Você está na página 1de 11

Available online at www.sciencedirect.

com

Psychiatry Research 165 (2009) 252 – 262


www.elsevier.com/locate/psychres

Empathy and recognition of facial expressions of emotion in


sex offenders, non-sex offenders and normal controls
Isabelle Gery a,⁎, Raphaële Miljkovitch a , Sylvie Berthoz b , Robert Soussignan c,⁎
a
Laboratoire de Développement Social et Emotionnel, Université Paris X Nanterre, France
b
Service de Psychiatrie de l'Adolescent et du Jeune Adulte, Institut Mutualiste Montsouris, Université Rene Descartes, Paris, France
c
Laboratoire Vulnérabilité, Adaptation et Psychopathologie, CNRS, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire de la Salpêtrière, Paris, France
Received 24 February 2007; received in revised form 16 June 2007; accepted 8 November 2007

Abstract

Research conducted on empathy and emotional recognition in sex offenders is contradictory. The present study was aimed to clarify
this issue by controlling for some affective and social variables (depression, anxiety, and social desirability) that are presumed to influence
emotional and empathic measures, using a staged multicomponent model of empathy. Incarcerated sex offenders (child molesters),
incarcerated non-sex offenders, and non-offender controls (matched for age, gender, and education level) performed a recognition task of
facial expressions of basic emotions that varied in intensity, and completed various self-rating scales designed to assess distinct
components of empathy (perspective taking, affective empathy, empathy concern, and personal distress), as well as depression, anxiety,
and social desirability. Sex offenders were less accurate than the other participants in recognizing facial expressions of anger, disgust,
surprise and fear, with problems in confusing fear with surprise, and disgust with anger. Affective empathy was the only component that
discriminated sex offenders from non-sex offenders and was correlated with accuracy recognition of emotional expressions. Although
our findings must be replicated with a larger number of participants, they support the view that sex offenders might have impairments in
the decoding of some emotional cues conveyed by the conspecifics' face, which could have an impact on affective empathy.
© 2007 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Empathy; Emotion; Emotional recognition; Sex offenders; Anxiety; Depression

1. Introduction 2002; Decety and Jackson, 2004; Blair, 2005). Although


there is little consensus among theorists regarding its
The construct of empathy has received much attention definition and its constitutive components, most agree that
in different domains of research (e.g., psychology, ethol- empathy is a multidimensional phenomenon that involves
ogy, cognitive neuroscience, and psychiatry) (see Eisen- an understanding of a person's subjective experience, a
berg and Strayer, 1987; Ickes, 1997; Preston and de Waal, perspective taking, and a vicarious sharing of emotional
states in response to another's affective cues, often re-
sulting in feelings of concern or compassion for this person
⁎ Corresponding authors. Soussignan is to be contacted at Laboratoire (i.e., sympathy), although it also can lead to self-oriented
Vulnérabilité, Adaptation, Psychopathologie, Centre Émotion, CNRS feelings (i.e., personal distress) (Davis, 1983; Hoffman,
UMR 7593, Hôpital de la Salpêtrière, 47 Bd de l'Hôpital, 75013 Paris.
Tel.: +33 1 42161744. 1984; Decety and Jackson, 2004). Although it is unclear
E-mail addresses: musette45@hotmail.com (I. Gery), whether all the components are required for the unfolding
soussign@ext.jussieu.fr (R. Soussignan). of the empathetic process, it is usually acknowledged that
0165-1781/$ - see front matter © 2007 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2007.11.006
I. Gery et al. / Psychiatry Research 165 (2009) 252–262 253

they play important functions in social bonds (Anderson viewpoint, is thought to be an essential component in the
and Keltner, 2002), prosocial behaviors (Eisenberg and mechanisms that account for the theory of mind. Stage 3,
Miller, 1987), altruism (Batson, 1991) and moral judgment emotion replication, involves some level of sharing or
(Hoffman, 1987). In contrast, empathy impairments have similarity in the feelings experienced by self and other.
been related to aggressive, delinquent and antisocial Finally, the last stage, response decision, refers to the
behaviors (Miller and Eisenberg, 1988; Jolliffe and observer's decision to exhibit or not, on the basis of his/
Farrington, 2004; Lovett and Sheffield, 2007), and deficits her feelings, a socially oriented behavior (e.g., helping or
in specific components of empathy have been proposed as comforting behaviors).
a feature of autism or psychopathy (Blair, 2005). The present study aims to focus on the first step of
The inverse relationship typically found between empathy process (recognition of emotional cues) in
empathy and aggression led a number of researchers different groups of participants (sex offenders, non-sex
and clinicians to suggest that sex offenders (e.g., child offenders, and normal controls) and investigates the
molesters, rapists) are likely deficient in their capacity to relationship between the ability to recognize emotional
empathize with their conspecifics (e.g., Marshall and facial expressions and self-reported empathy. The few
Barbaree, 1990). More specifically, it was assumed that available findings suggest that sex offenders might be less
because of a lack of compassion for the victim's distress, a accurate than controls or non-sex offenders in recognizing
child molester or a rapist does not inhibit an attack when the emotional states of others (Hudson et al., 1993; Lisak
he becomes sexually aroused toward a potential victim. and Ivan, 1995), although some negative findings have
However, empirical research on empathy in sexually also been reported in the form of dissertation abstracts
aggressive persons conducted during the last three (Franklin, 2000; Cuevas, 2004). To date, the most detailed
decades provided inconsistent findings (for reviews, see findings reported by Hudson et al. (1993) provided
Marshall et al., 1995; Geer et al., 2000; Covell and evidence that sexually aggressive men (child molesters
Scalora, 2002). Some studies reported lower levels of and rapists) were less accurate than were violent non-sex
generalized empathy or for some of its components (e.g., offenders at recognizing emotional expressions, with
empathic concern, perspective taking) in sexual offenders confusions between fear and surprise on the one hand,
compared with non-sexual offenders (Rice et al., 1994; and disgust and anger on the other hand. Such confusions
Lisak and Ivan, 1995; Lindsey et al., 2001; Marshall and between fear and surprise, in return, have been hypothe-
Moulden, 2001) or their non-offending counterparts sized to lead sex offenders to have difficulties in reading
(Hudson et al., 1993: study 2; Chaplin et al., 1995; the distress in the “eyes” of another. However, because sex
Burke, 2001), whereas other studies have found no offenders often display affective disturbances (i.e., anxiety
differences between sexual offenders and other groups of and depression) (Becker et al., 1991; Ahlmeyer et al.,
participants (Hoppe and Singer, 1976; Langevin et al., 2003; Dunsieth et al., 2004), which were not controlled for
1988; Hudson et al., 1993: study 1; Hanson and Scott, in previous studies, it is unclear whether these negative
1995), or showed empathy deficits in sex offenders effects have contributed to the variance of data and, thus,
only in specific situations or toward their own victims may explain inconsistency among findings. More speci-
(Pithers, 1999; Fernandez and Marshall, 2003). fically, because substantial evidence indicated that depres-
Marshall et al. (1995) proposed an appealing staged sion or anxiety in non-offenders may bias the recognition
multicomponent model of empathy that was designed to of emotional facial expressions (Gur et al., 1992;
examine potential deficiencies in sexual offenders at Surguladze et al., 2004; Montagne et al., 2006), it is
each stage of the process. According to this model, the important for studies examining emotion recognition in
ongoing process of empathy involves four discriminable sex offenders to partial out the effects of depression and
steps: (1) emotional recognition, (2) perspective taking, anxiety. In the present study, such variables were entered as
(3) emotion replication, and (4) response decision. Stage covariates in a design examining the ability of sex
1, the emotion-recognition stage, is needed to decode or offenders (child molesters) to decode facial expressions
read emotional signals of others and is considered nec- of basic emotions compared with non-sex offenders and
essary for the unfolding of subsequent stages. Such normal controls. Furthermore, because self-report mea-
signals, more specifically those conveyed by the face, sures may be affected by self-presentation, and because
constitute a primary way for communicating basic emo- socially desirable responding has been reported in studies
tional states or current intentions and play a powerful role of sex offenders (Curwen, 2003), various scales presumed
in the regulation of social interactions (e.g., Fridlund, to assess distinct constitutive components of empathy
1994; Ekman and Rosenberg, 2005). Stage 2, perspective (perspective taking, affective empathy, empathy concern,
taking, which is the ability to understand another person's personal distress) entered self-reported social desirability
254 I. Gery et al. / Psychiatry Research 165 (2009) 252–262

as a covariate. By partialling out potential confounding this series, we selected three intensity levels for each
effects such as depression, anxiety, and social desirability, emotional expression (40% = mild, 70% = moderate, and
we expected that our study should provide a more stringent 100% = strong) giving a total of 36 emotional stimuli and
assessment of the empathy process in both sex and non-sex 12 neutral faces.
offenders. The ability to decode distinct intensity levels of
emotional expressions was also investigated in order to 2.2.2. Empathy questionnaires
assess whether sex offenders were less accurate in
recognizing emotions when affective cues expressed by 2.2.2.1. The empathy scale of the Impulsivity–Venture-
others were reduced. Finally, by examining relationships someness–Empathy-7 questionnaire (ES-IVE-7, Eysenck
between recognition accuracy of emotions and distinct et al., 1985). The IVE-7 is a 54-item questionnaire in a
components of empathy in offenders and non-offenders, yes/no format that includes three scales: Impulsiveness
our study was also designed to explore the relevance of the (19 items), Venturesomeness (16 items), and Empathy
multidimensional staged process of empathy suggested by (19 items). We used only the empathy scale, which is
Marshall et al. (1995). presumed to assess the affective component of empathy.
This scale was derived from the Questionnaire Measure
2. Methods of Emotional Empathy (QMEE) of Mehrabian and
Epstein (1972), who defined emotional empathy as
2.1. Participants “vicariously emotional response to the perceived
emotional experiences of others”. Eysenck et al.
Thirty males divided into three groups matched for age (1985) reported an acceptable internal reliability
and high school education level (E.L.) (P N 0.05) were (alpha = 0.69) and published age norms for males
recruited from a prison located south of Paris: a) a group of (n = 559) and females (n = 761). Comparable findings
10 inmates (age= 43± 4.49 years; E.L. = 4.5 ± 1.96 years) of reliability and mean scores of empathy have been
incarcerated for child sexual abuse (SO: sex offenders); b) reported in a sample of French males and females (Caci
a group of 10 inmates (age= 38.1 ± 3.81 years; E.L. = 4.9 ± et al., 2003).
1.37 years) incarcerated for theft or fraud (NSO: non-sex
offenders); and c) a group of 10 prison staff members 2.2.2.2. The Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI, Davis,
(age = 40.3± 4.21 years; E.L. = 5.8 ± 1.55 years) directly 1983). The IRI consists of four 7-item subscales –
involved in the supervision of inmates (non-offender Perspective Taking (PT), Fantasy (FS), Empathic
controls). The criminal justice records were used to select Concern (EC), and Personal Distress (PD). The first
the groups of offenders. All the offenders had already had two subscales (PT and FS) measure the cognitive facet
an experience of incarceration before being transferred to of empathy, whereas the other subscales (EC and PD)
the prison after their conviction. None of them were are presumed to tap the affective facet of empathy.1
followed by the psychiatric service of the prison or were The PT subscale assesses the ability to adopt the
being treated during the study. Written informed consent viewpoint of another person. The FS subscale mea-
was obtained from all participants. sures the tendency to identify oneself with fictional
characters in books, movies, or plays. The EC subscale
2.2. Stimuli and material reflects feelings of sympathy or compassion for
another person. The PD subscale assesses self-oriented
2.2.1. Emotion recognition test feelings of distress or anxiety in emergency situations
Black-and-white slides of models (6 males and 6 or in reaction to negative emotions of another. Each
females), each one displaying a distinct facial expression item of the IRI is scored on a 5-point Likert scale
of a basic emotion (happiness, sadness, anger, surprise, ranging from 0 (does not describe me well) to 4
fear, and disgust), and an emotionally neutral face were (describes me very well). Satisfactory internal (from
chosen as prototype images from Ekman and Friesen's 0.71 to 0.77) and test–retest (from 0.62 to 0.71)
(1976) Pictures of Facial Affect. The slides were scanned reliabilities have been reported for the four subscales
to 700 × 466 pixels to allow computerised presentation on (Davis, 1983), and sufficient external validity of the
a 17″ screen of a laptop. For each prototypical emotion,
facial expressions differing in intensity by 10% steps were
then constructed using the MorphMan 4.0 software on the 1
Although EC was designed to assess affective empathy, this
neutral face (0% of emotional intensity) and the full- subscale is usually considered as a measure of sympathy and contains
blown expression (100% of emotional intensity). From few items assessing affective sharing.
I. Gery et al. / Psychiatry Research 165 (2009) 252–262 255

IRI was provided by comparing it with other measures choice response format, participants had to judge, after
of empathy (Davis and Franzoi, 1991). each stimulus presentation, the emotion portrayed on the
photograph by choosing one of the seven labels displayed
2.2.2.3. The Empathy Quotient (EQ, Lawrence et al., on the screen (anger, disgust, fear, happiness, neutral,
2004). The EQ is a self-report questionnaire contain- sadness, and surprise). They were also asked to rate the
ing 40 empathy items and 20 filler items included to intensity of each emotional expression from 0 (no intensity
distract the participant from a relentless focus on — neutral) to 9 (extremely strong). Participants were
empathy. Unlike other scales, it was explicitly devel- shown examples of facial expressions of emotions
oped to measure global empathy in both healthy differing in intensity (40%, 70%, and 100%) to ensure
individuals and psychiatric populations with empathic that they perceived differences between intermediate
dysfunction (e.g., autistic spectrum disorders). expressions of a specific emotion. Furthermore, in order
Responses are given on a 4-point scale ranging from to familiarize the participants with the procedure, they
‘strongly agree’ to ‘strongly disagree’. Baron-Cohen and were asked to complete two practice trials during which
Wheelwright (2004) reported high test–retest reliability the experimenter helped them if they did not understand
for the EQ over a period of 12 months, and satisfactory the task. The 48 photographs were presented in a random
concurrent validity evidenced by moderate correlations order across participants for 6 s at a distance of about
with the EC and PT subscales of the IRI. Satisfactory 40 cm. Stimulus presentation and timing were controlled
psychometric qualities of the French version of the using the SuperLab software. After the emotion-recogni-
EQ have been reported in a healthy sample of 201 tion test, the participants completed self-report question-
males and 209 females (Berthoz et al., 2008). naires to assess empathy, depression, anxiety and social
desirability.
2.2.3. The Depression Inventory—short form
Depression was measured with the French version of 2.4. Statistical analyses
the shortened 13-item Beck Depression Inventory (BDI)
(Beck et al., 1974; Collet and Cottraux, 1986), a widely Because the offenders reported higher depression and
used self-rating scale with established reliability and anxiety scores than the participants of the control group
validity (Beck et al., 1988). (see Section 3), recognition-accuracy, scores (hit rate) and
intensity ratings of emotional expressions were submitted
2.2.4. The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI-Y; to a 6 × 3 × 3 (Emotion × Group × Intensity) multivariate
Spielberger, 1983) analysis of covariance (MANCOVA, Pillai's Trace), using
The state form of the STAI (French version: BDI and S-STAI scores as covariates. Emotion (anger,
Bruchon-Schweitzer and Paulhan, 1993) was used in disgust, fear, happiness, sadness, surprise) and Intensity
this study. The state form (S-STAI) is a well-validated (40%, 70%, and 100%) were introduced as within-subject
self-report scale containing 20 items from which factors, and Group (sex offenders, non-sex offenders,
participants must indicate their degree of approval on controls) as the between-subjects factor. Signal detection
a 4-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 ‘‘No’’ to 4 statistics were used to assess discriminability/sensitivity
‘‘Yes’’. and response bias. Sensitivity, defined as the overlap
between the signal and the noise distributions, was
2.2.5. The Social Desirability Scale (SDS) calculated by d′ (Stanislaw and Todorov, 1999). The
The SDS (Crowne and Marlowe, 1960) contains 33 estimation of d′ was obtained by subtracting the z score
true–false items tapping the tendency to respond to corresponding to the false alarm rate (FAR) from the z
items in a socially desirable manner. A high score on this score corresponding to the hit rate (HR). FAR is the
scale indicates that persons tend to over-report socially proportion of choices of a facial expression not matched to
desirable information about themselves. the appropriate emotion. Larger positive values of d′
indicate greater sensitivity; values close to 0 indicate a low
2.3. Procedure sensitivity to the presence of the target emotional
expression. Values less than 0 indicate a lack of sensitivity
Participants were tested individually in a quiet room. At that might result from response confusion. Response bias,
the beginning of the session, the experimenter told them which measures the general tendency to provide yes or no
that a series of photographs showing men and women responses to the target stimulus, was assessed by C = −1/
displaying emotional expressions would be presented over 2 * (ZHR +ZFAR). When C is negative, the participant is
a few seconds on the screen of a laptop. Using a forced liberal (i.e., tends to select an emotional expression more
256 I. Gery et al. / Psychiatry Research 165 (2009) 252–262

often), whereas when C is positive, the participant is nificant difference was detected between sex offenders
conservative (i.e., tends to underestimate the presence of (M = 11.7, S.D. = 4) and non-sex offenders (M = 15.2,
the emotional target). Then, d′ and C were submitted to a S.D. = 3.08), P N 0.05.
MANCOVA using Emotion as a within-subject factor,
Group as a between-subjects factor, and BDI and S-STAI 3.2. Accuracy rates of emotional recognition
scores as covariates.
Because self-reports of empathy were strongly cor- Table 1 shows mean rates of accuracy as a function of
related with depression, anxiety and social desirability emotion category, group and intensity level of facial
(see Section 3), these variables were entered as covariates expressions. The MANCOVA indicated a significant
using analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs) on ES-IVE, main effect of BDI covariate on the accuracy rates of
EQ, and IRI subscales scores (PT, EC, FS, PD). To control emotional recognition, F(1, 25) = 4.04, P = 0.05, ηp2 =
the familywise error rate due to multiple post hoc 0.14. A main effect for Group was also found, F(2, 25) =
comparisons, Bonferroni correction of alpha level was 47.76, P b 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.79, indicating that the sex
used (Bonferroni t). The strength of association between
significant effects and the dependent variables (i.e., effect
size) was estimated by partial eta squared (ηp2). Table 1
Accuracy rates of emotion recognition as a function of group (SO = sex
offenders, SNO = non-sex offenders, controls = non-offenders controls)
3. Results
and intensity levels (40% = mild, 70% = moderate, 100% = strong) of
facial expressions
3.1. Depression, anxiety and social desirability and
Emotion Group
their relations with other measures intensity
SO NSO Controls Total
level (%)
An analysis of variance (ANOVA) revealed that the Mean S.D. Mean S.D. Mean S.D. Mean S.D.
sex offenders (SO) reported higher depression scores Neutral 0.82 0.13 0.86 0.12 0.94 0.08 0.87 0.12
(M = 25.3, S.D. = 4.14) than the other participants Anger 0.70 0.26 1 0 1 0 0.90 0.20
(NSO: M = 13, S.D. = 2.21; controls: M = 0.9, S.D. = (40%)
Anger 0.75 0.35 1 0 1 0 0.92 0.23
1.28), F (2, 27) = 188.67, P b 0.001, with the non-sex
(70%)
offenders having more depressive mood than the controls Anger 0.90 0.21 1 0 1 0 0.96 0.13
(P b 0.001, Bonferroni test). The SO group also obtained (100%)
higher S-anxiety scores (M = 67.9, S.D. = 3.93) than did Disgust 0.05 0.16 0.55 0.16 0.65 0.24 0.43 0.32
the other groups (NSO: M = 61, S.D. = 2.79; controls: (40%)
Disgust 0 0 1 0 1 0 0.67 0.48
M = 28.4, S.D. = 3.50), and the NSO were more anxious
(70%)
than the controls [F(2, 27) = 376.38 and Bonferroni test, Disgust 0.05 0.16 1 0 1 0 0.68 0.46
P b 0.0001]. When social desirability was entered as a (100%)
covariate, the pattern of findings between the groups of Fear (40%) 0.10 0.21 0.75 0.26 0.95 0.16 0.60 0.42
participants did not change for depression and anxiety. Fear (70%) 0 0 1 0 1 0 0.67 0.48
Fear (100%) 0.10 0.32 1 0 0.95 0.16 0.68 0.46
BDI scores correlated significantly (P b 0.002) across
Happiness 0.90 0.21 1 0 1 0 0.97 0.13
groups with the following empathic measures: rs (28) = (40%)
−0.92 for EQ, −0.92 for ES-IVE, −0.93 for PT, −0.55 for Happiness 1 0 1 0 1 0 1 0
FS, −0.93 for EC, and 0.83 for PD. Similarly, strong (70%)
significant correlations were found between empathic Happiness 1 0 1 0 1 0 1 0
(100%)
measures and S-STAI scores (P b 0.0001): rs (28) = −0.93
Sadness 0.65 0.42 0.80 0.26 0.90 0.32 0.78 0.34
for EQ, −0.82 for ES-IVE, −0.91 for PT, −0.70 for FS, (40%)
−0.91 for EC, and 0.71 for PD. Social desirability also Sadness 0.60 0.46 1 0 1 0 0.87 0.32
correlated significantly (P b 0.01) with the following (70%)
measures: rs (28) = 0.81 for EQ, 0.66 for ES-IVE, 0.79 Sadness 0.95 0.16 1 0 1 0 0.98 0.09
(100%)
for PT, 0.49 for FS, 0.78 for EC, and −0.67 for PD.
Surprise 0.40 0.32 0.80 0.26 1 0 0.73 0.34
Furthermore, an ANOVA carried out on scores of social (40%)
desirability revealed a main effect of Group, F(2, 27) = Surprise 0.55 0.44 0.95 0.16 0.95 0.16 0.82 0.33
27.84, P b 0.0001. Controls had higher scores of social (70%)
desirability (M = 23.3, S.D. = 3.62) than the other groups Surprise 0.50 0.41 0.95 0.16 0.95 0.16 0.80 0.34
(100%)
of subjects (P b 0.001, Bonferroni tests), while no sig-
I. Gery et al. / Psychiatry Research 165 (2009) 252–262 257

offenders (M = 0.51, S.D. = 0.096) were less accurate than sex offenders, P = 0.09 (controls: M = −0.14, S.D. = 0.29;
the non-sex offenders (M = 0.93, S.D. = 0.03) and the NSO: M = −0.21, S.D. = 0.60; SO: M = 0.10, S.D. = 0.67).
controls (M = 0.96, S.D. = 0.03) for recognizing emotional
expressions (Bonferroni test, P b 0.0001). Significant 3.3. Intensity ratings of facial expressions
Emotion × Group, F(10, 44) = 4.16, P b 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.49,
and Emotion × Intensity × Group interactions were de- The MANCOVA failed to show significant effects for
tected, F(20, 34) = 2.56, P = 0.008, ηp2 = 0.60. One-way the distinct variables. Only the Emotion × Intensity × BDI
ANCOVAs conducted on each emotional expression interaction reached significance, F(10, 16) = 2.55,
showed that these effects mainly reflect the fact that the P b 0.05, ηp2 = 0.61, suggesting the importance of depres-
sex offenders recognized less accurately disgust, F(2, 25) = sion for explaining differences in intensity ratings of
65.83, P b 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.84, fear, F(2, 25) = 29.13, emotional expressions as a function of their intensity
P b 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.70, and anger expressions, F(2, 25) = level. Significant effects were found for the distinct
10.22, P = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.45, than did the other groups variables (Intensity, Group, Emotion, Emotion × Group,
of participants. They also decoded less accurately sur- and Intensity × Group) only when the covariates were
prise, F(2, 25) = 3.75, P = 0.04, ηp2 = 0.23, than the non- excluded from the analysis.
sex offenders (Bonferroni test, P b 0.05). Furthermore, the
sex offenders were less accurate in decoding mild levels 3.4. Empathy ratings
of disgust F(2, 25) = 5.75, P = 0.009, ηp2 = 0.315, and fear
expressions, F(2, 25) = 4.12, P = 0.03, ηp2 = 0.25, than Self-rated empathy scores from the three question-
were the non-sex offenders (Bonferroni tests, all naires are reported in Table 2. The ANCOVA and the
P b 0.05). It should be noted that the effect sizes are Bonferroni test indicated that the controls had signifi-
large, more particularly when the significant effects cantly higher EQ scores than the offenders, F(2,24) =
concerned the three groups of participants (i.e., see effect 7.18, P b 0.003, ηp2 = 0.37. Differences between SO and
sizes for fear and disgust), suggesting that the effects can NSO approached significance, P = 0.053 (Bonferroni
be accepted with confidence. test). For the ES-IVE score, a group effect was also
Significant effects for Group, F(2, 25) = 27.49, found, F(2,24) = 8.41, P = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.41; the SO
P b 0.001, ηp2 = 0.69, and Emotion × Group interaction group reported lower affective empathy scores than
were found for the emotional sensitivity index (d′), F(10, the other groups, P b 0.05, whereas no difference was
44) = 3.01, P = 0.006, η2p = 0.41, with sex offenders being found between the NSO and the controls. An ANCOVA
less sensitive (M = 0.041, S.D. = 0.78) than the other groups was also applied on each IRI subscale. For PT, a
(controls: M = 4.18, S.D. = 0.49; NSO: M = 3.59, S.D. = significant difference was found among the three
0.47). One-way ANCOVAs, using Group as the between- groups, F(2, 24) = 4.04, P = 0.03, ηp2 = 0.25. The controls
subjects factor, were also conducted on d′ to specific target had a higher score on the PT subscale than the other
faces (e.g., surprise responses to fear faces). The analyses groups (P b 0.05, Bonferroni test), but no significant
revealed that sex offenders falsely identified disgust
expressions as anger expressions, F(2, 25) =22.633,
P b 0.001, ηp2 = 0.64 (controls: M = 3.55, S.D. =0.87; Table 2
NSO: = 3.00, S.D. = 0.87; SO: M = −2.98, S.D. =1.32). Mean scores on Empathy Quotient (EQ) scale, and on subscales of
Furthermore, sex offenders provided more false surprise Empathy–IVE and Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) in sex
offenders (SO), non-sex offenders (NSO) and controls
responses when viewing fear faces, F(2, 25) =15.39,
P b 0.001, ηp2 = 0.55 (controls: M = 4.33, S.D. =1.03; SO NSO Controls
NSO: M = 3.42, S.D. = 1.36; SO: M = −3.87, S.D. =1.74). Mean S.D. Mean S.D. Mean S.D.
No differences were detected between non-sex offend- Empathy 5.60 1.07 19.8 5.39 50.5 6.36
ers and normal controls (P N 0.05, Bonferroni test). The Quotient (EQ)
bias index (C) submitted to the MANCOVA indicated an Empathy 4.60 1.57 12.3 2.06 17.0 1.70
effect for depression, F(1, 25) = 6.22, P = 0.02, η2p = 0.20), subscale-IVE
Perspective 6.60 1.26 12.2 2.39 20.9 1.66
and for Emotion × Group interaction, F(10,44) = 2.06,
Taking (PT-IRI)
P = 0.049, η2p = 0.32). The ANCOVAs conducted on C Fantasy (FS-IRI) 10.50 1.58 8.50 3.44 16.7 3.77
yielded a significant main effect of Group only for the Empathic 4.60 1.90 12.0 2.79 23.9 2.60
sadness expression, F(2,25) = 4.08, P = 0.03, η2p = 0.25). Concern (EC-IRI)
Post hoc comparisons revealed only that sex offenders Personal 18.10 1.20 12.7 1.49 10.3 2.58
Distress (PD-IRI)
tended to select less frequently the sad label than the non-
258 I. Gery et al. / Psychiatry Research 165 (2009) 252–262

difference was found between SO and NSO. For the FS surprise (pr = − 0.46, P = 0.01) and sadness (pr = − 0.45,
subscale, the Group factor did not reach significance, P = 0.02).
F(2, 24) = 1.82, P = 0.13. Differences between groups
were also found for EC, F(2, 24) = 3.73, P = 0.04, 4. Discussion
ηp2 = 0.24. Post hoc analysis revealed that the controls
had higher ratings of EC than the SO only, P b 0.05. This research addressed two main issues derived from
Finally, a significant difference between the groups the multidimensional staged model of empathy proposed
was observed for PD, F(2, 24) = 3.55, P = 0.04, by Marshall et al. (1995). The first objective was to
ηp2 = 0.23. Bonferroni tests revealed that the SO had investigate whether sex offenders, compared with other
higher PD scores than the NSO, P = 0.05. The participants, were impaired in their ability to recognize
inspection of size effects for the IRI subscales indicates emotional cues (first stage of the model) conveyed by a
that they are rather small (b 0.3), suggesting that the conspecific's face. The present study demonstrated that
relationships between empathy components and delin- sex offenders, and more particularly child molesters, were
quency are not consistent. In contrast, only the ES-IVE less accurate in decoding emotional facial signals than
scale revealed a moderate size effect (N 0.4). non-sex offenders or normal controls. More specifically,
the sex offenders were less sensitive than the other
3.5. Relationships between accuracy rates of emotion participants in recognizing facial expressions of anger,
recognition and empathy scores disgust, surprise and fear. These findings reflected the fact
that they often confused fear with surprise on the one
Partial correlations (pr), using BDI and S-STAI scores hand, and disgust with anger on the other hand. When
as covariates, were performed between emotional recog- viewing fear expressions, sex offenders frequently
nition accuracy and empathy scores across participants. interpreted them as surprise signals, whereas disgust
Positive correlations were found between ES-IVE scores expressions were frequently decoded as anger signals.
and accuracy rates for the following emotions: disgust Such findings cannot be explained by offenders' self-
(pr= 0.56, P = 0.002), fear (pr = 0.45, P = 0.02), surprise reported negative affective states, such as depression and
(pr= 0.33, P = 0.08), and sadness (pr= 0.33, P = 0.08). In anxiety, which were partialled out in the present study.
other terms, the ability to recognize some emotions was Taken together, the pattern of results on emotional
greater in participants who obtained high scores of confusion is consistent with that obtained by Hudson et
affective empathy. No significant correlations, however, al. (1993) on a sample of sex offenders including both
were detected between EQ, PT, FS or EC scores and child molesters and rapists. Impairments in the recogni-
accuracy rates of emotion recognition. Nevertheless, ne- tion of fearful cues from the face have also been reported
gative correlations were found between PD and recogni- in adolescents with antisocial or aggressive behaviors
tion accuracy rates for the following emotions: disgust (Blair and Coles, 2000; Carr and Lutjemeier, 2005).
(pr= −0.34, P = 0.08), fear (pr = −0.364, P = 0.057), sur- What kind of explanation could account for the high
prise (pr = −0.43, P = 0.02), anger (pr = −0.48, P = 0.01), rates of confusion in sex offenders' emotional recogni-
and sadness (pr = −0.36, P = 0.06). tion? Regarding fear and surprise, one possibility is to
Partial correlations between empathic and recognition consider the degree of similarity in the perceptual
accuracy rates were also examined for the distinct levels properties of these expressions that have in common a
of facial expressions of emotions. Recognition accuracy number of muscular actions in the upper part of the face
rates were positively correlated with ES-IVE scores for (i.e., brow raising, and upper lid raising). Similarly, for
moderate (pr = 0.57, P = 0.001) and strong (pr = 0.50, anger and disgust, although the morphology of facial
P = 0.006) levels of fear, and for mild (pr = 0.36, configurations is somewhat different, they also share a
P = 0.058) and strong (pr = 0.36, P = 0.058) levels of muscular action in the upper part of the face (i.e., brow
surprise. Finally, a positive correlation was found lowering). Thus, one cannot rule out the hypothesis that
between the ability to decode moderate expressions of the sex offenders' misinterpretation in the decoding
sadness and ES-IVE scores (pr = 0.42, P = 0.02). In of basic emotions was possibly biased by selective
contrast, PD scores were negatively correlated with processing of some facial cues. Additional studies are
recognition accuracy rates for moderate (pr = − 0.46, needed to test this hypothesis by manipulating the nature
P = 0.01) and strong pr = − 0.39, P = 0.03) levels of of facial cues presented to sex offenders.
disgust. PD scores were also negatively correlated with Another level of analysis for explaining sex offenders'
recognition accuracy rates for moderate expressions of difficulties in interpreting emotional recognition of
anger (P = − 0.52, P = 0.006), fear (pr = − 0.46, P = 0.01), conspecifics might be related to their history or past
I. Gery et al. / Psychiatry Research 165 (2009) 252–262 259

experiences. Although our findings cannot resolve this empathy and partialling out both negative affective states
issue, a tentative interpretation may be proposed by (depression and anxiety) and social desirability, we
examining studies that have highlighted the potential expected to sort out conflicting results in the literature
influence of early adverse social environments on the and assess the relevance of the multidimensional staged
recognition of emotional signals. Because many sex model of empathy proposed by Marshall et al. (1995). Our
offenders have been exposed to negative interpersonal findings revealed that the non-offender controls reported
experiences and trauma during childhood (i.e., physical or higher levels of empathy than did the offenders for the
sexual abuses) (e.g., Burton, 2000; Jonson-Reid and Way, three scales used in this study. However, inspection of size
2001), research on emotional recognition in abused effects indicated that the findings were more consistent for
children is likely relevant. For instance, abused children the ES-IVE scale (i.e., an index of affective empathy),
were found to be less accurate than non-abused children in which was the only empathy component that discrimi-
recognizing anger expressions (Camras et al., 1988), nated sex offenders from non-sex offenders. Compared
tended to over-interpret expressive signals as threatening, with IRI-affective subscales (empathy concern, and
and over-identified anger relative to fear and sadness personal distress), the ES-IVE scale is more affective
(Pollak and Kistler, 2002). It might be that abused sharing-oriented. Although the ES-IVE scale is not
children, by becoming defensively hypervigilant to designed to specifically assess affective sharing/similarity
hostile cues, could display a hostile attributional bias between the self and the other, it is usually considered that
toward others (Dodge et al., 1995), more particularly it targets affective empathy more than the IRI subscales of
when the affective cues are ambiguous or are conveyed by empathy concern and distress, which are designed to tap
distinct expressions. It is, thus, possible that sex offenders sympathy and self-oriented distress, respectively. Because
in our study also displayed a hostile attribution bias when the available data on empathy in sex offenders are
viewing distinct facial expressions (e.g., anger vs. disgust) inconsistent, we suggest that, if differences exist between
conveying a similar affective cue. Regarding fear and these persons and other control groups, affective empathy,
surprise, although we have no clear explanation account- and possibly affective sharing, are likely more altered than
ing for their confusion in sex offenders, Blair and Coles cognitive empathy (i.e., perspective taking) and sympathy
(2000) have hypothesized that individuals who display (i.e., empathy concern). For personal distress, our findings
antisocial behaviors are less responsive to fearful signals revealed that the sex offenders reported a higher level than
of others because they have not learned during socialisa- did the non-sex offenders, which can be interpreted as a
tion to associate harmful actions with the victim's proclivity of sex offenders to display more negative self-
aversive cues that are thought to act as punishing stimuli. oriented feeling. However, it should be noted that the IRI
In the case of sexual offenders, one cannot rule out the subscale of personal distress imperfectly assesses the
possibility that, because of their history of maltreatment or distress dimension of empathy because most of the items
negative interpersonal experience, they have difficulties tap anxious and distress reactions not related to negative
in attributing social meaning to fear/distress cues and in emotional expressions of others. Thus, further research is
associating them with related contextual information. necessary to delineate more clearly whether distress
Whether this theoretical interpretation is relevant for our would be observed in sex offenders in response to
findings remains to be investigated. The issue of the role emotional displays of their conspecifics.
of past experience in fear recognition could be indirectly The view that affective empathy is more impaired than
explored by measuring emotional recognition via the cognitive empathy or sympathy in sex offenders is also
generation of associated knowledge (Adolphs, 2002). supported by the correlational findings that indicated that
More specifically, because fear expressions may activate only ES-IVE scores were positively related to recognition
conceptual knowledge related to both causal antecedents accuracy of emotional expressions such as disgust, fear,
(e.g., punishment, danger, or aggression) and subsequent surprise, and sadness, whereas personal distress negatively
behavior (e.g., running away, inhibiting an action), it correlated with these emotions. Thus, these findings give
would be interesting in future studies to examine whether some support to the multidimensional staged model of
such contextual social knowledge is impaired in sexual empathy proposed by Marshall et al. (1995). Although the
offenders. correlational nature of our data does not permit a causal
A second main objective of the current study was to relationship to be established between emotion recognition
examine self-reports of constitutive components of and empathy processes, the pattern of findings obtained in
empathy and their relationships with emotional recogni- our study is consistent with the hypothesis that the
tion in sex offenders, non-sex offenders and controls. recognition of affective cues from conspecifics might be
Unlike previous studies, by using multiple rating scales of crucial in inducing affective empathy, and that difficulties
260 I. Gery et al. / Psychiatry Research 165 (2009) 252–262

in recognizing some emotional expressions in sex offenders to investigate whether the association between
offenders might be involved in impairments in affective sex offending and psychopathy is related to differences
empathy. Obviously, our findings did not rule out the in measures of empathy and emotional recognition.
view that other constitutive components, such as perspec- Finally, given the shortcomings of self-report measures
tive taking, may induce affective empathy or sympathy, of empathy, which imperfectly assess distinct facets of
via other pathways, even if individuals have no direct empathy (e.g., affective sharing), our findings should be
access to perceptually visible emotional cues. For instance, replicated using not only emotional recognition tasks
imaging the reactions and feelings of persons that were and self-reported empathy scales, but also experimental
exposed to painful stimuli may induce personal distress paradigms designed to measure both subjective and
(‘imagine self’) or empathic concern (‘imagine other’), and psychophysiological (i.e., autonomic and facial electro-
these two forms of perspective taking (self vs. other) are myographic activity) indices of affective sharing.
related to distinct patterns of cerebral activation (Lamm
et al., 2007). Acknowledgements
The link between emotion recognition (perception)
and affective empathy (feeling and action) is also We thank the Head of the prison of Fresnes (France),
supported by both psychophysiological and cognitive who gave us permission to meet the prisoners and made
neuroscience research, which has suggested that the link the study possible. We are also grateful to all the
stems from an automatic perception–action mechanism participants (offenders and controls) without whom this
leading to emotional contagion (Decety and Lamm, study could not have been carried out.
2006). For instance, when unconsciously viewing angry
and happy facial expressions, participants reacted with
References
facial muscular actions corresponding to these emotions
(Dimberg et al., 2000). Accumulating evidence from
Adolphs, R., 2002. Recognizing emotion from facial expressions:
neuroimaging studies also suggests that the same cerebral
psychological and neurological mechanisms. Behavioral and
network is activated when humans perceive emotional Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews 1, 21–62.
expressions and when they feel these emotions or Ahlmeyer, S., Kleinsasser, D., Stoner, J., Retzlaff, P., 2003. Psycho-
represent the actions associated with them (Wicker pathology of incarcerated sex offenders. Journal of Personality
et al., 2003; de Gelder et al., 2004; see also Decety and Disorders 17, 306–318.
Lamm, 2006 for a recent review). For example, observing Anderson, C., Keltner, D., 2002. The role of empathy in the formation
and maintenance of social bonds. Behavioral and Brain Sciences
people expressing disgust faces and experiencing disgust 25, 21–22.
oneself activated the same brain areas (e.g., anterior Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., 2004. The Empathy Quotient: an
insula, and anterior cingulate cortex) (Wicker et al., 2003). investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning
Thus observing and feeling emotions may lead to a shared autism, and normal sex differences. Journal of Autism and
neural network between self and other. It is likely that such Developmental Disorders 34, 163–175.
Batson, C.D., 1991. The Altruism Question: Toward a Social
neuroimaging studies, if they were conducted in sex Psychological Answer. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.
offenders in the future, could help to clarify whether such Beck, A., Rial, W., Rickels, K., 1974. Short form of depression
shared representations between perception and feeling are inventory: cross validation. Psychological Reports 34, 1184–1186.
altered in sex offenders participating in emotional Beck, A., Steer, R., Garbin, M., 1988. Psychometric properties of the
Beck Depression Inventory: twenty-five years of evaluation.
recognition and emotional induction tasks.
Clinical Psychology Review 8, 77–100.
Finally, several limitations of our study must be Becker, J.V., Kaplan, .S., Tenke, C.E., Tartaglini, A., 1991. The
acknowledged. First, despite the fact that our results incidence of depressive symptomatology in juvenile sex offenders
showed strong size effects, they should be replicated on with a history of abuse. Child Abuse and Neglect 15, 531–536.
a larger sample of sex offenders. Second, although Berthoz, S., Wessa, M., Kedia, G., Wicker, B., Grèzes, J., 2008. Cross-
offenders and controls had similar education levels, they cultural validation of the empathy quotient in a French-speaking
sample. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 53, 469–477.
were not matched for IQ. Thus, the possibility that Blair, R.J., 2005. Responding to the emotions of others: dissociating
variations of IQ among groups could be related to forms of empathy through the study of typical and psychiatric
differences in expression recognition data cannot be populations. Consciousness and Cognition 14, 698–718.
ruled out. Third, although the sex offenders of this study Blair, R.J., Coles, M., 2000. Expression recognition and behavioural
were probably not psychopaths (given their increased problems in early adolescence. Cognitive Development 15, 421–434.
Bruchon-Schweitzer, M., Paulhan, I., 1993. Manuel français de l'échelle
levels of depression and anxiety), no data on psycho- d'anxiété-trait, anxiété-état de CD. ECPA, Paris. Spielberger.
pathy was reported in the present sample. In the future, it Burke, D.M., 2001. Empathy in sexually offending and nonoffending
would be interesting to study a large sample of sex adolescent males. Journal of Interpersonal Violence 16, 222–233.
I. Gery et al. / Psychiatry Research 165 (2009) 252–262 261

Burton, D.L., 2000. Were adolescent sexual offenders children with Ekman, P., Friesen, W.V., 1976. Pictures of Facial Affect. Consulting
sexual behavior problems? Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research Psychologists Press, Palo Alto, CA.
and Treatment 12, 37–48. Ekman, P., Rosenberg, E., 2005. What the Face Reveals: Basic and
Caci, H., Nadalet, L., Bayle, F.J., Robert, P., Boyer, P., 2003. Cross- Applied Studies of Spontaneous Expression using the Facial
cultural study of the Impulsiveness–Venturesomeness–Empathy Action Coding System (FACS), First edition. Oxford University
Questionnaire (IVE-7). Comprehensive Psychiatry 44, 381–387. Press, New York.
Camras, L., Ribordy, S., Hill, J., Martino, S., Spaccarelli, S., Stefani, R., Eysenck, S.B., Pearson, P.R., Easting, G., Allsopp, J.F., 1985. Age
1988. Recognition and posing of emotional expressions by abused norms for impulsiveness, venturesomeness and empathy in adults.
children and their mothers. Developmental Psychology 24, 776–781. Personality and Individual Differences 6, 613–619.
Carr, M.B., Lutjemeier, J.A., 2005. The relation of facial affect recog- Fernandez, Y.M., Marshall, W.L., 2003. Victim empathy, social self-
nition and empathy to delinquency in youth offenders. Adolescence esteem, and psychopathy in rapists. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of
40, 601–619. Research and Treatment 15, 11–25.
Chaplin, T.C., Rice, M.E., Harris, G.T., 1995. Salient victim suffering Franklin, K.D., 2000. Emotion recognition skills in adolescent sex
and the sexual response of child molesters. Journal of Consulting offenders. Dissertation Abstract International: Science Engineering
and Clinical Psychology 48, 635–653. 60 (8B), 4221.
Collet, L., Cottraux, J., 1986. Inventaire abrégé de la dépression de Beck Fridlund, A.J., 1994. Human Facial Expression: An Evolutionary
(13 items) Étude de la validité concurrente avec les échelles de View. Academic Press, San Diego.
Hamilton et de ralentissement de Widlöcher. L'Encéphale 12, 77–79. Geer, J.H., Estupinan, L.A., Manguno, M., Gina, M., 2000. Empathy,
Covell, C.N., Scalora, M.J., 2002. Empathic deficits in sexual offenders: social skills, and other relevant cognitive processes in rapists and
an integration of affective, social and cognitive constructs. Aggres- child molesters. Aggression and Violent Behavior 5, 99–126.
sion and Violent Behavior 7, 251–270. Gur, R.C., Erwin, R.J., Gur, R.E., Zwil, A.S., Heimberg, C., Kraemer,
Crowne, D.P., Marlowe, D., 1960. A new scale of social desirability H.C., 1992. Facial emotion discrimination: II. Behavioral findings
independent of psychopathology. Journal of Consulting Psychol- in depression. Psychiatry Research 42, 241–251.
ogy 24, 349–354. Hanson, R.K., Scott, H., 1995. Assessing perspective taking among
Cuevas, C.A., 2004. Obstacles to empathy in sex offenders: recognition sexual offenders, nonsexual criminals and nonoffenders. Sexual
and interpretation of children's emotional cues. Dissertation Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment 7, 259–277.
Abstract International: Science Engineering 65 (5-B), 2618. Hoffman, M.L., 1984. Interaction of affect and cognition in empathy.
Curwen, T., 2003. The importance of offense characteristics, victim- In: Izard, C., Kagan, J., Zajonc, R. (Eds.), Emotion, Cognition, and
ization history, hostility, and social desirability in assessing em- Behavior. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp. 103–131.
pathy of male adolescent sex offenders. Sexual Abuse: A Journal Hoffman, M.L., 1987. The contribution of empathy to justice and
of Research and Treatment 15, 347–364. moral judgment. In: N. Eisenberg, N., Strayer, J. (Eds.), Empathy
Davis, M.H., 1983. Measuring individual differences in empathy: evi- and its Development. Cambridge University Press, New York,
dence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and pp. 47–80.
Social Psychology 44, 113–126. Hoppe, C.M., Singer, R.D., 1976. Overcontrolled hostility, empathy,
Davis, M.H., Franzoi, S.L., 1991. Stability and change in adolescent and egocentric balance in violent and nonviolent psychiatric
self-consciousness and empathy. Journal of Research in Person- offenders. Psychological Reports 39, 1303–1308.
ality 25, 70–87. Hudson, S.M., Marshall, W.L., Wales, D., McDonald, E., Bakker, L.W.,
Decety, J., Jackson, P.L., 2004. The functional architecture of human McLean, A., 1993. Emotional recognition skills of sex offenders.
empathy. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Review 3, 71–100. Annals of Sex Research 6, 199–211.
Decety, J., Lamm, C., 2006. Human empathy through the lens of social Ickes, W., 1997. Empathic Concern. Guilford, New York.
neuroscience. Scientific World Journal 6, 1146–1163. Jolliffe, D., Farrington, D.P., 2004. Empathy and offending: a systemat-
de Gelder, B., Snyder, J., Greve, D., Gerard, G., Hadjikhani, N., 2004. ic review and meta-analysis. Aggression and Violent Behavior 9,
Fear fosters flight: a mechanism for fear contagion when 441–476.
perceiving emotion expressed by a whole body. Proceedings of Jonson-Reid, M., Way, I., 2001. Adolescent sexual offenders:
the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America incidence of childhood maltreatment, serious emotional distur-
101, 16701–16706. bance, and prior offenses. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry
Dimberg, U., Thunberg, M., Elmehed, K., 2000. Unconscious facial re- 71, 120–130.
actions to emotional facial expressions. Psychological Science 11, Lamm, C., Batson, C.D., Decety, J., 2007. The neural substrate of
86–89. human empathy: effects of perspective-taking and cognitive
Dodge, K.A., Pettit, G.S., Bates, J.E., Valente, E., 1995. Social appraisal. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 19, 42–58.
information–processing patterns partially mediate the effect of Langevin, R., Wright, P., Handy, L., 1988. Empathy, assertiveness, and
early physical abuse on later conduct problems. Journal of defensiveness among sex offenders. Annals of Sex Research 1,
Abnormal Psychology, 104, 632–643. 533–547.
Dunsieth, N.W., Nelson, E.B., Brusman-Lovins, L.A., Holcomb, J.L., Lawrence, E.J., Shaw, P., Baker, D., Baron-Cohen, S., David, A.S.,
Beckman, D., Welge, J.A., Roby, D., Taylor, P., Soutullo, C.A., 2004. Measuring empathy: reliability and validity of the Empathy
McElroy, S.L., 2004. Psychiatric and legal features of 113 men Quotient. Psychological Medicine 34, 911–919.
convicted of sexual offenses. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 65, Lindsey, R.E., Carlozzi, A.F., Eells, G.T., 2001. Differences in the
293–300. dispositional empathy of juvenile sex offenders, non-sex offending
Eisenberg, N., Miller, P.A., 1987. The relation of empathy to prosocial delinquent juveniles, and nondelinquent juveniles. Journal of
and related behaviors. Psychological Bulletin 101, 91–119. Interpersonal Violence 16, 510–522.
Eisenberg, N., Strayer, J., 1987. Empathy and its Development. Lisak, D., Ivan, C., 1995. Deficits in intimacy and empathy in sexually
Cambridge University Press, New York. aggressive men. Journal of Interpersonal Violence 10, 296–308.
262 I. Gery et al. / Psychiatry Research 165 (2009) 252–262

Lovett, B.J., Sheffield, R.A., 2007. Affective empathy deficits in Pollak, S.D., Kistler, D.J., 2002. Early experience is associated with the
aggressive children and adolescents: a critical review. Clinical development of categorical representations for facial expressions of
Psychology Review 27, 1–13. emotion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
Marshall, W.L., Barbaree, H.E., 1990. An integrated theory of sexual United States of America 99, 9072–9076.
offending. In: Marshall, W.L., Laws, D.R., Barbaree, H.E. (Eds.), Preston, S.D., de Waal, B.M., 2002. Empathy: its ultimate and
Handbook of Sexual Assault: Issues, Theories, and Treatment of proximate bases. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25, 1–72.
the Offender. Plenum Press, New York, pp. 257–275. Rice, M.E., Chaplin, T.C., Harris, G.T., Coutts, J., 1994. Empathy for
Marshall, W.L., Moulden, H., 2001. Hostility toward women and the victim and sexual arousal among rapists and nonrapists. Journal
victim empathy in rapists. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research of Interpersonal Violence 9, 435–449.
and Treatment 13, 249–255. Spielberger, C.D., 1983. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory
Marshall, W.L., Hudson, S.M., Jones, R., Fernandez, Y.M., 1995. Em- (Form Y) ‘‘Self-Evaluation Questionnaire’’. Consulting Psychol-
pathy in sex offenders. Clinical Psychology Review 15, 99–113. ogists Press, Palo Alto, CA.
Mehrabian, A., Epstein, N., 1972. A measure of emotional empathy. Stanislaw, H., Todorov, N., 1999. Calculation of signal detection
Journal of Personality 40, 525–543. theory measures. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and
Miller, P.A., Eisenberg, N., 1988. The relation of empathy to aggressive Computers 31, 137–149.
and externalizing/antisocial behavior. Psychological Bulletin 103, Surguladze, S.A., Young, A.W., Senior, C., Brebion, G., Travis, M.J.,
324–344. Phillips, M.L., 2004. Recognition accuracy and response bias to
Montagne, B., Schutters, S., Westenberg, H.G.M., van-Honk, J., Kessels, happy and sad facial expressions in patients with major depression.
R.P.C., de-Haan, E.H., 2006. Reduced sensitivity in the recognition Neuropsychology 18, 212–218.
of anger and disgust in social anxiety disorder. Cognitive Neuro- Wicker, B., Keysers, Ch., Plailly, J., Royet, J.-P., Gallese, V., Rizzolatti,
psychiatry 11, 389–401. G., 2003. Both of us disgusted in my insula: the common neural
Pithers, W.D., 1999. Empathy: Definition, enhancement, and rele- basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron 40, 655–664.
vance to the treatment of sexual abusers. Journal of Interpersonal
Violence 14, 257–284.

Você também pode gostar