Você está na página 1de 7

DEVELOPMENTAL MEDICINE & CHILD NEUROLOGY ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Global developmental delay and its relationship to cognitive skills


1
EMILIE M RIOU MD FRCPC | SHUVO GHOSH MD FRCPC 2 | EMMETT FRANCOEUR MD CM FRCPC 2 | MICHAEL I
1
SHEVELL MD CM FRCPC

1 Division of Pediatric Neurology Montreal Children's Hospital. McGill University Health Center, Montreal Canada. 2 Child Development Program; Montreal Children's
Hospital – McGill University Health Center, Montreal, Canada.

Correspondence to Dr Michael Shevell at Division of Child Neurology, Montreal Children's Hospital, 2300 Tupper Street, Montreal, Quebec, H3H 1P3, Canada. E-mail:
michael.shevell@muhc.mcgill.ca

PUBLICATION DATA Global developmental delay (GDD) is defined as evidence of significant delays
Accepted for publication 3rd October 2008. in two or more developmental domains. Our study determined the cognitive
Published online 6th January 2009. skills of a cohort of young children with GDD. A retrospective chart review of all
children diagnosed with GDD within a single developmental clinic was carried
ABBREVIATION
out. Scores on fine motor (Peabody Developmental Motor Scale 2), expressive
GDD Global developmental delay
language (Expressive One Word Picture Vocabulary Test) and receptive
language (Reynell Developmental Language Scales or Clinical Evaluation of
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors are grateful to Sbastien Dub
Language Fundamentals – Preschool 2) testing, and cognitive performance
for assistance with statistical analysis and (Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence, Third Edition) were
Alba Rinaldi for secretarial assistance. obtained. A multiple regression analysis was performed and correlations
Michael Shevell is grateful for the support of obtained. Results from a total of 93 patients (86 males, seven females) were
the MCH Foundation during the preparation of retained for analysis. Mean age was 3 years 8 months (SD 10mo, range
this manuscript. 2.5–4.75y). Cognitive scores were widely distributed, with 73% of participants
displaying a global IQ score of 70 or more, despite concurrent global delay.
Significant correlation was present for fine motor and expressive language
scores, when isolated and compared with cognitive performance (p values of
0.04 and 0.05 respectively). The conclusion was made that an initial diagnosis
of GDD is not necessarily associated with objective cognitive impairment.

Developmental delay is common in pediatric practice. social ⁄ personal, cognition, and activities of daily living.8,9
Although some diagnosed delays are benign, certain pre- Typically, it is assumed that delay in two developmental
sentations are more worrisome. It is generally recognized domains is associated with delay across all domains
that global developmental delay (GDD) in a child portends evaluated.
a worse prognosis than isolated single-domain delay and is Significant controversy still surrounds the symptom
more likely to be associated with an underlying, presum- complex of GDD. One reason for this lies in the epidemio-
ably causative, pathology.1–4 Adequate identification of logy of the condition, mainly diagnosed in children under
GDD versus isolated delay in a child allows better selection the age of 5 years. Since formal psychometric testing
of necessary rehabilitation resources, which has been dem- often cannot be reliably undertaken in the young
onstrated to allow better long-term functional outcome.5–7 child,10,11 a clear-cut, objective threshold for diagnosis, as
Delay in development can be defined as a difficulty in in the case of cognitive delay, often cannot be clearly
achieving specific developmental milestones compared established. Therefore, GDD diagnosis relies on the sum-
with chronological peers. Significant delay is captured by mation of clinical findings in several developmental areas.
performance that is two standard deviations or more below Another cause for controversy is the apparent wide vari-
the mean on age-appropriate, standardized, norm-refer- ability of clinical presentations that fall under the GDD
enced testing. Shevell et al.8 defined GDD as evidence of diagnostic umbrella. Effectively, GDD is a symptom com-
significant delay in two or more of the following develop- plex analogous to cerebral palsy with heterogeneous pre-
mental domains: gross ⁄ fine motor, speech ⁄ language, sentations, causes, associated conditions, and evolution

ª The Authors. Journal compilation ª Mac Keith Press 2008


600 DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2008.03197.x
over time. Although it is presumed that children with A diagnosis of GDD was ascribed to all children pre-
GDD are or will eventually be cognitively impaired, this senting with significant delays (defined as performance
has not yet been demonstrated formally. Indeed, if we that is two SDs below the mean on age-appropriate,
consider the strict current definition of GDD, it appears standardized, norm-referenced testing) in two or more
to allow inclusion of children with combined delays whose developmental domains. As part of standard clinical pro-
profile could include intact cognitive skills despite delays tocol, all children found to have GDD undergo a for-
in other domains. For lack of effective psychometric test- mal cognitive assessment with a trained pediatric
ing in very young children, GDD is sometimes considered neuropsychologist or clinical psychologist between 4 and
as equivalent to cognitive delay, and children with GDD 5 years of age.
often receive similar rehabilitation measures, interven-
tions, and programmatic follow-up. In view of these Developmental assessment
potential limitations, it is important to examine this Language development and fine motor development were
widely-held assumption of equivalence between GDD chosen as study variables because generally accepted norms
and cognitive delay. are used in the evaluation of these two domains in young
The rationale for this study lies in rigorously evaluating children. Furthermore, early language skills are often clini-
this assumption: that GDD in young children is equivalent cally used as a proxy for cognitive capabilities. Motor per-
to intellectual delay. By analyzing cognitive testing results formance was assessed by a trained occupational therapist
of children diagnosed with GDD in an ambulatory setting using the standardized and validated Peabody Develop-
based on performance in other developmental domains, we mental Motor Scale 2 (PDMS-2).13–15 Fine motor perfor-
hoped to clarify the potential correlation between elements mance was compiled as the overall score given after the
of GDD and cognitive performance. To address these PDMS-2, then transferred into a centile using the
questions, we sought to determine the relationship age-appropriate norms given by this scale.
between results on formal psychometric cognitive testing Language performance was assessed in either English or
and a combination of developmental scores for language French (depending on the child’s better language) by a
and motor performance in pre-school-age children identi- trained speech ⁄ language pathologist (fluent in both lan-
fied as being globally delayed in a multidisciplinary devel- guages) using the following measures. First, expressive
opmental clinic. vocabulary was evaluated by way of the Expressive One-
Word Picture Vocabulary Test (third edition), a standard-
METHOD ized and validated test of expressive vocabulary in
A detailed retrospective chart review of all children diag- pre-school children.16,17 Scores were compared with
nosed with GDD in the Montreal Children’s Hospital age-appropriate norms and converted into centiles. Form
Ambulatory Developmental Progress Clinic between Janu- and use (pragmatics) of language, although assessed in the
ary 2002 and December 2004 was undertaken (i.e. a 3-year clinic, were not scored objectively and thus could not be
inclusive interval). Permission was obtained from the hos- retained in the analysis. Second, receptive language was
pital’s Director of Professional Services. Children were evaluated using the Reynell Developmental Language
identified by review of the charts of all patients assessed in Scales for Children (English and French, first edition) in
the Developmental Progress Clinic during the time period children 2 to 4 years of age and the Clinical Evaluation of
studied. Referrals to the clinic derive mainly from commu- Language Fundamentals – Preschool 2 in children 4 years
nity pediatricians or family physicians in the greater Mon- and older. Both tests have been validated and standardized
treal area. The population seen reflects a wide range of in the pre-school population.18,19 (French versions have
different ethnic and socio-economic backgrounds. A previ- been validated and a working standardization is used; final
ous survey of referring physicians disclosed a local ten- standardization is pending.) Scores were converted into
dency of community physicians to refer children with centiles by comparison with age-appropriate norms.
suspected developmental delays for specialty assessment.12 Cognitive performance was routinely evaluated accord-
Thus the cohort can be viewed as generalizable to commu- ing to clinic protocol at 4 to 5 years of age by a clinic-affili-
nity pediatric settings. ated psychologist, using the Wechsler Preschool and
All children referred to the clinic undergo a formal Primary Scale of Intelligence (third edition, WPPSI-III),
multidisciplinary developmental assessment (occupational in French or English. The WPPSI-III is a well-validated
therapy, speech ⁄ language pathology, complete history, and scale in the pediatric population.20 Scores are given for
developmental pediatrics examination). A diagnostic con- verbal, performance, and global IQ. Results were compiled
clusion from these evaluations is drawn after the first or as rank categories in the chart, corresponding to a range
second visit to the clinic. of centiles, as determined by the WPPSI. (Numerical

Global Developmental Delay and Cognitive Skills Emilie M Riou et al. 601
results are not compiled in the chart for ethical [i.e. local ilies of children tested (53, 23, and 31% respectively). Very
privacy] reasons.) few children had evidence of an underlying genetic or
For children with significant delays in speech and lan- neurological disorder at time of initial assessment. Twenty
guage that prevented appropriate administration of the six per cent of the cohort were subsequently found to meet
WPPSI-III, the Griffiths Mental Developmental Scales criteria for an autistic spectrum disorder, as assessed by
were used. The Griffiths is a standardized test of develop- standardized testing in a specialized autism clinic.
mental performance in preschool children.21,22 Language, Forty-seven children with GDD did not undergo later
performance, and global scores were retained and psychological testing (either because of loss to follow-up
expressed in rank categories. We reviewed the charts of all [38 patients] or refusal from the parents [nine patients])
children with GDD on initial assessment who underwent and therefore were not analyzed. No significant difference
psychological testing. Children with incomplete evalua- was noted in the demographics of these patients (age at
tions were excluded from the final analysis. Sociodemo- assessment, sex, family history, and associated conditions)
graphic characteristics were, however, retained and compared with those undergoing later cognitive testing.
compiled for general comparison with the study group. The results of the cognitive scores are shown in Figures 1
A value of 33% or higher was used as evidence of signifi- and 2. Performance, Verbal, and Global IQ scores were
cant correlation for calculation of sample size. Considering widely distributed within the cohort. Mean categories were
an a error of 5%, a minimum of 70 participants was in the mild deficiency, low average, and borderline ranges
required to reach 80% power in this study. Regression for Performance, Verbal, and Global IQ, respectively.
analysis was undertaken by least squares. Table II summarizes the results of the multiple regres-
sion analysis. Expressive vocabulary scores appeared to be
Data analysis associated with Verbal and Global IQ scores ( p=0.006 and
Developmental scores were recorded as centiles for the 0.05). Fine motor performance correlated significantly
following variables fine motor performance, expressive with Performance IQ ( p=0.027) and Global IQ scores
vocabulary, and receptive language. Cognitive scores were ( p=0.037).
mainly reported as rank categories in patients’ charts. Such A combination of fine motor, expressive vocabulary, and
rankings on the Griffiths and WPPSI scales were accepted receptive language scores showed no statistically significant
as concordant (rank categories correspond to the same cen- correlation with all modalities of IQ scores, as evidenced
tile ranges) and, for the purpose of this study, were taken by non-significant p values. Further, r2 values confirmed
to be equivalent. the poor correlation value of the combined scores for IQ
All data gathered were analyzed using SPSS software determination.
version 11.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago). Fine motor, expressive
vocabulary, and receptive language scores were used and
correlated by multiple regression analysis with perfor-
mance, Verbal, and Global IQ scores of the WPSSI, and Table I: Baseline characteristics of study cohort
language, performance and global scores of the Griffiths
Males, n (%) 86 (93)
scales. A p value of 0.05 or less was considered to be statis-
Family history, n (%):
tically significant (p values did not undergo Bonferroni cor-
Neurological disorders 25 (23)
rection as all outcomes were considered as independent
Genetic disorders 2 (2)
hypotheses).23 Approval for chart review and information Learning disorders 57 (53)
retrieved was obtained from the hospital’s Director of Psychiatric disorders 34 (31)
Professional Services. Consanguinity )
Associated conditions, n (%):
RESULTS Sensory deficit 4 (4)
A total of 140 children initially diagnosed with GDD were Chromosomal anomaly )
identified over the study inclusion interval. Of these, 93 Genetic syndrome 2 (2)
children (86 males, seven females) underwent detailed cog- Neurological disorder 3 (3)

nitive assessment and their scores were retained for analy- Psychosocial deprivation 3 (3)
Autistic spectrum disorder 28 (26)
sis. The demographic characteristics of the cohort are
Mean age at initial assessment, mo 44.3 (SD 9.6)
outlined in Table I. The mean age of patients at initial
Mean age at cognitive assessment, mo 54 (SD 9.9)
evaluation was 3 years 8 months (SD 10mo, range 2.5–
Mean delay between initial and 9.6 (SD 9.0)
4.75y). Specific learning disorders, psychiatric disorders, cognitive assessments, mo
and neurological disorders were fairly common in the fam-

602 Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2009, 51; 600–606


DISCUSSION order to provide early beneficial interventions to the child
Recent guidelines strongly recommend the systematic and family.24 In light of this, it appears relevant to question
screening of developmental delay in young children, in the accuracy of diagnoses of those identified with develop-

30

25

20
Frequency

15

10

9)

)
)

9)
)

)
)

30
25

69

79

89

19
39

54

10

12

>1
(<

5–

0–

0–

–1
5–

0–

0–

0–
(5

(7

r(
(8

10
(2

(4
nd

(9

12

rio
(1
ild

e
ou

e
re

r(
lin

ag
at

pe
ve

ag
of

e
er

rio
er

er

ag
Pr

Se

su
er
od

rd

pe
av

er
Av
Bo

ry
M

Su
av
w

Ve
Lo

h
ig
H

IQ categories

Figure 1: Distribution of Global IQ scores.

35
Performance IQ
Verbal IQ
30

25
Frequency

20

15

10

0
)
)

9)

9)
)

)
)

9)

29
69

89
54

19

30
25

–7

10
–3

–1
5–

0–
0–

–1

>1
(<

70

0–
25

20
(5

(8
(4

10

r(
nd

(9
(

rio
1
ild

(1
e

e
e
re
ou

r(
lin

ag

e
at
ve

pe
M

ag

e
of

rio
er

er

er

ag
Se
Pr

su
er
od

rd

av

pe
er
Av
Bo
M

ry
Su
av
w

Ve
Lo

h
ig
H

IQ categories

Figure 2: Distribution of Performance and Verbal IQ scores.

Global Developmental Delay and Cognitive Skills Emilie M Riou et al. 603
cognitive impairment. This would be better assessed in a
Table II: Multiple regression analysis
larger prospective cohort of children with GDD.
The poor predictive value of the combined scores may
Predictor(s) IQ modality r2 p
also be related to an inherent defect of our definition of
Combined score (FM, EV, RL) GDD. It appears that evidence of significant delay in at least
Verbal IQ 0.07 0.056 two areas of fine motor, expressive vocabulary, or receptive
Performance IQ 0.066 0.07 language is not predictive of cognitive performance in our
Global IQ 0.069 0.06 population. This seems contrary to what was found with
Receptive language
respect to the developmental and functional outcomes of
Verbal IQ 0.01 0.25
children with GDD.27 The answer to this apparent conun-
Performance IQ 0 0.86
drum may be that cognitive performance is not equivalent
Global IQ 0.012 0.26
to developmental performance or functionality in a child,
Expressive vocabulary
Verbal IQ 0.07 0.006a
and thus all three should be considered as distinct, and thus
Performance IQ 0.025 0.099 potentially complementary, outcome measures.
Global IQ 0.036 0.05a Although we were unable to assess personal ⁄ social, gross
Fine motor motor, and activities of daily living domains in a more
Verbal IQ 0.004 0.542 comprehensive standardized fashion, significant delays in
Performance IQ 0.045 0.027a these areas were found during the initial evaluation and
Global IQ 0.04 0.037a were included in the diagnostic process. Hence, most of
a
our participants met more than the present minimum cri-
Significant correlations. FM, fine motor; EV, expressive
teria for impairment in at least two domains.
vocabulary; RL, receptive language.
Scores on cognitive testing were somewhat widely dis-
mental concerns. This is especially so if a diagnosis of a tributed. However, most children fell within the low aver-
GDD is put forward. age to mild deficiency range. This is in keeping with recent
The paucity of studies assessing this particular issue estimates establishing that 90% of individuals with mental
probably reflects the relative novelty of the concept of retardation* ⁄ intellectual disability function within the mild
GDD.8 Retrospective studies have detected evidence of range of impairment.10,28–30 Surprisingly, a small propor-
early childhood developmental delays in children affected tion of our participants scored in the high average range.
with specific learning disabilities.25,26 However, the specific This is again in contradiction to the common assumption
subject of cognitive delay has not yet been addressed in the of equivalence between GDD and cognitive limitation. It
GDD population as a whole. appears that a non-trivial proportion of children with GDD
Shevell et al.27 have prospectively assessed the functional have average intelligence (e.g. 20% of our cohort). This
and developmental outcomes of a cohort of GDD patients, finding may not be completely foreign to the developmental
using the Battelle Developmental Inventory and the Vine- and functional outcomes found by Shevell et al.27 Indeed,
land Adaptive Behavior scale. On follow-up evaluation, one may hypothesize that the subgroup of their cohort
96% of their cohort still met criteria for GDD and 70% attending regular classes without aid (17%) may have had
showed functional impairments in at least two domains. somewhat close to average overall intellectual performance.
Only 17% of the children attended a regular class without Thus there may be two subsets of children with GDD;
an aid. Interestingly, the degree of initial delay was not those with and without cognitive impairment.
found to be predictive of later developmental outcome. Another issue pertains to the significant number of par-
Our study addressed the specific topic of cognitive delay in ticipants with autistic features in our subject population. It
children meriting a concurrent diagnosis of GDD. Our aims may be that children with autistic spectrum disorders are
were twofold: (1) to explore the relationship between initial less amenable to valid developmental testing as toddlers,
language and fine motor scores and cognitive performance; increasing the risk of overestimating delay initially. Chil-
and (2) to determine the overall distribution of cognitive dren with autistic spectrum disorder may perform better at
scores in a cohort of young children with confirmed GDD. later cognitive testing, especially with nonverbal cognitive
We found no statistically significant correlation between subcategories. This could be an explanation for the wide
a combination of fine motor, expressive vocabulary, and distribution of IQ scores obtained. It also raises the ques-
receptive language scores and the results of cognitive test- tion whether children with autistic features should be
ing. However, a trend was evident towards a possible cor- consistently excluded from studies involving GDD
relation (p value of 0.06) and thus we would not completely
exclude this combination in the evaluation of possible *UK usage: learning disability.

604 Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2009, 51; 600–606


cohorts, as their cognitive profile is likely to differ from Developmental Progress Clinic, the clinical source of all
children with GDD alone. our study participants, limits potential biases, as all patients
Taken in isolation, expressive vocabulary and fine motor undergo the same standardized evaluations, recorded in a
scores accurately predicted verbal IQ scores and perfor- predetermined uniform format. We reviewed the charts of
mance IQ scores, respectively. This further supports the all participants presenting to the Developmental Progress
validity and inference of the expressive vocabulary and fine Clinic within a selected defined time interval. A review of
motor scales that we used. Since cognitive testing relies the 47 children with GDD not analyzed because of lack of
heavily on language-associated tasks, it is not surprising to cognitive assessment revealed comparable demographics
find a significant association between expressive vocabulary and developmental profiles with our study group: hence,
deficits and global IQ scores. This has interesting implica- potential selection biases are likely to be limited.
tions for prognosis in GDD. It may be that children with Most of the children assessed had no identifiable cause
GDD with higher expressive vocabulary scores are more for their GDD and few had associated medical conditions.
likely to have a favorable cognitive outcome. This remains This is probably representative of the majority of patients
to be studied further in a systematic prospective fashion. seen in developmental pediatrics and community pediatrics
Fine motor performance was also predictive of global IQ, settings. Locally, children with associated neurologic
which probably relates to the important overlap between impairment appear to be preferentially referred to hospi-
fine motor performance and cognition in the pre-school tal-based child neurology clinics. Patients with obvious
child as assessed by present developmental assessments. causes for delay or significant associated medical condi-
Surprisingly, we found no correlation between receptive tions may have very different developmental and cognitive
language scores and verbal IQ, which contradicts the gen- profiles, with a different referral pattern (e.g. child neuro-
eral assumption that language comprehension predicts logy clinic evaluation), which we could not assess because
cognition, specifically verbal IQ. This could be partly of the nature of recruitment in this particular study.
related to the scales used for receptive language assess- GDD remains a symptom complex with an as yet un-
ment. However, this is unlikely as both the Reynell and the defined nosologic profile and implications. Our study
Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals – Pre- found no correlation between a combination of develop-
school 2 (CELF) tests have been validated for assessment mental scores and cognitive scores in children with GDD.
of receptive and expressive language in a population of pre- However, fine motor and expressive vocabulary scores
school children. However, it may be that the receptive lan- taken in isolation were predictive of global cognitive
guage function assessed through these scales is not directly scores, which could have important implications for accu-
implicated in verbal IQ function, a hypothesis that has rate diagnosis and eventual prognosis.
never been explored. Thus, verbal IQ may depend on A previous study has shown persistence of develop-
aspects of cognition other than language comprehension mental and functional impairments at age 7 in children
alone and could not be predicted with these scales. diagnosed with GDD as young children.27 According to
Inherent to our study was the possibility of variations recent consensus, mental retardation ⁄ intellectual disability
in the status of delay in our participants between the is defined as significantly sub-average functioning existing
initial evaluation and slightly later cognitive testing. concurrently with related limitations in at least two appli-
Some children may have improved significantly, while cable adaptive skill areas.10 Adaptive skills were not
others may have plateaued or worsened (i.e. gap widen- assessed in our cohort; we cannot, therefore, comment on
ing) relative to age-equivalent norms. Since our aim was the proportion of our participants meeting current criteria
to determine the nosologic value of our initial diagnosis, for diagnosing mental retardation ⁄ intellectual disability.
we do not consider this variation as a bias, but as evi- Even so, we believe our results shed some doubts on the
dence of the wide variety of developmental profiles of validity of the assumption of GDD–mental retarda-
children with GDD. A corollary to this is the possibility tion ⁄ intellectual disability equivalence.
of the child’s benefiting from rehabilitation resources Prediction of outcome of GDD should take into consid-
between the initial assessment and cognitive evaluation. eration intellectual performance, developmental perfor-
This is likely to have had no impact on the overall mance, and, most importantly, functional performance.
results, because of the lengthy average waiting time for The current definition of GDD likely needs to be revisited,
rehabilitation resources in our community (most chil- to better embrace the now well-established variety of pro-
dren would have been assessed cognitively before any files of children with several areas of delay apparent at a
actual rehabilitation intervention being provided). young age and an apparent impairment dichotomy in
One limitation of the study is its retrospective design. children with GDD with reference to actual associated
However, we believe that the overall structure of the cognitive profile.

Global Developmental Delay and Cognitive Skills Emilie M Riou et al. 605
REFERENCES Washington, DC: American Association The Psychological Corporation, Harcourt
1. Majnemer A, Shevell MI. Diagnostic yield on Mental Retardation; 2002. Brace and Company; 1991.
of the neurologic assessment of the devel- 11. Johnson JH, Goldman J. Developmental 21. Griffiths R. The abilities of young chil-
opmentally delayed child. J Pediatr 1995; assessment in clinical child psychology. dren: a comprehensive system of mental
127: 193–99. New York, NY: Pergamon Press, 1990. measurement for the first eight years of
2. Shevell MI, Majnemer A, Rosenbaum P, 12. Shevell MI, Majnemer A, Rosenbaum P, life. London, UK: Child Development
Abramovitcz M. Etiologic yield of subspe- Abrahamowicz M. A profile of referrals for Research Centre, 1970.
cialists’ evaluation of young children with early childhood developmental delay 22. Griffiths R. The Griffiths Mental Devel-
global developmental delay. J Pediatr to ambulatory sub-specialty clinics. J Child opment Scales. In: Huntley M, editor.
2000; 136: 593–98. Neurol 2001; 16: 645–50. Henley: The Test Agency Ltd; 1996.
3. Shevell MI, Majnemer A, Rosenbaum P, 13. Palisano RJ, Kolobe TH, Haley SM, 23. Rothman KJ. No adjustments are needed
Abramovitcz M. Etiologic yield of single Lowes LP, Jones SL. Validity of the Pea- for multiple testing. Epidemiology 1990; 1:
domain developmental delay: a prospective body Developmental Gross Motor Scale as 43–46.
study. J Pediatr 2000; 137: 633–37. an evaluative measure of infants receiving 24. American Academy of Pediatrics, Com-
4. Battaglia A, Bianchini E, Carey JC. Diag- physical therapy. Phys Ther 1995; 75: mittee on Children with Disabilities.
nostic yield of the comprehensive assess- 939–48. Developmental surveillance and screening
ment of developmental delay ⁄ mental 14. Provost B, Crowe TK, McClain C. Con- of infants and young children. Pediatrics
retardation in an institute of child neuro- current validity of the Bayley Scales of 2001; 108: 192–96.
psychiatry. Am J Med Genet 1999; 82: 60– Infant Development II Motor Scale and 25. Shapiro BK, Palmer FB, Antell S, et al.
66. The Peabody Developmental Motor Precursors of reading delay: neurodevelop-
5. First LR, Palfrey JS. The infant or young Scales in two-year-old children. Phys Occup mental milestones. Pediatrics 1990;
child with developmental delay. NEJM Ther Pediatr 2000; 20: 5–18. 85: 416–20.
1994; 330: 478–83. 15. Gebhard AR, Ottenbacher KJ, Lane SJ. 26. Silva PA, McGee R, Williams S. Some
6. Bennett FC, Guralnick MJ. Effectiveness Interrater reliability of the Peabody Devel- characteristics of 9-year-old boys with
of developmental intervention in the first opmental Motor Scales: fine motor scale. general reading backwardness or specific
five years of life. Pediatr Clin North Am Am J Occup Ther 1994; 48: 976–81. retardation. J Child Psychol Psychiatry
1991; 38: 1513–28. 16. Gray S, Plante E, Vance R, Henrichsen 1985; 26: 407–21.
7. Shonkoff JP, Hauser-Cram P. Early inter- M. The diagnostic accuracy of four vocab- 27. Shevell M, Majnemer A, Platt RW, Web-
vention for disabled infants and their fami- ulary tests administered to preschool-age ster R, Birnbaum R. Developmental and
lies: a quantitative analysis. Pediatrics 1987; children. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch 1999; functional outcomes at school age of pre-
80: 650–58. 30: 196–206. school children with global developmental
8. Shevell M, Ashwal S, Donley D, et al. 17. Gardner MF. Expressive One-Word delay. J Child Neurol 2005; 20: 648–54.
Practice parameter: evaluation of the child Picture Vocabulary Test. Novato, CA: 28. King BH, State MW, Shah B, Davanzo P,
with global developmental delay. Neurol- Academic Therapy Publications, 1979. Dykens E. Mental retardation: a review of
ogy 2003; 60: 367–80. 18. Reynell JK. Reynell Developmental Lan- the past 10 years Part I. J Am Acad Child
9. Sherr EH, Shevell MI. Mental retardation guage scales manual. 2nd rev. Huntley M, Adolesc Psychiatry 1997; 36: 1656–63.
and global developmental delay. In: Swai- ed. Windsor, England: NFER-NELSON 29. State MW, King BH, Dykens E. Mental
man KF, Ashwal S, Ferreiro DM, eds. Publishing; 1985. retardation: a review of the past 10 years.
Pediatric neurology principles and prac- 19. Wiig EH, Secord W, Semel E. Clinical Part II. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry
tice. 4th edn. Philadelphia: Mosby Else- Evaluation of Language Fundamentals ) 1997; 36: 1664–71.
vier; 2006: 799–820. Preschool. San Antonio, TX: The Psycho- 30. Curry CJ, Stevenson RE, Aughton D,
10. American Association on Mental Retarda- logical Corporation, 1992. et al. Evaluation of mental retardation:
tion. Mental retardation: definition, classi- 20. Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale recommendations of a consensus confer-
fication and systems of supports 10th edn. for Children. 3rd edn. San Antonio, TX: ence. Am J Med Genet 1997; 72: 468–77.

606 Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2009, 51; 600–606

Você também pode gostar