Você está na página 1de 7

This article was downloaded by: [41.235.172.

98]
On: 10 July 2011, At: 11:51
Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,
37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Journal of New Music Research


Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/nnmr20

Remembering, Recognizing and Describing Singers'


Sound Identities
a b
Helen F. Mitchell & Raymond A.R. MacDonald
a
University of Sydney, Australia
b
Glasgow Caledonian University, UK

Available online: 31 Mar 2011

To cite this article: Helen F. Mitchell & Raymond A.R. MacDonald (2011): Remembering, Recognizing and Describing Singers'
Sound Identities, Journal of New Music Research, 40:1, 75-80

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09298215.2010.545130

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

This article may be used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic
reproduction, re-distribution, re-selling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply or distribution in any form to
anyone is expressly forbidden.

The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents
will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae and drug doses should
be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims,
proceedings, demand or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in
connection with or arising out of the use of this material.
Journal of New Music Research
2011, Vol. 40, No. 1, pp. 75–80

Remembering, Recognizing and Describing Singers’ Sound Identities

Helen F. Mitchell1 and Raymond A.R. MacDonald2


1
University of Sydney, Australia; 2Glasgow Caledonian University, UK

Abstract 1. Introduction
Perceiving music performers through sound quality is Perception of individual music performers’ sound quality
important for musicians, teachers and every music requires a complex synthesis of sensory information. A
Downloaded by [41.235.172.98] at 11:51 10 July 2011

listener. A performer’s musical identity is influenced by performer’s musical identity is influenced by his or her
the unique sound they create, yet little is known about ‘sound’ (MacDonald, Miell, & Hargreaves, 2002) and
listeners’ mental representation of an individual’s listeners take for granted not only their capacity to
sound quality. A verbal overshadowing (VO) effect distinguish between musical instruments, but also their
has been extensively reported in visual recognition, ability to discriminate between performers playing the
where the act of verbal description distorts subsequent same instrument by their sound alone. Despite the
recall, and the same may apply in the auditory domain importance of this auditory perception, little is known
and specifically, in music performance reception. In about how listeners discriminate between, recognize and
this study we explore if musicians also exhibit a VO verbalize different sound qualities as listeners use a limited
effect, by investigating how listeners form auditory selection of descriptors to explain how they process sound
impressions of a singer and testing the impact of verbal (Davidson & Coimbra, 2001; Mitchell & Kenny, 2008).
description on listeners’ recognition accuracy and Perceptions of auditory experiences in music remains a
confidence. Listeners (n ¼ 50) heard a single target largely intuitive process and listeners are rarely asked to
voice and were assigned to either a verbal description define or describe their mental processing of sound
group or a control group. The verbal group wrote a (Stanley, Brooker, & Gilbert, 2002). Can listeners recog-
detailed description of the target voice and the control nize individual music performers from a group of like
group did not. All listeners heard a voice line-up performers? Do words adequately capture their impres-
(n ¼ 6) and were asked to identify the original target sions of the sound quality? Or do verbal descriptions
voice and rate their confidence in the decision. There interfere with listeners’ sound recall of specific performers?
was a reliable VO effect where providing a verbal This study aims to provide an insight into the way auditory
description reduced listeners’ identification accuracy of experts, such as musicians, identify and articulate indivi-
the target voice, but did not reduce listeners’ con- dual singers’ voices. It will investigate if verbal descriptors
fidence rating of their decision. Verbalization of singing are an imperfect intermediary in the communication of a
voices was an inadequate representation of listeners’ performer’s sound and if words actually distort musicians’
auditory experience and limited listeners’ ability to memory and recall of an individual performer.
recall and identify singers’ sound quality. These
results have profound implications for the way in
1.1 Verbal overshadowing
which we consider musical performance and how music
listeners recognize, process and describe a performer’s A verbal overshadowing (VO) effect can occur when we
sound. use words to describe sensory experiences. Verbal

Correspondence: Helen F. Mitchell, Conservatorium of Music C41, University of Sydney, NSW 2006, Australia.
E-mail: helen.mitchell@sydney.edu.au

DOI: 10.1080/09298215.2010.545130 Ó 2011 Taylor & Francis


76 Helen F. Mitchell and Raymond A.R. MacDonald

description, or verbal encoding, impairs later recall of the Even trained professionals find it hard to agree on precise
experience and individuals are less able to identify the descriptions of sound quality in singers (e.g. Ekholm,
stimuli from a line-up (Schooler, Fiore, & Brandimonte, Papagiannis, & Chagnon, 1998; Garnier, Henrich,
1997; Parr, Heatherbell, & White, 2002; Perfect, Hunt, & Castellengo, Sotiropoulos, & Dubois, 2007) far less
Harris, 2002). VO has been most apparent in face how listeners construct and perceive an individual
recognition, where witnesses to crimes have to describe singer’s sound. Communication about performers usual-
the perpetrator of a robbery in detail, and later select ly focuses on the most easily verbalized characteristics in
their face from a photographic line-up. The act of verbal the sound rather than individual composition of these
description negatively influences individuals’ ability to characteristics which makes a sound unique (Kenny &
recognize the face they described in words. Empirical Mitchell, 2006; Mitchell & Kenny, 2006).
studies have confirmed this VO effect in facial recogni- Verbalizing sound quality presents a challenge to
tion by comparing verbal description groups with musicians and pedagogues in describing a complex
memory-only control groups (Meissner & Brigham, sensory phenomenon. Verbal description may be an
2001). More recent studies have demonstrated the same imperfect means to convey sound, but talking about
distortion effect in the auditory domain. Listeners’ ability music represents an important part of musical commu-
to recognize spoken voices following verbal description nication (MacDonald, Miell, & Wilson, 2005) and assists
diminished while no-description control groups were able listeners in describing and constructing musical processes
to isolate the voice from a line-up (Perfect et al., 2002; (MacDonald & Wilson, 2006). It also influences how we
Downloaded by [41.235.172.98] at 11:51 10 July 2011

Vanags, Carroll, & Perfect, 2005). Our preliminary hear and perform music (Hargreaves, MacDonald, &
investigations of singing voice recognition followed the Miell, in press). The perception of music does not usually
same responses as the spoken voice studies and suggested depend on a fixed semantic interpretation (Peretz &
that music listeners may be similarly susceptible to the Zatorre, 2005; Schneck & Berger, 2006) although
effects of VO when they describe singing voices (Mitchell associative memories with sound may form the basis of
& MacDonald, 2009). semantic preparation for recall or subsequent recognition
(Koelsch et al., 2004). This study will extend current
knowledge on mental processing of auditory information
1.2 Verbalizing sound quality in the music domain
to a music performance context.
In music, the auditory modality of the stimuli does not
match the task (verbal description), yet we rely on verbal
1.3 Project aims
descriptors to explain and communicate our perception
of sound quality in training and assessment. Singing Current knowledge about ‘sound’ evaluation of music
voice assessors have used a limited selection of verbal performers must extend beyond intuition, and requires a
descriptors to describe the voices and focused on the more rigorous approach to demystify the perceptual
more easily verbalized technical and performance com- processes of audiences and evaluators. While there is a
ponents rather than describing overall quality (Davidson growing body of knowledge focused on categorizing
& Coimbra, 2001; Stanley et al., 2002; Kenny & Mitchell, terminology used to describe sound quality, there is still
2006; Mitchell & Kenny, 2008). Examiners have even much to be learned about the value of verbalization in
noted candidates’ dress and stage manner to facilitate characterizing listeners’ perceptions of music performers.
later recall of individual singers’ performances for Verbal descriptions may only be effective when a
discussion and assessment, at the expense of describing performer’s sound translates easily and completely into
sound quality (Davidson & Coimbra, 2001). This words. The aim of the current project is to investigate the
provides one potential clue to suggest the use of language way in which auditory experts, such as musicians,
may hinder the recall of performers and performances. identify and articulate individual singers’ sound quality
There is an expectation that music listeners have a and to discover if verbalizing that sound distorts
superior ability to discriminate between, recognize and subsequent memory and identification of the singer.
verbalize different sound qualities. Musicians are gen-
erally more proficient in object (musical instrument or
voice) recognition (e.g. Chartrand & Belin, 2006) but are
2. Method
also surprisingly unreliable in recognizing the same
instrument or singer performing notes even an octave 2.1 Participants
apart (Handel & Erickson, 2001, 2004; Erickson & Perry,
2.1.1 Singers
2003). Conceptualizing an individual performer’s sound
is complex, as it contains multiple acoustic properties as Six soprano singers studying classical singing at the
well as musical, technical and aesthetic cues, all of which Sydney Conservatorium of Music (SCM) volunteered to
contribute to the overall ‘sound quality’ (Handel, 2006). take part. They were informed that the study was
Remembering, recognizing and describing singers’ sound identities 77

investigating the way listeners use words to describe and 2.3 Perceptual test
remember a music performance.
2.3.1 Stimuli
Audio files were edited and consisted of around 15 s
2.1.2 Listeners
extracts of each singer for each song. Each extract was a
Listeners (n ¼ 50) were aged between 20 and 66 (M ¼ 36 complete musical phrase, and included bars 5 to 12 of
years). Participants were sent information about the Caro mio ben or bars 13 to 28 of Seligkeit. Target voice
project and were invited to take part in a perceptual CDs were prepared for each singer singing Caro mio ben
study investigating the possible effect of visual and verbal (first take) and a single line-up CD of six singers singing
stimuli on listeners’ ability to recall a music performance. Seligkeit (first take).
They were required to attend a single listening session
lasting around 25 min and to complete a short
2.3.2 Procedure
questionnaire about their musical performance instru-
ments and teaching experience and basic demographic Listeners were asked to attend a single listening session
information. and were informed that the purpose of the study was to
Forty-four listeners currently played one or more investigate the effect of visual and verbal tasks on the
musical instruments/voice and of these, 25 were taking ability to recognize aural stimuli. Audio samples were
lessons on their principal instrument and 26 currently played from a CD player (Sony DEJ885W) via circum-
Downloaded by [41.235.172.98] at 11:51 10 July 2011

taught a musical instrument. Table 1 shows the demo- aural closed-back stereo monitoring headphones (Senn-
graphic profile of the listener cohort, including gender, heiser HD 270) in a quiet room to ensure that each
age and principal instrument played. listener heard the same quality of sample. Listener
participants were assigned to one of two groups; a verbal
description group or a non-description control group in
2.2 Singer Protocol
order as they arrived to take the perceptual test.
Singers were required to attend a single recording session Figure 1 illustrates the project tasks for each group.
and perform unaccompanied excerpts of Caro mio ben All listeners were presented with a short sample of a
(Caccini) and Seligkeit (Schubert), twice for a recording. single singer performing Caro mio ben (for encoding) on
They were presented with a starting note, and a CD. Target voice presentation was randomized from the
metronome beat before starting each song. six singers. Listeners were then asked to perform a visual
Singers were recorded in a sound treated studio at maze task on paper (a filler-task) for 10 min. In the next
SCM using a matched pair of stereo microphones (Neu- five minutes, the verbal description group was asked to
mann KU100, ORTF configuration) 2.75 m from the write a detailed description of the voice they heard at the
singer. The level was kept constant via a pre-amplifier with start of the test while the control group completed an
stepped controls and the audio channels were digitized, anagram puzzle. Finally, both groups were presented a
transferred to computer and saved as wave files (24 bit, line-up of six voices singing Seligkeit and were asked to
48 kHz). identify the original voice they heard and to rate their

Table 1. Listeners’ demographic profiles by experimental group,


including age, gender and principal instruments played.

Experimental group

Verbal (n ¼ 25) Control (n ¼ 25)

Mean age (SD) 35.1 (10.1) 33.9 (12.2)


Gender
Female 10 12
Male 15 13
Principal instrument
Voice 6 (24%) 4 (16%)
Keyboard 4 (16%) 4 (16%)
Woodwind 5 (20%) 8 (32%)
Brass 2 (8%) 0 –
Percussion 0 – 2 (8%) Fig. 1. Listener tasks by group (verbal description versus
control).
78 Helen F. Mitchell and Raymond A.R. MacDonald

confidence in their response on a scale of 1–10 (not darker sound than for soprano’ or ‘The voice sounded
confident to very confident). fairly low pitched for a female’.
Listeners debated the singer’s age, from a ‘young
singer in her 20s’ to a ‘mature female’ or ‘a woman over
the age of 40 as the voice sounded ‘‘mature’’’.
3. Results Vibrato was a key feature in the target voices, and 15
Listeners’ responses were initially examined for the total listeners mentioned its presence or absence, and re-
number of correct voice identifications from the line-up marked on its quality. For example:
(Table 2). The outcome measure was a single result
(correct, incorrect) and the group designation (verbal ‘Smooth even vibrato on long notes but none on shorter
description versus control group) was tested as an notes’;
independent predictor of verbal overshadowing. Data
were entered into a binary logistic regression to see if ‘The vibrato added warmth to the tone ensuring it was not
verbal overshadowing predicted line-up identification harsh on the ears’;
success. There was a reliable verbal overshadowing effect,
W(1) ¼ 3.81, p ¼ 0.05 and verbally describing the singing ‘Vibrato quite nondescript (ordinary, unchanging)’.
voice led to poorer identification performance (24%
correct) than the control group (48% correct). Overall, Fifteen listeners mentioned the singer’s diction or
Downloaded by [41.235.172.98] at 11:51 10 July 2011

only 18 of 50 listeners correctly identified their target articulation of language and would say things like:
voice from the line-up (37%).
‘Good diction’;

3.1 Confidence scores


‘The articulation of the words was extremely clear’;
Table 2 also shows listeners’ confidence scores in their
target voice selections. Mean scores were similar for ‘Italian, not very clear pronunciation – couldn’t understand
verbal description (M ¼ 5.2, SD ¼ 2.3) and control language until 4th word’;
groups (M ¼ 5.5, SD ¼ 2.3). There was no significant
difference between the two groups’ scores and listeners ‘ . . . the singer is obviously not an Italian’.
were unaware of the effect of their tasks on their ability
to recognize the target voice. The majority of listeners used generic terms usually
associated with the classical singing voice to describe the
quality of the voice such as ‘rich and warm’, ‘breathy’,
3.2 Verbal descriptions
‘clear and pure’, ‘bright’ while others used technical
The 25 listeners in the verbal group were instructed to descriptors like ‘support’ and ‘projection’. For two
focus on the characteristics of the voice singing the target listeners, the sound they heard evoked a mental picture
song, rather than the song the singer sang when writing of the target voice singing, for example:
their description of the voice. Most listeners identified
that the singer was a soprano, or that she was female. ‘English accent/Anglo-Saxon, blonde, quite tall’;
Some listeners debated whether their target singer might
be a mezzo soprano, with comments such as ‘Slightly ‘Image evoked based on sound: larger brunette female . . .
solid, fairly tall’.

Table 2. Correct responses and mean confidence scores for all


listeners and for each experimental group (verbal description
versus control).
4. Discussion
The aim of this study was to investigate the way in which
Mean listeners process individual singers’ sound quality. It
Experimental Correct % of correct confidence tested if verbal description influenced listeners’ mental
group responses responses scores (SD) representations of a performers’ idiosyncratic sound. It
investigated the impact of the verbal overshadowing
Verbal 6 24% 5.2 (2.3) (VO) phenomenon when listeners attempt to put these
(n ¼ 25 )
sound experiences into words. There was a reliable VO
Control 12 48% 5.5 (2.3)
(n ¼ 25 )
effect for singing voice recognition, where listeners had to
Total sample 18 37% 5.5 (2.3) identify a singer in a line-up singing different musical
(n ¼ 50 ) stimuli. Verbalizing their perceptions of the singer
reduced listeners’ likelihood of identifying her from a
Remembering, recognizing and describing singers’ sound identities 79

line-up of similar singers, compared to listeners who did description. However, a study involving wine tasting
not describe her voice. Verbalization did not affect suggested that perceptual skills are not automatically
listeners’ confidence ratings about their decision and enhanced or assured by linguistic knowledge. Expert
listeners were essentially unaware that verbalizing their wine-tasters were more likely than novices to recognize
perception of the target singer distorted their memory of odours by their verbal labels, but all had knowledge of
the sound and reduced their ability to successfully language used to describe wine (Parr et al., 2002).
identify her singing a new song in the line-up. Novices may have lacked the ability to separate
The VO phenomenon has been evident in face, taste perceptual and linguistic skills while experts’ superior
and spoken voice recognition and perception of music perceptual skills protected them from linguistic inter-
performance requires a comparable complex synthesis of ference to taste recognition. The ability to recognize an
auditory information. The target voice and line-up parade object, (e.g. a face) is achieved in its totality rather than
structure mimicked visual and auditory recognition the sum of its features.
experimental designs; the results here follow both auditory Musical listeners in this study were unaware that
studies’ reliable VO effect, where listeners heard a repeat verbalizing sound quality affected their evaluation of the
of the same sentence (Perfect et al., 2002), or a different singer line-up. Listeners’ confidence scores were remark-
sentence at line-up (Vanags et al., 2005). The complex task ably similar (verbal M ¼ 5.2, control M ¼ 5.5) and
structure, using a different song at line-up, was based on indicated that listeners in the experimental group did
the premise that musicians are equipped with advanced not view verbalization as problematic to their recognition
Downloaded by [41.235.172.98] at 11:51 10 July 2011

listening skills and aimed to focus listeners’ attention to performance. If listeners had been aware of the negative
the individual sounds qualities, rather than the execution effects of their description, confidence scores may have
of a repeated task (Perfect et al., 2002). reflected this and been lower than those of the control
The available studies of VO in auditory recognition group. It seems that these specialist listeners were also
have not discussed the specific use of language (Perfect oblivious to the potential hazards of verbalizing vocal
et al., 2002; Vanags et al., 2005) but for music studies this quality (Perfect et al., 2002).
presents one of the most important components of a
study. Listeners’ descriptions of these target voices
illustrated the limits of language to communicate their
perception of music performers. Generic terms used to
5. Conclusions
describe the target voices were essentially uninformative These findings present a challenge for musicians,
and not useful in capturing the unique configuration of pedagogues and for every music listener. As in both the
sound qualities in an individual voice (Ekholm et al., spoken voice VO studies (Perfect et al., 2002; Vanags
1998; Garnier et al., 2007). Listeners found verbalization et al., 2005), all listeners’ likelihood of recognizing the
of the target voice an unusual challenge and resorted to target performer from the line-up was below 40%,
descriptions of the song, or even the person they regardless of task (verbal description/control) from the
imagined singing, rather than focus on the auditory-only line-up presentation. While a low recognition rate may be
features of the sound. Listeners not only lacked the expected from average auditory audiences (such as
vocabulary to verbalize subtle sound differences but also psychology class students taking part in experiments
inherently limited their memories of unique sound for course credit), musicians and musicians in training
qualities through their use of vocabulary. The existing are expected to demonstrate expert auditory skills to
evidence from singing performance evaluation suggests interpret music performances, and the unique sound of
that music listeners find vocal quality difficult to music performers. Over half of the control group, who
articulate and often avoid explicit descriptions to just processed and remembered the singer’s sound were
elucidate their intuitive assessments of the overall sound unable to recognize her at line-up. This has profound
(Davidson & Coimbra, 2001; Kenny & Mitchell, 2006; implications for the way in which we consider unique
Mitchell & Kenny, 2008). It may be that listeners lack the sound of music performers and how audiences recognize,
semantic framework to define unique sound qualities or process and describe a performer’s sound. How do we
that verbalization is an inadequate means to capture process and conceptualize an individual’s sound quality?
mental impressions of singers’ sound. This provides a
crucial area for future research to develop our under-
5.1 Implications for the music domain
standing of music perception. Subsequent studies must
ascertain if enhancing perceptual and semantic knowl- The results are pertinent to studies of auditory
edge through advanced training removes the VO effect cognition as they demonstrate some complexities in
or, if the VO effect remains regardless of the aural acuity translating information from one perceptual domain to
and linguistic skills of the listeners. another. More specifically this work suggests that even
Awareness of suitable verbal descriptors or prototypes experienced music listeners are vulnerable to the VO
may offer listeners a more effective lexicon for sound effect when they assess performers. Verbalization does
80 Helen F. Mitchell and Raymond A.R. MacDonald

not adequately capture listeners’ auditory experiences MacDonald, R.A.R., Miell, D., & Hargreaves, D.J. (Eds.).
and may explain why music examiners and auditioners (2002). Musical Identities. Oxford: Oxford University
avoid describing singing voices in more than the broadest Press.
terms (Davidson & Coimbra, 2001; Mitchell & Kenny, MacDonald, R.A.R., Miell, D., & Wilson, G.B. (2005).
2008). Future research must seek to understand in more Talking about music: A vehicle for identity development.
detail how listeners process and describe individual In D. Miell, R.A.R. MacDonald, & D.J. Hargreaves
musicians’ sound and develop new ways to evaluate (Eds.), Musical Communication (pp. 321–338). Oxford:
listeners’ mental representations of sound. Oxford University Press.
MacDonald, R.A.R., & Wilson, G.B. (2006). Constructions
of jazz: How jazz musicians present their collaborative
Acknowledgements musical practice. Musicae Scientiae, 10(1), 59–85.
Meissner, C.A., & Brigham, J.C. (2001). A meta-analysis of
The first author was funded by an Early Career
the verbal overshadowing effect in face identification.
Researcher Grant from the Conservatorium of Music,
Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15(6), 603–616.
University of Sydney. Sincere thanks to the singers and Mitchell, H.F., & Kenny, D.T. (2006). Can experts identify
listeners who took part in this study. ‘open throat’ technique as a perceptual phenomenon?
Musicae Scientiae, X(1), 33–58.
References Mitchell, H.F., & Kenny, D.T. (2008). The tertiary singing
audition: Perceptual and acoustic differences between
Downloaded by [41.235.172.98] at 11:51 10 July 2011

Chartrand, J.-P., & Belin, P. (2006). Superior voice timbre successful and unsuccessful candidates. Journal of Inter-
processing in musicians. Neuroscience Letters, 405(3), disciplinary Music Studies, 2(1&2), 95–110.
164–167. Mitchell, H.F., & MacDonald, R.A.R. (2009). Linguistic
Davidson, J.W., & Coimbra, D. (2001). Investigating limitations of describing sound quality: Is talking about
performance evaluation by assessors of singers in a music like dancing about architecture? In A. Williamon,
music college setting. Musicae Scientiae, 5(1), 33–53. S. Pretty, & R. Buck (Eds.), Proceedings of the
Ekholm, E., Papagiannis, G.C., & Chagnon, F.P. (1998). International Symposium on Performance Science
Relating objective measurements to expert evaluation of (ISPS09) (pp. 45–50). Utrecht, the Netherlands: Eur-
voice quality in Western classical singing: Critical opean Association of Conservatoires (AEC).
perceptual parameters. Journal of Voice, 12(2), 182–196. Parr, W.V., Heatherbell, D., & White, K. (2002). Demys-
Erickson, M.L., & Perry, S.R. (2003). Can listeners hear tifying wine expertise: Olfactory threshold, perceptual
who is singing? A comparison of three-note and six-note skill and semantic memory in expert and novice wine
discrimination tasks. Journal of Voice, 17(3), 353–369. judges. Chemical Senses, 27(8), 747–755.
Garnier, M., Henrich, N., Castellengo, M., Sotiropoulos, Peretz, I., & Zatorre, R.J. (2005). Brain organization for
D., & Dubois, D. (2007). Characterisation of voice music processing. Annual Review of Psychology, 56(1),
quality in Western lyrical singing: From teachers’ 89–114.
judgements to acoustic descriptions. Journal of Inter- Perfect, T.J., Hunt, L.J., & Harris, C.M. (2002). Verbal
disciplinary Music Studies, 1(2), 62–91. overshadowing in voice recognition. Applied Cognitive
Handel, S. (2006). Perceptual Coherence: Hearing and Psychology, 16(8), 973–980.
Seeing. New York: Oxford University Press. Schneck, D.J., & Berger, D.S. (2006). The Music Effect:
Handel, S., & Erickson, M.L. (2001). A rule of thumb: The Music Physiology and Clinical Applications. London: J.
bandwidth for timbre invariance is one octave. Music Kingsley Publishers.
Perception, 19(1), 121–126. Schooler, J.W., Fiore, S.M., & Brandimonte, M.A. (1997).
Handel, S., & Erickson, M.L. (2004). Sound source At a loss from words: Verbal overshadowing of
identification: The possible role of timbre transforma- perceptual memories. In D.L. Medin (Ed.), The Psychol-
tions. Music Perception, 21(4), 587–610. ogy of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research
Hargreaves, D.J., MacDonald, R.A.R., & Miell, D. (Eds.). and Theory (Vol. 37, pp. 291–340). San Diego, CA:
(in press). Musical Imaginations. Oxford: Oxford Uni- Academic Press.
versity Press. Stanley, M., Brooker, R., & Gilbert, R. (2002). Examiner
Kenny, D.T., & Mitchell, H.F. (2006). Acoustic and perceptions of using criteria in music performance
perceptual appraisal of vocal gestures in the female assessment. Research Studies in Music Education, 18,
classical voice. Journal of Voice, 20(1), 55–70. 43–52.
Koelsch, S., Kasper, E., Sammler, D., Schulze, K., Gunter, Vanags, T., Carroll, M., & Perfect, T.J. (2005). Verbal
T., & Friederici, A.D. (2004). Music, language and overshadowing: A sound theory in voice recognition?
meaning: Brain signatures of semantic processing. Nature Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19(9), 1127–1144.
Neuroscience, 7, 6.

Você também pode gostar