Você está na página 1de 14

At Issue: Vulnerability to Schizophrenia and

Lack of Common Sense


by Qiovanni Stanghellini

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/schizophreniabulletin/article-abstract/26/4/775/1923817 by guest on 27 January 2019


Abstract psychotic symptoms and a persistent feature in nonremit-
ting psychoses. Several theoretical approaches consider
This article explores the hypothesis that the relational impairments of social functioning to be not just an out-
deficit in schizophrenia is not a consequence of acute come of psychotic syndromes but a fundamental phenom-
symptoms and course but instead is a fundamental enon of schizophrenic vulnerability. Vulnerability to
aspect of schizophrenic vulnerability. This basic rela- schizophrenic psychoses seems to involve a basic rela-
tional deficit could be better understood as disconnect- tional deficit (Maj 1998) that can be intuited but is diffi-
edness from common sense. Common sense is a tool cult to define reliably. Nonoperationalized but far-reach-
for adaptation whose main scope is establishing cause- ing concepts—such as the "schizoid-syntonic" distinction
and-effect and motivational relationships in the physi- (Bleuler 1922; Kretschmer 1922; Minkowski 1927)—
cal and social realms. The common sense deficit focus on the attitudes of people with psychosis toward
appears to involve a lack of intuitive attunement their social environments. In this article, I will try to make
(impaired capacity to accurately typify the mental these concepts compatible with empirical testing.
states of other persons because of the incapacity to be Schizophrenic vulnerability will be reinterpreted in rela-
involved in their mental lives) and a damaged social tion to the disconnection from common sense found in
knowledge network (disorders of the background of many people with psychosis.
knowledge useful for organizing everyday experi- To describe situations characterized by detachment
ences). Three dimensions of schizophrenic vulnerabil- from common sense, psychopathology adopts terms like
ity can be distinguished: the sensory, conceptualiza- "perplexity" in acute conditions and "autism" in chronic
tion, and attitudinal dimensions. Sensory disorders are cases. People with or vulnerable to schizophrenia seem to
aberrations of self, body, and world perceptions. be especially prone to bracket common sense, especially
Conceptualization disorders are disturbances in the when compared to those with bipolar disorder (Kraus
attribution of meanings and intentions. Attitudinal dis- 1982). They seem to lack, or sometimes to reject, com-
orders consist of eccentricities in the individual's mon sense categories that are normally used to typify
structure of values and beliefs, characterized by dis- everyday experiences. In short, people with schizophrenia
trust toward conventional knowledge and attunement. are hypoconnected to common sense, while those with
This article describes the present state and possible bipolar disorder are hyperconnected. Traditional vulnera-
future directions of qualitative analyses and empirical bility models (Zubin and Spring 1977; Nuechterlein and
investigations relevant to assessing the interplay Dawson 1984; Strauss et al. 1995) have generally ignored
between vulnerability dimensions and disorders of the issue of disconnectedness from common sense in peo-
common sense. ple with schizophrenia. Little attention has been paid to
Keywords: Common sense, attunement, relational the common sense issue in schizotypal personality (Meehl
deficit, schizophrenia, social knowledge, vulnerability. 1962; Raine et al. 1995; Claridge 1997). More attention to
the patients' attitudes toward common sense is paid by
Schizophrenia Bulletin, 26(4):775-787,2000.
phenomenologically oriented psychopathological studies
on schizophrenic vulnerability (Parnas and Bovet 1991).
Schizophrenia involves severe impairments in social func-
tioning, typically in interpersonal relationships. Social
Send reprint requests to Dr. G. Stanghellini, U.O. Sperimentale di
dysfunction is described in diagnostic manuals (American Psichiatria, Dipartimento di Salute Mentale, Universita di Firenze, Viale
Psychiatric Association 1994) as a consequence of acute Michelangiolo 41,1—50125 Firenze, Italy; e-mail: stan@dada.it.

775
Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 26, No. 4, 2000 G. Stanghellini

My thesis is that the common sense issue is crucial for Basic Disorders of Experience. The recognition of sub-
understanding schizophrenic worlds, since the crisis of jective experience provides the basis for psychopathologi-
common sense is probably the main root of the schizo- cal research on psychoses (Schizophrenia Bulletin, 15[2],
phrenia patient's vulnerable condition, and consequently, 1989). Basic disorders of experience in prodromal phases
the origin of full-blown psychotic symptoms. The concept of schizophrenia have been described as depersonalization
of "crisis of common sense" (Blankenburg 1971) is prob- and derealization phenomena (i.e., strange changes in self
ably the most accurate way to characterize the prodromal and world awareness in which patients feel as if they, the

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/schizophreniabulletin/article-abstract/26/4/775/1923817 by guest on 27 January 2019


changes in thought process, as well as in awareness of and external environment, or both are unreal). Huber and
attitude toward both self and world, that are observed in coworkers and Chapman and coworkers were among the
patients with psychosis. Persisting signs of withdrawal first who emphasized the importance of investigating
and positive symptoms can be interpreted as reactions to early subjective complaints in people with schizophrenia.
disordered emotional and cognitive attunement to the Table 1 summarizes their findings.
world. The main purpose of this article is to redescribe
vulnerability to schizophrenic psychoses in the light of the The Role of the Person in Dealing With Basic
concept of common sense. Disorders. The active role that the person with psychosis
can play in the shaping of psychotic syndromes has been
emphasized in phenomenological psychopathology
Vulnerability Revisited (Stanghellini 1997b) and is implicit in Bleuler's (1911)
distinction between primary symptoms—direct expres-
In a revised vulnerability model, two main components sions of the schizophrenic illness process—and secondary
must be highlighted: (1) the clinical characteristics of symptoms that result from psychoreactive coping strate-
basic disorders of experience that occur in the prodromal gies. Dialectic vulnerability models emphasize the active
phases of schizophrenic episodes, that lead to full-blown role that patients have in interacting with their own dis-
psychotic symptoms, and that last after remission of acute turbing experiences and suggest that this may be the
breakdowns; and (2) the role of the patient in experienc- prime factor in the development of psychotic symptoms
ing prepsychotic phenomena, coping with them, and con- and worldviews. Table 2 summarizes the main character-
stituting psychotic symptoms. istics of these models.

Table 1. Basic disorders of experience


Area of Study
Huber: Chapman:
basic symptoms perceptive aberrations
Prepsychotic, subjective complaints (Huber 1966,1983) Attenuated versions of schizophrenic experiences
(Chapman 1966, Chapman et al. 1978)
Subjective side of schizophrenic vulnerability Indicators of psychosis-proneness (Chapman et
(Klosterkoetter 1988; Huber 1990) al. 1994, 1995)
Loss of control and automatism; impairments of Distortions in perceptions of one's body and
perception, language, thought, memory, and motricity; external objects (Chapman et al. 1978)
anhedonia and anxiety (Suellwold 1986)

Table 2. Dialectic vulnerability models


Author Disturbing experience Personal reaction

Bleuler(1911) Primary symptoms + Psychological reaction


Minkowski(1927) Schizophrenic evolutive factor + Schizoid personality
Wyrsch (1949) Schizophrenic process + Person's position taking
Janzarik(1959) Irruptive dynamic + Stabilizing intentionality
Huber (1983) Basic symptoms + Psychoreactive coping
Mundt(1985) Intentionality breakdown + Intentionality repair
Strauss (1989) Illness + Coping

776
At Issue Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 26, No. 4,2000

Two Main Shortcomings (and Possible Improvements) Vignette 2. Disordered attribution


of the Vulnerability Paradigm. Although perceptual dis-
orders and coping styles represent key features of psy- C.D.B. is a 27-year-old insightful man with schizophre-
nia. He says that nothing is obvious to him; everything
chotic vulnerability, other phenomena should be taken
can be uncanny. The world is complicated, difficult to
into account.
understand: "Why does this happen? What does that
mean? How to explain these facts? Why?" Facts are
The role of conceptualization disorders. The basic rela- not self-evident. Only explanations can give a shape to

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/schizophreniabulletin/article-abstract/26/4/775/1923817 by guest on 27 January 2019


tional deficit in psychosis involves at least two different his experiences. He feels the need for a general theory
levels of disturbed experience: (1) depersonalization and that makes the world understandable and his actions
derealization phenomena that are mainly related to disor- possible. Each theory he builds up is short-lived; it
ders of sensory experience, and (2) disorders of the con- needs to be tested every time a new fact happens. He
ceptualization of experiences (Mundt 1991). does not experience sensory disorders but experiences
Whereas perceptual aberrations often involve derealiza- severe disorders of the conceptualization of reality.
tion feelings (vignette 1), disorders of conceptualization
involve impairments of the symbolic representation of per- Vignette 3. Impairment in social cognition
ceptual stimuli. In the latter, the attribution of their function
to objects, of meanings to social situations, and of motiva- P.S. is a 42-year-old man who has been affected by
tions to other persons' behaviors are disordered (vignette 2). paranoid schizophrenia since the age of 20. At the
onset of his psychosis, he was trying in various ways to
A special category within conceptualization disorders is the
compensate for his difficulties in getting in touch with
disorders of social cognition (Perm et al. 1997), that is, dis- other people. He had no secure ground to interpret the
orders of relatedness to other persons (vignette 3). others' intentions. He lacked the structure of the rules
Conceptualization disorders involving impairments in of social life and systematically set about searching for
social cognition are better accounted for by theories that a well-grounded and natural style of behavior. For
focus on interpersonal awareness, such as phenomenolog- instance, he was busy with an ethological study of the
ical theories of schizophrenic vulnerability based on con- "biological" (i.e., not artificial) foundation of others'
structs of disordered intentionality (Mundt 1985) and cri- behaviors through a double observation of animal and
sis of common sense (Blankenburg 1971; Wiggins et al. human habits. The former was done through television
documentaries, the latter via analyses of human inter-
1990; Parnas and Bovet 1991), and by cognitive
actions in public parks. An atrophy in his knowledge of
approaches such as the theory-of-mind deficit hypothesis, the "rules of the game" led him to engage in intellectual
which is based on the model of child autism interpreted as investigations and to establish his own "know-how" for
the failure to attribute mental states to other people social interactions in a reflective way.
(Baron-Cohen et al. 1994). Blankenburg (1971) argued
that the fundamental source of relationship disturbances
occurring in people with schizophrenia is the loss of nat- ural evidence of commonsensical everyday reality. This is
the core of schizophrenic vulnerability. People with schiz-
Vignettei. Derealization feelings ophrenia seem no longer able to typify their experiences
F.B. is a 24-year-old man whose main psychopathologi- along common sense categories; the at-homeness that
cal feature is disorganization. Especially at the sunset accompanies most of us in our everyday life is not there.
hour, he complains of paniclike attacks of psychotic The role of eccentricity. A second shortcoming of
severity. During these attacks, which he calls "feelings the vulnerability paradigm is that the role of the person
of vagueness," he sees colors more brightly and hears with psychosis in the constitution of the syndrome is
sounds more acutely than usual, and he undergoes mainly conceived as involving coping strategies to try to
experiences of self and body transformation. He is compensate for feelings of derealization and strangeness.
aware that these are pathological experiences and can- This underestimates the individual's attitude toward ordi-
not find any suitable explanation for them. He also sees nary perception and common sense categorizations of the
other people as strange and unfamiliar. These experi-
world. Persons vulnerable to schizophrenic psychoses
ences of derealization and depersonalization are some-
may hold opinions and convictions that are critical of
what "naked" and not otherwise conceptualized.
Although these phenomena of world and self transfor-
common sense conventions, and for this reason these peo-
mation are often accompanied by experiences of refer- ple may attempt deliberately to situate themselves outside
ence, only sometimes do they become truly delusional, the constraints of shared beliefs and values. This is an
that is, changes in the outside world or in the facial active self-positioning outside of commonsensical reality,
expressions of people are usually not interpreted not a passive or merely reactive response to experiences
according to a paranoid attributional style. of derealization and depersonalization.

777
Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 26, No. 4, 2000 G. Stanghellini

Traditional vulnerability models stress derealization ment without any reflection, commonly felt by a whole
and depersonalization as prime causes of overt symptoms order, a whole people, a whole nation or the whole human
and psychotic breakdowns, and they neglect eccentricity, kind." The 18th-century Scottish philosopher T. Reid
i.e., active positioning at the margins of socially shared (Reid 1997) postulated that the human mind could harmo-
worldviews (vignette 4) and the refusal of social norms nize with common sense because of an innate "instinct."
(vignette 5). The structure of values and beliefs of the per- In contrast to Vico's and Reid's praise for common sense,
son who is vulnerable to schizophrenia is also of central modem scientists and philosophers, with few but signifi-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/schizophreniabulletin/article-abstract/26/4/775/1923817 by guest on 27 January 2019


importance. But whether it is a passive experience of cant exceptions, more often have a critical attitude toward
deprivation of one's normal anchoring in the world (i.e., it (Koir6 1965). The major contributions to a positive
disorders of perception and conceptualization) or an "philosophy of common sense" are given by E. Husserl
active rejection of taken-for-granted assumptions about (1970) and his followers, and by L. Wittgenstein (1969),
the world itself (i.e., eccentricity), the crucial element who affirmed that common sense is a set of rules of the
involves distortions in the experience of common sense. game that discipline human behaviors.

Vignette 4. Active self-marginalization Common Sense as a Tool for Adaptation. Common


V.V. is a 22-year-old university student of the humani-
sense is a kind of understanding whose purpose is not the-
ties. She plans to get rid of what she herself calls "het- oretical (knowing about something) but practical (doing
eronomia" (i.e., depending on the rules established by something). It has its roots in evolution, and its purpose is
others). She has recently started a course on Sumerian an adaptive one, since it provides an image of reality built
language: "Since this was the first written language, I up for the pragmatic aim of survival and adaptation
think that in it are expressed parts of the mind that were (Horton 1982). Locke (1688) emphasized that common
working by that age and are silent at present." sense consists of a set of empirical cause-and-effect rela-
Exploring parts of the human mind that were active tionships useful for practical orientation in everyday
once, and are no longer active now, could help her find
actions. Modern psychologists argue that the fundamental
that original and eccentric view on the human condition
that she is looking for. Her personal fight against the
feature of human thinking is its need to understand the
rules established by others results in solitude and a causes and motivations of behavior and everyday events
highly intellectualized style of living. (Heider 1958). Evolutionary psychologists state that "our
literal survival depends upon a finely tuned knowledge of
the causal texture of the world" (Plotkin 1997, p. 185).
Vignette 5. Hyperawareness
E.B. is a 30-year-old man who says of himself "I am a Common Sense and Intersubjectivity. Not only the
paranoid schizophrenic. That's science's verdict. So let it adjustment to and survival of individuals in their immedi-
be." He is very religious and shows esoteric and meta- ate environments, but also the integrity of a whole society
physical interests. During one hospitalization, he was depend on the possession of this "primary theory" of the
often perplexed and found it very difficult to relate to other world that we call common sense. Common sense has
people and in general to the external world. Almost every adaptive significance for both the natural environment and
time he perceived an object, the perception evoked in culture. It is mainly the latter aspect, (i.e., social adapta-
him one concept or word, and that word was kindling an
tion based on an adequate degree of connectedness to
overwhelming trend of word associations. It was like a
'Hood of words" that made him lose his grasp on the
common sense) that is of relevance in psychopathology.
actual situation. Therefore he could not engage in the Social scientists and philosophers highlight the intersub-
most simple activities and participate in conversations. jective character of common sense and its implications for
He explains that in the beginning it was a way "to exer- mutual understanding (Schutz 1962). The main character-
cise memory through perception. A way to enhance my istic of common sense is its self-evidence for all members
mind. I was looking at one thing and I hadto put my of a given community—it comprises all that the members
strength in order to evoke another thing, and so on. I was of a given society are disposed to take as obvious and
fixed on potentiating my brain. It all started as a voluntary unproblematic (Husserl 1970).
action that later turned into a passive experience."

The Social Imprinting of Common Sense. Our knowl-


What Is Common Sense? edge of the world we live in derives only in part from our
firsthand experience; to a much greater extent, it is
"Common sense" is not an easy concept to define. The socially derived—handed down by reports and teachings
18th-century Italian philosopher G.B. Vico (1744, Degnita of others (Schutz 1962). The common sense world is an
12) gave the following sweeping definition: "It is a judge- old inheritance. We learn it as children who faithfully

778
At Issue Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 26, No. 4, 2000

accept the teachings of adults at a stage of psychological conceptualized as a stock of knowledge useful at the level
growth that is prior to doubts and uncertainties. In short: of everyday life. Common sense is a set of rules of infer-
we are imprinted with it. ence that is the medium for organizing the everyday expe-
riences of individuals and social groups (Flick 1998). It is
Preservation of Common Sense. The common sense the foundation of people's expectations and of meaning
world enjoys relative stability and consistency among attributions (Moscovici and Hewstone 1983). This process
individuals who belong to the same group. The remark- of inference is mainly automatic in nature—motivations
able stability of common sense is due to the tendency to and meanings are attributed prereflectively.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/schizophreniabulletin/article-abstract/26/4/775/1923817 by guest on 27 January 2019


assimilate the unusual into the usual. As a rule, experi-
ences that fit common sense typifications are felt as more Common Sense as Intuitive Attunement. Phenome-
real than those that are idiosyncratic. If an experience nological definitions (Merleau-Ponty 1945; Husserl 1970)
does not fit common sense schemata, it is usually not the emphasize the second aspect of common sense: the
schemata that are called into question; rather, the new importance of preconceptual perceptive attunement
character of the experience is ruled out and normalized. between the subject and the outer world that is rooted in
intuition, corporeality, and the affective or emotional life.
What Is Common Sense About? Two main categories of Bovet and Parnas (1993, p. 583) write, "Common sense
facts are dealt with by common sense—physical states reflects our preconceptual attunement to the world. It
and psychological ones. Common sense represents a reveals not so much what is evident but how it is evident,
guideline for orientation in both realms. The 18th-century the constantly present and tacit frame of experience . . . .
Scottish philosopher D. Hume (1739-1740) held that the The defect in common sense can manifest itself in . . . a
human mind's main purpose was to detect cause-and- lack of the sense for the 'rules of the game' of human
effect relationships. Modern psychology seems to confirm behavior." However, the concept of attunement is rather
Hume's theory (Sperber et al. 1995) in arguing that every- vague. What sort of psychological process is involved?
one is a natural physicist and a natural psychologist What is it that a person is (or is not) attuned with?
(Plotkin 1997). While physical causation deals with the In everyday experience, we feel familiarly related to
relationships between material objects (e.g., "If I touch the environment and to current social situations. At a per-
fire, then my hand will burn"), psychological causation is ceptual level, objects in the world and persons appear
concerned with interpretation of others' mental states familiar (i.e., as we expect them to be according to our
through the attunement with and observation of others' past experience). In social situations, the movements,
behaviors (actions, facial expressions, etc.; e.g., "If she expressions, and utterances of others also appear typical
smiles, she is happy"). This property of detecting the and understandable. There is a break with ordinary experi-
intentions and motivations behind somebody's behaviors ence when objects or persons disagree perceptually with
is called "mind reading" (Baron-Cohen et al. 1994). expectancies (e.g., when colors are too bright or details
Social cognition (Perm et al. 1997) is especially relevant magnified—perceptive aberrations). But one can also
to the issue of psychotic vulnerability, since disorders of have this feeling of strangeness without any change on the
the capacity to attribute meanings to social situations are ' perceptual level, that is, when the meanings of objects in
involved in psychotic syndromes. Children with autism the world (e.g., "What is this chair for?") and of the
seem to have an incapacity to make sense of others' actions of others (e.g., "Why is he laughing?") appear
behaviors (Baron-Cohen et al. 1994). In people with uncanny. The attunement concept refers mainly to this
schizophrenia, the ability to attribute mental states is also second level of experience, which involves the intuitive
impaired (Frith 1992) and delusions can be seen as the attribution of meanings to objects and situations, and of
effect of the "exclusion of casualness" (Berner 1982). intentions to other people. Phenomenological research in
psychopathology has been mainly concerned with what
Common Sense as Social Knowledge. One may ask the philosopher J.R. Searle (1995) calls "social reality,"
whether common sense is a set of "commonplaces" whose main components may be considered "mental
involving particular beliefs or whether it is more ade- facts" and "social facts," as opposed to brute physical
quately represented as a "sense" useful for attuning with facts (e.g., "There is snow on Mt. Everest"). The concept
what is "common." Is common sense a network of knowl- of intuitive attunement—reshaped within the framework
edge relevant to ordinary life situations shared by the of the theory-of-mind (Baron-Cohen et al. 1994)—may
members of a given society? Or is it more of a psychic help to explain some aspects of the phenomena that hap-
function aimed at understanding current social situations pen in the social space and involve social cognition in
and at harmonizing with them? Or both? The construct of attributing mental states to partners. Social attunement
common sense has two aspects. The first aspect can be involves the ability to intuit (from Latin intuire, intus +

779
Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 26, No. 4, 2000 G. Stanghellini

ire, "to go inside") what is going on in somebody else's These different dimensions of schizophrenic vulnera-
mind. I can attune with a given social situation when I am bility can be outlined as follows:
able to comprehend thoughts and feelings of others by
1. Sensory-level disorders (perception of self, body,
observing their behavior and to explain others' behavior
and world)
in terms of intentional (e.g., "He is looking for an
2. Conceptualization-level disorders (attribution of
aspirin") or nonintentional (e.g., "He has a headache")
meaning and intention)
mental facts. Social cognition is based on a fundamental
2.1 Damaged social knowledge network
feature of the human mind, the urge to establish motiva-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/schizophreniabulletin/article-abstract/26/4/775/1923817 by guest on 27 January 2019


2.1.1 Atrophy of social knowledge network
tional relationships, and understanding other minds proba-
2.1.2 Hypertrophy of social knowledge
bly depends on coordinating one's own movement and
network
facial expressions with those of others—a capacity
2.2 Lack of intuitive attunement
already present in newborn babies (Butterworth 1999).
3. Attitudinal-level disorders (eccentric structure of
values and beliefs): distrust toward conventional
Schizophrenic Vulnerability as a Disorder knowledge and intuitiveness
3.1 Skepticism
of Common Sense Typification 3.2 Hypertolerance of ambiguity
Studies on premorbid adjustment of people with schizo-
phrenia (Malmberg et al. 1998) and high-risk studies Sensory-Level Disorders. Sensory-level disorders are
(Dworkin et al. 1993; Dworkin et al. 1994; Olin et al. aberrations in the perception of self, body, and world.
1998; Hodges et al. 1999) demonstrate that early prob- Strictly speaking, these are not disorders of common
lems in interpersonal relationships are strongly associated sense. I mention this category of disorders, however,
with schizophrenia. Boys who will develop schizophre- because they are involved in the pathogenesis of schizo-
nia show disruptive behaviors and social anxiety, and phrenic psychoses and are presumably related to the dis-
they suffer from rejection by peers; girls are often ner- orders of conceptualization of reality. These disorders of
vous and withdrawn. Severe impairments in social com- subjective experience occur very early in the course of
petence such as social isolation, sensitivity, odd behavior, schizophrenia (Huber 1983) and are relevant for the
and odd speech are common features in people with development of full-blown psychoses (Chapman et al.
schizotypal personality (Kendler et al. 1989). A basic 1978; Klosterkoetter 1988).
relational deficit is supposed to be a core phenomenon of
the schizophrenic condition (Maj 1998). In this section, Conceptualization-Level Disorders
the global deficit involved in schizophrenic vulnerability Damaged social knowledge network. Conceptual-
will be conceptualized as a disconnection from common ization disorders involve impairments in the attribution of
sense. Two main areas of this construct are at issue: com- meanings and intentions that can be related either to an
mon sense as social knowledge, and common sense as atrophy or to a hypertrophy of the background of knowl-
intuitive attunement. Since the concept of "typification" . edge useful for typifying everyday experiences.
(Schutz 1962) includes both these aspects, I will adopt Atrophy. In the case of atrophy, there is a lack of
this term to designate that feature of human thinking available knowledge of socially shared categories to typ-
through which we make sense of the world by attributing ify the actions of both others and oneself. People with
meanings, causes, and intentions to objects, situations, schizophrenia, relative to those with neuroses or bipolar
and other persons. In the case of schizophrenic vulnera- disorder (Cutting and Murphy 1988, 1990) are markedly
bility, we may speak of hypotypification. Deficits of com- deficient in "real-world knowledge"—the majority of peo-
mon sense typification can be described as a deficit in the ple with schizophrenia seem to have deficient knowledge
knowledge of commonsensical beliefs or rules—what can concerning events and behaviors in the world around
be termed the damaged social knowledge network them. Those with hebephrenia especially are impaired in
hypothesis. It is also possible that abnormalities in com- their knowledge of how other people might behave in a
mon sense typification are a consequence of an individ- certain situation (see also vignette 3). According to these
ual's distrust toward social knowledge and criticism findings, Cutting and Murphy argue that incompetent
toward its conventionality. Deficits in common sense typ- social judgment is an intrinsic feature of schizophrenia
ification can also be interpreted as consequences of a lack (especially of hebephrenia) and not a consequence of
of intuitive attunement, or alternatively as an attitude of environmental factors or actual illness. These findings
distrust toward intuition (and an emphasis on rationality, confirm classic phenomenological hypotheses about the
for instance). pathogenetical role of lack of common sense in schizo-

780
At Issue Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 26, No. 4, 2000

phrenia, in which the pragmatic use of things is affected Vignette 6. Impaired intuitive attunement
(Minkowski 1927).
M.M. is a 42-year-old woman affected by paranoid
Hypertrophy. Disorders of conceptualization can also schizophrenia. During a therapy session she asked me
be due to a hypertrophied background of knowledge-at- and herself, "What is reasonableness? It is the light that
hand. The phenomenon of "automatic consciousness" allows the interpretation of reality. In me it is switched
experienced by E.B. (vignette 5) is a well-known and off. It is not spontaneous. One must work on it. I feel
fundamental feature of psychotic thinking. Complex switched off here in my forehead. Interpreting reality is
associations are passively triggered by a single word or also saying: 'I don't like him!' It is a subjective interpre-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/schizophreniabulletin/article-abstract/26/4/775/1923817 by guest on 27 January 2019


perceptual detail. This stream of consciousness generates tation of reality. Everything becomes gray, flat. Anything
the emergence of meanings that would usually remain goes. Indeed, nothing goes: I cannot move away from
implicit in normal experience. The emergence of the my bed. I do feel a 'lunar' energy that is tackling the
explicit from the implicit is responsible for an "expansion sun and eclipses it. I experience the others as if they
were empty. The sun is missing. The perception of one-
of the horizon of meanings" (Schwartz et al. 1997, p.
ness and multiplicity is missing. When a subject gets to
178). An enlarged horizon of meanings may reflect a
know something, he becomes one with it. But subjectiv-
hypertrophied network of knowledge-at-hand and a lack ity is needed in order to get to know something. In me,
of inhibition in the associations between semantically it is as if subjectivity were subjugated, and I do not
related concepts. The phenomenon of automatic con- know by what. I dive a lot of times into my psycholo-
sciousness shows that disentanglement from common gist's head because I can manage to get out from it. I
sense can occur not only through a defective state (atro- do not dive into my mother's head because I am
phy of social knowledge network), but also vice-versa scared. She was a medium. I am afraid to get lost in
via a positive condition. Indeed, these free-association her head, to get caught in it."
variations may lead to a condition of hyperawareness in
which all possible meanings linked to a given situation she has no motivation to move away from her bed.
emerge. A hyperextended network of semantic associa- Avolition, social isolation, and apathy are consequences
tion stored in the data base is actualized by a single expe- of what she calls the "eclipse" of the "light that allows
rience. Relevant meanings are no longer selected from interpretation of reality." She also explains how she tries
the set of all possible meanings. There is a failure of a to react. If the world does not spontaneously seem signifi-
purpose-at-hand device that can no longer select just cant and worthy, then one "must work on it." Reflective
those meanings that are relevant to a given situation. forms of thinking may be useful to compensate for this
These abnormalities in the conceptualization of experi- impairment of intuitiveness or common sense; indeed, a
ence are studied according to the experimental paradigms very complicated, hyperreflexive (Sass 1992) paraphilo-
of cortical maps and semantic priming (Evans 1997; sophical system arose soon after the onset of her psy-
Schwartz et al. 1997), which generate hypotheses of neu- chosis. Last but not least, she alludes to a possible patho-
robiological relationships that can be empirically tested genesis of her basic lack of intuitiveness, relating it to a
(Spitzer et al. 1993). developmental context in which her mother's mind was
Lack of intuitive attunement. Lack of intuitive experienced as dangerous and scary. What she seems to
attunement can be defined as an impairment in the capac- be lacking is the basic confidence, probably based on
ity to typify the mental states of others accurately because mother-infant "affective-cognitive-conative" relatedness
of the incapacity to be involved in others' mental lives (Hobson 1994), necessary to direct oneself to someone
(vignette 6). else's mind without being afraid to lose one's subjectivity.
What is "reasonableness" to M.M.? It is the capacity Experimental studies designed to assess such disor-
to interpret reality spontaneously and the ability to take ders of intuitive attunement test the ability to infer inten-
someone else's point of view without feeling subjugated. tions behind indirect speech (Cocoran et al. 1995), to
Reasonableness is common sense as intuitive detect characters' beliefs in short stories (Frith and
attunement—the ability to be aware that others have Cocoran 1996), or to attribute mental states in nonverbal
minds and to understand their thoughts and feelings by scenarios (Sarfati et al. 1997a, 19976). The theoretical
being attuned to their behaviors. M.M. describes what paradigm of these studies is the theory-of-mind deficit in
happens when someone lacks spontaneous reasonableness schizophrenia (Bebbington 1988; Frith 1992). These stud-
or intuition: reality looks flat and colorless; people seem ies confirm that people with schizophrenia have an
empty and anonymous. The image of "lunar" energy impaired ability to accurately perceive the intentions of
eclipsing the light of the sun neatly expresses this experi- other individuals. The failure to attribute relevant inten-
ence of a desolate and dehumanized world. Since every- tions to others is more severe in disorganized people with
thing becomes equally uninteresting ("anything goes"), schizophrenia than in other schizophrenic subgroups and

781
Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 26, No. 4, 2000 G. Stanghellini

normal controls (Sarfati et al. 1997a, 1997b). Thought, Vignette 8. Bracketing of common sense1
language, and communication disorders are especially
highly correlated with this inability. People with schizo- F. is now 26 years old. He was once a sensible boy, who
loved to be alone. At the age of fifteen he began feeling
phrenia who fail to appreciate other people's mental states
the pure and romantic desire of having a girl friend. No
do not seem to rely on what is specifically relevant to the
sensual factor was involved. But since he was very shy,
context, but on two competing strategies of decoding: he could not fulfil his desire. This did not trouble him
either they choose the prevailing meaning of an ambigu- very much, nonetheless he thought it was suitable at his
ous stimulus (e.g., a word), thus relying on their back- age to have a sentimental life and so he tried to behave

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/schizophreniabulletin/article-abstract/26/4/775/1923817 by guest on 27 January 2019


ground knowledge (Swiney 1984), or else they show as an individual who is in love and sad. He would like to
facilitation for word recognition by semantic categoriza- feel melancholic, but he cannot. Therefore, in order to
tion (Koh and Peterson 1978; Traupman 1980). be coherent ('logique') with his plan, he spared no effort
to look as if he were in love and sad, and he began to
Attitudinal-Level Disorders (Eccentric Structure of "derange." So he decides to try not to see reality as it is.
Reality humiliates him, he does his best to deny it. But
Values and Beliefs): Distrust Toward Conventional
he also defends himself from all pleasures and wants to
Knowledge and Intuitiveness. The concept of attitude
suppress little by little all his life. He puts forth all his
refers to the individual's philosophy of life, that is, the strength of imagination to deny reality. He does every-
structure of values and beliefs that orients attitudes and thing he can to disengage from reality. Now he can
actions. Persons vulnerable to schizophrenia often have an dominate his own state. He becomes aware of all his
attitude of distrust toward conventional knowledge, as for actions. When he happens to stand up mechanically, he
instance E.C. (vignette 7), who expresses his disdainful sits down and makes that movement again, this time
refusal of conventional values and beliefs, or V.V. consciously. He does not want to do anything mechani-
(vignette 4), whose existential project—getting rid of het- cally. He hates spontaneity, because it would lead him
eronomia (dependence on the rules established by oth- back to his normal life.
ers)—led her to solitude, instinct suppression, and a 1
Minkowski 1924 [italics added].
highly intellectualized and eccentric style of living. Their
behaviors may reflect not only the effort to compensate
ity. This "mind's perverse self-apotheosis" can be seen as
for the loss of common sense, but also the deliberate
the "end-point of the trajectory consciousness follows
attempt to disengage from all sources of spontaneity,
when it separates from the body and the passions, and
instinctiveness, automatic actions, and unwitting typifica-
from the social and practical world, and turns upon itself
tions. Vignette 8 is taken from Minkowski's (1924)
(Sass 1994, p. 12; see also Sass 1992).
description of a young man with schizophrenia who
Skepticism. Common sense conceptualizations may
refuses spontaneity and uses all his strength to disconnect
be dismantled by rational criticism. This is the case with a
himself from vital contact with reality.
skeptical attitude toward common sense because of a dis-
Cases of this kind are rare in the literature and trust of prereflective typification and an emphasis on
unusual in clinical practice. Nonetheless they illustrate the rationality. When this happens, the destruction of the
possibility that a schizophrenic condition can evolve, building blocks of everyday experience comes from
rather than from a loss of common sense, from an active above, rather than from below: common sense is dis-
and voluntary bracketing of common sense (Stanghellini missed by means of a rational or pseudophilosophical
skepticism. The German philosopher Rosenzweig (1984)
1997a, 19976). Schizophrenia can also be generated from
ironically defined this paralysis of common sense as
within rationality itself, rather than by the loss of rational- "apoplexia philosophica"—an intellectualistic condition
Vignette 7. Refusal of social norms (vignette 9, very similar to self-reports of persons recov-
E.C. is a 17-year-old boy with psychosis and is showing ered from acute catatonic schizophrenia) derived from a
severe difficulties at school and in social relationships. distrust of common sense typification and related to
He withdrew to his bedroom and was aggressive with hyperreflexivity.
those who tried to get in touch with him. Although he Cutting (1997) described this condition as related to a
complains of cognitive problems (perception, concen- shift to the left along the intelligence-intuition axis. This
tration, and memory), he nonetheless gets involved in issue is also addressed by Minkowski's (1927) "morbid
extensive readings about Eastern philosophies and rationalism" and Sass's (1992) hyperreflexivity (see also
tries to get original insights from writing with his left
vignette 2). Whereas the latter emphasizes the schizo-
hand (he is right-handed). He himself speaks of his dis-
phrenic mind's self-destruction, morbid rationalism is a
dainful refusal of conventional values and beliefs, which
he identifies with his parents' teachings. coping reaction that invades the psychic life of the indi-
vidual who has lost vital contact with reality—an attempt

782
At Issue Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 26, No. 4, 2000

Vignette 9. Apoplexia philosophies1 ivity and the tendency to reify abstractions. It is also possi-
ble for an attitude based on the hypertolerance of ambigu-
"We moved to the patient's bedside. The patient had to
ity to generate the refusal of conventional common sense
confine himself to his bed; he was no longer able to
typifications that do not reflect enough of the complexity
perform any of the essential daily actions. He felt para-
lyzed. The rigidity of his stupor had taken control over
of reality and to promote the search for idiosyncratic and
him. His hands could no longer grasp, for who was it esoteric cues to interpret reality itself.
that gave them the right to take things? His feet could

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/schizophreniabulletin/article-abstract/26/4/775/1923817 by guest on 27 January 2019


no longer walk, for who could ensure ground for their
steps? His eyes could no longer look, for who could Conclusions
prove to him that no dream was fooling them? The
same was true for his ears that could no longer listen, The basic assumption of this article is that the relational
for was it really worthwhile to pull something out of the deficit observed in patients with schizophrenia is not a
void?" consequence of acute symptoms or a social or institutional
1
artifact but a fundamental aspect of schizophrenic vulner-
Rosenzweig 1984.
ability. Phenomenological psychopathology has attempted
to describe this basic relational deficit as disconnected-
to re-establish new rules for typification when life can no ness from common sense. The first aim of this study was
longer be lived but only thought of (Minkowski 1947). to gather philosophical cues and empirical evidence
Both pathways, active bracketing and passive loss of (mainly from developmental psychology) to stipulate a
common sense, would seem to be possible. definition of common sense. In my view, common sense
Hypertolerance of ambiguity. Hypotypification, is a tool for adaptation that is generally shared by the
whose source can be traced back to the vulnerable individ- members of a given community, derived from a process of
ual's set of values and beliefs, can also originate from a social imprinting, and preserved by assimilation of the
hypertolerance of ambiguity (Frenkel-Brunswik 1949). As unusual into the usual. Its main purpose is the discerning
in the case of skepticism, this can also be a deliberate epis- of cause-and-effect relationships in the physical realm and
temological attitude, related to a concern for the complex- motivational ones in the social world. The latter are espe-
ity of reality, to a realization that facts-in-the-world, with cially relevant in psychopathology. There is considerable
their infinite profiles, cannot be caught by a single concep- empirical evidence that the schizophrenic condition is
tualization or reduced to a unidimensional form of under- characterized by loss of common sense of two kinds: lack
standing. Such an attitude of intolerance toward the of intuitive attunement (i.e., impairment in the capacity to
restricted horizon of common sense typifications is also accurately typify the mental states of others because of an
partly reflected in the case of the patient E.B. (vignette 5), incapacity to be involved in their mental lives) and a dam-
who does all he can to enhance his mind's capacity to aged network of social knowledge (i.e., of the background
understand. He uses all his power to connect the percep- knowledge useful for organizing everyday experiences).
tion of the meaning of one object with a trend of associ- In the proposed model of schizophrenic vulnerability, I
ated meanings. An enlarged horizon of meanings may not distinguish three dimensions: sensory disorders, conceptu-
simply reflect an automatic train of more or less relevant alization disorders, and attitudinal disorders. Although
associations. It may also reflect a deliberate attempt to sensory disorders (aberrations of self, body, and world
enlarge one's own mental capacity for conceptualization. perceptions) are not common sense disorders in a strict
The philosophical paragon of this attitude is the "art of sense, they are tightly interconnected with common-sense
memory," a mental exercise to improve memory through disorders. Conceptualization disorders (disturbances in
the activation of fluid semantic associations (Yates 1966; the attribution of meanings and intentions) are related
Rossi 1983). The art of memory is also an esoteric either to a damaged social knowledge network or to a lack
hermeneutic instrument to break through the innermost of intuitiveness. Conceptualization disorders include
secrets of nature. In fact, E.B.'s mnemonics evolve toward social knowledge network disorders and disorders of intu-
overt psychopathology in two ways. There is a shift from itive attunement. Social knowledge network disorders can
activity to passivity: he begins with a voluntary attempt to involve either atrophy of the social knowledge data base
enlarge his mental capacities and ends up by being discon- or a kind of hypertrophy because of a hyperextended net-
nected from common sense typifications. There is a second work of semantic associations between the concepts
shift from using the art of memory as a mnemonic device stored in the data base. Both atrophy and hypertrophy lead
to using it as a tool to interpret the complex symbolism of to deficits in social functioning. Disorders of intuitive
reality. In short, a skeptical attitude toward common sense attunement are manifest as a defective ability to attribute
typifications can generate a metaphysical move (asking for mental states in short stories or nonverbal scenarios. This
the proper meaning of something), leading to hyperreflex- approach to disorders of intuitive attunement relies on the

783
Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 26, No. 4, 2000 G. Stanghellini

theory-of-mind deficit paradigm. Attitudinal disorders Berner, P. Psychiatrische Systematik. Bern-Stuttgart-


(eccentric structures of values and beliefs) are character- Wien, Germany: Huber, 1982.
ized by distrust toward common sense (i.e., conventional Blankenburg, W. Der Verlust der natuerlichen
knowledge and intuitiveness). They include a skeptical Selbstvertaendlichkeit: Ein Beitrag zur Psychopathologie
attitude toward common sense and a hypertolerance of Symptomarmer Schizophrenien. Stuttgart, Germany:
ambiguity, reflecting a refusal to conceptualize the world Enke, 1971.
through the simplifying views given by common sense.
Bleuler, E. Dementia praecox oder Gruppe der
Further empirical investigations are needed to verify

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/schizophreniabulletin/article-abstract/26/4/775/1923817 by guest on 27 January 2019


Schizophrenien. Leipzig, Germany: Deutike, 1911.
the basic hypothesis that disorders of common sense are
primary phenomena of schizophrenic vulnerability and not Bleuler, E. Die Probleme der Schizoidie und der Syntonie.
side effects of schizophrenic symptoms and course. This Zeitschrift fuer die Gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie,
could be done in longitudinal studies, especially with high- 78:373-399, 1922.
risk subjects, by assessing conceptualization disorders Bovet, P., and Parnas, J. Schizophrenic delusions: A phe-
before the onset of overt schizophrenic symptoms and by nomenological approach. Schizophrenia Bulletin,
looking for their possible correlation with disorders of 19(3):579-597,1993.
social competence in people who subsequently develop Butterworth, G. A developmental-ecological perspective
schizophrenia. The relationship between attitudinal-level on Strawson's "The Self." In: Gallagher, S., and Shear, J.,
disorders and social competence should also be investi- eds. Models of the Self. Thorverton, U.K.: Imprint
gated in high-risk subjects before the onset of psychotic Academic, 1999. pp. 203-211.
symptoms. Interview-based research instruments should be
developed to assess the patterns of values and beliefs of Chapman, J. The early symptoms of schizophrenia.
people at risk for schizophrenia in order to test the hypoth- British Journal of Psychiatry, 121:225-263, 1966.
esis that, in some cases, an eccentric attitudinal orientation Chapman, L.J.; Chapman, J.P.; and Kwapil, T.R. Scales for
can be involved in the pathogenesis of the schizophrenic the measurement of schizotypy. In: Raine, A.; Lencz, T; and
relational deficit. Also, the interplay between the three dif- Mednick, S.A., eds. Schizotypal Personality. Cambridge,
ferent dimensions of schizophrenic vulnerability needs NY: Cambridge University Press, 1995. pp. 79-106.
more investigation. It is still unclear how the different Chapman, L.J.; Chapman, J.P.; Kwapil, T.R.; Eckbald,
components of each dimension interact with each other M ; and Zinser, M.C. Putatively psychosis prone subjects
(i.e., how disorders of the social knowledge data base and ten years later. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
of intuitiveness are related to each other). It is also unclear 103:171-183,1994.
how different dimensions affect each other (i.e., how a dis- Chapman, L.J.; Chapman, J.P.; and Raulin, M.L. Body
order in one dimension can affect a nondisordered one, or image aberrations in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal
how a nondisordered dimension may prevent a schizo- Psychology, 87:339-347, 1978.
phrenic breakdown when other dimensions are affected).
In the model I propose, the three dimensions are relatively Claridge, G., ed. Schizotypy: Implications for Illness and
independent of each other, that is, one can be disordered Health. Oxford, U.K.: Oxford University Press, 1997.
independently from the other two (vignettes 1 and 2). But Cocoran, R.; Mercer, G.; and Frith, C D . Schizophrenia,
it can also happen that a disorder in one dimension may symptomatology and social inference: Investigating the
affect another (vignette 5). It is tempting to hypothesize "theory of mind" in people with schizophrenia.
that at least two dimensions must be affected to imply a Schizophrenia Research, 17:5-13, 1995.
schizophrenic syndrome in the full clinical sense. Cutting, J. Principles of Psychopathology: Two Worlds—
Two Minds—Two Hemispheres. Oxford, U.K.: Oxford
University Press, 1997.
References
Cutting, J., and Murphy, D. Schizophrenic thought disor-
American Psychiatric Association. DSM—IV: Diagnostic der: A psychological and organic interpretation. British
and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed. Journal of Psychiatry, 152:310-319, 1988.
Washington, DC: APA, 1994. Cutting, J., and Murphy, D. Impaired ability of schizo-
Baron-Cohen, S.; Tager-Flushberg, H.; and Cohen, D.J. phrenics, relative to manics or depressives, to appreciate
Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from Autism. social knowledge about their culture. British Journal of
Oxford, U.K.: Oxford Medical Publications, 1994. Psychiatry, 157:355-358, 1990.
Bebbington, P., ed. Social Psychiatry: Theory, Methodology, Dworkin, R.H.; Cornblatt, B.A.; Friedman, R.;
and Practice. New Brunswick, NJ: Transactions, 1988. Kaplansky, L.M.; Lewis, S.A.; Rinaldi, A.; Shilliday, C ;

784
At Issue Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 26, No. 4, 2000

and Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L. Childhood precursors of affec- Phenomenological Philosophy. Husserliana VII.


tive vs. social deficits in adolescents at risk for schizophre- Evanston, IL: The Northwestern University Press, 1970.
nia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 19(3):563-577,1993. Janzarik, W. Dynamische Grundkonstellationen in endo-
Dworkin, R.H.; Lewis, J.A.; Cornblatt, B.A.; and genen Psychosen. Berlin, Germany: Springer, 1959.
Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L. Social competence deficits in ado- Kendler, K.S.; Lieberman, J.A.; and Walsh, D. The
lescents at risk of schizophrenia. Journal of Nervous and Structured Interview for Schizotypy (SIS): A preliminary
Mental Disease, 182:103-108,1994. report. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 15(4):559-571, 1989.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/schizophreniabulletin/article-abstract/26/4/775/1923817 by guest on 27 January 2019


Evans, J.L. Semantic activation and preconscious process- Klosterkoetter, J. Basissymptome un Endphaenomene der
ing in schizophrenia and schizotypy. In: Claridge, G., ed. Schizophrenic. Berlin, Germany: Springer, 1988.
Schizotypy: Implications for Illness and Health. Oxford,
U.K.: Oxford University Press, 1997. pp. 80-97. Koh, S.D., and Peterson, R.A. Encoding orientation and
the remembering of schizophrenic young adults. Journal
Flick, U. Everyday knowledge in social psychology. In: of Abnormal Psychology, 87:303-313, 1978.
Flick, U., ed. The Psychology of the Social. Cambridge, NY:
Cambridge University Press, 1998. pp. 1-12. Koire\ A. Newtonian Studies. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press, 1965.
Frenkel-Brunswik, E. Intolerance of ambiguity as an emo-
tional and perceptual personality variable. Journal of Kraus, A. Identity and psychosis of the manic-depressive.
Personality, 18:108-143,1949. In: De Koning, A.J.J., and Jenner, F.A., eds. Phenom-
enology and Psychiatry. London, U.K.: Academic Press,
Frith, CD. The Cognitive Neuropsychology of Schizophre-
1982. pp. 201-216.
nia. Hove, U.K.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1992.
Kretschmer, E. Koerperbau und Charakter. Berlin-
Frith, CD., and Cocoran, R. Exploring "theory of mind" in
Goettingen-Heidelberg, Germany: Springer, 1922.
people with schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine,
26:521-530,1996. Locke, J. An Essay Concerning Human Understanding.
Heider, F. The Psychology of Interpersonal Relations. New (1688) Edited by A.C. Frazer. Oxford, U.K.: Oxford
York, NY: Wiley, 1958. University Press, 1979.
Hobson, R.P. Understanding persons: The role of affect. Maj, M. Critique of the DSM-FV operational diagnostic
In: Baron-Cohen, S.; Tager-Flushberg, H.; and Cohen, criteria for schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry,
D.J., eds. Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives From 172:458-460, 1998.
Autism. Oxford, U.K.: Oxford Medical Publications, Malmberg, A.; Lewis, G.; David, A.; and Allebeck, P.
1994. pp. 204-227. Premorbid adjustment and personality in people with
Hodges, A.; Byrne, M.; Grant, E.; and Johnstone, E. People schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry,
at risk of schizophrenia: Sample characteristics of the first 172:308-313, 1998.
100 cases in the Edinburgh High-Risk Study. British Meehl, P.E. Schizotaxia, schizotypy, schizophrenia.
Journal of Psychiatry, 174:547-553,1999. American Psychologist, 17:827-838,1962.
Horton, R. Tradition and modernity revisited. In: Hollis, M., Merleau-Ponty, M. Phenomenologie de la Perception.
and Lukes, S. eds. Rationality and Relativism. Oxford, Paris, France: Gallimard, 1945.
U.K.: Basil Blackwell, 1982. pp. 201-260. Minkowski, E. Psychiatrie et me'taphysique: A la
Huber, G. Reine Defektsyndrome und Basisstadien endo- recherche de l'humain et du vecu. Revue de Metaphysique
gener Psychosen. Fortschritte fuer Neurologie und et de Morale, 52:3-A, 33-358, 1947.
Psychiatrie, 34:409-426, 1966. Minkowski, E. La Schizophrenic: Psychopathologie des
Huber, G. Das Konzept substratnaher Basissymptome und Schizoides et des Schizophrenes. (1927) 2nd ed. Paris,
seine Bedeutung fuer Theorie und Therapie schizophrener France: Payot, 1953.
Erkrankungen. Nervenarzt, 54:23-32,1983.
Minkowski, E. Les schizophrenes peints par eux-memes.
Huber, G., ed. Idiopatische Psychosen: Psychopatho- {Medicine, 5, 1924.) Reprinted in: Postel, J., ed. La
logie—Neurobiologie—Therapie. Stuttgart, Germany: Psychiatrie. Paris, France: Larousse, 1994.
Schattauer, 1990.
Moscovici, S., and Hewstone, M. Social representations
Hume, D. A Treatise of Human Nature. (1739-1740) and social explanations: From the "naive" to the "ama-
Oxford, U.K.: Clarendon Press, 1955. teur" scientist. In: Hewstone, M., ed. Attribution Theory:
Husserl, E. The Crisis of the European Sciences and Social and Functional Extensions. Oxford, U.K.: Basil
Transcendental Phenomenology: An Introduction to Blackwell, 1983. pp. 98-125.

785
Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 26, No. 4, 2000 G. Stanghellini

Mundt, C. Das Apathiesyndrom der Schizophrenen. Eine Netherlands: Nijhoff, 1962. pp. 3-27.
psychopathologische und computertomographische Schwartz, M.A.; Wiggins, O.P.; and Spitzer, M. Psychotic
Untersuchung. Berlin, Germany: Springer, 1985. experience and disordered thinking: A reappraisal from
Mundt, C. Constituting reality—its decline and repair in the new perspectives. Journal of Nervous and Mental
long-term course of schizophrenic psychoses: The intention- Disease, 185:176-187, 1997.
ality model. In: Marneros, A.; Andreasen, N.C.; and Tsuang, Searle, J.R. The Construction of Social Reality. New
M.T., eds. Negative Versus Positive Schizophrenia. Berlin, York, NY: Free Press, 1995.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/schizophreniabulletin/article-abstract/26/4/775/1923817 by guest on 27 January 2019


Germany: Springer, 1991. pp. 96-108.
Sperber, D.; Premack, D.; and Premack, A.J., eds. Causal
Nuechterlein, K.H., and Dawson, M.E. A heuristic vulner- Cognition. Oxford, U.K.: The Clarendon Press, 1995.
ability/stress model of schizophrenic episodes.
Spitzer, M.; Braun, U.; Hermle, L.; and Maier, S.
Schizophrenia Bulletin, 10(2):300-312, 1984.
Associative semantic network dysfunctions in thought-
Olin, S.C.; Mednick, S.A.; Cannon, T.; Jacobsen, B.; Parnas, disordered schizophrenic patients: Direct evidence from
J.; Schulsinger, R; and Schulsinger, H. School teacher rat- indirect semantic priming. Biological Psychiatry,
ings predictive of psychiatric outcome 25 years later. British 34:864-877, 1993.
Journal of Psychiatry, 172(Suppl 33):7-13,1998.
Stanghellini, G. Antropologia della Vulnerabilita. Milan,
Parnas, J., and Bovet, P. Autism in schizophrenia revis- Italy: Feltrinelli, 1997a.
ited. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 32:7-21, 1991. Stanghellini, G. For an anthropology of vulnerability.
Penn, D.L.; Corrigan P.W.; Bentall, R.P.; Racenstein, Psychopathology, 30:1-11, 19976.
J.M.; and Newman, L. Social cognition and schizophre- Strauss, J.S. Mediating processes in schizophrenia:
nia. Psychological Bulletin, 121:114-132, 1997. Towards a new dynamic psychiatry. British Journal of
Plotkin, H. Evolution in Mind. London, U.K.: Penguin, Psychiatry, 155(Suppl 5):22-28, 1989.
1997. Strauss, J.S.; Hafez, H.; Lieberman, P.; Harding, CM. The
Raine, A.; Lencz, T.; and Mednick, S.A., eds. Schizotypal course of psychiatric disorder: HI. Longitudinal principles.
Personality. Cambridge, NY: Cambridge University American Journal of Psychiatry, 142:289-296, 1985.
Press, 1995. Suellwold, L., and Huber, G. Schizophrene Basisstoerun-
Reid, T. Inquiry Into the Human Mind: On the Principles gen. Berlin, Germany: Springer, 1986.
of Common Sense. Edinburgh, Scotland: Brookes, 1997. Swiney, D.A. Theoretical and methodological issues in
Rosenzweig, F. Das Buechlein vom gesunden und kranken cognitive science: A psycholinguistic perspective. In:
Menschverstand. Konigstein, Germany: Athenaum, Kintsch, W; Miller, J.R; and Poltson, P.G., eds. Methods
Jiidischer Verlag, 1984. and Tactics in Cognitive Science. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Rossi, P. Clavis Universalis: Arti Delia Memoria e Logica Erlbaum, 1984. pp. 121-158.
Combinatoria da Lullo a Leibniz. Bologna, Italy: II Traupman, K.L. Encoding process memory for categori-
Mulino, 1983. cally related words by schizophrenic patients. Journal of
Sarfati, Y.; Hardy-Baile\ M.C.; Besch, C ; and Abnormal Psychology, 89:704-713,1980.
Widloecher, D. Attributions of intentions to others in peo- Vico, G.B. La Scienza Nuova. (1744) Milan, Italy:
ple with schizophrenia: A non-verbal exploration with Rizzoli, 1998.
comic strips. Schizophrenia Research, 25:199-209, Wiggins, O.P.; Schwartz, M.A.; and Northoff, G. Toward a
1997a. Husserlian phenomenology of initial stages of schizophrenia.
Sarfati, Y; Hardy-Baile\ M.C.; Nadel, J.; Chevalier, J.F.; In: Spitzer, M., and Maher, B.A., eds. Philosophy and
and Widloecher, D. Attribution of mental states to others Psychopathology. New York, NY: Springer, 1990. pp. 21-34.
by schizophrenic patients. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, Wittgenstein, L. On Certainty. Oxford, U.K.: Basil
2:1-17, 19976. Blackwell, 1969.
Sass, L.A. Madness and Modernism: Insanity in the Light Wyrsch, J. Die Person des Schizophrenen: Studien zur
of Modern Art, Literature, and Thought. Cambridge, MA: Klinik, Psychologie, Daseinsweise. Bern, Germany:
Harvard University Press, 1992. Haupt, 1949.
Sass, L.A. The Paradoxes of Delusions. Ithaca, NY: Yates, F.A. The Art of Memory. London, U.K.: Routledge
Cornell University Press, 1994. and Kegan Paul, 1966.
Schutz, A. Common sense and scientific interpretation of Zubin, J., and Spring, B. Vulnerability: A new view on
human action. In: Natanson, M., ed. Collected Papers: I. schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
The Problem of Social Reality. The Hague, the 86:103-126, 1977.

786
At Issue Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 26, No. 4, 2000

Acknowledgments
I wish to thank Prof. M. Maj and Prof. L.A. Sass for their
helpful comments on earlier versions of this manuscript.

The Author

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/schizophreniabulletin/article-abstract/26/4/775/1923817 by guest on 27 January 2019


Giovanni Stanghellini is Psychiatrist, Florence
Department of Mental Health, Florence, Italy; Secretary,
World Psychiatric Association Clinical Psychopathology
Section and Italian Society for Psychopathology; and
Professor of Social Psychopathology, Postgraduate School
of Psychiatry, University of Florence, Florence, Italy.

787
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/schizophreniabulletin/article-abstract/26/4/775/1923817 by guest on 27 January 2019

Você também pode gostar