac-
2008). For example, those with greater self-reported resilience had tive problem-focused coping strategies and self-perceived resilience.
fewer PTSD symptoms and adjustment issues, and better health and
wellbeing outcomes following trauma (Connor & Davidson, 2003; Smith 2. Method
et al., 2008). We argue that through increasing stress tolerance and the
preference for active management of stressors (Zuckerman, 1994), 2.1. Procedure and participants
sensation seeking modulates primary appraisals by framing situations
as challenges rather than threats. Additionally, secondary appraisals are The research was approved by the Swinburne University Human
influenced by sensation seeking indirectly through increasing beliefs in Research Ethics Committee. Three-hundred-and-fifty-three participants
one's ability bounce back quickly and employ active coping strategies. responded to an online survey using Qualtrics (Qualtrics, 2015). Ap-
Thus, sensation seeking supports the management of adversity and proximately half of the participants were undergraduate psychology
experience of resilience by modulating appraisals. To test these types of university students who participated as part of a research experience
models operational definitions for both adversity and positive adapta- program, the remainder were recruited using snowball sampling tech-
tion aspects of resilience are required. niques through Facebook, Reddit, and the Australian Whirlpool Forum.
Eighty-four participants who omitted more than 10% of the items were
1.3. Operational definitions of adversity and resilience removed from further analyses. None of the outcome resilience mea-
sures were correlated with experiencing or witnessing trauma. Since
1.3.1. Measuring adversity our model makes no predictions about those without trauma, 18 par-
A widely used indicator of adversity is cumulative lifetime adversity – ticipants who had not witnessed or experienced trauma were removed.
a set of major negative life events (e.g., assault, disaster, bereavement) The final sample comprised 250 participants (59% female) aged 19–69
statistically related to poor adjustment and mental disorders (M = 34.41 SD = 9.52). Ten-per-cent had not finished high school,
(Lloyd & Turner, 2008; Seery, 2011). 22% had completed high school, 29% had a diploma, 29% a bachelor
degree, and 10% a masters. Respondents resided in Australia. Ethnicity
1.3.2. Measuring healthy adaptation of participants was 82% Australian, 2% American, 2% Chinese, 2%
Research has used two types of indicators of positive adaptation: British, 1% New Zealand, and 1% Sri Lankan with the remainder either
competence outcomes and psychological responses to adversity (Masten, not reporting ethnicity or reporting multiple ethnic backgrounds.
2001). Competence outcomes (e.g., higher salaries and school grades)
are useful indicators of resilience in specific populations, but are poor 2.2. Measures
general indicators because norms are culturally, historically, and life-
stage specific (Fletcher & Sarkar, 2013). School grades, for example, are Unless otherwise stated, instruments are scored by summing item
not comparable between completers and early school leavers. In- responses, with higher scores representing greater expression of the
dividuals can show competence while experiencing distress, suggesting construct measured.
that competence is not unambiguously indicative of positive adaptation
(Masten, 2001). Thus, psychological responses confer the advantage of 2.2.1. Sensation seeking
capturing a broader range of experiences and outcomes that can be Sensation seeking was measured using the Sensation Seeking Scale-
compared across groups and the lifespan (Luhmann, Hofmann, V (Zuckerman, 1994). The SSS-V contains 40 items comprising four 10-
Eid, & Lucas, 2012). item subscales: Thrill and adventure seeking (TAS), experience seeking
Psychological responses (e.g., subjective wellbeing, perceived (ES), disinhibition (DIS), and boredom susceptibility (BS). Items are di-
trauma) can differ across domains (e.g., affective vs cognitive), there- chotomous forced-choice. One point is allocated to each high sensation
fore it is important to assess short- and long-term adjustment across seeking response and each low sensation seeking score is assigned 0
multiple domains (Fletcher & Sarkar, 2013). Perceptions of stress can points. The individual subscales and total (SSVtot) score exhibit poor to
also differ between people experiencing objectively similar circum- excellent reliability (Cronbach's α = 0.60–0.86; Zuckerman & Aluja,
stances, and thus can indicate quality of adaptation (Cohen, 2015).
Kamarck, & Mermelstein, 1983). We argue that psychological measures
capturing short- and long-term adaptation to adversity avoid the lim- 2.2.2. Brief resilience scale
itations of competence-based measures and provide a better picture of (BRS; Smith et al., 2008). BRS is a self-perceived resilience measure.
resilience for a general population. Participants rate 6-items using a 5-point Likert scale. The scale has
demonstrated good reliability in previous studies (α ≥ 0.80; Smith
1.4. The present study et al., 2008).

The current study tests whether sensation seeking influences resi- 2.2.3. The COPE inventory
lience directly during primary appraisals by increasing perceptions of (Carver, Scheier, & Weintraub, 1989). Coping style use was mea-
challenge, and indirectly during secondary appraisals by increasing sured using the 60-items Cope Inventory. Participants indicate the
individuals' beliefs in their abilities to handle adversity or through af- frequency they use each coping behaviour on a 4-point scale from 1 (I
fecting their coping style selection. Several researchers (e.g., usually don't do this at all) to 4 (I usually do this a lot). Carver et al. (1989)
Misra & Stokols, 2012; Ravert et al., 2013; Smith et al., 2008) have recommend factor analysing the inventory for every cohort to

101
S. McKay et al. Personality and Individual Differences 121 (2018) 100–105

determine the factor structure. Following the recommended method the

0.17⁎⁎
current study found a social coping (Scope; α = 0.92) factor comprising

15.


eight items from the original Instrumental and Emotional Social Support
scales, a problem-focused coping (PFcope; α = 0.88) factor comprising

0.02
− 0.07
eight items from the Active and Planning subscales, the original four

14.

0.59⁎⁎⁎ –
item Substance Use Coping (α = 0.95) factor, and the original four-item
Religious Coping (α = 0.96) factor.

− 0.08
0.01
13.


2.2.4. Life events checklist
(LEC-5; Weathers et al., 2013). The LEC-5 is a 17-item measure of

− 0.48⁎⁎⁎
− 0.20⁎⁎
traumatic events across the lifespan. For each event participants in-

0.00
0.01
dicate if: it has happened to them; they have witnessed it; learned about

12.


it; it was part of their job; are not sure if they have experienced it, or; it
does not apply to them. One point is scored for each response indicating

0.57⁎⁎⁎
−0.36⁎⁎⁎
−0.58⁎⁎⁎
−0.10
0.03
experience of trauma. Only the experienced (LECex) and witnessed

11.
(LECwit) subscales were used in this study.


2.2.5. Perceived stress scale

0.40⁎⁎⁎
0.48⁎⁎⁎
−0.48⁎⁎⁎
−0.61⁎⁎⁎
0.12
0.12
(PSS; Cohen et al., 1983). The PSS assesses recent stress levels with

10.
10-items asking participants about feelings of stress over the last month


using a 5-point scale from 0 (never) to 4 (very often). Internal con-
sistency has been very good in previous samples (α ≥ 0.84; Cohen

0.37⁎⁎⁎
0.30⁎⁎⁎
0.47⁎⁎⁎
−0.23⁎⁎⁎
−0.41⁎⁎⁎
0.17⁎⁎
0.07
et al., 1983).

9.

0.29⁎⁎⁎ –
2.2.6. Positive and negative affect scales

0.22⁎⁎
0.21⁎⁎
− 0.14

0.10
− 0.07
− 0.12
− 0.10
(PANAS; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988). Participant's mood was
assessed using the PANAS. Participants indicated the extent to which
8.


they felt each 20 mood-descriptive items (10 positive = PA; 10 nega-

0.17⁎⁎

0.15⁎
−0.05
0.06

0.05

0.05
−0.10
−0.01
−0.07
tive = NA) over the past two weeks on a 5-point scale from 1 (Very
slightly or not at all) to 5 (Extremely). The PANAS has demonstrated
7.


excellent reliability (α ≥ 0.88; Segerstrom, 2001).

0.42⁎⁎⁎

0.18⁎⁎

0.22⁎⁎

− 0.13⁎
− 0.05
0.09

0.12

0.03

− 0.12
− 0.01
2.2.7. Satisfaction with life scale
6.


(SWLS; Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985). Life satisfaction
0.52⁎⁎⁎
0.34⁎⁎⁎
was measured using the SWLS. Participants responded to five items on a

0.19⁎⁎
0.16⁎
0.15⁎
0.16⁎
0.07

−0.01
−0.09
0.02
0.03
7-point Likert scale. The scale has been widely used and consistently
5.

demonstrates good to excellent reliability (α ≥ 0.80; Pavot & Diener,


0.49⁎⁎⁎ –
0.51⁎⁎⁎
0.34⁎⁎⁎

0.23⁎⁎⁎

1993). 0.26⁎⁎⁎
0.19⁎⁎

0.17⁎⁎

0.13⁎
0.00

− 0.06
− 0.12
0.01
4.

3. Results

0.81⁎⁎⁎
0.74⁎⁎⁎
0.82⁎⁎⁎
0.66⁎⁎⁎

0.25⁎⁎⁎

0.28⁎⁎⁎
0.17⁎⁎

0.17⁎⁎

−0.15⁎
−0.01

0.00

−0.04
0.04
3.1. Data analysis
3.


Alpha coefficients, means, standard deviations, and correlations between variables.

Data were analysed using SPSS Version 24.0 and MPlus Version 7.1
(Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2012). Prior to analysis, data were screened
−0.17⁎⁎

0.19⁎⁎
−0.16⁎
−0.16⁎

0.13⁎

−0.13⁎

−0.16⁎
−0.05

−0.08
−0.10

0.09

0.02

−0.07

−0.02

for out of range, missing values, and outliers (+/− 3SD's from the
2.

mean). Missing Value Analysis indicated the remaining missing data


was missing completely at random (MCAR χ2(3523) = 2954.80,


0.25⁎⁎⁎
0.23⁎⁎⁎

0.20⁎⁎
0.19⁎⁎
− 0.19⁎⁎

0.13⁎

− 0.13⁎
− 0.16⁎

p = 0.99). This missing data was imputed using SPSS Expectation


0.01

0.12

− 0.01

− 0.03
0.03
0.09

0.08

Maximisation routine.
1.

3.1.1. Scale analysis


18–69
0–40
0–10
0–10
0–10
0–10

0–17
0–17
1–4
1–4
1–5
1–7
1–5
1–5
1–5

Before conducting further analyses the Cope Substance Use factor


NA
TR

was omitted due to item overlap with the SSS-V leading to item re-
dundancy. The religious coping factor was also removed because over
9.52
7.16
2.92
1.90
2.54
2.00
0.75
0.58
0.79
1.41
0.74
0.66
0.69
2.21
2.45
NA
SD

half the respondent did not use this coping style at all and very few used
the style regularly.
34.42
20.69
6.08
6.48
5.12
3.02
2.59
3.00
3.54
4.69
3.54
1.85
2.57
3.11
2.64

The means, standard deviations, alpha scores, and correlations be-


NA
M

tween the remaining scales are shown in Table 1 below.


Before assessing the structural model, one-factor congeneric models
0.85
0.81
0.54
0.73
0.56
0.92
0.88
0.89
0.89
0.92
0.90
0.89
0.65
0.65
NA
NA

were used to assess the dimensionality of the BRS, SWLS, PSS, PA and
α

p < 0.001.

NA scales against the fit criteria of Hu and Bentler (CFI > 0.95,


p < 0.01.
Note. N = 250.
p < 0.05.

RMSEA < 0.06, SRMR < 0.08). All models were found to fit the data,
16. LECWit
15. LECEx
9. PFCope
1. Gender

3. SSVTot

11. SWLS
8. SCope

10. BRS

with the exception of the PSS (χ2(35) = 106.54 p < 0.001;


14. PSS
13. NA
12. PA
4. TAS
2. Age

6. DIS
Table 1

7. BS
5. ES

CFI = 0.920, SRMR = 0.050 RMSEA = 0.091), the PA scale (χ2(35)


⁎⁎⁎
⁎⁎

= 137.64 p < 0.001; CFI = 0.921, SRMR = 0.046 RMSEA = 0.109)

102
S. McKay et al. Personality and Individual Differences 121 (2018) 100–105

Fig. 1. Full structural model of sensation seeking in-


creasing resilience both directly and indirectly
through coping. Note: * p < 0.05 *** p < 0.001.

Table 2
Unstandardized results for the mediation analysis of the effect of sensation seeking on resilience through problem-focused coping.

Dependent variable Independent variable Parameter Estimate (SE) 95% CI p

LL UL

Coefficients
PFCope SS ab 0.748 (0.327) 0.157 1.461 0.022
Resilience PFCope bb 0.541 (0.112) 0.340 0.782 < 0.001
Resilience SS cb 0.823 (0.401) 0.075 1.647 0.040

Indirect effect (mediator = PFCope)


Resilience SS abbb 0.405 (0.194) 0.094 0.868 0.036

Total effect of SSV with PFCope mediator


Resilience SS c′ 1.227 (0.434) 0.473 2.160 0.001

Note. CI = confidence interval; LL = lower limit, UL = upper limit. SS = Sensation Seeking.

and the NA scale (χ2(35) = 226.44 p < 0.001; CFI = 0.843, relationship between the SSV and a latent Resilience factor comprised
SRMR = 0.068 RMSEA = 0.148). An inspection of the standardised of PA, SWLS and PSS. The initial model using ML estimation did not fit
residuals and modification indices suggested that there may be a the data (χ2(1205) = 1906.98 p = 0.002; CFI = 0.900, SRMR = 0.096
method bias occurring with positive and negatively worded items on RMSEA = 0.048). The path from the SSV to the SCope factor was found
the PSS. Accordingly, the associated items were allowed to correlate (1 to be not significant and thus the SCope was removed from the model.
with 3, 5 with 8, and 4 with 7) and the revised model provided an Additionally, the standardised residuals and modification indices de-
adequate fit to the data (χ2(32) = 59.55 p = 0.002; CFI = 0. 969, monstrated that BRS did not clearly fit as either a mediator or outcome
SRMR = 0.039 RMSEA = 0.059). A similar issue was found with the variable in the model, as it loaded on the resilience factor and also
PA and after allowing the associated items to correlate (12 with 17, 14 showed a strong positive association with the remaining coping med-
with 16 and 16 with 17) the model provided a reasonable fit the data iator. Thus, the BRS was also removed from the model. The final model
(χ2(32) = 81.48 p < 0.001; CFI = 0.962, SRMR = 0.036 fit the data adequately (χ2(617) = 941.17 p < 0.001; CFI = 0.928,
RMSEA = 0.079). The same process was undertaken with the NA. SRMR = 0.060 RMSEA = 0.046). Although the CFI in this model is
However, even after significant model revision, the model fit was found slightly lower than the ≥0.95 cut-off proposed by Hu and Bentler
to be unacceptable. Given the absence of a significant correlation be- (1999), research has demonstrated that CFI scores tend to decrease in
tween SS and NA, PA was removed from the final model. In addition, a correctly specified complex models with a large number of variables,
confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was performed using the re- while the RMSEA improves (Kenny & McCoach, 2003). Given that the
commended WLSMV estimator for categorical data (Kline, 2010) on the RMSEA is well within the acceptable range, these results suggest that
four subscales and the higher order factor for SSV. The full model did the model demonstrates an acceptable fit. The standardised parameter
not produce an adequate fit to the data (χ2(780) = 3210.58 estimates and the variance explained in the latent factors for the model
p < 0.001; CFI = 0.898, WRMR = 1.111 RMSEA = 0.037). It was are shown in Fig. 1.
found that several items did not significantly load onto the associated As can be seen in figure one, the resilience factor explained a little
sub-factors and these were removed (SSV sub factor and scale item over 60% of variance in positive affect, life satisfaction and perceived
number: BS 15 34 39; ES 14 37; DIS 13 33 36; & TAS 11). The final stress, supporting the conceptual model of resilience. Additionally, the
model provided a good fit to the data, χ2(430) = 517.50 p = 0.002; paths from sensation seeking to problem-focused coping and resilience,
CFI = 0.954, WRMR = 0.959 RMSEA = 0.029. Since the modified SSV and problem-focused coping to resilience were all significant. These
scale was found to fit the data well and the moderate size of the sample, results were supported by 10,000 bootstrapped confidence intervals
item parcels for each modified SSV subscales were used as indicators of that are shown along with the unstandardized results of the mediation
sensation seeking in the full structural model. analysis in Table 2.
As shown in Table 2 sensation seeking had a significant direct effect
3.1.2. Structural analysis and indirect effect through problem-focused coping on resilience. Al-
Due to the lack of relationship between experiences of trauma and though, the effect of sensation seeking on resilience was decreased after
the psychological resilience markers, we were unable to undertake the controlling for the mediating effect of problem-focused coping, it was
moderation analysis and instead tested only the mediation aspect of the still significant suggesting that this form of coping only partially med-
model. A structural model was specified to test the proposed model, iates the relationship between sensation seeking and resilience. In total,
which hypothesized that BRS, PFCope and SCope would mediate the the model explained 4% of the variance in problem-focused coping and

103
S. McKay et al. Personality and Individual Differences 121 (2018) 100–105

32% of the variance in resilience. 4.3. Measurement issues

Two measurement issues were identified in this study. First, the SSS-
4. Discussion V scale required removal of several items to obtain an adequate fit
during CFA. Several studies have shown problems with this measure
This study tested whether sensation seeking was associated with (Arnett, 1994; Roth, Hammelstein, & Brähler, 2007), and it appears that
increased psychological resilience in persons who had experienced the out-dated language of some questions may be the reason. Alter-
trauma, and if this relationship was mediated by coping. Sensation native sensation seeking scales have been developed (e.g. AISS; Arnett,
seeking was associated with increased psychological resilience in- 1994 & NISS; Roth et al., 2007), however there are still many issues
dicated by increased positive affect and life satisfaction, and decreased with capturing the construct (Zuckerman & Aluja, 2015). The negative
perceived stress. Contrary to expectations, sensation seeking was not affect subscale of the PANAS also did not fit the data well during CFA.
related to negative affect. Problem-focused coping partially mediated Previous studies have shown that the PANAS requires large numbers of
the relationships between sensation seeking and resilience. modifications during CFA (Crawford & Henry, 2004), which suggests
Additionally, although removed from the final model there was positive further measurement development may be required.
relationship between sensation seeking and perceptions of resilience.
4.4. Limitations and future research

4.1. Sensation seeking and resilience Our study had two potential weaknesses. First, it mixed a university
student and general population sample. However, these samples
The final latent psychological resilience factor in the structural showed no major differences in the SEM model and were therefore
model included positive affect, life satisfaction and perceived stress, but combined to provide more power. A second issue is that all measures
did not include negative affect. These three retained measures represent were collected concurrently using self-report questionnaires, which
three important markers of wellbeing that can capture psychological may inflate correlations. Longitudinal research using multiple mea-
responses to adversity and challenge (Luhmann et al., 2012). The sures, which captures the outcome variables at different times with a
measures also offer insight into the processes that may be supporting broader range of participants would strengthen our conclusions.
positive psychological function in the face of adversity. The positive
relationship between sensation seeking and resilience supports our ar- 4.5. Conclusion
gument that sensation seeking changes the primary appraisal process.
For many individuals, the inherent change and novelty of daily life can The negative portrayal of sensation seeking in much of the existing
be taxing (Kashdan & Silvia, 2009). However, sensation seekers often literature presents an incomplete picture of how this construct func-
actively pursue novel and stressful experiences for their potential po- tions. Our study presents evidence that sensation seeking acts as a re-
sitive affect payoffs (Roberti, 2004; Watson et al., 1988). The positive source that supports the management of stress and adversity, and builds
relationship between sensation seeking and positive affect support this psychological resources supporting long term positive functioning and
idea. Additionally, sensation seekers tend to have lower perceptions of resilience. Research on sensation seeking should continue to address
potential negative consequences from stressful situations both the positive and negative outcomes of the trait.
(Jorgensen & Johnson, 1990; Misra & Stokols, 2012). Our results also
demonstrated that sensation seeking was related to lower perceptions of References
stress. Sensation seeking is theorised to decrease perceived stress by
influencing appraisals of stressful events as less overwhelming and Arnett, J. J. (1994). Sensation seeking: A new conceptualization and a new scale.
more controllable (Jorgensen & Johnson, 1990). Our findings, indicate Personality and Individual Differences, 16, 289–296. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0191-
8869(94)90165-1.
that sensation seekers tend to appraise stressors as challenges and op- Bouchard, G., Guillemette, A., & Landry-Léger, N. (2004). Situational and dispositional
portunities for growth and positive affect payoffs rather than perceiving coping: An examination of their relation to personality, cognitive appraisals, and
them as risks for harm or loss of wellbeing. Through this process, sen- psychological distress. European Journal of Personality, 18, 221–238. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1002/per.512.
sation seekers have more resilience indicated by increased positive af- Campbell-Sills, L., Cohan, S. L., & Stein, M. B. (2006). Relationship of resilience to per-
fect and less stress in relation to adversity. sonality, coping, and psychiatric symptoms in young adults. Behaviour Research and
The positive relationship between sensation seeking and the more Therapy, 44, 585–599. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2005.05.001.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1994). Situational coping and coping dispositions in a
stable resilience marker of life satisfaction is important because it
stressful transaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 184–195. http://
suggests that sensation seekers who have experienced trauma have dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.66.1.184.
greater long term life satisfaction than non-sensation seekers. This Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Weintraub, J. K. (1989). Assessing coping strategies: A
theoretically based approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56,
means that sensation seekers are more satisfied with how they have
267–283. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.56.2.267.
handled adversity, are more accepting of their experience, or are able to Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress.
move on more effectively after challenges. However, it is not possible to Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24, 385–396. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/
conclude which of these processes is occurring form the current results. 2136404.
Connor, K. M., & Davidson, J. R. T. (2003). Development of a new resilience scale: The
Connor-Davidson resilience scale (CD-RISC). Depression and Anxiety, 18, 76–82.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/da.10113.
4.2. Coping and perceived resilience Crawford, J. R., & Henry, J. D. (2004). The positive and negative affect schedule
(PANAS): Construct validity, measurement properties and normative data in a large
non-clinical sample. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 43, 245–265. http://dx.doi.
The partial mediation of the relationship between sensation seeking org/10.1348/0144665031752934.
and resilience through problem-focused coping supports the secondary Diener, E. D., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life
scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71–75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/
appraisal argument put forward in the introduction. The finding in- s15327752jpa4901_13.
dicates that sensation seeking acts as a resource to increase belief in and Fletcher, D., & Sarkar, M. (2013). Psychological resilience. European Psychologist, 18,
actual ability to use effective coping strategies. Higher levels of pro- 12–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000124.
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure
blem-focused coping are related to greater ability to actively deal with
analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip.
hardship (Carver & Scheier, 1994). Therefore, it appears that sensation J. 6, 1–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118.
seekers through their awareness of their ability to use such strategies Jorgensen, R. S., & Johnson, J. H. (1990). Contributors to the appraisal of major life
changes: Gender, perceived controllability, sensation seeking, strain, and social
during secondary appraisals consider stressors as more managable.

104
S. McKay et al. Personality and Individual Differences 121 (2018) 100–105

support. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 20, 1123–1138. http://dx.doi.org/10. Pavot, W., & Diener, E. (1993). Review of the satisfaction with life scale. Psychological
1111/j.1559-1816.1990.tb00395.x. Assessment, 5, 164–172. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.5.2.164.
Kashdan, T. B., & Silvia, P. J. (2009). Curiosity and interest: The benefits of thriving on Qualtrics (2015). Qualtrics [Computer software]. Provo, Utah: Qualtrics.
novelty and challenge. In S. J. Lopez, & C. R. Snyder (Eds.). Oxford handbook of Ravert, R. D., Kim, S. Y., Schwartz, S. J., Weisskirch, R. S., Zamboanga, B. L., Ham, L. S.,
positive psychology (pp. 367–375). . http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/ ... Bersamin, M. M. (2013). The association between sensation seeking and well-being
9780195187243.013.0034. among college-attending emerging adults. Journal of College Student Development, 54,
Kenny, D. A., & McCoach, D. B. (2003). Effect of the number of variables on measures of 17–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/csd.2013.0004.
fit in structural equation modeling. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Roberti, J. W. (2004). A review of behavioral and biological correlates of sensation
Journal, 10, 333–351. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15328007SEM1003_1. seeking. Journal of Research in Personality, 38, 256–279. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
Kline, R. B. (2010). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (3rd ed.). New S0092-6566(03)00067-9.
York, NY: Guilford Presshttp://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2012.687667. Roth, M., Hammelstein, P., & Brähler, E. (2007). Beyond a youthful behavior style - age
Lazarus, R., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York, NY: Springer and sex differences in sensation seeking based on need theory. Personality and
Publishing Company. Individual Differences, 43, 1839–1850. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.06.
Litman, J. A., & Lunsford, G. D. (2009). Frequency of use and impact of coping strategies 004.
assessed by the COPE inventory and their relationships to post-event health and well- Seery, M. (2011). Resilience: A silver lining to experiencing adverse life events? Current
being. Journal of Health Psychology, 14, 982–991. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 390–394. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
1359105309341207. 0963721411424740.
Lloyd, D. A., & Turner, R. J. (2008). Cumulative lifetime adversities and alcohol depen- Segerstrom, S. C. (2001). Optimism, goal conflict, and stressor-related immune change.
dence in adolescence and young adulthood. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 93, Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 2, 441–467. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/
217–226. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.09.012. A:1012271410485.
Luhmann, M., Hofmann, W., Eid, M., & Lucas, R. E. (2012). Subjective well-being and Smith, B. W., Dalen, J., Wiggins, K., Tooley, E., Christopher, P., & Bernard, J. (2008). The
adaptation to life events: A meta-analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, brief resilience scale: Assessing the ability to bounce back. International Journal of
102, 592–615. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0025948. Behavioral Medicine, 15, 194–200. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10705500802222972.
Lupton, D., & Tulloch, J. (2002). “Life would be pretty dull without risk”: Voluntary risk- Solomon, Z., Ginzburg, K., Neria, Y., & Ohry, A. (1995). Coping with war captivity: The
taking and its pleasures. Health, Risk & Society, 4, 113–124. http://dx.doi.org/10. role of sensation seeking. European Journal of Personality, 9, 57–70. http://dx.doi.org/
1080/13698570220137015. 10.1002/per.2410090105.
Masten, S. (2001). Ordinary magic. Resilience processes in development. The American Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief
Psychologist, 56, 227–238. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.56.3.227. measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and
Misra, S., & Stokols, D. (2012). Psychological and health outcomes of perceived in- Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063.
formation overload. Environment and Behavior, 44, 737–759. http://dx.doi.org/10. Weathers, F. W., Blake, D. D., Schnurr, P. P., Kaloupek, D. G., Marx, B. P., & Keane, T. M.
1177/0013916511404408. (2013). The life events checklist for DSM-5 (LEC-5). Instrument available from the
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2012). Mplus User's Guide (Seventh Edition). Los National Center for PTSD at www.ptsd.va.gov.
Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén. Zuckerman, M. (1994). Behavioral expressions and biosocial bases of sensation seeking. New
Neria, Y., Solomon, Z., Ginzburg, K., & Dekel, R. (2000). Sensation seeking, wartime York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
performance, and long-term adjustment among Israeli war veterans. Personality and Zuckerman, M., & Aluja, A. (2015). Measures of sensation seeking. In G. J. Boyle, D. H.
Individual Differences, 29, 921–932. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(99) Saklofske, & G. Matthews (Eds.). Measures of personality and social psychological con-
00243-3. structs (pp. 352–380). Oxford: Academic Press.

105

Você também pode gostar