Você está na página 1de 759

Neuropsychological Assessment of

Neuropsychiatric and Neuromedical Disorders


This page intentionally left blank
Neuropsychological
Assessment of
Neuropsychiatric and
Neuromedical Disorders
Third Edition

Edited by

Igor Grant, M.D., F.R.C.P.(C)


Distinguished Professor and Executive Vice Chair
Department of Psychiatry
Director, HIV Neurobehavioral Research Center
University of California, San Diego School of Medicine
La Jolla, California

Kenneth M. Adams, Ph.D., A.B.P.P


Professor of Psychology and Psychiatry
The University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
Associate Chief, Mental Health and Chief Psychologist
Veterans Affairs Ann Arbor Healthcare System
Ann Arbor, Michigan

1
2009
1
Oxford University Press, Inc., publishes works that further
Oxford University’s objective of excellence
in research, scholarship, and education.

Oxford New York


Auckland Cape Town Dar es Salaam Hong Kong Karachi
Kuala Lumpur Madrid Melbourne Mexico City Nairobi
New Delhi Shanghai Taipei Toronto
With offices in
Argentina Austria Brazil Chile Czech Republic France Greece
Guatemala Hungary Italy Japan Poland Portugal Singapore
South Korea Switzerland Thailand Turkey Ukraine Vietnam

Copyright © 2009 by Oxford University Press, Inc.

Published by Oxford University Press, Inc.


198 Madison Avenue, New York, New York 10016
www.oup.com

Oxford is a registered trademark of Oxford University Press.

All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced,


stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means,
electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, or otherwise,
without the prior permission of Oxford University Press.

The first and second editions were entitled


Neuropsychological Assessment of Neuropsychiatric Disorders.

Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data


Neuropsychological assessment of neuropsychiatric and neuromedical
disorders / edited by Igor Grant, Kenneth M. Adams.—3rd ed.
p. ; cm.
Includes bibliographical references and index.
ISBN 978-0-19-537854-2
1. Mental illness—Diagnosis. 2. Nervous system—Diseases—Diagnosis.
3. Neuropsychological tests. I. Grant, Igor, 1942- II. Adams, Kenneth M., 1948-
[DNLM: 1. Central Nervous System Diseases—diagnosis. 2. Delirium, Dementia,
Amnestic, Cognitive Disorders—diagnosis. 3. Neuropsychological Tests.
WM 141 N494 2009]
RC473.N48N47 2009
616.89'075—dc22
2008034750

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
Printed in the United States of America
on acid-free paper
To JoAnn Nallinger Grant
and
To the memory of Carol Bracher Adams
This page intentionally left blank
Contents

List of Color Illustrations ix


Preface xi
Contributors xiii

I. Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological


Assessment
1 The Halstead–Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery for Adults—Theoretical,
Methodological, and Validational Bases 3
Ralph M. Reitan and Deborah Wolfson
2. The Analytical Approach to Neuropsychological Assessment 25
Pat McKenna and Elizabeth K. Warrington
3. The Boston Process Approach to Neuropsychological Assessment 42
William P. Milberg, Nancy Hebben, and Edith Kaplan
4. The Iowa-Benton School of Neuropsychological Assessment 66
Daniel Tranel
5. Computer-Based Cognitive Testing 84
Thomas H. Crook, Gary G. Kay, and Glenn J. Larrabee
6. Cognitive Screening Methods 101
Maura Mitrushina
7. Demographic Influences and Use of Demographically Corrected Norms
in Neuropsychological Assessment 127
Robert K. Heaton, Lee Ryan, and Igor Grant

II. Neuropsychiatric Disorders


8. The Neuropsychology of Dementia 159
Mark W. Bondi, David P. Salmon, and Alfred W. Kaszniak
9. Neuropsychological Aspects of Parkinson’s Disease and Parkinsonism 199
Susan McPherson and Jeff rey Cummings
10. Huntington’s Disease 223
Jason Brandt
viii Contents

11. The Neuropsychiatry and Neuropsychology of Gilles de la Tourette Syndrome 241


Mary M. Robertson and Andrea E. Cavanna
12. The Neuropsychology of Epilepsy 267
Henry A. Buchtel and Linda M. Selwa
13. The Neuropsychology of Multiple Sclerosis 280
Allen E. Thornton and Vanessa G. DeFreitas
14. Cerebrovascular Disease 306
Gregory G. Brown, Ronald M. Lazar, and Lisa Delano-Wood
15. Neuropsychological Effects of Hypoxia in Medical Disorders 336
Amanda Schurle Bruce, Mark S. Aloia, and Sonia Ancoli-Israel
16. Diabetes and the Brain: Cognitive Performance in Type 1 and Type 2 Diabetes 350
Augustina M. A. Brands and Roy P. C. Kessels
17. Neuropsychological Aspects of HIV Infection 366
Steven Paul Woods, Catherine L. Carey, Jennifer E. Iudicello, Scott L. Letendre, Christine
Fennema-Notestine, and Igor Grant
18. The Neurobehavioral Correlates of Alcoholism 398
Sean B. Rourke and Igor Grant
19. Neuropsychological Consequences of Drug Abuse 455
Raul Gonzalez, Jasmin Vassileva, and J. Cobb Scott
20. Neuropsychological, Neurological, and Neuropsychiatric Correlates
of Exposure to Metals 480
Roberta F. White and Patricia A. Janulewicz
21. Clinical Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia 507
Philip D. Harvey and Richard S. E. Keefe
22. Neuropsychology of Depression and Related Mood Disorders 523
Scott A. Langenecker, H. Jin Lee, and Linas A. Bieliauskas
23. The Neuropsychology of Memory Dysfunction and Its Assessment 560
David P. Salmon and Larry R. Squire

III. Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological


Impairment
24. Neurobehavioral Consequences of Traumatic Brain Injury 597
Sureyya Dikmen, Joan Machamer, and Nancy Temkin
25. Neuropsychiatric, Psychiatric, and Behavioral Disorders Associated
with Traumatic Brain Injury 618
George P. Prigatano and Franziska Maier
26. Neuropsychology in Relation to Everyday Functioning 632
Erin E. Morgan and Robert K. Heaton
27. Neuropsychological Performance and the Assessment of Driving Behavior 652
Thomas D. Marcotte and J. Cobb Scott
28. Neuropsychological Function and Adherence to Medical Treatments 688
Steven A. Castellon, Charles H. Hinkin, Matthew J. Wright, and Terry R. Barclay

Author Index 713


Subject Index 731
List of Color Illustrations

8–1 The neuropathology of Alzheimer’s disease. Grossly apparent cortical atrophy in Alzheimer’s
disease (Figure 1A) compared to normal aging (Figure 1B), and neocortical amyloid plaques (Figure 2),
neurofibrillary tangles (Figure 3), and cerebrovascular amyloid angiopathy (Figure 4).
8–2 Histopathologic abnormalities in the limbic system and neocortex in Dementia with Lewy
Bodies (DLB). The typical appearance of vacuolization in the entorhinal cortex, cortical Lewy
bodies in the temporal lobe neocortex, and Lewy neurites (i.e., neurons containing abnormal alpha-
synuclein fi laments) in the CA3 region of the hippocampus.
12–1 Ictal SPECT scan of a 19-year-old patient with unusual episodes of fidgeting, mumbling, and
rubbing her legs. She has bilateral epileptiform activity, but the SPECT scan shows clear right tem-
poral hyperperfusion during a seizure (radiological convention: right is on the left).

13–1 Axial MRI scans of a 42-year-old female with relapsing—remitting MS and an Expanded
Disability Status Scale of 3.5. The patient’s disease duration is 11 years and she is not being man-
aged with any disease modifying therapy. From left to right: T2-weighted, FLAIR and T1-weighted
post-gadolinium contrast images. Periventricular hyperintense lesions and frontoparietal lesions are
noted on the T2 and FLAIR images; a gadolinium-enhancing lesion is also apparent.

17–1 Structural morphometry in an individual infected with HIV. These two coronal sections high-
light regions of abnormality in the white matter (shown in yellow), which may be related to markers
of HIV disease and neurobehavioral performance. Dark blue = cortex, light blue = subcortical gray,
purple = sulcal/subarachnoid CSF, red = ventricular fluid, yellow = abnormal white matter, dark
gray = normal appearing white matter, light gray = cerebellum, maroon = infratentorial CSF.
17–2 Diff usion tensor images from Gongvatana et al. (2008) showing that individuals with HIV-
associated neurocognitive disorders (HAND) have lower fractional anisotropy in the anterior callosal
region (shown in blue) relative to HIV-infected comparison participants without HAND. Images are
presented in axial sections moving from inferior (upper left) to superior (lower right) slices.

18–2 Brain MRI showing loss of brain volume in alcoholic (A) versus healthy control (B). Note
reduced cortical thickness and increased sulcal volume.

18–4 Cartoon showing some typical facial features in child with fetal alcohol syndrome.
18–5 One anomaly seen in FAS is agenesis, or absence, of the corpus callosum. The top left MRI
scan (A), is a control brain. The other images are from children with FAS. In the top middle the corpus
x List of Color Illustrations

callosum is present, but it is very thin at the posterior section of the brain (B, arrow). In the upper right
the corpus callosum is essentially missing (C, arrow). The bottom two pictures (D, E) are from a 9-year
old girl with FAS. She has agenesis of the corpus callosum and the large dark area in the back of her
brain (E, arrow) above the cerebellum is a condition known as colpocephaly. It is essentially empty
space.
18–6 MRI brain images of two cases. Case 1 was abstinent 1 week (A) and rescanned after 8 months
of continued abstinence (B). Note lessening prominence of suci and ventricles in B. Case 2 was absti-
nent 30 days (C) and rescanned at 10 months after relapsing in the interim. Note tissue loss in D,
particularly in periventricular white matter, cerebellar vermis, and surrounding IV ventricle.
18–7 Color coded images representing intensity of uptake of the tracer HMPAO during a cognitive
activation task in a nonalcoholic control (A), alcoholic abstinent over 18 months (B), and a recently
detoxified alcoholic (C) abstinent 4 weeks. Cooler colors indicate less perfusion, especially in frontal
areas in C versus A. Case B has values intermediate between A and C, suggesting recovery.
19–1 Cocaine abusers tend to show decreased glucose metabolism in areas of prefrontal
cortex (A) relative to healthy controls.
19–2 Decreased rCBF in putamen (A) and frontal (B) white matter of a methamphetamine user
compared to a healthy control. Increased rCBF in a methamphetamine user compared to a healthy
control in parietal brain regions (C).
20–1 Activation during a Task of Photic Stimulation. (a) The figure shows activation for the high
exposure group during a task of photic stimulation predominantly in the primary occipital cortex
bilaterally (Brodmann’s area 17). (b) The low exposure group shows bilateral activation primarily in
the occipital association cortex (Brodmann’s areas 18 and 19). (c) When the two groups are compared
there is greater activation in the primary occipital cortex in the high exposure group than the low
exposure group, representing recruitment of different neuronal resources in the two groups.
22–1 Frequent Regions of Interest Reported in Structural and Imaging Studies Relevant to
Understanding Depression and Related Psychiatric Disorders. Numbers indicate center of foci,
although some foci are collapsed across the left-right axis to reduce the number of images necessary
to display these foci.
22–2 Activation to emotionally salient stimuli for those with Major Depressive Disorder (MDD,
n = 13) compared to control (CON, n = 15) participants. Mean Hamilton Depression Rating Scale-17
Item score for the MDD group was 19.2 (SD = 7.6). The Emotion Word Stimulus Test is nine blocks
each of positive (Pos), Negative (Neg) and Neutral (Neut) words (from the Affective Norms for
Emotional Words set; Bradley and Lang, 1999) presented to participants during 3 Tesla functional
MRI (GE Scanner). Images were analyzed with Statistical Parametric Mapping 2 (SPM2; Friston,
1996, thresholds p<.001, minimum cluster size = 120 mm3) and Region of Interest (ROI) posthoc
analyses were conducted using the MARSBAR tool from SPM2 (Brett et al., 2002). Panels A–F (radio-
logical orientation) represent group differences with red indicating CON>MDD and blue indicat-
ing MDD>CON. Panels A–C illustrate group comparisons for Neg-Neut and panels D–F represent
contrasts for Pos-Neut. There was an area of increased activation for both contrasts, MDD>CON
in right middle frontal gyrus (MFG—Brodmann area 9/46, Panels C, F) that was further explored
in posthoc analyses. Panel G illustrates the spherical ROI created in the right MFG (in green). An
identical spherical ROI was created in the left MFG to test the laterality theory of MDD. The theory
states that there will be increased activation in right MFG for Neg-Neut and decreased activation in
left MFG for Pos-Neut in MDD compared to CON (Davidson, 2002). This theory is not supported
(Panel H bars) with increased right MFG activation for both emotion contrasts (MDD>CON: left
bar set = CON; right bar set = MDD). Left MFG was not different between groups in any contrast.
We interpret these findings as increased emotion regulatory demand for emotional words in MDD
irrespective of Pos or Neg valence.
Preface

The first edition of this book, published in 1986, Neuropsychological Society (INS) had jumped
attempted to capture in one book the dynamic to about 2000 members. By 2008, the member-
developments in neuropsychology with a fun- ship of the INS stood at well over 4000 mem-
damental focus on methodological approaches bers. Many other organizations of various
and clinical applications, particularly to neuro- disciplinary or scientific focus have grown too.
psychiatric conditions. The first edition had two These organizations all share a common interest
sections, one devoted to methods of compre- in neuropsychological research, education, and
hensive neuropsychological assessment and the practice. This expansion of neuropsychological
second to neuropsychiatric disorders. talent has been accompanied by an explosive
On the basis of the positive responses to this growth in the neuropsychological literature.
work, the second edition, published in 1996, fol- Indeed, research in neuropsychology, which
lowed in broad scope the general plan envisioned was at one time concentrated on mapping the
in the first edition. Two important changes were cognitive sequelae of brain injuries and atten-
made, however. The first had to do with adding dant “brain–behavior relationships,” has now
a third section on functional consequences of expanded to much broader considerations of
neuropsychological impairment. This addition the effects of systemic disease, infection, medi-
recognized the growing interest in the clinical cations, and inflammatory processes on neu-
community of understanding how neuropsy- rocognition and emotion. The third edition of
chological deficits impacted life quality and this work attempts to coalesce these currents
everyday functioning. The second edition also while continuing to adhere to the objective of
added new chapters to reflect areas of signifi- presenting them in a concise manner in a single
cant and increasing interest to neuropsychology book.
such as Huntington’s disease, hypoxemia, and The third edition is a thoroughly revised and
HIV infection. updated work. It contains seven entirely new
This third edition once again follows the chapters on new topics that were not in the
general plan of the 1996 work, organizing our first two editions, including consideration of
information in terms of methods, disorders, common sources of neurocognitive morbidity
and psychosocial consequences. The book also such as multiple sclerosis, diabetes, and expo-
hopefully reflects the enormous developments sure to heavy metals. There are also new chap-
in neuropsychology in terms of research, clin- ters in an expanded Part III, which deals with
ical applications, and growth of new talent psychosocial consequences of neuropsycho-
that has occurred in the past decade. As an logical impairment. These include chapters on
example, in the preface to the first edition we psychiatric and behavioral disorders associated
noted that societies such as the International with traumatic brain injury, neuropsychology
xii Preface

in relation to everyday functioning, the effects in medical specialties and problems of a very
of cognitive impairment on driving skills, and wide variety.
adherence to medical treatments. In bringing forward this third edition we wish
There are also five chapters covering content to thank all the many contributors and support-
that was in the second edition, but which repre- ers who have taken time out of their busy sched-
sents completely new work by new contributors. ules to provide major updates of their work, or
Included are new chapters on the neuropsychol- to provide entirely new chapters. We are deeply
ogy of epilepsy, hypoxia including sleep apnea, indebted also to Ms. Felicia Roston, Igor Grant’s
drug abuse, schizophrenia, and depression. Executive Assistant, for helping us in the many
As a consequence, approximately 50% of the phases of the development of the third edition,
third edition represents entirely new work. The including facilitating author communication,
remaining 50% consists of chapters by contrib- compilation of the works, editorial assistance,
utors to the second edition who have all updated and coordination with our publisher, Oxford
their work. We are also pleased that the third University Press. On the side of the publisher,
edition has attracted 34 new authors and co- we are most grateful to Ms. Shelley Reinhardt
authors, which underscore our commitment to for her strong encouragement and support of
bringing fresh points of view while maintaining the development of the third edition. Her en-
overall thematic continuity. thusiasm for this process was essential in bring-
Finally, readers may note a small change in ing this project to fruition.
the title of book where we have added “neuro-
medical” to be seen alongside our original “neu-
Igor Grant, M.D. La Jolla, CA
ropsychiatric” focus of our book. This change is
Kenneth M. Adams, Ph.D. Ann Arbor, MI
intended to capture the increasing presence and
importance of neuropsychological assessment
Contributors

Kenneth M. Adams, Ph.D., A.B.P.P. Mark W. Bondi, Ph.D.


Professor of Psychology and Psychiatry, Professor, Department of Psychiatry,
The University of Michigan, University of California, San Diego School of
Associate Chief, Mental Health and Chief Medicine, La Jolla
Psychologist, VA Ann Arbor Healthcare
System, Ann Arbor, Michigan Augustina M. A. Brands, Ph.D.
Senior Lecturer, Department of Experimental
Mark S. Aloia, Ph.D. Psychology, Utrecht University,
Associate Professor, Neuropsychologist, Neuropsychology Regional
Department of Medicine, Psychiatric Center, Zuwe Hofpoort Hospital,
Director of Sleep Research, Woerden, The Netherlands
National Jewish Health;
Denver, Colorado Jason Brandt, Ph.D.
Professor and Director, Division of Medical
Sonia Ancoli-Israel, Ph.D. Psychology, Departments of Psychiatry and
Professor, Department of Psychiatry, Behavioral Sciences and Neurology,
University of California, The John Hopkins University School of
San Diego School of Medicine, Medicine, Affi liate Staff, Psychiatry and
La Jolla Behavioral Sciences, The John Hopkins
Terry R. Barclay, Ph.D. Hospital, Baltimore, Maryland
Clinical Neuropsychologist,
Gregory G. Brown, Ph.D.
Department of Psychology,
Professor, Department of Psychiatry,
Neurovascular Institute and Health
University of California, San Diego School of
East Hospitals, St. Paul, Minnesota
Medicine, La Jolla, Associate Director, VISN 22
Linas A. Bieliauskas, Ph.D. MIRECC, Psychology Service, VA San Diego
Associate Professor, Neuropsychology Section, Healthcare System
Department of Psychiatry,
The University of Michigan, Amanda Schurle Bruce, Ph.D.
Staff Psychologist, Mental Health, Postdoctoral Fellow, Hoglund Brain Imaging
VA Ann Arbor Healthcare System, Center, University of Kansas School of
Ann Arbor, Michigan Medicine, Kansas City
xiv Contributors

Henry A. Buchtel, Ph.D. Sureyya Dikmen, Ph.D.


Associate Professor of Psychology, Professor, Department of Rehabilitation
Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, Medicine, University of Washington School of
The University of Michigan, Staff Psychologist, Medicine, Seattle
Neuropsychology Section, Mental Health
Christine Fennema-Notestine, Ph.D.
Service, VA Ann Arbor Healthcare System
Assistant Adjunct Professor, Department
Ann Arbor, Michigan
of Psychiatry and Radiology, University of
Catherine L. Carey, Ph.D. California, San Diego School of Medicine,
Postdoctoral Scholar, Department of La Jolla
Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego
Raul Gonzalez, Ph.D.
School of Medicine, La Jolla
Assistant Professor of Psychology in
Steven A. Castellon, Ph.D. Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry,
Associate Research Psychologist, Department University of Illinois College of Medicine,
of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, Chicago
David Geffen School of Medicine at the,
Igor Grant, M.D., F.R.C.P.(C)
University of California, Los Angeles, Director,
Distinguished Professor and Executive
Postdoctoral Training in Psychology, Mental
Vice-Chair, Department of Psychiatry;
Health, and Psychiatry, VA Greater Los
Director, HIV Neurobehavioral Research
Angeles Healthcare System
Center, University of California, San Diego
Andrea E. Cavanna, M.D. School of Medicine, La Jolla, California
Honorary Research Fellow
Philip D. Harvey, Ph.D.
Sobell Department of Motor Neuroscience and
Professor, Department of Psychiatry, Emory
Movement Disorders, Institute of Neurology,
University School of Medicine, Atlanta,
London, Consultant in Behavioral Neurology,
Georgia
Department of Neuropsychiatry, Birmingham
and Solihull Mental Health NHS Foundation Robert K. Heaton, Ph.D., ABPP-CN
Trust, Birmingham, United Kingdom Professor, Department of Psychiatry,
University of California, San Diego School of
Thomas H. Crook, Ph.D.
Medicine, La Jolla
Affi liate Professor, Department of Psychiatry,
and Behavioral Medicine, University of Nancy Hebben, Ph.D.
South Florida College of Medicine, Tampa, Clinical Professor in Psychology, Department
Chief Executive Officer, Cognitive Research, of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School, Boston,
Corporation, St. Petersburg, Florida Clinical Associate, Department of Psychology,
McLean Hospital, Belmont,
Jeffrey Cummings, M.D.
Massachusetts
August Rose Professor and Director,
Mary S. Easton Center for Alzheimer’s Disease Charles H. Hinkin, Ph.D., ABPP-CN
Research, Department of Neurology, David Professor In Residence, Department of
Geffen School of Medicine at the University of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences,
California, Los Angeles David Geffen School of Medicine at the
University of California, Los Angeles, Director,
Vanessa G. DeFreitas, M.A. Neuropsychology Assessment Laboratory,
Candidate for Doctor of Philosophy degree, Mental Health and Psychiatry, VA Greater Los
Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, British Angeles Healthcare System
Columbia, Canada
Jennifer E. Iudicello, B.A.
Lisa Delano-Wood, Ph.D. Doctoral Candidate, Departments of
Assistant Professor, Department of Psychiatry, Psychology and Psychiatry, San Diego State
University of California, San Diego School of University and University California,
Medicine, La Jolla, Research Psychologist, VA San Diego Joint Doctoral Program in Clinical
San Diego Healthcare System Psychology
Contributors xv

Patricia A. Janulewicz, M.P.H., D.Sc. Columbia University Medical Center,


Clinical Project Coordinator, Epidemiology New York, New York
Department, Boston University School of
H. Jin Lee, Ph.D.
Public Health, Boston, Massachusetts
Adjunct Research Investigator,
Edith Kaplan, Ph.D. Neuropsychology Section, Department of
Associate Professor of Psychiatry and Psychiatry, The University of Michigan,
Neurology, Boston University School of Ann Arbor
Medicine, Boston, Massachusetts, Affi liate Scott L. Letendre, M.D.
Professor, Department of Psychology, Clark Associate Professor in Residence, Department
University, Worcester, Massachusetts, of Medicine, University of California,
Consulting Neuropsychologist, Department of San Diego School of Medicine, La Jolla
Psychology, Baycrest Center for Geriatric Care,
North York, Ontario, Canada Joan Machamer, M.A.
Department of Rehabilitation Medicine,
Alfred W. Kaszniak, Ph.D. University of Washington School of Medicine,
Professor and Head, Department of Seattle
Psychology, University of Arizona, Tucson
Thomas D. Marcotte, Ph.D.
Gary G. Kay, Ph.D. Associate Professor, Department of Psychiatry,
Associate Professor, Department of Neurology, University of California, San Diego School of
Georgetown University School of Medicine, Medicine, La Jolla
Washington, DC
Franziska Maier, M.S.
Richard S. E. Keefe, Ph.D. Department of Neurology, University Hospital
Professor of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences Cologne, Cologne, Germany
and Psychology and Neuroscience, Departments Pat McKenna, Ph.D.
of Psychiatry and Psychology, Duke University Consultant Clinical Neuropsychologist,
School of Medicine, Durham, North Carolina Rookwood Hospital, Cardiff, Wales, United
Roy P. C. Kessels, Ph.D. Kingdom
Professor of Neuropsychology, Donders Susan McPherson, Ph.D.
Institute for the Brain, Cognition, and Associate Clinical Professor of Neurology,
Behavior, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Department of Neurology, University of
Netherlands; Clinical Neuropsychologist, Minnesota School of Medicine, Minneapolis
Departments of Medical Psychology and
Geriatric Medicine, Radboud University William P. Milberg, Ph.D.
Nijmegen Medical Centre, Nijmegen, Associate Professor of Psychology, Department
The Netherlands of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School;
Director, Geriatric Neuropsychology
Scott A. Langenecker, Ph.D. Laboratory, Geriatric Research, Education,
Assistant Professor, Department of Psychiatry, and Clinical Center, VA Boston Healthcare,
The University of Michigan, Ann Arbor Jamaica Plain, Boston, Massachusetts
Glenn J. Larrabee, Ph.D., ABPP-CN Maura Mitrushina, Ph.D.
Independent practice, Sarasota, Florida Professor, Department of Psychology,
California State University, Northridge;
Ronald M. Lazar, Ph.D., PAHA
Associate Clinical Professor, Psychiatry and
Professor of Clinical Neuropsychology
Biobehavioral Sciences, Semel Institute for
in Neurology and Neurological Surgery,
Neuroscience and Human Behavior,
Department of Neurology, Columbia,
University of California, Los Angeles
University College of Physicians and Surgeons;
Attending Neuropsychologist, Division of Erin E. Morgan, M.S.
Stroke and Critical Care, Department of Graduate Student Researcher, Departments of
Neurology, New York Presbyterian Hospital at Psychology and Psychiatry, San Diego
xvi Contributors

State University and University California, La Jolla; Research Career Scientist,


San Diego Joint Doctoral Program in Clinical VA San Diego Healthcare System
Psychology
Nancy Temkin, Ph.D.
George P. Prigatano, Ph.D. Professor, Department of Neurological Surgery,
Newsome Chair, Clinical Neuropsychology, University of Washington School of Medicine,
Barrow Neurological Institute, Phoenix, and Department of Biostatistics, University
Arizona of Washington School of Public Health and
Community Medicine, Seattle
Ralph M. Reitan, Ph.D.
Reitan Neuropsychology Laboratory, Allen E. Thornton, Ph.D.
South Tucson, Arizona Associate Professor, Department of Psychology,
Simon Fraser University, Burnaby,
Mary M. Robertson, M.B.Ch.B., M.D., D.Sc.
British Columbia, Canada
(Med), D.P.M., F.R.C.P. (UK), F.R.C.P.C.H.,
F.R.C.Psych. Daniel Tranel, Ph.D.
Emeritus Professor in Neuropsychiatry, Professor, Departments of Neurology and
University College London; Visiting Professor Psychology, University of Iowa; Chief, Benton
and Honorary Consultant, St George’s Hospital Neuropsychology Laboratory, University of
and Medical School, London, United Kingdom Iowa Hospitals & Clinics, Iowa City
Sean B. Rourke, Ph.D. Jasmin Vassileva, Ph.D.
Associate Professor, Department of Psychiatry, Research Assistant Professor, Department of
University of Toronto; Director of Research, Psychiatry, University of Illinois College of
Mental Health Service, and Scientist, Medicine, Chicago
St. Michael’s Hospital, Toronto, Ontario,
Elizabeth K. Warrington, B.Sc., D.Sc.
Canada
Professor, Dementia Research Centre, Institute
Lee Ryan, Ph.D. of Neurology, London, United Kingdom
Associate Professor, Department of Psychology,
Roberta F. White, Ph.D.
University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona
Professor and Chair, Associate Dean for
David P. Salmon, Ph.D. Research, Department of Environmental
Professor in Residence, Department of Health, Boston University School of Public
Neuroscience, University of California, Health; Attending Neuropsychologist,
San Diego School of Medicine, La Jolla Department of Neurology, Boston University
School of Medicine, Boston, Massachusetts
J. Cobb Scott, M.S.
Graduate Student Researcher, Departments Deborah Wolfson, Ph.D.
of Psychology and Psychiatry, San Diego State Reitan Neuropsychology Laboratory,
University and University of California, San South Tucson, Arizona
Diego, Joint Doctoral Program in Clinical
Steven Paul Woods, Psy.D.
Psychology
Assistant Professor, Department of Psychiatry,
Linda M. Selwa, M.D. University of California, San Diego School of
Professor, Department of Neurology, Medicine, La Jolla
The University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
Matthew J. Wright, Ph.D.
Larry R. Squire, Ph.D. Director of Neuropsychology, Psychology
Professor, Departments of Psychiatry, Division, Department of Psychiatry,
Neuroscience, and Psychology, University of Harbor/University of California, Los Angeles
California, San Diego School of Medicine, Medical Center
Part I

Methods of Comprehensive
Neuropsychological Assessment
This page intentionally left blank
1

The Halstead–Reitan Neuropsychological


Test Battery for Adults—Theoretical,
Methodological, and Validational Bases
Ralph M. Reitan and Deborah Wolfson

The development of the Halstead–Reitan Neu- the circumstances that they had observed, and
ropsychological Test Battery (HRB) began in in reaching meaningful conclusions about the
1935 when Ward C. Halstead founded, at the situations they faced in everyday life.
University of Chicago, the first full-time lab- The initial orientation and approach to a
oratory for studying brain–behavior relation- research program may have long-term impli-
ships. Halstead used a model based on inferring cations. For example, Binet and Simon (1916)
behavioral functions from observations and began developing intelligence tests using aca-
test results representing differences between demic competence as the criterion. IQ tests,
persons with known brain lesions and non– though used to assess cerebral damage, are
brain-damaged controls. At this time there probably still best known for their relationship
were essentially only individual tests designed to school success. It was important and for-
to detect “brain damage” rather than batteries tuitous that Halstead decided to observe the
developed to provide comprehensive assess- adaptive processes and difficulties that brain-
ment of the individual person. Thus, Halstead’s damaged persons demonstrated in everyday
first step in evaluating brain-damaged patients life. If he had focused only on academic achieve-
was to observe them in their everyday living sit- ment or classroom activities, it is probable that
uations and to attempt to discern which aspects the tests he eventually developed would have
of their behavior were different from the behav- been quite different. The tests that were finally
ior of normal individuals. His observations of included in the HRB emanated from a consid-
persons with cerebral lesions made it apparent eration of neuropsychological impairment and,
at the very beginning that brain-damaged indi- as a result, have much more general relevance
viduals had a wide range of deficits and that a than IQ measures to practical aspects of reha-
single test would not be able to adequately iden- bilitation and adaptive abilities.
tify and evaluate the nature and severity of their On the basis of these observations, Halstead
deficits. Some patients had specific language began to devise and experiment with a great
deficits representing dysphasia. Other individu- number of psychological testing procedures.
als were confused in a more general yet perva- The design of the resulting procedures placed
sive way (even though in casual contact they some of them more in the context of standard-
often appeared to be quite intact). ized experiments than conventional psycho-
As Halstead studied brain-damaged persons metric tests.
in their typical everyday living situations, he The culmination of Halstead’s efforts resulted
observed that most of them seemed to have dif- in 10 tests that ultimately formed the principal
ficulties in understanding the essential nature basis for his concept of biological intelligence.
of complex problem situations, in analyzing Only 7 of the original 10 tests have withstood

3
4 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

the rigors of both clinical and experimental When Reitan started his laboratory at the
evaluation. Indiana University Medical Center, it soon
Halstead (1947) described his 10 tests and became clear that Halstead’s 10 tests represented
presented his studies of persons with and with- only a beginning toward effective and valid
out cerebral lesions in his book, Brain and clinical evaluation of the individual person.
Intelligence: A Quantitative Study of the Frontal The eventual development of the HRB was the
Lobes. Following this publication, his research product of a long-term experiment conceived
and writing largely turned to other aspects by Ralph Reitan and Robert F. Heimburger,
of brain–behavior relationships, with illness, MD, a neurological surgeon, and implemented
which led to his death in 1969, being the limit- over a course of about 15 years with the sup-
ing factor of his scientific efforts. port and participation of a number of neurop-
Halstead’s initial work constituted the first sychologists, neurologists, neurosurgeons, and
organized attempt to measure and under- neuropathologists. The plan was basically sim-
stand the complexities and diversification of ple: patients with definitively diagnosed brain
higher-level abilities of the human brain and, disease or damage would be referred for neu-
in this sense, can be thought of as represent- ropsychological testing; the testing would be
ing the beginning of the discipline of clinical done without knowledge of the patient’s history
neuropsychology. or diagnosis; a report would be written on the
basis of the test results alone; this report would
be compared with an independent summary of
Unique Features of the HRB
the medical and neurological findings, up to
The HRB has a number of features that are not and including autopsy when available (which
shared by any other batteries or approaches served as the criterion); and inaccuracies or lim-
to the assessment of brain impairment. These itations of the neuropsychological test results in
distinctive properties arise from the way the reflecting the brain disease or damage would
battery was developed, the research studies thereby become apparent.
that over the years provided extensive validity The neuropsychological testing began using
findings, and the widespread testing of clini- Halstead’s 10 tests. These tests usually per-
cal applicability in many different geograph- mitted a correct conclusion about whether
ical areas and through use by thousands of the patient was brain damaged or a control,
clinicians. but inferences about lateralization, chronic-
ity, recovery potential, type of lesion or dis-
ease, and so forth could not be done with any
The Process Followed in Developing
degree of accuracy. At this point, it was clear
the HRB
that additional tests were needed, as well as a
The development of many tests and approaches framework for these additional tests, includ-
in neuropsychology, such as those followed by ing the complementary use of various test-
Strauss and Werner (1941) in the early phases ing approaches (level of performance, specific
of child neuropsychology, focused on the deficits, right–left comparisons, and patterns
behavioral characteristics of persons presumed of test relationships). Tests were added experi-
to have damaged brains. In contrast, Halstead, mentally and, on a case-by-case basis, evaluated
working in conjunction with neurosurgeons, for their contribution to correct insights about
began his formal studies by examining per- the patient’s brain condition. Many of the tests
sons with known brain lesions. The focus was included on an experimental basis, when eval-
to develop an understanding of brain–behavior uated across many patients, did not contribute
relationships, which was possible only when significantly to the neuropsychological infer-
the condition (pathology) of an individual’s ences, and were discontinued (as were 3 of
brain was well described and the behavior was Halstead’s 10 tests). Other tests, however, pro-
examined and tested under well-defined and vided verified insights about involvement of one
controlled conditions. hemisphere or the other as well as about more
The Halstead–Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery for Adults 5

focal cerebral involvement, chronicity, and type presumed dimensions of brain functions and, if
of lesion or disease, and these tests were added they measure enough functions, to merit des-
as the battery was gradually expanded. This pro- ignation as being comprehensive, or, alterna-
cess required testing of many patients as well as tively, to base an evaluation on the client’s own
controls. The controls were primarily inpatients complaints or perceived cognitive deficits. This
and were not identified as controls at the time of latter approach is essentially a procedure that
testing, so in these cases the testing and report accepts the client’s self-diagnosis as a basis for
writing proceeded in exactly the same man- composing a test battery.
ner as with patients who had brain disease or
damage.
The Extensive Research Supporting
This process continued for about 15 years
the Validity of the HRB
and included thousands of cases. Over this
period tests were added, evaluated, and dis- The HRB has a very long paper trail consisting
carded or included, and we finally reached a of hundreds of published studies of its effec-
point at which the various neurological (med- tiveness. Dean (1985), in his review of the HRB,
ical) findings were usually correctly inferred noted that it is the “most researched” battery
from the independently collected neuropsycho- in the United States. Reitan and his colleagues
logical test results. Correct inferences covered alone have published more than 300 books,
the entire range of brain damage and disease, chapters, and research papers, mainly on the
including neoplasms, vascular disorders, trau- HRB. It is not possible to review all of these
matic injury, infectious diseases, demyelinating studies in the present context, but some studies
diseases, degenerative diseases, and toxic expo- can be mentioned briefly.
sure. The fact that the final battery, which was Our initial study (Reitan, 1955b), concerned
completed in the early 1960s, reflected the neu- with the sensitivity of tests included in the HRB,
ropsychological effects of this broad range of used results obtained on Halstead’s 10 tests
neurological conditions in such a large number to compare a group of 50 subjects with docu-
of patients lent confidence to our belief that the mented cerebral damage or disease and a group
test battery (which had become rather exten- of 50 controls who showed no past or present
sive) encompassed the broad range of abilities symptoms of cerebral disease or dysfunction.
subserved by the brain. Only after the battery A heterogeneous and diverse group of subjects
was complete were we in a position to review with cerebral damage was deliberately included
the tests that had emerged as overall brain to ensure that an extensive range of neurologi-
indicators and, on that basis, propose a neuro- cal conditions would be represented.
psychological model of brain functions (now The study also intentionally included a num-
referred to as the Reitan–Wolfson model). ber of controls with medical conditions other
Meier (1985) described the HRB as hav- than brain damage. Twenty-four percent of the
ing “a long and illustrious history of clinical control group was composed of normally func-
research and application in American clini- tioning individuals. The remaining 76% of the
cal neuropsychology,” and noted that it “has control group was composed of patients hos-
had perhaps the most widespread impact of pitalized for a variety of difficulties not involv-
any approach in clinical neuropsychology.” If ing impaired brain functions. A substantial
these words are correct, this outcome is due to number of paraplegic and neurotic patients
the rigorous implementation, over the years, of were included in this group in an attempt to
the grand experiment planned by Reitan and minimize the probability that any differences
Heimburger. between the groups could be attributed to vari-
It is interesting to note that the approach that ables such as hospitalization, chronic illness, or
laid the foundation for the development of the affective disturbances.
HRB is quite different from the current popu- The two groups were composed by matching
lar tendency in the field to base a neuropsycho- pairs of individuals on the basis of race and gen-
logical evaluation on tests selected to measure der and, as closely as possible, on chronological
6 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

age and years of formal education. The differ- Neuropsychological Deficit Scale (GNDS),
ence in mean age for the two groups was 0.06 which was based on a standard score from each
years, and the difference in mean education of 42 variables. Each standard score reflec-
was 0.02 years. The two groups were therefore ted the degree of normality or impairment on
closely matched for age and education, and the each test.
standard deviations for age and education in the Data analyses, based on comparisons of 169
two groups were nearly identical. persons with independent evidence of brain
Although the two groups should have pro- damage and 41 controls, identified a GNDS
duced essentially comparable results on the cutoff score of 25/26 that yielded a 90% accu-
basis of the controlled variables, the presence racy rate in identifying brain-damaged persons
of brain damage in one group was responsi- (sensitivity) and a 90% accuracy rate in iden-
ble for a striking difference in the test results. tifying non–brain-damaged (control) persons
Seven of the measures devised by Halstead (specificity). These sensitivity and specificity
showed differences between the mean scores findings clearly indicate the power of brain
for the two groups, with relation to variability damage as an independent variable in deter-
estimates, which achieved striking significance mining the GNDS scores. In fact, 85% of the
from a statistical point of view. In fact, accord- 169 brain-damaged persons earned GNDS
ing to the most detailed tables we have seen, the scores of 35 or more, and every control sub-
probability estimates not only exceeded the .01 ject earned a GNDS score of 34 or less. In clin-
or .001 level, but also exceeded 10 –12. However, ical practice, this finding often represents a
even these tables were inadequate to express relatively absolute cutoff for concluding that
the appropriate statistical probability level for brain damage is present in the individual case,
these seven tests. These seven measures were although evaluation of the results of the entire
the Category Test, the Tactual Performance Test HRB is obviously necessary to explicate the
(TPT; Total Time, Memory, and Localization nature and pattern of the individual’s neuro-
components), Finger Tapping–Preferred Hand, psychological impairment.
Speech-sounds Perception Test (SSPT), and the One of the most comprehensive studies of the
Rhythm Test. HRB (Wheeler et al., 1963) used discriminant
As noted in the preceding paragraph, statis- functions to predict whether subjects, on the basis
tical comparisons of the two groups reached of their test scores, fell in the category of presence
extreme probability levels on 7 of the 10 mea- of brain damage or that of absence of brain dam-
sures contributing to the Halstead Impairment age. Test data consisted of results for each subject
Index. The most striking intergroup differences on 11 Wechsler subtests and 13 scores from the
were shown by the Halstead Impairment Index HRB. In some instances, combinations of vari-
(even though 3 of the 10 measures contribut- ables were also evaluated (e.g., the 24-variable
ing to it were not particularly sensitive) and the discriminant function, the Impairment Index,
Category Test. Not a single brain-damaged sub- the sum of Verbal and Performance weighted
ject performed better than his or her matched scores from the Wechsler Scale, and other com-
control on the Impairment Index (although the binations of possible interest) as well as age and
Impairment Indices were equal in 6 of the 50 education. Comparisons were made between
matched pairs). The Category Test was the most 61 non–brain-damaged control subjects and 79
sensitive of any single measure to the effects of persons with unequivocal neurological, neuro-
cerebral damage. In three instances the subjects surgical, or autopsy evidence of cerebral disease
with brain lesions performed better than their or damage. These 79 persons with brain dam-
matched control subjects, but in the 47 remain- age were subdivided according to the location
ing pairs of subjects the controls performed of lesions (left hemisphere, N = 25; right hemi-
better on the Category Test. sphere, N = 31; and diffuse or bilateral involve-
Many years later, Reitan and Wolfson ment, N = 23). The principal comparisons were
(1988) did a similar study based on 42 vari- between the controls and each brain-damaged
ables that summarized results of the entire group as well as controls and the total group of
HRB. These variables comprised the General brain-damaged persons.
The Halstead–Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery for Adults 7

The discriminant function in each com-


The Value of Using Complementary
parison produced a single weighted score for
Approaches to Assessment of the
each subject, an optimum least squares type
Various Ways that Brain Damage
of separation between pairs of groups. The
Is Expressed
distributions of summed weighted scores in
each comparison of pairs of groups were then The HRB was deliberately designed to incorpo-
inspected to determine the point of least over- rate various methods or approaches to assess-
lap. The weighted score for each person, fall- ment that can be applied in a complementary
ing above or below this point of least overlap, manner in evaluation of the individual person.
determined whether the individual belonged The first of these methods concerned level of
to one group or the other in the pair of groups performance, or how well the subject performed
being compared. These assignments were then on each test. This is the method customarily
compared with the group to which the subjects used by neuropsychologists in producing data
actually belonged (as based on definitive neuro- either for clinical evaluation or for research anal-
logical diagnoses), and expressed as percentages yses. This method is based on tests that produce
of correct predictions. continuous distributions, which, of course, cover
The results of this study yielded valid- a range of variability in normal as well as brain-
ity fi ndings that have never been equaled by damaged samples. The distributions for these
any other neuropsychological test battery. two groups invariably overlap because level of
The Impairment Index, evaluated as a single performance is invariably influenced and deter-
score, was correct in about 9 of 10 cases. The mined by many factors. This overlap complicates
percentage of correct classifications by the the use of the method in attempting to differen-
Impairment Index in various pairs of groups tiate individual members of each group.
was as follows: controls versus the entire Another approach frequently used by neu-
group of brain-damaged subjects, 87.2%; con- ropsychologists compares the subject’s scores on
trols versus diff use brain damage, 94.5%; con- various tests and evaluates these relationships
trols versus left cerebral damage, 90.2%; and as possible indicators of impairment. Clinical
controls versus right cerebral damage, 87.9%. interpretation of the HRB is also facilitated by
Even higher accuracy rates were achieved with including tests of intelligence and academic
the 24-variable discriminant function. The achievement as well as measures of personality
percentage of correct classifications was as fol- traits or disturbances.
lows: controls versus all brain-damaged cases, The aforementioned methods are routinely
90.7%; controls versus diff use brain damage, used and have a long history. We realized early
98.8%; controls versus left cerebral damage, in our evaluation of persons with brain dam-
93.0%; controls versus right cerebral damage, age that additional approaches, though not
92.4%; and right cerebral damage versus left necessarily useful in every case, often provided
cerebral damage, 92.9%. These results demon- definitive evidence of brain damage, analogous
strate the sensitivity of the tests in the HRB to evidence obtained from specialized neuro-
as they relate differentially to scores produced logical tests such as computed tomography and
by normal controls versus brain-damaged magnetic resonance imaging of the brain. Not
groups. infrequently, neuropsychological tests based on
These publications dated back many years, the presence or absence of brain-related deficits
but neurological diagnostic methods at that produce unequivocal evidence of brain dys-
time were quite capable of identifying brain function that is of great specificity in identifying
disease and damage, and persons included in the examinee as brain damaged. In such cases,
the brain-damaged group had definitive diag- persons with brain damage manifest deficits
noses in every case. The controls had also been on simple tasks that are performed easily and
carefully examined by neurologists and neuro- adequately by persons without brain damage.
surgeons, and none had a history or examina- These tests are represented in the HRB by the
tion findings that suggested prior brain disease Reitan–Indiana Aphasia Test and the Reitan–
or damage. Kløve Sensory-Perceptual Test.
8 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

Investigation of such deficits in various test results were compared for the individual
groups of brain-damaged and control subjects subject, producing intraindividual scores that
has a long history in clinical use of the HRB, were then validated through correlation with
having shown that certain specific deficits, independent neurological findings. Currently,
while rarely judged to be present among non– these intraindividual difference scores can
brain-damaged controls, were not infrequently be converted into Neuropsychological Deficit
found among persons with brain disease or Scale scores that range through four categories
damage and, in addition, were sometimes quite from perfectly normal to clinically severe defi-
specific in their relationship to one cerebral cits (Reitan & Wolfson, 1993).
hemisphere or the other (Wheeler & Reitan,
1962).
The Theoretical Model and
It may be noted that these approaches to neu-
Content of the Halstead–Reitan
ropsychological assessment were derived from
Neuropsychological Test Battery
the field of neurology rather than psychology,
and this may be the basic reason that they have As noted previously in this chapter, the content
been relatively neglected by neuropsycholo- of the HRB was not derived from presumptions
gists. In fact, many medical procedures produce about what should be measured but, instead,
“presence” or “absence” (binary or dichoto- from a very practical consideration—what tests
mous) conclusions (such as computed tomog- were needed to differentiate persons with and
raphy and magnetic resonance imaging of the without independent neurological evidence of
brain) in contrast to the continuous distribu- brain disease or damage and to discern, from
tions produced by most psychological tests. the test results alone, the many additional
This difference in the two disciplines may stem, aspects of the patient’s condition (diff use ver-
in turn, from the need in medicine to identify sus focal disease; right versus left involvement;
categorically and accurately various diseases, anterior versus posterior involvement; acute
whereas in psychology the tradition in mea- versus chronic disorders; and finally, the specific
surement has been to rate the comparative level disease entity or type of damage). Our purpose
of an individual’s performance with relation to was to establish a firm, empirical relationship of
normative standards. In the very beginning we neuropsychological test results to the complete
recognized the potential for integrating these diagnosis derived from the history, neurological
two approaches in a complementary manner; examination, results of specialized neurologi-
the HRB therefore included methods derived cal tests, findings at surgery of the brain, and
both from neurology and psychology, integrat- finally, autopsy of the brain.
ing them as a single assessment procedure. The A basic presumption was that when we had
HRB is still the only neuropsychological testing developed a neuropsychological battery that
method to have been developed in this manner. permitted accurate predictions of the many
Another approach to assessment, derived aspects of brain disease and damage we would
from neurology but deliberately adapted for inevitably have included—measurement of the
inclusion in the HRB, was based on the anat- fundamental cognitive, intellectual, motor,
omy of the central nervous system and more and sensory-perceptual aspects of brain func-
specifically on the differential functional rela- tion. Many tests, including the Wechsler
tionship of each cerebral hemisphere to the Memory Scale (but not the Wechsler–Bellevue
contralateral side of the body. Many, if not Intelligence Scale), were found to add little (if
most, brain disorders affect one hemisphere any) uniqueness. This led to a conceptualization
to a greater extent than the other hemisphere. that memory, in a clinically meaningful sense
In turn, one side of the body is often more and distinct from immediate reproduction, was
impaired than the other side with regard to sen- a very complex function that was already repre-
sory-perceptual and/or motor functions. In the sented in tests of registration through the vari-
early stages of development of the HRB, tests ous avenues of sensory input, scanning against
were developed and included to assess these the body of knowledge already possessed by
lateralized deficits in a formal manner. These the brain, related to the principal specialized
The Halstead–Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery for Adults 9

functions of the cerebral hemispheres (language (see Reitan & Wolfson, 1993, for a description of
and speech in a broad sense and visual– the anatomical structures and systems for each
spatial and auditory–temporal abilities ranging of the elements of the Reitan–Wolfson model).
from simple registration and reproduction to Once sensory information reaches the brain,
immensely complex interactions) and, perhaps the first step in central processing is the “regis-
most important, to basic ability in abstraction, tration phase” and represents alertness, atten-
reasoning, establishing relationships, logical tion, continued concentration, and the ability
analysis, and so forth. All of these abilities, to screen incoming information in relation to
in the aggregate, contribute to and compose prior experiences (immediate, intermediate,
memory in its complete dimensions. and remote memory). When evaluating this
The long-term process of developing a set level of functioning, the neuropsychologist is
of tests that permitted prediction of the pres- concerned with answering questions such as
ence, nature, and extent of damage to the brain the following: How well can this individual
led to the development of a general theory of pay attention to a specified task? Can he or she
brain–behavior relationships, referred to as the utilize past experiences (memory) effectively
Reitan–Wolfson model of neuropsychological and efficiently to reach a reasonable solution
functioning (see Figure 1–1). to a problem? Can the person understand and
A neuropsychological response cycle first follow simple instructions?
requires input to the brain from the external If an individual’s brain is not capable of
environment via one or more of the sensory registering incoming information, relating the
avenues. Primary sensory areas are located in new information to past experiences (memory),
each cerebral hemisphere, indicating that this and establishing the relevance of the informa-
level of central processing is widely represented tion, the subject is almost certainly seriously
in the cerebral cortex and involves the tempo- impaired in everyday behavior. A person who
ral, parietal, and occipital areas particularly is not able to maintain alertness and a degree of
concentration is likely to make very little pro-
gress as he or she attempts to solve a problem.
Persons with such severe impairment have lim-
Output
ited opportunity to effectively utilize any of the
other higher-level abilities that the brain sub-
serves, and they tend to perform quite poorly
Concept Formation on almost any task presented to them.
Reasoning Because alertness and concentration are
Logical Analysis necessary for all aspects of problem solving, a
comprehensive neuropsychological test bat-
tery should include measures that evaluate the
subject’s attentiveness. Such tests should not be
Language Visuospatial complicated and difficult, but should require
Skills Skills the person to pay close attention over time to
specific stimulus material. The Halstead-Reitan
Battery evaluates this first level of central pro-
cessing primarily with two measures: the
Speech-sounds Perception Test (SSPT) and the
Attention, Concentration, Memory
Rhythm Test.
The SSPT consists of 60 spoken nonsense
words that are variants of the “ee” sound. The
stimuli are presented on a tape recording, and
Input the subject responds to each stimulus by under-
lining one of four alternatives printed on an
Figure 1–1. A graphic representation of the Reitan– answer sheet. The SSPT requires the subject
Wolfson model of neuropsychological functioning. to maintain attention through the 60 items,
10 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

perceive the spoken stimulus through hear- Screening Test (AST) is used to evaluate lan-
ing, and relate the perception through vision to guage functions such as naming common
the correct configuration of letters on the test objects, spelling simple words, reading, writing,
form. enunciating, identifying individual numbers
The Rhythm Test requires the subject to dif- and letters, and performing simple arithmetic
ferentiate between 30 pairs of rhythmic beats. computations.
The stimuli are presented by a standardized The AST is organized so that performances
tape recording. After listening to a pair of stim- are evaluated in terms of the particular sen-
uli, the subject writes “S” on the answer sheet sory modalities through which the stimuli
if he or she thinks the two stimuli sounded the are perceived. Additionally, the receptive and
same, and writes “D” if they sounded different. expressive components of the test allow the
The Rhythm Test requires alertness to non- neuropsychologist to judge whether the limit-
verbal auditory stimuli, sustained attention to ing deficit for a subject is principally receptive
the task, and ability to perceive and compare or expressive in character. The verbal subtests
different rhythmic sequences. Although many of the WAIS are also used to obtain information
psychologists have presumed that the Rhythm about verbal intelligence.
Test is dependent on the integrity of the right Right cerebral hemisphere functions are par-
hemisphere (because the content is nonverbal), ticularly involved with spatial abilities (medi-
the test is actually an indicator of generalized ated principally by vision but also by touch and
cerebral functions (apparently because of the auditory function) and spatial and manipula-
requirement of attention and concentration) tory skills (Reitan, 1955a; Wheeler & Reitan,
and has no lateralizing significance (Reitan & 1962). It is again important to remember that
Wolfson, 1989). an individual may be impaired in the expres-
After an initial registration of incoming sive aspects or the receptive aspects of visual–
material, the brain customarily proceeds to spatial functioning, or both. It must also be kept
process verbal information in the left cerebral in mind that we live in a world of time and space
hemisphere and visual–spatial information in as well as in a world of verbal communication.
the right cerebral hemisphere. At this point the Persons with impairment of visual–spatial abil-
specialized functions of the two hemispheres ities are often severely handicapped in terms of
become operational. efficiency of functioning in a practical, everyday
The left cerebral hemisphere is particularly sense.
involved in speech and language functions or The HRB assesses visual–spatial functions
the use of language symbols for communica- with simple as well as complex tasks. Particu-
tion purposes. It is important to remember that larly important are the drawings of the square,
deficits may involve simple kinds of speech and cross, and triangle of the Aphasia Screening
language skills as well as sophisticated higher- Test (AST), the WAIS Performance subtests,
level aspects of verbal communication. It must and to an extent, Parts A and B of the Trail
also be recognized that language functions may Making Test.
be impaired in terms of expressive capabili- In evaluating the drawings on the AST, the
ties, receptive functions, or both (Reitan, 1984). criterion of brain damage relates to specific
Thus, the neuropsychological examination distortions of the spatial configurations rather
must assess an individual’s ability to express than to artistic skill. The square and triangle
language as a response, to understand language are relatively simple figures, and do not usu-
through both the auditory and visual avenues, ally challenge an individual’s appreciation and
and to complete the entire response cycle, which production of spatial configurations. The cross
consists of perception of language informa- involves many turns and a number of directions,
tion, central processing and understanding of and can provide significant information about a
its content, and the development of an effective subject’s understanding of visual–spatial form.
response. Comparisons of performances on the two
The HRB measures both simple and complex sides of the body, using both motor and senso-
verbal functions. The Reitan–Indiana Aphasia ry-perceptual tasks, provide information about
The Halstead–Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery for Adults 11

the integrity of each cerebral hemisphere, and the cerebral hemispheres. Ability to correctly
more specifically, about areas within each hemi- place the variously shaped blocks on the board
sphere. Both finger tapping and grip strength depends on tactile form discrimination, kines-
yield information about the posterior frontal thesis, coordination of movement of the upper
(motor) areas of each cerebral hemisphere. extremities, manual dexterity, and an appre-
The Tactual Performance Test (TPT) requires ciation of the relationship between the spatial
complex problem-solving skills and can pro- configuration of the shapes and their location
vide information about the adequacy of each on the board. Obviously, the TPT is consid-
cerebral hemisphere. The subject is blindfolded erably more complex in its problem-solving
before the test begins and is not permitted to requirements than either finger tapping or grip
see the formboard or blocks at any time. The strength.
first task is to fit the blocks into their proper The components of the HRB sensory-
spaces on the board using only the preferred perceptual examination include tests for
hand. After completing this task (and without bilateral simultaneous sensory stimulation
having been given prior warning), the subject is including tactile, auditory, and visual stimuli
asked to perform the same task using only the presented unilaterally and then, without warn-
nonpreferred hand. Finally, and again without ing, simultaneously on both sides. Impaired
prior warning, the task is repeated a third time perception of stimulation occurs on the side
using both hands. The amount of time required of the body contralateral to a damaged hemi-
to perform each of the three trials provides a sphere. The Tactile Form Recognition Test
comparison of the efficiency of performance of requires the subject to identify shapes through
the two hands. The Total Time score of the test the sense of touch and yields information about
reflects the amount of time needed to complete the integrity of the contralateral parietal area
all three trials. (Reitan & Wolfson, 2002b). Finger localization
After the subject has completed the third and finger-tip number writing perception also
trial, the board and blocks are taken out of the provide information about the parietal area of
testing area and the blindfold is removed. The the contralateral cerebral hemisphere. Finger-
subject is then asked to draw a diagram of the tip number writing requires considerably more
board with the blocks in their proper spaces. alertness and concentration, or perhaps even
The Memory score is the number of shapes more general intelligence, than finger localiza-
correctly remembered; the Localization score tion (Fitzhugh et al., 1962).
is the number of blocks correctly identified by In the Reitan–Wolfson theory of neuropsy-
both shape and position on the board. chological functioning, the highest level of
An important aspect of the TPT relates to the central processing is represented by abstrac-
neurological model. The test’s design and pro- tion, reasoning, concept formation, and logi-
cedure allow the functional efficiency of the two cal analysis skills. Research evidence indicates
cerebral hemispheres to be compared and pro- that these abilities have a general rather than
vide information about the general efficiency a specific representation throughout the cere-
of brain functions. During the first trial, data bral cortex (Doehring & Reitan, 1961). The
is being transmitted from the preferred hand to generality and importance of abstraction and
the contralateral cerebral hemisphere (usually reasoning skills may be suggested biologi-
from the right hand to the left cerebral hemi- cally by the fact that these skills are distrib-
sphere). Under normal circumstances, positive uted throughout the cerebral cortex rather
practice effect results in a reduction of time of than being limited as a specialized function of
about one-third from the first trial to the sec- one cerebral hemisphere or a particular area
ond trial. A similar reduction in time occurs within a hemisphere. Generalized distribution
between the second trial and the third trial. of abstraction abilities throughout the cerebral
The TPT is undoubtedly a complex task in cortex may also be significant in the interac-
terms of its motor and sensory requirements, tion of abstraction with more specific abilities
and successful performance appears to be (such as language) that are represented more
principally dependent on the middle part of focally.
12 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

Impairment at the highest level of central itself represents a significant aspect of efficiency
processing has profound implications for the in brain functioning.
adequacy of neuropsychological functioning. The HRB uses several measures to evaluate
Persons with deficits in abstraction and rea- abstraction skills, including the Category Test,
soning functions have lost a great deal of the the Trail Making Test, and the overall efficiency
ability to profit from their experiences in a of performance demonstrated on the TPT.
meaningful, logical, and organized manner. The Category Test has several characteristics
However, since their deficits are general rather that make it unique compared to many other
than specific in nature, such persons may tests. The Category Test is a relatively complex
appear to be relatively intact in casual contact. test of concept formation that requires abil-
Because of the close relationship between orga- ity (1) to note recurring similarities and dif-
nized behavior and memory, these subjects ferences in stimulus material, (2) to postulate
often complain of “memory” problems and are reasonable hypotheses about these similarities
grossly inefficient in practical, everyday tasks. and differences, (3) to test these hypotheses by
They are not able to organize their activities receiving positive or negative information (bell
properly, and frequently direct a considerable or buzzer), and (4) to adapt hypotheses on the
amount of time and energy to elements of a sit- basis of the information received after each
uation that are not appropriate to the nature of response.
the problem. The Category Test is not particularly difficult
This nonappropriate activity, together with for most normal subjects. Since the subject is
an eventual withdrawal from attempting to deal required to postulate solutions in a structured
with problem situations, constitutes a major (rather than permissive) context, the Category
component of what is frequently (and impre- Test appears to require particular competence
cisely) referred to as “personality” change. On in abstraction ability. The test in effect presents
clinical inquiry, such changes are often found each subject with a learning experiment in con-
to consist of erratic and poorly planned behav- cept formation. This is in contrast to the usual
ior, deterioration of personal hygiene, a lack of situation in psychological testing, which requires
concern and understanding for others, and so solution of an integral problem situation.
forth. When examined neuropsychologically, The primary purpose of the Category Test is
it is often discovered that these behaviors are to determine the subject’s ability to use both
largely represented by cognitive changes at the negative and positive experiences as a basis for
highest level of central processing rather than altering and adapting his or her performance
emotional involvement per se. (i.e., developing different hypotheses to deter-
Finally, in the solution of problems or expres- mine the theme of each subtest). The precise
sion of intelligent behavior, the sequential ele- pattern and sequence of positive and negative
ment from input to output frequently involves information (the bell or buzzer) in the Category
an interaction of the various aspects of central Test is probably never exactly the same for any
processing. Visual–spatial skills, for example, two subjects (or for the same subject on repe-
are closely dependent on registration and con- tition of the test). Since it can be presumed
tinued attention to incoming material of a that every item in the test affects the subject’s
visual–spatial nature, but analysis and under- response to ensuing items, the usual approaches
standing of the problem also involves the high- toward determination of reliability indices may
est element of central processing, represented be confounded. Nevertheless, the essential
by concept formation, reasoning, and logical nature of the Category Test, as an experiment in
analysis. Exactly the same kind of arrangement concept formation, is clear.
between areas of functioning in the Reitan– The Category Test is probably the best mea-
Wolfson model would relate to adequacy in sure in the HRB of abstraction, reasoning,
using verbal and language skills. In fact, the and logical analysis abilities, which in turn
speed and facility with which an individual are essential for organized planning. As noted
carries out such interactions within the con- earlier, subjects who perform especially poorly
tent categories of central processing probably in on the Category Test often complain of having
The Halstead–Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery for Adults 13

“memory” problems. In fact, the Category Test only a single person is involved. The accuracy
requires organized memory (as contrasted with of conclusions about the individual person is an
the simple reproduction of stimulus material inescapable consideration whenever the conclu-
required of most short-term memory tests), sion is actually applied to the individual per-
and is probably a more meaningful indication son. This statement is obviously a truism. Yet,
of memory in practical, complex, everyday many (if not most) neuropsychological methods
situations than most so-called memory tests, of examination have never been submitted to a
especially considering that memory, in a pur- test of accuracy regarding the identification of
poseful behavioral context, necessarily depends brain damage in the individual person. The
on relating the various aspects of a situation to HRB has always focused on the individual
each other (see Reitan & Wolfson, 1988, 1993, person through research that has rigorously
for a discussion of this concept). evaluated the accuracy of clinical conclusions.
The Trail Making Test is composed of two Realizing that clinical accuracy was a major
parts, Part A and Part B. Part A consists of objective in neuropsychological testing, the
25 circles printed on a sheet of paper. Each cir- HRB long ago was evaluated in this respect
cle contains a number from 1 to 25. The sub- (Reitan, 1964). The first step in this validational
ject’s task is to connect the circles with a pencil procedure was to identify subjects with criteri-
line as quickly as possible, beginning with on-quality frontal, nonfrontal, or diff use cere-
the number 1 and proceeding in numerical bral lesions based on all available information
sequence. Part B consists of 25 circles numbered from the neurologists and neurosurgeons who
from 1 to 13 and lettered from A to L. The task had treated them, as well as from neuropatho-
in Part B is to connect the circles, in sequence, logical findings. In order to provide a rigorous
alternating between numbers and letters. The test of the generality of any conclusion relat-
scores represent the number of seconds required ing to these various groups, we designed the
to complete each part. study so that each group had the same number
The Trail Making Test requires immedi- of subjects with different types of lesions.
ate recognition of the symbolic significance of Each regional localization group was therefore
numbers and letters, ability to scan the page composed of equal numbers of subjects with
continuously to identify the next number or intrinsic tumors, extrinsic tumors, cerebral
letter in sequence, flexibility in integrating the vascular lesions, and focal traumatic lesions.
numerical and alphabetical series, and comple- Classification of these subjects was based solely
tion of these requirements under the pressure on medical findings and with no reference to
of time. It is likely that the ability to deal with their HRB test results. In total, we identified 64
the numerical and language symbols (numbers persons with focal lesions (16 in each quadrant)
and letters) is sustained by the left cerebral and 48 persons with diff use cerebral damage,
hemisphere, the visual scanning task necessary for a total of 112 persons.
to perceive the spatial distribution of the stim- The next step was to refer only to the HRB
ulus material is represented by the right cere- test results, and rate each of the 112 persons with
bral hemisphere, and speed and efficiency of regard to focal or diff use involvement, the type
performance may reflect the general adequacy of brain disease or lesion, and, if focal, identify
of brain functions. It is therefore not surpris- the quadrant involved. On the basis of the HRB
ing that the Trail Making Test, which requires results alone, 46 of the 48 persons with diff use
simultaneous integration of these several abili- involvement and 57 of the 64 persons with focal
ties, is one of the best measures of general brain damage were identified correctly.
functions (Reitan, 1955c, 1958). The number of correct classifications based
on HRB results regarding type of lesion was
as follows: intrinsic tumor, 13 of 16; extrinsic
Clinical Inferences Regarding
tumor, 8 of 16; cerebral vascular disease, 28
Individual Persons
of 32; head injury, 30 of 32; and multiple scle-
Inferences about average performances for rosis, 15 of 16. Thus, 94 of the 112 patients were
groups of subjects are of little relevance when classified correctly according to type of lesion.
14 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

This degree of concurrence between neuro- the knowledge, experience, and practical test-
logical and neuropsychological ratings could ing that might be required for acceptance.
scarcely have happened by chance. The results Neuropsychological tests and procedures, which
confirmed that HRB test results are differen- have been subject to peer review and publica-
tially influenced by (1) focal and diff use lesions, tion in acceptable outlets, however, might well
(2) the cerebral hemisphere that is damaged, be viewed as having appropriate acceptance by
(3) frontal and nonfrontal lesions within the the scientific community.
hemisphere involved, (4) intrinsic tumors, Our recent experiences have mainly con-
extrinsic tumors, cerebral vascular lesions, cerned (3) above—the error rate. The error rate
head injuries, and multiple sclerosis, (5) focal is quite a different matter than the statistical
occlusion as compared with generalized insuffi- probability of accepting the null hypothesis (or
ciency in cerebral vascular disease, and (6) focal a chance effect), which some neuropsychologists
as compared with diff use damage from head have referred to in responding to this require-
injuries. ment. Even supplying a probability statement
for multiple tests is hardly an answer, inasmuch
as the independence of the tests is unknown
Forensic and Clinical Implications
and, in any case, this kind of information says
of Validity Studies of the HRB
nothing about the rate of errors in conclusions
The United States Supreme Court identified about the individual person. In legal matters
four considerations for trial judges to use in concerning personal injury, data relating to the
evaluating expert testimony, or in some cases, probability that groups of subjects are, or are
in deciding whether to admit expert testimony. not, drawn from the same population are essen-
This directive requires that neuropsychologists tially irrelevant, because the matter concerns
carefully consider their testing methodology in the individual person rather than inferential
accordance with whether their procedures meet statistics based on group comparisons.
these four criteria. Briefly, these include the In fact, the customary models of statistical
following: analyses—the ones we all learned in school
and are usually required to meet publication
1. Has the method been tested? standards—have limited meaning when judg-
2. Has the method been subjected to peer ments, diagnoses, and conclusions must be
review and publication? made about the individual person, particularly
3. What is the error rate in applying the when based on tests that produce continuous
method? score distributions. We often offer “more prob-
4. To what extent has the method received able than not” statements with little or no hard
general acceptance in the relevant scientific evidence to support these conclusions. If knowl-
community? edge of the error rate concerning the question
of brain damage or impairment was actually
In our experience, trial judges and lawyers required to permit one to testify as an expert,
are giving increased consideration to these not many neuropsychologists would be allowed
guidelines, and give serious consideration to to testify. Few studies on individual tests, or
the admissibility of testimony based on whether even conclusions based on batteries of tests, even
these guidelines have been met. In the main, report the number of false-positives and false-
concerns have centered around (3) and (4) negatives. Such data are published for other
aforementioned, namely, the error rate and the diagnostic methods that require conclusions
question of general acceptance. about the individual person (such as computed
It would seem perfectly legitimate, and even tomography and magnetic resonance imag-
necessary, to know how accurate a method ing), and questions of accuracy were among the
is in achieving its purpose. Acceptance in the first issues studied when these procedures were
scientific community, however, would carry developed and became available.
with it a time factor, inasmuch as newer meth- Why have psychologists failed to produce
ods would need time to be incorporated into such data, when it clearly is so necessary as a
The Halstead–Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery for Adults 15

basis for giving neuropsychological methods a to the judge to exclude the neuropsychologist’s
degree of credence? There are a number of pos- testimony on the grounds that his methodol-
sible answers to this question, a principal one ogy had not been adequately tested, had not
being that a specified procedure or set of tests been subjected to peer review, and had an error
is required, applied to each subject in a precise rate that was unknown. The attorney argued
and standardized manner, in order to evalu- that the neuropsychological community would
ate the accuracy of the method, as shown by disagree that neuropsychological tests should
concurrence with independent criterion infor- count for 10% and clinical impressions for 90%.
mation. In cases involving litigation, neuropsy- The defense attorney said that, in his opinion,
chologists appear to be shying away from such the judge “came very close to excluding the neu-
specified and standardized procedures in favor ropsychologist’s testimony, but in the end felt
of so-called flexible batteries, apparently want- that he did not have enough information about
ing (or needing) to give whatever tests they wish prevailing practices in neuropsychology to
and to thereby gain the right to draw whatever make such a decision.” Another consideration
conclusions they wish (or that may be requisite may have been that the judge was reluctant to
in terms of the role they have accepted and the make a ruling that would have devastated the
conclusions they have committed themselves to plaintiff ’s case since, aside from the neuropsy-
support). chologist’s opinion, there was little additional
In a recent case a federal judge was asked, credible evidence of brain impairment.
on the basis of a Daubert challenge, to dismiss It seems clear that the time has come for
the testimony of a prominent neuropsychol- neuropsychologists to support their conclusions
ogist who was testifying on behalf of a plain- with scientific evidence. We can hardly be proud
tiff who had sustained a head injury. In many that it is the legal processes that are forcing us
prior instances, both in his publications and in this direction rather than a feeling of clinical
sworn testimony, this neuropsychologist had responsibility to our clients and patients.
supported the use of neuropsychological tests The issue of testing the accuracy of neu-
in evaluating individual persons. In this case, ropsychological test data must receive much
however, the extensive set of tests that had been more critical consideration than it has up to
administered under his direct supervision fell this point. One approach might be to make
essentially in the normal range. He admitted predictive judgments using whatever test data
that the test findings were essentially normal, was available for each subject, and checking the
and, in fact, this conclusion was readily sub- accuracy of these predictions against criterion
ject to documentation, inasmuch as they were information. The problem with this approach
based on the HRB. However, he insisted that the would lie in the fact that there would be no
plaintiff had sustained significant and serious clear definition of the test data used for making
impairment. There was strong evidence against the predictions, inasmuch as the tests admin-
this conclusion, such as the plaintiff having istered would surely vary from one subject to
earned a greater income the year after the injury another.
than the year before the injury. However, this Neuropsychologists who use so-called flex-
prominent neuropsychologist pointed out that ible batteries would face exactly this problem,
he had interviewed the plaintiff, his relatives, because their test batteries, designed according
and a few friends, all of whom cited a host of to the supposed complaints or deficits of the cli-
complaints and problems that they attributed to ent, necessarily vary from one client to another.
the injury. In fact, a procedure that first discerns the client’s
When asked about the disparity between his area of possible deficit via history information,
conclusions and the normal neuropsychological relatives’ observations of the client, or interview
test results, the neuropsychologist replied that information, and then seeks to confirm or refute
neuropsychological tests contributed no more such hypotheses through selection of a range of
than 10% to his conclusions and that his clin- tests judged to be appropriate, is clearly circular
ical impressions were the primary basis for his and can only be considered to be relevant to the
testimony. The opposing attorney then appealed complaints initially deemed to be significant.
16 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

Under these circumstances, there can be no accordance to the many things that go wrong
assurance that the battery of tests selected for with the brain, or recognize that the neuro part
each client represents a comprehensive, bal- of neuropsychology is added for respectability
anced, and validated set of measures of brain alone. If we are unable to document valid rela-
function or dysfunction. Of course, if each of tionships to brain status, in terms of our scien-
these many symptom-oriented batteries had tific methods and procedures, as responsible
been checked for accuracy in correlating with professionals we should at least admit that the
and identifying brain lesions of a diff use and/or Supreme Court was quite reasonable and cor-
focal nature, in varying locations of the brain, rect in requiring that the error rate in neuropsy-
representing the full range of types of brain dis- chology be specified with respect to individual
ease and injury in identifying both chronic and subjects. Such an admission for many neurop-
stabilized brain lesions versus acute and pro- sychologists would also verify their inadequacy
gressive brain conditions and effectively differ- to function as expert witnesses.
entiating this entire cadre of people with brain One way to resolve this dilemma would be
involvement from non–brain-damaged people, competently to use a neuropsychological test
there would be no problem. The field of neuro- battery for which published evidence is avail-
psychology (the discipline based on establishing able that meets the four criteria identified by
brain–behavior relationships) would be secure. the Supreme Court as necessary for admis-
Obviously, though, the diverse collections of sion as an expert witness. The Halstead–Reitan
tests used by many neuropsychologists have not Neuropsychological Test Battery meets these
been tested in this manner. four criteria, but competent interpretation
There is no likely prospect that multiple test (which can be judged by the many published
batteries can be checked out in the detail nec- examples of test interpretation) is required
essary to establish their relationships to the over and beyond training in test administra-
broad range of conditions that, in total, repre- tion. Fortunately, detailed information is avail-
sent brain damage. Yet, unless there is evi- able for both administration and interpretation
dence that all the categories of brain damage of these batteries of neuropsychological tests
are reflected by a particular neuropsychological for adults, older children, and young children
battery (or that the battery validly differenti- (Reitan & Wolfson, 1988, 1990, 1992, 1993),
ates between brain-damaged persons and non– and the interested neuropsychologist can learn
brain-damaged persons regardless of all of the much of the basics of HRB test interpretation
variations that occur under the rubric of brain from the many case examples that have been
damage), one cannot presume that the battery provided.
validly reflects brain pathology.
In the absence of evidence supporting this
Recent Research and Current
presumption, we clearly lose the claim that
Issues in Neuropsychology
identifies the essential nature of our field as the
area of psychology that relates behavioral mea- The development, content, and design of the
surement to the biological status of the brain. HRB allow its continual use in pursuing many
Without a firm anchor to the brain, we become theoretical, practical, and clinical issues in
clinical psychologists, school psychologists, neuropsychology.
consulting psychologists, educational psycholo-
gists, or whatever type of psychologist appro-
The General Neuropsychological
priate to our particular interest. Of course, if
Deficit Scale (GNDS)
we have not been intensively trained in these
areas, we will also find ourselves failing to com- The GNDS is a summary score based on 42
pare favorably to the experts who have been so variables from the HRB. In addition to tests
trained and experienced. based on level of performance, the GNDS
It is apparent that we cannot have it both also includes additional variables that reflect
ways. We either have to respect the brain as the the ways in which the brain expresses its defi-
basis for behavior, and validate our measures in cits, including the occurrence of specific signs
The Halstead–Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery for Adults 17

(as opposed to general indicators) of brain in neuropsychology, however, much as the


damage, relationships among test results that Full Scale IQ, based on the Wechsler Scales,
deviate from normal expectation, and intra- does for general intelligence. While the IQ has
individual differences on the two sides of been criticized as lacking specificity because
the body. it averages various scores (as does the GNDS),
Since the variables contributing to the any reasonably intelligent psychologist would
GNDS, in themselves, yield significant differ- review the individual test scores (which are
ences in groups of brain-damaged persons and obviously available) if greater specificity of abil-
controls, it is not surprising that the GNDS ities is needed. The same situation applies to
is highly effective in this regard. Reitan and the GNDS in neuropsychological evaluation of
Wolfson (1988, 1993) found that only about 10% the individual person. The GNDS stood as the
of a group of brain-damaged persons and 10% only validated neuropsychological summary
of a group of controls were misclassified by the score available for a number of years. Recently,
GNDS, without any adjustments for age, edu- however, Heaton et al. (2006) have developed
cation, or IQ, demonstrating a very striking a summary score that is similar to the GNDS,
degree of sensitivity as well as specificity. but utilizes demographic adjustments.
Rojas and Bennett (1995) compared a group
with mild head injuries with a group of control
Traumatic Brain Injury
subjects and found that the GNDS correctly
identified 92% of the subjects, whereas the The HRB has a long history in the area of trau-
Stroop Neuropsychological Screening Test did matic brain injury, with two volumes sum-
not differentiate the groups. Despite the many marizing published research regarding its
studies of the Impairment Index over the years, sensitivity, specificity, and clinical interpre-
indicating its sensitivity to brain damage, Rojas tation (Reitan & Wolfson, 1986, 1988). More
and Bennett found in their study that the GNDS recently, a number of studies reported in the
was more sensitive than the Impairment Index. literature have found that mild head injury
Perhaps this is to be expected, considering that may result in neuropsychological deficits, but
the GNDS summarizes results on 42 variables that these deficits usually resolve completely in
versus the seven variables that contribute to the two to three months. These studies have been
Impairment Index. cited and reviewed in the book Mild Head
Oestreicher and O’Donnell (1995) com- Injury: Intellectual, Cognitive, and Emotional
pared the GNDS in three groups of young Consequences (Reitan & Wolfson, 2000b).
adults: (1) subjects with traumatic brain inju- Having examined many hundreds of persons
ries, (2) subjects with learning disabilities, and over the years with traumatic head injuries,
(3) volunteer controls. The groups were equiva- we were aware that while many persons with
lent in gender and Full Scale IQ. The controls all mild head injuries recover quite well, many also
had GNDS scores of 27 or lower (normal range had persistent and essentially permanent com-
is 25 or lower); 49% of the learning-disabled plaints, supported by findings on testing with
persons had GNDS scores of 27 or higher and the HRB.
97% of the brain-injured subjects had GNDS Our procedure of correlating test results with
scores of 27 or higher. This study confirmed findings from neurological evaluation indi-
earlier findings by O’Donnell and his coworkers cated that some persons with mild head inju-
and also demonstrated that many young adults ries (including even a few persons who had not
with learning disabilities also show evidence of lost consciousness) did have definitive evidence
neuropsychological impairment on the GNDS. of brain damage resulting from intracranial
Inasmuch as the GNDS is a summary mea- bleeding. There was obviously something wrong
sure, it must be noted that while it provides an with the conclusion of “full recovery” reported
overall indication of neuropsychological status, in the neuropsychological literature. These
it cannot be substituted for the detailed infor- observations led to a series of studies reported
mation provided by the individual tests on in the book cited above as well as in other
which it is based. It serves a valuable purpose sources (Reitan & Wolfson, 1999b, 2000b). Our
18 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

main focus was on the method used to compose who were instructed, with varying degrees
head-injured groups. In research studies, the of thoroughness, to act as if they were brain-
procedure had usually been to admit subjects to injured or otherwise impaired. Malingering
a study in consecutive order when they met pre- in real life undoubtedly is a complex behavior,
determined criteria. No reference was usually arising from a host of complex stressors and
made about whether the subjects had contin- determinants and expressed in a variety of ways
uing or even increasing signs and symptoms and to different degrees. Thus, to ask normal
following the head injury. Since most persons and often naive participants to simulate the
with a mild head injury recover rather routinely complex behavior represented by malingering,
(estimated at 85%–90%), the persons with more and to expect such playacting to produce valid
serious and persisting deficits would likely “fall results, is at least somewhat naive. Brain dam-
between the cracks” when included in the total age, in its range of pathological characteristics
group. and its varied manifestations, is at least equally
A study was needed to compare subjects complex. The field of clinical neuropsychology
who were accessed into the study according is indeed fortunate that its knowledge base
to predetermined criteria with a group of per- was not derived principally by evaluation of
sons with equivalently mild injuries who later persons instructed to pretend that they were
sought medical care because of persisting com- brain damaged. Clinical psychology and psy-
plaints, deficits, and sometimes evidence of chiatry would be similarly skewed in their
brain lesions when finally examined neurolog- knowledge base had they been developed from
ically. Fortunately, we had available both such normal persons feigning mental and emotional
groups from our large pool of head-injured illnesses.
persons who had been given the HRB. The HRB This approach to the development of knowl-
results showed only mild but statistically sig- edge regarding malingerers has undoubtedly
nificant deficits in the group that had been rou- arisen from the fact that on one hand, the prob-
tinely accessed into the ongoing research study lem is important and, on the other hand, no
(much in accordance with results reported in one has been able to compose a group of “true”
the literature), but the group composed of per- malingerers; generally, they do not confess.
sons who had returned months after their mild More recently, neuropsychologists have
head injuries with significant complaints (per- developed what is called a “known-group
sisting headaches, loss of efficiency at work, design,” presumably because it is deemed capa-
significantly lower school grades, and in some ble of definitively identifying malingerers as
instances, development of posttraumatic epi- well as non-malingerers. This method was
lepsy) performed significantly poorer on the recently reviewed by Greve et al. (2006), par-
HRB, showing definite neuropsychological ticularly with respect to documentation of the
impairment. Although these patients repre- validity of the Test of Memory Malingering.
sented only a fraction of any total group of per- The method initially requires identification of
sons with head injuries (our estimate was 13%), an external incentive, supplemented by judged
they were being lost in the routine research disparities between self-report of the history
practices being reported in the literature, even and documented history, information given by
though they were the persons in need of com- other informants, behavioral observations, and
prehensive evaluation and treatment. Our stud- judgments regarding exaggeration or fabrica-
ies, while being quite definitive, appear still to tion of psychological dysfunction. All of these
be unappreciated by many neuropsychologists, circumstances depend, of course, on the infor-
judging from the frequency with which state- mation (complete or incomplete) that is avail-
ments are being made regarding full recovery able and the subjective judgments of the
among persons with mild head injuries. psychologist who happens to be involved in
the evaluation. These subjective conclusions
are supplemented by psychological testing,
Malingering and Dissimulation
requiring “positive findings” on any one of
Most published studies of malingering are five tests of malingering. These five tests, how-
based on results obtained from participants ever, have never been validated on any actually
The Halstead–Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery for Adults 19

known malingerers. Finally, if a conclusion of In fact, there was scarcely any overlap in the
malingering is reached using this known-group score distributions for the two groups. These
design, the findings supporting the conclusion results indicate that head-injured persons in
must not be fully accounted for by psychiat- litigation, often under the pressure of finan-
ric, neurological, or developmental factors, to cial strain with a great deal depending on the
the extent that they are known or judged to be outcome of the second testing (and often prob-
applicable. ably even without a conscious realization that
The essential impossibility of defining cri- they are not putting forth a maximal effort) do
teria for accurate identification of malingerers not perform consistently on two examinations
based on available information (which may be (which in this study were separated by about
incomplete, biased, or inaccurate) as judged by one year). There is a great advantage in using the
a psychologist (whose judgments are not sub- individual as his or her own control in deriv-
ject to any tests of reliability or validity and ing data about the reliability and validity of test
certainly will differ from other psychologists to results. The full details, including the tests and
at least a degree), supplemented by results from test items used, the score-transformation proce-
psychological tests using results from “pre- dures used for the two Indexes, and other data
sumed” non-malingerers, with all of the above and procedures are presented in several papers
classifying persons as malingerers without a (Reitan & Wolfson, 1995b, 1996d, 1997a) and in
shred of evidence or data having been gained the book entitled Detection of Malingering and
from actual known malingerers, certainly Invalid Test Scores (Reitan & Wolfson, 1997b).
becomes clear.
The HRB has also been studied regarding the
Conation: A Neglected Aspect of
validity of psychological test data, but rather
Neuropsychological Functioning
than to try to label people as malingerers in
the absence of any data on known malingerers, Conation, also referred to as mental fatigue
Reitan and Wolfson (2002a) developed pro- (Boring, 1942), has a long history in psychol-
cedures that focused on determining the reli- ogy. A more complete definition would describe
ability of the test data. The procedure requires conation as the ability to apply oneself persis-
two testings of each individual, and compares tently, diligently, and constructively over a
the scores and responses on the two testings to period of time in solution of a problem or com-
see if the individual is responding at the same pletion of a task. This ability, although probably
level and in the same manner on the second central to performance capabilities in everyday
testing as on the first. Two scales were devel- living, has essentially been neglected in neuro-
oped: one scale to assess the consistency of test psychology and, to a large extent, in the field of
scores and another scale to assess the consis- psychology as a whole. The HRB, however, was
tency of responses to individual test items. If, developed with this ability in mind. Halstead,
for example, a person’s test score became worse in conversation and in his publications, often
on the second testing, as if the person had given referred to the frequent clinical observation of
a correct response initially to a test item fol- disparities between high general intelligence
lowed on the second testing by an incorrect and limited professional contributions and pro-
answer or “I don’t know” response, the results ductive capabilities, citing an apparent lack of
would contribute to the inconsistency score. intellectual energy or persistence on an observa-
These two scales were then combined to form a tional level and the power factor in his concept
Dissimulation Index. of biological intelligence on a scientific level.
The Dissimulation Index was evaluated for We felt that it was important, in assessing the
two head-injured groups: one group involved effects of brain impairment, to use a broad sam-
in litigation and another group who had pling of functions, ranging from simple tasks to
never even considered litigation. The mean ones that were fairly complex and demanding.
Dissimulation Indexes for the two groups were In accordance with the concept of composing
decidedly different. The group in litigation had tests to evaluate the intraindividual aspects of
a Dissimulation Index that was almost twice as neuropsychological functioning, which also has
large as the Index for the group not in litigation. been a basic aim in using the HRB, we felt that it
20 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

was important to discern the individual’s ability to brain damage depending on the degree to
to apply continuing effective energy to the solu- which they were dependent on conation.
tion of complex tasks. In a sense, the HRB was These results raise a serious question about
designed not only to assess possible impairment the current emphasis on “effort” tests as indica-
in various areas of neuropsychological func- tors of possible malingering. Is reduced effort an
tioning, but also to assess conation. indicator of the invalidity of test results, or is it
Our first study of conative ability (Reitan & due to brain damage? Most “effort” tests require
Wolfson, 2000a) evaluated the differences the subject to focus attention to paired symbols,
between a brain-damaged group and a con- establish an association (learning), and then be
trol group on tests that required little cona- tested on what has been learned. There seems
tion (Information and Vocabulary from the to be little question that such requirements
Wechsler Scale) and tests that required sus- depend upon conation. However, given the cur-
tained attention, concentration, and effort over rent emphasis and climate in neuropsychology,
time (SSPT and the Henmon–Nelson Test of failure of one effort test is considered to be a
Intelligence). Although the Henmon–Nelson sign of invalid results on all tests administered,
Test never had been considered a test for brain if not actual malingering. Under these circum-
damage, it did require the subject to work dili- stances, a person with brain damage could well
gently and independently for 30 minutes, thus be penalized for being brain damaged.
obviously tapping conation. The results indi- Although there have been published claims
cated that the largest difference between the two that mentally retarded, learning-disabled,
groups occurred on the Henmon–Nelson Test, and other impaired persons can pass “effort”
suggesting that conative ability had a powerful tests that require close attention, concentra-
effect in determining the impairment of persons tion, and paired-associate learning, it would
with brain damage. appear that this overall situation is quite com-
A second study by Reitan and Wolfson plex and requires much further evaluation. It
(2004a) compared brain-damaged and control may not generally be remembered, but tests of
groups regarding IQ differences in tests that paired-associate learning, using both words
required relatively little conation (Wechsler and designs pairs, were among the first ever
Verbal IQ) with the Henmon–Nelson Test, formally proposed for the assessment of brain
using a standard score transformation that damage, rather than lack of effort (Hunt, 1943).
permitted comparative evaluations based on
actual performances. The Henmon–Nelson Test
Screening Tests to Predict When
differentiated the groups more effectively than
Comprehensive Testing Is Needed
Wechsler Verbal IQ. Conation seemed to be a
significant factor in determining IQ values. Our next research effort was motivated by the
Finally, we were interested in the extent to profession’s need for an objective and vali-
which conation was a general determinant of dated method to identify, on the basis of brief
deficits due to brain damage across a broad testing, those persons who would show brain-
range of neuropsychological tests (Reitan & related deficits, and be in obvious need of com-
Wolfson, 2005). Nineteen tests from the HRB prehensive neuropsychological testing. A basic
were rated according to the degree that they requisite for solving this problem would be the
appeared to require conation. These 19 tests availability of data on a brief set of screening
were then ranked according to their sensitivity tests for every potential person to be included in
(t-ratios) in comparing a group of controls and the study and the results of comprehensive test-
a group of brain-damaged persons. A rank-dif- ing for these same persons. These data would
ference correlation was completed between the be needed for a non–brain-damaged control
two sets of ratings, yielding a coefficient of 0.86. group and for a comparable group of persons
This result indicated that the extent to which a evaluated neurologically (medically) and found
test required conation was almost perfectly cor- to have unequivocal evidence of mild or more
related with its sensitivity to brain damage. The severe brain damage or disease. A criterion
tests studied seemed to be increasingly sensitive value, such as the GNDS score, would be needed
The Halstead–Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery for Adults 21

as a marker representing the comprehensive the brain-damaged children as needing com-


testing and a summary value for the many tests prehensive testing and correctly placed 88% of
administered. Finally, considering the overall the controls in the normal range. Again, when
age range involved among brain-damaged peo- borderline scores occurred in each group, even
ple, the above requirement would need to be though falling just within the appropriate score
duplicated by a factor of three (young children, range, recommendations were given to appraise
older children, and adults). all additional sources of information.
Fortunately, HRB results were available that The study based on adults (Reitan & Wolfson,
met all of these requirements. This study could 2008c) indicated that the screening tests cor-
never have been done without the input of neu- rectly identified 82% of the controls as having
rologists, neurosurgeons, and neuropatholo- normal scores on comprehensive testing. The
gists, nor would it have been possible were it not majority of controls who deviated from expec-
for administration of a comprehensive neuro- tation based on the screening results were older,
psychological test battery to every potential and previous studies of the HRB have indi-
candidate for inclusion in the research project. cated that some older people, classified as con-
(It was necessary to have a much larger pool trols on the basis of neurological examination,
initially in order to compose age-comparable earn impaired neuropsychological test scores.
groups in each of the three age ranges.) While In the brain-damaged group, however, 96%
clinical application of the research results would of the sample were correctly classified by the
require use of the predictor tests, the compre- screening tests as showing impairment on com-
hensive evaluation could, of course, be done with prehensive testing. In total, 89% were correctly
any tests of the neuropsychologist’s choice, even identified by screening tests. Again, in terms of
though the outcome of comprehensive testing in clinical application of the results, recommenda-
the project had been based on the HRB. tions were given to consider all additional infor-
Three studies were done based on young mation (including age) in evaluating borderline
children, older children, and adults. The “pre- scores on the screening tests. Horton (personal
dictor,” or screening tests, was selected from the communication) performed a study of adults in
corresponding HRB for each age range, and in which he found an overall correct rate of 92%
each age range required only 30 to 45 minutes using the cutoff points presented by Reitan and
to administer. Thus, the problem became one of Wolfson.
predicting the outcome of the comprehensive It would appear from the above findings that
HRB from a short but diversified group of tests preliminary testing, requiring only a relatively
which in each age range included measures of short time, may well provide an objective basis
sensory-perceptual abilities, motor functions, for recommending comprehensive neuropsy-
and higher-level aspects of brain functioning. chological evaluations across a broad age range.
The results for young children (Reitan & The availability of such information may be
Wolfson, 2008a) indicated that the brief screen- a factor in gaining approval and payment for
ing tests correctly identified as impaired 92% of comprehensive testing.
the brain-damaged children when compared to It should be clearly recognized that brief
results obtained on comprehensive testing; 85% screening is in no way a substitute for com-
of the control children were correctly identified prehensive evaluation, since the range of rel-
as having scores in the normal range on compre- evant domains, necessary for evaluation of
hensive testing. In total, 11.5% of children were the individual client, have not been examined
misclassified. However, these “misclassified” with the screening tests proposed for use in the
children had borderline scores, and recommen- aforementioned studies. It is quite likely that
dations were given about the need to consider many potential candidates for neuropsycho-
carefully all additional sources of information, logical evaluation, as based on available clinical
especially when borderline screening scores referral information, are not being examined
were obtained. because the complaints are personal, subjec-
The study of older children (Reitan & tive, not subject to independent confirmation,
Wolfson, 2008b) correctly identified 92% of and in many cases, not supported by medical
22 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

(neurological) findings. Many such persons, body (Reitan & Wolfson, 2008e) as methods
especially with prior head injuries or toxic of unequivocal identification of brain damage
exposure, have deficits which can be described, in a substantial proportion of brain-damaged
classified, and documented by neuropsycho- samples.
logical examination, with results that provide Readers wishing more information on these
a basis for treatment and intervention. Perhaps and other topics are referred to complete
the greatest benefit of validated screening pro- descriptions of the HRB tests for young chil-
cedures will accrue to this group of people. dren, older children, and adults that have been
There are additional recent studies that have provided in Reitan and Wolfson (1992, 1993),
derived from the development and content of together with a review of many research studies
the HRB that can be mentioned only briefly and many instructional examples of interpreta-
because of space limitations. The availability tion of results for individual persons.
of a strongly sensitive and specific summary
measure of the HRB (the GNDS) permitted
a number of studies concerned with the pos- References
sible differential relationship among groups Binet, A., & Simon, T. (1916). The development of intel-
with and without evidence of brain damage ligence in children. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.
of age and education to neuropsychological Boring, E. G. (1942). Sensation and perception in
test results. These studies compared groups of the history of experimental psychology. New York:
adults (Reitan & Wolfson, 1995d), older chil- D. Appleton-Century Co.
dren (Reitan & Wolfson, 1995c), children with Dean, R. S. (1985). Review of Halstead–Reitan
learning disabilities (Reitan & Wolfson, 1996a, Neuropsychological Test Battery. In J. V. Mitchell
(Ed.), The ninth mental measurements yearbook
1996c), and adults with mild head injuries
(pp. 642–646). Highland Park, NJ: The Gryphon
(Reitan & Wolfson, 1996b, 1997c, 1999a). In each Press.
of these studies the effects of age and education Doehring, D. G., & Reitan, R. M. (1961). Certain
on neuropsychological test scores were dimin- language and non-language disorders in brain-
ished (if not essentially eliminated) by brain damaged patients with homonymous visual
trauma or learning disabilities. In comput- field defects. AMA Archives of Neurology and
ing IQ values, impaired children were affected Psychiatry, 5, 294–299.
adversely in using norms developed for nor- Fitzhugh, L. C., Fitzhugh, K. B., & Reitan, R. M.
mal children because the development of their (1962). Sensorimotor deficits of brain-damaged
abilities did not match the rate of normal chil- subjects in relation to intellectual level. Perceptual
dren. Chronological age advanced at a standard and Motor Skills, 15, 603–608.
Greve, K. W., Bianchini, K. J., Black, F. W., Heinly,
rate for all children, but development of abili-
M. T., Love, J. M., Swift, D. A., et al. (2006).
ties was slower for impaired children, resulting Classification accuracy of the Test of Memory
in progressive lowering of their IQ’s the older Malingering in persons reporting exposure to
they became (Reitan & Wolfson, 1996a, 1996c). environmental and industrial toxins: Results of
Obviously, the practice of applying normative a known-groups analysis. Archives of Clinical
standards developed for normal persons to Neuropsychology, 21, 439–448.
impaired persons is fraught with problems. Halstead, W. C. (1947). Brain and intelligence: A
Additional publications have dealt with the quantitative study of the frontal lobes. Chicago:
difficulties in producing specific evidence of University of Chicago Press.
selective deficits in persons with frontal lobe Heaton, R. K., Miller, S. W., Taylor, M. J., & Grant, I.
lesions (Reitan & Wolfson, 1994, 1995a), the (2006). Revised comprehensive norms for an
expanded Halstead–Reitan Battery: Demogra-
validity and clinical usefulness of individ-
phically adjusted neuropsychological norms for
ual tests (Reitan & Wolfson, 2002b, 2004b, African American and Caucasian adults. Profess-
2004c), the interaction of emotional status ional Manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment
and neuropsychological capabilities (Reitan & Resources, Inc.
Wolfson, 2000b), and assessment of specific Hunt, H. F. (1943). A practical clinical test for organic
deficits (Reitan & Wolfson, 2008d), and differ- brain damage. Journal of Applied Psychology, 27,
ences in performances on the two sides of one’s 375–381.
The Halstead–Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery for Adults 23

Meier, M. J. (1985). Review of Halstead–Reitan lobe functions. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 9,


Neuropsychological Test Battery. In J. V. Mitchell 50–56.
(Ed.), The ninth mental measurements yearbook Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1995b). Consistency of
(pp. 646–649). Highland Park, NJ: The Gryphon responses on retesting among head-injured sub-
Press. jects in litigation versus head-injured subjects not
Oestreicher, J. M., & O’Donnell, J. P. (1995). Vali- in litigation. Applied Neuropsychology, 2, 67–71.
dation of the General Neuropsychological Deficit Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1995c). Influence of age
Scale with nondisabled, learning-disabled, and and education on neuropsychological test perfor-
head-injured young adults. Archives of Clinical mances of older children. Child Neuropsychology,
Neuropsychology, 10, 185–191. 1, 165–169.
Reitan, R. M. (1955a). Certain differential effects Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1995d). Influence of age
of left and right cerebral lesions in human and education on neuropsychological test results.
adults. Journal of Comparative and Physiological The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 9, 151–158.
Psychology, 48, 474–477. Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1996a). Can WISC–R IQ
Reitan, R. M. (1955b). An investigation of the validity values be computed validly for learning-disabled
of Halstead’s measures of biological intelligence. children? Applied Neuropsychology, 3, 15–20.
Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 73, 28–35. Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1996b). Differential
Reitan, R. M. (1955c). The relation of the Trail relationships of age and education to WAIS subtest
Making Test to organic brain damage. Journal of scores among brain-damaged and control groups.
Consulting Psychology, 19, 393–394. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 11, 303–311.
Reitan, R. M. (1958). The validity of the Trail Making Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1996c). The diminished
Test as an indicator of organic brain damage. effect of age and education on neuropsychological
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 8, 271–276. performances of learning-disabled children. Child
Reitan, R. M. (1964). Psychological deficits resulting Neuropsychology, 2, 11–16.
from cerebral lesions in man. In J. M. Warren & Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1996d). The ques-
K. A. Akert (Eds.), The frontal granular cortex and tion of validity of neuropsychological test scores
behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill. among head-injured litigants: Development
Reitan, R. M. (1984). Aphasia and sensory-perceptual of a Dissimulation Index. Archives of Clinical
deficits in adults. Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychology Neuropsychology, 11, 573–580.
Press. Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1997a). Consistency
Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1986). Traumatic brain of neuropsychological test scores of head-injured
injury. Vol. I. Pathophysiology and neuropsycho- subjects involved in litigation compared with
logical evaluation. Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychology head-injured subjects not involved in litigation:
Press. Development of the Retest Consistency Index. The
Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1988). Traumatic brain Clinical Neuropsychologist, 11, 69–76.
injury. Vol. II. Recovery and rehabilitation. Tucson, Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1997b). Detection of
AZ: Neuropsychology Press. malingering and invalid test scores. Tucson AZ:
Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1989). The Seashore Neuropsychology Press.
Rhythm Test and brain functions. The Clinical Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1997c). The influence
Neuropsychologist, 3, 70–77. of age and education on neuropsychological per-
Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1990). Neuropsychological formances of persons with mild head injuries.
evaluation of young children. Manual. Tucson, AZ: Applied Neuropsychology, 4, 16–33.
Neuropsychology Press. Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1999a). The influence
Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1992). Neuropsycho- of age and education on neuropsychological per-
logical evaluation of older children. Tucson, AZ: formances of persons with mild traumatic brain
Neuropsychology Press. injuries. In M. J. Raymond, T. L. Bennett, L. C.
Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1993). The Halstead– Hartlage, & C. M. Cullum (Eds.), Mild traumatic
Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery: Theory brain injury. Austin: Pro-ed.
and clinical interpretation (2nd ed.). Tucson, AZ: Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1999b). The two
Neuropsychology Press. faces of mild head injury. Archives of Clinical
Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1994). A selective and Neuropsychology, 14, 191–202.
critical review of neuropsychological deficits and the Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (2000a). Conation: A
frontal lobes. Neuropsychology Review, 4, 161–198. neglected aspect of neuropsychological func-
Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1995a). The Category tioning. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 15,
Test and Trail Making Test as measures of frontal 443–453.
24 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (2000b). Mild head Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (2008b). Serial testing
injury: Intellectual, cognitive, and emotional con- of older children as a basis for recommending
sequences. Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychology Press. comprehensive neuropsychological evaluation.
Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (2002a). Detection of Applied Neuropsychology, 15, 11–20.
malingering and invalid test results using the Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (2008c). The use of serial
Halstead–Reitan Battery. Journal of Forensic testing in evaluating the need for comprehen-
Neuropsychology, 3, 275–314. sive neuropsychological testing of adults. Applied
Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (2002b). Using the Neuropsychology, 15, 21–32.
Tactile Form Recognition Test to differentiate Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (2008d). Can neuropsy-
persons with brain damage from control subjects. chological testing produce unequivocal evidence
Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 17, 117–121. of brain damage? I. Testing for specific deficits.
Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (2004a). The differential Applied Neuropsychology, 15, 33–38.
effect of conation on intelligence test scores among Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (2008e). Can neuropsy-
brain-damaged and control subjects. Archives of chological testing produce unequivocal evidence
Clinical Neuropsychology, 19, 29–35. of brain damage? II. Testing for right versus left
Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (2004b). The Trail differences. Applied Neuropsychology, 15, 39–43.
Making Test as an initial screening procedure for Rojas, D. C., & Bennet, T. L. (1995). Single versus
neuropsychological impairment in older children. composite score discriminative validity with the
Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19, 281–288. Halstead–Reitan Battery and the Stroop Test in
Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (2004c). Use of the mild head injury. Archives of Clinical Neuropsy-
Progressive Figures Test in evaluating brain- chology, 10, 101–110.
damaged children. Archives of Clinical Neuropsy- Strauss, A. A., & Werner, H. (1941). Disorders of con-
chology, 19, 305–312. ceptual thinking in the brain-injured child. Journal
Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (2005). The effect of of Nervous and Mental Diseases, 96, 153–172.
conation in determining the differential variance Wheeler, L., Burke, C. J., & Reitan, R. M. (1963). An
among brain-damaged and non-brain-damaged application of discriminant functions to the prob-
persons across a broad range of neuropsycholog- lem of predicting brain damage using behavioral
ical tests. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 20, variables. Perceptual and Motor Skills, [Monograph
957–966. supplement], 16, 417–440.
Reitan, R. M. & Wolfson, D. (2008a). The use of Wheeler, L., & Reitan, R. M. (1962). The presence
serial testing to identify young children in need and laterality of brain damage predicted from
of comprehensive neuropsychological evaluation. responses to a short Aphasia Screening Test.
Applied Neuropsychology, 15, 1–10. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 15, 783–799.
2

The Analytical Approach to


Neuropsychological Assessment
Pat McKenna and Elizabeth K. Warrington

Today, it is taken for granted that, of all human theory of brain function and the concomitant
biological systems, the brain represents the limitations of clinical tests.
most complex and highly organized—indeed, The present academic climate is far more
the quest to map its organization is a twenty- optimistic: developments in neuropsycholog-
first-century challenge, resulting in an explo- ical research are accelerating, and we are now
sion of fMRI studies mapping brain–behavior attempting to incorporate the new levels of
correlates. This was not always the case. Until understanding into an expanding battery of
the late 1950s, clinicians felt forced to concede more efficient and specific tests of brain func-
that the brain was characterized by homogene- tion. These developments have resulted from
ity of function. Less pervasive equipotentialist a method that combines traditional neurolog-
thinking can still be found, but this is con- ical observation with the modern empiricism
fined to selective aspects of organization such of cognitive psychology, which we claim over-
as proposing that semantic representation is comes the very real barrier posed by the notions
dependent on neural networks spread over a of equipotentiality. The analytic approach and
distributed (and large) area of the cerebrum. For theoretical orientation outlined in this chapter
the most part, however, clinicians and neurosci- is one that has arisen from our practice as clin-
entists agree about the broad functional special- ical neuropsychologists and is based on intense
ization and localization along the dimensions of and regular clinical work with patients. However,
language, perception, memory, movement, and many clinical neuropsychologists practice with-
executive function. out a coherent theoretical background and still
Beyond this point, however, any further inves- use a cookbook, test-based approach, acting as
tigation encounters diverse schools of thought, a test administrators. Often, few tests are used and
proliferation of documented syndromes of cog- performance on these tests may rely on the integ-
nitive disorders, and an unwieldy empirical lit- rity of many different cognitive functions. The
erature without, more often than not, any clear method of interpretation relies on normative data
theoretical basis, and certainly not one that is and there is little room for qualitative features or
shared throughout the field. This state of the sci- improvisation in the use of clinical materials, nor
ence clearly limits the contribution of clinical of experience of the examiner. The test is deter-
neuropsychologists to the diagnosis, assessment, minative, not the examiner, so the conclusion
and treatment of patients because the efficiency may just be that “there is a 67% chance of abnor-
of clinical tools must ultimately depend on the mality,” rather than a reasoned, clinical opinion
progress of research. Indeed, Yates (1954) and such as “assessment reveals clear features of the
Piercy (1959) very clearly described the frustra- early stages of a semantic dementia.”
tion of being a clinician at this stage of devel- Until the 1970s, clinical tests fell into one of
opment and lamented the lack of an adequate two categories. First, experimental psychologists

25
26 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

provided formal tests based on global facets of most commonly observed in a patient’s inabil-
cognitive behavior along gross dimensions such ity to draw, is often described as arising from
as aptitudes and intelligence. These tests were either left or right hemisphere damage. It now
really measures of complex behavioral skills— seems clear that such deficits arising as a result
the final orchestrated result of many differ- of right hemisphere damage are secondary to
ent cognitive functions. They were originally impaired space perception, which precludes the
intended for, and far better suited to, group ability to draw, whereas left hemisphere lesions
studies within the normal population. Second, give rise to primary praxis deficits, the inability
a “hunch” led the more innovative clinicians to carry out purposeful voluntary movements
to improvise test stimuli to collect samples or to perform these in the correct sequence
of behavior that indicated more skill-specific (McCarthy & Warrington, 1990; Warrington,
difficulties in particular patients. This lat- 1969).
ter approach underlies the anecdotal evidence Some of the most persuasive evidence for
of neurologists who, though providing new hemisphere specialization comes from the rela-
insights, could not progress without formal tests tionship between perception and meaning. It
to validate and replicate results. Our method is has been shown that the post–Rolandic regions
a synthesis of both the empirical and intuitive of the right hemisphere appear to be critical for
styles and is one that started to evolve during visuospatial and perceptual analysis, whereas
the 1960s and still continues to do so. In this the post–Rolandic regions of the left hemi-
chapter, we attempt to describe how this meth- sphere are implicated in the semantic analysis
odology has affected the theoretical orientation, of perceptual input. Furthermore, if one accepts
research techniques, and clinical tests resulting this differentiation of modalities, certain con-
from our approach, and finally to explain their troversies are resolved. For example, a deficit in
application to our assessment of neurological word comprehension (e.g., not understanding
and neuropsychiatric patients. We have not the word “cat” when one hears it) is generally
attempted to be comprehensive and have not acknowledged to be a predominantly left hemi-
discussed, for example, action systems nor sphere dysfunction, but the visual equivalent,
systems that support propositional speech. visual object agnosia (e.g., being able to see a
cat perfectly well but no longer recognizing
what it is), is often denied (Riddoch et al., 1988),
Theoretical Orientation
ignored (Caramazza & Hillis, 1990), or implic-
Evidence is rapidly accumulating to show that itly attributed to the right hemisphere (Farah,
the organization of cognitive functions is more 1990). This state of affairs provides enormous
complex than has hitherto been supposed. scope for clinicians and researchers alike to
Beyond the first sensory levels of analysis, the communicate at cross-purposes and, like the
cumulative evidence for higher stages of infor- constructional apraxia example above, illus-
mation processing has not revealed any par- trates the conceptual and terminological con-
allel processing between the hemispheres but, fusion that often serves to fuel and perpetuate
instead, increasingly points to their independent controversies in the literature.
organization and specialization. This appears The degree and complexity of specialization of
to be particularly applicable to the temporal brain functioning are even more striking when
and parietal lobes, those areas that subserve one investigates a particular cognitive system.
functions with which we have made the most The following sections outline some of the evi-
progress. The focus of this research has been on dence for the delineation of complex behaviors
memory (short-term, semantic, and event), per- into systems and their subsystems and illustrate
ception and reading and writing, and executive that the deficits witnessed in patients with cere-
processes, as well as how these systems interre- bral pathology can be analyzed in terms of a
late. The benefits of a commitment to the theory greater degree of functional specialization than
of cerebral specialization are already evident has hitherto been supposed.
in the analysis of complex neurological syn- A major consequence of this theoretical ori-
dromes. For example, constructional apraxia, entation has been a reappraisal of the role of
The Analytical Approach to Neuropsychological Assessment 27

traditional neurological syndromes—which for diagnosis and assessment, the ultimate


tend to be clusters of commonly occurring aim being to provide an exhaustive battery of
symptoms in neurological patients—as the function- and subfunction-specific tests.
basis for research. This basis of classification Shallice (1979, 1988) has provided a full dis-
may reflect no more than the facts of anatomy, cussion of the single case study approach, but
such as the distribution of the arterial system, for the purpose of this chapter it will suffice to
and may contribute little to the understanding say that given a patient with an observed defi-
of the cerebral organization of the components cit that appears to be selective (to a system or
of complex skills. The commonly adopted subsystem) and is consistent and quantifiably
strategy of comparing patients with Broca’s significant, then a series of exhaustive experi-
and Wernicke’s aphasia is a clear example. ments can be prepared and repeated to specify
The traditional syndromes of language break- the nature and extent of the deficit. One impor-
down are now seen to fractionate. As an early tant aspect of single case study methodology is
example, we found conduction aphasia to be the notion of dissociation. For example, given
a double deficit of at least two partially unre- a patient who has a specific difficulty in read-
lated functions—articulation and short-term ing abstract words as opposed to concrete ones
memory (Shallice & Warrington, 1977a). The (Shallice & Warrington, 1975), the conclusion
already quoted example of constructional that concrete and abstract words are organized
apraxia is a further illustration of how dif- separately requires the prediction that it is
ferent sets of components can give rise to the equally possible to observe the reverse deficit,
same traditional neurological syndrome, and such that a patient cannot read concrete words
while it is understandable how neurologists but can read abstract words (Warrington,
came to give the same label to such fundamen- 1981a). Thus, for any particular hypothesis of
tally different deficits there is little justification functional organization, it is possible to draw
for neuropsychologists to perpetuate the con- up a table of predictions of double or even triple
fusion. A syndrome should now be function dissociations (Warrington, 1979). Without the
based rather than symptom based and should use of single case studies, it would be impossible,
serve to elucidate the nature of a neurobehav- or extremely difficult, to progress in mapping
ioral system or one of its subsystems. out the organization of cognitive skills.
In summary, it is our experience that cogni- The second, or intermediate, stage in our
tive functions can best be studied and under- research is, when appropriate, to prepare a
stood by an information-processing approach series of tests based on the results of a single case
to the analysis of a complex skill into its func- study for a group study to provide information
tional components and subcomponents. This on lateralization, localization, and frequency of
approach has resulted in a commitment to the observation in the clinical population. For
a theory of differentiation and localization example, having described single incidents of
between and within cognitive functions that material-specific deficits of topographical and
overrides notions of equipotentiality. face recognition (Whiteley & Warrington, 1977,
1978), a consecutive series of patients with right
hemisphere lesions were tested using the same
Research Methods
stimulus materials to determine the frequency
There are three stages in our approach: first, the of these dissociations (Warrington, 1982).
use of a single case study to observe and doc- The third stage aims to standardize tests that
ument properties of a neurological syndrome have been successfully validated in order to pro-
or cognitive deficit; second, the validation of vide more appropriate tools for clinical use in
significant findings in appropriate clinical the diagnosis and assessment of cognitive defi-
groups to test their pragmatic strength in terms cits. Examples of these standardized tests in the
of frequency of occurrence, detectability, and areas of perception, semantics, literacy, mem-
their localization value; and third, the harness- ory, and reasoning will be described in the fol-
ing of results of these validation studies to new lowing sections, all of which have evolved from
tests that have greater specificity and sensitivity our analytical research investigations.
28 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

Clinical Testing of Cognitive based formulae of cognitive domains contained


Functions in the WAIS-III (e.g., Perceptual Organizational
Index and Working Memory Index). The sub-
test Digit Symbol Coding is a Performance
Intelligence and General Factors
subtest but is directly influenced by a left pari-
Despite our increasing awareness of and sen- etal lesion resulting in acquired dyslexia. Thus,
sitivity to individual variation in strengths blind following of the full WAIS-III formula-
and weaknesses of different cognitive skills, it tion could produce a false picture of left versus
is undoubtedly the case that patients can still right hemisphere functioning or even verbal
usefully be screened according to the general versus nonverbal functioning given the medley
level of their intellectual ability. Though age of both verbal and nonverbal skills seen in some
affects many skills to their detriment, an indi- tests within both the Verbal and Performance
vidual’s intelligence level remains constant in Scales. We have traditionally evolved a short
relation to his age group. Furthermore, in any form, which minimizes these errors and on
given individual, levels of performance in dif- which we base correlational data with evolving
ferent aspects of cognitive behavior will tend tests. Whereas before, we would have used the
to be more similar than not. Many clinical Raven’s Matrices for a purer test of nonverbal
neuropsychologists use the concept of IQ as ability (Raven, 1960), a similar test has been use-
measured by the various reformulations of the fully incorporated within the WAIS-III. Kaplan
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) for a and her colleagues (Kaplan, 1988; Kaplan et al.,
preliminary overview of the patient (Wechsler, 1991) also emphasize a qualitative interpreta-
1955, 1981, 1997). Though it is an example of tion, which they term a “process approach,” to
a test and subtests that are sampling patterns the interpretation of the WAIS-III. Recognizing
of skills rather than specific functions, it is a need for tests of general intellectual ability
able to give a rough guide to some of the more within verbal and nonverbal domains that do
commonly occurring functional deficits. In our not rely on rich motor or articulatory response,
view, the full version of the test has not proved we standardized and validated a test of induc-
helpful in the neuropsychological setting. For tive reasoning that has parallel verbal and
example, Information and Comprehension spatial forms (Langdon & Warrington, 1995).
are considered to be tests of general and social At this general level of clinical assessment,
knowledge respectively and thus too culturally the overriding and growing problem is to detect
determined to be helpful in terms of brain– an incipient decline in intellectual powers over
behavior correlates. Within the shortened ver- and above the aging process, and often in the
sion of the WAIS-III that we utilized in prior presence of depression. Our efforts to provide
work, there are patterns of subtest results that some indication of a premorbid level of func-
can help highlight a lateralized and localized tioning have resulted in a formula based orig-
deficit (Warrington et al., 1986). For instance, inally on the Schonell Reading Test, which
a much reduced Digit Span backwards, com- can predict optimal level of functioning up
pared to forwards, indicates an executive to IQ 115, and in the National Adult Reading
difficulty and if this is accompanied by a selec- Test (NART), which has a higher ceiling of
tive difficulty in Similarities and/or Picture IQ 131 (Nelson, 1992; Nelson & McKenna,
Arrangement, the evidence for a dysexecutive 1975). These tests were made viable in the first
syndrome is even greater. A disproportionate instance on the finding that reading vocabu-
difficulty with Picture Completion can indi- lary is IQ related (reinforcing the point made
cate a semantic processing deficit implicating above that performances on different tests tend
the left temporal lobe, even though Picture to be correlated) and that reading is one of the
Completion is a subtest within the Performance most resistant skills in any process of cognitive
Scale. Selective difficulty with Arithmetic and decline (Paque & Warrington, 1995). The NART
Digit Span may highlight a left parietal dys- resulted from research on dyslexic syndromes
function. Experience based on clinical practice that showed word knowledge to be essential for
and observation may override the statistically reading irregularly spelled words.
The Analytical Approach to Neuropsychological Assessment 29

Of note, the revised versions of WAIS (e.g., a lesion in the contralateral hemisphere (Cole
WAIS-III) has followed this methodology to et al., 1962). This also appears to be the case
produce the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading for color imperception (Albert et al., 1975). The
(WTAR). inference from such observations is that the
functions of the secondary visual cortex, as is
the case for the primary visual cortex, maintain
Visual Perception
a retinotopic organization. Thus, there appears
We are often so preoccupied with the complexity to be no lateralization at this stage of visual anal-
of meaning that it renders us insensitive to our ysis, and the identification of such deficits with
remarkable (and probably more perfected) skill “free” vision indicates bilateral dysfunction.
in organizing our visual world. This is in spite of This level of visual analysis is automatic and
there being a comparable, if not greater, area of subconscious, which is why those people who
brain serving the visual function. Our evidence have sudden brain disturbances can be unaware
indicates that the perceptual systems are capable that they cannot see properly. In mild cases, the
of equating diverse percepts of a single stimulus individual may go to the optician only to be told
object and of categorizing certain visual stimuli their acuity is normal and they do not need new
before, and independently of, any investment of glasses. To screen for difficulties at this level
meaning in the percept. Should this appear par- of analysis, we produced the Cortical Vision
adoxical, it is only because semantic identifica- Screening Test (CORVIST, James et al., 2001).
tion is the more conscious aspect of the process This battery consists of 10 screening tests that
and the essential criterion of intelligent behav- focus on occipital lobe damage. As this taps the
ior. Our evidence points to two distinct stages sensory automatic level of processing, no norma-
of visual perception prior to semantic analysis, tive data were considered necessary as normal
and individual case studies show dissociations subjects would be expected to score at ceiling.
between and within all three stages. The more In the more severe cases, which often occur in
complex processes of the second stage of percep- the context of sudden and severe brain injury,
tual analysis appear to be functions lateralized it is as if the higher levels of more conscious
to the right hemisphere. Two major systems have analysis interpret the deranged input sensibly
been identified—one subserving space percep- but, inevitably, incorrectly. These people think
tion and the other subserving form perception. they can see but become distressed when this is
The overriding conclusion from research to date put to the test and they become aware that they
is that these two classes of deficit can dissociate cannot see an object placed in front of them.
(for review see McCarthy & Warrington, 1990). This phenomenon of denial of cortical blind-
Furthermore, recent findings suggest that each ness is termed Anton’s syndrome and is often,
of these may fractionate into subcomponents. incorrectly in our opinion, thought to represent
a psychiatric syndrome.
Early Visual Processing
Space Perception
Before implicating a deficit at the level of cat-
egorical perception, it is necessary to establish The concept of space perception implies more
that visual analysis is intact. It is known that than the location of a single point in space; it
lesions of the primary and secondary visual implies the integration of successive or simul-
cortex give rise to, in the first place, impaired taneous stimuli in a spatial schema. The essen-
brightness and acuity discrimination and, in tial principle guiding our methods of testing
the second, deficits of color, contour, and loca- for spatial disorders is that the involvement of
tion. Visual disorientation, sufficiently marked other cognitive skills be minimized—in partic-
to be a handicap in everyday life, is invariably ular, praxis skills, including drawing. The test
associated with bilateral lesions. However, more we have evolved, the Visual Object and Space
detailed investigation has revealed unilateral Perception (VOSP) battery (Warrington &
visual disorientation can occur just within the James, 1991), has incorporated four such tests
right or left half field of vision in patients with ranging in level of difficulty from counting
30 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

scattered dots to fine position discrimination Taylor, 1978), even though they may not be able
(the subject is required to make a fine compari- to identify the stimulus item. A final set of tests
son of two locations). The ability to perform such that manipulate the angle of view of silhouettes
tests has been shown to be selectively impaired of objects was derived from these earlier studies
in patients with right parietal lesions (Taylor & and is also incorporated in the VOSP. A further
Warrington, 1973). The more abstract facility of test, based on similar principles of departure
spatial imaging is assessed in the Cube Analysis from the prototype, using letters as stimuli, is
test. This test, derived from an early version also included in the VOSP. In addition to pro-
of the Stanford Binet battery, was first applied viding a more comprehensive measure of form
clinically by O. L. Zangwill at the National perception, discrepancies in the performance of
Hospital, and later incorporated in a group some individuals alerted us to the possibility of
study of patients with unilateral lesions and of further fractionation of this perceptual func-
right and left hemisphere lesions. Patients with tion, namely, the material selectivity of percep-
right parietal lesions got the highest error score tual categorization.
(Warrington & Raven, 1970). Thus, this test was
incorporated in the VOSP. The degree to which
The Semantic System
this test also implicates executive functioning is
still to be determined. Our phylogenetic and ontogenetic develop-
ment as primates would support the primacy
of vision as our dominant sense and the notion
Form Perception
that we first master the world in visual terms
This stage of visual perceptual analysis— before the verbal. Language, or linguistics, is a
postsensory but presemantic—is difficult to second-order development, which arises from
conceptualize and is best introduced by the our visual understanding and is intimately con-
research findings that led it to being postulated. nected with it. Visual and verbal semantics,
First, we have shown that although some patients which we believe to be dissociable, are nonethe-
were able to identify and name prototypical less closely connected and form the major part
views of common objects, they were signifi- of the semantic system. Experimental psycholo-
cantly impaired in identifying the same object gists have also found it theoretically necessary
from an unfamiliar orientation or less typical to differentiate semantics and word retrieval in
view (Warrington & Taylor, 1973). In a further research on language, and there is much evi-
experiment, it was shown that when a prototyp- dence to show that these are dissociable aspects
ical view (equivalent to a flat two-dimensional of language in aphasic patients. Finally, we shall
side view) was paired with a less usual view complete this section with a discussion on liter-
patients with a right parietal lesion were unable acy skills, a third-order development.
to judge whether the two had the same physical We take the view that verbal semantics is
identity (Warrington & Taylor, 1978). It was on built on the bedrock of visual semantics and
the basis of these studies that an unusual-view with development and usage achieves a degree
photograph test was devised. Comparing per- of independence. Thus, there will be references
ception of objects, faces, letters, and buildings to visual semantics in this section where appro-
in 50 patients with right hemisphere lesions, we priate. The function of the semantic system is to
have observed 9 of the possible 12 single dissoci- process visual and auditory percepts at the level
ations (Warrington, 1982). Again, these deficits of meaning. It is not a reasoning system but a
are all held to be at a postsensory but preseman- store of concepts, perhaps analogous to a the-
tic perceptual processing stage, lateralized to saurus or encyclopedia. In the domain of non-
the right hemisphere. Furthermore, it has been verbal knowledge systems in humans, the visual
shown in both individual cases and group stud- modality is by far the most important as opposed
ies that patients with unilateral lesions of the left to touch, taste, or smell. Extensive knowledge of
hemisphere resulting in impaired semantic pro- the visual world is a very early acquisition and,
cessing can do these tests in a relatively normal almost certainly, precedes verbal knowledge.
manner (e.g., Warrington, 1975; Warrington & However, language, par excellence, illustrates a
The Analytical Approach to Neuropsychological Assessment 31

system that subserves meaning. Our approach anomias for objects, symbols, colors, and body
to investigating and assessing the verbal seman- parts can occur, and our own growing evidence
tic system has been to focus on single-word from individual case studies would add proper
comprehension, thus mirroring an early man- names with further subdivision of people and
ifestation of language acquisition. place names (McKenna & Warrington, 1978,
Our evidence to date suggests that there can 1980), animate and inanimate, and concrete
be a double dissociation between deficits of and abstract dimensions (Warrington, 1981b;
visual and verbal knowledge, indicating that Warrington & McCarthy, 1987; Warrington &
the semantic system fractionates into at least Shallice, 1984). We would interpret these data
two independent modality-specific systems and in terms of the categorical organization of the
that they are both associated with damage to the semantic knowledge systems.
posterior dominant hemisphere (for a review see These categorical effects are also observed
Gainotti, 2007; McKenna & Warrington, 1993). at the level of word retrieval as if the organiza-
This conclusion, is based on evidence from tion of the semantic systems provides a blue-
(1) patients with intact visual representations print for organization of the lexical output
of a concept but not its verbal representation; systems. Selective impairments of relatively fine
(2) patients with intact verbal representations of grain categories have been observed in word
a concept but not its visual representation; and retrieval tasks in patients in whom comprehen-
(3) studies of patients in whom both verbal and sion is claimed to be intact (Farah & Wallace,
visual representatives of a concept are intact but 1992; Hillis & Caramazza, 1991; McKenna &
a disconnection between the two systems (optic Warrington, 1980).
aphasia) can be demonstrated (e.g., Lhermitte & In summary, it is held that there are modality-
Beauvois, 1973; Manning & Campbell, 1992). specific semantic systems that are hierarchically
Furthermore, the emergence of patients with and categorically organized (Warrington &
specific-category loss restricted to one modal- McCarthy, 1987). This is evidenced in the pat-
ity has further strengthened the argument for tern of difficulties that patients have in visual
multiple representations of concepts in separate recognition, verbal comprehension, and word
modality stores (e.g., Kartsounis & Shallice, retrieval. Our understanding of these systems
1996; McCarthy & Warrington, 1988). is still too incomplete as yet to other than spec-
Within these two domains, the predomi- ulate on the range of modality-specific subsys-
nant and recurrent findings are a hierarchical tems and categories and on their interaction
organization and category specificity. These with episodic memory, reasoning, and linguis-
findings derive from two lines of investiga- tics. However, the disproportionate difficulty in
tion. The first draws on evidence of the pattern later life of learning new “facts” and skills com-
of loss of conceptual knowledge in patients, pared with the recall of ongoing “events” would
where it is shown that superordinate informa- possibly be explained in maturational terms by
tion is often relatively preserved. The order of the capacity of the semantic system to reach its
the loss of conceptual knowledge appears to be asymptote by the time adulthood is attained.
constant, going from the particular to the gen- The clinical relevance of our findings is two-
eral. Thus, a canary can be identified as living, fold. First, at a conceptual level, it has enabled us
animal, and a bird but not as yellow, small, and to differentiate and delineate a deficit of seman-
a pet. These effects have been demonstrated for tic processing, as opposed to sensory or percep-
visual as well as verbal knowledge and also for tual processing, and a deficit of word retrieval
comprehension of the written word (Hodges in the context of intact semantic processing.
et al., 1995; Warrington, 1975; Warrington & Second, new tests have been developed based on
Shallice, 1979). Second, our investigations sug- our findings. A simple two-choice word com-
gest that the selective impairment of particular prehension test was included within the embry-
semantic categories in comprehension tasks onic Coughlan and Warrington (1978) language
occurs much more commonly than has hith- battery that was validated in patients with uni-
erto been supposed. It has long been accepted lateral cerebral lesions. A new abstract and con-
in the neurological literature that selective crete synonym test was developed to improve
32 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

and expand on this first attempt (Warrington to match photographs of objects by physical
et al., 1998). Further justification for this disso- and functional identity (Warrington & Taylor,
ciation between abstract and concrete concepts 1978) and by the Object Decision Test of the
is now reported. Crutch and Warrington (2005) VOSP, a test that requires the viewer to recog-
describe very different organizational principles nize which black shape is actually a real object.
for concrete and abstract vocabularies inas- More recently, we have developed a within-
much as a concrete vocabulary is organized by modality test of semantic knowledge. This test
similarity (categories) and abstract vocabularies probes attribute knowledge (size and weight)
are organized by associative links. of animals and objects, separately, in the visual
The proper names/common nouns disso- and verbal domains (Warrington & Crutch,
ciation led to the development of our graded- 2007). This combination of standardized clini-
difficulty naming test that has a matched cal tests and research techniques can thus pro-
graded-difficulty proper nouns naming test vide an extensive array of methods with which
(McKenna & Warrington, 1980). The evidence to explore semantic deficits.
of category dissociations within the concrete
world vocabulary led to the development of
Literacy
four matched category-specific naming tasks
incorporating two categories of natural kinds Neurologists have long since identified two
(30 animals and 30 fruits/vegetables) and two major syndromes of reading disorders: dys-
categories of man-made objects, one with 30 lexia with dysgraphia and dyslexia without
objects that reflect the use of culturally skilled dysgraphia. The value of this distinction was to
movements and one with 30 objects that have no acknowledge some independence of the read-
particular skilled movements involved in their ing and writing skills, but they did not succeed
use. Comprehension of these items is also tested in developing this taxonomy further. Since the
in a word–picture matching paradigm. Group 1960s, there has been a consistent and lively
studies on patients with left hemisphere pathol- academic interest in this area. Patients with
ogy have provided feedback on the incidence unique reading and writing difficulties have
in our clinical population of category-specific been investigated using experimental methods,
impairment (McKenna, 1997; McKenna & Parry, and a detailed analysis of their deficits is yield-
1994a, 1994b). Equally striking category-specific ing a coherent perspective of the organization
deficits have been reported in a series of pati- of these skills.
ents with schizophrenia (Laws et al., 2006). With regard to reading, it has been suggested
Most recently, we have standardized two fur- that acquired dyslexia can arise from “periph-
ther category-naming and word–picture match- eral” or “central” deficits. Peripheral dyslexias
ing tests. One, an easy test, probes a very basic share the property of failing to achieve a visual
vocabulary (10 items from 5 categories), and the word-form (the integrity of the pattern or gestalt
other probes an intermediate vocabulary with provided by the written word, or part thereof)
again 10 items from 5 categories (Crutch et al., at a purely visual level of analysis. These include
2007). (1) neglect dyslexia, characterized by letter
One of the limitations of picture identifica- substitutions at either end of the word, usu-
tion tests for comprehension is that they are ally the beginning (Kinsbourne & Warrington,
clearly cross-modality tests and cannot provide 1962; Warrington, 1991), (2) attentional dys-
an independent assessment of either visual or lexia, when a single letter can be read but not if
verbal knowledge. Although a formal clinical accompanied by other letters in the visual field
battery of visual semantic tests is not available, (Shallice & Warrington, 1977b; Warrington
the Pyramids and Palm Trees Test can be used to et al., 1993), and (3) the commonly witnessed
compare visual and verbal semantics (Howard & word-form or spelling dyslexia, characterized
Patterson, 1992). The differentiation of a deficit by letter-by-letter reading resulting in greater
at the level of perceptual processing and seman- difficulty reading words written in script than
tic processing entirely within the visual domain in print (Warrington & Langdon, 1994, 2002;
can be tested by comparing the patient’s ability Warrington & Shallice, 1980). We consider
The Analytical Approach to Neuropsychological Assessment 33

these difficulties do not reflect general proper- be commonly the case as found by McKenna
ties of the perceptual system but are specific to and Parry (1994b). For example, a person with
the reading system (for review see Behrmann such a deficit was unable to understand spoken
et al., 1998). names but understand written names of fruit
Central dyslexias describe an inability to and vegetables, while all other categories were
derive meaning from the written word given normally processed in both spoken and written
intact visual analysis of it. There is little dis- forms. Another was able to understand spoken
agreement in the present literature that there names of man-made objects but not their written
appear to be two main reading routes—the pho- names, while simultaneously not understand-
nological and the semantic. These are the inev- ing the spoken names of natural kinds (fruit
itable inferences from characteristics from two and vegetables and animals) but understanding
classes of acquired dyslexias. In the first type, their written names (a double dissociation).
there is complete, or near complete, dependence Following these advances in research on dys-
on the use of phonology for reading. Thus, lexia, data have emerged from investigations on
the patient can read regular words (those that dysgraphic patients that show a similar organi-
use commonly occurring grapheme–phoneme zation for writing (Baxter & Warrington, 1987).
correspondences) but is unable to read irreg- A single case study of a patient who could not
ular words: the greater the deviation from the write irregular words but could write phono-
regular grapheme–phoneme correspondence, logically regular words, whether real or non-
the greater the difficulty in reading. In these sense words, has been reported (Beauvois &
patients, in whom the direct semantic route is Derouesne, 1979). Evidence for a double disso-
inoperative, the characteristics of the phonolog- ciation between the inferred phonological and
ical route are open to inspection. Though some direct routes to writing has now been found.
patients in this category can apply only the Shallice (1981) has reported a patient who could
most regular, grapheme–phoneme rules, others write both real words and letter names but
show a much more versatile facility, which led could write neither nonsense syllables nor letter
us to believe that the properties of phonologi- sounds.
cal processing were more extensive than at first Graded-difficulty irregular-word reading
thought, such patients being able to use irregu- tests are already available for clinical purposes,
lar rules to some extent (Cipolotti & Warrington, as described earlier, and a comparable graded-
1995; McCarthy & Warrington, 1986; Shallice difficulty irregular spelling test is also available
et al., 1983). Formal tests of nonword reading (Baxter & Warrington, 1994). The assessment of
are now available (e.g., Snowling). nonsense-word reading (Snowling et al., 1996)
In the second type of central dyslexia, there and writing together with these two standard
is an inability to use phonology, and words tests is sufficient to identify the majority of
are recognized as units analogous to pictures. acquired central dyslexic syndromes in the neu-
Patients are able to read real words but cannot rological population.
begin to read nonsense words. In the extreme
case, they are unable to sound single letters
Event Memory
or pronounce two-letter combinations. This
type of dyslexic has a relatively (sometimes Psychologists and lay people alike often use
completely) intact semantic route, such that the concepts of knowing and remembering
the visual word-form has direct access to ver- interchangeably. Even repeating a heard word,
bal semantic systems (Beauvois & Derouesne, phrase or sentence is also implicated in com-
1979; Orpwood & Warrington, 1995). The monsense ideas of “being able to remember.”
properties and characteristics of the semantic Among psychologists, both experimental and
route can be investigated in patients in whom clinical, short-term memory is now generally
the phonological route is inoperative and there acknowledged to be an independent and dis-
has been partial damage to the semantic route. sociable system that can be conceptualized as a
For example, category specificity has been limited-capacity system that “holds” auditory–
observed (Warrington, 1981a) or indeed may verbal information in an acoustic/phonological
34 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

code for very short time durations. In this case, and consequently the assessment of new learn-
the term “memory” is a misnomer, and it is bet- ing and retention over short recall intervals is
ter conceptualized as a measure of the present appropriate and sufficient to document event
(or at least, “memory for the here and now”). memory impairment. This strategy has the
More controversial is the relationship between additional advantage that artifacts such as dif-
“knowing” and “remembering.” Indeed, many ferences in salience of past experiences, inter-
psychologists argue that the difference is one ference during recall intervals, and differences
of degree and that the same cognitive systems in rehearsal activity can be avoided. The subjec-
subserve, for example, “knowing” a word and tive ease with which well-worn memories from
“remembering” who telephoned yesterday. the distant past are continually evoked in older
However, the evidence now supports the view individuals may indicate that they have attained
that memory for facts and memory for events the status of semantic concepts.
are independent processes that can be selec- This fact-like recall was well illustrated in a
tively impaired. The amnesic syndrome is char- patient with global amnesia who would repeat
acterized by an almost total inability to recall or a skeletal account of his main life events such
recognize autobiographical events either before as number of children, education, date of mar-
or since the onset of illness, yet an amnesic’s riage, and so on, in almost identical struc-
memory for other classes of knowledge can be ture whenever asked (Warrington & McCarthy,
on a par with normal subjects, and an amnesic 1988). The same patient displayed intact seman-
can score normally on tests of intelligence. The tic knowledge of famous public figures in the
complementary syndrome, the impairment of sense that he could designate a famous face in
semantic systems, or of memory for facts, has an anonymous group and he could complete
been observed in patients in whom memory the name given a prompt but had no idea of the
for past and present events is relatively well public events that individual was famous for,
preserved. A triple dissociation between these nor have any subjective recognition of the per-
three systems, short-term memory (memory son. Finally, he had not only retained semantic
for the immediate present), memory for facts concepts (e.g., AIDS, Thatcherism) that he had
(semantics), and memory for events, has been acquired during the long period for which he
documented (for review see Warrington, 1979). had lost autobiographical knowledge but had
In this section we discuss only the investigation also learned a new vocabulary that had come
and assessment of event memory. into the language since his illness (McCarthy
Event memory appears to be an indepen- et al., 2005). Another amnesic patient who had
dent system with unique properties for map- no ability to retain ongoing events (McKenna &
ping ongoing experiences on to an individual’s Gerhand, 2002) was able to learn new vocabu-
schemas of events. Contrary to the commonly lary and new objects in the natural world. Thus,
held assumption that memory for remote events on a family holiday in France, he could spon-
is more stable than memory for recent events, taneously point out a mouflon, which he had
Warrington and Sanders (1971) showed that, newly learned, but needed to be accompanied
although memory for events (tested either by at all times in novel surroundings beyond home
recall or recognition) declined with age in nor- and previously known places. These observa-
mal subjects, vulnerability was the same for tions serve to illustrate the dissociation of mem-
both recent and remote events in the older age ory for facts and memory for events.
groups. Similarly, no sparing of remote memo- Numerous investigations have established
ries could be demonstrated in amnesic patients the occurrence of modality-specific memory
(Sanders & Warrington, 1971). Furthermore, deficits. Since the classic studies of Milner
after closed head injuries the severity of the (1966, pp. 109–133) it is widely accepted that
anterograde deficit roughly correlates with the verbal memory deficits arise from unilateral
severity of the retrograde deficit (Schacter & lesions of the left hemisphere and nonverbal
Crovitz, 1977). We would interpret this evi- memory deficits with the right hemisphere. Our
dence as indicating that a unitary memory sys- aim was to develop a test for specific investiga-
tem subserves both recent and remote events, tion of event memory that would incorporate
The Analytical Approach to Neuropsychological Assessment 35

the verbal/nonverbal dichotomy. A recognition amnesic syndrome”—a very severe yet circum-
paradigm was chosen in preference to recall scribed memory impairment. It became increas-
because the former task appears to be much ingly apparent that the amnesic memory deficit
less influenced by executive dysfunction, affec- was neither as absolute nor as dense as either clin-
tive disorders, and the normal aging process; in ical impressions or conventional memory test
addition, identical procedures can be used for results would suggest. For example, strikingly
the separate assessment of verbal and nonver- different results are obtained when retention
bal material. Consequently, a forced two-choice is tested by cueing recall and prompted learn-
recognition memory test for 50 common words ing (now termed “implicit memory”); retention
and 50 unknown faces (previously described by scores can be normal or near normal and learn-
Warrington, 1974, in the context of the analy- ing can occur albeit more slowly than for the
sis of the amnesic syndrome) was standard- normal subject (Warrington & Weiskrantz,
ized to produce the Recognition Memory Test 1968, 1970). First, retention of words tested by a
(Warrington, 1984). The normalized scores pro- yes/no recognition procedure is compared with
vide a quantitative measure of performance that retention tested by cueing recall with the first
can be compared directly with other measures three letters of the word (now termed stem com-
of cognitive skills. Validation studies confirmed pletion). A discrepancy in the level of perfor-
that a right hemisphere group was shown to be mance on these two tasks (cued recall superior
impaired on the face recognition memory test to recognition memory) was interpreted to indi-
but not on the word recognition test. By con- cate a subcortical amnesia. Second, perceptual
trast, the left hemisphere group, although mildly learning was tested by giving repeated trials to
impaired on the face recognition, had a clear- identify fragmented visual stimuli. These tech-
cut deficit on the word recognition test. Perhaps niques are now featured in the latest version of
of greater relevance for the majority of assess- the Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test as the
ment problems was the fact that the test was suf- Implicit Memory Test (Wilson et al., 2007).
ficiently sensitive to detect memory deficits in This approach to the assessment of memory
patients with only a mild degree of atrophy. deficits illustrates the three stages of investi-
Since then, these techniques were extended gation we initially outlined. Analytic research
to produce the Camden Memory Test battery studies led to group studies, which in turn have
(Warrington, 1996), which incorporates shorter led to the development of standardized tests
versions of recognition for words and faces that can be used in a much broader population
and, in addition, tests of recognition memory of neurological and neuropsychiatric patients.
for pictorial materials of naturalistic scenes
and topography as well as a paired associate
Reasoning and Behavior: The
memory test. Furthermore, these methods
Executive System
have been adopted by other researchers to pro-
duce excellent material-specific visual memory The functions underlying our reasoning, judg-
tests (e.g., doors in the Doors and People Test, ment, appropriate social behavior, and organi-
Baddeley et al., 1994). Thus, the findings of a zational skills are those that neuropsychologists
number of investigations have led to the devel- find the most baffling and elusive phenomena to
opment of a test with the discriminative power study. It is not unusual for a patient to recover
to detect minor degrees of modality-specific from frontal lobe treatment to be sent home
memory deficits. An easy version of these tests with no discernable deficit only to be re-referred
is also available for assessment of older sub- a few weeks later with a history of job incompe-
jects (Clegg & Warrington, 1994). Other batter- tence or other atypical behavior. Again, perfor-
ies, which include recall as well as recognition, mance on formal structured tests of cognitive
were modeled and later developed based on this function can be normal. Even more frustrat-
methodology (AMIPB; Coughlan & Hollows, ing for the clinician is to assess the patient’s
1985). behavior subjectively as somewhat “odd” but be
Further characteristics of the event memory unable to be more specific. Blanket terms such
system emerged from investigations of the “pure as “inappropriate,” “apathetic,” “impulsive,” or
36 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

even “disinhibited” behavior gives no clue as to Evans, 1978). More recently, we have added
what neuropsychological process is implicated. a further test of executive function, which
Nevertheless, it is clear that the frontal lobe fine-tunes some aspects of verbal fluency
plays a major, if little understood, part in the (Warrington, 2000).
planning, orchestration, and adaptation of our Further research into different aspects of
cognitive systems in our ongoing behavior, and “novel” manipulation in problem solving using
of the systems subserving affect and initiation. the methodology of group studies has led to
At a theoretical level, neuropsychological the development of the Behavioral Assessment
research has clarified to some extent which of the Dysexecutive Syndrome battery (Wilson
cognitive operations can be eliminated from et al., 1996) and the Hayling and Brixton Tests
the reasoning process. For example, our inves- (Burgess & Shallice, 1997). These tests have
tigation of a patient with acalculia suggests incorporated a more ecologically valid theoret-
that the core deficit was assessing the “facts” of ical basis in that some of the research methods
arithmetic; that is, given that the sum 3 + 2 is were based on experiments that actually took
comprehended, the solution 5 can normally be place in the community (Alderman et al., 2003).
accessed directly, computation being unneces- However, we are still no further forward in iso-
sary (though this presumably need not be the lating the subsystems of executive functioning
case during acquisition). Indeed, the generally distributed within the frontal lobes, nor in a
accepted finding that patients with frontal lobe concomitant theoretically grounded taxonomy.
lesions can perform relatively well on intelli- Instead, we can only describe the type of cog-
gence tests that follow the format of the WAIS-R nitive or behavioral difficulties people experi-
may well be due to its loading on stable, well- ence because of pathology in the frontal lobes.
practiced cognitive skills, which it seems clear Beyond generalities, how a dysexecutive syn-
are subserved by post–Rolandic regions of the drome might reveal itself in particular behav-
cerebral hemisphere. We concur with the gen- iors or test performance in any individual is not
erally held view that impairment of reason- predictable with one exception—that damage to
ing abilities in such patients may only emerge, the suborbital inferior parts of the frontal lobes
but not invariably, with tests that require rel- will affect social behavior whereas damage to
atively novel cognitive strategies. The content the superior lateral areas will affect cognitive
of tests of frontal lobe functioning can also be processes. Traditionally, tests have concen-
a confounding factor if the individual being trated on aspects of cognitive function, and
tested has a primary deficit in processing that only recently are tests of social executive func-
material—for example, testing verbal fluency tioning being explored (e.g., TACIT, Theory of
in an aphasic patient, or attempting the Trail- Mind). Thus, the search for more fine-tuned
Making Test with someone who has spatial dissection of the subcomponents of executive
problems. Such considerations have led Stuss function continues.
et al. (2005) to use very simple displays in reac-
tion time tasks in order to attempt to map the
Differential Diagnosis
different components of basic self-monitoring
and integration of cognitive processing. Most, but not all, of our research efforts are
So at this stage of our understanding, bat- concentrated on the neurological patient pop-
teries are composed of pragmatically validated ulation, which has provided a most beneficial,
materials for localization purposes and cannot if oblique, approach to differential diagnosis in
as yet provide a functional breakdown of the the neuropsychiatric patient group. The funda-
processes that make up reasoning. Examples mental problem posed by this group is to distin-
of such tests are the Weigl Sorting Test (Weigl, guish between impairments of a predominantly
1941), the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (Berg, organic nature and impairments of a predom-
1948), the Stroop Test (Stroop, 1935), the Trail- inantly functional nature. The most obvious
Making Test (Lezak et al., 2004, pp. 371–374), indication is a mismatch between subjective
fluency tests (Lezak et al., 2004, pp. 519–521), complaints and objective performance. A fur-
and the Cognitive Estimates Test (Shallice & ther indication lies in the recurrent theme in
The Analytical Approach to Neuropsychological Assessment 37

all areas of our research—that cognitive sys- intellectual failure. More commonly, the mis-
tems not only fractionate but they do so along match is the reverse; namely, patients and their
dimensions that do not necessarily follow com- relatives complain of failing intellectual and
monsense ideas of what constitutes a function. memory skills, which even after exhaustive test-
Unless the patient is aware of the “rules” of a ing cannot be demonstrated objectively. In the
breakdown, he cannot produce the correct pat- area of word retrieval skills, a failure to show
tern of disability other than on an organic basis. the very robust frequency effects of either accu-
Thus, it becomes more and more possible, with racy or latency would be a very strong indica-
our increasing understanding of cognitive orga- tion for nonorganic factors. A skilled clinician
nization, to differentiate between functional is able to detect a mismatch across tasks in the
and organic components of a symptom. normal course of assessment. For instance, an
Most referrals that touch on this problem apparently impaired performance on the Digits
request a differential diagnosis between depres- Span forwards test can be checked by adminis-
sion and dementia, organic or functional mem- tering the modified version of the Token Test,
ory loss, and investigation of general complaints ostensibly a task of verbal comprehension,
of intellectual inefficiency. One test of memory when the latter test is virtually an impossibil-
described earlier has proved particularly useful ity for patients with a reduced auditory span of
and illustrates the mismatch of common sense attention.
and the “rules” of cognitive breakdown, namely, Many examples of mismatch in abilities are
an implicit learning task. This task makes small now formalized in stand-alone tests for malin-
demands on memory resources, for it has been gering such as the Test of Memory Malingering
shown that patients with dementia and those (Tombaugh, 1996) and the Word Memory Test
with the amnesic syndrome are able to learn (2001). The increasing use of these tests reflects
and retain such information for a relatively long the current trend to automatically screen for
time. A gross impairment on this learning task effort testing in both clinical and medical legal
can be accepted as a strong indication of nonor- assessments.
ganic factors in the context of otherwise normal Sometimes the reverse situation occurs, when
cognitive skills. Similarly, intact performance our knowledge of a cognitive system confirms
on this test effectively eliminates a memory dis- that an apparently bizarre or non-common-
order. The Camden Memory Test also includes sense symptom could indeed have an organic
a deceptively easy test of pictorial recognition base. For example, individual case studies may
of distinctive pictorial scenes at which normal suggest that infrequently there may be a tre-
subjects score at ceiling. A mismatch between ble dissociation in the deficits associated with
performance on this and the graded-difficulty the secondary visual cortices, namely, visual
memory tests is a pointer to memory deficit of analysis of contour, color, and location. Visual
nonorganic origin. A further example from the disorientation, the inability to locate in space, is
amnesic syndrome that has very direct applica- a particularly disabling syndrome that together
tion to this differential diagnosis is the generally with normal acuity may suggest a mismatch
accepted “rule” that the degree of anterograde when in fact none exists.
amnesia is highly correlated with the degree of
retrograde amnesia. Thus, an early finding was
Summary
that patients who present with severe antero-
grade amnesia but no retrograde amnesia do We have outlined our approach to neuropsy-
not conform to any known organic pattern chological assessment that has been develop-
(Pratt, 1977). However, later case reports sug- ing since the 1960s, when an impasse had been
gest that this formula may be too simplistic (e.g., reached in the understanding and measurement
Hodges & McCarthy, 1993). of cognitive impairment. In 1954, Yates claimed
An occasional observed mismatch is the that “a purely empirical approach is unlikely
patient who is alert and able to perform relatively to yield satisfactory results, nor is an approach
normally in a day-to-day situation but obtains based on a theory which has not been adequately
test scores compatible with a gross degree of tested experimentally.” Our strategy attempts
38 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

to use the findings of analytical research either Berg, E. A. (1948). A simple objective technique
from single cases or group studies in a clinical for measuring flexibility in thinking. Journal of
situation by devising more specific tests of cog- General Psychology, 39, 15–22.
nitive function. Thus, we are committed in the Burgess, P. W., & Shallice, T. (1997) The Hayling and
Brixton tests. England, Bury St. Edmunds: Harcourt
first instance to research aimed at furthering
Assessment/The Psychological Corporation.
the understanding of cognitive functions (albeit
Caramazza, A., & Hillis, A. E. (1990). Where do
still at an embryonic stage of development) semantic errors come from? Cortex, 26, 95–122.
and, second, to improved clinical tests based Cipolotti, L., & Warrington, E. K. (1995). Semantic
on this knowledge. We have cited investiga- memory and reading abilities: a case report.
tions that, for the most part, have originated at Journal of International Neuropsychology Society,
the National Hospital, Queen Square, to illus- 1, 104–110.
trate our approach. A full account of the theo- Clegg, F., & Warrington, E. K. (1994). Four easy mem-
retical orientation and the large body of earlier ory tests for older adults. Memory, 2, 167–182.
work emanating from this department is now Cole, M., Schutta, H. S., & Warrington, E. K. (1962).
published (McCarthy & Warrington, 1990). Visual disorientation in homonymous half-fields.
Neurology, 12, 257–263.
However, our theory, methodology, and test
Coughlan, A. K., & Hollows, S. E. (1985). The adult
paradigms also underlie group-specific batteries
memory and information processing battery.,
such as the MEAMS (Golding, 1989) for older Leeds, UK: Psychology Department, St. James’
adults and the Rookwood Driving Assessment University Hospital.
Battery (McKenna & Bell, 2007) for predicting Coughlan, A. K., & Warrington, E. K. (1978). Word-
fitness to drive. However, it is important that comprehension and word retrieval in patients with
our test procedures in no way override our use localized cerebral lesions. Brain, 101, 163–185.
of other available tools and techniques. Indeed, Crutch, S. J., Randlesome, K., & Warrington, E. K.
we are of the opinion that a flexible and eclectic (2007). The variability of country map knowledge
approach is essential for the assessment of neu- in normal and aphasic subjects: Evidence from
ropsychological and neuropsychiatric patients. two new category-specific screening tests. Journal
of Neuropsychology. 1, 171–187.
Crutch, S. J., & Warrington, E. K. (2005). Abstract and
concrete concepts have structurally different repre-
References
sentational frameworks. Brain, 128, 615–627.
Albert, M. L., Reches, A., & Silverberg, R. (1975). Farah, M. J. (1990). Visual agnosia: Disorders of
Hemianopic colour blindness. Journal of Neurology, object recognition and what they tell us about
Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 38, 546–549. normal vision. London: MIT Press.
Alderman N., Burgess, P. W., Knight, C., & Henman, Farah, M. J., & Wallace, M. A. (1992). Semantically
C. (2003). Ecological validity of the multiple bounded anomia: Implications for the neural
errands shopping task. Journal of the International implementation of naming. Neuropsychologia, 30,
Neurological Society, 9, 31–44. 609–621.
Baddeley, A. D., Emslie, H., & Nimmo-Smith, I. Gainotti, G. (2007). The anatomical locus of lesion
(1994). Doors and people test: A test of visual and in category-specific disorders and the format of
verbal recall and recognition. England Bury St underlying conceptual representations. In J. Hart
Edmunds: Thames Valley Test Co. & M. A. Kraut (Eds.), The neural basis of seman-
Baxter, D. M., & Warrington, E. K. (1987). tic memory (pp. 28–61). England Cambridge:
Transcoding sound to spelling: Single or multiple Cambridge University Press.
sound unit correspondence? Cortex, 23, 11–28. Green, P. W., Astner, K., & Allen, L. M. (2001). The
Baxter, D. M., & Warrington, E. K. (1994). Measuring word memory test. Canada: Green’s Publishing.
dysgraphia: A graded-difficulty spelling test Golding, E. (1989). The middlesex elderly assessment
(GDST). Behavioural Neurology, 7, 107–116. of mental states. England: Thames Valley Test Co.
Beauvois, M. F., & Derouesne, J. (1979). Phonological Hillis, A. E., & Caramazza, A. (1991). Category-
alexia: Three dissociations. Journal of Neurology, specific naming and comprehension impairment:
Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 42, 1115–1124. A double dissociation. Brain, 114, 2081–2094.
Behrmann, M., Plaut, D. C., & Nelson, J. (1998). A lit- Hodges, J. R., Graham, N., & Patterson, K. (1995).
erature review and new data supporting an inter- Charting the progression in semantic dementia:
active account of letter-by-letter reading. Cognitive Implications for the organisation of semantic
Neuropsychology, 15, 7–51. memory. Memory, 3, 463–495.
The Analytical Approach to Neuropsychological Assessment 39

Hodges, J. R., & McCarthy, R. A. (1993). Autobiogra- McKenna, P. (1997). The Category Specific Names
phical amnesia resulting from bilateral parame- Test. Cardiff, Wales, UK: Cardiff and Vale NHS
dian thalamic infarction: A case study in cognitive Trust, Psychology Department UK.
neurobiology. Brain, 116, 921–940. McKenna, P., & Bell, V. (2007). Fitness to drive
Howard, D., & Patterson, K. E. (1992). Pyramids and following cerebral pathology: The Rookwood
palm trees. Bury St. Edmunds: Thames Valley Test Driving Battery as a tool for predicting on-road
Company. driving performance. Journal of Neuropsychology,
James, M., Plant, G. T., & Warrington, E. K. (2001). 1, 85–100.
CORVIST: Cortical vision screening test. England, McKenna, P., & Gerhand, S. (2002). Preserved semantic
Bury St. Edmunds: Harcourt Assessment/The Psy- learning in an amnesic patient. Cortex, 38, 37–58.
chological Corporation. McKenna, P., & Parry, R. (1994a). Category spec-
Kaplan, E. (1988). A process approach to neuropsy- ificity in the naming of natural and manmade
chological assessment. In T. Boll & B. K. Bryant objects: Normative data from adults and children.
(Eds.), Clinical Neuropsychology and brain Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 4, 283–305.
function: Research measurement and practice. McKenna, P., & Parry, R. (1994b). Category and
Washington DC: APA. modality deficits of semantic memory in patients
Kaplan, E., Fein, D., Morris, R., & Delis, D. (1991). with left hemisphere pathology. Neuropsychological
WAIS-R as a neuropsychological instrument. San Rehabilitation, 4, 255–281.
Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. McKenna, P., & Warrington, E. K. (1978). Category
Kartsounis, L. D., & Shallice, T. (1996). Modality specific naming preservation: A single case study.
specific semantic knowledge loss for unique items. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry,
Cortex, 32, 109–119. 41, 571–574.
Kinsbourne, M., & Warrington, E. K. (1962). A variety McKenna, P., & Warrington, E. K. (1980). Testing
of reading disabilities associated with right hemi- for nominal aphasia. Journal of Neurology, Neuro-
sphere lesions. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery surgery and Psychiatry, 43, 781–788.
and Psychiatry, 25, 339–344. McKenna, P., & Warrington, E. K. (1993). The neu-
Langdon, D. W., & Warrington, E. K. (1995). VESPAR: ropsychology of semantic memory. In F. Boller &
A verbal and spatial reasoning test. Hove, Sussex: J. Grafsman (Eds.), Handbook of Neuropsychology/
Lawrence Erlbaum. VIII (pp. 193–213). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science
Laws, K. R., Leeson, V. C., & McKenna, P. J. (2006) Publishers.
Domain-specific deficits in schizophrenia. Milner, B. (1966). Amnesia following operation on
Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 11, 537–556. the temporal lobes. In C. W. M. Whitty & O. L.
Lezak, M. D., Howies, D. B., & Loring, D. W. (2004). Zangwell (Eds.), Amnesia (pp. 109–133). London:
Neuropsychological assessment (4th ed.). New Butterworth.
York: Oxford University Press Inc. Nelson, H. E. (1992). The National Adult Reading Test
Lhermitte, F., & Beauvois, M. F. (1973). A visual– Manual/II. Windsor, England: NFER—Nelson
speech disconnection syndrome: Report of a case Publishing Company Limited.
with optic aphasia, agnosia, alexia and colour Nelson, H. E., & McKenna, P. (1975). The use of cur-
agnosia. Brain, 96, 695–714. rent reading ability in the assessment of dementia.
Manning, L., & Campbell, R. (1992). Optic aphasia British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology,
with spared action naming: A description and 14, 259–267.
possible loci of impairment. Neuropsychologia, 30, Orpwood, L., & Warrington, E. K. (1995). Word spe-
587–592. cific impairments in naming and spelling but not
McCarthy, R. A., Kopelman, M. D., & Warrington, E. reading. Cortex, 31, 239–265.
K. (2005). Remembering and forgetting of seman- Paque, L., & Warrington, E. K. (1995). A longitu-
tic knowledge in amnesia: A 16 year follow up of dinal study of reading ability in patients suffer-
RFR. Neuropsychologia, 43, 356–372. ing from dementia. Journal of the International
McCarthy, R. A., & Warrington, E. K. (1986). Neuropsychological Society, 1, 517–524.
Phonological reading: Phenomena and paradoxes. Piercy, M. (1959). Testing for intellectual impairment—
Cortex, 22, 359–380. some comments on the test and testers. Journal of
McCarthy, R. A., & Warrington, E. K. (1988). Evidence Medical Science, 105, 489–495.
from modality-specific meaning systems in the Pratt, R. T. C. (1977). Psychogenic loss of memory. In
brain. Nature, 334, 428–430. C. W. M. Whitty & O. L. Zangwill (Eds.), Amnesia
McCarthy, R. A., & Warrington, E. K. (1990). Cognitive (pp. 224–232). London: Butterworth.
neuropsychology: A clinical introduction. London: Raven, J. C. (1960). Guide to the standard progressive
Academic Press. matrices. London: H. K. Lewis.
40 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

Riddoch, M. J., Humphreys, G. W., Coltheart, M., & Warrington, E. K. (1975). The selective impair-
Funnel, E. (1988). Semantic system or systems? ment of semantic memory. Quarterly Journal of
Neuropsychological evidence re-examined. Cogni- Experimental Psychology A, 27, 635–657.
tive Neuropsychology, 5, 3–25. Warrington, E. K. (1979). Neuropsychological evi-
Sanders, H. I., & Warrington, E. K. (1971). Memory dence for multiple memory systems. In Brain and
for remote events in amnesic patients. Brain, 94, Mind Ciba foundation Series 69 (pp. 153–166).
616–668. Exerpta Medica, Elsevier North Holland.
Schacter, D. L., & Crovitz, H. F. (1977). Memory Warrington, E. K. (1981a). Concrete word dyslexia.
function after closed head injury: A review of the British Journal of Psychology, 72, 175–196.
quantitative research. Cortex, 13, 150–176. Warrington, E. K. (1981b). Neuropsychological
Shallice, T. (1979). Case study approach in neuro- studies of verbal semantic systems. Philosophical
psychological research. Journal of Clinical Neuro- Transactions of the Royal Society of London (B),
psychology, 1, 183–211. 295, 411–423.
Shallice, T. (1981). Phonological agraphia and the Warrington, E. K. (1982). Neuropsychological studies
lexical route in writing. Brain, 104, 413–429. of object recognition. Philosophical Transactions
Shallice, T. (1988). From neuropsychology to men- of the Royal Society of London (B), 298, 15–33.
tal structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Warrington, E. K. (1984). Manual for Recognition
Press. Memory Tests. Windsor, England: NFER—Nelson
Shallice, T., & Evans, M. E. (1978). The involvement Publishing Company Limited.
of the frontal lobes in cognitive estimation. Cortex, Warrington, E. K. (1991). Right neglect dyslexia: A
14, 294–303. single case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8,
Shallice, T., & Warrington, E. K. (1975). Word recog- 193–212.
nition in a phonemic dyslexic patient. Quarterly Warrington, E. K. (1996). The Camden Memory Tests.
Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 27, 187–199. UK: Psychology Press.
Shallice, T., & Warrington, E. K. (1977a). Auditory– Warrington, E. K. (2000) Homophone meaning
verbal short-term memory impairment and con- generation. A new test of verbal switching for the
duction aphasia. Brain and Language, 4, 479–491. detection of frontal lobe dysfunction. Journal of
Shallice, T., & Warrington, E. K. (1977b). The possi- the International Neuropsychological Society, 6,
ble role of selective attention in acquired dyslexia. 643–648.
Neuropsychologia, 15, 31–41. Warrington, E. K., Cipolotti, L., & McNeil, J.
Shallice, T., Warrington, E. K., & McCarthy, R. A. (1993). Attentional dyslexia: A single case study.
(1983). Reading without semantics. Quarterly Neuropsychologia, 31, 871–855.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662. Warrington, E. K., & Crutch, S. J. (2007). A within-
Snowling, M., Stothard, S., & McLean, J. (1996). modality test of semantic knowledge: The size/
Graded Non-word Reading Test. Bury St. Edmonds: weight attribute test. Neuropsychology, 21, 803–811.
Thames Valley Test Co. U.K. Warrington, E. K., & James, M. (1991). The Visual
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in Object and Space Perception battery. Bury St.
serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Edmunds: Thames Valley Test Company.
Psychology, 18, 643–662. Warrington, E. K., James, M., & Maciejewski, C.
Stuss, D. T., Alexander, M. P., Shallice, T., Picton, (1986). The WAIS as a lateralising and localising
T. W., Binns, M. A., Macdonald, R., et al. (2005). diagnostic instrument. A study of 656 patients
Multiple frontal systems controlling response with unilateral cerebral lesions. Neuropsychologia,
speed. Neuropsychologia, 43, 396–417. 24, 223–239.
Taylor, A. M., & Warrington, E. K. (1973). Visual Warrington, E. K., & Langdon, D. (1994). Spelling
discrimination in patients with localized cerebral dyslexia: A deficit of the visual word-form. Journal
lesions. Cortex, 9, 82–93. of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 57,
Tombaugh, T. N. (1996). Test of Memory Malingering. 211–216.
New York: Multi-Health Systems, Inc. Warrington, E. K., & Langdon, D. W. (2002). Does
Warrington, E. K. (1969). Constructional Apraxia. the spelling dyslexic read by recognising orally
In P. J. Vinkin & G. W. Bruyn (Eds.), Handbook spelled words? An investigation of a letter-by-
of clinical neurology/IV (pp. 66–83). Amsterdam: letter reading. Neurocase, 8, 210–218.
Elsevier Science Publishers. Warrington, E. K., & McCarthy, R. A (1987). Categories
Warrington, E. K. (1974). Deficient recognition of Knowledge: Further fractionations and an
memory in organic amnesia. Cortex, 10, 289–291. attempted interpretation. Brain, 110, 1273–1296.
The Analytical Approach to Neuropsychological Assessment 41

Warrington, E. K., & McCarthy, R. A (1988). The Wechsler, D. (1955). The Wechsler Adult Intelligence
fractionation of retrograde amnesia. Brain and Scale: Manual. New York: Psychological Corporation.
Cognition, 7, 184–200. Wechsler, D. (1981). WAIS-R Manual: The Wechsler
Warrington, E. K., McKenna, P., & Orpwood, L. Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised. New York:
(1998). Single word comprehension: A concrete and Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovich.
abstract word synonym test. Neuropsychological Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-
Rehabilitation, 8, 143–154. III. New York: The Psychological Corporation.
Warrington, E. K., & Raven, P. (1970). Perceptual Weigl, E. (1941). On the psychology of so-called pro-
matching in patients with cerebral lesions. cesses of abstraction. Journal of Abnormal and
Neuropsychologia, 8, 475–487. Social Psychology, 36, 3–33.
Warrington, E. K., & Sanders, H. I. (1971). The fate of Whiteley, A. M., & Warrington, E. K. (1977).
old memories. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Proposagnosia: A clinical, psychological and ana-
Psychology, A, 23, 432–442. tomical study of three patients. Journal of Neuro-
Warrington, E. K., & Shallice, T. (1979). Semantic logy, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 40, 395–403.
access dyslexia. Brain, 102, 43–63. Whiteley, A. M., & Warrington, E. K. (1978). Selective
Warrington, E. K., & Shallice, T. (1980). Word-form impairment of topographical memory, a single
dyslexia. Brain, 103, 99–112. case study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and
Warrington, E. K., & Shallice, T. (1984). Category spe- Psychiatry, 41, 575–578.
cific semantic impairments. Brain, 107, 829–853. Wilson, B. A., Alderman, N., Burgess, P. W., Emslie,
Warrington, E. K., & Taylor, A. M. (1973). The con- H., & Evans, J. J. (1996). Behavioural assessment of
tribution of the right parietal lobe to object recog- the dysexecutive syndrome. Harcourt Assessment/
nition. Cortex, 9, 152–164. The Psychological Corporation.
Warrington, E. K., & Taylor, A. M. (1978). To cate- Wilson, B. A., Greenfield, E., Clare, L., Baddeley, A.,
gorical stages of object recognition. Perception, 7, Cockburn, J., Watson, P., et al. (2007). Rivermead
695–705. Behavioural Memory Test—Third Edition (RBMT – 3).
Warrington, E. K., & Weiskrantz, L. (1968). New Harcourt Assessment. U.K.
method of testing long-term retention with special Yates, A. (1954). The validity of some psychological
reference to amnesic patients. Nature, 217, 972–974. tests of brain damage. Psychological Bulletin, 51, 4.
Warrington, E. K., & Weiskrantz, L. (1970). Amnesic
syndrome: Consolidation or retrieval? Nature,
228, 628–630.
3

The Boston Process Approach to


Neuropsychological Assessment
William P. Milberg, Nancy Hebben, and Edith Kaplan

The Boston Process Approach is based on a desire research directly relating test scores to central
to understand the qualitative nature of behavior nervous system (CNS) damage. It is now possi-
assessed by clinical psychometric instruments, ble for an experienced clinician to use a series
a desire to reconcile descriptive richness with of test scores to reliably determine the presence
reliability and quantitative evidence of valid- or absence of brain damage in nonpsychiatric
ity, and a desire to relate the behavior assessed patients and somewhat less reliably to localize
to the conceptual framework of experimental and establish the etiology of this pathology.
neuropsychology and cognitive neuroscience Unfortunately, most of the assessment tech-
(see Delis et al., 1990). Before its emergence in niques used in clinical neuropsychology evolved
the late 1970s, clinical neuropsychology as a with little attention to advances in experimen-
discipline had developed as a strongly empirical tal, cognitive, and developmental psychology
approach to the problem of the assessment of and the clinical science of behavioral neurol-
brain damage, which had for the most part been ogy. The historical separateness of clinical and
isolated from what can be seen in retrospect experimental psychological science has been
as a paradigm shift from behaviorism to cog- lamented by many (e.g., Cronbach, 1957), but
nitivism in academic psychology. The Boston it could be argued that in no discipline is this
Process Approach was an important bridge separateness more obvious than in the practice
between Post-War Clinical Psychology and the of clinical neuropsychology. For example, many
increasingly biological and neuroscience based of the assessment techniques require the clini-
approaches that characterize contemporary cian to use norms (Heaton et al., 2006; Reitan &
clinical research and practice. Davison, 1974), keys (Russell et al., 1970), and
In the past 30 years, the practice of clini- patterns of scores (Golden, 1981), while little
cal neuropsychology has progressed rapidly. emphasis is given to the cognitive functions
Initially, neuropsychological assessment tech- that underlie these scores, the way the patient
niques were known only to a few self-trained attained these scores, the preserved functions
clinicians or consisted of test batteries designed the scores reflect, or the way in which these
with the modest goal of determining the pres- scores relate to the patient’s daily life and reha-
ence of the clinical diagnosis of “organicity.” bilitation program.
These formerly esoteric practices have grown
into a widely respected specialty of clini-
Development of the Boston
cal assessment based on a growing body of
Process Approach
research (Lezak et al., 2004). To its credit, the
American tradition of clinical neuropsychology The Boston Process Approach had its origins in
is supported by a bulwark of empirical clinical the efforts of one of the authors (Kaplan, 1983)

42
The Boston Process Approach to Neuropsychological Assessment 43

to apply Heinz Werner’s distinction between core and satellite tests used clinically as part of
“process” and “achievement” in development the Boston Process Approach.
(Werner, 1937) to understanding the dissolu- If one were to examine the literature from
tion of function in patients with brain damage. the 1940s to the 1970s and later in clinical psy-
The early studies focused on apraxia. It was chology journals, one would find that many of
observed that the loss of voluntary movement the tests originally intended as tests of person-
to a command was not a unitary phenomenon ality (e.g., Rorschach Test: Rorschach, 1942),
in that the clinical subtypes of motor, ideomo- cognitive development (e.g., Bender-Gestalt
tor, and ideational apraxia were understood Test: Bender, 1938), and cognitive function
best when one actually observed the incorrect (e.g., Standard Progressive Matrices: Raven,
attempts of patients to follow simple commands 1960; Seguin-Goddard Formboard or Halstead
(Goodglass & Kaplan, 1963). Tactual Performance Test: Halstead, 1947;
The quality of the patients’ responses differed WMS: Wechsler, 1945), were also sensitive to
depending on the size and location of their brain damage in both adults and children.
lesion. Some patients would be unresponsive to Research on neuropsychological applications of
certain commands; others attempted to follow the Rorschach continued into the 1990s. Perry
the command with a primitive, undifferentiated et al. (1996), for example, found that a neuropsy-
version of the response, such as using a body chological process approach to the analysis of
part as the object; and still others used well- Rorschach responses was sensitive to the types
differentiated but irrelevant responses, such as of perseverative and linguistic errors character-
brushing their teeth when they were asked to istic of the deficits seen in patients with demen-
comb their hair (paramimia). These early obser- tia of the Alzheimer type.
vations permitted precise description of the A number of principles have been formulated
clinical phenomena and provided important to account for tests that appear to differentiate
data for understanding the development of ges- patients with dysfunction from those with-
tural behavior (Kaplan, 1968) and the disrup- out brain dysfunction (Russell, 1981). These
tion of such behavior in relationship to the locus include the principles of “complexity” and “flu-
of the lesion (Geschwind, 1975). idity.” Complex functions are those composed
A similar strategy was then applied to ana- of a number of simpler subelements; fluid func-
lyzing the process by which patients pass or tions are those requiring the native intellec-
fail various Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale tual ability of an individual. Fluid intellectual
(WAIS) (Wechsler, 1955) and Wechsler Adult functions are distinguished from crystallized
Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R) (Wechsler, intellectual functions, the latter being well-
1981) subtests. This led to the development of the learned abilities that are dependent on train-
WAIS-R as a Neuropsychological Instrument ing and cultural experience (Horn & Cattell,
(WAIS-R NI) (Kaplan et al., 1991). The focus on 1967). The tests most sensitive to brain damage
process also led to modifications in administra- are those that measure complex and fluid func-
tion and scoring of the Wechsler Memory Scale tions. Unfortunately, although tests of complex
(WMS) (Wechsler, 1945) subtests, as well as a functions can be used to measure specific cog-
number of other commonly employed clinical nitive domains (e.g., abstraction), most are not
measures. sufficiently differentiated to allow the clinician
Another test developed with the process to specify what component of intellectual com-
approach in mind, the Boston Diagnostic petence is impaired or what cognitive strategies
Aphasia Examination (Goodglass & Kaplan, the patient used to solve specific problems. A
1972), allows the precise characterization of sorting test developed by Delis et al. (1992) was
the breakdown of language function in patients one of the first that permitted a componential
with aphasia using a series of finely grained analysis of abstract problem-solving ability.
quantitative scales. As the Boston group and Modern experimental psychology has dem-
other investigators developed new and bet- onstrated that each general category of human
ter tests to measure brain function, they were cognitive function is made up of many subcom-
adapted and integrated into the collection of ponents (Neisser, 1967), and that as information
44 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

is processed it appears to pass through numer- may consist of standardized tests or a set of
ous, distinct subroutines. These subroutines are tasks specifically designed for each patient. The
not necessarily used rigidly by every individual only limits to the procedures that are employed
in the same way, and there is variation that nat- (beyond the patient’s tolerance and limitations)
urally occurs in the selection and sequencing of are the examiner’s knowledge of available tests
these subcomponents. Subjects vary in their use of cognitive function and his or her ingenuity
of the underlying cognitive components, and in creating new measures for particular-deficit
thus they may be said to differ in their cognitive areas (e.g., Delis et al., 1982; Milberg et al.,
style (Hunt, 1983), skill (Neisser et al., 1963), or 1979). Of the patients seen clinically during
general level of intellect (Hunt, 1983; Sternberg, the final 5 years of Dr. Kaplan’s tenure at the
1980). Veterans Administration Medical Center in
Unfortunately, many of the paradigms of Boston (1983–1987), approximately 90% were
experimental psychology have had limited util- given a selection from the basic core set of tests
ity in the clinical setting. The major difficulty shown in Table 3–1.
has been the relative insensitivity of many When first developed, it was necessary to
experimental procedures to the effects of brain modify many original test measures to facilitate
lesions. Although some of the experimental the collection of data about individual cognitive
techniques might not be useful on their own, the strategies. In most cases, however, an attempt
Boston group believed that they held promise in was made to make modifications that did not
enhancing existing clinical neuropsychological interfere with the standard administration of
procedures. With this in mind, they gradually the tests. Thus, one could still obtain reliable
combined tests that had been proven valid in and generalizable test scores referable to avail-
the clinical discrimination of patients with and able normative data because most of the modi-
without brain damage with tests that purported fications involved techniques of data collection
to measure narrow specifiable cognitive func- and analyses rather than changes in the test
tions. They also performed careful systematic procedures themselves. For example, it was con-
observations of the problem-solving strategies sidered critical to keep a verbatim account of a
used by patients (i.e., the way they successfully patient’s answers in verbal tasks and a detailed
solved or failed to solve each problem presented account of a patient’s performance on visuospa-
to them). The resulting method allowed both tial tasks.
a quantitative assessment of a patient’s perfor- Dr. Kaplan also emphasized “testing the
mance and a dynamic serial “picture” of the limits” whenever possible. Patients with neuro-
information-processing style that each patient psychological disorders can meet the criterion
used (Kaplan, 1988, 1990). for discontinuing a subtest and still be able to
answer the more difficult items not yet adminis-
Description of the Process tered. This may occur for a variety of reasons—
Approach: Early Developments for example, because of fluctuations in attention,
or because brain damage often does not cleanly
disrupt a function. Thus, patients may be forced
General Procedures
to use new, less efficient strategies that pro-
Although the Boston Process Approach orig- duce an inconsistent performance. Information
inally employed a core set of tests for most can be preserved, but not be reliably accessible
patients, it is probably best characterized as a (Milberg & Blumstein, 1981). This can be tested
“flexible battery approach” because the tech- only by asking patients to respond to questions
nique can be used to assess the pattern of pre- beyond the established point of failure, and by
served and impaired functions no matter which simplifying response demands.
particular tests are used. In addition to the core In addition, certain forms of damage may
tests, several “satellite tests” are used to clar- produce a loss of the ability to initiate a response
ify particular problem areas and to confirm rather than a loss of the actual function tested.
the clinical hypotheses developed from early In these instances it would be critical to push
observations of the patient. The satellite tests beyond consistent “I don’t know” responses
The Boston Process Approach to Neuropsychological Assessment 45

Table 3–1. A Representative Sample of the Tests Used in the Boston Process Approach
to Neuropsychological Assessment
Name of Test Reference
Intellectual and Conceptual Functions
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised Wechsler, 1981
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised Kaplan et al., 1991
as a Neuropsychological Instrument
Standard Progressive Matrices Raven, 1960
Shipley Institute of Living Scale Shipley, 1940
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Grant and Berg, 1948
Proverbs Test Gorham, 1956
Visual Verbal Test Feldman and Drasgow, 1960
Memory Functions
Wechsler Memory Scale Wechsler, 1945
Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised Wechsler, 1987
California Verbal Learning Test Delis et al., 1987
Rey-Osterreith Complex Figure Osterreith and Rey, 1944
Benton Visual Recognition Test Benton, 1950
(Multiple Choice Form)
Consonant Trigrams Test Butters and Grady, 1977
Cowboy Story Reading Memory Test Talland, 1965
Spatial Span Kaplan et al., 1991
Language Functions
Narrative Writing Sample Goodglass and Kaplan, 1972
Boston Naming Test Kaplan et al., 1983
Tests of Verbal Fluency (Word List Generation) Thurstone, 1938
Visuoperceptual Functions
Cow and Circle Experimental Puzzles WAIS-R NI, Kaplan et al., 1991
Automobile Puzzle WAIS-R NI, Kaplan et al., 1991
Spatial Quantitative Battery Goodglass and Kaplan, 1972
Hooper Visual Organization Test Hooper, 1958
Judgment of Line Orientation Benton et al., 1983
Academic Skills
Wide Range Achievement Test Jastak and Jastak, 1984
Executive-Control and Motor Functions
Porteus Maze Test Porteus, 1965
Stroop Color-Word Interference Test Stroop, 1935
Luria Three-Step Motor Program Christiansen, 1975
Finger Tapping Halstead, 1947
Grooved Peg Board KlØve, 1963
The California Proverb Test Delis et al., 1984
Boston Evaluation of Executive Functions Levine et al., 1993
Screening Instruments
Boston/Rochester Neuropsychological Kaplan et al., 1981
Screening Test
Geriatric Evaluation of Mental Status Milberg et al., 1992
MicroCog Powell et al., 1993

and minimal responses of one- or two-word should have produced a better performance.
elliptical phrases. Test questions may have to When done after the subtest had been admin-
be repeated and patients encouraged to try istered in the standard fashion, it was hoped
again or try harder. Testing the limits and spe- that this encouragement could occur without
cial encouragement are critical when it appears substantially affecting the reliability of a test
that a patient’s premorbid level of functioning score.
46 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

Another procedural modification involves the WAIS-R (Kaplan & Morris, 1985) and was
time limits. In most cases, when a patient is near available from 1991 to 2004. In general, fewer
a solution as the time limit approaches, he or she modifications were made to the administration
is allowed additional time to complete the prob- of the verbal subtests than to the administration
lem at hand. Response slowing often accom- of the performance subtests. This is so because
panies brain damage, and its effects on test it was difficult to engineer modifications that
performance need to be examined separately made the covert processes underlying verbal
from the actual loss of information-processing problem solving accessible within the context of
ability. A patient who consistently fails because standard test administration.
of inertia in the initiation of a response or Overall, the verbal subtests represented an
because he or she works too slowly must be dis- opportunity to analyze the form and content
tinguished from a patient who cannot complete of a patient’s speech. On any verbal test it is
problems no matter how much time is given. important to look for basic speech and language
Allowing more time may also identify patients difficulties such as dysarthria, dysprosody,
who actually perform more poorly if allowed agrammatism, press of speech, perseveration,
additional time after their initial response. A and word-finding problems as evidenced in
record of response latencies is critical so that paraphasias, as well as tendencies to be cir-
performance on timed tests can be compared to cumlocutory, circumstantial, or tangential.
performance on untimed tasks. This compari- These lessons, derived from the observations of
son allows one to distinguish general slowing patients with aphasia, were directly applicable
from slowness related to the specific demands to the verbal subtests of the WAIS-R. In addi-
of a particular test. Modifications of time limits, tion, the verbal subtests required a patient to
however, may decrease the reliability and valid- comprehend orally presented information and
ity of standardized test scores. then to produce an oral response.
Both the verbal and performance subtests
can be examined for scatter because the items
Specific Test Modifications: The First
within most of the subtests are ordered in lev-
Two Decades
els of increasing difficulty. Patients from differ-
Originally, procedural modifications involved ent clinical populations can have the same total
the addition of new components to published subtest score, but differing amounts of scatter
tests so that the functions of interest were mea- within their protocol require different interpre-
sured more comprehensively. These additions tations of performance.
are described with examples of two of the most We turn now to the specific Wechsler
commonly used tests of that time: the Wechsler Intelligence Scale subtests. We should note that
Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R) the WAIS-R NI is no longer available from its
(Wechsler, 1981) and the Wechsler Memory publisher, and the norms upon which it was
Scale (WMS) (Wechsler, 1945, 1987). Following based are now out of date. Yet, this instrument
a description of our revised test procedures, we captured many of the features of the Boston
will give examples of the variety of data that Process Approach as it was originally developed.
can be collected with these techniques, and how Many of Dr. Kaplan’s methods have been incor-
these data can be used to answer clinical neu- porated into the WISC-III PI (Kaplan, 1989).
ropsychological questions. It should be kept in Information. The information subtest sampled
mind that, though its description is limited here knowledge gained as part of a standard elemen-
to two tests, the method can be used on all neu- tary and high school education. It was thought
ropsychological tests. that a pattern of failure on easy items and suc-
cess on more difficult items on this subtest might
suggest retrieval difficulties. Poor performance
The WAIS-R as a Neuropsychological
that is not due to difficulties in language pro-
Instrument (WAIS-R NI)
duction usually stems from difficulty retrieving
The WAIS-R NI (Kaplan et al., 1991) was information from long-term memory. Retrieval
largely based on the Boston modification of difficulties may arise because the information
The Boston Process Approach to Neuropsychological Assessment 47

was never learned, because overlearned infor- patient’s performance may be compromised
mation was not available, or because of a deficit by reduced span of apprehension or inatten-
recalling information from one of the specific tion. To address this issue, all questions may be
content areas represented (e.g., numerical visually presented; for those patients who were
information, geography, science, literature, and unable to generate interpretations for the prov-
civics). The latter difficulty may be observed erbs, a multiple-choice version is available. The
in some patients with functional rather than foils for this task as well as for all the multiple-
brain-related dysfunction. Some conditions choice subtests of the WAIS-R NI were carefully
that characteristically manifest fluctuations selected to provide rich information regarding
in attention, such as attention-deficit disorder the underlying cognitive problems a patient
and temporal lobe epilepsy, may account for may have.
the presence of significant scatter and result in Arithmetic. This subtest measured the patient’s
a specific impairment of this subtest (Milberg ability to perform computations mentally, and
et al., 1980). In contrast, a poor score may be thus a variety of factors that may impair perfor-
the result of a preponderance of “don’t know” mance should be controlled. On completion of
responses. Patients who have sustained a severe the subtest, patients with a short attention span,
traumatic brain injury, or who are clinically for example, are given a visual presentation of
depressed, show a marked tendency to be “mini- the auditorily presented verbal problems that
mal responders.” These individuals may perform they failed. In this way deficits in the ability to
considerably better when the information items organize the problem and solve it can be sepa-
are presented in a multiple-choice format, thus rated from short-term memory problems. If a
reducing demands for active retrieval processes. patient still cannot adequately execute the prob-
Further, the visual presentation may minimize lems mentally, paper and pencil are provided to
the effects of inattention and auditory acuity or assess his or her ability to transform the verbal
comprehension problems. Reducing the task to problem into a mathematical representation
one of recognition provides a better assessment and to evaluate his or her more fundamental
of the fund of information an individual still computational skills. In addition, by examining
has in remote memory. Joy et al. (1992) dem- the patient’s written formulation, errors due to
onstrated the efficacy of using the WAIS-R NI misalignment can be distinguished from those
Information Multiple-choice subtest in a pop- secondary to impairment in arithmetic func-
ulation of healthy community-dwelling elderly tions per se and from difficulties in ordering the
adults (see Table 3–2). series of operations.
Comprehension. This subtest addressed a Incorrect answers in this subtest are analyzed
patient’s ability to interpret orally presented to learn how a specific answer was derived. A
information. A patient’s answers can reveal typical error includes the use of numbers with-
thinking disorders such as concreteness, per- out consideration of the content of the problem.
severation, and disturbed associations. This This error occurs when a patient is impulsive or
subtest also can show specific deficits in a becomes stimulus bound and attempts to sim-
patient’s knowledge of the various areas repre- plify a multistep problem or when he or she is
sented: personal and social behavior, general distracted by the numbers themselves at the
knowledge, and social obligations. A num- expense of the computation required. To isolate
ber of the questions are rather lengthy, so a primary computation problems, the WAIS-R NI

Table 3–2. The WAIS-R NI Information Subtest Among Healthy Older Adults by Age Group
50–59 yrs. (n = 40) 60–69 yrs. (n = 51) 70–79 yrs. (n = 52) 80–89 yrs. (n = 34)
Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
Information 22.12 4.65 20.24 3.34 19.94 4.96 18.65 5.21
Standard
Information 22.20 3.37 21.67 2.85 22.15 3.69 21.06 2.92
Multiple choice
48 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

computational form of the arithmetic problems spans (Butters & Cermak, 1980). Repeating
is available in a response booklet form. digits backward makes far greater demands
Similarities. This test requires the patient to on working memory and requires some cog-
form a superordinate category relating pairs of nitive processing of the information. This may
words. The kind of errors a patient makes will be achieved by rehearsing the series of digits
vary. His or her answers might be concrete, or again and again, or by transforming the audi-
he or she might only be able to provide defini- torily presented information to a “visual” rep-
tions for each word but not be able to integrate resentation and then successively “reading” the
the pairs. He or she might provide an answer digits backwards. The former strategy is heavily
related to only one word in a pair or describe reliant on repetition and is susceptible to inter-
differences between the words, while ignoring ference and perseveration within and between
the task of finding similarities. For patients who series. The latter requires flexible movement
have difficulty establishing the set to identify between modalities. In either event, digit span
similarities, or who have difficulty articulating backward is more sensitive to brain dysfunction
or elaborating a response, or who tend to say than digit span forward. In general, digit span
“I don’t know,” the foils in the multiple-choice forward is usually equal to, or better than, digit
format for this subtest help to clarify the nature span backward. With some patients, however, it
of the underlying cognitive problems. is not uncommon to find their backward span to
Digit Span. In this subtest we considered be longer than their forward span because they
it especially important to record the patient’s perceive that the former is a more demanding
response verbatim. Although we discontinue task and thus requires a mobilization of energy
the subtest after failure of both trials of any and active engagement in the task. This finding
series, if a patient is able to recall all of the dig- is frequently noted in patients with depression
its, although in the wrong order, we administer and in patients giving inconsistent effort.
the next series. Because the patient’s “span of An analysis of the nature of errors such as
apprehension,” or the number of digits recalled, omissions, additions, substitutions of digits and
is separate from the process of making errors whether they occur at the beginning or end of
in the order of recall, two different scores for the series, may suggest problems that relate to
both forward and backward recall are available vulnerability to interference effects (proactive
for this subtest: the patient’s span with correct and retroactive).
order of recall and the patient’s span regardless Spatial Span. The WAIS-R NI introduced
of order. We also note if the patient “chunked” a 10-cube spatial span test that is now part of
digits by repeating them in sets of 2 or 3 digits the Wechsler Memory Scale-Third Edition
or multiple unit integers. In addition, the rec- (WMS-III) to provide a visual analog to Digit
ord indicates impulsive performances, such as Span. It is scored in the same way as Digit Span
patients beginning a series before the examiner is scored. In addition to the error types noted
is finished or repeating the digits at a very above, evidence of errors in the left visual field
rapid rate. versus errors in the right visual field provide
Although the WAIS-R manual gave equal lateralizing information.
weight to the number of series successfully Vocabulary. This subtest, like Information
repeated forward and backward in comput- and Comprehension, taps a patient’s established
ing the digit span-scaled score, we have found fund of knowledge and is highly related to edu-
that there is a dissociation between the capac- cational, socioeconomic, and occupational
ity to repeat digits forward and backward in experience. It is generally considered the best
patients with brain dysfunction (Lezak, 1995). single measure of “general intelligence” and is
Repeating digits forward seems to require only least affected by CNS insult except for lesions
the capacity to briefly hold several bits of sim- directly involving the cortical and subcorti-
ple information in short-term memory. The ele- cal language zones. The standard administra-
mentary nature of this process is underscored tion of this test calls for the examiner to point
by the fact that patients with severe amnesia to each word listed on a card while saying the
can have normal or even above normal digit word aloud. Because of the many visual and
The Boston Process Approach to Neuropsychological Assessment 49

attentional disorders in patients with CNS dys- with the symbols used in the subtest. If the
function, we also have available a printed ver- patient was proficient enough, however, to com-
sion with enlarged words to help focus patients plete more than three lines of the form within
who become distracted when a word is embed- 90 seconds, then he or she was stopped at 90
ded in a list of other words. seconds. At the end of three complete lines, the
Kaplan surmised that numerous types of patient was provided only the last row, and in
errors could occur in this subtest. One inter- the absence of the reference key the patient is
esting observation was the tendency to define a required to write the symbol for each of the dig-
word with its polar opposite. Kaplan suggested its. After this, the patient was instructed to write
that this might be seen in adults who had a his- in any order all the symbols he or she could
tory of developmental learning disabilities. remember. The measurement of paired and free
Patients may also be distracted by the pho- recall of the symbols permitted examination
netic or perceptual properties of words and of the amount of incidental learning that had
provide associative responses. A tendency to taken place during the subtest. These changes
perseverate may be seen in the presence of the were ultimately incorporated in the WAIS-III
same introduction to each response by a patient. as optional procedures.
In addition, although a patient can be credited Dr. Kaplan found it important to examine
with one of the two score points for responses the actual symbols produced by the patient.
that use examples to define the word, such Are they rotated, flipped upside down, or trans-
responses can reveal CNS dysfunction when formed into perceptually similar letters? Are
they reflect an inability to pull away from the the characters produced by the patient micro-
stimulus. or macrographic? Does the patient use the box
The multiple-choice version of this subtest as part of the symbol, that is, is the patient
allows us to determine whether a patient still “pulled” to the stimulus box? Does the patient
knows the meaning of a word even though he consistently make incorrect substitutions, or
or she is now unable to retrieve the word to skip spaces or lines of the task? All these attri-
adequately express his or her knowledge of the butes help define the patient’s cognitive diffi-
word. Thus, the WAIS-R NI multiple-choice culties and may aid in localizing pathology. For
format for the Vocabulary subtest can effec- example, we have observed that the systematic
tively provide the best estimate of an individu- inversion of symbols to form alphabetic charac-
al’s premorbid level of intelligence. ters (e.g., V for ⋀ or T for ⊥) may be associated
Digit Symbol. Adequate performance on this with pathology of the dorsolateral surface of
multifactorial subtest is dependent on a num- the right frontal lobe, whereas a patient is more
ber of abilities—for example, motor speed, inci- likely to become “stimulus bound” with bilat-
dental learning of the digit-symbol pairs, and eral frontal lobe pathology.
scanning ability (rapidly moving one’s gaze to One major change in the Digit Symbol sub-
and from the reference key). To understand the test was an addition called Symbol Copy, which
nature of the underlying difficulty a patient may was administered later in the evaluation. This
have on this task, we introduced the following version is like the original except that there is no
procedural modifications. To begin with, we key. The patient copied each symbol in the space
administered this subtest in the usual manner, directly below the symbol, and 30-second inter-
except that as the patient proceeded with the vals are marked for a total of 90 seconds. This
task, the examiner placed marks on the WAIS-R version allowed the dissociation of motor speed
NI record form every 30 seconds to indicate the in the patient’s performance from the process of
patient’s progress. This allowed an analysis of learning the symbols. This is especially impor-
changes over time in the rate of transcription, tant with older patients because motor slowing
changes that could signal fatigue or practice can confound interpretation of the test score.
effects. After the 90-second time limit expires, Joy et al. (1992) found that healthy, community-
the examiner allowed the patient to continue dwelling elderly between the ages of 50 and
until he or she completed at least three full lines 89 showed motor slowing on the symbol copy
of the form. This equalized patient experience condition (70% of the variance was attributable
50 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

to motor slowing), and that there was a disso- a right-hander (left to right and top to bottom),
ciation between paired digit/symbol recall and (3) whether the patient rotated the blocks in
free recall of just symbols. There was a marked place or in space, (4) whether the patient broke
reduction in paired incidental learning with the 2 × 2 or 3 × 3 matrix configurations on the
increasing age, while the number of symbols way to solution, (5) whether the patient pro-
freely recalled was not affected by age (see duced a mirror image or an up-down reversal
Table 3–3). of the actual design as his or her final product,
Picture Completion. This subtest required (6) whether the patient perseverated a design
visual discrimination and verbal labeling of, or across items, and (7) the side of the design the
discrete pointing to, the essential missing com- patient made more errors on was noted. Later in
ponent in meaningful visual stimuli. It was felt this chapter the strategy of “breaking the con-
that failures on specific items could be related figuration” (see point 4 above) will be addressed
to different cognitive factors. A patient’s percep- in more detail.
tion of the stimulus item may be impaired due Picture Arrangement. Visual perception, inte-
to primary visual problems, or visual or second- gration, memory of details, and serial ordering
ary visuo-organizational problems. Complete were considered important for success in this
misidentification of the stimulus may occur in subtest. As with the Picture Completion sub-
patients with visual agnosia. A patient may have test, the examiner had to be sensitive to visual
difficulty identifying missing embedded fea- field and visuospatial neglect deficits. The cards
tures but no difficulty when the important fea- might have to be placed in a vertical column in
ture belongs to the contour. A patient may have front of the patient to minimize such effects.
difficulty with items requiring inferences about After the subtest was completed, the patient
symmetry, inferences based on the knowledge was asked to tell the story for each sequence
of the object, or inferences based on knowledge as he or she saw it. Several consequences may
of natural events. Finally, he or she may have result: (1) the patient may provide the appro-
difficulty making a hierarchy of the missing priate story to a correctly sequenced series; (2)
details. Errors may also be analyzed with regard he or she may provide the correct story for a
to whether the missing part is on the left or right disordered arrangement; or (3) he or she may
side of the picture. provide neither the correct story nor the correct
Block Design. One of Dr. Kaplan’s earliest sequence of cards. The verbal account follow-
observations regarded performance on this ing each arrangement permits a closer analy-
test. She demonstrated that valuable informa- sis of the underlying problems in all incorrect
tion could be gained by observing the strategy arrangements. For some patients, giving a verbal
the patient used in his or her constructions on account will bring into focus illogical elements
this subtest. To do this, a flowchart was kept in their arrangements and may guide them to
of the exact process a patient went through in a correct rearrangement. The verbal account
completing a design. Information such as (1) may also reveal misperceptions of detail, lack of
the quadrant in which the patient began his appreciation of spatial relationships, or overat-
or her construction, (2) whether the patient tention to details, which results in the inability
worked in the normally favored directions for to perceive similarities across pictures.

Table 3–3. The WAIS-R NI Digit Symbol Subtest Among Healthy Older Adults by Age Group
50–59 yrs. (n = 40) 60–69 yrs. (n = 51) 70–79 yrs. (n = 52) 80–89 yrs. (n = 34)
Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
Digit Symbol 50.39 8.94 47.84 10.44 37.98 8.73 28.52 9.04
Time to End 123.34 25.81 133.86 31.31 166.86 40.40 235.27 72.23
Copying Time 59.12 15.55 69.10 15.56 75.08 12.09 98.91 26.08
Incidental 5.41 2.69 5.20 2.32 4.62 2.33 3.76 2.74
learning
Symbol free recall 7.41 1.41 7.16 1.34 6.96 1.34 6.39 1.52
The Boston Process Approach to Neuropsychological Assessment 51

By allowing a patient to work past the spec- material. Patients with prefrontal damage have
ified time limits on this subtest, his or her difficulty manipulating information in a flexible
capacity to comprehend and complete the task manner, and have difficulty shift ing from one
in spite of motor slowing or scanning deficits meaning of a word to another. In addition, these
could be evaluated. Again, the process by which patients are “captured” (Shallice, 1982; Stuss &
a patient arranged the cards was observed. Benson, 1986) by high-probability, familiar
Some patients were observed to study the cards word sequences and become stimulus bound.
and preplan their arrangement. Other patients Object Assembly. Three additional puzzles were
were observed to arrange them impulsively, and added to the four standard puzzles in this subtest
still other patients required the visual feedback in order to elucidate the effect of certain stimu-
of their productions as they arrange the cards, lus parameters such as the presence or absence
study them, and then rearrange them. of internal detail on solutions. As in the Block
Errors were observed to occur for a variety Design subtest, the actual process employed by
of reasons. A patient may not move cards from the patient to solve each puzzle was recorded. The
their original location because of a poor strat- Automobile puzzle from the Wechsler Intelligence
egy or because of attentional deficits. The former Scale for Children (WISC-III) (Wechsler, 1991)
suggests a strategy characterized by inertia, was added because it is rich in internal detail and
which is often seen in patients with frontal lobe permits a comparison between the puzzles that
dysfunction; the latter suggests a strategy more rely heavily on edge alignment information (i.e.,
often seen in posterior damage. A patient may Hand and Elephant) for solution.
fail because of inattention to detail or a focus on Two other experimental puzzles, the Circle
irrelevant details. A patient may misunderstand and the Cow (Palmer & Kaplan, 1985), were
the task and attempt to align the visual elements added to demonstrate a patient’s reliance
within the cards, or he may separate the cards on one of these two strategies to the exclu-
into subgroups based on similar features. sion of the other. The Circle can only be solved
Sentence Arrangement. This task was designed by using contour information, whereas the
to be a verbal analog to Picture Arrangement so Cow, constructed so that each juncture is an
that sequencing ability for verbal material could identical arc, cannot be solved by using con-
be contrasted with sequencing for pictorial tour information and demands, instead, a

Table 3–4. A Sampler of WAIS-R Modifications Included in the WAIS-R NI


Subtest Modification
Information Discontinue rule not followed; multiple choice version admin-istered later.
Picture Completion Time limit is not observed; discontinue rule is not followed.
Digit Span Discontinue rule is not followed.
Picture Arrangement Time limit is not observed; discontinue rule need not be followed. Examinee is
asked to tell a story for each of his or her arrangements.
Vocabulary Vocabulary multiple choice version; discontinue rule need not be followed.
Block Design Extra blocks provided; discontinue rule not followed. Examinee asked to judge
correctness of his or her constructions.
Arithmetic Time limit is not observed; discontinue rules need not be followed. Examinee is
pre-sented with printed version of failed items; for items then failed, paper and
pencil are provided; for items still failed, computational form is presented.
Object Assembly Examinee is asked to identify the object as soon as he or she srecognizes it; time
limit is not observed.
Comprehension Multiple choice version for proverbs. Examinee is presented printed version.
Digit Symbol Examinee is asked to complete third row if he or she has not completed it in 90
sees.; paired and unpaired recall of symbols is requested; symbol copy condition
presented.
Similarities Discontinue rule need not be followed; multiple-choice version is administered
later.
Source: Based on Kaplan et al. (1991), p. 5.
52 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

piece-by-piece analysis. Patients who rely too recall condition, a cued recall condition was
heavily on contour information will fail to solve administered. Here, specific questions about
the Cow, and patients who are unable to appre- the details in the passage served as prompts.
ciate the relationship between pieces will fail to For example, if, for the first story, the patient
solve the Circle. Many of the test modifications had not spontaneously said the woman’s name
described above have been incorporated into or where she was from, the examiner queries,
the WAIS-R NI (Kaplan et al., 1991). Table 3–4 “What was the woman’s name? Where was
presents a sample of some of the modifications she from?” These kinds of direct prompts were
contained within the WAIS-R NI. helpful in identifying whether the information
had indeed registered despite the impoverished
account the patient had given spontaneously.
Boston Revision of the Wechsler
Then, following the 20-minute delayed recall,
Memory Scale (WMS)
a multiple-choice condition is presented. With
Many of the changes recommended for the these modifications it was then possible to
WMS have been obviated by the publication of determine whether the information had been
the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised (WMS-R) registered and what the fate of the information
(Wechsler, 1987) and then the Wechsler Memory was over time. Patients with adequate atten-
Scale-Third Edition (WMS-III) (Wechsler, tional and rote memory may do well when they
1997). Because not all of the recommended initially recall the information but may show
changes for the WMS were incorporated in the severe deficits on delayed recall and may not
WMS-R, we will describe the additional subtests even benefit from a recognition task (multiple
and procedural modifications that had been choice). On the other hand, a patient (depressed
introduced to make the WMS a more complete or hypoactive) may have had a minimal account
assessment of a patient’s ability to learn and initially, but given the structure of the prompts,
recall new verbal and visuospatial information. may perform significantly better after a delay.
Impairment after a delay may be due to inad-
equate retrieval strategies or defective storage
General Information and Orientation
abilities.
A number of items based on autobiographical In addition to the two auditorily presented
information were added to these two subtests so paragraphs, a third paragraph (viz., the Cowboy
that recall of personal, current, and old infor- Story; Talland, 1965), which was read aloud by
mation could be assessed more fully. the patient, thus assuring registration, was
then tested for recall immediately and after a
20-minute delay. This additional paragraph
Mental Control
allowed an examination of complaints from
Two items that were found to be useful in the patients about an inability to retain information
characterization and localization of retrieval defi- that had been read, as well as testing for selec-
cits (Coltheart et al., 1975) were added to this sub- tive modality of input differences.
test. After reciting the alphabet, patients are asked Beyond quantifying how much information
to name all the letters of the alphabet that rhyme is learned and recalled, qualitative features of
with the word “key” and then to name all the let- the responses, such as impoverishment, con-
ters of the alphabet that contain a curve when fabulation, disorganization, and confusion of
printed as capital letters. These two items provide details across stories were also noted.
specific information about a patient’s ability to
retrieve information from memory based on spe-
Associative Learning
cific auditory or visual physical characteristics.
Three major modifications were made to this
subtest. First, immediately after the third stan-
Logical Memory
dard trial, “backward retrieval” was measured.
A number of additions were made to this sub- The order of each pair of words was reversed,
test. First, following the standard immediate and the patient was presented with the second
The Boston Process Approach to Neuropsychological Assessment 53

word of the pair and asked to recall the first. greater detail later in this chapter. In addition,
Second, 20 minutes later, free, uncued recall the type of errors a patient makes was noted.
of the pairs was assessed. Third, following Recall could be characterized by impoverish-
this, the first word of each pair was provided ment, simplification and distortion of details,
as a cue and paired recall was measured once disorganization, and confusion between
again. Patients who are able to perform bet- designs.
ter on the third trial than on the reversed trial The revision of the WMS (WMS-R) contained
have been found to perform less well on delayed delayed recall conditions for Logical Memory,
recall (Guila Glosser, personal communica- Verbal Paired Associates, Visual Reproductions,
tion). Presumably, these patients demonstrated and for a new subtest called the Visual Paired
a more shallow level of information processing Associates Test.
(phonemic), whereas patients who do not do
more poorly on the reversed condition have a
Screening Instruments
higher level of information processing (seman-
tic). As in the Logical Memory subtest, these In the 1990s several screening instruments
recall conditions allowed deficits in immediate emerged that attempted to capture some of
recall to be examined separately from those in the features of the procedures used in the
delayed recall. Patient responses on this task Boston Process Approach. Examples of these
might reflect internal and external intrusions, instruments include the Boston/Rochester
perseveration, or a simple inability to learn new Neuropsychological Screening Test (Kaplan
information. et al., 1981), the Geriatric Evaluation of Mental
Status (the GEMS) (Milberg et al., 1992), and
MicroCog, a computerized assessment of cog-
Visual Reproduction
nitive status (Powell et al., 1993).
Dr. Kaplan pointed out that it could not be The Boston/Rochester, which was the first
assumed that the difficulty a patient had screening battery designed to allow for the
reproducing a geometric design that was analysis of cognitive processes, includes men-
exposed for a brief period was an indication of tal status questions and measures of repetition,
poor visual memory. It may be that the patient praxis, reading comprehension, immediate
had difficulty perceiving the design, or had and delayed verbal and design memory, and a
difficulty at the visuomotor execution level. number of other tasks that lend themselves to a
The following conditions served to clarify detailed analysis of patients’ cognitive strategies
the source of the patient’s problem: After the and abilities. The Boston/Rochester takes 1–2
designs had been drawn (immediate repro- hours to administer.
duction), a multiple-choice task was presented The GEMS, developed more recently, was
(immediate recognition), followed by a copy designed to be extremely brief (15–20 minutes)
condition, a 20-minute delayed recall, and, and easily administered to elderly patients. It
fi nally, a matching condition was presented contains a number of tasks to assess visual and
if any question of a visuoperceptual problem verbal memory, language, and executive func-
remained. The copy condition provided an tions. A number of these tasks were designed
opportunity to assess a patient’s visuopercep- specifically for the GEMS, and contain features
tual analysis of the designs. The delayed recall that allow the examiner to make inferences
condition assessed changes in recall following about the details of a patient’s cognitive abilities.
an added exposure to the designs. The recog- The GEMS continues to undergo formal valida-
nition and matching conditions removed the tion (Hamann et al., 1993), but has already been
possible contamination a visuomotor problem shown to be considerably more sensitive than
might contribute. For all drawings a flow chart the Mini-Mental State Examination (Folstein
was created—that is, a record of the manner et al., 1975) in detecting general cognitive
in which the patient produced each design. impairment (Berger, 1993). It has been shown
Such analysis can provide information about to accurately classify 96% of a sample of 100
brain dysfunction, as will be discussed in inpatient geriatric patients and age-matched
54 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

controls with extremely low (7%) false-positive is repeated for five learning trials, after which
rates (Sachs et al., 1995). the examinee is read a second list of words for
MicroCog was a computer-administered recall. Following the second list, they are asked
battery of cognitive tests inspired by Process to recall the first list again, and this is followed
Approach ideas (Powell, 1993). It sampled a by a categorically cued recall trial. Free and
broad spectrum of cognitive functions and cued recall is repeated again after a long delay,
provided indices for attention/mental control, and there is a delayed YES/NO recognition
memory, reasoning/calculation, spatial process- trial. The CVLT. One of the important modifi-
ing, and reaction time. In addition, it allowed cations of the RAVLT contained in the CVLT is
the separation of information-processing speed a word list that consists of words drawn from
from information-processing accuracy. The several semantic categories. These words are
Standard Form contains 18 subtests and takes dispersed in pseudorandom order through-
about an hour; the Short Form has 12 subtests out the list, providing the opportunity to use
and can be completed in half an hour. It has not this information to aid recall. The CVLT and
gained broad popularity as of this writing. CVLT-II have been widely adopted, and used
in hundreds of published research papers. The
CVLT in many ways is the first Boston Process
New Developments
Approach–inspired psychological test that has
Since the second version of this chapter was become a gold standard measure of a neuro-
published more than a decade ago, a number psychological function. Yet it is important to
of new tests have appeared that have incorpo- note that while the basic quantitative measures
rated a number of procedures and features of (items recalled, recognized) have been careful
the Boston Process Approach into standardized norms and have been used in neuropsycho-
tests. In recognition of the common critique logical studies of memory disorders, many of
of the use of qualitative observations as a basis the submeasures (primacy/recency, clustering
for clinical prediction, these new generation of effects, etc.) though not initially supported by
neuropsychological tests have followed the tra- empirical investigations have received increas-
ditional methods of establishing reliability and ing attention from investigators. For example,
validity, and the collection of population-based Alexander et al. (2003) reported localization-
normative data. specific patterns of performance on the CVLT
in patients with frontal lobe lesions.
The California Verbal Learning
Test (CVLT) Clock Drawing Task
One of the earliest tests to be adapted into One of the earliest tasks to find its way into
the Boston Process Approach was the Rey the core batteries originally used at the Boston
Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) (Rey, VA by Dr. Kaplan and her associates was the
1964), which consisted of a 15-item list that was Clock Drawing Test. Clock Drawing to com-
repeated for five trials, with a second interference mand appears to have been an old bedside
list and delayed recall. One of the best known mental status examination technique that
and widely used of these tests derived from or became well known when examples of the
influenced by the Boston Process Approach is clocks produced by patients with hemispa-
the CVLT, which adapted many features of the tial neglect were presented in MacDonald
RAVLT. Critchley’s 1953 monograph entitled “The
This test of verbal memory in its current form Parietal Lobes” (e.g., Critchley, 1953). Many
(CVLT-II) consists of a list of 16 words that are of the procedures described by Critchley were
read aloud to the examinee who is asked to ultimately put together by Dr. Kaplan and her
recall as many of the items on the list as possi- colleagues into a test called the Boston Parietal
ble. The items that comprise the list are derived Lobe Battery, which included Critchley’s
from four semantic categories. This procedure clock copying drawing task, and time setting
The Boston Process Approach to Neuropsychological Assessment 55

tasks. The clock copying and drawing task in Recently, Grande and colleagues (submitted)
the Boston Parietal Lobe Battery required the developed a test called the Clock-in-a-Box task,
patient to copy/draw a clock set to the time which adds the requirement of having patients
of “three o’clock.” Sometime during the late draw a freehand clock in one of four color coded
1970s, Dr. Kaplan modified the clock draw- boxes. The data from this task is quite encour-
ing task with the instructions to set the time aging, suggesting that the task may be sensi-
to “ten after eleven.” As discussed in Freedman tive to a variety of cognitive problems (Munshi
et al. (1995),this had the advantage of requiring et al., 2006).
the patient to place the hands symmetrically
around a central up/down axis, and to employ
The Delis–Kaplan Executive Function
a greater degree of executive functioning. The
System (DKEFS)
latter presumably was derived from the fact
that the patient must use a relatively abstract The DKEFS is a battery of measures designed
rule for time telling and not get “pulled” or to assess a broad range of cognitive control
distracted by the actual numerals on the clock abilities known as “executive functions.” It
(i.e., they must interpret the 2 as 10 minutes consists of nine subtests that for the most part
after the hour, rather than a 2). The revised clock were adaptations of existing measures. These
first appears in print in the second edition of the include trail making, verbal fluency, design flu-
Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination man- ency, color–word interference, sorting, twenty
ual (Goodglass, 1983), but an examination of questions, word context, tower, and proverbs.
the data fi les at the Harold Goodglass Aphasia Rather than relying on direct observations of
Research Center conducted by the first author qualitative features of performances, each task
found that the “10 after 11” clock appeared in contains modifications designed to allow the
research fi les as early as 1979. examiner direct inferences about the “compo-
This task provided much fodder for observers nent functions of higher-level cognitive tasks”
of test behavior “process” as it is a deceptively (p. 3, Delis et al., 2001).
complex task requiring memory, constructional
praxis, and the executive functions mentioned
The Quantified Process Approach
above. Clock drawing has generated much
interest among clinicians with many variations Seeing a need to try to capture some of the
and scoring systems (Freedman, 1995; Grande, qualitative process procedures and observa-
submitted; Libon, 1993; Royall, 1998). Most of tions in a standardized format, Poreh (2000,
these scoring systems try to capture the various 2006) developed what he calls the “Quantified
striking “qualitative” features that seem to mark Process Approach,” modeled to some extent on
pathological performance in a standardized the Boston Process Approach as described in
scoring system. Such features as the shape of the the previous version of this chapter (Milberg
clock face, number placement, and hand length, et al., 1996) and other sources (e.g., Kaplan).
centering, and placement are captured in these Poreh (2007) has been working on producing
systems. Three of these systems Rouleau (1996), computerized automated versions of many
Freedman et al. (1995), and Libon et al. (1996) of the original tasks used by Kaplan and her
were shown to be comparably reliable (South colleagues (as well as several completely orig-
et al., 2001). inal tasks) that allow quantitative scoring of
Several systems have tried to make Clock a number of “qualitative” features. Poreh’s
Drawing more sensitive, particularly to the tasks include an updated version of the orig-
issue of “executive” functions. For example, inal Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (Rey,
the CLOX Text (Royall et al., 1998) compared 1964), Regard’s Five Point Test, Trail-Making
freehand Clock Drawing to copying to obtain a Test, and others. Th is is a promising approach
measure that is claimed to be more sensitive to that will allow for a careful empirical exam-
executive dysfunction than the standard clock ination of many of Dr. Kaplan’s original
drawing. observations.
56 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

Using the Process Approach to represent the first major point at which the basic
Localize Lesions and Describe units of language can be isolated from their use
Functions in expressing organized thought. Similarly,
a photograph of a street scene can be broken
The modifications of testing and data-record- down into low-level categorical units of per-
ing procedures specific to the Boston Process ception, such as cars, people, or litter, and then
Approach that allow the clinician to obtain a organized into relational information placing
dynamic record of a patient’s problem-solving these disparate elements into a larger concep-
strategy were described above. In this section, tual or spatial unit.
we will show how the specific strategic infor- To successfully interpret most test material
mation that can be collected with the process requires the use and integration of both fea-
approach can be useful in the analysis of brain tural and contextual information. Brain damage
and behavior relationships and in the prediction produces a lawful fractionation of a patient’s
of behavior outside the clinical laboratory. For ability to use both types of information simulta-
purposes of this discussion we will concentrate neously. Furthermore, the type of information
on several broad categories of cognitive strate- processing given priority is related to the lateral-
gies that can be observed across many different ity and location of a patient’s lesion. Specifically,
measures. patients with damage to the left hemisphere are
more likely to use a strategy favoring contextual
information, whereas patients with damage to
Featural versus Contextual
the right hemisphere are more likely to give pri-
Priority
ority to featural information.
Most tasks that are useful in assessing brain We can infer the type of informational strat-
damage consist of a series of elements or basic egy favored by a patient from many of the tasks
stimuli arranged together within a spatial, tem- described earlier. For example, a patient may in
poral, or conceptual framework. One impor- the course of assembling a block design shaped
tant strategic variable, therefore, is the extent like a diagonal rectangle within a square (see
to which patients give priority to processing Figure 3–1) align pairs of solid blocks to form a
low-level detail or “featural” information ver- diagonal rectangle without regard for the 3 × 3
sus higher-level configural or contextual infor- matrix in which it is placed (Figure 3–1a).
mation (see Schmeck & Grove, 1979, for related This is an example of a patient giving atten-
literature from experimental and educational tional priority to the internal features of the
psychology). There is now a considerable body of design without regard for the configuration.
literature that supports the observation of part/ Another patient may assemble the same design
whole processing differences among patients by retaining the 3 3 shape but drastically simpli-
with unilateral cortical lesions (e.g., Delis et al., fying the diagonal rectangle into a line of three
1986; Robertson & Delis, 1986; Thomas & Forde, solid red blocks (Figure 3–1b). In this case the
2006). patient is giving attentional priority to the con-
This dichotomy of information, featural figuration with little regard for the accuracy of
on one hand and contextual on the other, can the internal features. Similar performance strat-
be used to characterize both verbal and visu- egies have been found in analyses of block design
ospatial information within each of the sen- performance in normal subjects (Haeberle,
sory modalities. For example, words and their 1982; Royer, 1967) though not with the rigidity
basic phrase structure within a sentence can or consistency found in patients with pathology
be thought of as the basic elements or features of the CNS. Normal performance is typically
important in linguistic analysis. Phrases are put characterized by the integration of featural and
together into sentences, and sentences are put configural information, whereas pathological
together into a conceptually focused paragraph performance is characterized by their dissoci-
to create a higher-level context or organiza- ation. Thus, normal subjects will rarely neglect
tion. Aside from simple phonemes and acoustic one source of information completely while
energy transitions, the word or phrase seems to using the other.
The Boston Process Approach to Neuropsychological Assessment 57

performance. Some patients will copy the design


as if they were using a random scan path, add-
ing small line segments until their final design
resembles the original. Such a painstaking per-
Stimulus formance can be taken as evidence of a featural
priority strategy in the perceptual organization
of the design.
Other patients may approach the task of copy-
ing the design by producing the entire extreme
outline but omitting smaller features. This
approach is evidence of a strategy of contextual
A
priority. Additional evidence for the emphasis
of one or the other of these strategies can often
be seen in the patient’s recall of the design after
a delay. A patient who is overly dependent on
featural information may show a performance
like that seen in the left column of Figure 3–3,
whereas a patient who directs his attention pri-
marily to configural information may show a
production like that seen in the right column of
Figure 3–3.
Occasionally, patients will actually retrieve
featural information independently of the spa-
tial context in which it originally appeared.
B For example, a patient may recall one of the
designs from the Visual Reproduction subtest
of the WMS-III when asked to recall the Rey–
Osterreith Complex Figure, or he or she may
recombine features from two different designs
into one. Stern et al. (1994) have developed a
comprehensive qualitative scoring system that
provides scoring criteria for configural ele-
ments, clusters, details, fragmentation, plan-
Figure 3–1. Two examples of informational strate- ning, size (reduction and expansion), rotation,
gies pursued by patients in solving complex tasks. perseveration, confabulation, neatness, and
asymmetry. Shorr et al. (1992) have developed
Using featural information to the exclusion a scoring system to analyze perceptual cluster-
of contextual information can also be seen on ing. They found that for a population of neuro-
the Rey–Osterreith Complex Figure or Rey psychiatric patients, configural or perceptual
Complex Figure (Osterreith & Rey, 1944). By clustering on copy was a better predictor of
keeping a flowchart of the patient’s method of memory performance than was copy accuracy.
copying or recalling the Rey Figure (Rey, 1964) Similar deficits in a balance between featural
(see Figure 3–2), evidence about the strategy and contextual priorities can be seen in verbal
used by a patient can be obtained. The Rey tasks. A patient may show evidence of featural
Complex Figure includes smaller rectangles, priority when recalling the Logical Memory
squares, and other details placed within and stories from the WMS. He or she may recall
around it. many of the correct items from the original sto-
A normal strategy for copying this com- ries but in an incorrect order along with addi-
plex design makes use of the obvious organi- tional irrelevant information based on his or
zational features, such as the large rectangle her own associations to the stories or to other
and the large diagonals, to organize and guide stories presented in the course of testing.
Figure 3–2 The Rey–
Osterreith Complex Figure
(Osterreith and Rey, 1944).

A B

5 5
6 4 2 4
3
0–30 sec 1
3 1 6
2

13
12
31–60 7
10 8
11 9
9
8 12 10
11

16
15
14
17 18 15
16
61–90
18 17
19
13
14

Figure 3–3 Two examples of performance strategies by patients on the Rey–Osterreith Complex Figure.

58
The Boston Process Approach to Neuropsychological Assessment 59

For example, when recalling the second story be inefficient and even forgetful, but in many
from the WMS-III, a patient with a lesion in cases they will still be able to make accurate
the right frontal lobe may respond, “Joe Garcia long-range decisions and relate to others in a
from South Boston did not go out because the consistent, appropriate fashion. Patients who
temperature was only 56 degrees.” In this case, have recovered from aphasia often show this lat-
the patient has recombined elements from the ter pattern of deficits.
two stories into one. Anna Thompson, a char- Professionals who have sustained a head
acter in the first story, was robbed of 56 dollars, injury that resulted in aphasia may often return
and she was from South Boston. This patient to work even though they still have difficulty
has borrowed the elements of 56 and the loca- processing featural information. These patients
tion from that story and added them to the sec- will be less efficient and need more time to
ond story. A patient may also show evidence of accomplish tasks that they once accomplished
configural or contextual priority when recalling easily. Of course, their deficits are likely to
the stories. In that case he or she would be able be most pronounced in areas requiring ver-
to explain the general theme of the story but bal competence. Thus, the analysis of strategy
would rely too heavily on paraphrases and be can be useful in developing rehabilitation pro-
unable to recall specific details. grams. For example, Degutis et al. (2007) suc-
The dimension of featural versus configural cessfully used configural processing strategy
priority is useful in predicting behavior outside training to improve face recognition in adults
the clinical laboratory. For example, patients with congenital prosopagnosia.
who show an inability to process contextual
information despite a preserved ability to pro-
Hemispatial Priority
cess featural information are often found to be
handicapped in situations that require an abil- Though not strictly a cognitive strategy, the
ity to spontaneously organize and direct one’s direction in which patients deploy attention in
own behavior. This inability to organize per- analyzing and solving spatial problems, and the
sonal behavior, along with an impaired ability accuracy with which they are able to use infor-
to detect organization and to interpret complex mation presented visually to the left and right
arrays of information, greatly diminishes large- side of space, is an important source of data
scale goal-directed behavior. These deficits are concerning the integrity of the brain.
subtle, but often manifest in tasks that require It is well known that visual system lesions
responsibility and self-direction. posterior to the optic chiasm and in the occipi-
Thus, the business executive who favors a tal lobe result in visual field losses contralateral
strategy of “featural” priority after a significant to the side of the lesion (Carpenter, 1972). In
brain injury may begin to experience difficulty addition, lesions that occur in the anterior dor-
in his or her job because he or she is unable solateral portion of the occipital lobe or in the
to make long-range decisions, give consistent parietal cortex may result in neglect of, or inat-
orders, and complete complex assignments. tention to, the side of space contralateral to the
Despite this, the executive may still have an lesion (Heilman, 1979). Subtle manifestations
intimate knowledge of the workings of his or of “neglect” or “inattention” may be observed
her business and be able to function in a minor in a patient’s attempt to solve various spatial
advisory capacity and perform more circum- problems, even though the full-blown clinical
scribed tasks requiring less long-term planning. syndrome is not present.
It is not unusual for this loss of sensitivity to the For example, right-handed adults tend to
overall organization and cohesion of informa- begin scanning spatial problems on the left
tion to have a profound negative effect on social side of space, although over the course of many
and personal adjustment. problems they may shift from beginning on
In contrast, patients who have retained their one side of the stimulus to beginning on the
ability to process configural or contextual infor- other. In contrast, patients with lesions of the
mation but who suffer from a diminished ability right hemisphere will characteristically scan
to process the “fine details” of their world may from right to left on spatial problems, whereas
60 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

patients with lesions of the left hemisphere will haphazard strategies and achieve a normal test
often use a stereotyped left-to-right strategy. score, so his test score will not reflect impair-
The latter case can be distinguished from ment. In cases like this, the hit rate using the
a strong normal tendency to scan from left to qualitative analysis method is superior to the
right because in addition to using an inflexible hit rate from methods that do not take qualita-
left-to-right scanning approach to problems a tive information into account. A similar conclu-
patient with a lesion in the left hemisphere will sion was reached by Heaton et al. (1981) when
tend to make more errors and to be slower pro- they demonstrated that clinicians who rated
cessing information in the field contralateral to Halstead–Reitan results had better success in
his or her lesion. Hence, patients with lesions correctly classifying brain-damaged cases than
in the left hemisphere will often have difficulty did a psychometric formula approach rooted
completing the right side of a design, or they heavily in level of performance. Heaton et al.
will omit details from the right side of a design. (1981) believed that the clinicians’ superiority
Adults without brain lesions may show a strong was related to their ability to supplement test
preference for working from left to right on spa- scores with consideration of the qualitative and
tial problems but will not tend to make more configural features of their data. One of the
errors in one particular field. important limits of the “Process Approach” as
originally formulated is the difficulty in subject-
ing clinical judgments based on “qualitative”
Other Specific Strategies
observations to quantitative analysis of valid-
The observation of the informational and spa- ity and the ability to correctly classify patients
tial priorities that a patient uses can be made into diagnostic categories. A number of mea-
across materials, modalities, and functions. sures that have been inspired by this approach
These are only two of the many possible process have been found to have excellent psychometric
variables that have been isolated. They were pre- properties (e.g., the CVLT), but the technique of
sented here because of their pervasiveness and tracking the potentially large base of qualitative
ease of observation. Specific cognitive functions, observations itself has not been systematically
such as memory, praxis, and language, have subjected to systematic psychometric analysis.
special sets of process variables related to each
of them, and this information has been detailed
Clinical Relevance
in Butters and Cermak (1975) and Goodglass
and Kaplan (1972). The greatest strength of this method may be
its usefulness in treatment planning and its
relevance to patients’ daily lives. Qualitative
Strengths of the Process
analysis provides the most precise delineation
Approach
of functions available, and allows the relative
This method of qualitative analysis affords sev- strengths and weaknesses of each patient to
eral advantages over other approaches to the become obvious in a “face valid” manner.
assessment of the neuropsychological sequelae
of brain damage. For the purposes of diagnosis,
Resistance to Practice Effect
it is as valid for the detection and localization
of cortical lesions as other widely used methods This method also shares with other methods
(i.e., Halstead–Reitan: Reitan & Davison, 1974; the advantages of repeatability and compara-
Luria–Nebraska: Golden, 1981). Trained neu- bility across testing intervals (Glosser et al.,
ropsychologists using the procedures described 1982). Although strategic variables are to some
herein report agreement with radiological evi- extent more difficult to quantify, they are less
dence in at least 90% of their cases. In some susceptible to the practice and repetition effects
instances, the qualitative data are inconsistent that can confound interpretation of test scores.
with the quantitative data (i.e., test scores). This makes qualitative data more useful than
For example, a patient who works quickly may test scores alone in the assessment of recovery.
be able to overcome his use of pathological, Using both qualitative and quantitative data
The Boston Process Approach to Neuropsychological Assessment 61

assures the reliable estimate of change that can Verbal skills, naturally, are more sensitive to the
be evaluated normatively from test scores com- effects of education.
bined with an estimate of the effects of change
independent of the effects of practice.
Sensory Motor Handicaps
Our method emphasizes separating strategic
Effects of Aging
differences from generalized slowing. Being
Aging systematically alters neuropsychological slow must be distinguished from being slowed
test performance. Aging also affects strategic up by the difficulty of the task (Welford, 1977).
variables, changes that have been discussed in Peripheral handicaps often make it difficult
detail by Albert and Kaplan (1980). In brief, it to work quickly, but by observing the strategy
appears that normal aging produces strategic used by a disabled patient on verbal and visu-
changes akin to those observed among some ospatial tasks, one can distinguish the defects
patients with frontal system disorder, includ- caused by peripheral injury from those caused
ing cognitive slowing and loss of ability to pro- by cognitive dysfunction and inform efforts at
cess configural information (Hochanadel, 1991; rehabilitation.
Hochanadel & Kaplan, 1984).
The methods of the Process Approach have
Psychopathology
always had the potential for allowing the dif-
ferentiation of aging from specific asymmet- Differentiating severe psychopathology from
ric neuropathologies, such as left frontal or dysfunction related to neurologic processes is
right frontal disease. It is less effective in sort- one of the most difficult tasks for the neuro-
ing out aging from mild generalized cerebral psychologist. Patients with severe psychopathol-
disorder, as occurs in very early dementia. In ogy sometimes perform on neuropsychological
common with other approaches, we based our tests like patients with confirmed lesions of
differentiation partly on estimates of premorbid the CNS.
functioning by considering demographic indi- Chronic schizophrenics often have naming
ces (Karzmark, 1984; Wilson et al., 1979) and problems (Barr et al., 1989), difficulties analyz-
performance on tests relatively resistant to the ing details in visuospatial tasks, and difficulty
effects of brain damage. We also paid attention maintaining attention, deficits that we asso-
to memory impairment that exceeds that to be ciate with left hemisphere pathology. Patients
expected with the benign senescent forgetful- with severe depression resemble patients with
ness of normal aging, and to strategic pathol- subcortical depression (Massman et al., 1992)
ogies reflecting frontal lobe dysfunction that and may also be similar to patients with known
are more severe than one ordinarily encounters right hemisphere pathology and, in particular,
in the elderly. Regrettably, our normative work right frontal lobe dysfunction. These patients
is not yet sufficiently advanced to propose spe- can have difficulty analyzing contextual infor-
cific rules or norms to aid in this important mation relative to a preserved ability to use
distinction. details. In addition, they can have difficulty with
visuospatial memory, although their memory
for verbal materials is relatively intact in terms
Effects of Education
of recalling details.
In terms of qualitative information, it was sur-
mised that people who are 50–60 years old and
Summary
who have completed at least ninth grade show
little difference in strategy from individuals The Boston Process Approach was a systematic
who have completed high school and college on method for assessing qualitative neuropsycho-
most tasks involving visuospatial information. logical information and was an important step
Amount of education does not appear to pro- in integrating the “cognitive revolution” occur-
duce strategic differences in scanning, stimulus ring in academic experimental psychology
selectivity, and contextual or featural sensitivity. with clinical neuropsychological practice. The
62 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

original method of integrating process-based Alexander, M. P., Stuss, D. T., & Fansabedian, N.
modifications and observations with traditional (2003). California Verbal Learning Test: Perfor-
test performance data has added sensitivity and mance by patients with focal frontal and non-
meaning to neuropsychological assessment and frontal lesions. Brain, 126(6), 1493–1503.
Barr, W. B., Bilder, R. M., Goldberg, E., & Kaplan, E.
has evolved into a new generation of tests that
(1989). The neuropsychology of schizophrenic
incorporate many of the concepts developed
speech. Journal of Communication Disorders, 22,
by Dr. Kaplan and her colleagues. The orig- 327–349.
inal method still has important applications Bender, L. A. (1938). A visual motor gestalt test
for clinical descriptive uses, but the method and its clinical use. American Orthopsychiatric
itself, by the nature of its complexity, did not Association Research Monographs, No. 3.
result in a significant body of supportive scien- Benton, A. L. (1950). A multiple choice type of
tific literature. Rather, it served as the ground- visual retention test. Archives of Neurology and
work for new approaches, which in fact have Psychiatry, 64, 699–707.
become some of the central building blocks of Benton, A. L., Hamsher, K. deS., Varney, N. R., &
modern cognitive psychology and cognitive Spreen, O. (1983). Judgment of line orientation.
New York: Oxford University Press.
neuroscience. We have discussed two strategic
Berger, M. (1993). Sensitivity of neuropsychological
elements—featural versus contextual priority
instruments. Unpublished doctoral dissertation.
and hemispatial priority—to illustrate the pos- S.U.N.Y., Albany, NY.
sibilities of our approach. As the method we Butters, N., & Cermak, L. S. (1975). Some analyses of
have described is refi ned both in our laboratory amnesia syndrome in brain damaged patients. In
and by other investigators, we foresee that it K. Pribram & R. Isaacson (Eds.), The hippocampus
will help move neuropsychological assessment (pp. 377–409). New York: Plenum Press.
beyond a reliable cataloging of deficits toward Butters, N., & Cermak, L. S. (1980). The Alcoholic
an understanding of the underlying processes. Korsakoff ’s Syndrome. An information processing
With such an understanding, neuropsychology approach to amnesia. New York: Academic Press.
will be in a better position to assist in the more Butters, N., & Grady, M. (1977). Effects of predistrac-
tor delays on the short-term memory performance
important task of treatment planning and reha-
of patients with Korsakoff ’s and Huntington’s
bilitation. As has been emphasized throughout
Disease. Neuropsychologia, 13, 701–705.
this chapter, the Process Approach as originally Carpenter, M. B. (1972). Core text of neuroanatomy.
formulated, by its nature, has been difficult to be Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins.
directly used in scientific investigations, though Christiansen, A. L. (1975). Luria’s neuropsychological
it has had an important impact on the direction investigation: Text, manual, and test cards. New
of neuropsychological assessment, inspiring York: Spectrum.
a modernization of the practice of the clinical Coltheart, M., Hull, E., & Slater, D. (1975). Sex dif-
assessment of brain–behavior relationships. ferences in imagery and reading. Nature, 253,
438–440.
Critchley, M. (1953). The pareital lobes. London:
Acknowledgments Edward Arnold.
Cronbach, L. J. (1957). The two disciplines of sci-
The work was supported in part by a VA Merit
entific psychology. American Psychologist, 12,
Review grant to William Milberg at the West
671–684.
Roxbury VA Medical Center and NINDS DeGutis, J. M., Bentin, S., Robertson, L. C., &
Program Project Grant NS 26985 to Boston D’Esposito, M. (2007). Functional plasticity in
University School of Medicine. ventral temporal cortex following cognitive reha-
bilitation of a congential prosopagnosic. Journal of
Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(11), 1790–1802.
References
Delis, D. C., Direnfeld, L., Alexander, M. P., &
Albert, M. S., & Kaplan, E. (1980). Organic impli- Kaplan, E. (1982). Cognitive fluctuations associ-
cations of neuropsychological deficits in the ated with the on–off phenomenon in Parkinson’s
elderly. In L. W. Poon, J. L. Fozard, L. S. Cermak, disease. Neurology, 32, 1049–1052.
D. Arenberg, & L. W. Thompson (Eds.), New Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Fridland, A. J., &
directions in memory and aging (pp. 403–432). Kaplan, E. (1990). A cognitive science approach to
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. neuropsychological assessment. In P. McReynolds,
The Boston Process Approach to Neuropsychological Assessment 63

J. C. Rosen, & G. Chelune (Eds.), Advances in Grande, L., McGlinchey, R., Barber, C.,
psychological assessment (vol. 7, pp. 101–132). Rudolph, J., & Milberg, W. (Submitted). Detecting
New York: Plenum Press. cognitive impairment in individuals at risk for
Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., & Kaplan, E. (1984). cerebrovascular disease: The 90-second “Clock-
The California proverbs test. Boston: Boston in-the-Box” Screening Test. Archives of Clinical
Neuropsychological Foundation. Neuropsychology.
Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., & Kaplan, E. (2001). The Grant, D. A., & Berg, E. A. (1948). A behavioral analy-
Delis–Kaplan Executive Function System Test sis of degree of reinforcement and ease of shifting to
manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological new responses in a Weigl-type card sorting program.
Corporation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 404–411.
Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E. & Ober, B. Haeberle, K. C. (1982). Multidimensional scal-
A. (1987). The California Verbal Learning Test. ing of block design patterns. Paper presented at
Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological the annual meeting of the Eastern Psychological
Corporation. Association, Baltimore, Maryland.
Delis, D. C., Robertson, L. C., & Efron, R. (1986). Halstead, W. C. (1947). Brain and intelligence:
Hemisphere specialization of memory for visual Quantitative study of the frontal lobes. Chicago:
hierarchical organization. Neuropsychologia, 24, University of Chicago Press.
205–214. Hamann, C., McGlinchey-Berroth, R., Minaker, K., &
Delis, D. C., Squire, L. R., Bihrle, A., & Massman, P. J. Milberg, W. (1993). Validation of a neuropsy-
(1992). Componential analysis of problem-solving chological screening instrument of older adults.
ability: Performance of patients with frontal lobe Geriatric Research Training Center, Project 7
damage and amnesic patients on a new sorting (Core 2), Harvard Division on Aging.
test. Neuropsychologia, 30, 683–697. Heaton, R. K., Grant, I., Anthony, W. Z., & Lehman,
Feldman, M. J., & Drasgow, J. (1960). The Visual– R. A. W. (1981). A comparison of clinical and
Verbal Test. Manual. Beverly Hills, CA: Western automated interpretation of the Halstead–Reitan
Psychological Services. Battery. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 3,
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). 121–141.
“Mini-mental State.” A practical method for grad- Heilman, K. M. (1979). Neglect and related disorders.
ing the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. In K. M. Heilman & C. Valenstein (Eds.), Clinical
Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189–198. neuropsychology (pp. 268–307). New York: Oxford
Freedman, M., Leach, L., Kaplan, E., Winocur, G., University Press.
Shulman, K., & Delis, D. (1995). Clock drawing: Hochanadel, G. (1991). Neuropsychological changes
A neuropsychological analysis. New York: Oxford in aging: A process oriented error analysis. Unpub-
University Press. lished doctoral dissertation. Clark University,
Geschwind, N. (1975). The apraxias: Neural mecha- Worcester, MA.
nisms of disorders of learned movement. American Hochanadel, G., & Kaplan, E. (1984). Neuropsychology
Scientist, 63, 188–195. of normal aging. In M. L. Albert (Ed.), Clinical neu-
Glosser, G., Kaplan, E., & LoVerme, S. (1982). rology of aging (pp. 231–244). New York: Oxford
Longitudinal neuropsychological report of apha- University Press.
sia following left subcortical hemorrhage. Brain Hooper, H. E. (1958). The Hooper Visual Organization
and Language, 15, 95–116. Test Manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological
Golden, C. J. (1981). A standardized version of Luria’s Services.
neuropsychological tests. In S. J. Filskov & T. J. Boll Horn, J. L., & Cattell, R. B. (1967). Age differ-
(Eds.), Handbook of clinical neuropsychology (pp. ences in fluid and crystallized intelligence. Acta
608–642). New York: John Wiley and Sons. Psychologica, 26, 107–129.
Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (1963). Disturbance Hunt, E. (1983). On the nature of intelligence. Science,
of gesture and pantomime in aphasia. Brain, 86, 219, 141–146.
708–720. Jastak, J. F., & Jastak, S. R. (1984). The Wide Range
Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (1972). The assessment Achievement Test Manual (Revised). Los Angeles:
of aphasia and related disorders. Philadelphia: Lea Western Psychological Services.
and Febiger. Joy, S., Kaplan, E., & Fein, D. (1992a). The Information
Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. F. (1983). Boston diag- Test of the WAIS-R as a Neuropsychological
nostic aphasia examination (2nd ed.). Media, PA: Instrument among healthy older adults. Paper
Williams & Wilkins. presented at the 14th European Conference of
Gorham, D. R. (1956). Proverbs test. Missoula: the International Neuropsychological Society,
Psychological Test Specialists. Durham, England.
64 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

Joy, S., Kaplan, E., & Fein, D. (1992b). Factors affect- Massman, P. J., Delis, D. C., Butters, N., Dupont, R. M.,
ing Digit Symbol performance in aging. Paper pre- & Gillin, J. C. (1992). The subcortical dysfunc-
sented at the 100th Annual Convention, American tion hypothesis of memory deficits in depression:
Psychological Association, Washington, DC. Neuropsychological validation in a subgroup of
Kaplan, E. (1968). Gestural representation of imple- patients. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
ment usage: An organismic-developmental study. Neuropsychology, 5, 687–706.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Worcester, Milberg, W., & Blumstein, S. E. (1981). Lexical deci-
MA: Clark University. sion and aphasia: Evidence for semantic process-
Kaplan, E. (1983). Process and achievement revisited. ing. Brain and Language, 14, 371–385.
In S. Wapner & B. Kaplan (Eds.), Toward a holistic Milberg, W., Cummings, J., Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E.
developmental psychology (pp. 143–156). Hillsdale, (1979). Case report: A global sequential process-
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.. ing disorder following head injury: A possible role
Kaplan, E. (1988). A process approach to neuro- for the right hemisphere in serial order behavior.
psychological assessment. In T. Boll & B. K. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 1, 213–225.
Bryant (Eds.), Clinical neuropsychology and brain Milberg, W., Greiffenstein, M., Lewis, R., &
function: Research, measurement, and prac- Rourke, D. (1980). Differentiation of tempo-
tice (pp. 125–167). Washington, DC: American ral lobe and generalized seizure patients with
Psychological Association. the WAIS. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Kaplan, E. (1990). The process approach to neu- Psychology, 48, 39–42.
ropsychological assessment of psychiatric Milberg, W., MacDonald, R., Odenheimer, G.,
patients. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical McGlinchey-Berroth, R., Hamann, C., Weitzen,
Neurosciences, 2, 72–87. S., et al. (1992). Geriatric evaluation of mental sta-
Kaplan, E., Caine, E., & Morse, P. (1981). Boston/ tus. Unpublished Test, Brockton/West Roxbury
Rochester Neuropsychological Screening Test. Geriatric Research, Education, Clinical Center.
Unpublished Test. Munshi, M., Grande, L., Hayes, M., Ayres, D.,
Kaplan, E., Fein, D., Kramer, J., Delis, D., & Morris, Suhl, E., Capelson, N. P., Lin, S., et al. (2006).
R. (Ed.). (1989). WISC-111 as a process instrument Cognitive dysfunction is associated with poor dia-
(WISC-III PI). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological betes control. Diabetes Care, 29(8), 1794–1799.
Corporation. Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology. New York:
Kaplan, E., Fein, D., Morris, R., & Delis, D. C. (1991). Appleton, Century, Crofts.
WAIS-R as a Neuropsychological Instrument. Neisser, U., Novick, R., & Lager, R. (1963). Searching
Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological for ten targets simultaneously. Perceptual and
Corporation. Motor Skills, 17, 955–961.
Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H., & Weintraub, S. (1983). Osterreith, P., & Rey, A. (1944). Le test de copie d’une
The Boston Naming Test (2nd ed.). Philadelphia: figure complexe. Archives de Psychologie, 30,
Lea and Febiger. 206–356.
Kaplan, E., & Morris, R. (1985). Boston modification Palmer, P., & Kaplan, E. (1985). The cow and cir-
of the WAIS-R. Unpublished Test. cle experimental object assemblies. New Object
Karzmark, P., Heaton, R. K., Grant, I., & Mathews, Assembly components of the WAIS-R NI. San
C. G. (1984). Use of demographic variables to pre- Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
dict full scale IQ and level of performance on the Perry, W., Potterat, E., Auslander, L., Kaplan, E., &
Halstead–Reitan Battery. Journal of Consulting Jeste, D. (1996). A neuropsychological approach
and Clinical Psychology, 52, 663–665. to the Rorschach in patients with dementias of the
Kløve, H. (1963). Grooved pegboard. Lafayette, IN: Alzheimer type. Assessment, 3, 351–363.
Lafayette Instruments. Poreh, A. M. (2000). The quanitifed process approach:
Levine, B., Milberg, W., & Stuss, D. (1993). Boston An emerging methodology to neuropsychological
evaluation of executive functions. Unpublished assessment. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 14(2),
Test. 212–222.
Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., & Loring, D. W. Poreh, A. M. (2006). The quantified approach to neu-
(2004). Neuropsychological assessment (3rd ed.). ropsychological assessment. New York: Psychology
New York: Oxford University Press. Press.
Libon, D. J., Swenson, R., Barnoski, E. J., & Sands, L. Porteus, S. D. (1965). Porteus Maze Test. Palo Alto:
P. (1993). Clock Drawing as an assessment tool for Pacific Books.
dementia. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 8, Powell, D. H., Kaplan, E., Whitla, D., Weintraub, S.,
405–415. Catlin, R., & Funkenstein, H. H. (1993). Microcog
The Boston Process Approach to Neuropsychological Assessment 65

Assessment of Cognitive Functioning Manual. San clustering, encoding, and storage. Neuropsychology,
Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. 6, 43–50.
Raven, J. C. (1960). Guide to the standard progressive South, M. B., Greve, K. W., Bianchini, K. J., &
matrices. London: H. K. Lewis. Adams, D. (2001). Interater reliability of three
Reitan, R. M., & Davison, L. A. (1974). Clinical neu- clock drawing test scoring systems. Applied
ropsychology: Current status and applications. Neuropsychology, 8, 174–179.
New York: Winston/Wiley. Stern, R., Singer, E. A., Duke, L. M., Singer, N. G.,
Rey, A. (1964). L’Examen Clinique en Psychologie. Morey, C. E., Daughtrey, E. W., et al. (1994).
Paris Presses Universitaires de France. The Boston Qualitative Scoring System for the
Robertson, L. C., & Delis, D. C. (1986). “Part-whole” Rey–Osterreith Complex Figure: Description and
processing in unilateral brain-damaged patients: interrater reliability. The Clinical Neuropsychologist,
Dysfunction of hierarchical organization. 8, 309–322.
Neuropsychologia, 24, 363–370. Sternberg, R. J. (1980). Sketch of a componential sub-
Rorschach, H. (1942). Psychodiagnostics: A diag- theory of human intelligence. Behavioral Brain
nostic test based on perception (P. Lemkau and Science, 3, 573–614.
B. Kronenberg, translators). Berne: Huber; U.S. Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in
Distributor: Grune and Stratton. serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental
Rouleau, I., Salmon, D. P., & Butters, N. (1996). Psychology, 18, 643–662.
Longitudinal analysis of clock drawing in Stuss, D. T., & Benson, D. F. (1986). The frontal lobes.
Alzheimer’s disease patients. Brain and Cognition, New York: Raven Press.
31, 17–34. Talland, G. A. (1965). Deranged memory. New York:
Royall, D. R., Cordes, J. A., & Polk, M. (1998). Academic Press.
CLOX: An executive clock drawing task. Journal Thomas, R., & Forder, E. (2006). The role of local and
of Neurological Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 64, global processing in the recognition of living and
588–594. non-living things. Neuropsychologia, 44, 982–986.
Royer, F. L. (1967). Information processing in the Thurstone, L. L. (1938). Primary mental abilities.
Block Design task. Intelligence, 1, 23–50. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Russell, E. W. (1981). Some principles of psychomet- Wechsler, D. A. (1945). A standardized memory scale
ric neuropsychology and the Halstead–Reitan for clinical use. Journal of Psychology, 19, 87–95.
Battery. Perspectives in V. A. Neuropsychology Wechsler, D. A. (1955). Wechsler Adult Intelligence
Rehabilitation: Proceedings of the Mental Health Scale. New York: The Psychological Corporation.
and Behavioral Sciences Service Conference. Salt Wechsler, D. A. (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence
Lake City, Utah. Scale-Revised. New York: The Psychological
Russell, E. W., Neuringer, C., & Goldstein, G. (1970). Corporation.
Assessment of brain damage: A neuropsychological Wechsler, D. A. (1987). Wechsler Memory Scale-
key approach. New York: John Wiley and Sons. Revised. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological
Sachs, W., Milberg, W., & McGlinchey-Berroth, R. Corporation.
(1995). The detection of cognitive impairment in Wechsler, D. A. (1991). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for
elderly inpatients using the Geriatric Evaluation of Children-III. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological
Mental Status. Unpublished Manuscript. Corporation.
Schmeck, R. R., & Grove, E. (1979). Academic Wechsler, D. A. (1997). Wechsler Memory Scale—3rd
achievement and individual differences in learn- Edition (WMS-III) (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX: The
ing processes. Applied Psychological Measurement, Psychological Corporation.
3, 43–49. Welford, A. T. (1977). Causes of slowing of per-
Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairment in planning. formance with age. Interdisciplinary Topics in
In D. E. Broadbent & L. Weiskrantz (Eds.), The neu- Gerontology, 11, 43–45.
ropsychology of cognitive function (pp. 199–209). Werner, H. (1937). Process and achievement: A basic
London: The Royal Society. problem of education and developmental psychol-
Shipley, W. C. (1940). A self-administering scale for ogy. Harvard Educational Review, 7, 353–368.
measuring intellectual impairment and deteriora- Wilson, R. S., Rosenbaum, G., Brown, G., & Grisell, J.
tion. Journal of Psychology, 9, 371–377. (1979). An index of premorbid intelligence.
Shorr, J. S., Delis, D. C., & Massman, P. J. (1992). Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46,
Memory for the Rey–Osterreith figure: Perceptual 1554–1555.
4

The Iowa-Benton School


of Neuropsychological Assessment
Daniel Tranel

Historical Introduction operation gradually grew in scope, however,


fueled by the labor provided by a succession of
Early Developments
graduate students. By 1952, a number of grad-
The Iowa-Benton (I-B) school of neuropsy- uate students had begun to receive practicum
chological assessment dates back more than a training in neuropsychological assessment, with
half century. In 1950, Arthur Benton set up a their training in neuropsychology supplemented
small neuropsychology unit, at the invitation by attendance at the Saturday morning grand
of Dr. Adolph Sahs, who was the Head of the rounds of the Neurology and Neurosurgery
Department of Neurology at the University of staffs. In 1953, thesis and dissertation research
Iowa Hospitals and Clinics. Dr. Benton’s service in neuropsychology was instituted. Benton’s first
was placed in the Department of Neurology, students included Heilbrun, Wahler, Swanson,
where it has remained until the current day, and and Blackburn, all of whom earned their Master’s
this close affi liation with Neurology has been and/or PhD degrees in the mid-1950s. In 1956,
an important influence in the development of Benton initiated a seminar in neuropsychology,
the Iowa approach. In the initial arrangement, aimed at residents in neurology and neurosur-
Benton agreed to evaluate patients referred by gery and also available to graduate students.
either Dr. Sahs or Dr. Russell Meyers, who was During the first several years of the neuropsy-
the Chair of the Division of Neurosurgery at chology operation, Benton’s research focused on
UIHC. In return, Benton and his students were development and disturbances of body schema
permitted to use case material from Neurology (Benton, 1955a, 1955b; Benton & Abramson,
and Neurosurgery for research purposes. These 1952; Benton & Cohen, 1955; Benton & Menefee,
strong ties to Neurology and Neurosurgery 1957; Swanson, 1957; Swanson & Benton, 1955).
(the latter now being a department, headed by Another early theme was hemispheric differ-
Dr. Matthew Howard) continue to catalyze ences in neuropsychological performance pat-
both the clinical and research functions of the terns (Heilbrun, 1956, 1959). Reaction time
Benton Laboratory. was another early research interest (Blackburn,
In the early days, the Neuropsychology Clinic 1958; Blackburn & Benton, 1955), as were qual-
was a modest operation. Benton was a full-time itative features of performance (Wahler, 1956).
member of the Department of Psychology and In addition, Benton wrote several papers cov-
the director of its graduate program in clinical ering historical aspects of the development of
psychology, and these roles occupied most of neuropsychology (Benton, 1956), including
his time. He typically would spend two after- one in which he discussed the Gerstmann syn-
noons and a Saturday morning each week in the drome (Benton & Meyers, 1956; for an interest-
Neuropsychology Clinic. The Neuropsychology ing historical comparison, see Benton, 1992).

66
The Iowa-Benton School of Neuropsychological Assessment 67

Neuropsychology Expands A Philosophy of Assessment


In 1958, Benton accepted a joint appointment Virtually all of Benton’s professional career
as Professor of Neurology and Psychology, had been spent in medical facilities, where he
and he moved his main office to the University had had the opportunity of watching skilled
Hospitals. Thereafter, the Neuropsychology neurologists and psychiatrists such as Spafford
unit expanded considerably, becoming what Ackerly, Macdonald Critchley, Raymond
would be known as the “Neuropsychology Garcin, and Phyllis Greenacre evaluate patients.
Laboratory.” A technician and full-time sec- He noted that, having conducted a brief “men-
retary were engaged. In 1961, Otfried Spreen tal status” examination, they would probe the
joined the laboratory as a second professional diagnostic possibilities by diverse questions
staff member, and the Laboratory became a and maneuvers (the reasons for which were
major center for practicum training and MA not always apparent to the auditors at grand
and PhD research. Between the years of 1959 rounds!). Benton observed that their evaluations
and 1978, the Neuropsychology Laboratory were extremely variable in length. Some were
sponsored 25 PhD dissertations and 17 MA completed in 15 minutes, and others took more
theses. The research program was highly pro- than an hour, after which the “chief” would dis-
ductive, and the influence of the Laboratory cuss the significance of the findings generated
permeated the cognate specialties of behavioral by the questions and procedures in relation to
neurology and neuropsychiatry, as well as the the diagnosis. What forcibly impressed him was
field of neuropsychology. During the 1960s and the flexibility in choice of procedures and the
1970s, the Laboratory produced about 170 scien- continuous hypothesis testing that these astute
tific and scholarly publications on diverse topics examiners engaged in as they explored the diag-
in neuropsychology, many of which are recog- nostic possibilities.
nized still as standards in the field (cf. Costa & Benton was also struck by the essential iden-
Spreen, 1985; Hamsher, 1985). tity in the approaches of an “organic” neurologist
Benton achieved emeritus status in 1978, (Critchley) and a psychoanalytically oriented
and he remained fully active as a scholar, men- psychiatrist (Greenacre). Both adopted flexible
tor, and leader in the field of neuropsychology. procedures as they pursued one or another lead
At that point, the Neuropsychology Laboratory that might disclose the basic neuropathology or
became a core facility in the newly created psychopathology underlying a patient’s overt
Division of Behavioral Neurology and Cognitive disabilities. Benton concluded that neuropsy-
Neuroscience, established in the late 1970s by chological assessment should follow the model
Drs. Antonio Damasio and Hanna Damasio. exemplified by the diagnostic strategy of these
Under the leadership of Paul Eslinger, PhD, the eminent physicians. Even though neuropsycho-
Laboratory played a key role in the development logical assessment involved the employment of
of the ambitious research program in cognitive standardized objective tests, it could be flexible.
neuroscience instituted by the Damasios in the It need not consist of the administration of a
early 1980s. In 1986, Daniel Tranel, PhD, assumed standard battery of tests measuring a predeter-
direction of the laboratory and, at this point, offi- mined set of performances in every patient. In
cially designated the laboratory as the “Benton this sense, it was more akin to a clinical exam-
Neuropsychology Laboratory,” a title it keeps to ination than to a laboratory procedure. On one
this day. The delivery of clinical services in the occasion, he wrote:
Benton Laboratory expanded considerably, to the
point where the annual throughput of patients neuropsychological assessment is essentially a
refinement of clinical neurological observation
began to number close to 2000. In the late 1990s,
and not a ‘laboratory procedure’ in the same class
the Benton Neuropsychology Laboratory moved as serology, radiology, or electroencephalogra-
to a spacious, newly remodeled area, which phy. . . . Neuropsychological testing assesses the same
included a waiting room, patient check-in, four behavior that the neurologist observes clinically. It
exam rooms, a rehabilitation laboratory, a driv- serves the function of enhancing clinical observa-
ing simulator, and faculty and staff offices. tion. . . . neuropsychological assessment is very closely
68 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

allied to clinical neurological evaluation and in fact test procedures, it elicits types of performance that
can be considered to be a special form of it. (Benton, are not accessible to the clinical observer. (Benton,
1975, p. 68) 1991; p. 507)
Subsequently, he restated his position in these This conception of the nature of neuropsy-
words: chological assessment was reflected in Benton’s
Neuropsychology unit at the University of Iowa
The fact is that none of the batteries that are widely
Hospitals and Clinics. In the 1960s the core
used today adequately meet the need for a well focused
analysis of the cognitive status of patients with actual battery for a nonaphasic patient would consist
or suspected brain disease. Such a battery should pro- typically of three WAIS subtests (Information,
vide reliable assessments of a number of learning and Comprehension, Arithmetic) from the Verbal
memory performances and of at least the semantic Scale and three (Block Design, Picture Arran-
aspects of language function as well as of visuoper- gement, Picture Completion) from the Perfor-
ceptive and visuospatial functions. But even a fairly mance Scale. Then, depending upon the referral
comprehensive battery of tests cannot be regarded as question and the characteristics of the patient’s
necessarily being the endpoint of assessment since it performance on the core battery, additional
cannot possibly answer (or attempt to answer) all the tests (which might include some of the remain-
questions that may be raised about different patients.
ing WAIS subtests) would be given to explore
Moreover, the administration of such a battery tends
the diagnostic possibilities. This core battery
to be wasteful of time and expense.
Instead it may be useful to think in terms of a core was gradually modified over the years so that, for
battery of modest length, perhaps five or six care- example, in the late 1970s, only the Arithmetic
fully selected tests that would take not more than and Block Designs subtests of the WAIS were
30 minutes to give. Then, depending upon both given while other tests such as temporal orien-
the specific referral question and the patient’s pat- tation, the Token Test and the Visual Retention
tern of performance on the core battery, explora- Test found a place in the battery. It was in the
tion of specific possibilities may be indicated, e.g., context of giving additional tests to answer spe-
an aphasic disorder, a visuoconstructive disability, cific questions that the need for different types
a visuospatial disorder, or specific impairment in of tests emerged. This provided the impetus
abstract reasoning. For this purpose we should have
for the development of the diverse test meth-
available a large inventory of well-standardized tests
ods associated with the Iowa laboratory, for
from which a selection can be made in an attempt
to answer the diagnostic questions. Administration example, facial discrimination (Benton & Van
of the core battery may suffice to answer the refer- Allen, 1968), controlled oral word association
ral question in some cases. In other cases, 20 tests (Bechtoldt et al., 1962; Fogel, 1962), three-
may have to be given and even then the answer to dimensional constructional praxis (Benton &
the question may not be forthcoming. In short, I Fogel, 1962), motor impersistence (Joynt et al.,
think that we should regard neuropsychological 1962), and judgment of line orientation (Benton
assessment in the same way as we view the physi- et al., 1978).
cal or neurological examination, i.e., as a logical
sequential decision-making process rather than as
simply the administration of a fi xed battery of tests. Current Practice
(Benton, 1985; p. 15)
The I-B School of Neuropsychology
More recently, he wrote:
The philosophy of neuropsychological assess-
Neuropsychological assessment consists essen- ment in the current I-B school has evolved over
tially of a set of clinical examination procedures the past several decades from the early tenets
and hence does not differ in kind from conven- established by Benton. In its current prac-
tional clinical observation. Both neuropsychological
tice, the I-B school can be conceptualized as
assessment and clinical observation deal with the
same basic data, namely, the behavior of the patient.
somewhat of a hybrid approach that maintains
Neuropsychological assessment may be viewed as a Benton’s emphasis on flexibility, efficiency, and
refi nement and extension of clinical observation—a the clinical context, while also incorporating a
refinement in that it describes a patient’s perfor- more formal “laboratory procedure” mentality
mances more precisely and reliably, and an exten- that emphasizes basic coverage of all higher-
sion in that, through instrumentation and special order cognitive and behavioral functions by
The Iowa-Benton School of Neuropsychological Assessment 69

the examination. The principal objective is implemented with an eye toward increasing
to obtain quantitative measurements of key the type of information that will be especially
domains of cognition and behavior, in a time- helpful in the neuropsychological rehabilitation
efficient manner, in sufficient breadth and depth setting, which is in many cases the next stop-
that the referral question can be answered. off for patients referred for neuropsychological
The amount of testing required to meet this assessment.
objective varies considerably across different Over the years, the segment of the I-B exam-
patients, situations, and referral questions, ination that is devoted to providing direct
ranging from a lower figure of 15–30 minutes feedback to the patient, family, and other care-
to a high of 7 hours or even longer. The I-B givers has increased substantially—in fact, this
approach is flexible and hypothesis driven. The is probably one of the most notable changes in
selection of tests given to a patient is guided the I-B examination over the past two decades.
by a number of factors, including the nature of In earlier days, the feedback component of the
the patient’s complaint, the questions raised by examination was minimal, and the neuro-
the referring entity (physician, family, social psychologist often left to the referring physician
agency, etc.), the impressions gained from the the task of providing feedback on “laboratory
initial interview, and, especially, the diagnos- tests,” including the neuropsychological exam.
tic possibilities raised by the patient’s perfor- But this has changed, and it is customary now
mances during the course of the examination. for the attending neuropsychologist to pro-
In brief, the procedure involves the adminis- vide extensive feedback to the patient and rel-
tration of relevant tests selected from the rich evant persons who accompany the patient to
armamentarium available to neuropsycholo- the exam, regarding the nature of the findings
gists. The results of the tests are interpreted in from the testing, the meaning of the findings
the context of other contextual and diagnos- vis-à-vis diagnostic considerations, and the
tic information, which typically includes the implications of the findings in regard to prac-
medical history, neurological fi ndings, and tical matters such as day-to-day functioning.
neuroimaging (computed tomography [CT], Recommendations for rehabilitation or related
magnetic resonance imaging [MRI]) data, and forms of treatment are frequently provided,
may also encompass electroencephalographic and an effort is made to identify specific local
(EEG) fi ndings, neurosurgical reports, and providers with whom the patient can follow up
functional imaging (e.g., positron emission (e.g., a clinical psychologist). It is common for
tomography [PET]) results. The interpretation the neuropsychologist to provide specific rec-
strategy used in the I-B school is integrative ommendations in regard to driving privileges,
and hypothesis focused, in a manner similar management of finances, living arrangements,
to the neuropsychological testing procedure. and other important life situations that may be
Thus, the current practice of the I-B approach profoundly equivocal in patients with major
is properly considered a laboratory procedure, neurological disease. Oftentimes, this type of
but one that retains Benton’s emphasis on flex- feedback is the first time the patient and fam-
ibility and efficiency. ily will be hearing messages such as this, and
The amount of testing used in the I-B approach a rapport-based, therapeutic context is critical
is not strictly predetermined, although we typi- for the feedback to be understood and assimi-
cally employ a core set of procedures (described lated. The feedback is often the most challeng-
below) that usually takes about 3–3½ hours to ing aspect of the examination, demanding
administer. To some extent, the test protocol is from the neuropsychologist a full deployment
formulated anew for each patient, according to of clinical psychological skills to maximize the
the particular exigencies of the situation, and chances that the information would yield the
the examination is pursued to the extent neces- largest benefit to the patient and family. Telling
sary to achieve the main objective of answering a patient that “you should stop driving, and
the referral question as definitively as possible. move to a nursing home” is not a trivial matter,
And in many cases, our assessments are guided and providing feedback of this nature in a tact-
by rehabilitation considerations (cf. Anderson, ful, respectful, and effective manner requires a
1996, 2002). For example, tests are chosen and full repertoire of clinical skills. Moreover, we
70 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

normally provide feedback immediately after Table 4–1. The Iowa-Benton Core Batterya
the testing, so that patients and families leave
our clinic with a full understanding of the out- 1. Interview
2. Orientation to time, personal information,
come of our testing and relevant recommenda- and place
tions. Immediate feedback is widely regarded 3. Wide Range Achievement Test-4, Reading
as very valuable to patients and families, but Subtest (or Wechsler Test of Adult Reading)
this service places high demands on the clinical 4. Recall of recent presidents
neuropsychologists, who must conceptualize 5. Information subtest (WAIS-IV)
6. Complex Figure Test (copy and delayed
the case on the spot and formulate immediate recall)
interpretations and recommendations. Current 7. Auditory Verbal Learning Test (with delayed
training in the Benton Laboratory places a high recall) (or California Verbal Learning Test)
priority on these skills. 8. Draw a clock
9. Arithmetic subtest (WAIS-IV)
10. Block Design subtest (WAIS-IV)
A Core Battery. It was noted earlier that Benton 11. Digit Span subtest (WAIS-IV)
developed a core battery of tests, which evolved 12. Similarities subtest (WAIS-IV)
over the years. This tradition continues in the 13. Trail-Making Test
current I-B school. In fact, Benton has pointed 14. Coding subtest (WAIS-IV)
out that one risk of a “flexible” approach is that 15. Controlled Oral Word Association Test
16. Benton Visual Retention Test
it can become too flexible, so that no two exam- 17. Benton Facial Discrimination Test
inations are the same, and neuropsychologists 18. Boston Naming Test
begin using idiosyncratic sets of tests that have 19. Picture Arrangement subtest (WAIS-III)
little overlap with those used by other practi- 20. Geschwind-Oldfield Handedness
tioners (not to mention the frequent shortcom- Questionnaire
21. Beck Depression Inventory-II
ings in normative data). To avoid this problem 22. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory
and to provide a structured set of tests that
a
serves as a starting point for neuropsychologi- References for tests in the Core Battery are Benton et al.
(1983), Lezak et al. (2004), Spreen and Strauss (1998), and
cal assessment, we utilize a Core Battery in the Wechsler (2008).
I-B school. The Core Battery is enumerated in
Table 4–1 (the tests are listed in order of adminis-
tration). (As of November 2008, we began using of his or her situation. Typically, the neuro-
the newly published WAIS-IV (Wechsler, 2008) psychologist will formulate a working hypoth-
in our evaluations. Where relevant, the WAIS- esis about the case during the initial interview.
III subtests listed in the Core and follow-up In keeping with the I-B philosophy, the inter-
procedures below have been replaced by their view varies considerably in length, depending
WAIS-IV counterparts. We have retained the upon how rapidly and to what degree of cer-
use of Picture Arrangement from the WAIS-III, tainty the neuropsychologist can formulate a
however, as this subtest was not carried forward testable hypothesis about the case. During the
in the WAIS-IV battery and we have found interview, the neuropsychologist oftentimes
Picture Arrangement useful as an index of non- administers a few tests to the patient. The exam-
verbal social reasoning.) ination then proceeds with the collection of
All examinations begin with an interview of formal test data by a technician. Following rec-
the patient by the neuropsychologist. Family ommendations and proscriptions of the govern-
members are typically present during this phase, ing bodies of the field (American Academy of
and they are called upon to supplement and Clinical Neuropsychology; National Academy
corroborate the history provided by the patient. of Neuropsychology), we use a technician-based
The interview is an indispensable and crucial method of assessment (DeLuca & Putman, 1993;
source of information. It provides clues about National Academy of Neuropsychology, 2000a),
the nature and cause of the patient’s presenting and the testing is conducted in the absence of
complaints, the patient’s capacity and motiva- third-party observation (American Academy
tion to cooperate with the testing procedures, of Clinical Neuropsychology, 2001; National
and the extent to which the patient is aware Academy of Neuropsychology, 2000b).
The Iowa-Benton School of Neuropsychological Assessment 71

The Core Battery has evolved over the years not uncommon in the early stages of multiple
to satisfy mutual objectives of comprehensive- sclerosis, progressive dementia syndromes (e.g.,
ness and efficiency. The examination is struc- Alzheimer’s disease, Pick’s disease (or what is
tured to probe all major domains of cognition, now more commonly called “frontal-temporal
including intellectual function, memory, speech dementia”)), HIV-related dementia, or mild
and language, visual perception, psychomotor/ head injury. However, we have found that most
psychosensory functions, higher-order exec- basic motor and sensory tests are less helpful
utive functions (judgment, decision making, in this regard—and far less economical—than
planning), and attention and orientation, as tests aimed at higher-order cognitive capacities.
well as screening of mood and affect. Strategic Moreover, the validity of many basic psycho-
sampling of these functions usually suffices motor and psychosensory tests vis-à-vis brain–
to reveal patterns of performance that can be behavior relationships has remained elusive.
related to particular diagnoses and etiologies,
and to provide key information for formulation The Importance of Neuroanatomy. As indi-
of a rehabilitation program. Whenever neces- cated in the Historical Introduction, the I-B
sary, and depending on a multitude of factors, school derives from a strong tradition of neu-
including the referral question, the patient’s ropsychology practiced in a medical setting.
stamina, time considerations, and, in particular, Benton practiced within the medical com-
the evolving performance profile of the patient, plex, working closely with Sahs, Van Allen, the
the Core Battery is supplemented with vari- Damasios, and other physicians. Tranel has con-
ous follow-up procedures. The most frequent tinued that tradition. Influenced by this asso-
procedures utilized for in-depth follow-up are ciation and by the long-standing connection to
presented in Table 4–2, grouped according to the cognitive neuroscience research program
domain of function. The tests are drawn from of the Damasios, the Benton Neuropsychology
various sources throughout the field of neuro- Laboratory has remained decidedly committed
psychology, and many come from test batteries to a close connection between neuropsychologi-
used in other major neuropsychological assess- cal assessment and sophisticated neuroanatom-
ment approaches. ical analysis.
One area in which the I-B method has tra- In the I-B school, the interpretation of neu-
ditionally placed less emphasis is the assess- ropsychological data is informed, to as large an
ment of basic motor and sensory functions. extent as possible, by knowledge of the neuroan-
This may be curious, since such testing has atomical findings in a patient. Neuroanatomical
received significant emphasis in some neuro- information on patients is often available in our
psychological assessment philosophies, espe- facility. Many patients come to the clinic with
cially fi xed-battery methods. One of the main a neuroimaging study (CT, MR), and we usu-
reasons that the I-B method has de-emphasized ally have access to both the interpretation of the
this domain is that such testing provides rela- study (as provided by the radiologist) and the
tively little information for the time investment. “raw data” (the CT or MR study). Our neurop-
This is especially true in the age of modern sychologists frequently can avail themselves of
neuroimaging, which has reduced substantially first-hand readings of neuroimaging data, and
the extent to which neuropsychology is needed this information is incorporated directly into
for “lesion localization.” For example, there is the neuropsychological examination, both as a
little point to spending an hour determining means of guiding test selection and as informa-
that the patient suffers “left hemisphere dys- tion to be factored into the impressions, conclu-
function” based on deficient motor and sensory sions, and recommendations. In keeping with
performances with the right hand, when a neu- this tradition, an overarching principle in the I-B
roimaging study (e.g., brain MRI) has revealed method is to generate information that will elu-
clearly that the patient has a left frontal tumor. cidate and spur hypotheses regarding the status
Of course, neuropsychological testing may of neural systems in a particular patient’s brain.
reveal signs of hemispheric dysfunction in cases That is, our endeavor is not simply to answer the
in which neuroimaging is negative, an outcome question, “What is wrong with the patient?” It
Table 4–2. Follow-Up Neuropsychological Tests

1. Intellectual abilities
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV (Wechsler, 2008)
2. Memory
a. Wechsler Memory Scale-III (Wechsler, 1997)
b. Recognition Memory Test (Warrington, 1984)
c. Iowa Autobiographical Memory Questionnaire (Tranel & Jones, 2006)
d. Iowa Famous Faces Test (Tranel, 2006)
e. Brief Visuospatial Memory Test—Revised (BVMT-R; Benedict, 1997)
3. Language
a. Multilingual Aphasia Examination (Benton & Hamsher, 1978)
b. Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination (Goodglass & Kaplan, 1983)
c. Boston Naming Test (Kaplan et al., 1983)
d. Benton Laboratory Assessment of Writing (Benton Laboratory)
e. Iowa-Chapman Reading Test (Manzel & Tranel, 1999)
f. Category Fluency Test (Benton Laboratory)
4. Academic achievement skills
Wide Range Achievement Test-4: Reading, Spelling, Arithmetic, and Sentence Comprehension
subtests (Wilkinson & Robertson, 2006)
5. Perception and attention
a. Judgment of Line Orientation (Benton et al., 1983)
b. Hooper Visual Organization Test (Hooper, 1983)
c. Agnosia Screening Evaluation (Benton Laboratory)
d. Screening evaluation for visual, auditory, and tactile neglect (Benton Laboratory)
e. Rosenbaum Visual Acuity Screen
f. Pelli-Robson Contrast Sensitivity Chart (Pelli et al., 1988)
6. Visuoconstruction
a. Drawing of a house, bicycle, flower (Lezak et al., 2004, pp. 550-556)
b. Three-Dimensional Block Construction (Benton et al., 1983)
7. Psychomotor and psychosensory functions
a. Grooved Pegboard Test (Heaton et al., 1991)
b. Right–Left Discrimination (Benton et al., 1983)
c. Finger Localization/Recognition (Benton et al., 1983)
d. Dichotic Listening (adapted from Kimura, 1967; Damasio & Damasio, 1979)
e. Line Cancellation Test (Benton et al., 1983)
8. Executive functions
a. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (Heaton et al., 1993)
b. Stroop Color and Word Test (Golden, 1978)
c. Visual Image (Nonverbal) Fluency (Benton Laboratory)
d. Category Test (DeFilippis et al., 1979; Halstead, 1947)
e. Tower of London Test (Shallice, 1982) or Tower of Hanoi Test (Glosser & Goodglass, 1990)
f. Iowa Gambling Task (Bechara et al., 1994)
g. Delis–Kaplan Executive Function System (Delis et al., 2001)
9. Personality and affect
a. Beck Anxiety Inventory (Beck, 1993)
b. Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2 (MMPI-2) (Hathaway & McKinley, 1989)
c. Iowa Scales of Personality Change (Barrash et al., 2000)
d. Geriatric Depression Scale (Yesavage et al., 1983)
10. Symptom Validity Testing
a. Test of Memory Malingering (Tombaugh, 1996)
b. Forced Choice Memory Assessment (Binder, 1993)
c. Rey 15-Item Test (Rey, 1964)
d. Structured Interview of Malingered Symptomatology (Smith & Burger, 1997)
11. Miscellaneous Instruments
a. Dementia Rating Scale (Mattis, 1988)
b. Smell Identification Test (Doty et al., 1984)
c. Useful Field of View Test (Ball & Roenker, 1998)
d. Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS, Randolph, 1998)

72
The Iowa-Benton School of Neuropsychological Assessment 73

goes on to address the more specific question, sixth day after lesion onset confirms this precise
“What is wrong with the patient’s brain?” pattern of lesion locus.
Another factor that has been important in Another fairly common scenario is head
influencing the strong anatomical tradition of trauma, or even situations in which brain
the I-B school has been the location of the neu- injury is caused by severe acceleration/deceler-
ropsychological operation in the Department ation forces in the absence of head trauma per
of Neurology, within the University of Iowa se. Acute structural neuroimaging studies (CT
Hospitals and Clinics. The Neuropsychology or MR), and even neuroimaging procedures
Clinic is situated within the outpatient and obtained in the chronic epoch, sometimes fail
inpatient units of Neurology, and the neurop- to reveal evidence of structural abnormalities.
sychologists and technicians have direct access Detailed neuropsychological examination, how-
to neurological patients. The neuropsycholo- ever, may furnish clear evidence of significant
gists have the opportunity to be involved in the impairments; for instance, it is not uncommon
acute management of neurological patients, to see indications of “executive dysfunction,”
and are frequently requested to perform with deficits on tests such as verbal associative
examinations at bedside in patients who are fluency, the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, and
only a few hours or days out of a cerebrovascu- the Trail-Making Test. The patient may have
lar accident, anoxic/ischemic event, traumatic a striking postmorbid change in personality.
brain injury, or other brain injury (e.g., Tranel, Such data are strongly indicative of dysfunction
1992a). in the ventromedial prefrontal region, includ-
There are a couple of relatively common sce- ing orbital and lower mesial prefrontal cortices,
narios in which neuropsychological data are caused by the shearing and tearing of brain tis-
especially helpful in detecting focal brain dys- sues in this region produced by movement of
function, and where neuroimaging data may the ventral part of the frontal lobes across bony
not be immediately contributory. One instance protrusions from the inferior surface of the cra-
is where neuropsychological findings indi- nium (Barrash et al., 2000; Tranel et al., 1994).
cate the presence of focal brain dysfunction, Examples such as these indicate that neuro-
even though early neuroimaging studies have psychological data can furnish important infor-
been negative (in a significant number of cases, mation regarding dysfunction of particular
CT—and even MR—scans conducted within 24 neural systems, even in cases in which neuroim-
hours of lesion onset are negative). For exam- aging studies are negative. However, it should be
ple, a patient presents with a severe aphasia, noted that the use of neuropsychological proce-
with marked defects in both comprehension dures to localize lesions has declined sharply
and speech production; however, the patient following the advent of modern neuroimaging
has no motor or sensory defect. No lesion is evi- techniques, particularly CT in the mid-1970s
dent on an acute CT conducted the day of onset. and MR in the mid-1980s (e.g., Benton, 1989;
Neuropsychological examination the following Boller et al., 1991; Tranel, 1992b). These meth-
day indicates that the patient has a severe global ods have tremendous power to detect even min-
aphasia, with marked defects in all aspects of imal structural abnormalities, and the “find the
speech and language. The pattern indicates pro- lesion” aspect of neuropsychological assessment
nounced dysfunction of the perisylvian region, is no longer much of a mandate.
including both Broca’s area and Wernicke’s area; Empirical tests of the “localizing value”
however, the absence of a right-sided motor of some of Benton’s tests have recently been
defect is unusual and suggests that the lesion undertaken, and results from these studies
is not of the typical middle cerebral artery pat- are included in several articles published in
tern. The neuropsychologist concludes that the a special issue of the Journal of Clinical and
findings suggest the condition of global aphasia Experimental Neuropsychology devoted to the
without hemiparesis, which typically involves legacy of Arthur Benton (Tranel & Levin, in
two separate, noncontiguous lesions affecting press). In one of the studies, we investigated
Broca’s and Wernicke’s areas, but sparing pri- two of the most successful and widely used
mary motor cortices. An MR conducted on the Benton tests, the Facial Recognition Test (FRT)
74 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

and Judgment of Line Orientation (JLO) test, effects of therapy, and making long-range deci-
which measure visuoperceptual and visuospa- sions regarding educational and vocational
tial functions typically associated with right rehabilitation.
hemisphere structures (Tranel, Vianna, et al., 2. Monitoring the neuropsychological sta-
in press). A new lesion-deficit mapping tech- tus of patients who have undergone medical or
nique was used to investigate the neuroanatom- surgical intervention. Serial neuropsychologi-
ical correlates of FRT and JLO performance. cal assessments are used to track the course of
The results showed that failure on the FRT was patients who are undergoing medical or surgi-
most strongly associated with lesions in the cal treatment for neurological disease. Typical
right posterior-inferior parietal and right ven- examples include drug therapy for patients
tral occipitotemporal (fusiform gyrus) areas, with Parkinson’s disease or seizure disorders,
and failure on the JLO test was most strongly and surgical intervention in patients with nor-
associated with lesions in the right posterior mal pressure hydrocephalus, brain tumors, or
parietal region. These findings extend and pharmacoresistant seizure disorders (typically
sharpen previous work, especially early work temporal lobectomies). Neuropsychological
by Benton and his students, which had pointed assessment provides a baseline profile of cog-
to right posterior structures as being impor- nitive and behavioral functioning, to which
tant for FRT and JLO performance (see Benton changes can be compared, and it provides a sen-
et al., 1994). sitive means of monitoring changes that occur
in relation to particular treatment regimens.
Indications for Neuropsychological Assess- 3. Distinguishing organic from psychiatric dis-
ment. Some of the common applications of ease. Neuropsychological assessment provides
neuropsychological assessment in the Benton crucial evidence to distinguish conditions that
Neuropsychology Laboratory are enumerated are primarily or exclusively “organic” from those
below. that are primarily or exclusively “psychiatric.”
For example, a common diagnostic dilemma
1. General appraisal of cognitive and behav- faced by neurologists, psychiatrists, and general
ioral functioning. As alluded to earlier, many of practitioners is distinguishing between “true
our assessments are aimed at characterizing the dementia” (e.g., cognitive impairment caused
cognitive capacities of brain-injured patients so by Alzheimer’s disease) and “pseudodemen-
as to determine rehabilitation needs, placement, tia” (e.g., cognitive impairment associated with
return to work, and recommendations for inde- depression). Neuropsychological assessment
pendent living. For example, in patients who frequently yields definitive evidence to make
have suffered brain injury due to stroke, head this distinction. Another common referral is
trauma, infection, or anoxia/ischemia, neuro- for the assessment of patients with so-called
psychological assessment can provide detailed nonepileptic seizure disorders, or what is often
information regarding the cognitive and behav- referred to as “pseudoseizures,” where psycho-
ioral strengths and weaknesses of the patients. logical factors are typically thought to play a
In most instances, the initial assessment is per- role in the patient’s condition. Documenting
formed as early in the recovery epoch as pos- and clarifying the psychological status of such
sible, provided the patient is awake and alert patients can be very helpful to the neurolo-
enough to cooperate with the procedures. This gist, and can factor prominently into treatment
evaluation, termed the “acute epoch” assess- decisions.
ment, provides a baseline to which further 4. Medicolegal situations. Neuropsychological
recovery can be compared, and it initiates con- assessment can be very helpful to resolve claims
tact with the neuropsychologist and related of “brain injury” that are frequent in plaintiffs
professionals who will figure prominently who sustained head trauma in motor vehicle,
in the long-term management of the patient. slip-and-fall, and other accidents, were exposed
“Chronic epoch” assessments, conducted three to toxic chemicals, suffered carbon monoxide
or more months following onset of brain injury, poisoning, or sustained an electrical injury or
assist in monitoring recovery, determining the any other of a variety of “compensable” alleged
The Iowa-Benton School of Neuropsychological Assessment 75

insults. In many of these cases, “objective” signs neuropsychological data will be helpful in
of brain dysfunction (e.g., weakness, sensory tracking the trajectory of decline, informing
loss, impaired balance) are absent, structural family members about placement and caregiv-
neuroimaging and EEG are normal, and the ing issues, deciding on the need for supervision,
entire case for “brain damage” rests on claims of and so on.
cognitive deficiencies. Here, neuropsychological 8. Wada testing. The intracarotid amobarbi-
data are especially informative to adjudicate the tal procedure (Wada & Rasmussen, 1960) is rou-
question of whether there is brain dysfunction; tinely performed in patients prior to surgery for
moreover, the neuropsychologist is often in the pharmacoresistant epilepsy to establish hemi-
best position to factor the influence of affective spheric dominance for language and other ver-
variables, the possibility of malingering, and the bally mediated functions. Neuropsychological
patient’s premorbid status into the diagnostic assessment during the Wada procedure is used
picture. Teasing out the contributions of phys- to measure cognitive functioning in each hemi-
iopathology and psychopathology can be a very sphere. One hemisphere is anesthetized, and the
challenging endeavor, and neuropsychological functions of the other “awake” hemisphere are
data provide one of the best solutions in these tested. The procedure allows a determination of
situations (Alexander, 1995; Hom, 2003). whether verbal processing—especially language
5. Developmental disorders. Neuropsycholo- and to a lesser extent verbal memory—is “local-
gical assessment can assist in identifying devel- ized” to one hemisphere of the brain.
opmental learning disorders that may influence 9. Carbon monoxide poisoning. We have
the cognitive and behavioral presentation of a established a 24-hour, on-call neuropsychology
patient, such as dyslexia, attention-deficit disor- service to respond to emergency situations in
der, and nonverbal learning disability. which patients have suffered carbon monoxide
6. Conditions in which known or suspected poisoning and may be in need of treatment in
neurological disease is not detected by standard a hyperbaric oxygen chamber. A set of neuro-
neurodiagnostic procedures. As noted earlier, psychological tests is given to the patients to
there are some situations in which findings from determine whether there is evidence of cogni-
standard neurodiagnostic procedures, including tive dysfunction that might warrant hyperbaric
neurological examination, structural neuroim- treatment. If the patient is normal on neuro-
aging, and EEG, are negative, even though the psychological testing, such treatment may be
history indicates that brain injury or brain dis- deferred; if the patient is impaired, the treat-
ease is likely. Mild closed head trauma, the early ment may be implemented. Repeated testing is
stages of degenerative dementia syndromes conducted to monitor recovery and determine
(e.g., Alzheimer’s disease, frontal-temporal the need for further hyperbaric treatments.
dementia), and early HIV-related dementia are 10. Driving privileges. We are called upon
examples. Neuropsychological assessment in frequently to answer the basic question of
such cases frequently provides the most sen- whether a patient is competent and safe to oper-
sitive means of evaluating the patient’s brain ate a motor vehicle.
functioning. 11. Competence. The question frequently
7. Monitoring changes in cognitive function- arises as to whether a patient is mentally com-
ing across time. A situation that warrants special petent to execute legal decisions, draw up a will,
mention is the evolution of cognitive and behav- make decisions about their medical care, and
ioral changes across time. In the degenerative the like. Neuropsychological testing often pro-
dementias in particular, it is not uncommon to vides the best means of addressing these issues.
have equivocal findings in the initial diagnostic 12. The influence of pain on cognitive func-
workup, or the patient may meet only the cri- tioning. Many neurological patients suffer debi-
teria for so-called mild cognitive impairment. litating pain syndromes, such as intractable
In such cases, follow-up neuropsychological headaches, neck and back pain, or other somatic
evaluations can provide important confirm- pain. Neuropsychological testing in such pati-
ing or disconfirming evidence regarding the ents can be very helpful in determining the
patient’s status. As the disease progresses, the extent to which pain problems are interfering
76 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

with cognitive functioning, and in establish- primary sources of such information are the
ing the patient’s psychological resources for neurological examination, neuroimaging proce-
coping with chronic pain, how the patient will dures, EEG studies, and the history. In keeping
respond to potentially addictive medications, with the basic philosophy of the I-B school, the
and whether the patient might be capable of interpretation strategy is flexible and hypoth-
developing nonmedical methods of coping esis focused, and varied degrees of efforts are
with pain. expended to obtain and factor in other infor-
13. Obstructive sleep apnea. Obstructive mation, depending on the particulars of the
sleep apnea is a common malady that can have case. For example, if the premorbid caliber of
adverse effects on cognition and affective status, the patient is equivocal or difficult to estimate,
and neuropsychological testing in such patients we typically request the patient’s academic
is helpful in sorting out whether there is in fact transcripts from high school and college (if rel-
evidence for genuine cognitive impairment evant). Other helpful sources of information are
suggestive of brain dysfunction. And of even patient “collaterals”—that is, spouses, children,
greater practical importance is the fact that and other individuals who know the patient well
our testing is not uncommonly the juncture at and who can provide details about the patient’s
which the basic problem of sleep apnea is iden- typical behavior in the day-to-day environment
tified as a likely etiologic factor in what other- (in fact, there are cases in which we devote more
wise appear to be vague, nonspecific complaints time interviewing collateral sources than we do
of fatigue, poor concentration, lowered energy, testing the patient). It is also important to men-
and the like. tion that interpretation of neuropsychological
14. Information and teaching. A common data takes into account the nature and pattern
and very important function carried out by our of the patient’s performances, in addition to the
neuropsychologists is to provide information to actual scores and outcomes.
patients and families about neurological, psy-
chological, and neuropsychological conditions.
Training Model
As noted earlier, we spend a significant portion
of the overall examination time in feedback, The training required for neuropsychologists
and we arrange the examination schedules so who wish to practice according to the I-B school
that feedback sessions can run as long as neces- follows, in general, the outline provided by INS/
sary for patients and families to leave with a rea- Division 40 task force guidelines, and specifi-
sonably complete understanding of the patient’s cally, the so-called “Houston Model” guidelines
condition—what is wrong, what caused the (Hannay et al., 1998; see also the American
problem, what can be done about it, what to Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology, 2007).
expect down the road, and so on. Historically, We emphasize, in particular, the generic psy-
this function too often received short shrift in chology core, the generic clinical core, and
the context of neuropsychological assessment. training in basic neurosciences (Table 4–3). Our
And nowadays, when the demands of modern philosophy is that solid graduate student train-
health care have wrung physicians until there is ing in these areas is a necessary foundation for
literally not a second to waste on anything that is postgraduate specialization in neuropsychol-
not directly germane to diagnosis and treatment ogy. A second important feature is an empha-
as dictated by health-care managers whose sole sis on basic research training. The I-B method
mission seems to be to bolster the profit margin, rests squarely on the scientific principles of
the role of the neuropsychologist in providing hypothesis testing, probabilistic reasoning, and
information and in teaching patients and fam- inferential conclusion drawing, and practitio-
ilies about neurological diseases has taken on ners must be solidly trained in basic research
more importance than ever. methodology.
All five of the current staff neuropsychologists
Interpretation. The results of the neuropsy- in the Benton Neuropsychology Laboratory were
chological examination are interpreted in the trained in clinical psychology. For several rea-
context of other pertinent information. The sons, this training background is considered in
The Iowa-Benton School of Neuropsychological Assessment 77

Table 4–3. Core Graduate Training in Preparation the reason for referral. The neuropsychologist
for Clinical Neuropsychologya is frequently called upon to document such
presence, to determine its relationship to other
A. Generic Psychology Core aspects of the patient’s cognition and behavior,
1. Statistics and Methodology
2. Learning, Cognition, and Perception
and finally, to offer impressions about its cause.
3. Social Psychology (3) Typically, referrals to neuropsychologists
4. Personality Theory are from neurologists and psychiatrists. There
5. Physiological Psychology is considerable overlap in the populations of
6. Developmental Psychology patients that originate in these two specialty
7. History
B. Generic Clinical Core
areas, and in the types of signs and symptoms
1. Psychopathology that such patients present. Having the capabil-
2. Psychometric Theory ity of factoring in accurately the contributions
3. Interview and Assessment Techniques of psychopathology to the presentations of such
i. Interviewing patients is extremely important.
ii. Intelligence Assessment
iii. Personality Assessment
A final rationale for coming to neuropsychol-
4. Intervention Techniques ogy from a clinical psychology background is
i. Counseling and Psychotherapy that such a background furnishes basic train-
ii. Behavior Therapy ing in psychological appraisal, which underlies
5. Professional Ethics the standardized nature of clinical neuropsy-
C. Neurosciences Core
1. Basic Neurosciences
chological assessment. Knowledge about test
2. Advanced Physiological Psychology and development and construction, reliability, and
Pharmacology validity is crucial when employing standard-
3. Neuropsychology of Perceptual, Cognitive, ized psychological tests to measure cognition
and Executive Processes and behavior.
4. Neuroanatomy
5. Neuroimaging Techniques
a
Reprinted with permission from the Report of the INS- Other Considerations
Division 40 Task Force on Education, Accreditation, and
Credentialing, The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 1987.
Personnel. The Benton Neuropsychology
Laboratory is staffed currently by five clinical
neuropsychologists and two postdoctoral fel-
lows. There are several neuropsychology tech-
our view to be essential for the practice of clini- nicians and support staff. Practicum training
cal neuropsychology. First, basic graduate train- is provided, and at any given time some three
ing in psychopathology incorporating issues of to five practicum students (mainly from the
theory, assessment, and treatment is of indis- clinical psychology or counseling psychology
pensable value. Knowledge about psychopathol- graduate programs) are training in the Benton
ogy becomes essential in neuropsychological Laboratory.
practice on several accounts: (1) Many patients
bring to the neuropsychological examination Use of Neuropsychology Technicians. The
some degree of psychopathology, whether that I-B method has utilized neuropsychology tech-
be of an incapacitating degree or simply the dis- nicians since its inception. Technicians are
tress that often characterizes persons who have charged with the responsibility for most of the
sustained cerebral insult. This has an inevitable hands-on test administration. They collect per-
influence on the manner in which the patient tinent background information, record present-
approaches and deals with the neuropsycho- ing complaints, and administer the series of tests
logical assessment situation. Understanding prescribed by the supervising neuropsycholo-
this influence is critical for accurate interpre- gist. The technicians are specifically trained in
tation of the performances of patients on tests, the I-B method, and are encouraged to facilitate
not to mention the collection of reliable and the flexible, hypothesis-driven approach. This
valid data. (2) The presence of significant psy- requires that they maintain an awareness of the
chopathology is often a salient component of patient’s ongoing performance profi le, so that
78 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

on-line adaptations in the testing procedure a variety of assessment procedures. We also


can be made. The technicians do not adminis- encourage “testing the limits,” that is, finding
ter a rigid set of tests to a patient if it is obvious out whether a patient can perform a particular
that the data being collected are of questionable task under conditions that are less demanding
reliability or validity or, equally important, if than those called for by the formal test protocol.
the data appear to be uninformative vis-à-vis This process often furnishes information about
the referral question. Final decisions regarding patient capabilities that is not reflected in test
changes in testing (e.g., pursuing a particular scores per se but that has important value for
hypothesis and dropping others) rest with the diagnostic purposes and, especially, for design-
supervising neuropsychologist; nevertheless, ing rehabilitation programs (cf. Kaplan, 1985;
the technicians have the prerogative to raise Milberg et al., this volume).
questions about alternative hypotheses during The use of technicians offers a distinct advan-
the course of test administration. tage in permitting “blind” hypothesis testing in
The I-B method places significant demands on the neuropsychological assessment procedure.
the neuropsychology technicians. The method The technician has relatively little at stake in the
requires a certain degree of decision making, outcome of the procedure; by contrast, the neu-
creativity, and vigilance on the part of the tech- ropsychologist may have any number of pre-
nicians; by contrast, administering a prescribed vailing presuppositions about how the patient
set of tests in adherence to a fi xed-battery phi- might perform, due to forensic issues, research
losophy is simpler and more straightforward. considerations, and so on. Hence, the techni-
Accordingly, training technicians in the I-B cian is probably in a better position than the
school probably requires a greater initial invest- neuropsychologist to collect and record objec-
ment; however, we have found that once techni- tive findings and to avoid problems related to
cians are comfortable with our basic philosophy “experimenter bias.”
and the various procedures, they tend to engage We find that it is cost and time efficient to
the assessment process at a deeper level than utilize technicians. The standard I-B procedure
might be the case when their charge is simply to can be performed on two and sometimes even
collect a specified set of test data. three patients a day by a seasoned technician.
Technicians are recalibrated annually.
Neuropsychologists observe the technicians, Report Writing. Reports from our Neuro-
with attention to the manner in which the psychology Clinic comprise three main sec-
technicians conform to specified instructions tions: (1) identification and background, (2) data
for administration of the tests. Departures from reporting, and (3) impressions and recommen-
standard procedure are discussed and rectified dations. The scope of the report varies according
as appropriate. This may involve a reminder to to the nature of the situation, but most consulta-
the technician about the standard method of tion reports generated for referrals from within
test administration. At times, however, a tech- the University of Iowa Hospitals and Clinics are
nician may have developed a procedure that relatively brief, on the order of 250–500 words.
proves to be superior to the standard method, In more complicated cases, our reports are typ-
and this may ultimately be incorporated as a ically somewhat longer, in the range of 500–750
revision in the basic method. Making a perma- words. We typically produce longer reports for
nent revision is a sensitive issue, as one must outside referral agencies, for example, when
be extremely careful not to create a situation in more detailed questions regarding long-term
which the normative information for the test management of the patient are raised, or in
becomes uninformative or misleading because forensic referrals. Such reports may extend up
of a different method of administration. We to 1000 or more words.
are, however, quite open to the possibility of We have a policy of not including scores and
revising our procedures, and over the years the other “raw” data in our reports. The primary
insightful observations of highly experienced reason for this is the potential misuse of such
technicians have proven invaluable in making information by persons not trained in neuropsy-
decisions about improving the effectiveness of chology or psychological appraisal. Psychological
The Iowa-Benton School of Neuropsychological Assessment 79

data, particularly IQ scores, memory quotients, more than a decade. This service provides on-
and scores reported as “percentiles,” are quite site rehabilitation programs for brain-injured
vulnerable to misinterpretation, because non- patients. The approach to cognitive rehabilita-
experts frequently fail to appreciate the impor- tion follows the same philosophical approach
tance of factors such as the estimated premorbid as the I-B assessment method. Interventions are
level of functioning of the patient, the quality of tailored to individual patients to meet circum-
normative information, and the type of popula- scribed goals in a time-efficient manner. Specific
tion on which the test was standardized. Since interventions are selected from an inventory
reports are usually placed in hospital charts or of empirically supported procedures, with the
other sources that can be accessed fairly easily selection process guided by the findings of
by nonexperts, it seems prudent to omit raw the neuropsychological evaluation and by the
data from such reports. Rather than scores, complaints and goals expressed by the patient
we emphasize the reporting of interpretations and family. Emphasis is placed on education of
of patient performances—that is, whether the the patient and family regarding the patient’s
patient performed normally or abnormally, the neuropsychological condition and on com-
degree of abnormality, and so forth. pensatory strategies designed to minimize the
Two additional comments are in order here. consequences of the acquired cognitive impair-
First, with regard to IQ scores, the reporting of ments. Interventions range from a single session
such scores is not only unwise for the reasons or once-a-year consultation (e.g., for the family
mentioned above, but the use of such scores per of a patient with Alzheimer’s disease) to daily
se in neuropsychological assessment has been sessions over several weeks (e.g., for a patient
seriously questioned (Lezak, 1988). (We might with attention and language impairments from
also note here that the classic “Verbal IQ” and a left hemisphere stroke). Among the services
“Performance IQ” indices have been dropped provided are training in the use of compensa-
in the most recent version of the WAIS, the tory devices (e.g., memory books) and strategies
WAIS-IV.) Second, the Ethical Principles of (e.g., use of American Sign Language by severely
the American Psychological Association have aphasic patients), computer-assisted attentional
outlined a clear position against the release of training, psychotherapeutic interventions for
“raw” data to nonexperts (see Tranel, 1994). depression, anxiety, and behavioral control, and
Hence, inclusion of data (numbers, scores) in awareness training for anosognosia.
reports that are likely to end up in the hands of Typically, patients are referred to the
nonexperts is hazardous from an ethical point Cognitive Rehabilitation Laboratory after an
of view as well. There are different views on this initial examination in the Neuropsychology
issue (e.g., see Freides, 1993), but we have found Clinic, when the conclusion has been reached
that refraining from using scores in reports in that the patient needs and would probably bene-
no way diminishes the quality or usefulness of fit from cognitive rehabilitation. Thereafter, the
the reports and, in many cases, actually encour- Neuropsychology Clinic provides periodic reex-
ages the authors (neuropsychologists) to pro- aminations, to track the course of the patient’s
vide a higher level of interpretation of the data, recovery.
which ought to be an objective of a good report.
Overall, our approach is generally in line with
Summary
recent recommendations set forth by the gov-
erning bodies of clinical neuropsychology The I-B school of neuropsychological assessment
(Attix et al., 2007). has been developed as a flexible, hypothesis-
driven approach to standardized measurement
of higher brain functions in patients with
Cognitive Rehabilitation Laboratory
known or suspected brain disease. The method
A Cognitive Rehabilitation Laboratory, devel- is focused on the objective of obtaining quanti-
oped and directed by neuropsychologist Steven tative measurements of key domains of cogni-
Anderson (Anderson, 1996), has been part of tion and behavior, in a time-efficient manner,
the Benton Neuropsychology Laboratory for in sufficient breadth and depth that the referral
80 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

question can be answered. Starting from a Core American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology.
Battery, the selection of tests is guided by a mul- (2007). American Academy of Clinical
titude of factors, including the nature of the Neuropsychology (AACN) practice guidelines for
patient’s complaint, the questions raised by the neuropsychological assessment and consultation.
The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 21, 209–231.
referring agent, the impressions gained from the
Anderson, S. W. (1996). Cognitive rehabilitation
initial interview, and above all, by the diagnos-
in closed head injury. In M. Rizzo and D. Tranel
tic possibilities raised by the patient’s perfor- (Eds.), Head injury and postconcussive syndrome
mances during the course of the examination. (pp. 457–468). New York: Churchill Livingstone.
A close link to neuroanatomy is maintained, Anderson, S. W. (2002). Visuoperceptual impair-
and the neuropsychological data both inform ments. In P. J. Eslinger (Ed.), Neuropsychological
and are informed by neuroanatomical findings. interventions (pp. 163–181). New York: The
Whenever possible, the neuropsychological Guilford Press.
findings are used to infer the integrity, or lack Attix, D. K., Donders, J., Johnseon-Greene, D., Grote,
of it, of various neural systems in the patient’s C. L., Harris, J. G., & Bauer, R. M. (2007). Disclosure
brain. Interpretation of neuropsychological of neuropsychological test data: official position
of Division 40 (Clinical Neuropsychology) of the
data is conducted in the context of pertinent
American Psychological Association, Associa-
historical and diagnostic information regarding
tion of Postdoctoral Programs in Clinical Neuro-
the patient. Patients and families are provided psychology, and American Academy of Clinical
immediate feedback about the results of the Neuropsychology. The Clinical Neuropsychologist,
evaluation, in a detailed feedback session that 21, 232–238.
is incorporated into the end phase of the neu- Ball, K. K., & Roenker, D. L. (1998). UFOV: Useful
ropsychological evaluation. Recommendations Field of View. San Antonio, TX: Psychological
for further diagnostic procedures, treatment, Corporation, Harcourt Brace.
and follow-up neuropsychological assessment Barrash, J., Damasio, H., Adolphs, R., &Tranel, D.
are provided during feedback. (2000). The neuroanatomical correlates of route
learning impairment. Neuropsychologia, 38, 820–836.
Barrash, J., Tranel, D., & Anderson, S. W. (2000).
Acknowledgment Acquired personality disturbances associated
with bilateral damage to the ventromedial pre-
Arthur Benton created the neuropsychologi- frontal region. Developmental Neuropsychology,
cal assessment approach that has evolved into 18, 355–381.
the I-B School, and his genius and hard work Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., &
are why the approach caught on and became Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future
a cornerstone in the field of clinical neuropsy- consequences following damage to human pre-
chology. I had the great good fortune of having frontal cortex. Cognition, 50, 7–15.
Dr. Benton as one of my teachers early in my Bechtoldt, H. P., Benton, A. L., & Fogel, M. L. (1962).
career. Dr. Benton passed away on December 27, An application of factor analysis in neuropsychol-
2006, after living, working, and teaching for ogy. Psychological Record, 12, 147–156.
Beck, A. T. (1993). Beck anxiety inventory. San
the better part of a century. His contributions to
Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
our field are prodigious, and his influences live
Benedict, R. H. B. (1997). Brief Visuospatial Memory
on in the clinic at Iowa that bears his name and Test—Revised. Odessa, FL: Professional manual,
in his many students who trained there. Psychological Assessment Resources Inc.
Benton, A. L. (1955a). Development of fi nger
localization capacity in school children. Child
References
Development, 26, 225–230.
Alexander, M. P. (1995). Mild traumatic brain injury: Benton, A. L. (1955b). Right–left discrimination and
Pathophysiology, natural history, and clinical finger localization in defective children. Archives
management. Neurology, 45, 1253–1260. of Neurology and Psychiatry, 74, 383–389.
American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology. Benton, A. L. (1956). Jacques Loeb and the method
(2001). Policy statement on the presence of third of double stimulation. Journal of the History of
party observers in neuropsychological assess- Medicine and Allied Sciences, 11, 47–53.
ments. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Benton, A. L. (1975). Neuropsychological assess-
Neuropsychology, 15, 433–439. ment. In D. B. Tower (Ed.), The nervous system,
The Iowa-Benton School of Neuropsychological Assessment 81

Vol. 2. The clinical neurosciences (pp. 67–74). New Blackburn, H. L., & Benton, A. L. (1955). Simple and
York: Raven Press. choice reaction time in cerebral disease. Confinia
Benton, A. L. (1985). Some problems associated Neurologica, 15, 327–338.
with neuropsychological assessment. Bulletin of Boller, F., Swihart, A. A., Forbes, M. M., & Denes, G.
Clinical Neurosciences, 50, 11–15. (1991). Neuropsychology in its daily practice: Past
Benton, A. L. (1989). Neuropsychology: Past, pre- and present. In F. Boller and J. Grafman (Eds.),
sent and future. In F. Boller & J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology (vol. 5; pp. 379–388).
Handbook of neuropsychology (vol. 1; pp. 1–27). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Amsterdam: Elsevier. Costa, L., & Spreen, O. (1985). Studies in neuropsychol-
Benton, A. L. (1991). Basic approaches to neuro- ogy: Selected papers of Arthur Benton. New York:
psychological assessment. In S. R. Steinhauer, Oxford University Press.
J. H. Gruzelier, & J. Zubin (Eds.), Handbook of Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (1979). “Paradoxic”
schizophrenia (vol. 5; pp.505–523). Amsterdam: ear extinction in dichotic listening: Possible ana-
Elsevier. tomic significance. Neurology, 29, 644–653.
Benton, A. L. (1992). Gerstmann’s syndrome. DeFilippis, N. A., McCampbell, E., & Rogers, P.
Archives of Neurology, 49, 445–447. (1979). Development of a booklet form of The
Benton, A. L., & Abramson, L. S. (1952). Gerstmann Category Test: Normative and validity data.
symptoms following electroshock therapy. Archives Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 1, 339–342.
of Neurology and Psychiatry, 68, 248–257. Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, J. H. (2001).
Benton, A. L., & Cohen, B. D. (1955). Right–left dis- Delis–Kaplan Executive Function System: Technical
crimination and finger localization in normal and manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological
brain-injured subjects. Proceedings of the Iowa Corporation.
Academy of Science, 62, 447–451. DeLuca, J. W., & Putman, S. H. (1993). The pro-
Benton, A. L., & Fogel, M. L. (1962). Th ree- fessional/technician model in clinical neuro-
dimensional constructional praxis: a clinical test. psychology: Deployment characteristics and
Archives of Neurology, 7, 347–354. practice issues. Professional Psychology: Research
Benton, A. L., & Hamsher, K. (1978). Multilingual and Practice, 24, 100–106.
Aphasia Examination. Iowa City: University of Doty, R. L., Shaman, P., Kimmelman, C. P., & Dann,
Iowa (Manual, revised). M. S. (1984). University of Pennsylvania Smell
Benton, A. L., Hamsher, K., Varney, N. R., & Spreen, Identification Test: A rapid quantitative olfac-
O. (1983). Contributions to neuropsychological tory function test for the clinic. Laryngoscope, 94,
assessment. New York: Oxford University Press. 176–178.
Benton, A. L., & Menefee, F. L. (1957). Handedness Fogel, M. L. (1962). The Gerstmann syndrome and
and right–left discrimination. Child Development, the parietal symptom-complex. Psychological
28, 237–242. Record, 12, 85–90.
Benton, A. L., & Meyers, R. (1956). An early descrip- Freides, D. (1993). Proposed standard of professional
tion of the Gerstmann syndrome. Neurology, 6, practice: Neuropsychological reports display all
838–842. quantitative data. The Clinical Neuropsychologist,
Benton, A. L., Sivan, A. B., Hamsher, K. deS., Varney, 7, 234–235.
N. R., & Spreen, O. (1994). Contributions to neuro- Glosser, G., & Goodglass, H. (1990). Disorders in
psychological assessment (2nd edition). New York: executive control functions among aphasic and
Oxford University Press. other brain-damaged subjects. Journal of Clinical
Benton, A. L., & Van Allen, M. W. (1968). Impairment and Experimental Neuropsychology, 12, 485–501.
in facial recognition in patients with unilateral Golden, C. J. (1978). Stroop Color and Word Test. A
cerebral disease. Cortex, 4, 344–358. manual for clinical and experimental use. Chicago,
Benton, A. L., Varney, N. R., & Hamsher, K. (1978). IL: Stoelting Co.
Visuospatial judgment: A clinical test. Archives of Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (1983). The assessment of
Neurology, 35, 364–367. aphasia and related disorders. 2nd ed. Philadelphia,
Binder, L. M. (1993). Assessment of malinger- PA: Lea and Febiger.
ing after mild head trauma with the Portland Halstead, W. C. (1947). Brain and intelligence: A
Digit Recognition Test. Journal of Clinical and quantitative study of the frontal lobes. Chicago:
Experimental Neuropsychology, 15, 170–182. University of Chicago Press.
Blackburn, H. L. (1958). Effects of motivating Hamsher, K. (1985). The Iowa group. International
instructions on reaction time in cerebral disease. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 295–305.
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 56, Hannay, H. J., Bieliauskas, L. A., Crossen, B. A.,
359–366. Hammeke, T. A., Hamsher, K. deS., & Koffler, S. P.
82 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

(1998). Proceedings of the Houston Conference on National Academy of Neuropsychology. (2000a). The
specialty education and training in clinical neuro- use of neuropsychology test technicians in clin-
psychology. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, ical practice: Official statement of the National
13, 157–158. Academy of Neuropsychology. Archives of Clinical
Hathaway, S. R., & McKinley, J. C. (1989). The Neuropsychology, 15, 381–382.
Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory—2. National Academy of Neuropsychology. (2000b).
New York: Psychological Corporation. Presence of third party observers during neuro-
Heaton, R., Grant, I., & Matthews, C. (1991). psychological testing: Official statement of the
Comprehensive norms for an expanded Halstead– National Academy of Neuropsychology. Archives
Reitan neuropsychological battery: Demographic of Clinical Neuropsychology, 15, 379–380.
corrections, research findings, and clinical appli- Pelli, D. G., Robson, J. G., & Wilkins, A. J. (1988). The
cations. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment design of a new letter chart for measuring contrast
Resources. sensitivity. Clinical Vision Sciences, 2, 187–199.
Heaton, R. K., Chelune, G. J., Talley, J. L., Kay, G. Randolph, C. (1998). Repeatable Battery for the
G., & Curtiss, G. (1993). Wisconsin Card Sorting Assessment of Neuropsychological Status Manual.
Test manual: Revised and Expanded. Odessa, FL: San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc. Reports of the INS-Division 40 Task Force on
Heilbrun, A. B., Jr. (1956). Psychological test per- Education, Accreditation, and Credentialing.
formance as a function of lateral localization (1987). The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 1, 29–34.
of cerebral lesion. Journal of Comparative and Rey, A. (1964). L’examen clinique en psychologie.
Physiological Psychology, 49, 10–14. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.
Heilbrun, A. B., Jr. (1959). Lateralization of cerebral Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning.
lesion and performance on spatial–temporal tasks. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of
Archives of Neurology, 1, 282–287. London - Series B: Biological Sciences, 298, 199–209.
Hom, J. (2003). Forensic neuropsychology: Are we Smith, G. P., & Burger, G. K. (1997). Detection of
there yet? Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 18, malingering: Validation of the structured inven-
827–845. tory of malingered symptomatology. Journal of the
Hooper, H. E. (1983). Hooper Visual Organization American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 25,
Test. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological 183–189.
Services. Spreen, O., & Strauss, E. (1998). A compendium of
Joynt, R. J., Benton, A. L., & Fogel, M. L. (1962). neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms
Behavioral and pathological correlates of motor and commentary. New York: Oxford University
impersistence. Neurology, 12, 876–881. Press.
Kaplan, E. (1985). A process approach to neuropsy- Swanson, R. A. (1957). Perception of simultaneous
chological assessment. In T. Boll and B. K. Bryant tactual stimulation in defective and normal chil-
(Eds.), Clinical neuropsychology and brain func- dren. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 61,
tion: Research, measurement, and practice (pp. 743–752.
125–167). Washington: American Psychological Swanson, R. A., & Benton, A. L. (1955). Some aspects
Association. of the genetic development or right–left discrmi-
Kaplan, E. F., Goodglass, H., & Weintraub, S. (1983). nation. Child Development, 26, 123–133.
The Boston naming test, 2nd edn. Philadelphia: Lea Tombaugh, T. (1996). Test of Memory Malingering
& Febiger. (TOMM). New York: MultiHealth Systems.
Kimura, D. (1967). Functional asymmetry of the Tranel, D. (1992a). Neuropsychological assessment.
brain in dichotic listening. Cortex, 3, 163–178. In J. Biller and R. Kathol (Eds.), Psychiatric clin-
Lezak, M. D. (1988). IQ: R.I.P. Journal of Clinical and ics of North America: The interface of psychiatry
Experimental Neuropsychology, 10, 351–361. and neurology (pp. 283–299). Philadelphia: W.B.
Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D., & Loring, D. W. (2004). Saunders.
Neuropsychological assessment, Fourth Edition. Tranel, D. (1992b). The role of neuropsychology in
New York: Oxford University Press. the diagnosis and management of cerebrovascu-
Manzel, K., & Tranel, D. (1999). Development and lar disease. In H. P. Adams (Ed.), Handbook of
standardization of a reading test for brain-dam- cerebrovascular diseases (pp. 613–636). New York:
aged subjects. Developmental Neuropsychology, 15, Marcel Dekker.
407–420. Tranel, D. (1994). The release of psychological data
Mattis, S. (1988). Dementia Rating Scale: Professional to non-experts: Ethical and legal considerations.
Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 25,
Resources. 33–38.
The Iowa-Benton School of Neuropsychological Assessment 83

Tranel, D. (2006). Impaired naming of unique land- clinical observations. Journal of Neurosurgery, 17,
marks is associated with left temporal polar dam- 266–282.
age. Neuropsychology, 20, 1–10. Wahler, H. J. (1956). A comparison of reproduc-
Tranel, D., Anderson, S. W., & Benton, A. L. (1994). tion errors made by brain-damaged and control
Development of the concept of “executive func- patients on a memory-for-designs test. Journal of
tion” and its relationship to the frontal lobes. In Abnormal Psychology, 52, 251–255.
F. Boller & J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of neu- Warrington, E. K. (1984). Recognition memory
ropsychology (vol. 9; pp. 125–148). Amsterdam: test. New Windsor, England: The NFER-Nelson
Elsevier. Publishing Co. Ltd.
Tranel, D., & Jones, R. D. (2006). Knowing what Wechsler, D. (1997). Manual for the Wechsler Adult
and knowing when. Journal of Clinical and Intelligence Scale-III. New York: Psychological
Experimental Neuropsychology, 28, 43–66. Corporation.
Tranel, D., & Levin, H. (2009). The legacy of Arthur Wechsler, D. (2008). Manual for the Wechsler Adult
Benton. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Intelligence Scale-IV. New York: Psychological
Neuropsychology, 31. (in press). Corporation.
Tranel, D., Vianna, E. P. M., Manzel, K., Damasio, Wilkinson, G. S., & Robertson G. J. (2006). Wide
H., & Grabowski, T. (2009). Neuroanatomical range achievement test—4th Edition. Odessa FL:
correlates of the Benton Facial Recognition Test Professional Manual, Psychological Assessment
and Judgment of Line Orientation Test. Journal of Resources Inc.
Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 31. (in Yesavage, J. A., Brink, T. L., Rose, T. L., Lum, O.,
press) Huang, V., Adey, M. B., et al. (1983). Development
Wada, J., & Rasmussen, T. (1960). Intracarotid injec- and validation of a geriatric depression rating
tion of sodium Amytal for the lateralization of scale: A preliminary report. Journal of Psychiatric
cerebral speech dominance: Experimental and Research, 17, 37–49.
5

Computer-Based Cognitive Testing


Thomas H. Crook, Gary G. Kay, and Glenn J. Larrabee

More than a decade ago, in the second edition of deal of overlap between many batteries. Thus, we
this book, we described the general merits and have chosen among the available test batteries
limitations of using computers in cognitive test- those with the best psychometric foundations,
ing and went on to describe a number of specific and those that have taken a novel approach to
batteries available to clinicians and researchers testing. But, before the review, let us consider
(Larrabee & Crook, 1996). In looking back, we the value that computers can bring to clinical
see that, like old soldiers, several of the test bat- neuropsychological evaluation.
teries we described have just faded away, while
others have been further refined and strength-
Advantages of Computers
ened, and yet others, many others, have emerged
in Neuropsychology
and may provide useful new tools in neuropsy-
chological evaluation. Aside from these new Even two decades ago, computers were seen as
tools, though, the decade has illustrated the having advantages in testing far beyond data
limitations of computerized cognitive testing. storage. These were said to include data scor-
Principally, that neuropsychological diagnostic ing and analysis, analysis of test profi les for
testing without a neuropsychologist is probably diagnostic classification, increased reliability,
not a great idea. A number of Web sites and enhanced capacity to generate complex stim-
test vendors have purported to do just that and uli, greater accuracy and superior time resolu-
have, thankfully, failed to find acceptance in tion, standardization of stimulus presentation,
the marketplace. That is not to say that comput- and ease of administration, which reduces the
erized memory testing via the Internet is not need for highly skilled personnel (Adams, 1986;
appropriate for preliminary screening, and even Adams & Brown, 1986). Limitations were also
repeated testing over time to detect change, but recognized, and these were said to include alter-
it is not now, and is not likely to become, a sub- ations in the patient’s perception of and response
stitute for traditional clinical diagnostic eval- to the test when a standardized paper and pen-
uation. Moreover, any Internet-based testing cil test is adapted for computerized administra-
should be under the supervision of a qualified tion (Adams & Brown, 1986) and differential
psychologist to minimize the chances of misat- familiarity of patients with computer manipu-
tribution of results and related iatrogenic fac- landa used in some computerized test batteries
tors (Mittenberg et al., 1992; Suhr & Gunstad, (Kapur, 1988). Larrabee and Crook (1991) also
2002). emphasized the need to thoroughly validate
There are so many new computerized cogni- computerized batteries and not assume that a
tive test batteries that a complete review could validated paper and pencil test remains valid
occupy this entire volume. There is also a great when administered via computer, the need for

84
Computer-Based Cognitive Testing 85

alternate forms and extensive normative data, to simulate memory and other cognitive tasks
and the need to develop tests that are “ecologi- of everyday life.
cally valid,” that is, relevant to the tasks patients
perform in everyday life and the symptoms that
underlie many neurocognitive disorders. Computerized Batteries for
Although there are advantages to computer-
Assessing Memory and Related
ized cognitive assessment, there are also limi-
Cognitive Abilities
tations. Adapting existing neuropsychological
tests to computerized administration changes
MicroCog
the nature of the tests. This, in turn, can influ-
ence the nature of the patient’s perception of MicroCog was developed in an effort to identify
the test, which can affect his or her motivation cognitive status changes in physicians and other
and response style (Adams & Brown, 1986). professionals that might interfere with occupa-
Kapur (1988) cautions that most computer- tional performance (Kane, 1995; Powell et al.,
ized measures have significant visuoperceptual 1993), but it has subsequently become a general
demands, which can cause difficulty for patients neuropsychological screening instrument. Since
with reduced visual acuity or neglect. In addi- 2003, it has been available from Psychological
tion, many computerized test batteries require Corporation on a Windows platform. The bat-
the patient to utilize manipulanda that may be tery has not been widely accepted by either
unfamiliar, such as a mouse or computer key- clinicians or researchers. Nevertheless, in recent
board. This may pose particular difficulty for years, the program was revised and now oper-
severely impaired patients, especially among ates on a standard PC running the Windows
the elderly (Larrabee & Crook, 1991). operating system. Responses are entered using
Finally, it is important that psychologists uti- a standard keyboard, and this mode, of course,
lizing computers for evaluation adhere to pro- may pose a problem because some testees, par-
fessional standards regarding such instruments ticularly older males, may have limited experi-
(Division 40 Task Force Report on Computer- ence with a keyboard.
Assisted Neuropsychological Evaluation, 1987; Five primary neurobehavioral domains are
Matthews et al., 1991). Computerized neuropsy- assessed, including Attention/Mental Control,
chological evaluation, in general, and memory Memory, Reasoning/Calculation, Spatial Pro-
testing, in particular, should be conducted in cessing, and Reaction time. The Attention/
line with APA guidelines for test instruments Mental Control Domain includes span tasks
concerning reliability and validity, and user (numbers forward and reversed), a continuous
qualifications. performance task (Alphabet subtest) requiring
Two basic approaches have been employed identification of letters of the alphabet as they
in computerized cognitive assessment. The appear in sequence within a series of random
first, exemplified by the MicroCog (Powell letters, and a supraspan word list test (Word
et al., 1993), MindStreams (Dwolatzky et al., Lists subtest) presented in continuous perfor-
2004), CogScreen (Kay, 1995), Cognitive Drug mance format, with subsequent assessment
Research (CDR; Wesnes et al., 1987), Cambridge of incidental learning. Memory is assessed for
Neuropsychological Tests Automated Battery immediate and delayed recognition of the con-
(CANTAB; Morris et al., 1987), ImPACT (Miller tent of two stories read by the examinee and
et al., 2007), Automated Neuropsychological delayed recognition of a street address, assessed
Assessment Metrics (ANAM; Reeves et al., in a multiple-choice (i.e., recognition) format.
2007), and Cog State (Maruff et al., in press) Reaction time assesses visual and auditory
batteries, adapts standard cognitive tasks for simple reaction time. The Spatial Processing
computerized administration and scoring. The domain also includes a visual working memory
second approach, exemplified by the Psychologix subtest, in which the subject must reproduce,
Battery developed by Crook, Larrabee, and following a one-second presentation, a grid pat-
Youngjohn (e.g., Larrabee & Crook, 1991), makes tern in a 3 × 3 matrix, in which three, four, or
use of video and computer graphics technologies five of the spaces are colored.
86 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

MicroCog can be administered in the 18- and Reversed loaded on the same attention fac-
subtest standard form in one hour or as a tor as WMS-R Digit Span and Visual Memory
12-subtest short form in 30 minutes. Scores Span, whereas MicroCog immediate recogni-
are provided for accuracy, speed, and profi- tion/recall of two stories loaded on a memory
ciency (a combination of speed and accuracy). factor with WMS-R Logical Memory I. A recent
Measures of general cognitive function and study (Helmes & Miller, 2006) among older sub-
proficiency are also computed. There are only jects in the community found modest correla-
two alternate forms, and thus the battery is not tions between MicroCog and Wechsler Memory
appropriate for many research applications or, Scale-Three (WMS-III; Wechsler, 1997) subtests
perhaps, longitudinal clinical follow-up. of the same construct. Correlations between the
A strength of MicroCog is the normative visual subtests of the two tests were not even
database of 810 adults (45 females and 45 males statistically significant. So, it seems clear that
in each of nine age groups: 18–24, 25–34, 35–44, MicroCog is not a substitute for the WMS-R.
45–54, 55–64, 65–69, 70–74, 75–79, 80–89). Test Kane (1995) reviewed MicroCog and noted
data are also presented for a variety of clinical that its strengths included the computerization
groups, including dementia, lobectomy, depres- of a number of traditional neuropsychological
sion, and schizophrenia. Data are provided for measures, the addition of proficiency scores, the
percentage correct classification, sensitivity, provision of detailed information on standard
and specificity for each clinical group. In addi- error of measurement for subtests and general
tion, mean levels of performance for the five performance indices, and sizable age- and edu-
neurobehavioral domain index scores, process- cation-based norms. Weaknesses were noted to
ing speed, processing accuracy, general cogni- be the use of a multiple key interface and lack of
tive function, and proficiency are provided for motor and divided attention tasks. Also, Kane
each clinical group. and Kay (1992) noted that much of the psycho-
Test–retest reliability does not appear to be a metric data cited for MicroCog was actually
great strength of MicroCog, although it is dif- obtained for earlier versions of the test.
ficult to know from the paper most frequently
cited (Elwood, 2001) how it was established. It
MindStreams
is not clear whether subjects were tested on dif-
ferent days, with different forms, and so on. In a MindStreams is an “Advanced Cognitive Health
proper test–retest study in a small sample of 40, Assessment” battery developed by NeuroTrax, a
normal subjects (Raymond et al., 2006) ranged company founded in Israel in 2000, specifically
from .49 to .84 at 2 weeks and from .59 to .83 at to develop and commercialize computerized
3 months. cognitive testing. MindStreams was launched
Factor analytic data are provided that demon- in 2003 and consists of a battery of mostly tra-
strate two factors: information-processing accu- ditional neuropsychological tests administered
racy and information-processing speed (Powell and scored by computer. Tests are downloaded
et al., 1993). Concurrent validity data are pro- over the Internet and scores are transmitted
vided that demonstrate the correlation of vari- back to a NeuroTrax central computer, where
ous MicroCog scales with external test criteria. data are processed and scores calculated. Tests
For example, the MicroCog Attention/Mental can be administered on a standard PC, and a
Control Index correlates .57 with the Wechsler combination mouse/key pad used by the sub-
Memory Scale-Revised (WMS-R; Wechsler, ject in responding during testing is provided by
1987) Attention/Concentration Index and the MindStreams.
MicroCog Memory Index correlates .44 with the Individual test scores and index scores for
WMS-R General Memory Index (Powell et al., Memory, Executive Function, Visual Spatial
1993). These are not impressive correlations. and Verbal Function, Attention, Information-
Data relevant to construct validity are provided Processing Speed, and Motor Skills are calcu-
by Ledbetter and Hurley (1994). In a combined lated and transmitted back to the user within
MicroCog WMS-R factor analysis, MicroCog minutes in the form of a detailed report.
Alphabet, Word List, and Numbers Forward The report compares an individual’s scores
Computer-Based Cognitive Testing 87

with scores in earlier test sessions and with (Ritsner et al., 2006), and Gaucher’s disease
those of subjects matched for age and edu- (Elstein et al., 2005).
cation within a normative database of 1659 There are several versions of the MindStreams
subjects between the ages of 9 and 95. Tests in battery designed for research in ADHD, MCI
the Global Assessment Battery include Verbal and Alzheimer’s disease, Parkinson’s disease,
and Nonverbal Memory, Problem Solving and schizophrenia. There are also some lim-
(Nonverbal IQ), Stroop Interference, Finger ited data suggesting that MindStreams tests
Tapping, Catch Game, Staged Information- are sensitive to changes induced by alcohol
Processing Speed, Verbal Function, and Visual (Jaffe et al., 2005) and to the effects of stimulant
Spatial Processing. Shorter batteries have been drugs in children with ADHD (Leitner et al., in
developed for research in conditions ranging press) and older patients with Parkinson’s dis-
from attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder ease (Auriel et al., 2006), but these are small
(ADHD) to Alzheimer’s disease. There are four sample pilot studies.
alternate forms of each test, and they are avail- In general, MindStreams appears to be a
able in multiple languages. The average time useful battery for the clinician assessing cog-
required for completion of the MindStreams nitive function and, particularly, attempting
Global Assessment Battery is 45–60 minutes. to distinguish among the healthy elderly those
Test–retest reliability has been demonstrated with MCI and those with mild dementia. In a
over hours, weeks, and months, and correlation study of 161 individuals with expert diagnoses
coefficients cited by NeuroTrax from multiple (Doniger et al., 2005b), the validity of the battery
studies (e.g., Doniger & Simon, 2006; Schweiger in making these distinctions was demonstrated
et al., 2003) range from .52 for Memory to .83 convincingly. The value of the battery in some
for Motor Skills. The correlation cited for the repeated-measures research will be limited by
Global Cognitive Score is .77. The low reliability the availability of only four alternate forms. Of
on memory performance is a limitation in the interest to clinicians will be the quality of the
battery. clinical report generated by MindStreams. In
Construct validity has been demonstrated our view, the report is superior in detail and
in several studies, including a comparison of clarity to those produced by the other comput-
MindStreams and standard neuropsycholog- erized batteries reviewed.
ical test performance in a cohort of 54 elderly
subjects, some of whom were healthy and others
CogScreen
met the criteria for Mild Cognitive Impairment
(MCI; Dwolatzky et al., 2003). Correlations CogScreen (Kay, 1995) was developed twenty
between the MindStreams Verbal Memory score years ago in response to the Federal Aviation
and Wechsler Memory Scale-Third Edition Administration’s (FAA) need for an instrument
(WMS III; Wechsler, 1997) Logical Memory that could detect subtle changes in cognitive
I and II scores as well as Visual Reproduction function relative to poor pilot judgment or slow
II were in the range of .70, and the Nonverbal reaction time in critical flight situations. As
Memory score was correlated at the same level, such, CogScreen was intended to measure the
with eight scores derived from the Rey Auditory underlying perceptual, cognitive, and informa-
Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT; Rey, 1961) and tion-processing abilities associated with flying.
WMS III. Significant correlations were also In general, CogScreen is focused on measures
reported between other MindStreams scores of attention, concentration, information pro-
and scores on standard neuropsychological cessing, immediate memory span, and working
tests designed to measure the same constructs. memory, as would be expected given the ori-
Construct validity was also demonstrated in gin of the battery. CogScreen includes Back-
children and adolescents (Doniger & Simon, ward Digit Span; Mathematical Reasoning; a
2006; Schweiger et al., 2007) and in several clin- Visual Sequence Comparison Task, in which
ical populations, including patients with move- the subject must identify two simultaneously
ment disorders (Doniger et al., 2006), multiple presented alphanumeric strings as same or dif-
sclerosis (MS; Simon et al., 2006), schizophrenia ferent; a Symbol Digit Coding Task (analogous
88 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

to WAIS-R Digit Symbol), which also includes variables (e.g., age, education, flight status, total
immediate and delayed incidental learning tri- flight hours logged, and nature of referral, such
als; a Matching-to-Sample Task involving pre- as alcohol-related or head injury). Norm-based
sentation of a 4 × 4 grid of fi lled and empty reports are provided. The CogScreen U.S. avi-
cells, followed by a brief delay and presentation ator normative base includes 584 U.S. pilots
of two new matrices, one of which is identi- screened for health status and alcohol and sub-
cal to the original (requiring visuoperceptual stance abuse. The CogScreen manual includes
speed, spatial processing, and visual working analysis of age effects, gender, education, and
memory); the Manikin subtest, which presents IQ on performance. Gender and education had
a male human figure at different orientations minimal effects, whereas age and IQ reflected
holding a flag that the subject must identify modest degrees of association with CogScreen
as being in the left or right hand; a Divided performance (maximum age effect, 12.3% of the
Attention Task, combining visual monitoring variance with any one measure; maximum IQ
and visual sequencing; an Auditory Sequence effect, 9% of variance).
Comparison, requiring identification of two Validity data are provided in terms of concur-
tonal sequences as the same or different (analo- rent validity (correlation of CogScreen measures
gous to the Seashore Rhythm Test); a Pathfinder with related WAIS-R tasks, and correlations of
Test, similar to the Trail-Making Test; a Shifting CogScreen measures with specialized neuro-
Attention Test, requiring subjects to alter their psychological tasks such as the Wisconsin Card
responses dependent on changing rules (involv- Sorting Test, PASAT, and Trail-Making Test).
ing attribute identification, mental flexibil- Several factor analytic studies of CogScreen
ity, sustained attention, deductive reasoning, have explored the factor structure of the bat-
response interference, and a variety of other tery (Taylor et al., 2000). Additional data are
cognitive skills); and Dual Task, which presents provided contrasting CogScreen performance
two tasks (visual-motor tracking and a visual of pilots selected from the normative base and
memory span task for numbers) independently, matched on age and education with nonpilot
then simultaneously. Most tests are available in controls and with patients with mild brain
99 alternate forms. dysfunction. There were no significant differ-
The entire CogScreen battery requires about ences between pilots and nonpilot controls;
45 minutes for administration, and all subject however, both groups performed at a superior
responses are input with a light pen or touch level compared to the patient group. Additional
screen except for the tracking component of data are provided on pilot groups with suspi-
the Dual Task, which requires the subject to use cion of alcohol abuse, pilots with questionable
the keyboard’s arrow keys (Kane & Kay, 1992). proficiency, pilots referred for evaluation of the
The test battery runs on PCs with the Windows impact of psychiatric impairment of cognitive
98 or 2000 or XP operating system. There are function, and pilots with both suspected and
now 10 different versions of CogScreen, which confirmed neurologic disorders.
consist of different groupings of CogScreen Although developed as a test to be used for
subtests. These versions of CogScreen were the medical certification of pilots with psychi-
developed for specific research applications. Test atric or neurological conditions, CogScreen has
instructions are available in English, Spanish, many other applications. In the aviation world,
French, and Russian. CogScreen is currently CogScreen is used for pilot selection by major
being used in North and South America, airlines and by military organizations. The test
Europe, Asia, Africa, and Australia. Scoring has been proven to be a good predictor of pilot
is provided for response speed, accuracy, success. It is also used to periodically mon-
throughput (number of correct responses per itor the neurocognitive functioning of HIV
minute), and, on certain tasks, process mea- seropositive pilots. In the area of biomedical
sures (e.g., impulsivity, perseverative errors). research, CogScreen has been used to study
CogScreen also provides for entry of impor- the effect of environmental stressors on cogni-
tant demographic and performance-related tion (e.g., hypoxia and sleep deprivation), the
Computer-Based Cognitive Testing 89

neurocognitive effects of medical treatments Table 5–1. Core Tests in the CDR Battery
(e.g., nasal CPAP treatment for sleep apnea), and the Constructs They Measure
and for evaluating the adverse or beneficial cog-
Attention
nitive effects of medications (e.g., the sedating
Simple Reaction Time
effect of antihistamines). Choice Reaction Time
The strength of CogScreen is in the area of
Working Memory
attention and information-processing speed. The
Articulatory Working Memory
test includes sensitive measures of multitasking, Spatial Working Memory
working memory, and executive functions. On
the other hand, it provides very limited testing Episodic secondary memory
of memory. For this reason, in clinical studies, Word Recall
Word Recognition
CogScreen is often administered together with Picture Recognition
the Psychologix battery, described in later pages
(e.g., Kay et al., 2006). CogScreen’s use in “high
stakes” testing (e.g., job selection, and as pri- subsequently are on the list being held. Both
mary outcome measure in pharmaceutical and speed and accuracy are recorded on this and
National Institutes of Health studies) is unpar- the other test of working memory. The Spatial
alleled. The validity of the test was affirmed Working Memory Test utilizes a three by three
by the National Transportation Safety Board array of lights, said to be windows in a house.
(Hoover v. FAA). Furthermore, CogScreen Four of the nine windows are randomly chosen
results have been used to support drug claims and lighted, and on subsequent presentations
made by pharmaceutical companies (e.g., the individual windows are lighted and the subject
“nonsedating” label for Claritin). must indicate whether or not that window was
among the four initially lighted. The Episodic
Secondary Memory tests utilize traditional
Cognitive Drug Research (CDR) Battery
recall (immediate and delayed) and recognition
As the name implies, the CDR battery has been procedures.
used almost exclusively in drug research. The Five factors have been identified among CDR
origins of the battery date back to the late 1970s outcome measures using Principal Compon-
(Wesnes, 1977), and the core tests in the current ents Analysis (Wesnes et al., 2000), and these,
battery were introduced in the mid-1980s. In together with the measures that load on each
the ensuing decades, the CDR was used in hun- factor, are shown in Table 5–2.
dreds of clinical drug trials aimed at establish- Although these are very simple and tra-
ing efficacy or assessing unintended cognitive ditional tests, their use for more than two
side effects of medication (e.g., Wesnes, 2003). decades in many populations has generated a
Core tests in the CDR battery and the con- very significant body of data demonstrating,
structs they are intended to measure are given for example, sensitivity to and the existence of
in Table 5–1. a distinctive test profi le for aging, stroke, mul-
Each test is brief, usually one to three min- tiple sclerosis, chronic fatigue syndrome, diabe-
utes, and subjects respond in testing by press- tes, MCI, and many other conditions (Wesnes,
ing “Yes” or “No” buttons. Fift y alternate forms 2003). CDR tests have been shown sensitive to
are available for most tests, and very substan- the effects of a vast array of drugs and dietary
tial data are available related to the reliability, supplements (e.g., Wesnes et al., 2000), but none
validity, and utility of the system (Mohr et al., of these findings has led to drug approval or a
1996). The reaction time tasks in the battery change in labeling, and, thus, questions related
require no explanation, but the Articulatory to specificity may arise.
Working Memory Test is based on the Sternberg The CDR battery is provided as a turnkey sys-
procedure and involves the subject holding a tem consisting of a standard laptop computer
short series, primarily through self-repetition, with testing soft ware, a proprietary USB key
and then identifying whether digits presented for data storage, and a response pad/box with
90 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

Table 5–2. CDR Outcome Measures Using measuring reaction time. For many years before
Principal Components Analysis this version was introduced, CANTAB was used
by researchers in aging, Alzheimer’s disease,
Speed of Memory Processes
schizophrenia, depression, and many other
Picture Recognition Speed
Word Recognition Speed conditions and disease states (e.g., Owen et al.,
Numeric Working Memory Speed 1990; Sahakian, 1990; Sahakian et al., 1988). It
Spatial Working Memory Speed has also been shown sensitive to drug effects
Quality of Episodic Secondary Memory (e.g., Jones et al., 1992), although far more evi-
Immediate Word Recall Accuracy dence of drug sensitivity has been shown for
Delayed Word Recall Accuracy the CDR.
Word Recognition Accuracy The CANTAB battery consists of 19 tests,
Picture Recognition Accuracy beginning with two Motor Screening Tests, fol-
Power of Attention lowed by four Visual Memory Tests. These are
Simple Reaction Time a Delayed Matching-to-Sample Test, including
Choice Reaction Time perceptual matching, immediate and delayed
Digit Vigilance Detection Speed recall tasks in which the subject is shown a
Continuity of Attention complex visual pattern, followed by four pat-
Digit Vigilance Detection Accuracy terns from which he or she must choose the
Choice Reaction Time Accuracy pattern first shown. This is followed by a Paired-
Digit Vigilance False Alarms
Tracking Error
Associates Learning Test, in which the subject
must remember which patterns are associated
Quality of Working Memory with which positions on the touch screen. Still
Numeric Working Memory Accuracy within the Visual Memory construct, a Pattern
Spatial Working Memory Accuracy
Recognition Memory Test is given, involving
two-choice forced discriminations and a Spatial
Recognition Memory Test, also involving two-
a separate USB connection. Tests can only be choice forced discriminations. Moving on to a
scored at CDR headquarters in England. seemingly heterogeneous “Executive Function,
Working Memory, and Planning” domain, the
first test is the ID/ED Shift Test, which measures
Cambridge Neuropsychology Test
the ability to attend to a specific attribute of a
Automated Battery (CANTAB)
complex visual stimulus and to shift the attrib-
CANTAB, like CDR, comes from a British com- ute when required. This is followed by the more
pany that has developed very traditional tests colorfully named Stockings of Cambridge Test of
and gathered quite a lot of data with them over spatial planning, based on the Tower of London
the past two decades. Indeed, both companies Test. Next is the Spatial Span test, in which nine
claim to provide the world’s most widely used white squares appear in random positions on
computerized neuropsychological test battery. the screen and between two and nine of them
In the case of CANTAB, it is reported to be used are then lighted in different colors in random
in 50 countries, at 500 research institutes, with order. The subject must remember that order.
more than 300 publications. Also like CDR, Finally within this domain, the Spatial Working
CANTAB tests appear dated, although there Memory Test involves a series of red boxes on
is clear value in the databases that have been the screen, some of which when touched, reveal
developed by both companies during the past 20 a blue box. The object is to remember which
years. CANTAB, unlike CDR, has not focused red boxes one has touched and find the blue
primarily on drug development and appears boxes without returning to a red box previously
less well suited to that task. CANTAB may be of touched.
greater interest to clinicians, however. Performance in the Attention domain is
A current version of CANTAB, CANTAB assessed on CANTAB with several simple
eclipse, employs a touch screen as a primary and complex reaction time tasks, a Matching-
response device, as well as a press pad for to-Sample Visual Search Test, and a Rapid Visual
Computer-Based Cognitive Testing 91

Information-Processing test, in which the dig- substantial data on reliability and validity are
its 2–9 are presented in a pseudorandom order available. Clinicians comparing MindStreams
at the rate of 100 per second. The subject must or CogState test presentation, scoring, and
identify consecutive odd or even digits as reporting with CANTAB may find the latter
quickly as possible. dated, particularly because CANTAB test com-
There are two tests of the Semantic/Verbal puters must be purchased from the company
Memory construct, the first of which is the and the tests cannot be downloaded and scored
Graded Naming Test, in which subjects must via the Internet. Also, the cost of the battery
identify each of 30 black and white drawings and a 10-year license is, as of this writing, US
of objects/animals presented in an increasing $14,000, and for these reasons the battery is
order of difficulty. The other test of the con- used by few clinicians.
struct is the Verbal Recognition Memory Test,
in which subjects are presented with a list of
CogState
12 words, followed by free recall and forced
choice recall. A final domain is Emotional CogState tests (with one exception) are game-like
Decision Making, and here there are two tests, and entirely lacking in face validity. There has
the Affective Go–No Go Test, in which a sub- been a serious effort in recent years to establish
ject must recognize the emotional valence of construct and criterion validity in the absence of
words that are presented on the screen, and the face validity. Also, unlike CANTAB, CogState
Cambridge Gambling Test, which is intended to utilizes a technologically sophisticated platform
assess decision-making and risk-taking behav- for Internet testing. The company has slightly
ior outside a learning context. This final test different batteries for clinical trials, academic
involves betting on the color of the box, among studies, use in sports, testing by physicians (and
ten, that contains a token and appears appro- presumably available to neuropsychologists), and
priately named, but an odd measure of decision use in the workplace. Tests in their Academic
making in everyday life. Battery, together with the cognitive domain said
As noted, more than 300 studies have been to be measured and the time required for admin-
reported using the CANTAB tests, and very istration, are listed in Table 5–3.

Table 5–3. Tests in CogState Academic Battery


CogState task Cognitive domain Time required (minutes)
Detection* psychomotor function 2
Identification* visual attention 2
One Card: learn* visual learning 5
One Card: delayed recall visual memory
1.25
One Word: Learn verbal learning 5
One Word: Recall verbal memory
1.25
One Back* working memory 2
Congruent reaction time* visual attention 2
Monitoring* Attention 2
Prediction* executive function 5
Prediction: delayed recall* visual memory 0.5
Associate learning* Memory 5
International Shopping verbal learning 5
List Task (ISLT)
ISLT:delayed recall verbal memory 1
Groton Maze Learning executive function 10
Task (GMLT)
GMLT: delayed recall spatial memory 2
GMLT: reverse maze executive function 2
92 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

A visual paired-associates learning and recall Spatial Span test, and the Tower of London Test
task, a set-shift ing task, and a social–emotional from CANTAB; all tests are described in detail
cognition test have recently been added to the elsewhere (e.g., Lezak, 1995). In the psychomo-
battery as well, although data on their reliabil- tor domain, correlations between standard and
ity and validity have not been published to our CogState measures ranged from .71 for Trails
knowledge. A to .32 for Grooved Pegboard (dominant). On
CogState measures of test–retest reliabilities attentional measures, correlations ranged from
ranging from .67 for memory to .89 for psycho- .67 on Digit Symbol and .56 on Trails B to .10 on
motor performance and also for attention have the cancellation task. In the memory domain,
been reported, with playing cards utilized as the all accuracy coefficients were highly significant
stimuli (Collie et al., 2003; Faletti et al., 2003). (p < .01), ranging from .86 on the Rey Figure test
Each of the tests above designated with an and .85 on the CANTAB paired-associates test
asterisk uses playing cards as stimuli, and, thus, to .73 on Benton Visual Retention. Correlations
the measures are remote from both traditional were also highly significant on measures of
neuropsychological tests and clinical reality. executive function. On accuracy scores, the
Prior to the addition of the ISLT, the CogState correlations were .86 with strategy on Spatial
battery was deficient in measures of verbal Working Memory, .69 with Spatial Span, and
learning and memory, and this addition clearly .67 with Tower of London performance. As to
strengthens the battery. Many of the tests in the criterion validity, four separate studies reported
battery also appear far too difficult for many by Maruff in this same paper demonstrate that
clinical populations. On the other hand, the CogState performance clearly distinguishes
technology employed in testing and scoring is between comparable normals and individuals
impressive, and the battery is well suited for use suffering from mild head injury resulting from
across languages and cultures. It is currently auto accidents, MCI, schizophrenia, and AIDS
available in 15 languages and is easily translated dementia. The pattern of tests distinguishing
into others. Also, the CogState battery has been subjects with each of the disorders from normals
used in a number of creative studies during the was as one would expect. For example, MCI
past 5 years, and more than 50 peer-reviewed deficits are limited largely to memory, while
articles have been published or are in press. attentional and executive function deficits are
Maruff (In Press) has recently attempted also seen in schizophrenia. Other studies (e.g.,
to demonstrate construct validity and crite- Collie et al., 2002; Cysique et al., 2006) have also
rion validity by showing sensitivity to MCI, addressed the validity of Cogstate tests in dis-
schizophrenia, and AIDS dementia. This study tinguishing among these and other diagnostic
included only the tests in which playing cards groups. There are also data on the validity of the
are used as stimuli. One hundred and thirteen Groton Maze Learning Test (e.g., Pietrzak et al.,
healthy young adults participated in the con- 2007), but it is much more limited than that on
struct validity study, taking both CogState, a tests using playing cards as stimuli.
battery of standard neuropsychological tests, CogState soft ware can be downloaded over
and a paired-associates memory test from the the Internet on to most PCs, and the primary
CANTAB computerized battery. Tests in the response mechanism is the keyboard space
standard battery for psychomotor function bar. Scoring and reporting are done via the
were Trail Making A and Grooved Pegboard Internet.
(dominant and nondominant). Attentional
function was measured with the Digit Symbol
The Automated Neuropsychological
Substitution test, cancellation task, and Trail
Assessment Metrics (ANAM)
Making Part B. The standard memory tests
were the Paired-Associates Learning Test ANAM is described as “a library of computer-
from the CANTAB, Benton Visual Retention, ized tests and test batteries designed for a broad
and delayed recall from the Rey Figure Test. spectrum of clinical and research applications”
Standard tests of executive function were the (Reeves et al., 2007). The current version of
Spatial Working Memory Test strategy score, ANAM, Version 4, is a Windows-based program
Computer-Based Cognitive Testing 93

that uses a mouse and keyboard as response erythematosus, hypothermia and Alzheimer’s
input devices. There is also a “Web-enabled” disease, multiple sclerosis, and traumatic brain
and Palm-OS version of the test. The battery is injury, as well as individuals exposed to ionizing
available through C-Shop at the University of radiation. ANAM has also been used in evaluat-
Oklahoma. The battery was originally devel- ing the effects of a nicotine patch for treatment
oped by the Department of Defense and is of Age-Associated Memory Impairment. In
similar to most of the early DOD Performance addition, studies have documented the reliabil-
Assessment Batteries (PABs). There have been ity and validity of ANAM subtests.
numerous iterations of the ANAM battery over In spite of these developments, ANAM
the last 20 years. The test is highly configurable remains more of a “performance assessment
with respect to such parameters as inclusion battery” than a standardized test battery. The
or exclusion of subtests, number of items to test is not widely used by either clinicians or
include in a subtest, and interstimulus interval. investigators. Until recently, this DOD-funded
The subtests included in ANAM are shown in test was in the “public domain.” The test has
Table 5–4. been acquired by the University of Oklahoma,
In addition, there are Tapping, Tower of which now sells and licenses the battery.
Hanoi, Stroop, and other standard cognitive
tasks. Reeves described a “standard” or “default”
ImPACT Test Battery
version of ANAM that consists of 13 subtests.
Although recognized, primarily, as a research The ImPACT Test is a computer-administered
tool, the creators of ANAM claim that it is now test battery developed to assess concussion,
being developed as a clinical instrument. primarily from sports-related injuries. The
A PubMed search of “ANAM” results in 25 ImPACT 2005 is a Windows-based program
citations between 1996 and 2007, with seven that claims to measure response times with one-
of these originating from the recent U.S. hundredth of a second resolution. The program
Army-sponsored supplement in the Archives is capable of creating an unlimited number of
of Clinical Neuropsychology. The articles in alternate forms. The test battery takes approx-
the supplement document the use of ANAM imately 20 minutes and includes 6 “modules,”
in “extreme environments,” sports medicine, which provide assessment of attention span,
pharmacological studies, and in clinical popula- working memory, sustained and selective atten-
tions. There are now very extensive norms avail- tion, response variability, nonverbal problem
able for active duty young military individuals solving, and reaction time. There is a 20-minute
(N = 2371). Prior publications document the delayed recall task for the word discrimination
use of ANAM in patients with systemic lupus and design memory subtests. In addition to
these two subtests, there are traditional mea-
sures of symbol digit substitution, a choice reac-
Table 5–4. Subtests Included in ANAM tion task, a Sternberg three-letter recall task,
2-Choice Reaction Time and a visual working memory task. Results are
4-Choice Reaction Time immediately available upon completion of the
Code Substitution exam in a well-designed report that compares
Grammatical Reasoning the current scores to the subject’s own base-
Logical Reasoning
Manikin line or to the normative database. The program
Matching Grids can be installed on stand-alone PCs and does
Matching to Sample not require Internet connection for scoring or
Mathematical Processing administration. The test generates a series of
Memory Search scores that are sensitive to head trauma, including
Running Memory (CPT)
Simple Reaction Time five composite scores: Memory Composite (ver-
Spatial Processing bal), Memory Composite (visual), Visual Motor
Continuous Performance Test Speed, Reaction Time, and Impulse Control.
Switching The authors incorporated a Reliable Change
Visual Vigilance Index for identifying meaningful changes in
94 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

scores across administrations. The test is used by Test (Crook et al., 1993), which assesses the sub-
National Football League teams, Major League ject’s recall of the name of the city each person
Baseball teams, and numerous colleges and uni- in Name–Face Association identifies as his/her
versities. However, at present, the research base home. Associative learning is also evaluated
on the ImPACT test is almost entirely limited to with a primarily nonvisual task, the First–Last
sports injury studies (e.g., Miller et al., 2007). Names Test (Youngjohn et al., 1991), which
measures associate learning and recall of four
to six paired first and last names over three to
Psychologix Computer-Simulated
six trials (the subject must recall the first name
Everyday Memory Test Battery
associated with each last name). Narrative Recall
The preceding batteries all share the feature of measures the subject’s ability to answer 25 fac-
evaluating memory using traditional psycho- tual, multiple-choice questions about a 6-minute
metric stimuli. By contrast, the Psychologix television news broadcast (Crook et al., 1990a;
Computer-Simulated Everyday Memory Battery Hill et al., 1989). Selective Reminding uses the
(previously known as the Memory Assessment paradigm devised by Buschke (1973) to evalu-
Clinics Battery) is unique in that stimuli that ate learning and retention of 15 grocery items
are immediately relevant to everyday memory over five trials with a 30-minute delayed recall
tasks are employed. The current battery repre- (Youngjohn et al., 1991). The Misplaced Objects
sents the fift h generation of technology in a test Test (Crook et al., 1990b) is a visual–verbal asso-
development effort by Crook and colleagues, ciative task in which the subject “places” (by
which began more that 25 years ago (e.g., Crook touching the touch-screen) 20 common objects
et al., 1979, 1980). From the outset, the goal was (e.g., shoes, eyeglasses) in a 12-room house;
to simulate, in testing, tasks of everyday life that 40 minutes later, the subject is given a first and
must be performed by virtually everyone, which a second chance at object location recall. Two
are frequently affected by trauma, neurological measures are employed for facial recognition
disease, and developmental conditions. Tests memory assessment (Crook & Larrabee, 1992).
were designed, in multiple alternative forms, The first, Recognition of Faces—Signal Detection,
using computer-imaging technology to simu- employs signal detection procedures for evalu-
late demands of everyday life. This heightened ation of recognition memory, employing 156
everyday realism was combined with traditional facial photographs, with scores based on rec-
memory measurement paradigms such as selec- ognition over varying periods of time rang-
tive reminding, signal detection, delayed non- ing from no delay to 1 minute, 3 minutes, and
matching to sample, and associate learning. 5 minutes to a 40-minute delayed recognition
Procedures include the Name–Face Asso- period. The second, Recognition of Faces—
ciation Test (Crook & West, 1990), in which live Delayed Nonmatching to Sample, employs a
video recordings of persons introducing them- delayed nonmatching to sample paradigm
selves are presented in different paradigms and (Mishkin, 1978), in which the subject must iden-
both immediate and delayed recall are assessed. tify, by touching the screen, the new facial pho-
In the most frequently used paradigm, four- tograph added to an array over 25 trials, each
teen individuals appear on the screen, one after successive trial separated by an 8-second delay.
another, and each introduces himself/herself Working memory and attention are evaluated
by saying, “Hi, I’m (First Name).” Each then with the Telephone Dialing Test and Reaction
reappears in a different order, and the task of Time. Telephone Dialing (West & Crook, 1990)
the subject is to recall each name. On each recall requires the subject to dial 7- or 10-digit num-
trial the person to be remembered states the bers on a representation of a telephone dialing
name of the city where he/she lives as a potential pad on the computer screen after seeing them
recall cue. Depending on the population, there displayed on the screen. The test is also admin-
are two or three such acquisition and imme- istered with an interference format, in which
diate recall trials, followed by a delayed recall the subject, after dialing, hears either a ring
trial 30–40 minutes later. The city “cues” also or a busy signal. If the busy signal is heard,
provide the stimuli for an Incidental Memory the subject must hang up, and then redial the
Computer-Based Cognitive Testing 95

telephone. Reaction Time (Crook et al., 1993) function analysis that correctly distinguished
can be administered in one of two formats. 88.39% of subjects with Alzheimer’s disease
First, reaction time can be measured under from subjects with Age-Associated Memory
the single-task condition in which the sub- Impairment. Also, Ivnik and colleagues (1993)
ject must lift his or her finger off a computer- demonstrated that the Reaction Time, Name–
simulated (on the touchscreen) image of a gas Face Association, and Incidental Memory
pedal or brake pedal in response to a red or procedures were highly sensitive to the cogni-
green traffic light. Both lift and travel (from gas tive effects of dominant temporal lobectomy.
to brake pedal or vice versa) reaction times are Psychologix tests have been shown highly reli-
computed. Second, this task can be adminis- able (Crook et al., 1992) and sensitive to the
tered under the simultaneous processing task effects of drugs that both improve (e.g., Crook
(divided attention) condition, where the sub- et al., 1991; Pfizer study) and impair (e.g., Kay
ject must perform the gas pedal/brake pedal et al., 2006; Nickelsen et al., 1999) cognition in
maneuvers while listening to a radio broadcast many studies. Of greatest significance, because
of road and weather conditions. In this admin- Psychologix tests are used almost exclusively
istration, both lift and travel reaction times are in developing treatments for adult onset cogni-
computed—as well as the subject’s recall of the tive disorders, is the very high sensitivity of all
radio broadcast information. tests to the effects of aging. For example, on the
The Psychologix Battery has undergone Name–Face Association Test (Crook & West,
extensive standardization and psychometric 1990; Crook et al., 1993), the decline in perfor-
analysis. Crook and Larrabee (1988) and Tomer mance among healthy individuals is approxi-
and colleagues (1994) factor analyzed a variety mately 60% between ages 25 and 65. The effects
of scores from the battery and demonstrated fac- of age on performance of Psychologix tests have
tors of General Memory, Attentional Vigilance, been examined in more than 50 peer-reviewed
Psychomotor Speed, and Basic Attention. The publications, and individual differences that
factor structure did not vary as a function of affect test performance have been examined in
age, suggesting that although level of perfor- detail (e.g., West et al., 1992).
mance changed with age, the interrelationships Crook et al. (1992) analyzed the equivalency
of the tests did not. Hence, one can be assured of alternate forms of the various tests. At least
that the tests are measuring the same con- six equivalent forms were found for Telephone
structs, regardless of the adult subject’s age. In a Dialing, Name–Face Association, First–Last
second study, Larrabee and Crook (1989a) dem- Name Memory, and Selective Reminding. Eight
onstrated a more varied factor structure, when equivalent forms were found for Misplaced
First–Last Names and Selective Reminding Objects and Recognition of Faces—Delayed
were added to the battery. In this study, both Nonmatching to Sample.
verbal and visual factors emerged, in addition American normative data for adults ages
to an attentional factor and psychomotor speed 18–90 range from 488 (TV News) to 2204
factor. Concurrent validity was established by (Name-Face Memory), with sample sizes in the
a combined factor analysis of the Psychologix 1300 to 1900 range for most measures (Crook &
Battery, WAIS Vocabulary, WMS-R Logical West, 1990; Larrabee & Crook, 1994; West
Memory and Paired-Associates Learning, and et al., 1992). Additional data are collected on
the Benton Visual Retention Test. Further evi- over 500 persons with Alzheimer’s disease and
dence on validity is provided by many other more than 2000 persons with Age-Associated
studies. For example, Larrabee and Crook Memory Impairment. The tests are available in
(1989b) reported cluster analyses that yielded a several languages including English, a British
variety of everyday memory subtypes. Larrabee (Anglicized) version, French, Italian, Swedish,
et al. (1991) demonstrated a significant canon- Finnish, Danish, and German, and normative
ical correlation of 0.528 between memory self- data are available in all these languages. A true
ratings (MAC-S; Crook & Larrabee, 1990) and random sample of the Italian population, com-
factor scores from the Psychologix Battery. prising 1800 adults of all ages, is included in the
Youngjohn et al. (1992) reported a discriminant normative database (Crook et al., 1993).
96 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

The principal current use of the Psychologix life, on which developmental change or the
Battery is for evaluation of treatment effects in effects of neurological disease or trauma are
clinical trials of pharmacologic compounds, first noted. For example, we are now using
with potential benefit for ameliorating age- sophisticated multiple-screen, computerized
related memory disorders, and also in trials of driving simulators that provide quite realistic
drugs for the full range of medical indications graphics and representations of driving a car
where unwanted cognitive impairment may under a wide variety of circumstances. We have
occur (e.g., Ferris et al., 2006; Kay et al., 2006; validated our driving simulator and shown it
Seltzer et al., 2004). Although the current ver- sensitive to drug effects (Kay et al., 2004), but
sion of the Psychologix Battery is run on PCs beyond this technology lies the entire field of
with the Windows XP operating system and virtual reality. Future testing could allow sub-
commercially available touchscreens, use of the jects to enter an interactive world in which they
battery requires specialized support, and, thus, it would be called upon to perform a wide variety
is not well suited to most clinical applications. of cognitive tasks while their performance is
precisely measured. Such development efforts
Summary are already under way (Rizzo, 2007). At pre-
sent, we validate computerized tests against
The test batteries reviewed in this chapter dem- standard neuropsychological tests or in their
onstrate the many advantages of computer- ability to distinguish between groups based on
assisted cognitive evaluation, but it is important age or disease states. Of course, our ultimate
to exercise caution in the application of comput- concern is with the individual’s ability to func-
ers in clinical settings. Despite the technological tion in a cognitively demanding environment,
sophistication of several available computerized and, in our view, simulating that environment
memory tests, they do not all meet APA crite- in cognitive testing is an important direction
ria for test instruments concerning reliability, for the future.
validity, normative data, or with respect to their
test manuals. References
Nonetheless, significant progress has been
made in the application of computers to memory Adams, K. M. (1986). Concepts and methods in the
design of automata for neuropsychological test
evaluation in recent years. Continued growth
interpretation. In S. B. Filskov and T. J. Boll (Eds.),
can be expected in this area. There is a certain Handbook of clinical neuropsychology (vol. 2;
“technological seduction” concerning comput- pp. 561–576). New York: Wiley.
erized assessment and remediation, but current Adams, K. M., & Brown, G. C. (1986). The role of the
as well as future applications of computers with computer in neuropsychological assessment. In
cognitively impaired patients should be carefully I. Grant and K. M. Adams (Eds.), Neuropsychological
considered in relation to the Division 40 guide- assessment of neuropsychiatric disorders (pp. 87–89).
lines for use of computers in evaluation and New York: Oxford.
rehabilitation (Division 40 Task Force Report Auriel, E., Hausdorff, J. M., Herman, T., Simon, E. S., &
on Computer-Assisted Neuropsychological Giladi, N. (2006). Effects of methylphenidate
Evaluation, 1987; Matthews et al., 1991). on cognitive function and gait in patients
with Parkinson’s disease: A pilot study. Clinical
Neuropharmacology, 29, 15–17.
Future Directions Buschke, H. (1973). Selective reminding for analy-
sis of memory and learning. Journal of Verbal
We believe that the use of computers to admin-
Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 543–549.
ister standard paper and pencil tests is a very Collie, A., Maruff, P., & Currie, J. (2002). Behavioral
early stage in the development of computerized characterization of mild cognitive impair-
neuropsychological testing. We believe that ment. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
current technologies can be employed to pro- Neuropsychology, 24, 720–733.
vide highly realistic simulations of the cogni- Collie, A., Maruff, P., Darby, D. G., & McStephen, M.
tive tasks that must be performed in everyday (2003). The effects of practice on the cognitive test
Computer-Based Cognitive Testing 97

performance of neurologically normal individu- Doniger, G. M., Dwolatzky, T., Zucker, D. M.,
als assessed at brief test–retest intervals. Journal Chertkow, H., Crystal, H., Schweiger, A., et al.
of the International Neuropsychological Society, 9, (2005). Computerized cognitive testing battery
419–428. identifies MCI and mild dementia even in the pres-
Crook, T. H., Ferris, S., & McCarthy, M. (1979). The ence of depressive symptoms. American Journal
Misplaced-Objects Task: A brief test for memory of Alzheimer’s Disease and Other Dementias, 21,
dysfunction in the aged. Journal of the American 28–36.
Geriatrics Society, 27, 284–287. Doniger, G. M., Okun, M. S., Simon, E. S.,
Crook, T. H., Ferris, S. H., McCarthy, M., & Rae, D. Rodriguez, R. L., Jacobson, C. E., Weiss, D.,
(1980). Utility of digit recall tasks for assessing et al. (2006). Validation of a computerized neu-
memory in the aged. Journal of Consulting and ropsychological assessment (Mindstreams) in
Clinical Psychology, 48, 228–233. movement disorders: Interim analysis. 20th
Crook, T. H., & Larrabee, G. J. (1988). Interrelationships Annual Symposia on the Etiology, Pathogenesis,
among everyday memory tests: Stability of factor and Treatment of Parkinson’s Disease and Other
structure with age. Neuropsychology, 2, 1–12. Movement Disorders. Chicago, Illinois, United
Crook, T. H., & Larrabee, G. J. (1990). A self-rating States, October 8–11, 2006.
scale for evaluating memory in everyday life. Doniger, G. M., & Simon, E. S. (2006). Construct
Psychology and Aging, 5, 48–57. Validity of Mindstreams® Comparison with Paper-
Crook, T. H., & Larrabee, G. J. (1992). Changes in Based Tests. Internal Document. New York:
facial recognition memory across the adult life NeuroTrax Corporation.
span. Journal of Gerontology, 47, 138–141. Doniger, G. M., Zucker, D. M., Schweiger, A.,
Crook, T. H., Larrabee, G. J., & Youngjohn, J. R. (1993). Dwolatzky, T., & Simon, E. S. (2005). Towards
Age and incidental recall for a simulated everyday practical cognitive assessment for detection of
memory task. Journal of Gerontology, 48, 45–47. early dementia: A 30-minute computerized battery
Crook, T. H., Tinklenberg, J., Yesavage, J., Petrie, W., discriminates as well as longer testing. Current
Nunzi, M. G., & Massari, D. C. (1991). Effects of Alzheimer Research, 2, 117–124.
phosphatidylserine in Age Associated Memory Dwolatzky, T., Whitehead, V., Doniger, G. M.,
Impairment. Neurology, 41, 644–649. Simon, E. S., Schweiger, A., Jaffe, D, et al. (2003).
Crook, T. H., & West, R. L. (1990). Name recall per- Validity of a novel computerized cognitive battery
formance across the adult life span. British Journal for mild cognitive impairment. BMC Geriatrics, 3, 4.
of Psychology, 81, 335–349. Dwolatzky, T., Whitehead, V., Doniger, G. M.,
Crook, T. H., West, R. L., & Larrabee, G. J. (1993). Simon, E. S., Schweiger, A., Jaffe, D., et al. (2004).
The Driving-Reaction Time Test: Assessing age Validity of the Mindstreams computerized cogni-
declines in dual-task performance. Developmental tive battery. Journal of Molecular Neuroscience, 24,
Neuropsychology, 9, 31–39. 33–44.
Crook, T. H., Youngjohn, J. R., & Larrabee, C. J. (1990a). Elstein, D., Guedalia, J., Doniger, G. M., Simon, E. S.,
The TV News Test: A new measure of everyday Antebi, V., Arnon, Y., et al.. (2005). Computerized
memory for prose. Neuropsychology, 4, 135–145. cognitive testing in patients with type I Gaucher
Crook, T. H., Youngjohn, J. R., & Larrabee, C. J. disease: Effects of enzyme replacement and sub-
(1990b). The Misplaced Objects Test: A measure strate reduction. Genetics in Medicine, 7, 124–130.
of everyday visual memory. Journal of Clinical and Elwood, R. W. (2001). MicroCog: Assessment of cog-
Experimental Neuropsychology, 12, 819–833. nitive functioning. Neuropsychology Review, 11,
Crook, T. H., Youngjohn, J. R., & Larrabee, C. J. 89–100.
(1992). Multiple equivalent test forms in a com- Falleti, M. G., Maruff, P., Collie, A., Darby, D. G., &
puterized everyday memory battery. Archives of McStephen, M. (2003). Qualitative similarities
Clinical Neuropsychology, 7, 221–232. in cognitive impairment associated with 24 h of
Cysique, L. A., Maruff, P., Darby, D., & Brew, B. J. (2006). sustained wakefulness and a blood alcohol con-
The assessment of cognitive function in advanced centration of 0.05%. Journal of Sleep Research, 12,
HIV-1 infection and AIDS dementia complex using 265–274.
a new computerized cognitive test battery. Archives Ferris, S., Schneider, L., Farmer, M., Kay, G., & Crook, T.
of Clinical Neuropsychology, 21, 185–194. (2006). A double-blind, placebo-controlled trial of
Division 40 Task Force Report on Computer Assisted memantine in age-associated memory impairment
Neuropsychological Evaluation. (1987). Clinical (memantine in AAMI). International Journal of
Neuropsychologist, 2, 161–184. Geriatric Psychiatry, 21, 1–8.
98 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

Helmes, E., & Miller, M. (2006). A comparison of Larrabee, G. J., & Crook, T. H. (1994). Estimated prev-
MicroCog and the Wechsler Memory Scale (3rd ed.) alence of Age-Associated Memory Impairment
in older adults. Applied Neuropsychology, 13, 28–33. derived from standardized tests of memory func-
Hill, R. D., Crook, T. H., Zadeik, A., Sheikh, J., & tion. International Psychogeriatrics, 6, 95–104.
Yesavage, J. (1989). The effects of age on recall Larrabee, G. J., & Crook, T. H. (1996). Computers
of information from a simulated television news and memory. In I. Grant and K. M. Adams (Eds.).
broadcast. Educational Gerontology, 5, 607–613. Neuropsychological assessment of neuropsychi-
Ivnik, R. J., Malec, J. F., Sharbrough, F. W., Cascino, atric disorders (Second Edition) (pp. 102–117).
G. D., Hirschorn, K. A., Crook, T. H., et al. (1993). New York: Oxford University Press.
Traditional and computerized assessment proce- Larrabee, J. G., West, R. L., & Crook, T. H. (1991). The
dures applied to the evaluation of memory change association of memory complaint with computer-
after temporal lobectomy. Archives of Clinical simulated everyday memory performance. Journal
Neuropsychology, 8, 69–81. of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 4,
Jaffe, D., Doniger, G. M., Simon, E. S., & Neumark, Y. 466–478.
(2005). Computerized cognitive test perfor- Ledbetter, M., & Hurley, S. (1994). A construct vali-
mance varies with acute alcohol consumption. dation study of the attention and memory domains
158th Annual Meeting of the American Psychiatric on the Assessment of Cognitive Functioning Test.
Association. August 18–21, Washington, DC. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 9, 154.
Jones, G., Sahakian, B. J., Levy, R., Warburton, D. M., Leitner, Y., Doniger, G. M., Barak, R., Simon, E. S., &
& Gray, J. (1992). Effects of acute subcutaneous Hausdorff, J. M. (In Press). A novel multi-domain
nicotine on attention, information processing computerized cognitive assessment for atten-
and short-term memory in Alzheimer’s disease. tion deficit hyperactivity disorder: Evidence for
Psychopharmacology, 108, 485–494. widespread and circumscribed cognitive deficits.
Kane, R. L. (1995). MicroCog: A review. Bulletin of the Journal of Child Neurology.
National Academy of Neuropsychology, 11, 13–16. Lezak, M. D. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment
Kane, R. L., & Kay, G. G. (1992). Computerized assess- (3rd. ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
ment in neuropsychology: A review of tests and test Matthews, C. G., Harley, J. P., & Malec, J. F. (1991).
batteries. Neuropsychology Review, 3, 1–117. Guidelines for computer-assisted neuropsycho-
Kapur, N. (1988). Memory disorders in clinical logical rehabilitation and cognitive remediation.
practice. London: Butterworths. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 5, 319.
Kay, G. G. (1995). CogScreen-Aeromedical Edition. Maruff, P., Thomas, E., Cysique, L., Brew, B.,
Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Collie, A., Snyder, P., et al. (In Press). Validity
Assessment Resources. of CogState brief battery: Relationship to
Kay, G. G., Crook, T., Rekeda, L., Lima, R., Ebinger, U., standardized tests and sensitivity to cognitive
Arguinzoniz, M., et al. (2006). Differential effects impairment in mild traumatic brain injury and
of the antimuscarinic agents darifenacin and oxy- AIDS dementia complex. Archives of Clinical
butynin ER on memory in older subjects. European Neuropsychology.
Association of Urology, 50, 317–326. Miller, J. R., Adamson, G. J., Pink, M. M., & Sweet, J. C.
Kay, G. G., Pakull, B., Clark, T. M., Sea, D., Mays, D. A., & (2007). Comparison of preseason, midseason, and
Tulloch, S. J. (2004). The effect of Adderall XR® postseason neurocognitive scores in uninjured
treatment on driving performance in young adults collegiate football players. American Journal of
with ADHD. Paper presented at 17th Annual US Sports Medicine, 35(8), 1284–1288.
Psychiatric and Mental Health Congress, San Diego, Mishkin, M. (1978). Memory in monkeys severely
CA, November 2004. impaired by combined but not by separate
Larrabee, G. J., & Crook, T. H. (1989a). Dimensions removal of amygdale and hippocampus. Nature,
of everyday memory in Age Associated Memory 273, 297–298.
Impairment. Psychological Assessment, 1, 92–97. Mittenberg, W., DiGiulio, D. V., Perrin, S., & Bass, A.
Larrabee, G. J., & Crook, T. H. (1989b). Performance E. (1992). Symptoms following mild head injury:
subtypes of everyday memory function. Develop- expectation as etiology. Journal of Neurology,
mental Neuropsychology, 5, 267–283. Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 55, 200–204.
Larrabee, G. J., & Crook, T. H. (1991). Computerized Mohr, E., Walker, D., Randolph, C., Sampson, M., &
memory testing in clinical trials. In E. Mohr and Mendis, T. (1996). The utility of clinical trial
P. Brouwers (Eds.), Handbook of clinical trials: batteries in the measurement of Alzheimer’s and
The neurobehavioral approach (pp. 293–306). Huntington’s dementia. International Psycho-
Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger. geriatrics, 8, 397–411.
Computer-Based Cognitive Testing 99

Morris, R. G., Evenden, J. L., Sahakian, B. J., & Schweiger, A., Abramovitch, A., Doniger, G. M., &
Robbins, T. W. (1987). Computer-aided assessment Simon, E. S. (2007). A clinical construct valid-
of dementia: comparative studies of neuropsycho- ity study of a novel computerized battery for the
logical deficits in Alzheimer-type dementia and diagnosis of ADHD in young adults. Journal of
Parkinson’s disease. In S. M. Stahl, S. D. Iversen, Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 29,
and T. W. Robbins (Eds.). Cognitive neurochem- 100–111.
istry (pp. 21–36). New York: Oxford University Schweiger, A., Doniger, G. M., Dwolatzky, T., Jaffe, D.,
Press. & Simon, E. S. (2003). Reliability of a novel comput-
Nickelsen, T., Lufk in, E. G., Riggs, B. L., Cox, D. A., erized neuropsychological battery for mild cognitive
& Crook, T. H. (1999). Raloxifene hydrochloride, impairment. Acta Neuropsychologica, 1, 407–413
a selective estrogen receptor modulator: Safety Seltzer, B., Zolnouni, P., Nunez, M., Goldman, R.,
assessment of effects on cognitive function and Kumar, D., Ieni, J., et al. (2004). Efficacy of donepi-
mood in postmenopausal women. Neuroimmu- zil in early stage Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology,
noendocrinology, 24, 115–128. 61, 1852–1856.
Owen, A. M., Downes, J. J., Sahakian, B. J., Polkey, C. E., Simon, E. S., Harel, Y., Appleboim, N., Doniger, G.
& Robbins, T. W. (1990). Planning and spatial work- M., Lavie, M., & Achiron, A. (2006). Validation of
ing memory following frontal lobe lesions in man. the Mindstreams Computerized Cognitive Battery
Neuropsychologia, 28, 1021–1034. in multiple sclerosis. Neurology, 66, A239.
Pietrzak, R. H., Cohen, H., & Snyder, P. J. (2007). Suhr, J. A., & Gunstad, J. (2002). “Diagnosis threat”:
Spatial learning efficiency and error monitoring The effect of negative expectations on cognitive
in normal aging: An investigation using a novel performance in head injury. Journal of Clinical
hidden maze learning test. Archives of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24, 448–457.
Neuropsychology, 22, 235–245. Taylor, J. L., O’Hara, R., Mumenthaler, M. S.,
Powell, D. H., Kaplan, E. F., Whitla, D., Weintraub, S., & Yesavage, J. A. (2000). Relationship of
Catlin, R., & Funkenstein, H. H. (1993). MicroCog. CogScreen-AE to Flight Simulator Performance
Assessment of Cognitive Functioning. Manual. and Pilot Age. Aviation, Space, and Environmental
Orlando, FL: The Psychological Corporation. Medicine, 71, 373–380.
Raymond, P., Hinton-Bayre, A., Radel, M., Ray, M., Tomer, A., Larrabee, G. J., & Crook, T. H. (1994).
& Marsh, N. (2006). Test–rest norms and reli- Structure of everyday memory in adults with Age-
able change indices for the MicroCog battery in Associated Memory Impairment. Psychology and
a healthy community population over 50 years of Aging, 9, 606–615.
age. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 20, 261–270. Wechsler, D. A. (1987). Wechsler Memory Scale
Reeves, D. L., Winter, K. P., Bleiberg, J., & Kane, R. Revised: Manual. San Antonio, TX: The
L. (2007). ANAM® Genogram: Historical perspec- Psychological Corporation.
tives, description, and current endeavors. Archives Wechsler, D. A. (1997). Wechsler Memory Scale;
of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22S, S15–S37. 3rd Edition (WMS-III). San Antonio, TX: The
Rey, A. (1961). L’examen clinique en psychologie. Psychological Corporation.
Paris: Presses Universitaires de France. Wesnes, K. A. (1977). The effects of psychotropic
Ritsner, M. S., Blumenkrantz, H., Dubinsky, T., drugs on human behaviour. Modern Problems of
& Dwolatzky, T. (2006). The detection of neu- Pharmacopsychiatry, 12, 37–58.
rocognitive decline in schizophrenia using the Wesnes, K. A. (2003). The cognitive drug research
Mindstreams Computerized Cognitive Test computerised assessment system: Application
Battery. Schizophrenia Research, 82, 39–49. to clinical trials. In P. De Deyn, E. Thiery, and
Rizzo, A. (2007). T13: Virtual reality in mental health R. D’Hooge (Eds.), Memory: Basic concepts, disor-
and rehabilitation: An overview. HCI International ders and treatment (pp. 453–472).Leuven, Belgium:
22–27 July 2007, Beijing, China. ACCO.
Sahakian B. J. (1990). Computerized assessment of Wesnes, K. A., Simpson, P. M., & Christmas, L.
neuropsychological function in Alzheimer’s dis- (1987). The assessment of human information
ease and Parkinson’s disease. International Journal processing abilities in psychopharmacology. In
of Geriatric Psychiatry, 5, 211–213. I. Hindmarch and P. D. Stonier (Eds.), Human
Sahakian, B. J., Morris, R. G., Evenden, J. L., Heald, psychopharmacology: Measures and Methods, 1,
A., Levy, R., Philpot, M. P., et al. (1988). A compar- 79–92.
ative study of visuospatial memory and learning Wesnes, K. A., Ward, T., McGinty, A., & Petrini, O.
in Alzheimer-type dementia and Parkinson’s dis- (2000). The memory enhancing effects of a Ginko
ease. Brain, 111, 695–718. biloba/Panax ginseng combination in healthy
100 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

middle-aged volunteers. Psychopharmacology, Youngjohn, J. R., Larrabee, G. J., & Crook, T. H.


152, 353–361. (1991). First–last names and the grocery list selec-
West, R. L., & Crook, T. H. (1990). Age differences in tive reminding tests: Two computerized measures
everyday memory: Laboratory analogues of tele- of everyday verbal learning. Archives of Clinical
phone number recall. Psychology and Aging, 5, Neuropsychology, 6, 287–300.
520–529. Youngjohn, J. R., Larrabee, G. J., & Crook, T. H.
West, R. L., Crook, T. H., & Barron, K. L. (1992). Every- (1992). Test–retest reliability of computerized,
day memory performance across the life span: The everyday memory measures and traditional
effects of age and noncognitive individual differ- memory tests. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 6,
ence factors. Psychology and Aging, 7, 72–82. 276–286.
6

Cognitive Screening Methods


Maura Mitrushina

Screening for Cognitive Timely diagnosis of neuropsychiatric condi-


Impairment in Medical Settings tions yields the following benefits:

Why is Screening Important? 1. Providing indication for treatment of


reversible conditions;
Changes in cognitive status are associated with 2. Preventing a relapse of cognitive impa-
many medical conditions, including respiratory, irment through the use of prophylactic measures;
cardiovascular, and infectious diseases, as well 3. Slowing down a progression of cognitive
as autoimmune, renal, liver, kidney, pancre- deterioration or achieving improvement through
atic, and thyroid dysfunction, diabetes mellitus, the use of medications that stabilize cognitive
other metabolic and systemic diseases, nutri- functioning;
tional deficiencies, and toxic exposure (Boswell 4. Educating the patient and the family
et al., 2002; Demakis et al., 2000; Elias, 1998; regarding changes in cognition and behavior
Lal, 2007; Lezak et al., 2004; Murkin, 2005; that lead to reported changes in daily funct-
Ruchinskas et al., 2000; Salik et al., 2007; Sartori ioning;
et al., 2006; Tarter et al., 2001; Van den Berg 5. Allowing the patient and the family to
et al., 2006). Cognition might also be adversely make legal decisions early in the course of the
affected by surgical interventions for cardiovas- disease and to plan for future care.
cular diseases, radiation and chemotherapy for
cancer, and medications that are given to con- Cognitive impairment is commonly viewed
trol physical symptoms. Furthermore, cognitive in the context of dementia. The definition of
impairment is a common sequelae of diseases dementia captures deterioration of cognitive/
directly affecting the central nervous system. intellectual abilities leading to impairment in
Degenerative diseases of the cerebral cortex, as social and occupational functioning, and in
well as conditions compromising integrity of activities of daily life (American Psychiatric
subcortical structures of the brain, commonly Association, 1980; McKhann et al., 1984). Over
result in notable cognitive deterioration. the years, understanding of clinical profi les
Considering the high incidence of cognitive associated with dementia has been refined.
impairment in general medical and neurolog- Whereas in the past the diagnosis of dementia
ical conditions, timely detection of cognitive required impairment in memory (as in the case
decline may provide a clue to the presence of of Alzheimer’s disease), it is now believed that
an underlying physical disorder, as well as to memory impairment may not be prominent
its course and prognosis, and may determine a in several dementing conditions that involve
choice of treatment or medication adjustment. severe deficits in other cognitive domains

101
102 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

(e.g., vascular, frontotemporal, and Lewy body screening negative, and offering educational
dementias). Furthermore, a more recent clinical opportunities addressing health maintenance
entity, mild cognitive impairment (MCI), cap- issues (Lawrence et al., 2001).
tures less pronounced symptoms of cognitive
deterioration that do not interfere considerably
Is There an Ideal Screening Test?
with activities of daily life (Gauthier et al., 2006;
Petersen & O’Brien, 2006). Neuropsychological test batteries are specifi-
As in the case of dementia, MCI can be clas- cally designed to assess cognitive strengths and
sified into amnestic and nonamnestic sub- weaknesses across different functional domains.
types, depending on the presence of memory This information is often used in designing
impairment. cognitive remediation programs and monitor-
In geriatric settings, timely detection of insidi- ing effectiveness of treatment. However, use of
ous dementia is critical for secondary prevention neuropsychological test batteries for cognitive
(Ganguli, 1997). Whereas primary prevention of screening is not feasible due to their high cost,
dementia is not currently available, early identi- length, need for specially trained examiners,
fication of those individuals who show signs of and poor acceptance by the patients. Lengthy
cognitive impairment allows early diagnosis and test batteries are not well tolerated by medical
treatment, enhancement of functional ability, patients due to their short attention span, fati-
prevention of complications, community moni- gability, and, frequently, physical pain and dis-
toring, and planning of public health services. comfort. Furthermore, it is not practical to use
Early detection of cognitive impairment, neuropsychological test batteries in assessment
including dementia and MCI, is facilitated by of large populations of elderly individuals who
cognitive screening. The goals of cognitive are at risk for dementia.
screening can be classified into two categories: Moreover, a precise measurement of the func-
tional level within different cognitive domains
1. To establish the likelihood of global cog- is unnecessary for most medical patients and
nitive deterioration in individuals at risk and individuals at risk; the presence versus absence
assess the need for further diagnostic/neuropsy- of global cognitive impairment and the domains
chological workup. Screening is typically per- most severely impaired are of main concern.
formed by a primary care physician in response This task can be accomplished through the use
to complaints of cognitive symptoms reported of brief screening measures, which in a lim-
by the patient or other informant. Other risk ited amount of time allow the examiner to tap
factors for cognitive deterioration include age, important aspects of cognitive abilities.
significant medical problems, intake of multiple Many investigators agree that an ideal screen-
medications, and family history of dementia. In ing measure should be brief, well tolerated and
rehabilitation setting, screening is performed to acceptable to patients, easy to administer and
identify cognitive strengths and weaknesses to score, free of demographic biases, sensitive to
aid in rehabilitation efforts, and to monitor the the presence of cognitive dysfunction, and able
rates of recovery (Ruchinskas & Curyto, 2003). to assess a wide range of cognitive functions,
2. To identify individuals in the community to identify domains that are in need of further
who would benefit from diagnostic evaluation assessment, and to track cognitive changes over
but who have not yet spontaneously sought time to make them useful in planning for treat-
medical attention for cognitive symptoms. This ment and discharge in rehabilitation setting
type of a voluntary screening is typically per- (Doninger et al., 2006; Ruchinskas & Curyto,
formed by a trained paraprofessional. Other 2003; Shulman, 2000; Shulman & Feinstein,
benefits of voluntary screening programs are 2003).
freedom from ethical and practical concerns There is no single screening tool that would
expressed by physicians in respect to rou- meet all of the above criteria. The choice of a
tine screening of elderly patients, increasing screening test depends on the context and the
awareness of the community about issues of population being screened. Overall, the use
aging, providing reassurance to those who are of brief screening tests as screening tools for
Cognitive Screening Methods 103

detection of cognitive impairment is well justified rates, are generally evaluated against a “gold
in medical settings. The clinical value of screen- standard,” such as psychiatric or neurological
ing tests is reflected in their many strengths: diagnosis. In spite of the extensive clinical
workup that underlies diagnoses made for
1. Such measures are brief and nondemand- research purposes, such diagnoses, themselves,
ing for the patient, and can be easily adminis- have low reliability.
tered at the bedside. 4. Psychometric properties of a test derived
2. They show little practice effect. on a sample that is representative of a certain
3. They can be administered by appropriately population of patients cannot be generalized
trained paraprofessionals and do not require onto a different population as the test’s accuracy
much formal training for their interpretation. in identifying cognitive impairment is affected
4. They originate from traditional clinical by the prevalence of the condition and other
evaluation of mental status, are convenient, and properties of the population (see below for fur-
deal with constructs familiar to physicians. ther discussion).
5. They utilize a structured interview format, 5. Different studies use different standards
which provides uniformity in administration for diagnostic determination, which makes
and scoring. comparison of their results misleading. For
6. The test results are quantified, which facil- example, Erkinjuntti et al. (1997) found that the
itates decision processes, and allows compari- number of subjects classified by a test as having
son across time and among different clinicians. dementia can differ by a factor of 10 across six
diagnostic systems used in their study.
In spite of these assets of cognitive screening 6. A comparison of psychometric properties
tests, a consideration should be given to their of screening tests across studies is further con-
limitations: founded by the use of different cutoffs for cog-
nitive impairment by different authors. Lorentz
1. They produce high false-negative rates et al. (2002) advocate the use of the area under
because most of the questions making up receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve
screening tests can be answered by a major- to overcome this problem.
ity of patients, even if they have mild cogni- 7. Performance on the cognitive screening
tive impairment. Focal brain dysfunctions, instruments is frequently confounded by emo-
especially related to the nondominant cerebral tional factors. Cognitive dysfunction may lead
hemisphere, are likely to be missed because to depression and anxiety, whereas affective dis-
cognitive screening measures tend to be con- turbance is likely to cause or exacerbate cogni-
structed mainly of verbal items. In addition, tive deficits.
reliance on a global estimate of cognitive func- 8. Cognitive screening instruments do not
tioning makes the detection of isolated deficits distinguish between acute and chronic cogni-
less likely. Another source of false-negative tive dysfunction.
identifications is high premorbid intellectual 9. Cognitive disturbance associated with
and educational levels of the patient. many neuropsychiatric diseases has a waxing
2. False-positive errors arise primarily from and waning course. Any one evaluation would
confounding effects of age and ethnic back- not provide sufficient information on the course
ground, low premorbid intelligence, low edu- of cognitive changes, and serial evaluations
cation and illiteracy, and poor knowledge of might be warranted.
English. Several investigators have attempted to
control for these factors by stratifying norma- Performance of screening tests is also affected
tive data or using different cutoffs for different by the following sources of bias (Gifford &
demographic groups. Another source of false- Cummings, 1999):
positive errors is limited cooperation and moti-
vation, as well as sensory impairment. 1. Spectrum bias reflects variability in
3. Estimates of psychometric properties, accuracy of detecting cognitive disturbance
reported in validation studies as classification depending on the severity of the condition.
104 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

Consequently, screening tests that have high of a stable construct such as a cognitive ability
sensitivity in the populations with more severe (“true” variance). However, a certain degree of
disease manifestations do not perform well with variability is inherent in test performance due
less advanced stages of the disease. to transient factors associated with the testing
2. Workup (verification) bias affects accu- situation and the patient’s state at the time of
racy of the results of many validity studies testing (“error” variance). Reliability of a test
due to preferential referral for the diagnostic refers to the proportion of “true” variance to
workup of those patients who have positive “error” variance. In respect to different sources
test results. Since patients with negative results of error, the four most common methods of reli-
do not receive further evaluation, the calcula- ability measurements are described below:
tions of sensitivity–specificity of the screening
test are not accurate. Failure to include true- 1. Test–retest reliability measures the sta-
negative/false-negative rates into the analysis bility of test scores over time. It is presented
leads to increased sensitivity and decreased as a correlation between test scores at different
specificity. times. The length of time interval between the
3. Review bias refers to biased interpretation test probes should be considered in evaluating
of the test results, leading to inflated accuracy of this statistic.
the screening test, when both the screening test 2. Alternate form reliability assesses the cor-
and diagnostic assessment are performed by the relation between scores obtained by the same
same person. subjects on the alternate forms of a test.
3. Interrater reliability refers to the rate of
In addition to these empirically defined test agreement (correlation) in test scores, or in rat-
properties, the choice of a screening test should ings on individual items, when obtained by dif-
be based on evaluation of its statistically derived ferent examiners.
psychometric properties in respect to the popu- 4. Internal consistency reliability reflects the
lation being screened. extent to which all the test items measure the
same underlying construct and is expressed
as a degree of relationship between different
Review of Psychometric
test items. It is commonly measured with the
Properties of Screening Tests
Cronbach’s coefficient alpha, which represents
Accuracy and utility of cognitive screening the average of all correlations between each
examinations with different populations have item and all other test items.
been reported in numerous validation studies,
which differ in design, scoring, and presenta- Ideally, a highly reliable test would be pre-
tion of research findings. In order to facilitate ferred to a test with low reliability. However,
the reader’s understanding of the results of selection of a screening test should weigh
different studies described in this chapter, we heavily on considerations of practicality. Tests
include a brief review of psychometric proper- with lower reliability might be acceptable since
ties of tests. the cost of error in screening situations is lower
than in diagnostic decision making. For screen-
ing tests, reliability between 0.80 and 0.60 would
Reliability
be acceptable. Reliability estimates below 0.60
When using a screening test, we assume that are usually judged as unacceptably low as more
the test scores would be consistent over repeated than 40% of the variability in the test scores is
administrations, different raters, and different due to measurement error.
forms of the same test. If an individual some-
times receives high scores and sometimes low
Validity
scores on the same test, no inferences can be
made regarding the level of ability that is mea- When a test is used to assess cognitive status, it is
sured by this test. In other words, we have to assumed that the test measures what it is supposed
be assured that the test is a reliable measure to measure and that the test is useful in making
Cognitive Screening Methods 105

accurate decisions. Different validation strategies impaired or unimpaired as determined by the


are used to understand the meaning and implica- results of the screening test, judged against the
tions of the scores achieved on the test. Content reference criterion in a preselected sample, is
and construct validity indicate whether a test is addressed by the decision theory approach.
a valid measure of cognitive status. Criterion- According to the decision theory, all patients
related validity refers to the accuracy of decisions are classified into cognitively intact (negative) or
that are based on the test scores. impaired (positive) groups, based on their test
scores. Similar distinction is made according to
1. Content validity refers to the extent to the criterion (clinical diagnosis). Comparison
which the content of the questions adequately between these two classifications provides infor-
taps different aspects of mental status. It is not mation on the number of correctly identified
measured statistically, but is inferred in many and misidentified patients on the basis of their
studies from clinical relevance of the test and test scores. The relative number of cases in each
high internal consistency of the test items. cell representing true (T) or false (F) and nega-
Content validity is established if a test looks like tive (N) or positive (P) outcomes (i.e., TP, TN,
a valid measure (Murphy & Davidshofer, 1991). FP, FN) yields several indices of the test validity.
2. Construct validity points to agreement Sensitivity refers to the ability of a test to cor-
between different tests measuring the same rectly identify individuals who have cognitive
construct. The accuracy of the assumption that impairment—the ratio of “true positives” to all
cognitive screening tests actually measure the impaired patients (true positives/[true positives
underlying hypothetical construct of the cogni- + false negatives]). Specificity indicates the
tive component of mental status is established ability of the test to correctly identify absence of
by documenting high correlations of a particu- cognitive impairment—the ratio of “true nega-
lar test with other tests presumably measuring tives” to all intact patients (true negatives/[true
the same construct (convergent validity) versus negatives + false positives]). These characteris-
low correlations with tests that are expected tics of the test vary, depending on the “cutoff ”
to measure different constructs—for example, points for classification into negative and pos-
affective state (discriminant validity). Construct itive groups, which have been selected by an
validity is established if a test acts like a valid investigator on the basis of experience.
measure (Murphy & Davidshofer, 1991). Manipulation of the cutoffs affects the bal-
3. Criterion-related validity reflects the rela- ance between sensitivity and specificity and,
tionship between scores on the test and a ref- therefore, produces different cost–benefit ratios.
erence criterion, or “gold standard,” that is For example, if the cutoff is set so that only
assumed to represent a “true state” of a patient. patients making a very large number of errors
Reference criteria vary among different studies are considered impaired, only those patients
and include results of a neuropsychological eval- with pronounced cognitive impairment would
uation, judgment of a clinician, discharge dis- be identified. This would result in high speci-
position, staff ratings, management problems, ficity and a small number of “false positives.”
self-care capacity, response to treatment, and However, many mild cases would be missed,
duration of illness, as well as neuroimaging, resulting in low sensitivity and a high number
EEG, neuropathological, and lab findings. Most of “false negatives.” Failure to detect cognitive
studies use clinical diagnosis by a psychiatrist impairment precludes timely therapeutic inter-
or neurologist as a criterion. vention that otherwise might allow stabilization
or reversal of cognitive symptoms.
The criterion measures can be obtained after Fixing the cutoff at a small number of errors
decisions are made, based on the test scores in a allows the clinician to correctly identify a
random sample of the population about which majority of individuals with even mild signs
the decisions were made (predictive validity), or at of cognitive impairment, which ensures high
the same time when decisions are made in a prese- sensitivity of the test and reduces the num-
lected sample (concurrent validity). Accuracy of ber of “false negatives.” However, this strategy
decisions in finding individuals to be cognitively increases the proportion of intact individuals
106 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

who are identified as “positives,” thus lowering look discouraging, they should be interpreted
the specificity of the test and providing a high in the context of other measures that contribute
number of “false positives.” Costs of such an to the accuracy of decisions.
outcome include inappropriate treatment and The utility of a test is reflected in its incre-
psychological distress as well as adverse social mental validity—the degree of improvement
and economic consequences on the part of in the accuracy of decisions, that is, frequency
intact individuals who are mistakenly identified of TP and TN outcomes, beyond the random
as cognitively impaired. level, that are made using the test. The contri-
Therefore, the cutoff should often be set at bution of the criterion-related validity of a test
values that ensure a reasonable balance between to improvement in the accuracy of decisions
sensitivity and specificity, so that only “border- depends on the base rate and selection ratio.
line” patients will likely be incorrectly classi- The base rate reflects the proportion of an
fied. Under certain circumstances, however, one unselected population who meet the criterion
would prefer to maximize sensitivity even at the standard. Clinically, this term is used inter-
expense of specificity, and vice versa. Setting changeably with incidence or prevalence of a
the optimal cutoff yields the highest Hit Rate, disorder. In the general population, the base
that is, ability of the test to correctly identify rates for cognitive impairment are usually
presence and absence of impairment (expressed low and most of the “red flags” represent false
as the ratio of [TP + TN] to all individuals in alarms. The base rates among elderly, or those
the sample [TP + FP + FN + TN]). individuals who are referred for evaluation due
The trade-off between sensitivity and speci- to progressive symptomatology, are higher, and
ficity (more precisely, between true-positive and therefore the expected number of false alarms
false-positive rates of classification) for all possible would be lower.
cutoff scores is displayed by the ROC curve plot. The selection ratio is defined as the ratio of
The area under the ROC curve represents most TP + FP outcomes to the total number of sub-
useful index of diagnostic accuracy (Swets, 1996). jects. When the selection ratio is low, a test with
The score associated with the largest amount of even modest validity can make a considerable
area under the curve is the most sensitive cutting contribution to the accuracy of decisions.
score. Use of this approach allows one to compute Thus, incremental validity of a test is highest
probability of impairment for a certain test score, when the base rate is moderate, selection ratio is
taking into account different prevalence rates. low, and the criterion-related validity is high.
Usefulness of screening tests, as reported in In clinical practice, a clinician is concerned
different studies, is described by several statis- with the utility of a test in correctly identify-
tics. Criterion-related validity for most of the ing impairment in an individual patient, that
screening tests is relatively low, ranging between is, in the test’s predictive value, rather than in
0.2 and 0.5, due to the imperfect reliability of its accuracy in discriminating between groups.
the test and the reference criterion. Whereas a Positive Predictive Value represents the proba-
criterion is assumed to represent the “true state” bility that the patient is indeed impaired, given
of a patient, it is frequently based on subjective impaired test score (expressed as the ratio of
clinical judgment, which is inherently unre- TP to all individuals identified by the test as
liable (see discussion of limitations of screen- impaired [TP + FP]). Negative Predictive Value
ing tests in the previous section). To help the represents the probability that the patient is
reader understand the strength of the relation- intact, given nonimpaired test score (expressed
ship expressed by the correlation coefficient, we as the ratio of TN to all individuals identi-
will use an example. If a correlation coefficient fied by the test as nonimpaired [FN + TN]).
between a test and a criterion is 0.3, the pro- Because predictive value is a probabilistic con-
portion of the variance in the criterion that is struct relating the number of correctly classified
accounted for by the test (r2, coefficient of deter- individuals to the total number of individuals
mination) is 0.09. This means that only 9% of falling into a corresponding classification cat-
the variability in the criterion can be accounted egory, it is affected by the prevalence of the
for by the test scores. Although these numbers disturbance in the population.
Cognitive Screening Methods 107

Thus, accuracy and usefulness of a cognitive was later validated on a broad range of diagno-
screening test in detecting cognitive impairment ses. Comprehensive reviews of the history and
depends on the properties of the test and charac- utility of the MMSE are provided by Anthony
teristics of a population in which it is being used. et al. (1982), Harvan and Cotter (2006), Mossello
The following review provides descriptions and and Boncinelli (2006), Tombaugh (2005), and
results of validation studies for selected tests. Tombaugh and McIntyre (1992). Findings from
selected validation studies for the MMSE are
summarized in Table 6–2.
Description of Selected Screening
The original version of the MMSE com-
Tests for Cognitive Impairment
prises 11 items that have been derived from
The need for structured measures of mental the mental status examination proposed by
status was recognized in the beginning of the Meyer (1918) and yields a total score of 30 (see
twentieth century. Adolf Meyer (1918) intro- Table 6–1). Psychometric properties of the test
duced a uniform method of evaluation of men- have been extensively investigated with differ-
tal status, which required extensive narrative ent samples. A review of the literature indicates
descriptions of the patient’s behavior. Toward that performance on the MMSE is influenced
mid-twentieth century, numeric scales that by demographic factors, such as age (Almeida,
quantify emotional and cognitive aspects of 1998; Crum et al., 1993; Harvan & Cotter,
mental status were developed. Over time, many 2006; Magni et al., 1995; Strickland et al., 2005;
brief screening measures of cognition were Tangalos et al., 1996; Tombaugh et al., 1996; Van
introduced in response to a well-recognized Gorp et al., 1999), education (Almeida, 1998;
need for early detection of cognitive deteriora- Bertolucci et al., 1994; Borson et al., 2005; Crum
tion. A recent review of the literature by Cullen et al., 1993; Ganguli et al., 1995; Harvan & Cotter,
et al. (2007) identified 39 screening tests for cog- 2006; Lourenco & Veras, 2006; Magni et al.,
nitive impairment that met stringent selection 1995; Mungas et al., 1996; Murden et al., 1991;
criteria, including administration time of less Ostrosky-Solis et al., 2000; Rosselli et al., 2006;
than 20 minutes and availability in English. Strickland et al., 2005; Tangalos et al., 1996;
Practice guidelines developed by the Tombaugh et al., 1996; van Gorp et al., 1999),
American Academy of Neurology, following an ethnicity and language of test administration
evidence-based review of the literature, include (Mungas et al., 1996; Ramirez et al., 2001), intel-
recommendations for the use of cognitive screen- ligence level (MacKenzie et al., 1996), and phys-
ing instruments for detection of mild cognitive ical condition (Eslinger et al., 2003; MacKenzie
impairment and dementia in individuals with et al., 1996).
suspected cognitive impairment (Petersen et al., Similarly, in a large population-based study
2001). sponsored by National Institute of Mental
The screening tests summarized in Table 6–1 Health (Crum et al., 1993), the MMSE scores for
represent selected measures that are most 18,056 adult participants varied as a function
commonly used with medical and geriatric of age and education level. The median scores
patients. As statistical properties of tests vary for different age groups ranged from 29 (18–24
depending on the characteristics of a sample, it years) to 25 (>80 years), and for education
is not feasible within the scope of this chapter to groups, from 22 (0–4 years) to 26 (5–8 years)
provide statistics from different validation stud- to 29 (>9 years). In a study by Ostrosky-Solis
ies for all tests reviewed in Table 6–1. A review et al. (2000), normal Spanish-speaking illiterate
of selected validation studies is presented for participants obtained scores that correspond to
Mini-Mental State Examination and Cognistat. severe cognitive alterations, and those with low
education (1–4 years) scored within the range
consistent with moderate cognitive alterations.
Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE)
Authors concluded that the MMSE has little
and Related Measures
diagnostic utility in individuals with low edu-
The MMSE (Folstein et al., 1975) was originally cational level. The effect of demographics on
designed for use with psychiatric patients, but MMSE performance was also documented by
Table 6–1. Descriptions of Cognitive Screening Tests (Presented in the Order of Increasing Administration
Time)
Test/Authors, Functions Assessed Primary Use Test Properties
Administration Time
General Cognitive Screening Instruments
Mini-Cog Includes Clock Drawing Screening for Scores of 0–3 on CDT and
Borson et al. (2000) Test and 3-item recall task cognitive on recall task (score of
[3–4 minutes] from CASI impairment in 3—severe impairment);
primary care free of education and
settings language bias; used
with multiethnic
populations; simple
administration;
sensitive to MCI
Memory Impairment Four-item delayed and cued Screening for Total score is the sum of
Screen (MIS) recall dementia items passed on cued
Buschke et al. (1999) recall plus doubled
[4 minutes] number of items passed
on free recall, with
range from 0 to 8; test
has equivalent forms
General Practitioner Two parts: 9 cognitive Screening for Maximum score—9; if the
Assessment of Cognition questions (time cognitive score falls between 5
(GPCOG) orientation, clock impairment in and 8, informant-rated
Brodaty et al. (2002) drawing, recall of recent primary care items should be
[4–5 minutes] event, word recall task); settings administered; biased
and 6 informant-rated by sociodemographic
items factors, sensitive
to MCI
Abbreviated Mental Test Orientation, attention, Screening for 10-item test derived
(AMT) recent and remote dementia in from BIMCT; cutoff
Hodkinson (1972) memory, information primary care 7/8; developed
[5 minutes] in Great Britain;
culture-specific;
limited validity
Kokmen Short Test of Orientation, attention, Screening for mild Score—sum of the subtest
Mental Status (STMS) learning, calculation, dementia scores; maximum
Kokmen et al. (1987) similarities, information, score—38
[5 minutes] construction, delayed
recall
Blessed Orientation- Orientation, concentration, Geriatric patients Score—sum of errors;
Memory-Concentration immediate and delayed maximum score—28
Test (OMC) memory on 6 items; cutoff >10
(Short form of BIMC) errors
Katzman et al. (1983)
[3–6 minutes]
(used as the basis for
Halifax Mental Status
Scale, HMSS, Fisk et al.,
1991)
Seven-Minute Screen (7MS) Benton Temporal Screening for AD Provides indicator of
Solomon et al. (1998) Orientation, clock draw- in the primary probability of AD:
[7 minutes] ing, category fluency, care setting high, low, retest;
enhanced cued recall administration
requires considerable
training; requires use
of a handheld computer
to assess likelihood of
dementia
(continued)

108
Table 6–1. Continued
Test/Authors, Functions Assessed Primary Use Test Properties
Administration Time
Mini-Mental Status Orientation, concentration, Broad range of Score—sum of the correct
Examination (MMSE) serial 7’s, immediate and medical and responses; maximum
Folstein et al. (1975) delayed verbal memory, psychiatric score—30 on 11 items
[5–10 minutes] language, 3-step praxis, diagnoses
copy of geometric design
Mental Status Orientation, general and Institutionalized Score—sum of errors;
Questionnaire (MSQ) personal information geriatric patients 10 items derived from
Kahn et al. (1960) a 31-item version;
[5–10 minutes] maximum score—10
Short Portable Mental Orientation, long-term Detection of Score—sum of errors
Status Questionnaire recall, current event “organic brain with correction for race
(SPMSQ) information, serial 3’s syndrome” in and education; 10 items
Pfeiffer (1975) geriatric patients
[5–10 minutes]
Modified Mini-Mental Same as MMSE plus recall Broad range of Expands range of MMSE
Status Examination of date and place of medical and scores by allowing
(3MS) birth, animal naming, psychiatric for partial credit;
Teng and Chui (1987) similarities, additional diagnoses maximum score—100
[10 minutes] recall task on 15 items
Short and Sweet Screening Includes MMSE, verbal Community Consists of three cognitive
Instrument (SASSI) fluency, and temporal screening for tests stati stically
Belle et al. (2000) orientation tests dementia derived from a larger
[10 minutes] battery in MoVIES
community study
Cognitive Capacity Orientation, concentration, Detection of diff use Score—sum of the correct
Screening Exam (CCSE) serial 7’s, immediate and organic men- responses; maximum
Jacobs et al. (1977) delayed verbal memory, tal syndrome/ score—30 on 30 items;
[5–15 minutes] language, verbal concept delirium in cutoff <20 correct; as
formation, digit span, medical patients; part of CMC is used
arithmetic sensitive to cog- in combination with
nitive decline in MMSE
high-functioning
patients

Blessed Information- Orientation, concentration, Geriatric patients Score—sum of errors;


Memory-Concentration immediate and delayed maximum score—33
Test (BIMC) memory, and unique on 28 items;
(Fuld’s modification) items cutoff >7 errors
Fuld (1978)
[10–20 minutes]
Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Orientation/attention, Screening for Extension of MMSE;
Examination-Revised memory, verbal fluency, dementia and different patterns for
(ACE-R) language, visuospatial mild cognitive types of dementia;
Mioshi et al. (2006) perception/organization impairment, Score—sum of
[12–20 minutes] differentiates subscores on 5 scales;
between AD and maximum score—100
FTD profi les
Cognitive Abilities Attention, concentration, Broad range of Based on items
Screening Instrument orientation, short-term medical and from MMSE and
(CASI) and long-term memory, psychiatric 3MS, adapted for
Teng et al. (1994) language, visual diagnoses cross-cultural use;
[15–20 minutes] construction, verbal most individual
fluency, abstraction, item scores 0–10;
judgment maximum Score—100
on 25 items

(continued)

109
Table 6–1. Continued
Test/Authors, Functions Assessed Primary Use Test Properties
Administration Time

Cognistat, formerly LOC, orientation, atten- Medical, geriatric, Uses screen and metric
Neurobehavioral tion, comprehension, and neurological approach; allows to plot
Cognitive Status repetition, naming, patients a profi le for 10 domains
Examination (NCSE) construction, memory,
Kiernan et al. (1987) calculation, similarities,
[5–30 minutes] judgment
Repeatable Battery for Immediate and delayed Designed as a Provides five indexes, as
the Assessment of memory, visuospa- screening tool well as a total cognitive
Neuropsychological tial ability, language, to identify and impairment score; sen-
Status (RBANS) attention characterize sitive to mild demen-
Randolph et al. (1998) dementia in tia; provides distinct
[20–30 minutes] the elderly; has profi les for different
been used in types of dementia; has
schizophrenia, two alternate forms,
TBI, multiple allowing to track
sclerosis, change over time; high
stroke, affective test–retest reliabil-
disorders ity; standardized on
U.S. Census-matched
population between
20 and 89 years of age;
normative data are
stratified by demo-
graphic groups
High Sensitivity Cognitive Memory, language, atten- Screening for Scoring is based on
Screen (HSCS) tion, concentration, subtle cognitive interpretive algorithm,
Fogel (1991) visual and motor skills, decline in geriat- classifying each item
[20–30 minutes] spatial perception, self- ric and commu- into normal, border-
regulation, planning nity-dwelling line, abnormal range
HIV-infected (latter is rated as mild,
individuals moderate, or severe)
Cambridge Orientation, Elderly 105 items; criterion for
CognitiveExamination- comprehension, naming, patients with impairment—score
Revised (CAMCOG-R) verbal fluency, short-term neurological <80; maximum “total
Roth et al. (1999) and long-term memory, disorders, executive function
[25–30 minutes] attention, calculation, for example, score”—28; available
praxis, similarities, visual Parkinson’s for cross-national use
perception (Original disease,
CAMCOG); revised post-CVA
version also includes
ideational fluency and
visual reasoning
Mattis Dementia Rating Attention, initiation/ Screening for Score—sum of scores
Scale (DRS, MDRS) perseveration, dementia on five subscales;
Mattis (1988) visuospatial construc- 36 items; maximum
[20–45 minutes] tion, reasoning, memory score—144; uses
screen and metric
approach; allows test–
retest comparisons;
test adaptations used
cross-culturally;
minimal culture effect
(continued)

110
Table 6–1. Continued
Test/Authors, Functions Assessed Primary Use Test Properties
Administration Time
Brief Focused Screening Instruments
Clock Drawing Test (CDT) Visuoperceptual, Screening for Many administration
Goodglass and Kaplan constructional, dementia and scoring systems
(1983) conceptual exist; quantitative
[<1–5 minutes] scores represent total
ratings for clockface,
placement of the
hands and numbers;
qualitative scores
are based on the
analysis of error
types; limited utility
as a single screening
instrument; used in
combination with
other tests
Time and Change Test Telling time from a pre- Screening for Unaffected by race and
(T&C) set clock, and making dementia education; limited
Froehlich et al. (1998) change for a dollar using scope of assessed
[1 minute] coins domains
Informant-Based Instruments
Blessed Dementia Rating Caregiver ratings of behav- Screening for Most items rated present/
Scale (BDRS, BRS, BDS) ioral, personality, and dementia absent, three items
Blessed et al. (1968) emotional changes in the by severity level; 22
preceding six months; items; maximum
frequently used in com- score—28, highest
bination with BIMC or severity level, cutoff for
OMC impairment >4
Informant Questionnaire Informant ratings of cog- Screening for 26 items are rated
on Cognitive Decline in nitive decline over the dementia for on a 5-point scale;
the Elderly (IQCODE) previous 10 years individuals who measures a single
Jorm (2004) are unable to factor of cognitive
undergo direct decline; unaffected by
testing, or those education, premorbid
with low levels ability, language
of education and proficiency; affected
literacy by informant’s
depression, anxiety,
and quality of
relationship; coupled
with cognitive tests,
improves screening
accuracy; several short
forms developed;
translated into many
languages
Deterioration Cognitive Informant ratings of Screening for 19 items rated on a
Observee (DECO) changes in activity level, dementia 3-point Likert scale
Ritchie and Fuhrer long-term and short-term ranging from 0 to 2;
(1996) memory, visuospatial maximum score 38,
processing and new indicating no change;
skill learning over the cutoff at 30/29
preceding year
(continued)

111
112 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

Table 6–1. Continued


Test/Authors, Functions Assessed Primary Use Test Properties
Administration Time
Telephone Screening
Telephone Interview for Orientation, recall, calcula- Screening for 11 questions, 17 items;
Cognitive Status (TICS) tion, information, repe- dementia in maximum score—41;
Brandt et al. (1988) tition, verbal reasoning, epidemiological cutoff score < 27;
(TICS-m) (modified) ability to follow simple studies; follow-up correlates highly with
Welsh et al. (1993) commands (TICS-m assessment by MMSE in AD
Computer-assisted includes delayed recall telephone after
Version of TICS-m item) face-to-face
Buckwalter et al. (2002) assessment with
MMSE
Notes: LOC—level of consciousness; AD—Alzheimer’s disease; FTD—frontotemporal dementia; CMC—combined
screening test incorporating MMSE and CCSE; MCI—mild cognitive impairment; TBI—traumatic brain injury;
CVA—cerebrovascular accident.

Boustani et al. (2003). These findings suggest a sample of older Brazilian patients. Bertolucci
that demographic factors should be taken into et al. (1994) suggested a cutoff of 13 for illiterate
account in interpretation of test scores. individuals, 18 for those with less than 8 years
Traditionally, decisions regarding the pres- of education, and 26 for those with 8 or more
ence of cognitive impairment or determination years, based on a sample of Brazilian adults.
of severity ranges are guided by evaluating the The utility of the MMSE in tracking cogni-
patient’s total MMSE score in respect to cut- tive changes over time is supported by findings
off points for impairment. A score of <23 was of minimal practice effects. Tombaugh (2005)
originally proposed as the cutoff for cognitive reported small group changes over 3-month
impairment. Kukull et al. (1994) found that the and 5-year test–retest intervals (as measured
cutoff of <24 has low sensitivity (63%) yet high with a reliable change index) in a large sample
specificity (96%) relative to a one-year follow-up of neurologically intact elderly. In raw scores,
diagnosis in a sample of outpatients with cogni- changes in group means were less than one
tive complaints. Van Gorp et al. (1999) reported point over different test–retest intervals, with
a score of <26 as the optimal cutoff for demen- somewhat greater rate of change in those over
tia based on their sample of community-dwell- the age of 75. In Eslinger et al.’s (2003) large
ing elders. Tombaugh and McIntyre (1992) sample of healthy older men, 80% of partici-
expanded the use of cutoff points by dividing the pants remained within two points of their ini-
scale into ranges of 18–23 and <17, as indicative tial test scores on a 6-year follow-up testing.
of mild and severe impairment, respectively. Tangalos et al. (1996) also emphasized stabil-
However, the uniform criteria for impairment ity of test scores over time and suggested that
might be misleading when used with different a decline of at least 4 points in the total MMSE
demographic groups. Revised cutoff points for score over a period of 1–4 years represents sub-
different education levels have been reported in stantial cognitive deterioration.
a number of studies. Lourenco and Veras (2006) The original test underwent a number of
found the best MMSE performance in detec- modifications in efforts to adapt it to the needs
tion of dementia at a cutoff of 18/19 for illit- of different populations. Mayeux et al. (1981)
erate and 24/25 for literate individuals, based developed a modified version of the MMSE that
on a sample of elderly Brazilian outpatients. includes Digit Span (forward and backwards),
Almeida (1998) proposed a cutoff of 19/20 for recall of general information, confrontation
individuals with no formal education and 23/24 naming of 10 items from the Boston Naming
for those with a history of schooling, based on Test, an additional sentence for repetition, and
Table 6–2. Findings from Selected Validation Studies for the Mini-Mental State Examination and Cognistat
Test /Source Sample/Setting Findings
Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE)
Strickland et al. (2005) 93 healthy African American older Normative data stratified by age and
adults education are provided
Xu et al. (2002) 351 elderly volunteers with memory Sensitivity of 61% in identifying MCI,
complaints using conversion to dementia at
follow-up as a standard
Ostrosky-Solis et al. 430 intact Spanish-speaking elderly; Sensitivity and specificity of the
(2000) The sample was stratified into 3 age MMSE in both illiterate and 1–4
and 4 education groups years of education groups—50%
and 72.7%; in >5 years of education
group—86.4% and 86.4%
Van Gorp et al. (1999) 22 AD, 19 VaD patients, 12 normal Discriminatory power was maximized
elderly with a cutoff of ≤26, in reference to
the NINCDS/ADRDA and DSM-
III-R diagnostic criteria for the AD
and VaD, respectively
Tombaugh et al. (1996) Community-dwelling elderly: 3MS and MMSE scores (derived from
406 with no cognitive impairment; the 3MS) were compared as to their
119 with AD accuracy in discriminating between
the two groups; The consensus
diagnosis by physician and clinical
neuropsychologist was used as a
reference for AD; psychometric
properties of both tests for different
age and education groups, as
well as normative data stratified
by 2 age × 2 education levels are
provided
Mungas et al. (1996) 590 elderly participants from a Statistically derived adjusted MMSE
population-based community survey score, correcting for age and edu-
sample; 46.6% Hispanics, 53.4% cation is presented; Ethnicity and
non-Hispanics language of the test administration
strongly influenced the raw MMSE
score
Tangalos et al. (1996) 3513 elderly patients; Subsample of 185 Cutoff score of 23 yielded 69% sen-
patients with dementia and 227 sitivity and 99% specificity; use of
age- and sex-matched controls age- and education-specific cutoff
scores improved sensitivity to 82%
with no loss of specificity; With
typical base rate for dementia in
general medical practice, the PPV
< 35%; Determination of substan-
tial cognitive deterioration over a
period of 1–4 years requires at least
4-point decline in the total MMSE
score
MacKenzie et al. (1996) 150 community-dwelling elderly Effect of confounding variables (age,
social class, hearing acuity, history
of stroke, etc.) was assessed; MMSE
yielded fewer false positives than
other screening tests (sensitivity/
specificity of 80%/98%). A cutoff of
20/21 is recommended for routine
screening
(continued)

113
Table 6–2. Continued
Test /Source Sample/Setting Findings
Roper et al. (1996) 59 geriatric medical inpatients with Highest diagnostic accuracy (70%) was
diagnosed brain dysfunction, 46 achieved at a cutoff score of 26
without brain dysfunction
Cognistat (NCSE)
Doninger et al. (2006) 120 TBI inpatients 296 Rating scale analysis (using Rasch
community-residing adults with TBI analysis) was used on the origi-
nal test and modified version by
eliminating the easiest items. Three
performance strata were identified
in both acute and postacute samples
Strickland et al. (2005) 93 healthy African American older Normative data, stratified by age
adults and education, are provided for
individual subtests
Schrimsher et al. (2005) 202 African American and 110 Performance of two ethnic groups on
Caucasian males admitted to a individual subtests and failure rate
VA-based residential substance on screening items are compared
use treatment program
Drane et al. (2003) 108 healthy older adults Used modified version of the
Cognistat; Screen items were not
administered; Composite score
was introduced; Normative data
stratified by age and education are
provided; Use of a uniform cutpoint
for the classification of impairment
is not recommended
Eisenstein et al. (2002) 134 healthy elderly Normative data for the subtest perfor-
mance for ages 60–85 are provided
Ruchinskas et al. (2001) 86 urban geriatric rehabilitation Composite and subtest scores were
patients with medical and used; Rate of impaired scores for
neurological conditions the composite cutoff of 63 were
compared for medical and neu-
rologic groups
Wallace et al. (2000) 48 severe TBI patients >1 year Used impaired score on one scale
postinjury in postacute rehabilitation as a cutoff for impairment;
Kappa—0.45, sensitivity—92%,
specificity—22% in respect to
results of NP evaluation
Van Gorp et al. (1999) 22 AD, 19 VaD patients, 12 normal Discriminatory power was maximized
elderly with a criterion for impairment
being a score on at least 1 scale
falling in the impaired range, in ref-
erence to the NINCDS/ADRDA and
DSM-III-R diagnostic criteria for
the AD and VaD, respectively
Fladby et al. (1999) 50 psychogeriatric patients with Sensitivity—81%, specificity—60% in
late-onset psychiatric disorder respect to the diagnosis of organic
brain disorder made by a physician
Cammermeyer and 804 patients from 20 diagnostic groups Cognistat profi les for different
Prendergast (1997) diagnostic categories are illustarted,
based on a multicenter database
accumulated from seven medical
facilities
(continued)

114
Cognitive Screening Methods 115

Table 6–2. Continued


Test /Source Sample/Setting Findings
Fals-Stewart (1997) 51 detoxified substance- abusing In reference to the Neuropsychological
patients Screening Battery performance,
Cognistat demonstrated low
sensitivity (64% false-negative
rate), but high specificity (14%
false-positive rate); The authors do
not recommend use of Cognistat
in screening substance-abusing
patients
Roper et al. (1996) 59 geriatric medical inpatients with Highest diagnostic accuracy (69%)
diagnosed brain dysfunction, was achieved at a cutoff of 2
46 without brain dysfunction impaired scales
Notes: AD—Alzheimer’s disease; VaD—vascular dementia; NINCDS/ADRDA—National Institute of Neurological and
Communicative Disorders and Stroke/Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association; DSM-III-R—Diagnostic and
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Th ird Edition, Revised; MCI—Mild Cognitive Impairment.

copying of two designs. The maximum score is item substitutions for administration in special
57, and the cutoff is <25. Administration of this circumstances. The normative data (in T-scores),
version requires 30–45 minutes, but the inclu- stratified by age and education level, are pro-
sion of additional items improves the validity of vided in addition to recommended cutoff scores.
the test. The printed materials are supplemented with
Teng and Chui (1987) introduced the software for scoring, interpretation, and report
Modified Mini-Mental State Examination generation.
(3MS), which includes four added items, uti- The MMSE is frequently used in combi-
lizes a more graded scoring scale leading to an nation with other screening tests. Belle et al.
extended range of scores (0–100), and incor- (2000) introduced Short and Sweet Screening
porates other minor changes (see Table 6–1). It Instrument (SASSI), which combines MMSE
retains the brevity but improves the sensitivity with verbal fluency and temporal orientation
of certain items. tests (see Table 6–1). Xu et al. (2002) combined
Subsequent modification, Cognitive Abilities MMSE with Cognitive Capacity Screening
Screening Instrument (CASI), developed by Exam (CCSE) (Jacobs et al., 1977; see Table 6–1),
Teng et al. (1994), incorporates MMSE and which resulted in improved sensitivity of the test
3MS, as an adaptation for cross-cultural use (see to mild cognitive impairment as a prodrome for
Table 6–1). It retains the scale of 0–100, which is dementia in nondemented elderly. Improved per-
based on the scores on 25 items. formance of a combined test was also reported
A telephone version of the MMSE was devel- by Ferrucci et al. (1996) and Harvan and Cotter
oped by Roccaforte et al. (1992) for use with (2006), who used MMSE in combination with
geriatric patients. Another version adminis- the Clock Drawing Test (CDT) (Goodglass &
tered over the telephone, Telephone Interview Kaplan, 1983; see Table 6–1).
for Cognitive Status-Modified (TICS-m) (King Based on a review of the literature on the
et al., 2006; Welsh et al., 1993), is based on accuracy of screening methods in evaluation of
the MMSE, and is typically used in follow-up dementia, Harvan and Cotter (2006) concluded
assessments to track changes after initial MMSE that the MMSE has high sensitivity and spec-
administration (see Table 6–1). ificity in outpatients older than 65 years when
The standard MMSE, with minor subse- adjustments for age and education are made.
quent modifications made by the authors, was Norms stratified by age and education in samples
recently published by Psychological Assessment from different countries have been shown to
Resources. The published materials are available improve diagnostic accuracy (Grigoletto et al.,
in a variety of languages and include alternative 1999). Furthermore, sensitivity of the MMSE
116 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

depends on the degree of impairment, and In spite of an adequate validity of the overall
was found to be satisfactory with moderate to Cognistat performance, several authors argued
severe levels (Moore et al., 2004; Tombaugh & against the use of the scale profi le compari-
McIntyre, 1992). Tangalos et al. (1996) and son as an indication of cognitive strengths and
Tombaugh and McIntyre (1992) concluded that weaknesses, due to significant measurement
the MMSE is not appropriate for routine screen- errors (Doninger et al., 2000, 2006; Karzmark,
ing of all elderly, but should be used in popula- 1997; Marcotte et al., 1997; Wallace et al., 2000).
tions with high base rates (>10%) of cognitive Furthermore, the scale profi le was not useful
impairment. in distinguishing between Alzheimer’s dis-
ease and vascular dementia samples (Van Gorp
et al., 1999), right- and left-hemisphere stroke,
Cognistat (Neurobehavioral Cognitive
nor cortical versus subcortical stroke (Osmon
Status Examination, NCSE)
et al., 1992).
The Cognistat (Kiernan et al., 1987) provides A comparison of psychometric properties of
a profi le of cognitive status across 10 domains the Cognistat across different validity studies is
(see Mueller et al., 1988). Most sections, except confounded by a lack of agreement on what con-
for Memory and Orientation, use the screen stitutes the best criterion for cognitive impair-
and metric approach. They begin with a ment. According to the test authors, the criterion
screening item that is rather demanding (pro- is met when at least one subtest score falls in the
ducing about 20% failure rate in the normal impaired range (Mueller et al., 1988). This crite-
population). If the examinee passes the screen, rion is consistent with findings by Van Gorp et al.
the particular skill represented by the item is (1999). However, Osato et al. (1993) reported a
considered to be intact, and the examiner pro- cutoff of two subtest scores falling in the impaired
ceeds to assess another skill. If the screening range as most sensitive to differences in cognitive
item is failed, questions of graded difficulty are profiles between depressed patients and patients
administered to assess the level of competence with organic mental disorders, which is in agree-
in this particular skill. Performance within ment with Roper et al.’s (1996) report.
each domain is scored independently, placing The accuracy of the screen and metric
the score into intact versus mildly, moderately, approach was questioned by Drane and Osato
or severely impaired level. The results are pre- (1997), Marcotte et al. (1997), Oehlert et al.
sented in the form of a graph. Administration (1997), and Schrimsher et al. (2005), as screen
time varies between 5 minutes for cognitively results are frequently inconsistent with perfor-
intact individuals and 30 minutes for patients mance on metric items. Several authors recom-
with cognitive impairment. Statistical prop- mended that all metric items be administered
erties of this test with medical and psychiat- regardless of performance on the screening
ric populations and in healthy samples are items. Drane et al. (2003) abandoned the screen
described in a number of articles. Results of and metric approach and developed a compos-
selected validation studies are summarized in ite score that represents the sum of scores on all
Table 6–2. metric items across all scales (out of 82). This
The Cognistat was found to have adequate approach is consistent with the unitary factor
psychometric properties in geriatric, psychiat- underlying Cognistat scales, derived in factor-
ric, medical, and traumatic brain injury/stroke analytic studies by Engelhart et al. (1999) and
rehabilitation samples, and was more sensitive Whiteside et al. (1996), possibly representing
than MMSE and Cognitive Capacity Screening attention-based general cognitive function.
Examination in various samples (see Doninger There is also little agreement on the effect
et al., 2006; Nabors et al., 1997; Oehlert et al., of demographic variables on Cognistat perfor-
1997; Ruchinskas et al, 2001). Poon et al. (2005) mance. Whiteside et al. (1996) found that test
reported significant relationship between performance is relatively free of age and edu-
Cognistat performance at initial inpatient reha- cation effects in a sample of medical patients.
bilitation in traumatic brain injury patients and Similarly, Blostein et al. (1997) reported no
clinical outcome one year postinjury. effect of age on test scores in mild traumatic
Cognitive Screening Methods 117

brain injury patients in acute care setting. On and TE4D-Cog (Mahoney et al., 2005), which
the other hand, effect of age was reported by are not included in Table 6–1.
Drane et al. (2003), Macaulay et al. (2003), and Several tests are used with medical and psy-
Strickland et al. (2005) in large samples of the chiatric patients or in primary care settings
healthy elderly. Ruchinskas et al. (2001) found (Mini-Cog, General Practitioner Assessment of
that age correlates with Construction and Cognition, Mini-Mental Status Examination,
Memory subtests of the Cognistat. Effect of 3MS, Cognitive Capacity Screening Exam,
education on the overall performance and sub- Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument,
test scores was reported by Drane et al. (2003), Cognistat, Repeatable Battery for the Assessment
Ruchinskas et al. (2001), and Strickland et al. of Neuropsychological Status). Several tests
(2005). Effect of ethnicity was reported by assess cognitive decline associated with specific
Schrimsher et al. (2005). According to Lampley- medical conditions, such as HIV infection (High
Dallas (2001), further studies investigating the Sensitivity Cognitive Screen, HIV Dementia
effect of ethnicity on Cognistat performance Scale [van Harten et al., 2004], and CogState
are needed. Effect of the medical condition on [Cysique et al., 2006]; the latter two tests are not
test performance was reported by Ruchinskas included in Table 6–1), diff use organic brain
et al. (2001). syndrome/delirium (Short Portable Mental
Status Questionnaire, Cognitive Capacity
Screening Exam), Parkinson’s disease and post-
Other Commonly Used Cognitive
cerebrovascular accident (Cambridge Cognitive
Screening Measures
Examination-Revised), schizophrenia, trau-
The screening tests, summarized in Table 6–1, matic brain injury, multiple sclerosis, stroke,
are grouped into categories, according to the and affective disorders (Repeatable Battery for
setting and information sought. the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status).
1. General cognitive screening instruments are Additionally, Immediate Post-Concussion
based on a face-to-face structured interview Assessment and Cognitive Testing (ImPACT)
with the individual being assessed. A review of (Iverson et al., 2003), a computerized screening
Table 6–1 suggests wide variability in adminis- test, was developed specifically for detection of
tration time, test structure, domains assessed, cognitive changes in athletes with concussions
and outcome measures in these tests. In choos- (not included in Table 6–1).
ing a screening test that is most appropriate in The majority of screening tests provide
a given setting, many factors should be consid- a global measure of cognitive functioning,
ered. Time required for test administration is whereas several tests yield performance pro-
frequently an important determinant (General fi le across different domains (Addenbrook’s
Cognitive Screening Instruments are ordered in Cognitive Examination-Revised, Cognistat,
Table 6–1 in the order of increasing administra- Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of
tion time). Neuropsychological Status). As different neu-
Many screening tests are designed to detect ropathological conditions differentially affect
dementia in geriatric populations (or specifically cognitive functioning, information about the
screen for Alzheimer’s disease, as in the case of pattern of impairment helps identify subtypes
Seven Minute Screen). Only a few tests are repor- of dementia. Specifically, Addenbrook’s Cogni-
ted to be sensitive to mild cognitive impairment tive Examination-Revised capitalizes on assess-
or mild changes in cognition heralding insidi- ment of attention/concentration and executive
ous dementia (Mini-Cog, General Practitioner functioning, which makes it most suitable for
Assessment of Cognition, Kokmen Short Test distinguishing between Alzheimer’s and fron-
of Mental Status, Addenbrook’s Cognitive totemporal dementia profi les.
Examination-Revised, Repeatable Battery for Whereas the effect of demographic variables
the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status). on test performance commonly confounds
The newest additions to this category are screening results, several tests are reported to
DemTect (Kalbe et al., 2004), Montreal Cognitive be useful in assessment of multiethnic popula-
Assessment (MoCA) (Nasreddine et al., 2005), tions, as they are free of education and language
118 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

bias (Mini-Cog) or adapted for cross-cultural tests improve screening accuracy, as they reflect
use (Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument, longitudinal change, and bring into the picture
Cambridge Cognitive Examination-Revised, everyday cognitive abilities.
Mattis Dementia Rating Scale). Several tests 4. Telephone screening instruments have been
have normative data stratified by demographic developed for use in the context of large epi-
groups or corrected for demographic factors demiological studies and for a follow-up after
(Short Portable Mental Status Questionnaire, face-to-face screening. Telephone Interview for
Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Cognitive Status has been shown to improve
Neuropsychological Status). screening accuracy when used in conjunc-
Two tests utilize a screen and metric approach tion with other tests of cognitive functioning.
that reduces assessment time for intact indi- Other screening interviews administered over
viduals while allowing thorough assessment of the phone have been developed (e.g., TELE
those who demonstrate impairment (Cognistat, [Jarvenpaa et al., 2002], and Minnesota Cognitive
Mattis Dementia Rating Scale). Acuity Screen, MCAS [Knopman et al., 2000],
Availability of equivalent forms for use at both are not included in Table 6–1).
retest facilitates longitudinal follow-up (Memory 5. A recently introduced category uses direct
Impairment Screen, Repeatable Battery for observation of patient’s ability to perform cer-
the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status, tain task related to activities of daily living,
Mattis Dementia Rating Scale). such as ability to learn and use a hypothetical
The majority of cognitive screening tests have daily medication regimen, as assessed with the
been intended for use in primary or second- Medication Management Test (Gurland et al.,
ary care settings. In contrast, Short and Sweet 1995; not included in Table 6–1).
Screening Instrument is specifically developed The interested reader is referred for further
for community screening. information on the utility of cognitive screening
2. Brief focused screening instruments require tests to comprehensive reviews by Boustani et al.
little time and assess ability that is thought to (2003), Brodaty et al. (1998, 2006), Copersino
be representative of general cognitive status. In et al. (2003), Cullen et al. (2007), Demakis et al.
addition to Clock Drawing Test and Time and (2000), Heun et al. (1998), Lorentz et al. (2002),
Change Test, included in Table 6–1, popular Malloy et al. (1997), Meng (2004), Petersen et al.
neuropsychological tests such as Trail-Making (2001), Serper and Allen (2002), and Shulman
Test, Verbal Fluency, Similarities, and Hopkins et al. (2006).
Verbal Learning Test (see Lezak et al., 2004) are
used to detect global cognitive deterioration.
Summary
However, considering the limited scope of such
assessments, these tests should not be adminis- Timely detection of cognitive impairment and
tered in isolation, as this would likely result in the choice of appropriate treatment are facili-
false-negative misidentifications. tated by cognitive screening of those individuals
3. Informant-based screening instruments who suffer from medical and neuropsychiatric
are designed to obtain pertinent information conditions that are known to affect cognition
about changes in behavior of an individual or who fall in the “at risk” category for cogni-
being evaluated from a person who knows him/ tive dysfunction. Screening does not lead to a
her well. Blessed Dementia Rating Scale and diagnosis but is useful in assessing the need for
General Practitioner Assessment of Cognition further diagnostic/neuropsychological workup.
include informant-rated component. Informant It is performed in two settings: (1) by a pri-
Questionnaire on Cognitive Decline in the mary care physician in response to complaints
Elderly and Deterioration Cognitive Observe of cognitive symptoms and (2) by a clinician or
provide informant ratings of cognitive decline trained paraprofessional in order to identify
and changes in activity level over the previous individuals in the community who would ben-
10 years and 1 year, respectively. When used in efit from a diagnostic evaluation but who have
conjunction with cognitive assessment, these not yet sought medical attention for cognitive
Cognitive Screening Methods 119

symptoms. Whereas advancing age is a known off between sensitivity and specificity can be
risk factor for cognitive changes, there is little achieved through adjusting cutoff scores relative
support in the literature for screening all elderly to the demographic characteristics of the pop-
in a general population. Screening is most use- ulation being screened, and through the use of
ful in the populations with the base rates for the area under the receiver operating charac-
cognitive impairment exceeding 10% (e.g., resi- teristic curve in determining the most sensitive
dents of assisted care facilities). cutoff, given the prevalence rate of the condition.
A selection of an assessment instrument Incremental validity of a test is highest when the
should be based on its practicality. The use of prevalence (base rate) of the condition in the pop-
cognitive screening tests as tools for the detection ulation is moderate, selection ratio is low, and the
of cognitive impairment in medical patients and criterion-related validity of the test is high.
in large populations of individuals at risk is well What are the implications of the above dis-
justified by their ease in administration, famil- cussion for the individual clinician? Screening
iarity to the clinician, acceptance by the patient, for cognitive impairment in primary care set-
little practice effect, structured format allowing ting is indicated in cases of self- or informant-
uniformity in administration and scoring, and reported complaints of cognitive decline and in
quantified presentation of the results. those individuals who are “at risk” for cognitive
The chosen screening instrument should also impairment (e.g., secondary to a medical con-
have good psychometric properties in identi- dition). The choice of the instruments would
fying cognitive impairment in a specific pop- depend on the patient population. As the pat-
ulation. Typical reliability of a screening test, tern of cognitive impairment varies depending
ranging between .80 and .60, is relatively low. on the nature of the underlying dysfunction,
However, it is generally acceptable out of con- the selection of a screening instrument should
siderations of practicality as the cost of error in be guided by the usefulness of an individual test
screening situations is lower than in diagnostic in detecting deficits in those cognitive domains
decision making. Criterion-related validity for that are most vulnerable to decline in those
most of the screening tests is also relatively low, medical conditions that are seen in a given
ranging between .2 and .5, due to the imperfect clinic. As follows from the above review, cer-
reliability of the test and the reference criterion. tain screening tests have been identified as most
Low validity of the test leads to misclassification sensitive to cognitive impairments secondary to
of “borderline” patients, whereas intact or mod- specific conditions, ranging from different types
erately/severely impaired individuals are likely of dementia (those primarily affecting memory
to be correctly classified. vs. executive abilities) to HIV infection.
False-negative misclassifications are com- Because the predictive value of the instru-
mon in assessment of mild cognitive impair- ments depends on the prevalence of the distur-
ment, deficits produced by localized brain bance in a given population, norms should be
dysfunction, and in patients with high educa- collected locally (within each clinic using the
tional/intellectual level. This type of errors is of test), based on the performance of patients who
concern to a clinician, as reliance on the screen- are “typical” for this particular population. The
ing results might deprive the patient of further distribution of patients’ scores on the screen-
diagnostic assessment. False-positive misclassi- ing tests should be validated against the neu-
fications are commonly produced by low educa- ropsychological test results for these patients
tion/literacy levels, and other demographic and or against other reliable criteria. Separate sets
performance factors. Aside from economic and of norms should be developed with respect to
emotional considerations, this type of errors different age, education, literacy levels, and race
is of a lesser concern to the clinician, because groups. This would allow the clinician to adjust
the assumption of cognitive impairment can be the cutoff scores to achieve a reasonable balance
ruled out in the course of a subsequent workup. between sensitivity and specificity.
The best balance of false-positive/false- In addition to statistical adjustment of the
negative misclassifications and optimal trade- cutoffs for cognitive impairment, decisions
120 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

regarding the need for a diagnostic workup, References


especially for patients falling within the “bor-
derline” category, should take into account the Almeida, O. P. (1998). Mini-Mental State Examination
patient’s behavior and functional level, history, and the diagnosis of dementia in Brazil. Archives
of Neuropsychiatry, 56(3B), 605–612.
self-report of cognitive functioning, results of
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic
functional disability scales, structured direct and statistical manual of mental disorders, 3rd
observations of the patient’s efficiency in per- edition (DSM-III). Washington, DC: American
forming daily tasks, and informants’ percep- Psychiatric Association Press.
tions of the patient’s cognitive functioning as Anthony, J. C., LeResche, L., Niaz, U., von Korff, M.
well as recent changes in functional capacity. R., & Folstein, M. F. (1982). Limits of the “Mini-
Incremental validity of screening assessments Mental State” as a screening test for dementia and
might be further improved by supplementing a delirium among hospital patients. Psychological
standard screening examination with additional Medicine, 12(2), 397–408.
measures, particularly with tests tapping non- Belle, S. H., Mendelsohn, A. B., Seaberg, E. C.,
verbal abilities (e.g., Clock Drawing Test, CDT). & Ratcliff, G. (2000). A brief cognitive screen-
ing battery for dementia in the community.
Moreover, a combination of cognitive screen-
Neuroepidemiology, 19(1), 43–50.
ing with a structured informant report (e.g., Bertolucci, P. H., Brucki, S. M., Campacci, S. R., & Juliano,
Informant Questionnaire on Cognitive Decline Y. (1994). The Mini-Mental State Examination in a
in the Elderly, IQCODE) improves screening general population: Impact of educational status.
accuracy as it brings into consideration longitu- Archives of Neuropsychiatry, 52(1), 1–7.
dinal changes and cognitive efficiency in daily Blessed, G., Tomlinson, B. E., & Roth, M. (1968).
functioning. The association between quantitative measures
In many clinical settings, it is important to of dementia and of senile change in the cerebral
track progression of cognitive symptoms over grey matter of elderly subjects. British Journal of
time to follow-up on “borderline” changes in Psychiatry, 114(512), 797–811.
cognition, to assess the rate of deterioration in Blostein, P. A., Jones, S. J., Buechler, C. M., &
Vandongen, S. (1997). Cognitive screening in
progressive conditions or improvement in treat-
mild traumatic brain injuries: Analysis of the neu-
ment, and to plan for treatment and discharge robehavioral cognitive status examination when
in rehabilitation setting. Furthermore, serial utilized during initial trauma hospitalization.
evaluations improve diagnostic accuracy in Journal of Neurotrauma, 14(3), 171–177.
view of a fluctuating course of cognitive distur- Borson, S., Scanlan, J., Brush, M., Vitaliano, P., &
bance associated with many medical and neu- Dokmak, A. (2000). The Mini-Cog: A cognitive
ropsychiatric conditions. Whereas cognitive “vital signs” measure for dementia screening in
screening measures generate little practice effect, multi-lingual elderly. International Journal of
those tests that have equivalent forms would Geriatric Psychiatry, 15(11), 1021–1027.
be preferred in situations in which a retest is Borson, S., Scanlan, J. M., Watanabe, J., Tu, S. P., &
intended. Other strategies for follow-up assess- Lessig, M. (2005). Simplifying detection of cog-
nitive impairment: Comparison of the Mini-Cog
ments should also be considered. For example,
and Mini-Mental State Examination in a multi-
a Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status- ethnic sample. Journal of the American Geriatrics
Revised (TICS-R) has been shown to improve Society, 53(5), 871–874.
screening accuracy as a follow-up assessment Boswell, E. B., Anfinson, T. J., & Nemeroff, C. B.
after a face-to-face administration of the Mini- (2002). Neuropsychiatric aspects of endocrine dis-
Mental Status Examination (MMSE). orders. In S. C. Yudofsky and R. E. Hales (Eds.),
Textbook of neuropsychiatry and clinical neuro-
sciences (4th ed., pp. 851–875). Washington, DC:
Acknowledgment American Psychiatric Publishing.
Boustani, M., Peterson, B., Hanson, L., Harris, R.,
Dr. Mitrushina thanks Dr. Paula Altman Fuld & Lohr, K. (2003). Screening for dementia in pri-
for her coauthorship on the earlier version of mary care: A summary of the evidence for the U.S.
this chapter that was published in the second Preventive Services Task Force. Annals of Internal
edition of this book. Medicine, 138(11), 927–942.
Cognitive Screening Methods 121

Brandt, J., Spencer, M., & Folstein, M. (1988). Doninger, N. A., Bode, R. K., Heinemann, A. W., &
The Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status. Ambrose, C. (2000). Rating scale analysis of the
Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, & Behavioral neurobehavioral Cognitive Status Examination.
Neurology, 1(2), 111–117. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 15(1),
Brodaty, H., Clarke, J., Ganguli, M., Grek, A., Jorm, 683–695.
A. F., Khachaturian, Z., et al. (1998). Screening for Doninger, N. A., Ehde, D. M., Bode, R. K., Knight, K.,
cognitive impairment in general practice: Toward Bombardier, C. H., & Heinemann, A. W. (2006).
a consensus. Alzheimer Disease & Associated Measurement properties of the neurobehavioral
Disorders, 12(1), 1–13. cognitive status examination (cognistat) in trau-
Brodaty, H., Lee-Fay, L., Gibson, L., & Burns, K. matic brain injury rehabilitation. Rehabilitation
(2006). What is the best dementia screening instru- Psychology, 51(4), 281–288.
ment for general practitioners to use? American Drane, D. L., & Osato, S. S. (1997). Using the
Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 14(5), 391–400. Neurobehavioral Cognitive Status Examination
Brodaty, H., Pond, D., Kemp, N. M., Luscombe, G., as a screening measure for older adults. Archives of
Harding, L., Berman, K., et al. (2002). The GPCOG: Clinical Neuropsychology, 12(2), 139–143.
A new screening test for dementia designed for Drane, D. L., Yuspeh, R. L., Huthwaite, J. S., Klingler,
general practice. Journal of the American Geriatrics L. K., Foster, L. M., Mrazik, M., et al. (2003).
Society, 50(3), 530–534. Healthy older adult performance on modified
Buckwalter, J. G., Crooks, V. C., & Petitti, D. B. (2002). version of the Cognistat (NCSE): Demographic
A preliminary psychometric analysis of a computer- issues and preliminary normative data. Journal of
assisted administration of the telephone interview Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25(1),
of cognitive status-modified. Journal of Clinical and 133–144.
Experimental Neuropsychology, 24(2), 168–175. Eisenstein, N., Engelhart, C. I., Johnson, V., Wolf,
Buschke, H., Kuslansky, G., Katz, M., Stewart, J., Williamson, J., & Losonczy, M. B. (2002).
W. F., Sliwinski, M. J., Eckholdt, H. M., et al. Normative data for healthy elderly persons with
(1999). Screening for dementia with the Memory the Neurobehavioral Cognitive Status Exam
Impairment Screen. Neurology, 52(2), 231–238. (Cognistat). Applied Neuropsychology, 9(2), 110–113.
Cammermeyer, M., & Prendergast, V. (1997). Profi les Elias, M. (1998). Effects of chronic hypertension on
of cognitive functioning in subjects with neuro- cognitive functioning. Geriatrics, 53(Suppl 1),
logical disorders. Journal of Neuroscience Nursing, S49–S52.
29(3), 163–169. Engelhart, C. I., Eisenstein, N., Johnson, V., Wolf,
Copersino, M. L., Serper, M., & Allen, M. H. (2003). J., Williamson, J., Steitz, D., et al. (1999). Factor
Rapid screening for cognitive impairment in the structure of the Neurobehavioral Cognitive Status
psychiatric emergency service: II. A flexible test Exam (COGNISTAT) in healthy, and psychiatri-
strategy. Psychiatric Services, 54(3), 314–316. cally and neurologically impaired, elderly adults.
Crum, R. M., Anthony, J. C., Bassett, S. S., & Folstein, Clinical Neuropsychologist, 13(1), 109–111.
M. F. (1993). Population-based norms for the Mini- Erkinjuntti, T., Ostbye, T., Steenhuis, R., & Hachinski,
Mental State Examination by age and educational V. (1997). The effect of different diagnostic crite-
level. Journal of the American Medical Association, ria on the prevalence of dementia. New England
269(18), 2386–2391. Journal of Medicine, 337(23), 1667–1674.
Cullen, B., O’Neill, B., Evans, J. J., Coen, R. F., & Eslinger, P. J., Swan, G. E., & Carmelli, D. (2003).
Lawlor, B. A. (2007). A review of screening tests Changes in the Mini-Mental State Exam in
for cognitive impairment. Journal of Neurology, community-dwelling older persons over 6 years:
Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 78(8), 790–799. Relationships to health and neuropsychological
Cysique, L. A., Maruff, P., Darby, D., & Brew, B. J. measures. Neuroepidemiology, 22(1), 23–30.
(2006). The assessment of cognitive function in Fals-Stewart, W. (1997). Detection of neuropsycho-
advanced HIV-1 infection and AIDS dementia logical impairment among substance-abusing
complex using a new computerised cognitive patients: Accuracy of the Neurobehavioral
test battery. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, Cognitive Status Examination. Experimental and
21(2), 185–194. Clinical Psychopharmacology, 5(3), 269–276.
Demakis, G. J., Mercury, M. G., & Sweet, J. J. (2000). Ferrucci, L., Cecchi, F., Guralnik, J. M., & Giampaoli,
Screening for cognitive impairments in primary S. (1996). Does the clock drawing test predict cog-
care settings. In M.E. Maruish (Ed.). Handbook of nitive decline in older persons independent of the
Psychological Assessment in Primary Care Settings Mini-Mental State Examination? Journal of the
(pp. 555–582). London: Lawrence Erlbaum. American Geriatrics Society, 44(11), 1326–1331.
122 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

Fisk, J. D., Braha, R., & Walker, A. (1991). The Halifax Journal of the American Academy of Nurse
Mental Status Scale: Development of a new test Practitioners, 18(8), 351–360.
of mental status for use with elderly clients. Heun, R., Papassotiropoulos, A., & Jennssen, F.
Psychological Assessment, 3(2), 162–167. (1998). The validity of psychometric instruments
Fladby, T., Schuster, M., Gronli, O., Sjoholm, H., for detection of dementia in the elderly general
Loseth, S., & Sexton, H. (1999). Organic brain population. International Journal of Geriatric
disease in psychogeriatric patients: Impact of Psychiatry, 13(6), 368–380.
symptoms and screening methods on the diag- Hodkinson, H. M. (1972). Evaluation of a mental test
nostic process. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and score for assessment of mental impairment in the
Neurology, 12(1), 16–20. elderly. Age and Aging, 1, 233.
Fogel, B. S. (1991). The high sensitivity cognitive Iverson, G. L., Lovell, M. R., & Collins, M. W. (2003).
screen. International Psychogeriatrics. Special Interpreting change on ImPACT following sport
Issue: Delirium Advances in Research and Clinical concussion. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 17(4),
Practice, 3(2), 273–288. 460–467.
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). Jacobs, J. W., Bernhard, M. R., Delgado, A., & Strain,
Mini-mental state: A practical method for grad- J. J. (1977). Screening for organic mental syn-
ing the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. dromes in the medically ill. Annals of Internal
Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189–198. Medicine, 86, 40–46.
Froehlich, T. E., Robison, J. T., & Inouye, S. K. (1998). Jarvenpaa, T., Rinne, J. O., Raiha, I., Koskenvuo,
Screening for dementia in the outpatient setting: M., Lopponen, M., Hinkka, S., et al. (2002).
The Time and Change test. Journal of the American Characteristics of two telephone screens for cogni-
Geriatrics Society, 46(12), 1506–1511. tive impairment. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive
Fuld, P. A. (1978). Psychological testing in the differ- Disorders, 13(3), 149–155.
ential diagnosis of the dementias. In R. Katzman, Jorm, A. F. (2004). The Informant Questionnaire
R. D. Terry, and K. L. Bock (Eds.). Alzheimer’s on Cognitive Decline in the Elderly (IQCODE):
Disease: Senile Dementia and Related Disorders A review. International Psychogeriatrics, 16(3),
(Vol. 7; pp. 185–193). New York: Raven Press. 275–293.
Ganguli, M. (1997). The use of screening instruments Kahn, R. L., Goldfarb, A. I., & Pollack, M. (1960).
for the detection of dementia. Neuroepidemiology, Brief objective measures for the determination
16(6), 271–280. of mental status in the aged. American Journal of
Ganguli, M., Ratcliff, G., Chandra, V., Sharma, S., Psychiatry, 117, 326.
Gilby, J., Pandav, R., et al. (1995). A Hindi ver- Kalbe, E., Kessler, J., Calabrese, P., Smith, R.,
sion of the MMSE: The development of a cognitive Passmore, A. P., Brand, M., et al. (2004). DemTect:
screening instrument for a largely illiterate rural A new, sensitive cognitive screening test to support
elderly population in India. International Journal the diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment and
of Geriatric Psychiatry, 10(5), 367–377. early dementia. International Journal of Geriatric
Gauthier, S., Reisberg, B., Zaudig, M., Petersen, R. C., Psychiatry, 19(2), 136–143.
Ritchie, K., Broich, K., et al. (2006). Mild Cognitive Karzmark, P. (1997). Operating characteristics of the
Impairment. Lancet, 367, 1262–1270. Neurobehavioral Cognitive Status Exam using
Gifford, D. R., & Cummings, J. L. (1999). Evaluating neuropsychological assessment as the criterion.
dementia screening tests: Methodologic standards to Assessment, 4(1), 1–8.
rate their performance. Neurology, 52(2), 225–227. Katzman, R., Brown, T., Fuld, P., Peck, A., Schlechter,
Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (1983). The assess- R., & Schimmel, H. (1983). Validation of a short
ment of aphasia and related disorders (2nd ed.). Orientation-Memory-Concentration Test of cog-
Philadelphia: Lea and Febiger. nitive impairment. American Journal of Psychiatry,
Grigoletto, F., Zappala, G., Anderson, D. W., & 140, 734–739.
Lebowitz, B. D. (1999). Norms for the Mini-Mental Kiernan, R. J., Mueller, J., Langston, J. W., & Van
State Examination in a healthy population. Dyke, C. (1987). The Neurobehavioral Cognitive
Neurology, 53(2), 315–320. Status Examination: A brief but differentiated
Gurland, B. J., Wilder, D. E., Chen, J., Lantigua, R., approach to cognitive assessment. Annals of
Mayeux, R., & Van Nostrand, J. (1995). A flexible Internal Medicine, 107, 481–485.
system of detection for Alzheimer’s disease and King, J. T., DiLuna, M. L., Cicchetti, D. V., Tsevat,
related dementias. Aging, 7, 165–172. J., & Roberts, M. S. (2006). Cognitive function-
Harvan, J. R., & Cotter, V. (2006). An evaluation of ing in patients with cerebral aneurysms mea-
dementia screening in the primary care setting. sured with the Mini-Mental State Examination
Cognitive Screening Methods 123

and the Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status. Mahoney, R., Johnston, K., Katona, C., Maxmin, K.,
Neurosurgery, 59(4), 803–810. & Livingston, G. (2005). The TE4D-Cog: a new test
Knopman, D. S., Knudson, D., Yoes, M. E., & Weiss, for detecting early dementia in English-speaking
D. J. (2000). Development and standardization populations. International Journal of Geriatric
of a new telephonic cognitive screening test: The Psychiatry, 20(12), 1172–1179.
Minnesota Cognitive Acuity Screen (MCAS). Malloy, P. F., Cummings, J. L., Coffey, C. E., Duff y,
Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral J., Fink, M., Lauterbach, E. C., et al. (1997).
Neurology, 13(4), 286–296. Cognitive screening instruments in neuropsychi-
Kokmen, E., Naessens, J. M., & Offord, K. P. (1987). atry: A report of the committee on research of the
A short test of mental status: description and pre- American Neuropsychiatric Association. Journal
liminary results. Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 62, of Neuropsychiatry & Clinical Neurosciences, 9(2),
281–288. 189–197.
Kukull, W. A., Larson, E. B., Teri, L., Bowen, J., Marcotte, T. D., van Gorp, W., Hinkin, C. H., &
McCormick, W., & Pfanschmidt, M. L. (1994). Osato, S. (1997). Concurrent validity of the
The Mini-Mental State Examination score and the Neurobehavioral Cognitive Status Exam sub-
clinical diagnosis of dementia. Journal of Clinical tests. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Epidemiology, 47, 1061–1067. Neuropsychology, 19(3), 386–395.
Lal, B. K. (2007). Cognitive function after carotid Mattis, S. (1988). Dementia Rating Scale: Professional
artery revascularization. Vascular and Endo- manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment
vascular Surgery, 41(1), 5–13. Resources.
Lampley-Dallas, V. T. (2001). Neuropsychological Mayeux, R., Stern, Y., Rosen, J., & Leventhal, J.
screening tests in African Americans. Journal of (1981). Depresssion, intellectual impairment, and
the National Medical Association, 93(9), 323–328. Parkinson disease. Neurology, 31, 645–650.
Lawrence, J., Davidoff, D., Katt-Lloyd, D., Auerbach, McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., Katzman,
M., & Hennen, J. (2001). A pilot program of R., Prizce, D., & Stadlan, E. M. (1984). Clinical
improved methods for community-based screen- diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: Report of the
ing for dementia. American Journal of Geriatric NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices
Psychiatry, 9(3), 205–211. of Department of Health and Human Services
Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., & Loring, D. W. Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology, 34,
(2004). Neuropsychological Assessment (4th ed.). 939–944.
New York: Oxford University Press. Meng, K. (2004). Dementia screening: New develop-
Lorentz, W. J., Scanlan, J. M., & Borson, S. (2002). ments and the significance for diagnosis in gen-
Brief Screening Tests for Dementia. Canadian eral practice. Verhaltenstherapie & Psychosoziale
Journal of Psychiatry, 47(8), 723–733. Praxis, 36(4), 757–765.
Lourenco, R. A., & Veras, R. P. (2006). Mini-Mental Meyer, A. (1918). Outlines of Examinations. New
State Examination: Psychometric characteristics York: Bloomingdale Hospital Press.
in elderly outpatients. Revista de Saude Publica, Mioshi, E., Dawson, K., Mitchell, J., Arnold, R., &
40(4), 712–719. Hodges, J. R. (2006). The Addenbrooke’s Cognitive
Macaulay, C., Battista, M., Lebby, P. C., & Mueller, Examination Revised (ACE-R): A brief cognitive
J. (2003). Geriatric performance on the test battery for dementia screening. International
Neurobehavioral Cognitive Status Examination Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 21(11), 1078–1085.
(Cognistat) what is normal? Archives of Clinical Moore, D. J., Palmer, B. W., & Jeste, D. V. (2004). Use
Neuropsychology, 18(5), 463–471. of the Mini-Mental State Exam in middle-aged and
MacKenzie, D. M., Copp, P., Shaw, R. J., & Goodwin, older outpatients with schizophrenia: Cognitive
G. M. (1996). Brief cognitive screening of the impairment and its associations. American Journal
elderly: A comparison of the Mini-Mental State of Geriatric Psychiatry, 12(4), 412–419.
Examination (MMSE), Abbreviated Mental Test Mossello, E., & Boncinelli, M. (2006). Mini-Mental
(AMT) and Mental Status Questionnaire (MSQ). State Examintation: a 30-year story. Aging: Clinical
Psychological Medicine, 26(2), 427–430. and Experimental Research, 18, 271–273.
Magni, E., Binetti, G., Cappa, S., Bianchetti, A., & Mueller, J., Kiernan, R., & Langston, J. W. (1988).
Trabucchi, M. (1995). Effect of age and educa- The mental status examination. In H. H. Goldman
tion on performance on the Mini-Mental State (Ed.), Review of General Psychiatry, (2nd ed.). Los
Examination in a healthy older population and Altos: Lange Medical Publications.
during the course of Alzheimer’s disease. Journal Mungas, D., Marshall, S. C., Weldon, M., Haan, M.,
of the American Geriatrics Society, 43(8), 942–943. & Reed, B. R. (1996). Age and education correction
124 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

of Mini-Mental State Examination for English- Mild cognitive impairment (an evidence-based
and Spanish-speaking elderly. Neurology, 46(3), review). Neurology, 56(9), 1133–1142.
700–706. Pfeiffer, E. (1975). A short portable mental status
Murden, R. A., McRae, T. D., Kaner, S., & Bucknam, questionnaire for the assessment of organic brain
M. E. (1991). Mini-Mental State Exam scores vary deficit in elderly patients. Journal of the American
with education in Blacks and Whites. Journal of Geriatrics Society, 23, 433–441.
the American Geriatrics Society, 39(2), 149–155. Poon, W. S., Zhu, X. L., Ng, S. C., & Wong, G. K.
Murkin, J. M. (2005). Neurocognitive outcomes: The (2005). Predicting one year clinical outcome in
year in review. Current Opinion in Anesthesiology, traumatic brain injury (TBI) at the beginning of
18(1), 57–62. rehabilitation. Acta Neurochirurgica, Supplement,
Murphy, K. R., & Davidshofer, C. O. (1991). 93, 207–208.
Psychological testing: Principles and applications Ramirez, M., Teresi, J. A., Silver, S., Holmes, D.,
(2nd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice Gurland, B., & Lantigua, R. (2001). Cognitive assess-
Hall. ment among minority elderly: Possible test bias.
Nabors, N. A., Millis, S. R., & Rosenthal, M. (1997). Journal of Mental Health and Aging, 7(1), 91–118.
Use of the Neurobehavioral Cognitive Status Randolph, C., Tierney, M. C., Mohr, E., & Chase,
Examination (Cognistat) in traumatic brain T. N. (1998). The Repeatable Battery for the
injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, Assessment of Neuropsychological Status
12(3), 79–84. (RBANS): Preliminary clinical validity. Journal of
Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Bedirian, V., Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 20(3),
Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., et al. 310–319.
(2005). The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, Ritchie, K., & Fuhrer, R. (1996). The validation of an
MoCA: A brief screening tool for mild cognitive informant screening test for irreversible cognitive
impairment. Journal of the American Geriatrics decline in the elderly: Performance characteristics
Society, 53, 695–699. within a general population sample. International
Oehlert, M. E., Hass, S. D., Freeman, M. R., Williams, Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 11(2), 149–156.
M. D., Ryan, J. J., & Sumerall, S. W. (1997). The Roccaforte, W. H., Burke, W. J., Bayer, B. L., &
Neurobehavioral Cognitive Status Examination: Wengel, S. P. (1992). Validation of a telephone
Accuracy of the “screen-metric” approach in a version of the Mini-Mental State Examination.
clinical sample. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 53, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 40(7),
733–737. 697–702.
Osato, S. S., Yang, J., & La Rue, A. (1993). The Roper, B. L., Bieliauskas, L. A., & Peterson, M.
Neurobehavioral Cognitive Status Examination R. (1996). Validity of the Mini-Mental State
in an older psychiatric population: An exploratory Examination and the neurobehavioral cogni-
study of validity. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, tive status examination in cognitive screening.
& Behavioral Neurology, 6(2), 98–102. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, & Behavioral
Osmon, D. C., Smet, I. C., Winegarden, B., & Neurology, 9(1), 54–57.
Gandhavadi, B. (1992). Neurobehavioral Cognitive Rosselli, M., Tappen, R., Williams, C., & Salvatierra,
Status Examination: Its use with unilateral stroke J. (2006). The relation of education and gen-
patients in a rehabilitation setting. Archives of der on the attention items of the Mini-Mental
Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 73(5), State Examination in Spanish speaking Hispanic
414–418. elders. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 21(7),
Ostrosky-Solis, F., Lopez-Arango, G., & Ardila, A. 677–686.
(2000). Sensitivity and specificity of the Mini- Roth, M., Huppert, F., Mountjoy, C., & Tym, E. (1999).
Mental State Examination in a Spanish-speaking The Cambridge examination for mental disorders
population. Applied Neuropsychology. Special of the elderly—revised. Cambridge: Cambridge
Issue: Assessment of Spanish-Speaking Populations, University Press.
7(1), 24–31. Ruchinskas, R. A., Broshek, D. K., Crews, W. D.,
Petersen, R. C., & O’Brien, J. (2006). Mild Cognitive Jr., Barth, J. T., Francis, J. P., & Robbins, M. K.
Impairment should be considered for DSM-V. (2000). A neuropsychological normative database
Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 19, for lung transplant candidates. Journal of Clinical
147–154. Psychology in Medical Settings, 7(2), 107–112.
Petersen, R. C., Stevens, J. C., Ganguli, M., Tangalos, Ruchinskas, R. A., & Curyto, K. J. (2003). Cognitive
E. G., Cummings, J. L., & DeKosky, S. T. (2001). screening in geriatric rehabilitation. Rehabilitation
Practice parameter: Early detection of dementia: Psychology, 48(1), 14–22.
Cognitive Screening Methods 125

Ruchinskas, R. A., Repetz, N. K., & Singer, H. K. Tarter, R. E., Butters, M., & Beers, S. R. (2001).
(2001). The use of the neurobehavioral cogni- Medical neuropsychology (2nd ed.). New York:
tive status examination with geriatric rehabili- Kluwer.
tation patients. Rehabilitation Psychology, 46(3), Teng, E. L., & Chui, H. C. (1987). The Modified
219–228. Mini-Mental State (3MS) examination. Journal of
Salik, Y., Ozalevli, S., & Cimrin, A. H. (2007). Clinical Psychiatry, 48(8), 314–318.
Cognitive function and its effects on the quality Teng, E. L., Hasegawa, K., Homma, A., Imai, Y., Larson,
of life status in the patients with chronic obstruc- E., Graves, A., et al. (1994). The Cognitive Abilities
tive pulmonary disease (COPD). Archives of Screening Instrument (CASI): A practical test for
Gerontology and Geriatrics, 45(3), 273–280. cross-cultural epidemiological studies of dementia.
Sartori, E., Belliard, S., Chevrier, C., Trebon, P., International Psychogeriatrics, 6(1), 45–58.
Chaperon, J., & Edan, G. (2006). From psychom- Tombaugh, T. N. (2005). Test–retest reliable coeffi-
etry to neuropsychological disability in multiple cients and 5-year change scores for the MMSE and
sclerosis: A new brief French cognitive screen- 3MS. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 20(4),
ing battery and cognitive risk factors. Revue 485–503.
Neurologique, 162(5), 603–615. Tombaugh, T.N., McDowell, I., Kristjansson, B.,
Schrimsher, G. W., O’Bryant, S. E., Parker, J. D., & Hubley, A. M. (1996). Mini-Mental State
& Burke, R. S. (2005). The relation between eth- Examination (MMSE) and the Modified
nicity and cognistat performance in males seek- MMSE (3MS): A psychometric comparison and
ing substance use disorder treatment. Journal of normative data. Psychological Assessment, 8(1),
Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 27(7), 48–59.
873–885. Tombaugh, T. N., & McIntyre, N. J. (1992). The Mini-
Serper, M. R., & Allen, M. H. (2002). Rapid screening Mental State Examination: A comprehensive
for cognitive impairment in the psychiatric emer- review. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society,
gency service: I. Cognitive screening batteries. 40(9), 922–935.
Psychiatric Services, 53(12), 1527–1529. Van den Berg, E., Kessels, R. P., Kappelle, L. J., de
Shulman, K. I. (2000). Clock-drawing: Is it the ideal Hann, E. H., Biessels, G. J., Utrecht Diabetic
cognitive screening test? International Journal of Encephalopathy Study Group. (2006). Type 2 dia-
Geriatric Psychiatry, 15(6), 548–561. betes, cognitive function and dementia: Vascular
Shulman, K. I., & Feinstein, A. (2003). Quick cogni- and metabolic determinants. Drugs Today, 42(11),
tive screening for clinicians. London: Taylor and 741–754.
Francis Group. Van Gorp, W. G., Ma rcotte, T. D., Sultzer, D.,
Shulman, K. I., Herrmann, N., Brodaty, H., Chiu, Hinkin, C., Mahler, M., & Cummings, J. L.
H., Lawlor, B., Ritchie, K., et al. (2006). IPA sur- (1999). Screening for dementia: Comparison
vey of brief cognitive screening instruments. of three commonly used instruments. Journal
International Psychogeriatrics, 18(2), 281–294. of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology,
Solomon, P. R., Hirschoff, A., Kelly, B., Relin, M., 21(1), 29–38.
Brush, M., DeVeaux, R. D., et al. (1998). A 7 minute Van Harten, B., Courant, M. N. J., Scheltens, P.,
neurocognitive screening battery highly sensi- & Weinstein, H. C. (2004). Validation of the
tive to Alzheimer’s disease. Archives of Neurology, HIV Dementia Scale in an elderly cohort of
55(3), 349–355. patients with subcortical cognitive impair-
Strickland, T. L., Longobardi, P. G., Alperson, B. ment causedby subcortical ischaemic vascular
L., & Andre, K. (2005). Mini-Mental State and disease or a normal pressure hydrocephalus.
Cognistat performance in an older African Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 18(1),
American sample. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 109–114.
19(1), 87–98. Wallace, J. J., Caroselli, J. S., Scheibel, R. S., & High,
Swets, J. A. (1996). Signal detection theory and ROC W. M., Jr. (2000). Predictive validity of the neu-
analysis in psychology and diagnostics: Collected robehavioural cognitive status examination
papers. In Scientific Psychology Series: Hilsdale, (NCSE) in a postacute rehabilitation setting. Brain
N.J.: Laurence Erlbaum. Injury, 14(1), 63–69.
Tangalos, E. G., Smith, G. E., Ivnik, R. J., Petersen, Welsh, K. A., Breitner, J. C., & Magruder-Habib, K.
R. C., Kokmen, E., Kurland, L. T., et al. (1996). The M. (1993). Detection of dementia in the elderly
Mini-Mental State Examination in general medi- using telephone screening of cognitive status.
cal practice: Clinical utility and acceptance. Mayo Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, & Behavioral
Clinic Proceedings, 71, 829–837. Neurology, 6(2), 103–110.
126 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

Whiteside, D. M., Padula, M. A., Jeff rey, L. K., Xu, G., Meyer, J. S., Thornby, J., Chowdhury, M., &
& Zetterman, R. (1996). Cognitive screening Quach, M. (2002). Screening for mild cognitive
with the Neurobehavioral Cognitive Status impairment (MCI) utilizing combined mini-
Examination in a chronic liver disease mental-cognitive capacity examinations for iden-
population. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 10(4), tifying dementia prodromes. International Journal
459–463. of Geriatric Psychiatry, 17(11), 1027–1033.
7

Demographic Influences and Use


of Demographically Corrected Norms
in Neuropsychological Assessment
Robert K. Heaton, Lee Ryan, and Igor Grant

A primary goal of clinical neuropsychological corrected norms for widely used neuropsycho-
assessment is to determine whether a given set logical tests. We have added a discussion of
of test results suggests brain pathology. This is the information available on two groups that
done by comparing a person’s performance to are served particularly poorly by most avail-
available normative standards, which for most able normative data: the very elderly and ethnic
tests are based on the results of a “typical” North minorities. We begin with a review of general
American sample of neurologically normal issues related to the use of normative data in
adults. Unfortunately, available norms for most neuropsychological assessment.
neuropsychological tests do not include adjust-
ment for relevant demographic characteristics
“Normality” and Adequate
of the individual being considered. This state of
affairs is, quite clearly, a major impediment to
Normative Data
adequate clinical assessment of many patients.
Consider, for example, a 60-year-old woman The debate over what constitutes “normal” is
with a grade school education, whose first lan- clearly a central issue for neuropsychologists.
guage was Spanish and who began learning In determining whether an individual’s test
English at age 15. It would be quite inappropri- performance is the result of brain disorder, it
ate to compare this patient’s performances on is essential to distinguish that performance
most neurobehavioral tests with those of the from normal variations in cognitive ability.
average adult in North America. Neuropsychological assessment’s most straight-
In the first edition of this book, our chapter forward task is to identify a complete loss of
focused on a study assessing the differences function secondary to brain damage. The result-
in neuropsychological test performance associ- ing behavior falls well outside the range of abil-
ated with age, education, and sex (Heaton et al., ity for the entire normal population and thus is
1986). The results of the study highlighted the relatively simple to detect. Examples of a loss of
pressing need for more complete normative data function include visual agnosia (the inability to
that take multiple demographic variables into identify common objects visually despite intact
account. Since the publication of that chapter, visual functioning) and facial apraxia (the loss
a substantial amount of work has been done in of motor programs for facial expression). More
this area. For this edition we have expanded and often, however, neuropsychologists are asked to
updated the previous presentation by review- determine whether an impairment in function
ing new studies on the effects of demographic has occurred; that is, whether test performance
variables and by providing information on sev- constitutes a change in normal functioning for
eral recently available sets of demographically the individual. Such a judgment should not be

127
128 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

taken lightly as it may have a major impact on create an unbiased sample. This practice may
an individual’s medical evaluation and treat- result in biases in the sample that are difficult to
ment, self-esteem, family functioning, indepen- detect. Publications of standardization samples
dence, and access to future opportunities and should thus provide a description of the sam-
resources. These judgments are based primarily pling procedures and subject recruitment pro-
on tests developed by researchers of a particular cedures employed in the study, as well as rates
cultural, educational, and socioeconomic stra- of subject compliance, so that clinicians may
tum. An individual’s test performance is judged, make informed judgments regarding the gener-
perforce, in comparison to some standard that alizability of published norms.
the clinician chooses. We espouse a relativistic Second, assessing whether norms are appro-
approach to normality (and hence abnormality), priate for an individual requires sufficient
where normal is defined as the range of behav- knowledge of the characteristics of the subject
iors and abilities within a group of like individ- sample. The description of the criteria for sub-
uals who share social, educational, cultural, and ject inclusion will define the population from
generational backgrounds. Western Caucasian which the sample was drawn, and will essen-
middle- to upper-class clinicians cannot assume tially constitute the researchers’ definition
that their own demographically defined group of normality. Important characteristics may
may be used as a definitive standard. include the following: (1) the numbers of sub-
The purpose of normative data is to provide jects in various strata of demographic variables
information on the range of an ability within a such as age, education, socioeconomic status,
specifically defined, neurologically normal pop- and ethnic group; (2) whether the subjects were
ulation. The clinical utility of norms will depend living independently in the community; (3) cur-
on several factors, including the representative- rent or past histories of factors that might influ-
ness of the normative sample, the goodness of fit ence cognitive performance, including chronic
between the individual and the normative sam- illness, significant medical history (including
ple, and the degree to which the norms consider head trauma), drug and alcohol use, prescrip-
demographic variables that account for normal tion drug use, and psychiatric diagnoses; and
variations in test performance. Each of these (4) whether there was any independent screen-
factors will be discussed below. ing (e.g., mental status exam) to rule out serious
First, an adequate normative sample must be cognitive disturbance.
representative of the general population; that is, Finally, the utility of norms will depend on
the scores included in the data set are assumed whether demographic variables that have a
to be an unbiased sample of the population of significant impact on test performance have
interest. Often, normative standards are derived been adequately considered. Performance on
from small sample sizes that result in poor clas- most neuropsychological tests are significantly
sification rates when applied to larger samples. related to the subject’s age, education, eth-
For example, the Russell et al. (1970) norms nicity, and, for a few tests, sex (Heaton et al.,
for the Halstead–Reitan Battery (HRB)were 1991; Parsons & Prigatano, 1978). The influence
originally based on a combination of clinical of demographic factors is apparent for neu-
judgment and the test results of a sample of 26 rologically normal individuals as well as for
neurologically normal subjects. Large sample those who have cerebral disorders (Finlayson
size alone, however, does not ensure represen- et al., 1977; Reitan, 1955). Unfortunately, as
tativeness. Methods of recruitment, sampling Lezak (1987) notes, despite decades of aware-
techniques, inclusion and exclusion criteria, ness of the effects of demographic variables,
and compliance rates are all critical factors in most psychological and neuropsychological
assessing possible sources of bias in a sample tests in common use today do not stratify their
(for discussion, see Anastasi, 1988). Erickson samples on or otherwise correct for these vari-
et al. (1992) have pointed out that sampling ables. Even for test norms that stratify subject
procedures in normative studies are quite often groups on age or education, information con-
opportunistic, making use of a population at cerning the effects of such factors in combina-
hand rather than being expressly designed to tion often is lacking. Also potentially important
Demographic Influences and Use of Demographically Corrected Norms 129

is the issue of how recently norms were col- and Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS-III) has
lected. Demographic and cultural factors can- allowed the identification of six cognitive fac-
not be assumed to be comparable for groups tors that are assessed by the combined Wechsler
of subjects who were tested decades apart. For batteries: Verbal Comprehension, Working
example, cross-sectional studies may overes- Memory, Perceptual Organization, Processing
timate age effects by confounding these with Speed, Auditory Memory, and Visual Memory
educational, nutritional, or occupational differ- (Tulsky et al., 2003). Subsequently, Heaton
ences among groups of subjects from different et al. (2003) assessed age effects on education-
generations (Anastasi, 1988). Successive genera- corrected WAIS-III/WMS-III factor scores of
tions may differ in language patterns and prob- standardization sample subjects over the age
lem-solving styles (Albert, 1981), or perhaps of 19 years. Consistent with earlier studies,
along dimensions that have not yet been identi- there was virtually no age effect on the Verbal
fied (Flynn, 1984, 1987). Thus, norms collected Comprehension factor (R square < .01), whereas
20 years ago, even if they take into account age very large age effects were seen on Processing
and education, may contain systematic errors in Speed and Visual Memory (R square = .45
current use. in each case). Smaller, but still substantial,
While gathering normative data is a time- age effects occurred on the remaining factors
consuming and labor-intensive enterprise, the (R squares of .27, .24, and .20 for Perceptual
importance of such research cannot be overem- Organization, Working Memory, and Auditory
phasized. Standardization samples that provide Memory, respectively).
adequate descriptions of sampling methods and Performances on most of the individual tests
subject characteristics and that are stratified on or in the HRB also show a significant negative
otherwise corrected for important demographic relationship with age (Reitan, 1955, 1957; Vega
variables are critical to clinical neuropsychology. & Parsons, 1967). Reitan warned that after age
Expanded and updated norms will continue to 45 many normals score in Halstead’s brain-
be needed as society evolves over time. damaged range on the Impairment Index.
As an illustration, Price et al. (1980) studied
a group of 49 retired, healthy schoolteachers
Demographic Influences on Test
(mean age, 72 years) and found that 56% of the
Performance: Age, Education, Sex
subjects scored in the impaired range accord-
ing to the then-standard norms on the HRB.
Age
Considerable variability was seen in the num-
Of the instruments considered in this chapter, ber of subjects misclassified as brain-damaged
by far the most well known and widely used by the different subtests in the battery (ranging
are the various versions of the Wechsler Adult from 18% to 90%). Thus, impaired performance
Intelligence Scale (Wechsler, 1955, 1981, 1997a) on the HRB appears to occur with most but not
and Wechsler Memory Scale (Wechsler, 1945, all normal elderly subjects, and age-related defi-
1987, 1997b). Findings with the standardization cits are more pronounced on some tests than on
samples of all of these versions reflect lower aver- others. In their review of the literature, Reitan
age performances for each successive age group and Wolfson (1986) concluded that age effects
tested after their mid-thirties. Age differences on the HRB are found primarily in complex,
are more pronounced for nonverbal than for novel tasks that require reasoning, abstraction,
verbal subtests, reflecting the fact that different and logical analysis, while tests related to prior
abilities change at different rates; in the “classic learning, past experience, and language ability
pattern,” verbal skills and well-learned infor- are generally spared.
mation hold up best over time, while percep- Interestingly, Yeudall et al. (1987) did not
tual-integrative and psychomotor skills decline find a strong association between age and HRB
the most with advancing age (Botwinick, 1967; performance with subjects below the age of 40.
Leckliter & Matarazzo, 1989). In a sample of 225 community-recruited males
Co-norming of the third editions of the and females between the ages of 15 and 40 years,
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-III) correlation coefficients ranging from .00 to .27
130 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

were reported on various subtests. The evidence sample for those batteries. Again, teenagers
suggests that, as with the Wechsler scales, age were excluded because many of them were still
becomes an increasingly important factor in the in school when they were tested. Significant
fourth decade and beyond. It should be noted, education effects were seen on all WAIS-III/
however, that decreased performance in later WMS-III factors. These were largest on Verbal
years may not be solely attributable to age but Comprehension (R square = .32), Perceptual
may also be due to generational differences in Organization (R square = .18), and Processing
education, health services, access to media Speed (R square = .17), but education effects
information, and many other factors. Because also occurred on Working Memory, Auditory
the majority of studies in this area are cross- Memory, and Visual Memory (respective R
sectional rather than longitudinal, the presence squares = .11, .09, and .08). In general, then,
of mediating factors other than age cannot be education effects were largest on measures of
ruled out. intellectual abilities, and more modest on mea-
sures of episodic memory.
However, how clinically important are these
Education
education effects on the WAIS-III/WMS-III
Performance on the Wechsler Intelligence factors? To explore this question, Heaton et al.
Scales is strongly correlated with educational (2003) applied a 1-SD cutoff to define “impair-
achievement, hardly a surprising fi nding given ment” on age-corrected factor scores. (The
that the original purpose of intelligence tests choice of this cutoff is evaluated and discussed
was to predict academic success (Anastasi, in Taylor & Heaton, 2001.) False-positive error
1988). Matarazzo (1972) reported a Pearson rates (normals misclassified as “impaired”)
correlation of .70 between highest grade com- were computed for five education subgroups
pleted and WAIS IQ; Matarazzo and Herman within the national standardization sam-
(1984) reported a correlation of .54 for the ple: 16+ years (college graduates), 13–15 years
WAIS-R standardization sample of 16- to (some college), 12 years (high school gradu-
74-year-olds. The latter correlation appeared ates), 9–11 years (some high school), and ≤9
to be spuriously lowered because of the inclu- years (less than high school). The probabil-
sion of 16- to 24-year-olds, many of whom ity of being misclassified as impaired in these
had not yet completed their education. When respective education groups ranged from 2%
this age group was excluded, the correlation to 45% for Verbal Comprehension, 4–40% for
between IQ and years of education increased Perceptual Organization, 3–37% for Processing
to .62. Kaufman et al. (1988) also observed that Speed, 6–31% for Working Memory, 6–28%
education effects were less prominent in the for Auditory Memory, and 6–27% for Visual
youngest age group (16–19 years), since edu- Memory. From these results, it is clear that use
cation effects are attenuated among individu- of the standard, age-corrected factor scores to
als who are still actively participating in the classify neuropsychological impairment leads to
educational system. Examining the WAIS-R widely different diagnostic accuracy, especially
standardization sample, Reynolds et al. (1987) for people with relatively high (16+ years) and
reported significant differences across edu- low (<9 years) education levels. Although one
cation groups for Verbal, Performance, and might be tempted to conclude from these results
Full-Scale mean IQs. These IQ scores varied that classification accuracy is “better” for highly
from 26 to 33 points between the lowest educa- educated persons, it is important to recognize
tion group (less than 8 years) and the highest that this is true only for the specificity side of
(16 years or more). In contrast to age effects, the equation. Since specificity and sensitivity of
Kaufman et al. (1988) found that education classification cutoffs tend to be inversely related,
had more of an impact on verbal than nonver- it can be expected that use of the standard, age-
bal tests. corrected scores will have the “worst” classifi-
Heaton et al. (2003) explored education cation accuracy with highly educated persons
effects on age-adjusted WAIS-III/WMS-III fac- who have acquired cognitive abnormalities
tor scores, again using the joint standardization (Taylor & Heaton, 2001).
Demographic Influences and Use of Demographically Corrected Norms 131

Historically, the relationship between edu- available longitudinal studies do not support
cation and HRB performance has been less this hypothesis. However, these studies are lim-
well documented. For example, Finlayson ited by the use of a restricted range of tests over
et al. (1977) tested normal individuals with relatively brief age ranges and by the selective
grade school (less than 10 years), high school attrition of initially less able subjects. Birren
(grade 12, without college experience), and and Morrison (1961) found no significant age-
university level (at least 3 years’ college expe- by-education interaction effects using data from
rience) education. They found significant dif- the WAIS standardization sample. A similar
ferences between the groups on the Category pattern is apparent for the WAIS-R standard-
Test, the Seashore Rhythm Test, Speech- ization sample (Reynolds et al., 1987), but no
Sounds Perception Test, and Trail Making statistical analyses were done to assess the inter-
A and B. No effect of education was evident actions between age and education. The possi-
on the Tactual Performance Test or Finger bility of interactions between age and education
Tapping. With three similar groups of brain- on the HRB had not been investigated prior to
damaged individuals, education effects were our exploration of the issue for the first edition
less pronounced, and were significant only on of this book (see below).
the Seashore Rhythm Test and Speech-Sounds
Perception Test. Finlayson et al. (1977) sug-
Gender
gested that the effects of brain damage may
produce sufficient decreases in performance to Research on gender differences in ability has
“wash out” the effects of education. found males and females to be equivalent in gen-
Vega and Parsons (1967) reported Pearson eral intelligence. Reynolds et al. (1987) did not
correlations between years of education and find that the sexes differed significantly on any
HRB subtest performance as high as .58 for of the three summary scores on the WAIS-R.
Speech-Sounds Perception Test, .58 for the Sex differences on the WAIS and WAIS-R were
Seashore Rhythm Test, and .45 for errors on the minimized by design, in that questions that pro-
Category Test. When the effects of education duced clear-cut sex differences were excluded
were partialled out, the correlations between the or counterbalanced during the developmen-
HRB subtests and age weakened, but remained tal stages of the tests (Matarazzo, 1972, 1986).
significant, suggesting two independent sources Nevertheless, on tests of specific ability areas,
of performance variability. In reviewing the some sex differences are found. Males tend to
extant literature, Leckliter and Matarazzo (1989) do better on tests that involve manipulating
concluded that although correlation coefficients spatial relationships, quantitative skills, physi-
were generally lower between years of education cal strength, and simple motor speed, whereas
and HRB performance compared to age and females show advantages on tests of certain ver-
HRB performance, highly educated individuals bal abilities (for review, see Buffery & Gray, 1972;
consistently performed better than those indi- Maccoby & Jacklin, 1974). For example, on the
viduals with fewer years of education. HRB, Fromm-Auch and Yeudall (1983) found
that women were slower on Finger Tapping than
men and men had stronger grip strength on the
The Rate of Age-Related Decline
Dynamometer than women for both dominant
A question addressed in several previous stud- and nondominant hands.
ies is whether the rate of age-related cognitive Heaton et al. (2003) compared males and
decline is linked to subjects’ initial level of females within the joint WAIS-III/WMS-III
functioning or socioeconomic status (educa- standardization sample on the six-factor scores,
tion, occupation). In reviewing this literature, which had been corrected for both age and edu-
Botwinick (1967) noted that cross-sectional cation. Modest mean z-score differences favor-
studies have provided some inconsistent evi- ing males were seen on Verbal Comprehension
dence that subjects with lower initial ability (z = .25), Perceptual Organization (z = .17), and
and lower occupational status show greater Working Memory (z = .15), whereas slightly
age-related impairment on some tests. The larger differences favoring women occurred on
132 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

Processing Speed (z = .36) and the two episodic 513 were right-handed. Their ages ranged from
memory factors (Auditory Memory z = .25 and 15 to 81 years, with a mean of 39.3 (SD = 17.5
Visual Memory z = .22). At most, these gen- years). Years of education ranged from zero (no
der differences translated into only about a 5% formal education) to 20 (doctoral degree), with
difference in test score specificity for men and a mean of 13.3 (SD = 3.4) years. For analyses
women. designed to assess age-by-education interaction
effects, subjects were divided into three age cat-
egories (<40 years, 40–59 years, and ≥60 years)
Halstead–Reitan Normative and three education categories (<12 years,
Data Pool 12–15 years, and ≥16 years). Respectively, the
In the first edition of this book, we presented a total numbers of subjects in the three age cat-
study that highlighted the importance of demo- egories were 319, 134, and 100, and in the three
graphic variables for performance on the WAIS education categories, 132, 249, and 172. All nine
and an extended version of the HRB. The main age/education subgroups included over 25 sub-
points of the study will be discussed here; the jects except for the high age/high education cat-
reader is referred to the original chapter for egory (n = 17).
more details (Heaton et al., 1986). The goal of In order to assess sex differences in neuropsy-
the study was to address the issue of how per- chological test performance, males and females
formance on the various neuropsychological were individually matched within 5 years in age
tests relates to age and education when high and and within 2 years in education. This resulted in
low values of these demographic factors were 177 matched pairs, well matched on mean age
adequately represented in the subject sample. (36.6 for males, 36.7 for females) and education
Which tests are more sensitive to age effects (13.2 for males, 13.1 for females).
and which appear to be more related to edu- All subjects were tested by trained techni-
cational attainment? Are there significant age- cians, and all were rated as having put forth
by-education interaction effects on tests in the adequate effort on their evaluations. The
battery and, if so, are they consistent with the 11 subtests of the WAIS and an expanded
hypothesis that better-educated groups show HRB were administered. The latter bat-
less age-related decline in neuropsychologi- tery has been described elsewhere (Reitan &
cal functioning? How well or how poorly does Wolfson, 1986) and included the Category Test,
a previously published set of HRB norms work Tactual Performance Test (Time, Memory,
for groups of normal subjects at different age and Location components), Seashore Rhythm
and education levels? Finally, which tests in the Test, Speech-Sounds Perception Test, Aphasia
battery show significant sex differences, and Screening Exam, Trail-Making Test, Spatial
are these differences large enough to necessi- Relations Assessment, Sensory-Perceptual
tate developing separate norms for males and Exam, Tactile Form Recognition Test, Finger
females? Tapping Test, Hand Dynamometer, and
Grooved Pegboard Test.
Scores for the WAIS subtests were regular
Subjects and Methods
scaled scores (not age-corrected). The Russell
Subjects consisted of all normal controls (356 et al. (1970) scoring system was used for the
males and 197 females) for whom there were Aphasia Screening Exam, Spatial Relations,
complete WAIS and HRB data available at the Sensory-Perceptual Exam, and Average
neuropsychology laboratories of the University Impairment Rating. In addition, the cutoff
of Colorado (N = 207), University of California scores provided in the Russell et al. (1970) book
at San Diego (N = 181), and the University of were used to determine how many subjects in
Wisconsin (N = 165) Medical Schools. None the different age and education categories were
of the subjects had any history of neurological correctly classified as normal by the Average
illness, significant head trauma, or substance Impairment Rating and each of its component
abuse. Forty subjects (7.2%) were left-handed and test measures.
Demographic Influences and Use of Demographically Corrected Norms 133

Results Vocabulary, Information, and Comprehension.


Performance on several of the HRB tests was
Age Effects strongly related to age, particularly for mea-
sures of psychomotor speed, conceptual ability,
Figure 7–1 shows the percentage of variance flexibility of thought, and incidental memory. In
accounted for by age (R square) on each test contrast, scores on HRB tests of language skills
measure. All the correlations between test and simple sensory and motor abilities showed
measures and age were significant (p < .05), relatively weak associations with age.
with the exception of Vocabulary and Hand
Dynamometer, and consistently indicated
poorer performance associated with older Education Effects
age. Among the Wechsler subtests, substan-
tial age effects were apparent on Digit Symbol Education significantly correlated with all WAIS
and Picture Arrangement, whereas age showed and HRB measures, indicating better perfor-
minimal relationship with performance on mance with higher education levels. Figure 7–2

Halstead-
50% Wechsler Reitan
Subtests Battery
45%
40%
Accountable Variance (Age)

35% Grooved Pegboard

Digit symbol Category


30% TPT-Location
Trails-B
TPT-Time/Block
25%
20% Picture Arrangement TPT-Memory

15% Rhythm
Block Design, Object Assembly Speech-Sound Perception
10% Picture Complection Tactile form recognition
Similarities Aphasia, Sensory-Perceptual
Tapping
5% Digit Span Spatial Relations
Arithmetic Vocabulary, Information, Comprehension Dynamometer
0

Figure 7–1. Percentage of test variance accounted for by age.

Halstead-
50% Wechsler Reitan
Subtests Battery
45%
Accountable Variance (Education)

Vocabulary
40% Information

Similarities
35%
30% Comprehension
Arithmetic
25% Digit Symbol

20% Trails-B
Block Design Speech-Sounds Perception
Category Aphasia
15% Picture Completion
Digit Span

10% Picture Arrangement Rhythm, TPT-Memory, Grooved Pegboard


Spatial Relations
Object Assembly Sensory-Perceptual Tapping TPT-Time/Block
5% Tactile Form Recognition, TPT-Location
Dynamometer
0

Figure 7–2. Percentage of test variance accounted for by education.


134 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

shows the percentage variance in each test nonverbal tests of psychomotor speed and/or
accounted for by education (R squared). Even new problem solving are more age related.
for measures of simple motor and sensory func-
tions, better test performance was associated
The Interaction Between Age
with higher previous educational attainment.
and Education
In contrast to the results shown previously for
age, education level was most strongly related The 3 × 3 analyses of variance (ANOVAs) per-
to scores on the WAIS Verbal subtests, and formed on all test measures produced age and
somewhat less related to scores on the HRB. education main effects that were in agreement
Consistent with this, within the HRB, correla- with the results of the correlational analysis.
tions were highest with tests of language skills, That is, tests previously found to be more age
conceptual ability, and cognitive flexibility. related or more education related were simi-
larly classified by these analyses. In addition,
significant age-by-education interaction effects
Tests Showing Relatively Greater Age
were obtained on WAIS Comprehension,
or Education Sensitivity
Picture Completion, Block Design, Picture
Figure 7–3 shows for each test measure the differ- Arrangement, as well as on the HRB Impairment
ence between the amount of variance accounted Index, Average Impairment Rating, Category
for by age versus education. In general, WAIS Test (errors), Trail Making B, TPT Memory and
subtests tend to be more education related, and Location, and Speech-Sounds Perception.
the HRB subtests tend to be more age related. Three possible patterns of age-by-education
Not surprisingly, within both batteries tests interaction might be expected: (1) subjects
of verbal skills and previously accumulated with the most education show less age-related
knowledge are more education related, whereas impairment; (2) subjects in the lowest education

40 Halstead-
35 Wechsler Reitan
Subtests Battery
30
Grooved Pegboard
25
More Age-Related

TPT-Location

TPT-Time/Block
Accountable Variance (Age-Education)

20
Category
15
10 Picture Arrangement
Trails-B TPT-Memory
Digit Symbol
5 Object Assembly Rhythm Tactile Form Recognition
Tapping Sensory-Perceptual
0 Dynamometer
More Education-Related

–5 Block Design Spatial Relations


Picture Completion Speech-Sounds Perception
Aphasia
–10 Digit Span

–15
–20
Arithmetic
–25
Comprehension Similarities
–30

–35
Vocabulary Information
–40

Figure 7–3. Difference in percentage of test variance accounted for by age versus education (i.e., age variance
minus education variance).
Demographic Influences and Use of Demographically Corrected Norms 135

group show more age-related impairment; and 15


(3) a regression toward the mean occurs, with 14
less effect of education between older groups

Mean Picture Arrangement


13
than between younger groups. These three pat-
terns are not necessarily mutually exclusive. A 12

Scaled Score
test may be consistent with pattern (1) or (2) 11
across the first two age levels, but fit pattern (3)
10
between the second and third age levels.
To explore the possible patterns of interac- 9
tion effects, mean test scores of subgroups at 8 16+ yrs Education
12–15 yrs Education
each education level were plotted across the <12 yrs Education
7
three age levels. While it is tempting to consider
<40yrs 40–59yrs 60+yrs
this type of graph to be a longitudinal view of (mean = 26yrs) (mean = 49yrs) (mean = 68yrs)
how groups at each education level may change
with advancing age, it must be recognized that Figure 7–5. Results of groups in nine age/educa-
the curves do not reflect change in any direct tion categories on WAIS Picture Arrangement.
sense, as each point on the graph reflects the
performance of a separate subject group that
was tested only once. 15
Several of these graphs are presented here.
14
Figure 7–4 summarizes the data on Vocabulary,
Mean Block Desing Scaled Score

a test that is known to show little change in 13

older age groups. The curves for each educa- 12


tion level indicated no significant age-related
11
decrease in test performance. Furthermore, the
comparability of the three age subgroups within 10
12–15 yrs Education
education levels suggests that the subgroups 9 16+ yrs Education
were fairly well matched in terms of previously <12 yrs Education
8
learned information.
In contrast, consider the graphs for two 7
WAIS subtests that have significant age-by- <40yrs 40–59yrs 60+yrs
education interaction effects (see Figures 7–5 (mean = 26yrs) (mean = 49yrs) (mean = 68yrs)

Age Group

Figure 7–6. Results of groups in nine age/educa-


16 tion categories on WAIS Block Design.

15
16+ yrs Education
Mean Vocabulary Scaled Score

14 (mean = 17 yrs)
and 7–6). On Picture Arrangement, the results
13 across the first two age levels suggested more
12 12–15 yrs Education age-related impairment for the least educated
(mean = 13 yrs) subgroup, consistent with pattern (2) above. On
11
Block Design, the curves across the first two
10 <12 yrs Education age levels were more consistent with pattern (1),
(mean = 9 yrs) suggesting less age-related impairment for the
9
subgroup with 16+ years of education. On both
8
of these subtests, however, the curves from the
<40yrs 40–59yrs 60+yrs
(mean = 26yrs) (mean = 49yrs) (mean = 68yrs)
second to the third age level fit pattern (3); that
is, there was less difference between education
Figure 7–4. Results of groups in nine age/educa- subgroups at the older age level. Other tests,
tion categories on WAIS Vocabulary test. including Trail Making B and the Category Test
136 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

100% virtually no difference between these groups


on any WAIS IQ value or HRB summary score.
Percent Subjects Classified Normal

90%
Thus, the males and females were comparable
80%
with respect to general intelligence and overall
By Standard Cutoff

70% neuropsychological functioning. On individ-


60% ual tests, as expected, males did much better
50%
16+ yrs Education (p < .001) with each hand on tests of motor
12–15 yrs Education
speed (Finger Tapping Test) and grip strength
40%
(Hand Dynamometer). Differences were also
30% <12 yrs Education
obtained on the Tactual Performance Test
20% and the Aphasia Screening Exam that were
<40yrs 40–59yrs 60+yrs not expected, but are consistent with the gen-
(mean = 26yrs) (mean = 49yrs) (mean = 68yrs)
eral psychological literature on sex differences
Age Group in normals. Males did significantly better
Figure 7–7. Results of groups in age/education cat-
(p < .05) on the TPT Total Time score (13.5 min-
egories: percentage classified as normal by Average
utes versus 15.6 minutes for females). However,
Impairment Rating.
the females showed an advantage (p < .001) on
the Aphasia Screening Exam, obtaining a mean
error score of 2.7 versus 4.0 for males. Finally,
there was a statistically significant (p < .05)
showed patterns similar to Picture Arrangement; but clinically trivial difference on the WAIS
that is, they showed pattern (2) between the first Comprehension subtest (mean scores of 12.7 for
two age levels and pattern (3) between the sec- females and 12.1 for males).
ond and third age levels.
The final graph (Figure 7–7) shows the per-
WAIS versus WAIS-R versus
centage of subjects classified as normal by the
WAIS-III Patterns
Average Impairment Rating. This pattern was
apparent for most standard cutoff scores for the The WAIS has been replaced in clinical practice
test measures in the HRB. The results indicate by the WAIS-R in 1981 and by the WAIS-III in
that the cutoff scores are adequate for all edu- 1997. Albert and Heaton (1988) reported find-
cation subgroups at the first age level. At the ings similar to those described above on the
second age level the cutoffs misclassify only WAIS-R subtests. They compared the results of
a few more normal subjects in the two higher the WAIS-R standardization sample with results
education subgroups, but misclassify the major- of 543 adults who were given the WAIS. Similar
ity of the subjects in the lower education sub- relationships were obtained between test per-
group. Finally, although a significant minority formance and both age and education on the
of subjects in the oldest subgroup still perform two versions of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence
at a level that would be considered normal for a Scales. The relationship of scores to education
young adult, the vast majority of the subjects in was the inverse of that to age, education corre-
this age group are misclassified as “brain-dam- lating more highly with Verbal measures than
aged.” It is apparent that the standard cutoffs are with Performance measures. Once again, the
not appropriate as norms for most subjects over effect of sex on test performances appeared too
the age of 60 or, equally important, for most small to be of clinical significance. Similar find-
subjects with less than a high school education, ings have been noted above for the WAIS-III.
regardless of their age.
Specificity versus Sensitivity
Sex Effects
A separate but extremely important issue in
T-tests for paired samples were used to compare developing normative data is to know the use-
results of our matched male and female groups fulness of a test in distinguishing groups with
on all WAIS and HRB measures. There was known brain dysfunction from the normative
Demographic Influences and Use of Demographically Corrected Norms 137

sample group. That is, in addition to knowing Average Impairment Rating cutoff is relatively
the likelihood of correctly classifying normal poor at identifying brain-damaged individu-
individuals, the neuropsychologist needs to als who are young and/or well educated. In the
know the sensitivity of the norms, or the true older samples, more brain-damaged subjects
positive classification rate for people with var- are correctly classified (from 55% at <40 years
ious cerebral disorders. In the present study, to 94% at 60+ years of age), while as education
correct classification rates for the normal group increases, fewer subjects are correctly classified
(described earlier) were compared with those of as brain-damaged (from 87% at <12 years to
the 382 brain-damaged subjects. All subjects in 60% at 16+ years of education).
the patient group were clinical referrals for neu- This comparison highlights the need for val-
ropsychological testing, and all had structural idation of normative data and classification cut
brain abnormalities that were verified with points on known brain-damaged groups, as well
appropriate neuroradiological procedures (pri- as on normal controls at different levels of age
marily computerized tomographic scans and and education. The Russell et al. (1970) norms
magnetic resonance image scans). They had a obtain fairly good (and balanced) sensitivity
mean age of 43.7 years (SD = 1.0) and a mean and specificity at middle levels of age and edu-
of 12.9 years (SD = 3.1) of formal education; cation, but at the extremes of these demographic
249 (65.2% of the sample) were males. The most variables, result in unacceptable percentages of
frequent diagnoses were closed head injury false-positive or false-negative errors.
(n = 73), cerebrovascular accident (n = 65),
intrinsic tumor (n = 49), Alzheimer’s disease
Implications
(n = 48), extrinsic tumor (n = 30), hydroceph-
alus (n = 23), infectious or toxic encephalopa- The results of our study accord well with
thies (n = 22), penetrating head injury (n = 18), Cattell’s (1963) distinction between “fluid” and
multiple sclerosis (n = 9), and cerebral anoxia “crystallized” intelligence. Crystallized intel-
(n = 8); other etiologies included vascular mal- ligence is measured by tests of knowledge and
formations, epilepsy with structural abnor- skills that were acquired in previous learning
malities, other dementias, and depressed skull experiences. Crystallized intelligence develops
fractures (n = 37). rapidly during the first 20 years of life and then
Table 7–1 lists the percentages of the nor- levels off, remaining relatively stable over the
mal and brain-damaged subjects in six age and ensuing decades. Thus, performance on tests
education subgroups who were correctly clas- such as Information or Vocabulary from the
sified on the HRB Average Impairment Rating various WAIS versions does not decline with
using the Russell et al. (1970) normative crite- age, although it is clearly related to level of edu-
ria. Within the normal group, the percentage of cation. By contrast, fluid intelligence is consid-
subjects correctly classified as normal decreases ered most dependent upon biological factors
with age, but increases with education. In con- such as the normal development and continued
trast, the opposite pattern is evident for the integrity of the central nervous system (CNS).
brain-damaged group. Generally, the standard This form of intelligence is measured by tasks

Table 7–1. Percentage of Subjects (553 Normal Control Subjects and 382 Brain-damaged Subjects) in Six Age
and Education Subgroups who were Correctly Classified as Normal or Brain-Damaged by the Russell et al.
(1970) Criteria on the Average Impairment Rating from the Halstead-Reitan Battery
Age (in years) Education (in years)
<40 40–59 60+ <12 12–15 16+
Normal Controlled Subjects 97% 84% 39% 58% 90% 93%
(n = 319) (n = 134) (n = 100) (n = 132) (n = 249) (n = 172)
Brain-Damaged Subjects 55% 83% 94% 87% 71% 60%
(n = 167) (n = 138) (n = 77) (n = 99) (n = 191) (n = 92)
138 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

requiring learning, conceptual, and problem- education subgroups tend to have less intellec-
solving operations within the context of novel tually stimulating jobs and general life styles,
situations. Speed of responding and spatial so that fluid intelligence declines faster due to
“visualization” skills may be required in cer- “disuse” during the middle-age years; better-
tain tests of fluid intelligence, but are not inte- educated subjects might then catch up due to
gral parts of this ability factor. Rather, these increasing disuse during their retirement years.
might be considered as separate ability factors The latter hypothesis is attractive because it
under the rubric of “information-processing suggests that intellectually stimulating activ-
efficiency” (Shallice, 1988). Horn and Cattell ities may sustain fluid intelligence in old age.
(1966) presented data to suggest that fluid intel- We note once again that these data derive from
ligence develops as the result of biological mat- a cross-sectional design. As such, they demon-
uration and reaches its peak in the late teens strate age-related differences in abilities, but
or early twenties. This form of intelligence is not age-related changes in ability. Longitudinal
expected to deteriorate at a rate that is depen- studies are needed to establish that changes
dent upon various accumulating insults to the have occurred.
CNS that occur during the life span of the indi-
vidual. Thus, decline in performance on tests
Age- and Education-Corrected
such as the Category Test, Block Design, or
Norms
Picture Arrangement is not attributable to age
per se, but to the prevalence of accumulated Clearly, demographic variables such as age, edu-
CNS insults in older groups. Hence, it is pos- cation, and sex should be considered together
sible for healthy older individuals to show very when evaluating neuropsychological test per-
little decline in cognitive functioning. formance. In recent years, norms have been
The results of the study suggest that different published that present data corrected for both
patterns of age-related decline in fluid intel- age and education for several widely used tests.
ligence occur for groups with different educa- The data from the Halstead–Reitan normative
tional levels. Groups at the lowest education data pool described above have been developed
level showed greatest cognitive decline between into a set of norms for an extended version of
the young and middle-age periods. However, the HRB (Heaton et al., 1991). Norms are pre-
the better-educated group tended to “catch up” sented separately for males and females, for 10
by the later age period. At older ages, the level age groups (ages 20–34, then in increments of
of functioning was not much different between 5 years to age 80), crossed with education level
the three education levels. Once again, these (6–8, 9–11, 12, 13–15, 16–17, and 18+ years).
age-by-education interaction effects occurred Heaton (1992) also has developed age, edu-
only on certain test measures, mostly those of cation, and gender corrections for the WAIS-R
the fluid intelligence variety. subtests using the data from the WAIS-R
There are several possible explanations why standardization sample (Wechsler, 1981). The
such age-by-education interaction effects might norms are presented separately for males and
occur. First, subjects with lower education and females, with age groups in increments of 3
lower socioeconomic status may tend to have years, ranging from 18 through 74, crossed with
less optimal health care, resulting in a higher education levels ranging from 0 to 7 years to
prevalence during middle age of health prob- 16-plus years. In a validation sample of 420 sub-
lems that compromise brain function (e.g., high jects, less than 1% of the variance of the resul-
blood pressure). It might also be that people in tant demographically corrected T-scores could
the higher education groups tend to have better- be predicted by the demographic variables in
functioning CNSs to begin with and, as a result, question. Thus, the T-scores were found to be
are more resilient to absolute losses in CNS essentially free of any linear relationship with
integrity or the presence of pathology associ- age, education, or gender, and did not result in
ated with disorders of aging, a notion referred interactions of age-by-education on any WAIS-R
to as cognitive reserve (Stern et al., 2003). A variable. A recent study showed that, while raw
third possibility is that subjects in the lower scores from the WAIS and WAIS-R resulted in
Demographic Influences and Use of Demographically Corrected Norms 139

appreciable differences between two groups of While the authors suggest several explanations,
demographically matched normal subjects, the the larger variance may simply be due to the fact
corrected T-scores showed no differences for the that fewer subjects were included in the 0–4 year
two versions of the test (Thompson et al., 1989). education category (with n values as small as 17
Age, education, gender and ethnicity corrected and 23) than in the high school graduate cat-
norms are available for the WAIS-III/WMS- egory (with most cells containing 200 or more
III, and these have been incorporated within subjects). Although the subject sample was
The Psychological Corporation’s WAIS-III/ selected to reflect the racial distribution of the
WMS-III/WIAT-II Scoring Assistant soft ware 1980 census, the study does not present separate
program. data for racial groups, or even describe the per-
Age- and education-corrected norms have centages of the sample that come from varied
been published for two summary scores of the ethnic backgrounds. It should be emphasized
Benton Visual Retention Test (BVRT; Youngjohn that these are population-based norms; the sur-
et al., 1993), namely, number correct and total vey procedures did not screen out individuals
number of errors. Subjects included 1128 healthy with prior head injury, developmental disorder,
individuals, ranging in age from 17 to 84 years. or current cognitive dysfunction. Thus, at least
Unfortunately, subjects with less than a high some differences in the scores, particularly in
school education were not included in the study; the lowest education group and the oldest age
data are presented for three categories of educa- group, may be due to the inclusion of these sub-
tion, 12–14 years, 15–17 years, and 18-plus years. jects. For example, lower MMSE scores at later
Sex was not considered in the norms since it did ages may partially be attributable to the inclu-
not significantly add to performance prediction sion of elderly persons who are experiencing
in a multiple regression analysis. The BVRT has dementing diseases. Nevertheless, it is clear
good discriminant validity for the purposes of that both age and level of education should be
differentiating dementia from the effects of nor- considered in interpretation of the MMSE0
mal aging (Youngjohn et al., 1992). score.
The Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; Finally, Spreen and Strauss (1998) and
Folstein et al., 1975) is a brief, standardized Mitrushina et al. (2005) have compiled manu-
screening procedure for cognitive impairment als of norms for many other widely used neu-
that is widely used in both research and clinical ropsychological tests. Their purpose has been
settings. A recently published population-based to describe each test, present the best norma-
normative study for the MMSE highlights the tive data available at the time of publication,
importance of both age and education in per- and comment on issues such as the validity and
formance on the MMSE (Crum et al., 1993). The reliability of the test. In some cases an effort
study included 18,571 adult participants tested was made to combine normative samples from
between 1980 and 1984 in five centers across the several sources into one table, thereby increas-
United States. The age, education, and race dis- ing the numbers of subjects in each age group.
tributions of the sample matched those of the Most welcome is the addition, where available,
1980 U.S. census. Norms are provided for age of norms for children. While these efforts are
groups in increments of 5 years from 18 years to laudatory, they cannot make up for the basic
over 85 years, and are stratified by years of edu- deficiencies in the field. It remains the case
cation (0–4, 5–8, 9–12, and college experience that most tests are sorely lacking in adequate
or higher degree). The total score on the MMSE normative data and in information regarding
varied appreciably between educational groups the reliability and discriminant validity of the
and age groups, ranging from a median score of tests. Nevertheless, the above compendia are
19 (out of a possible 30 points) among individu- useful sources of the most recent references
als 85 years and older with 0–4 years of educa- on standardization samples and literature for
tion to a median score of 29 for the youngest currently used tests. The clinician employing
group with more than a high school diploma. a particular test is advised to refer to the cited
Scores were also found to be more variable sources if in doubt about the appropriateness of
among subjects with fewer years of education. the norms.
140 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

An issue of particular importance to consider


Norms for the Very Elderly
when employing norms for the elderly is the
Cognitive change is a typical, if not inevitable, health status of the participants in normative
consequence of aging. Visual spatial ability, studies. Given the frequency of chronic illness in
some aspects of memory, speed of process- adults over the age of 65, excluding elderly sub-
ing, and verbal fluency tend to decline with jects on the basis of current chronic illness such
increasing years, particularly over the age of 70 as diabetes or hypertension would likely result
(Labouvie-Vief, 1985; Poon, 1985). Generally, in a sample that is not representative of the gen-
ability on tasks involving basic skills and over- eral elderly population. This poses a dilemma for
learned information remains stable into very the researcher gathering normative data. Albert
late decades, whereas earlier decline is evident (1981) argues that, on the one hand, the clinician
on tasks that involve manipulation of novel may be concerned with differentiating between
situations or materials (Leckliter & Matarazzo, the cognitive changes related to chronic illness
1989). Generalized slowing of response is the and those related to aging. Systemic disease
most ubiquitous finding across all cognitive such as hypertension or metabolic dysfunction
tasks, although the removal of time limits does can have an adverse impact on cognitive func-
not improve the performances of older persons tions that, in some cases, ameliorates with treat-
to the level of younger adults (Lezak, 1983). ment. Conversely, norms that are based upon
The growing number of the elderly in our data from only healthy older individuals may be
society has increased the demand for special- setting standards that are unrealistically high
ized geriatric medical and psychological ser- for elderly patients. Several projects have dealt
vices in health-care settings. More and more with the issue differently. A normative project at
elderly are being seen in clinics for the assess- the Mayo Clinic (Ivnik et al., 1992b, described
ment of dementing diseases such as Alzheimer’s, below), for example, included in their sample
or for assessment of their ability to continue subjects with chronic illness such as hyperten-
to function independently in the community. sion and diabetes, but whose cognitive capacity
Surprisingly, even for the most thoroughly and daily functioning were not considered to be
developed and comprehensively normed neu- adversely affected by their illness. Alternatively,
ropsychological instruments, virtually none Birren et al. (1963) categorized subjects into
includes norms for the very elderly. Lezak (1987) those who were “optimally healthy” and those
notes that “the dearth of . . . adequate age-graded “with systemic disease.”
norms becomes even more astonishing in light The Spreen and Strauss (1998) and Mitrushina
of the hundreds of published studies on age- et al. (2005) manuals are, once again, worth
related changes for the full range of discrete to mentioning as sources of information on nor-
complex cognitive functions, sensory capacities, mative data for the elderly. In addition, a bibli-
and motor responses” (p. 2). Normative studies ography of articles containing norms for older
that do exist for a given test often obtain discon- persons on a number of neuropsychological
certingly different results. For example, D’Elia instruments is available (Erickson et al., 1992).
et al. (1989) compared studies with normative This bibliography is the result of an extensive
data for the Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS) that search for articles and publications that include
include elderly subjects between the ages of 55 norms for persons over 60 years of age. Studies
and 89 years (including, among others, Haaland with small sample sizes were not included
et al., 1983; Hulicka, 1966; Klonoff & Kennedy, when comparable larger samples were avail-
1966). They found wide discrepancies between able. The studies are tabulated under six catego-
the obtained means and standard deviations in ries: Mental Status Questionnaires, Intellectual
these studies, most likely due to the influence Abilities, Neuropsychological Batteries, Memory
of other important variables, such as educa- Functions, Perceptual Speed and Coordination,
tion (Bak & Greene, 1981), that were not con- and Executive Functions.
sidered. The Duke University study (McCarty As a result of their survey, Erickson et al.
et al., 1982) has identified sex and race as other (1992) make several observations regarding
important variables on the WMS. the state of affairs in the area of norms for the
Demographic Influences and Use of Demographically Corrected Norms 141

elderly. The authors have come to the sobering considered themselves normal, and were con-
conclusion that most of the available norms sidered normal by their primary care physician.
constituted little more than “rules of thumb,” The authors’ criteria for normality included the
particularly for persons over the age of 75, or for following: no active CNS or psychiatric condi-
those individuals who do not fit the description tions, no complaint of cognitive difficulty, no
of the average North American, namely, White, findings on physical examination suggesting a
educated at or beyond the high school level, and disorder with potential to affect cognition, and
middle-class. Too few studies, in their opinion, no psychoactive medication in amounts that
had been concerned with comparing the ability would be expected to compromise cognition.
of various instruments to discriminate between Prior history of disorders potentially affecting
individuals with other demographic character- cognition (e.g., head injury, substance abuse) or
istics who are normal versus those who have current chronic medical illness (e.g., diabetes,
acute neurological conditions or dementing dis- hypertension, cardiac problems) did not auto-
eases. Even fewer studies provide information matically exclude a subject from the study, as
on how people succeed or fail at a task as well as long as the condition was not reported by his
what they achieve and how quickly. Age-related or her physician to compromise cognition.
changes in individual cognitive processes may A major caveat is that the subject sample was
be important diagnostic factors that are not almost exclusively Caucasian with low average
reflected adequately in total scores on complex to superior intellectual functioning, residing in
tests that require the efficient coordination of a predominantly suburban setting with good
multiple abilities. access to health-care facilities. The use of these
norms for an individual with little education,
or whose ethnic or socioeconomic background
The Mayo Older Americans
differs markedly from the normative sample, is
Normative Studies Project
questionable.
(MOANS)
An additional goal of the MOANS project
A major research project that has made a consid- was to provide an extension of the existing nor-
erable contribution to the area of norms for the mative data for WAIS-R IQ scores (Wechsler,
elderly has been undertaken at the Mayo Clinic 1981) for subjects beyond age 74. The WAIS-R
in Rochester, Minnesota. The goal of the project was administered to 222 subjects over the age
is to provide normative data from a large sub- of 74 (range 75–97) and to 290 subjects between
ject sample that is representative of the elderly the ages of 56 and 74. The resultant MOANS
in the community. The MOANS project pro- norms were derived from age groups defined
duced excellent age- and education-corrected by overlapping, midpoint age ranges (Pauker,
norms for the WAIS-R, as well as age-corrected 1988) that maximize the amount of information
norms for the Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised obtained from the normative sample. Despite
and Rey’s Auditory–Verbal Learning Test (Ivnik relatively small sample sizes for each 5-year
et al., 1992a, 1992b, 1992c; Malec et al., 1992). interval, the normative data are presented for
The project is worth describing in some detail, age ranges with midpoints every 3 years from
not only because of the welcome contribution age 61 to age 88, with a 10-year range in each
it makes to the field but because of the excel- group. Thus, an individual can be compared to
lence of the design and the care the authors have the distribution that most closely fits his or her
taken in characterizing the normative sample. age, and the subject sample included in each
The initial MOANS subject sample included distribution is large enough to provide percen-
530 subjects between the ages of 54 and 74. tile information for the individual. The MOANS
The subjects were recruited from the commu- scaled scores and resultant IQs can be further
nity by random solicitation, with a 34% com- corrected on a set of tables for educational level.
pliance rate. All the subjects were cognitively Consistent with the research described earlier,
capable of independent living, although they the authors show that correction for educa-
had a variety of medical conditions common to tion level is most necessary at the extremes. In
the elderly (see Malec et al., 1993). The subjects fact, corrections for education for high school
142 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

graduates or those with up to a few years of col- test performance. The discussion is by no
lege result in little change in scaled scores, and means exhaustive; our goal is to highlight the
are thus unnecessary. pressing need for research in this area and the
A second major contribution of the MOANS development of culture-specific normative
project has been in the area of memory assess- standards for use in neuropsychological clin-
ment. Memory decline is typically associated ical practice. The reader is referred to other
with normal aging (as defined above) but is also sources for a more complete discussion, includ-
an important early manifestation of neurocog- ing Triandis and Berry (1980), Olmedo (1981),
nitive disorders, such as Alzheimer’s disease Reynolds and Brown (1984), Anastasi (1988),
(Poon, 1985). Difficulty with memory is one of Geisinger (1992), Fletcher-Jansen et al. (2000),
the most common complaints among the elderly Ferraro (2001), Anderson et al. (2004), Manly,
on referral for neuropsychological testing, and Jacobs et al. (2002), and Manly (2005). We
its evaluation is a critical component of a neu- should note that our work, and most published
ropsychological assessment. The MOANS pro- observations by others, emanate from experi-
ject has published norms for ages 55–97 on two ence in the United States. The point of view,
widely used memory assessment instruments, and the examples provided, therefore, reflect
the Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised (WMS-R; the North American context. While broader
Wechsler, 1987), and the Rey Auditory–Verbal generalizations might not be appropriate from
Learning Test (AVLT; Rey, 1964). The instruc- some of the specific data, the general principles
tions give precise information for test adminis- should be applicable to other multiethnic and
tration, scoring, and computation of the subtest multicultural settings.
scores. Clinicians should familiarize themselves At issue, when assessing a patient whose heri-
with the instructions in order to use the norms, tage differs from one’s own, is whether adequate
since several important changes in test admin- consideration has been given to the contribu-
istration of the WMS-R were incorporated, so tion of ethnic and cultural factors to test design
that the delayed recall index of the WMS-R can and performance. A culturally sensitive assess-
be computed taking into account the initial ment has been described as one that balances
level of learning (Cullum et al., 1990). The con- the application of general population norms
cordances between the MOANS WMS-R indi- with culture-specific norms (Lopez et al., 1989).
ces and the original WMS-R indices (Wechsler, Unfortunately, not only are normative data non-
1987) were within five points (plus or minus) existent for most ethnic groups on most tests,
for 96.4% of the sample on the verbal index, but in many cases we do not even know on which
90.5% on the visual index, 85.8% on the general tests ethnicity is or is not an important variable
memory index, and 93.8% on the attention/con- to consider. On the other hand, sufficient evi-
centration index. Norms for several summary dence exists to establish that it is a clinical myth
indices of the AVLT are also published (Ivnik that ethnicity is only an important variable on
et al., 1992c). These are an expanded version of language- or knowledge-based tests and that the
the norms previously published by this group effect of culture can be eliminated if only per-
(Ivnik et al., 1990). formance or verbal items were used (for review,
A new, and very significant extension of the see Rosselli & Ardila, 2003). Ethnic background
Mayo Older American Normative Studies pro- is sometimes, but not always, an important
gram has been a separate set of norms for elderly variable even on tests that are supposedly “cul-
African Americans, published in a special issue ture free.” Indeed, researchers have sometimes
of The Clinical Neuropsychologist (2005, 19, pp. observed even larger group differences across
162–269). cultural groups in performance and nonverbal
tests than in verbal tests (Anastasi, 1988; Irvine
& Berry, 1988). For example, Japanese sub-
Ethnicity in Neuropsychological Test
jects do better than American subjects on the
Performance
Porteus Mazes and other spatial tests (Lee et al.,
Finally, we turn to a discussion of the issue of 1991; Porteus, 1959). In contrast, American and
ethnicity as a variable in neuropsychological Italian children perform virtually the same on
Demographic Influences and Use of Demographically Corrected Norms 143

the Judgment of Line Orientation Test (Riva & was clearly not the case for all tests in the battery.
Benton, 1993). On several tests, including the Trail-Making
Whether or not a low test performance is in Test, Location and Memory components of the
part due to cultural influences may depend not Tactual Performance Test, and Finger Tapping,
only on ethnic background but on the individ- similar performances were observed across the
ual’s level of “acculturation,” referring to the groups. In contrast, the Mexican group was sig-
degree to which an individual engages in a par- nificantly slower than either comparison group
ticular culture’s customs, values, beliefs, social on Tactual Performance Test, even when age,
practices, and language (Arnold & Orozco, gender, and SES were controlled for, statisti-
1989). Integration into the North American cally. On the Category Test, Anglo-Americans
cultural setting will depend on numerous fac- scored higher than both Mexican Americans
tors, some of which include the age at which and Mexicans, while on the Seashore Rhythm
the person immigrated, the language of origin, Test, it was the Mexican American group that
the age at which English was learned, and years scored significantly higher than either Mexicans
of education in the American school system. or Anglo-Americans. The authors suggest this
Education and language fluency alone, how- latter finding may be due to an increased sen-
ever, are insufficient to gauge acculturation. sitivity to auditorily processed materials in
Some individuals and some ethnic groups con- bilingual individuals.
tinue to participate fully in the traditions and More recently, Boone et al. (2007) compared
social milieu of their culture, often continuing patients referred for neuropsychological eval-
to speak their own language at home and in uation in four racial groups, Caucasian (non-
social settings. Collectively, these factors have Hispanic), African American, Hispanic, and
a significant influence on the degree to which Asian. Significant group differences were pre-
ethnic differences influence cognition (Arnold sent on some measures of naming, attention
& Orozco, 1989; Gasquoine, 1999; Harris and speed, constructional ability, and executive
et al., 2003; Shuttleworth-Edwards et al., 2004; skills. However, these differences depended on
Touradji et al., 2001). several factors, including the number of years
The importance of considering accultura- in the United States, years of education in the
tion along with ethnicity or race is highlighted United States, the age at which conversational
by recent research with Mexican Americans English was first learned, and whether English
(see also Gonzales & Roll, 1985). For example, was acquired as a second language or learned
in a study by Arnold et al. (1994) three groups concurrently with another language.
of subjects were identified who differed in their Clearly, level of acculturation is a significant
level of North American acculturation: Anglo- factor in neuropsychological test performance,
Americans, Mexican Americans, and Mexicans. and importantly, not always in the direction of
Acculturation was assessed using an instrument “more Anglo-American is better.” However, few
developed by Cuellar et al. (1980; Acculturation objective instruments exist for reliably measur-
Rating Scale for Mexican Americans, ARSMA) ing acculturation among members of different
that considers such elements as oral language ethnic groups. The general paucity of research
usage, historical familial identification, con- in this area means that we simply do not know
tact with Mexico, ease with reading and writing what factors override ethnic differences. Ideally,
Spanish, and generalized perceptions of iden- future normative studies should stratify data
tification with the Mexican culture (Orozco according to cultural factors, rather than race
et al., 1993). The Mexican group was tested alone. In the interim, the best advice remains
using a Spanish translation of the HRB. The to use caution when interpreting the test scores
authors found considerable differences among of an individual from an ethnic minority. In
these three groups on the HRB. On the aggre- addition, clinical decisions based on such data
gate T-score index, the Anglo-American group should be informed, whenever possible, by
scored significantly higher than the Mexican the available literature on acculturation so as
Americans, who in turn scored significantly to avoid mischaracterization of the cognitive
higher than the Mexican group. However, this abilities of the individual, and should include
144 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

appropriate caveats regarding sources of mea- of the WAIS-R. All subtests produced significant
surement error (Pont’on, 2001). differences in favor of Whites, although the dif-
ferences were most prominent on Block Design
and Vocabulary. One common Wechsler short
African Americans and Tests of
form that is used for clinical screening is the
Intelligence
Vocabulary–Block Design dyad (Sattler, 1982).
Reynolds et al. (1987) used the WAIS-R Examiners should recognize that the use of that
standardization sample to investigate the effect dyad as a short form will be particularly penal-
of race and its interactions with education on izing to African Americans.
Verbal, Performance, and Full-Scale IQs (VIQ, The differences in IQ scores between African
PIQ, and FSIQ, respectively). The data from Americans and Whites exist even when scores
1664 Whites and 192 African Americans were are corrected for age, years of education, and
broken into four education categories: 0–8 sex. When Heaton’s (1992) T-scores corrected
years, 9–11 years, 12 years, and 13–16 years. Not for age, sex, and education are computed for
surprisingly, the FSIQs of individuals with a the WAIS-R standardization sample, the differ-
college education were two standard deviations ences among ethnic/racial groups are smaller
higher than those with minimal formal school- than those described by Kaufman’s group but
ing. Across all levels of education, a difference of remain appreciable (see Table 7–2).
about one standard deviation in favor of Whites The significance of these ethnicity differ-
over African Americans was found. The pattern ences for diagnostic decision making in neuro-
of results was similar for VIQ and PIQ measures. psychology is substantial. Heaton et al. (2003)
The overall difference of one standard deviation demonstrated this using data from the WAIS-
in IQ scores is consistent with previous research III/WMS-III standardization sample, stratified
(for review, see Reynolds & Brown, 1984). The by ethnicity. By applying a 1-SD cutoff with
increases in IQ for African Americans across factor scores corrected for age, education, and
educational levels were substantial but tended sex, these authors observed false-positive error
to be smaller than corresponding differences for rates for the African American cohort, which
Whites, although this interaction was not sta- were clearly excessive for all six factors: Verbal
tistically significant. African Americans with at Comprehension (36% versus 11% for Whites),
least 1 year of college scored 15–16.5 IQ points Perceptual Organization (37% versus 12%),
higher than those with 0–8 years of education; Processing Speed (38% versus 12%), Working
for Whites, the IQ differences were between Memory (37% versus 11%), Auditory Memory
18.5 and 24 IQ points; these larger education (30% versus 13%), and Visual Memory (25% ver-
effects for Whites than African Americans on sus 14%). Similar discrepancies have been seen
cognitive tests would be consistent with evi- for the HRB and the California Verbal Learning
dence that many African Americans histori- Test when a single set of norms was applied to
cally have experienced poorer education quality test scores of large samples of normal, African
despite similar years of education (Manly et al., American and Caucasian adults (Heaton et al.,
2002). Similar ethnicity effects were reported by 2004; Norman et al., 2000). Importantly, in each
Kaufman et al. (1988) on the individual subtests of these publications it was also demonstrated

Table 7–2. WAIS-R Mean Verbal IQ (VIQ), Performance IQ (PIQ), and Full
Scale IQ (FSIQ) Expressed as T scores, for Adult (age 20+) Standardization
Sample Subjects in Four Ethnic Groups, Computed Using the Heaton (1992)
Age-, Education-, and Sex- Corrected Norms
Total Sample White Black Hispanic Other
(n=1680) (n=1461) (n=166) (n=34) (n=19)
VIQ 50.0 50.7 44.6 45.9 45.5
PIQ 50.0 50.8 43.8 47.4 46.2
FSIQ 50.1 51.0 43.8 45.8 45.6
Demographic Influences and Use of Demographically Corrected Norms 145

that use of ethnicity corrections for the test some 29 million, and Hispanic Americans are
norms eliminated the previously observed dis- the fastest-growing minority group in the United
crepancies in false-positive error rates between States (U.S. Census Bureau, 2004). The popula-
Caucasians and African Americans. tion is composed primarily of Latin American
The source of these differences has been the immigrant families from Mexico, Puerto Rico,
subject of intense and often bitter debate. Recent Cuba, and Colombia. For first-generation immi-
evidence supports the notion that ethnic differ- grants, limited English language skills may
ences on intellectual testing (and undoubtedly preclude the use of many tests, regardless of
other neuropsychological measures as well) whether or not culture-specific norms exist. In
are a barometer of educational, economic, and these cases, several Spanish translations of neu-
environmental opportunity. Manly et al. (2002) ropsychological instruments are available that
found that cognitive test score differences were standardized with Spanish-speaking pop-
between education matched groups of White ulations. The most widely used of these tests is
and African American elders were greatly the Spanish translation of the WAIS, the Escala
attenuated when a measure of education qual- de Inteligencia Wechsler para Adultos (EIWA;
ity (reading level) was covaried in the analyses. Wechsler, 1968), standardized on a large sample
Also, Vincent (1991) has shown that IQ differ- of Spanish-speaking adults from Puerto Rico.
ences between African American and White Lopez and Romero (1988) examined the compa-
children in U.S. schools are shrinking. He rability of the EIWA and the WAIS (Wechsler,
reviewed pre-1980 and post-1980 studies com- 1955). They found that in the conversion of raw
paring the performance of African Americans scores to scaled scores, performance on a given
and Whites on various tests of intellectual func- subtest can result in a scaled score difference
tioning. Studies with adults included such tests of up to 5 points, depending on whether one
as the WAIS, WAIS-R, and Raven’s Progressive applies the WAIS or EIWA norms. For instance,
Matrices; studies with children employed the a raw score of 16 on the Object Assembly, a sub-
WISC-R, Raven’s Colored Progressive Matrices, test that is identical on the WAIS and EIWA,
the Kaufman-ABC, and the Stanford-Binet. translates into a scaled score of 5 for the WAIS
African American adults consistently obtained and 10 for the EIWA. Lopez and Romero (1988)
mean test scores that were approximately one conclude that for some Spanish-speaking
standard deviation below the U.S. general pop- adults, particularly for those with low educa-
ulation mean in studies carried out both before tional levels, low occupational status, and rural
and after 1980. In contrast, although African backgrounds, the EIWA may be most appropri-
American children tested prior to 1980 also ate, because this was the combined background
scored one standard deviation below the U.S. of most of the EIWA standardization sample
mean, studies conducted after 1980 obtained subjects. In interpreting EIWA scores, however,
group differences that were, on average, less it is important to note that they reflect ade-
than half as large. In one study (Krohn & Lamp, quacy of performance with reference to a par-
1989), no differences were found between ticular subject sample that differs considerably
African American and White children on the from the general American population and the
Kaufman-ABC and the Stanford-Binet when general Hispanic population within the united
socioeconomic factors were controlled. One States. Thus, if “Spanish speaking” is the only
logical explanation for these findings is that the criterion for deciding to use the norms, persons
shrinking ethnicity differences in intellectual with higher educational and occupational back-
test scores of children mirrors improved equity grounds than the EIWA standardization sam-
in education quality for Whites and African ple will produce overestimates of actual versus
Americans in recent years. expected intellectual functioning using this
instrument.
Boone et al. (2007) has compiled addi-
Norms for Spanish-speaking Adults
tional normative data for Hispanic, African
The United States has the fift h largest Spanish- American, and Asian individuals who were
speaking population in the world, numbering referred for neuropsychological assessment to
146 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

a public hospital-affi liated neuropsychology Status Exam (MMSE; Folstein et al., 1975).
clinic. The test battery included standard neu- Uncorrected, MMSE scores are biased against
ropsychological tests that were administered in individuals with lower levels of education, and
English, such as the WAIS-III, WMS-R, Boston the use of scoring adjustments for lower edu-
Naming Test, FAS, and Wisconsin Card Sorting cational levels has become standard practice.
Test. Test scores are stratified for individuals Rosselli and colleagues (2006) compared MMSE
with different levels of language acculturation. total scores on 102 unimpaired and 58 memory
These authors also provide an extensive list of impaired Hispanic individuals (ages 54–98),
other recent studies providing normative data stratified by education level and gender. They
for Hispanics, African Americans, and Asians also compared total MMSE scores that were cal-
on similar tests. culated using two approved forms of the tests,
Rosselli and colleagues (Rosselli et al., 1990, one that included the original “serial 7’s” item,
2000) have made significant contributions to and one that replaced serial 7’s with an appar-
the area of language assessment for Spanish- ently less difficult “backwards spelling” task.
speaking adults. Rosselli et al. (1990) com- For all the studied groups, MMSE scores were
piled normative data for the Boston Diagnostic higher by an average of 1.5 points when using
Aphasia Examination (BDAE)—Spanish ver- the “backwards spelling” task, and the correla-
sion (Garcia-Albea et al., 1986). A sample of tion between serial 7’s and backwards spelling
180 native Spanish speakers was obtained in was a modest .37. Both education and gender
Bogota, Colombia, and stratified according to explained significant amount of variance in the
sex, age (16–30, 31–50, and 51–65), and edu- MMSE scores. The authors note that the “back-
cation (0–5, 6–12, and 13 years or more). The wards spelling” and “serial 7’s” items do not
results are consistent with the data from U.S. appear to be equivalent in difficulty when test-
White/Anglophone subject samples (such as ing Hispanic older adults.
Borod et al., 1980) showing that the BDAE is
most markedly sensitive to education differ-
Questions about Use of
ences but is affected by age as well. The authors
Demographically Corrected
provide means and standard deviations for age
Norms in Neuropsychology
and education subgroups, together with cutoff
scores for normal performance on each subtest
When Should We Use Them
of the BDAE by years of education. The norms
and for What Purpose?
may be particularly useful for assessing pre-
dominantly Spanish-speaking subjects whose Demographically corrected norms are useful for
language functioning is in question. While comparing current test performance with what
these norms were collected in Colombia, more would be expected of the patient if he/she had no
recently, Rosselli et al. (2000) has obtained nor- brain disorder. Of course, the best way to mea-
mative data for older adults (ages 50–84) who sure possible changes in neurobehavioral func-
are Spanish bilinguals, Spanish monolinguals, tioning is to have premorbid baseline testing.
and English speakers on a variety of language Unfortunately, this is virtually never available.
tests, including Boston Naming Test, FAS, The next best option is to compare the patient’s
semantic fluency, and the Boston Diagnostic current test performances with best estimates
Aphasia Exam. of normal expectations for people who are as
As the number of Hispanic individuals within similar as possible to the patient in all charac-
the United States grows, the number of Hispanic teristics that relate to test performance. This is
elders continues to rise as well. The need for quite simply, a prediction problem. And it is a
culturally normed screening tools to assess fact that demographic characteristics help pre-
age-related cognitive impairment and early dict performances on our available neuropsy-
dementia is highlighted by a recent study show- chological tests.
ing that elderly Hispanics are more likely than Use of demographic predictions for this pur-
Europeans to be identified as demented (Espino pose may seem objectionable, because we know
et al., 2001) when assessed with the Mini Mental that demographics, themselves, do not cause
Demographic Influences and Use of Demographically Corrected Norms 147

differences in test performance. Rather, they are that causes the observed differences. Rather, a
surrogates for a complex array of causative fac- variety of complex factors that tend to be asso-
tors that are more difficult to reliably measure ciated with age are in various combinations
and use in the norming process. We need to be responsible for age-related differences in cogni-
clear about this: just as correlation coefficients tion. These factors obviously are important to
do not tell us anything about cause-effect rela- study and understand, and perhaps we eventu-
tionships, demographic adjustments of neuro- ally can measure them in norming to improve
psychological test norms do not imply anything predictions of which 65-year-olds are likely to
about the root causes of test performance dif- show more or less age-related cognitive change.
ferences that are associated with demographics. For example, factors like diet and exercise,
More will be said about this below in relation to exposure to adverse environmental influences,
individual demographic predictors. frequency with which cognitive abilities are
It is generally understood that demographi- used in everyday functioning, and various age-
cally corrected normative standards are based related health conditions (e.g., hypertension)
upon performances of adults who have devel- may all be important. Even genetics, which may
oped normally, have typical, mainstream educa- help predict successful and unsuccessful cogni-
tional backgrounds, and have no known history tive aging, most likely do not cause such differ-
of brain injury or disease. It follows logically ences but operate by increasing the likelihood
from this that such norms should be used with of biological processes, including illnesses, that
great caution, if at all, to identify acquired brain also correlate with age.
dysfunctions in patients who have developmen- So, when we correct for age, we are using it
tal disorders or other-than-mainstream educa- as a proxy for all these other factors that argu-
tional backgrounds (e.g., special education). For ably are more important but that also are much
example, it would be inappropriate to “adjust” a more difficult to evaluate as joint predictors of
mentally retarded person’s IQ upward because test performance in individual cases. On the
of a low education level, thereby potentially other hand, the proxy of age is readily assessed,
depriving him/her of social services or mitigat- it clearly helps in the prediction process, and
ing considerations in criminal prosecution. psychologists have no compunctions about
As we have noted, demographically corrected using it.
norms are used primarily to identify the presence
and nature of neurobehavioral changes due to
Normative Adjustments for Education
known or possible brain insult (injury or disease).
Such norms are generally not the best choice for Education, too, is a useful but very complex
characterizing the individual’s absolute level of predictor of cognitive performance. There is
functioning, or functioning in relation to the no doubt that it helps predict performance on
general population of normal adults (Heaton & most of our tests, yet direct causal links are
Pendleton, 1981). For the latter, we recommend tenuous. To a very limited extent, people with
norms that compare the individual’s test perfor- more education do better on our tests because
mance with results of a sample that conforms as they learned the answers in school. Perhaps
closely as possible to census characteristics. For more importantly, school experiences (espe-
example, with the Heaton et al. (2004) norms for cially early ones) teach basic academic skills
the expanded HRB, uncorrected scaled scores and teach how to learn, how to use new facts
(not demographically corrected T-scores) would and skills to solve problems, and how to take
be best suited for this purpose. tests. Also, school experiences may or may not
reinforce these activities as positive features of
peoples’ lifestyles. Education also is a compo-
Normative Adjustments for Age
nent of SES, which itself is complex and mul-
We have a long history of using age-corrected tifaceted, and years of education completed is
norms in neuropsychology. Noone would use a proxy for much of that complexity. There is
the same normative standards for a 25-year-old also a tendency for more cognitively able people
and a 65-year-old. However, it is not age per se to go farther in school: IQ in the second grade
148 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

is a significant predictor of how many years reading (e.g., dementing illnesses, or strokes
of school a person ultimately will complete or injuries affecting reading or language func-
(McCall, 1977). But SES and related experiences tions). Even if reading-corrected norms did
certainly affect early IQ measurements, just as exist, considerable clinical judgment may be
they do later in life. In adults, therefore, educa- needed to decide when to use them.
tion is a proxy for all of these factors. It may not be enough to show that current
There is no doubt that quality of education reading level correlates with performance on
is important, but this too is complex and may other neuropsychological tests in normals, or
be difficult to assess retrospectively in adult even that concurrent consideration of read-
patients (Byrd et al., 2006). Resources devoted ing scores reduces predictive value of demo-
to educational systems available to different graphic variables such as education or ethnicity.
segments of the U.S. population have varied Since current reading levels can be affected by
greatly, especially for African Americans dur- some neurologic conditions, and demographic
ing the era of segregation (Manly et al., 2002). variables are not, we need to know how much
Although this situation has improved some- reading scores can add to predictions based
what, it would be difficult to argue that the qual- upon demographic variables alone. Gladsjo
ity of a high school education in poor inner-city et al. (1999) considered this question using data
or rural areas of the United States is equivalent from an ethnically mixed sample of 141 healthy
to that experienced in wealthier suburbs or pri- adults, on WAIS-R IQs, the Average Impairment
vate schools. Also, because quality of education Rating from the HRB, and composite measures
in the United States has been associated with of learning and memory. Scores on the American
other complex determinants of cognitive devel- National Adult Reading Test (ANART) did sub-
opment (i.e., everything that goes with SES), stantially add to the demographic predictions
subgroup differences in cognitive outcome of Verbal IQ, Full-Scale IQ and learning, but
might not immediately disappear even if we provided little help in predicting Performance
could achieve complete equality in educational IQ, the Average Impairment Rating or memory.
experiences and opportunities. The major advantage in using reading scores
As was the case with age, years of education was with people who had at least 13 years of
completed is an easily obtained proxy for multi- education and relatively poor reading.
ple factors that have more direct (causal) effects Thus, for some patients and for some neu-
but also are more difficult to measure and use in ropsychological measures, it may well be help-
prediction models—probably including qual- ful to have norms that are adjusted for current
ity of education. In general, people with better- reading levels as well as demographics. To do
quality education through high school are more this properly, we need to co-norm all of our
likely to go to college and beyond. neuropsychological tests with a reading test
It has been proposed by Manly et al. (2002)— (ideally the same reading test). This has not
and others as well—that one’s current oral read- typically been done and, to our knowledge,
ing level may be a good index of the quality of no reading-adjusted norms are available at the
his/her prior education. Although reading level present time. However, we do recommend that
is easily assessed and may in fact have poten- a standard reading test be included in future
tial value in neuropsychological norms devel- norming projects.
opment, this has both conceptual and practical
complexities. Conceptually, reading level is mul-
Normative Adjustments for Gender
tiply- determined and not simply a measure of
quality of education. It is probably associated At least in the United States and other Western
with many other aspects of SES, and obviously countries, gender does not currently appear to
relates to years of education completed. From be very confounded with major experiential or
a practical point of view, if the goal of testing biological factors that affect cognition. Since
is to detect acquired brain impairment, such gender is not a proxy for powerful and com-
norms could not be used with patients who may plex determinants of cognitive performance,
have any disease or injury that could affect oral it ends up contributing little or nothing to the
Demographic Influences and Use of Demographically Corrected Norms 149

demographic predictions of performance on cultures and to the very nature of the concept
most neuropsychological tests. Nevertheless, of time (Munn, 1992). American children are
we see no reason to leave gender out of norma- exposed to timed tests from an early age, which
tive models in which it does add to prediction reinforces the value that the faster the perfor-
accuracy (e.g., expectations are slightly lower mance the better the result, while other cultures
for males on WAIS-III/WMS-III Processing place more value on accuracy and attention to
Speed and measures of episodic memory, but detail rather than speed (Nell, 2000; Puente &
are higher for males on the Finger Tapping Test Agranovich, 2003). Neuropsychological test-
and Hand Dynamometer test of grip strength). ing itself can be construed by some individu-
als as invasive and inappropriate. For example,
Dingfelder (2005) notes that, when dealing with
Normative Adjustments for Ethnicity
individuals from Latin countries, having the
Unlike gender, ethnicity in our time, and in examiner share details of their personal lives
Western society, is confounded with many com- would be considered an appropriate way to
plex and interacting background factors that make the client more comfortable and welcome,
can affect cognitive test performance. Although but this would likely be an unwelcome level of
the background factors that directly cause test intimacy for most Americans. But asking the
performances among different ethnic groups direct question, “Are you feeling depressed?”
are extremely important, and worthy of careful to the same Latin individual may be regarded
study, research so far suggests that they are dif- an inappropriate invasion of privacy. It is
ficult to assess and quantify retrospectively in unlikely that these (and other) social variables
adults (Byrd et al., 2006). In the absence of cur- can ever be captured adequately in normative
rently available methods for quantifying such data, because they depend upon the interac-
factors retrospectively and for using them to tion between the cultural values and individual
predict cognitive test performances in individ- characteristics and behaviors of both the client
ual cases, one can at least avoid excessive false- and the clinician.
positive error rates by using norms that are In sum, all of the demographic character-
based upon data from normal people who are istics that predict neurocognitive test perfor-
demographically (including ethnically) simi- mance are proxies for other more direct, causal
lar to the patient. As Manly (2005) has stated, influences—influences that are very complex
“Increased specificity of cognitive measures is and difficult to objectively assess in individ-
an unmistakable benefit of proper use of nor- ual patients. Nevertheless, use of these readily
mative information . . .” (p. 271). available proxy corrections does account for
It is important to note, however, that culture, substantial amounts of variance in the cogni-
values and beliefs can influence neuropsycho- tive test performance of normals. As a result,
logical testing in subtle but important ways demographic corrections substantially improve
that are difficult to quantify. For clinicians, it is predictions of expected test performance in
important to keep in mind that cognitive test- adults who do not have brain disease. Such
ing represents a social situation that is governed norms provide much more equal probability of
by implicit rules that are not shared by all cul- the patient’s being correctly classified as nor-
tures (Ardila, 2005). To cite several examples, mal or abnormal, regardless of whether he/she
the degree to which the patient is willing to is young or old, a high school dropout or college
obey instructions may depend on the personal graduate, male or female, or White, Black, or
characteristics of the examiner relative to the Hispanic. For diagnostic questions, this is the
examinee, such as differences in age, gender, major purpose of norms.
ethnicity, and class or caste. The notion of doing
one’s best on a test may be important to individ-
How to Assess Validity of
uals from a culture that values competition, but
Demographically Corrected Norms
not in less competitive societies. Speeded tests
may be particularly problematic because of dif- We have stated that demographically corrected
ference in attitudes to timed procedures across norms should be most useful in detecting and
150 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

characterizing acquired brain disorders. Is it


Summary
therefore reasonable to expect that, compared
to uncorrected norms, they will show greater The adequate assessment of an individual’s
overall specificity (accuracy in classifying the cognitive functioning entails a knowledge of
normal population as normal)? Probably not. the demographic variables that relate to neu-
As previously noted, demographic corrections ropsychological test performance. Many of the
should ensure equal probability of being cor- assumptions that clinicians and researchers
rectly classified as normal, regardless of one’s sometimes make, such as that Finger Tapping
demographic characteristics (age, education, is not associated with education, that “nonver-
gender, ethnicity). Uncorrected norms are likely bal” or “performance” tests are culture free, and
to have relatively high specificity with young, that matching on age and education level is suf-
well-educated, and mostly Caucasian groups ficient to equate performance on tests between
but much lower specificity with people who are people of different ethnic backgrounds, are
older, more poorly educated, and having ethnic not warranted. Whenever a subject differs in
minority backgrounds. If the overall popula- important characteristics from the “average”
tion has all of these kinds of people, in relatively person in a set of published norms, caution is
balanced proportions, specificity figures for the the best advice. For a 60-year-old woman with
total groups could be the same for demographi- a grade school education, whose first language
cally corrected and uncorrected norms (Heaton was Spanish and who began learning English at
et al., 1996, 2004). age 15, the most that neuropsychological tests
What about sensitivity to disease? The same will tell us for sure is whether and how much
concepts apply. Sensitivity is likely to vary she differs from the average American. They
according to demographics with uncorrected will not tell us whether she has experienced a
norms, and much less so with demographically decline in functioning, in what area this decline
corrected norms (Heaton et al., 1996). The per- has occurred, or, if decline has occurred, what
centages of people in the total population who impact it will have on her ability to care for her-
are classified as “impaired” could be quite simi- self and her family in the community. In this
lar. Again, the expected benefit of the corrected case, it is particularly important to consider the
norms is to have equivalent sensitivity as well test results within the context of a detailed and
as specificity, regardless of the demographics of comprehensive history from her family about
the people being assessed. how she copes with the demands of her daily life
Will corrected and uncorrected norms usually and the changes, if any, that have occurred in
disagree in their classification outcomes? Not recent days, weeks, or years. Also, the interpre-
necessarily. To the extent that patients’ demo- tation of initial neuropsychological results will
graphic characteristics approximate the aver- necessarily rely more on consideration of pattern
age member of the adult population (or at least features (e.g., right–left differences on sensory-
the average member of the normative sample), perceptual, motor, and psychomotor tests) than
the classifications are likely to be the same. The on level of performance, because the expected
largest differences will occur at the extremes of values for the latter are uncertain. On the other
the demographic spectra (e.g., old versus young, hand, interpreting major changes across suc-
very low versus high educational level). cessive neuropsychological evaluations is less
Finally, when comparing diagnostic results dependent on baseline norms and can contrib-
with corrected and uncorrected norms, it is ute to more secure diagnostic inferences.
most meaningful to use a constant test score Regardless of whether an individual does or
cutoff in all cases (e.g., T-score < 40 or standard does not match the demographic characteristics
score < 85). This is similar to the way scores of the normative samples, the interpretation of
would be interpreted clinically. Use of statisti- neuropsychological test scores will ultimately
cally determined, optimal cutoffs for a particu- depend on multiple factors, including one’s def-
lar group or subgroup (e.g., Strong et al., 2005) inition of normal and abnormal, the goal and
may obscure differences that would be seen in context of the assessment, the relationship of
the clinical situation. current test performance and daily functioning
Demographic Influences and Use of Demographically Corrected Norms 151

to prior levels of functioning, together with a Buffery, A. W., & Gray, J. A. (1972). Sex differences
consideration of the likely impact of the results in the development of spatial and linguistic skills.
on diagnostic and treatment decisions. As with In C. Ounsted and D. C. Taylor (Eds.), Gender dif-
all clinical information, judgment is required ferences: Their ontogeny and significance (pp. 123–
157). Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins.
to weigh the importance of a false-positive or
Byrd, D. A., Miller, S. W., Reilly, J., Weber, S., Wall,
false-negative error.
T. L., & Heaton, R. K. (2006). Early environmen-
tal factors, ethnicity, and adult cognitive test
References performance. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 20,
243–260.
Albert, M. S. (1981). Geriatric neuropsychology. Cattell, R. B. (1963). Theory of fluid and crystallized
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49, intelligence: A critical experiment. Journal of
835–850. Educational Psychology, 54, 1–22.
Albert, M. S., & Heaton, R. K. (1988). Intelligence Crum, R. M., Anthony, J. C., Bassett, S. S., & Folstein,
testing. In M. S. Albert and M. B. Moss (Eds.), M. F. (1993). Population-based norms for the Mini-
Geriatric neuropsychology (pp. 13–32). New York: Mental State Examination by age and educational
Guilford Press. level. Journal of the American Medical Association,
Anastasi, A. (1988). Psychological testing, 6th ed. New 269, 2386–2391.
York: Macmillan. Cuellar, I., Harris, L. C., & Jasso, R. (1980). An
Anderson, N. A., Bulatao, R. A., & Cohen, B. (Eds.). acculturation scale for Mexican American nor-
(2004). Critical perspectives on racial and ethnic mal and clinical populations. Hispanic Journal of
differentials in health in late life. Washington, DC: Behavioral Sciences, 2, 199–217.
National Academics Press. Cullum, C. M., Butters, N., Troster, A. I., & Salmon,
Ardila, A. (2005). Cultural values underlying psycho- D. P. (1990). Normal aging and forgetting rates on
metric cognitive testing. Neuropsychology Review, the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised. Archives of
15, 185–195. Clinical Neuropsychology, 5, 23–30.
Arnold, B. R., Montgomery, G. T., Castaneda, I., & D’Elia, L., Satz, P., & Schretlen, D. (1989). Wechsler
Longoria, R. (1994). Acculturation and perfor- Memory Scale: A critical appraisal of the norma-
mance of Hispanics on selected Halstead–Reitan tive studies. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
neuropsychological tests. Assessment, 1, 239–248. Neuropsychology, 11, 551–568.
Arnold, B. R., & Orozco, S. (1989). Physical dis- Dingfelder, S. F. (2005). Closing the gap for Latino
ability, acculturation, and family interaction patients. Monitor of Psychology, 36, 58–61.
among Mexican Americans. Journal of Applied Erickson, R. C., Eimon, P., & Hebben, N. (1992). A bib-
Rehabilitation Counseling, 20, 28–32. liography of normative articles on cognition tests
Bak, J. S., & Greene, R. L. (1981). A review of the for older adults. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 6,
performance of aged adults on various Wechsler 98–102.
Memory Scale subtests. Journal of Clinical Espino, D. V., Lichtenstein, M. D., Palmer, R. F.,
Psychology, 37, 186–188. & Hazuda, H. P. (2001). Ethnic differences in
Birren, J. E., Butler, R. N., Greenhouse, S. W., Sokoloff, Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) scores:
L., & Yarrow, M. R. (1963). Human aging: A biolog- Where you live makes a difference. Journal of the
ical and behavioral study. Washington, DC: U.S. American Geriatric Society, 49, 538–548.
Government Printing Office. Ferraro, F.R. (Ed.). (2001). Minority and cross-cultural
Birren, J. E., & Morrison, D. F. (1961). Analysis of aspects of African Americans. Lisse, Netherlands:
WAIS subtests in relation to age and education. Swets and Zeitlinger.
Journal of Gerontology, 16, 363–369. Finlayson, M. A., Johnson, K. A., & Reitan, R. M.
Boone, K. B., Victor, T. L., Wen, J., Razani, J., & Ponton, (1977). Relationship of level of education to neu-
M. (2007). The association between neuropsycho- ropsychological measures in brain-damaged and
logical scores and ethnicity, language, and accul- non-brain-damaged adults. Journal of Consulting
turation variables in a large patient population. and Clinical Psychology, 45, 536–542.
Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22, 355–365. Fletcher-Janzen, E., Strickland, T. L., & Reynolds,
Borod, J. C., Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (1980). C. R. (2000). Handbook of cross-cultural neuro-
Normative data on the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia psychology. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum
Examination and the Boston Naming Test. Journal Publishers.
of Clinical Neuropsychology, 2, 209–215. Flynn, J. R. (1984). The mean IQ of Americans:
Botwinick, J. (1967). Cognitive processes in maturity Massive gains 1932 to 1978. Psychological Bulletin,
and old age. New York: Springer. 95, 29–51.
152 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

Flynn, J. R. (1987). Massive IQ gains in 14 nations: assessment of neuropsychiatric disorders (pp. 100–
What IQ tests really measure. Psychological 120). New York: Oxford University Press.
Bulletin, 101, 171–191. Heaton, R. K., Grant, I., & Matthews, C. G. (1991).
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. Comprehensive norms for an expanded Halstead–
(1975). Mini-Mental State: A practical method for Reitan Battery: Demographic corrections, research
grading the cognitive state of patients for the clini- findings, and clinical applications. Odessa, FL:
cian. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189–198. Psychological Assessment Resources.
Fromm-Auch, D., & Yeudall, L. T. (1983). Normative Heaton, R. K., Matthews, C. G., Grant, I., & Avitable,
data for the Halstead–Reitan Neuropsychological N. (1996). Demographic corrections with com-
Tests. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 5, prehensive norms: An overzealous attempt or a
221–238. good start? Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Garcia-Albea, J. E., Sanchez-Bernardos, M. L., & Neuropsychology, 18(3), 449–458.
del Viso-Pabon, S. (1986). Test de Boston para el Heaton, R. K., Miller, W., Taylor, M. J., & Grant, I. (2004).
diagnostico de la afasia: Adaptacion Espanola. In Revised comprehensive norms for an expanded
H. Goodglass and E. Kaplan (Eds.), La Evaluacion Halstead–Reitan Battery: Demographically adjusted
de la Afasia y de Transtornos Relacionados (2nd norms for African American and Caucasian adults.
ed., pp. 129–198) (translated by Carlos Wernicke). Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.
Madrid: Editorial Medica Panamericana. Heaton, R. K., & Pendleton, M. G. (1981). Use of
Gasquoine, P. (1999). Variables moderating cultural neuropsychological tests to predict adult patients’
and ethnic differences in neuropsychological everyday functioning. Journal of Consulting and
assessment: The case of Hispanic Americans. The Clinical Psychology, 49, 807–821.
Clinical Neuropsychologist, 13, 376–383. Heaton, R. K., Taylor, M. J., & Manly, J. (2003).
Geisinger, K. F. (1992). Psychological testing of his- Demographic effects and use of demographically
panics. Washington, DC: American Psychological corrected norms with the WAIS-III and WMS- III.
Association. In D. S. Tulsky, G. C. Chelune, R. J. Irnik,
Gladsjo, J. A., Heaton, R. K., Palmer, B. W., Taylor, A. Prifitera, D. H. Saklofske, R. K. Heaton, et al.
M. J., & Jeste, D. V. (1999). Use of oral reading to (Eds.), Clinical interpretation of the WAIS-III and
estimate premorbid intellectual and neuropsycho- WMS-III (pp. 181–210). San Diego, CA: Academic
logical functioning. Journal of the International Press.
Neuropsychological Society, 5(3), 247–254. Horn, J. L., & Cattell, R. B. (1966). Age differences
Gonzales, R. R., & Roll, S. (1985). Relationship in primary mental ability factors. Journal of
between acculturation, cognitive style, and intel- Gerontology, 21, 210–220.
ligence. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology, 16, Hulicka, I. M. (1966). Age differences in Wechsler
190–205. Memory Scale scores. Journal of Genetic
Haaland, K. Y., Linn, R. T., Hunt, W. C., & Goodwin, Psychology, 109, 135–145.
J. S. (1983). A normative study of Russell’s variant Irvine, S. H., & Berry, J. W. (Eds.). (1988). Human
of the Wechsler Memory Scale in a healthy elderly abilities in cultural context. New York: Cambridge
population. Journal of Consulting and Clinical University Press.
Psychology, 51, 878–881. Ivnik, R. J., Malec, J. F., Smith, G. E., Tangalos, E.
Harris, J. G., Tulsky, D. S., & Schultheis, M. T. (2003). G., Petersen, R. C., Kokmen, E., et al. (1992a).
Assessment of the non-native English speaker: Mayo’s older Americans normative studies:
Assimilating history and research fi ndings to WAIS-R norms for ages 56 to 97. The Clinical
guide clinical practice. In S. D. Tulsky, D. H. Neuropsychologist, 6(suppl.), 1–30.
Saklofske, G. J. Chelune, R. K. Heaton, R. J. Ivnik, Ivnik, R. J., Malec, J. F., Smith, G. E., Tangalos, E.
R. Bornstein, et al. (Eds.), Clinical Interpretation G., Petersen, R. C., Kokmen, E., et al. (1992b).
of the WAIS-III and WMS-III (pp. 343–390). San Mayo’s older Americans normative studies:
Diego, CA: Academic Press. WMS-R norms for ages 56 to 94. The Clinical
Heaton, R. K. (1992). Comprehensive norms for an Neuropsychologist, 6(suppl.), 49–82.
expanded Halstead–Reitan Battery: A supple- Ivnik, R. J., Malec, J. F., Smith, G. E., Tangalos, E. G.,
ment for the WAIS-R. Odessa, FL: Psychological Petersen, R. C., Kokmen, E., et al. (1992c). Mayo’s
Assessment Resources. older Americans normative studies: Updated
Heaton, R. K., Grant, I., & Matthews, C. G. (1986). AVLT norms for ages 56 to 97. The Clinical
Differences in neuropsychological test perfor- Neuropsychologist, 6(suppl.), 83–104.
mance associated with age, education, and sex. In I. Ivnik, R. J., Malec, J. F., Tangalos, E. G., Petersen,
Grant and K. M. Adams (Eds.), Neuropsychological R. C., Kokmen, E., & Kurland, L. T. (1990). The
Demographic Influences and Use of Demographically Corrected Norms 153

Auditory–Verbal Learning Test (AVLT): Norms Manly, J. J. (2005). Advantages and disadvantages
for ages 55 and older. Psychological Assessment, 2, of separate norms for African Americans. The
304–312. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 19(2), 270–275.
Kaufman, A. S., McLean, J. E., & Reynolds, C. R. Manly, J.J., Jacobs, D. M., Touradji, P., Small, S. S.,
(1988). Sex, race, residence, region, and education & Stern, Y. (2002). Reading level attenuates dif-
differences on the 11 WAIS-R subtests. Journal of ferences in neuropsychological test performance
Clinical Psychology, 44, 231–248. between African American and White elders.
Klonoff, H., & Kennedy, M. (1966). A comparative Journal of the International Neuropsychological
study of cognitive functioning in old age. Journal Association, 8, 341–348.
of Gerontology, 21, 239–243. Matarazzo, J. D. (1972). Wechsler’s measurement and
Krohn, E. J., & Lamp, R. E. (1989). Concurrent valid- appraisal of adult intelligence. Baltimore: Williams
ity of the Stanford-Binet Fourth Edition and and Wilkins.
K-ABC for Head Start children. Journal of School Matarazzo, J. D., Bornstein, R. A., McDermott, P. A.,
Psychology, 27, 59–67. & Noonan, J. V. (1986). Verbal IQ vs. performance
Labouvie-Vief, G. (1985). Intelligence and cognition. IQ difference scores in males and females from the
In J. E. Birren and K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook WAIS-R standardization sample. Journal of Clinical
of the psychology of aging (2nd ed.; pp. 500–530). Psychology, 42, 965–974.
New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold Company. Matarazzo, J. D., & Herman, D. O. (1984).
Leckliter, I. N., & Matarazzo, J. D. (1989). The influ- Relationship of education and IQ in the WAIS-R
ence of age, education, IQ, gender, and alcohol standardization sample. Journal of Consulting and
abuse on Halstead–Reitan Neuropsychological Clinical Psychology, 52, 631–634.
Test Battery performance. Journal of Clinical McCall, R. B. (1977). Childhood IQs as predictors of
Psychology, 45, 484–512. adult educational and occupational status. Science,
Lee, G. P., Sasanuma, S., Hamsher, K. D., & Benton, 197, 482–483.
A. L. (1991). Constructional praxis performance McCarty, S. M., Siegler, I. C., & Logue, P. E. (1982).
of Japanese and American, normal and brain- Cross-sectional and longitudinal patterns of
damaged patients. Archives of Clinical Neuro- three Wechsler Memory Scale subtests. Journal of
psychology, 6, 15–25. Gerontology, 37, 169–175.
Lezak, M. D. (1983). Neuropsychological assessment Mitrushina, M., Boone, K. B., Razani, J., & D’Elia, L.
(2nd ed). New York: Oxford University Press. F. (2005). Handbook of normative data for neuro-
Lezak, M. D. (1987). Norms for growing older. psychological assessment (2nd edition). New York:
Developmental Neuropsychology, 3, 1–12. Oxford University Press.
Lopez, S. R., Grover, K. P., Holland, D., Johnson, M. J., Munn, N. D. (1992). The cultural anthropol-
Kain, C. D., Kanel, K., et al. (1989). Development of ogy of time: A critical essay. Annual Review of
culturally sensitive psychotherapists. Professional Anthropology, 21, 931–923.
Psychology: Research and Practice, 20, 369–376. Nell, V. (2000). Cross-cultural neuropsychological
Lopez, S., & Romero, A. (1988). Assessing the intel- assessment: Theory and practice. Mahwah, NJ:
lectual functioning of Spanish-speaking adults: Erlbaum.
Comparison of the EIWA and the WAIS. Professional Norman, M. A., Evans, J. D., Miller, S. W., & Heaton, R. K.
Psychology: Research and Practice, 19, 263–270. (2000). Demographically corrected norms for the
Maccoby, E. E., & Jacklin, C. N. (1974). The psy- California Verbal Learning Test. Journal of Clinical
chology of sex differences. Stanford CA: Stanford and Experimental Neuropsychology, 22, 80–93.
University Press. Olmedo, E. L. (1981). Testing linguistic minorities.
Malec, J. F., Ivnik, R. J., & Smith, G. E. (1993). American Psychologist, 36, 1078–1085.
Neuropsychology and normal aging: Clinician’s Orozco, S., Thompson, B., Kapes, J., & Montgomery,
perspective. In R. W. Parks, R. F. Zec, & R. S. G. T. (1993). Measuring the acculturation of
Wilson (Eds.), Neuropsychology of Alzheimer’s dis- Mexican Americans: A covariance structure anal-
ease and other dementias (pp. 81–111). New York: ysis. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling
Oxford University Press. and Development, 25, 149–155.
Malec, J. F., Ivnik, R. J., Smith, G. E., Tangalos, E. Parsons, O. A., & Prigatano, G. P. (1978).
G., Petersen, R. C., Kokmen, E., et al. (1992). Methodological considerations in clinical neuro-
Mayo’s older Americans normative studies: psychological research. Journal of Consulting and
Utility of corrections for age and education for the Clinical Psychology, 46, 608–619.
WAIS-R. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 6(suppl.), Pauker, J. D. (1988). Constructing overlapping
31–47. cell tables to maximize the clinical usefulness
154 Methods of Comprehensive Neuropsychological Assessment

of normative test data: Rationale and an exam- Rosselli, M, Ardila, A., Araugo, K., Weekes, V.,
ple from neuropsychology. Journal of Clinical Caracciolo, V., Padilla, M., et al. (2000). Verbal
Psychology, 44, 930–933. Fluency and Repetition Skills in Healthy
Pont´on, M. O. (2001). Research and assessment issues Older Spanish–English Bilinguals. Applied
with Hispanic populations. In M. O. Pont´on and Neuropsychology, 7, 17–24.
J. Leon-Carrion (Eds.), Neuropsychology and the Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., Florez, A., & Castro, C.
hispanic patient: A clinical handbook (pp. 39–58). (1990). Normative data on the Boston Diagnostic
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum. Aphasia Examination in a Spanish-speaking pop-
Poon, L. W. (1985). Difference in human memory ulation. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
with aging: Nature, causes, and clinical impli- Neuropsychology, 12, 313–322.
cations. In J. E. Birren and K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Rosselli, M., Tappen, R., Williams, C., & Savatierra,
Handbook of the psychology of aging (2nd ed.; J. (2006). The relation of education and gender
pp. 427–462). New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold on the attention items of the Mini-Mental State
Company. Examination in Spanish speaking Hispanic elders.
Porteus, S. D. (1959). The maze test and clinical psy- Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 21, 677–686.
chology. Palo Alto: Pacific Books. Russell, E. W., Neuringer, C., & Goldstein, G. (1970).
Price, L. J., Fein, G., & Feinberg, L. (1980). Assessment of brain damage: A neuropsychological
Neuropsychological assessment of cognitive func- key approach. New York: Wiley-Interscience.
tioning in the elderly. In L. W. Poon (Ed.), Aging in the Sattler, J. M. (1982). Assessment of children’s intelli-
1980s: Psychological issues (pp. 78–85). Washington, gence and special abilities (2nd ed.). Boston: Allyn
DC: American Psychological Association. and Bacon.
Puente, A. E., & Agranovich, A. V. (2003). The cul- Shallice, T. (1988). From neuropsychology to men-
tural in crosscultural neuropsychology. In M. tal structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Hersen, G. Goldstein, & S. R. Beers (Eds.), The Press.
handbook of psychological assessment (Vol. 1; pp. Shuttleworth-Edwards, A., Kemp, R., Rust, A.,
31–332). Wiley, New York. Muirhead, J., Hartman, N., et al. (2004).
Reitan, R. M. (1955). The distribution according to Cross-cultural effects on IQ test performance: A
age of a psychological measure dependent upon review and preliminary normative indications on
organic brain functions. Journal of Gerontology, WAIS-III Test performance. Journal of Clinical
10, 338–340. and Experimental Neuropsychology, 26, 903–920.
Reitan, R. M. (1957). Differential reaction of various Spreen, O., & Strauss, E. (1998). A compendium of
psychological tests to age. In Fourth Congress of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms,
the International Association of Gerontology (pp. and commentary (2nd edition). New York: Oxford
158–165). Fidenza: Tito Mattioli. University Press.
Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1986). The Halstead– Stern, Y., Zarahn, E., Hilton, H. J., Flynn, J., DeLa
Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery and aging. Paz, R., & Rakitin, B. (2003). Exploring the neural
In T. L. Brink (Ed.), Clinical gerontology: A guide basis of cognitive reserve. Journal of Clinical and
to assessment and intervention (pp. 39–61). New Experimental Neuropsychology, 5, 691–701.
York: Haworth Press. Strong, C. A., Donders, J., & Van Dyke, S. (2005).
Rey, A. (1964). L’examen Clinique en Psychologie. Validity of demographically corrected norms for
Paris: Presses Universitaires de France. the WAIS-III. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Reynolds, C. R., & Brown, R. T. (1984). Perspectives on Neuropsychology, 27, 746–758.
bias in mental testing. New York: Plenum Press. Taylor, M. J., & Heaton, R. K. (2001). Sensitivity and
Reynolds, C. R., Chastain, R. L., Kaufman, A. S., & specificity of WAIS-III/WMS-III demograph-
McLean, J. E. (1987). Demographic characteris- ically corrected factor scores in neuropsycho-
tics and IQ among adults: Analysis of the WAIS-R logical assessment. Journal of the International
standardization sample as a function of the strati- Neuropsychological Society, 7(7), 867–874.
fication variables. Journal of School Psychology, 25, Thompson, L. L., Heaton, R. K., Grant, I., & Matthews,
323–342. C. G. (1989). A comparison of the WAIS and
Riva, D., & Benton, A. (1993). Visuospatial judgment: WAIS-R using T-score conversions that correct
A crossnational comparison. Cortex, 29, 141–143. for age, education, and sex. Journal of Clinical and
Rosselli, M., & Ardila, A. (2003). The impact of cul- Experimental Neuropsychology, 11, 478–488.
ture and education on non-verbal neuropsycho- Touradji, P., Manly, J., Jacobs, D., & Stern, Y.
logical measurements: A critical review. Brain & (2001). Neuropsychological test performance: A
Cognition, 52, 326–333. study of non-Hispanic White elderly. Journal of
Demographic Influences and Use of Demographically Corrected Norms 155

Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23, Wechsler, D. (1968). Escala de Inteligencia


643–649. Wechsler para Adultos. New York: Psychological
Triandis, H. C., & Berry, J. W. (1980). Handbook of Corporation.
cross-cultural psychology (vols. 1 and 2). Boston: Wechsler, D. (1981). WAIS-R Manual: Wechsler Adult
Allyn and Bacon. Intelligence Scale-Revised. New York: Psychological
Tulsky, D. S., Ivnik, R. J., Price, L. R., & Wilkins, C. Corporation.
(2003). Assessment of cognitive functioning with Wechsler, D. (1987). Wechsler Memory Scale-
the WAIS-III and WMS-III: Development of a Revised. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological
six-factor model. In D. S. Tulsky, G. C. Chelune, Corporation.
R. J. Irnik, A. Prifitera, D. H. Saklofske, Wechsler, D. (1997a). WAIS-III administration
R. K. Heaton, et al. (Eds.), Clinical interpretation and scoring manual. San Antonio, TX: The
of the WAIS-III and WMS-III (pp. 147–179). San Psychological Corporation.
Diego, CA: Academic Press. Wechsler, D. (1997b). WMS-III administration
U.S. Census Bureau. (2004). Facts for features, his- and scoring manual. San Antonio, TX: The
panic heritage month 2004: September 15–Octo- Psychological Corporation.
ber 15. CB04-FF.14-2. Yeudall, L. T., Reddon, J. R., Gill, D. M., & Stefanyk, W.
Vega, A., Jr., & Parsons, O. A. (1967). Cross-validation O. (1987). Normative data for the Halstead–Reitan
of the Halstead–Reitan tests for brain damage. neuropsychological tests stratified by age and sex.
Journal of Consulting Psychology, 31, 619–625. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 43, 346–367.
Vincent, K. R. (1991). Black/White IQ differences: Youngjohn, J. R., Larrabee, G. J., & Crook, T. H. (1992).
Does age make the difference? Journal of Clinical Discriminating age-associated memory impair-
Psychology, 47, 266–270. ment from Alzheimer’s disease. Psychological
Wechsler, D. (1945). A standardized memory scale Assessment, 4, 54–59.
for clinical use. Journal of Psychology, 19, 87–95. Youngjohn, J. R., Larrabee, G. J., & Crook, T. H.
Wechsler, D. (1955). Manual for the Wechsler Adult (1993). New adult age- and education-correction
Intelligence Scale. New York: Psychological norms for the Benton Visual Retention Test. The
Corporation. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 7, 155–160.
This page intentionally left blank
Part II

Neuropsychiatric Disorders
This page intentionally left blank
8

The Neuropsychology of Dementia


Mark W. Bondi, David P. Salmon, and Alfred W. Kaszniak

As an increasing number of people survive into decline in a single factor such as working mem-
older age, there is growing clinical and research ory, inhibitory processes, or sensory function
interest in age-associated cognitive decline. (Kramer et al., 2004; Park et al., 2003).
This interest has focused to a large extent on the It has also been suggested that specific areas
detection and characterization of cognitive def- of the brain are particularly vulnerable to aging,
icits associated with age-related neurodegenera- and their deterioration leads to a decline in the
tive diseases such as Alzheimer’s disease (AD), cognitive abilities they mediate. For example,
but also includes identification of relatively there is evidence that atrophy of prefrontal
subtle cognitive changes that take place during cortex and loss of frontal white matter integ-
the course of normal healthy aging (for reviews, rity occurs as a normal consequence of aging
see Hedden & Gabrieli, 2004; Park et al., 2003). (Greenwood, 2000; West, 1996). These changes
Cognitive decline that occurs with healthy result in age-related declines in so-called frontal
aging is particularly evident in information- functions such as working memory, cognitive
processing abilities such as effortful encoding flexibility, verbal fluency, directed and divided
of new information, processing speed, inductive attention, and self-monitoring performance
reasoning, and working memory. Other cogni- (Grady & Craik, 2000; Raz, 2005; West, 1996).
tive abilities, such as semantic knowledge and Aging may also have a particularly adverse
vocabulary, autobiographical remote memory, effect on white matter tracts that integrate brain
and automatic memory processes (e.g., prim- regions (Bartzokis, 2004; O’Sullivan et al., 2001;
ing), show little age-related decline. This pat- Pfefferbaum et al., 2005; Raz, 2005), which leads
tern of decline has led to the development of to an age-related “disconnection” syndrome that
several psychological and neurological models causes cognitive changes beyond those that are
to account for these changes. One prominent usually considered frontal lobe functions (e.g.,
psychological model (Salthouse, 1996) sug- memory, visuospatial abilities). Other investiga-
gests that a general decline in processing speed tors propose that a staggered decline in multiple
underlies most of the cognitive decline that neurological processes may better explain cogni-
occurs with age. According to this model, an tive changes associated with normal aging than
age-related decline in information-processing any single neurobiological factor (Band et al.,
speed reduces the ability to efficiently inte- 2002; Buckner, 2004; Kramer et al., 2004).
grate and organize information, and causes a As our knowledge of the cognitive changes
decline in memory by reducing the efficiency of that occur throughout the lifespan has grown,
information encoding, rehearsal, and retrieval. research efforts in the neuropsychology of
Similar psychological models suggest that cog- aging and dementia have focused on differen-
nitive decline in the healthy elderly is caused by tiating these changes from the cognitive deficits

159
160 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

associated with neurodegenerative disorders et al., 2001) concludes that these original crite-
such as AD. In the remainder of this chapter, ria adopted in the DSM and by McKhann et al.
we review research that has identified the par- (1984) are sensitive and specific with respect to
ticular neuropsychological deficits that occur the diagnosis of the dementia of AD, but may
in the earliest stages of AD and show how this be less than ideal for the detection of demen-
knowledge has enhanced the ability to clinically tia associated with other neurodegenerative
differentiate the early stages of the disease from diseases. Knopman et al. (2001) suggest that
normal aging. We will also examine the impact consideration of a broader array of cognitive
of aging on the ability to detect AD, and identify (and behavioral) domains with less empha-
cognitive changes that might foreshadow the sis on memory might allow earlier and more
development of dementia in those with “pro- accurate detection of vascular dementia (VaD),
dromal” AD (see Dubois et al., 2007, for dis- dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB), and fronto-
cussion). Finally, we will compare and contrast temporal dementia (FTD). As we will describe
the patterns of cognitive deficits associated with in subsequent sections, memory impairment
AD and other age-related neurodegenerative is not necessarily a prominent part of the ini-
disorders, and show how this has improved dif- tial presentation of these non-AD dementias
ferential clinical diagnosis and provided impor- and should not be required in the definition of
tant information about the neurological basis of dementia.
various cognitive abilities. Before embarking on
this discussion, it is first important to define the
Prevalence
dementia syndrome, identify its prevalence and
associated risk factors, and describe the most Estimates of the prevalence of dementia vary
common age-related neurodegenerative dis- widely due to differences in dementia defini-
eases that lead to its manifestation. tions, sampling techniques, and sensitivity of
instruments used to identify cases. In studies
of dementia in various countries, prevalence
The Syndrome of Dementia:
rates have ranged from 2% to 25% for persons
Description, Prevalence, and
over the age of 65 (see Ineichen, 1987; Wancata
Risk Factors
et al., 2007). Cummings and Benson (1992),
calculating the average of prevalence estimates
Definition
across studies, suggest that approximately 6% of
Dementia refers to a syndrome of acquired cog- persons over the age of 65 have severe demen-
nitive impairment due to brain dysfunction that tia, with an additional 10–15% having mild
is of sufficient severity to interfere with usual to moderate dementia. The prevalence of the
social or occupational functioning. According to syndrome of dementia is age-related, doubling
most definitional schemes (e.g., Diagnostic and approximately every 5 years after age 65 (Jorm
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders [DSM]— et al., 1987). Not surprisingly, the prevalence of
4th edition, American Psychiatric Association, dementia is higher among hospital and nursing
1994; Bayles & Kaszniak, 1987; Cummings home residents than among those living within
& Benson, 1992; NINCDS-ADRDA criteria, the community (Kramer, 1986; Smyer, 1988).
McKhann et al., 1984), the syndrome of demen-
tia involves deterioration in memory and one or
Risk Factors
more of the following domains: language, visu-
ospatial skills, judgment or abstract thinking, Significant advances in our understanding of
or gnosis. These cognitive changes must repre- the epidemiology of dementia have occurred
sent a change from a previously higher level of in recent years. Kawas and Katzman (1999)
performance and adversely affect functional summarize a number of clear-cut findings
abilities. Changes in emotion or personality are that have emerged. First, by all accounts, age
also common, but not a necessary component of is the single most important risk factor for
the dementia syndrome. A Practice Parameter dementia. Community (population) studies in
article on the diagnosis of dementia (Knopman many different countries have confirmed that
The Neuropsychology of Dementia 161

the prevalence of the most common causes of may occur because these factors are a surrogate
dementia, AD and vascular disease, rises in an for brain or cognitive reserve. Such a reserve
approximately exponential fashion between the would help to delay the onset of the usual clini-
ages of 65 and 85. Importantly, however, data on cal manifestations of the disease. Stern et al.
individuals greater than age 85, who represent (1992) reasoned that if advanced education
the fastest-growing segment of our population, imparts a reserve, then more severe pathologi-
generally show prevalence rates for dementia cal brain changes would be present in patients
ranging between 40% and 60%, suggesting that with high education than in those with low edu-
it asymptotes beyond age 85. cation at a time when the groups were matched
Second, gender may be a significant risk factor for overall severity of dementia. Consistent
for dementia. A large-scale epidemiological sur- with this prediction, they found a significantly
vey in Shanghai identified female gender, along greater deficit in parietotemporal blood flow in
with age and education, as an independent pre- high-education probable AD patients than in
dictor of dementia (Zhang et al., 1990). A num- equally demented low-education patients. The
ber of other studies suggest that women have a relationship between dementia and educational
slightly greater risk for AD than men (although or occupational attainment is now supported by
men may be at somewhat greater risk for vas- numerous epidemiological and biological stud-
cular dementia). The results of these studies ies that show high education, occupational work
must be considered carefully, however, because complexity, and a mentally and socially inte-
the increased prevalence of AD in women may grated lifestyle postpones the onset of clinical
be attributable to differential survival after the dementia and AD (for reviews, see Fratiglioni &
onset of dementia due to their longer life expec- Wang, 2007; Kawas & Katzman, 1999).
tancy. Although a 2.8:1 (female/male) ratio of Fourth, a growing literature suggests that
AD was observed in the Framingham preva- environmental influences such as physical
lence study (Bachman et al., 1992), no gender activity and maintaining involvement in cogni-
difference in the incidence of either dementia or tively stimulating mental activity buttress cog-
AD was found in this cohort (Bachman et al., nitive functioning as we age (Hultsch et al.,
1993). However, results from the Cache County 1999; Kramer et al., 2004; Wilson et al., 2002).
epidemiological study of dementia demonstrate Furthermore, there is some evidence that phys-
that incidence rates among women increase ical and mental activity may result in struc-
after age 80 and exceed the risk among men by tural brain changes in animals and humans
more than twofold in late old age (Miech et al., (Churchill et al., 2002; van Praag et al., 2005),
2002; Zandi et al., 2002). although studies directly investigating the
Third, low education and low occupational impact of physical and/or mental activity on
attainment are associated with an increased risk brain structure in humans have been extremely
for dementia. An uneducated individual over limited. Long-term benefits of exercise and car-
age 75 has twice the risk for dementia as some- diorespiratory fitness on cognition have been
one who has completed at least a grade school established, and clinical trials have shown that
education (Katzman, 1993; Kawas & Katzman, a fitness training intervention has a positive
1999; Mortimer, 1988; Zhang et al., 1990). Stern impact on cognitive functioning (particularly
and colleagues (1994) demonstrated a similar executive functions; Barnes et al., 2003). Physical
relationship between risk for dementia and life- activity has been shown to reduce risk for
time occupational attainment, and showed that dementia and improve cognition in both cogni-
a combination of low education and low lifetime tively normal and cognitively impaired older
occupational attainment results in a greater adults (see Fratiglioni et al., 2007; Kramer &
relative risk (i.e., close to a threefold increase in Erickson, 2007, for reviews).
risk) than either one alone. Fift h, the risk of developing dementia is
Katzman (1993) and Stern et al. (1992, 1994) increased approximately fourfold by a family his-
were among the first to suggest that the decrease tory of AD in a first-degree relative (i.e., mother,
in risk of dementia with increasing levels of father, brother, or sister; van Duijn et al., 1991).
educational or lifetime occupational attainment Given the findings of specific point mutations
162 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

on the amyloid precursor protein gene of chro- across the lifespan. Early-life factors such as
mosome 21 and presenilin gene mutations on perinatal conditions, brain development, body
chromosomes 1 and 14, there is now little ques- growth, socioeconomic conditions, environ-
tion that this familial association is genetically mental enrichment, and cognitive reserve can
based and expressed in an autosomal dominant influence the ultimate development of demen-
fashion, although a number of newer candidate tia and AD (see Borenstein et al., 2006). Even
genes for AD also relate to oxidative stress and the major susceptibility gene for AD, the APOE
inflammatory response (Serretti et al., 2007). ε4 allele, may have divergent effects on cogni-
Furthermore, the ε4 allele of the apolipoprotein tion across the lifespan. For example, studies
E (APOE) gene located on chromosome 19 has suggest that the presence of the APOE ε4 allele
been identified as the single most important sus- may have a beneficial effect on cognition early
ceptibility gene for dementia and AD (Corder in the lifespan (Bloss et al., 2008; Alexander
et al., 1993; Saunders et al., 1993; Strittmatter et al., 2007; Han et al., 2007; Hubacek et al., 2001;
et al., 1993) because of its overrepresentation in Keltikangas-Jarvinen et al., 1993; Mondadori
AD relative to healthy elderly. et al., 2007; Wright et al., 1993), in contrast to its
Finally, head injury has been identified as well-known deleterious effects in late life. These
a risk factor for the development of demen- seemingly discrepant effects of the APOE ε4
tia (Jellinger, 2004; Mortimer et al., 1991). allele across the lifespan are consistent with the
Dementia pugilistica may occur in individuals theory of antagonistic pleiotropy (Albin, 1993;
who have suffered repeated blows to the head Williams, 1957), a concept from evolutionary
while boxing (Corsellis et al., 1973), and retired biology that posits that individual alleles can
professional football players with histories of have different effects on fitness at different ages
recurrent concussions also show increased (see Han & Bondi, 2008; Alexander et al., 2007,
cognitive impairments in late life compared for discussion). Regardless, the literature on risk
to retired players without concussive histories factors for dementia suggests that risk is likely
(Guskiewicz et al., 2005). The risk of developing not determined at any single point in time,
AD is doubled for individuals with a history of but results from a complex interplay between
a single head injury that led to loss of conscious- genetic and environmental exposures through-
ness or hospitalization (Mortimer et al., 1991), out one’s life (Bornstein et al., 2006).
and this relationship may be modulated by a
gene–environment interaction as Mayeux and
Specific Dementing Disorders:
colleagues (1995) found a tenfold increase in
Clinical and Neuropathologic
the risk of AD in people with both the APOE ε4
Features
allele and head injury risk factors. Jordan et al.
(1997) also found that possession of an APOE Dementia is associated with more than 70 dif-
ε4 allele was associated with increased severity ferent causes. As mentioned earlier, AD is the
of chronic neurologic deficits in high-exposure most common cause of dementia, account-
boxers. ing for roughly half of all cases (for review, see
The epidemiological evidence to date sug- Cummings & Benson, 1992; Kawas & Katzman,
gests that, as the population ages, dementia 1999). Vascular dementia (VaD) is usually
will increasingly become the dominant dis- regarded as the second most common cause,
order in late life. In the short term, advances although in some community surveys (partic-
in identifying risk factors may improve our ularly, but not exclusively, those in Japan and
ability to detect dementia in its earliest stages China), VaD has been found to be as prevalent
when palliative treatments may be most effec- as AD (Dong et al., 2007). It is not clear whether
tive and, in the long term, may lead to the dis- such differences in relative prevalence estimates
covery of specific biological mechanisms that reflect actual regional disparities, or rather,
cause the disease. Knowledge of the risk factors methodological variation across studies. Other,
for dementia, and particularly AD, is grow- less prevalent degenerative neurological diseases
ing rapidly, and now includes not only those (e.g., Huntington’s disease [HD], Parkinson’s
imparted in late life but also those that arise disease [PD], dementia with Lewy bodies [DLB],
The Neuropsychology of Dementia 163

and frontotemporal dementias [FTD]) may also neocortical and hippocampal levels of the neu-
produce dementia, and they too have been the rotransmitter acetylcholine (Whitehouse et al.,
focus of much neuropsychological research. 1982). Consistent with these widespread neuro-
The neuropathologic and clinical features of pathologic changes, the primary clinical mani-
some of the more common dementing disorders festation of AD is a progressive global dementia
are briefly presented below (the reader is also syndrome that usually begins in later life. The
referred to other chapters in this volume). dementia syndrome of AD is usually character-
ized by prominent amnesia with additional defi-
Alzheimer’s Disease cits in language and semantic knowledge (i.e.,
aphasia and agnosia), abstract reasoning, “exec-
AD is a progressive degenerative brain disor- utive” functions, attention, and constructional
der characterized by neuronal atrophy, synapse (i.e., apraxia) and visuospatial abilities (Salmon
loss, and the abnormal accumulation of diff use & Bondi, 1999). These cognitive deficits and the
and neuritic plaques and neurofibrillary tangles decline in everyday function they produce are
(Alzheimer, 1907; See Figure 8–1; see also the the core features of the AD dementia syndrome.
color figure in the color insert section). These
pathologic changes begin primarily in medial
Vascular Dementia
temporal lobe limbic structures (e.g., entorhinal
cortex, hippocampus) and then progress to the VaD refers to a cumulative decline in cognitive
association cortices of the frontal, temporal, and functioning secondary to multiple or strate-
parietal lobes (Braak & Braak, 1991). Primary gically placed infarctions, ischemic injury, or
motor and sensory cortices and most subcortical hemorrhagic lesions. The clinical and neuro-
structures are relatively spared. Degeneration pathologic presentation of VaD is quite hetero-
in the basal forebrain (e.g., the nucleus basa- geneous, and a variety of conditions fall under
lis of Meynert) results in a major decrement in the general rubric of VaD (see the chapter by

Pathology of Alzheimer’s disease

Brain atrophy and neuron loss Amyloid in plaques

Neurofibrillary tangle Cerebrovascular amyloid

Figure 8–1. The neuropathology of Alzheimer’s disease. Grossly apparent cortical atrophy in Alzheimer’s
disease (Figure 1A) compared to normal aging (Figure 1B), and neocortical amyloid plaques (Figure 2),
neurofibrillary tangles (Figure 3), and cerebrovascular amyloid angiopathy (Figure 4). (Images courtesy
of Drs. Eliezer Masliah, Robert Terry, and Larry Hansen).
164 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Brown in this volume). According to Hodges disorder (e.g., chorea, dysarthria, gait distur-
and Graham (2001), these conditions generally bance, oculomotor dysfunction), behavioral
fall into three large categories: multi-infarct changes (e.g., depression, irritability, and anx-
dementia (MID) associated with multiple large iety), and dementia. These deficits arise pri-
cortical infarctions (usually affecting 10 cc or marily from a progressive deterioration of the
more of brain tissue), dementia due to strategi- neostriatum (caudate nucleus and putamen)
cally placed infarction (e.g., left angular gyrus (Vonsattel & DiFiglia, 1998) that disrupts “fron-
damage related to infarction of the posterior tostriatal loops” that consist of projections from
branch of the medial cerebral artery), and sub- the frontal neocortex to the striatum, striatum
cortical ischemic vascular dementia due to to the globus pallidus, globus pallidus to thal-
subcortical small vessel disease that results amus, and thalamus back to specific neocorti-
in multiple lacunar strokes, leukoaraiosis cal regions of the frontal lobes (e.g., dorsolateral
(Binswanger’s disease) or diff use white matter prefrontal, orbitofrontal, and anterior cingulate
pathology. cortex) (Alexander et al., 1986). These circuits
Specific research criteria for the diagnosis are believed to have a subcortical influence on
of VaD have been proposed (e.g., Chui et al., both motor control and higher cognitive func-
1992, 2000; Roman et al., 1993). In general, tions (Alexander et al., 1986). The cognitive and
these guidelines require that multiple cognitive behavioral deficits associated with HD have
deficits (i.e., dementia) occur in the presence been described as a “subcortical dementia” syn-
of focal neurological signs and symptoms and/ drome that is broadly characterized by slow-
or laboratory (e.g., CT or MRI scan) evidence ness of thought, impaired attention, executive
of cerebrovascular disease that are thought to dysfunction, poor learning, visuoperceptual
cause the cognitive impairment (for review, see and constructional deficits, and personality
Chui, 2007). A relationship between demen- changes such as apathy and depression (Albert
tia and cerebrovascular disease is often indi- et al., 1974; McHugh & Folstein, 1975; also see
cated if the onset of dementia occurs within Chapter 10).
several months of a recognized stroke, there is
an abrupt deterioration in cognitive function-
Dementia with Lewy Bodies
ing, or the course of cognitive deterioration is
fluctuating or stepwise. In one set of diagnostic Dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) is a clinico-
criteria (Roman et al., 1993), VaD can be sub- pathologic condition characterized by a demen-
categorized on the basis of the suspected type tia syndrome that occurs:
of vascular pathology (as determined by clini-
cal, radiologic, and neuropathologic features), • in the presence of cell loss and the deposi-
and possible or probable VaD can be assigned tion of Lewy bodies (abnormal intracytoplas-
depending on the certainty of the contribu- mic eosinophilic neuronal inclusion bodies) in
tion of cerebrovascular disease to the dementia a subcortical pattern similar to that of PD (e.g.,
syndrome. Definite VaD is diagnosed only on in brain stem nuclei including the substantia
the basis of histopathologic evidence of cere- nigra, locus ceruleus, dorsal motor nucleus of
brovascular disease that occurs in the absence the vagus, and substantia innominata);
of neurofibrillary tangles and neuritic plaques • in the presence of Lewy bodies diff usely dis-
exceeding those expected for age and without tributed throughout the limbic system (e.g.,
clinical evidence of any other disorder capable cingulate, insula, amygdala, hippocampus,
of producing dementia (e.g., Pick’s disease, dif- entorhinal cortex, and transentorhinal cortex)
fuse Lewy body disease). and neocortex (e.g., temporal, parietal, and
frontal lobes), and in many cases AD pathology
(i.e., neuritic plaques, neurofibrillary tangles)
Huntington’s Disease
that occurs in the same general distribution
Huntington’s disease (HD) is an inherited auto- throughout the brain as in “pure” AD (Gomez-
somal dominant disease that results in the mid- Tortosa et al., 2000; Hansen et al., 1990; Ince
life (e.g., ages 30–40) development of movement et al., 1998; Kosaka et al., 1984; Perry et al., 1990)
The Neuropsychology of Dementia 165

Entorhinal cortex Temporal cortex CA3 hippocampus

Vacuolization Cortical Lewy boides Lewy neurites

Figure 8–2. Histopathologic abnormalities in the limbic system and neocortex in Dementia with Lewy
Bodies (DLB). The typical appearance of vacuolization in the entorhinal cortex, cortical Lewy bodies in the
temporal lobe neocortex, and Lewy neurites (i.e., neurons containing abnormal alpha-synuclein fi laments) in
the CA3 region of the hippocampus. (Images courtesy of Dr. Eliezer Masliah).

(see Figure 8–2; see also color figure in the color that include alterations in executive functions
insert section). and/or aphasia, often with relative sparing of
visuospatial abilities and memory (see Weder
DLB is associated with widespread depletion of et al., 2007, for review). Various forms of FTD
cortical choline acetyltransferase (ChAT) in the are thought to account for approximately 3–20%
neocortex and striatum (e.g., Tiraboschi et al., of all cases of dementia (Andreasen et al., 1999;
2002), and a disruption of dopaminergic input Brun, 1987; Neary, 1999).
to the striatum due to the loss of pigmented
substantia nigra neurons (Ince et al., 1998).
Neuropsychological Detection
Dementia with Lewy bodies is not rare and
of Alzheimer’s Disease
may occur in approximately 25% of all elderly
demented patients. A wealth of neuropsychological research over
the past two decades has identified the particu-
lar neuropsychological deficits that occur in the
Frontotemporal Dementia
earliest stages of AD and enhanced the ability to
FTD is a clinicopathologic condition char- clinically differentiate the disease from normal
acterized by deterioration of personality and aging. Given that the hippocampus and ento-
cognition associated with prominent fron- rhinal cortex are affected in the earliest stages
tal and temporal lobar atrophy (Brun et al., of AD (Braak & Braak, 1991), it is not surprising
1994). A number of disorders fall under the that measures of the ability to learn and retain
rubric of FTD, including Pick’s disease (Kertesz new information are among the most effective
et al., 1999; Pick, 1892), familial chromosome in differentiating between mildly demented AD
17-linked frontal lobe dementia (Wilhelmsen patients and normal older adults (e.g., Bayles
et al., 1994), dementia lacking distinctive his- et al., 1989; Delis et al., 1991; Eslinger et al., 1985;
topathology (DLDH; Knopman et al., 1990), Huff et al., 1987; Kaszniak et al., 1986; Storandt
semantic dementia (Snowden et al., 1989), and et al., 1984). Patients with early AD are partic-
primary progressive aphasia (Kertesz et al., ularly impaired on measures of delayed recall,
1994; Mesulam, 1982). Although each of these a characteristic that has important clinical util-
disorders has a somewhat unique clinical man- ity for the detection and differential diagnosis
ifestation, FTD usually begins insidiously with of the disease (e.g., Butters et al., 1988; Locascio
personality and behavioral changes (e.g., inap- et al., 1995; Welsh et al., 1991). Several studies
propriate social conduct, inertia and apathy, have shown that rapid forgetting expressed as
disinhibition, perseverative behavior, loss of absolute delayed recall scores or “savings” scores
insight, hyperorality, and decreased speech (i.e., amount recalled after the delay divided by
output). These behavioral changes are accom- the amount recalled on the immediate learn-
panied (or soon followed) by cognitive deficits ing trial) can differentiate mildly demented
166 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

AD patients from healthy elderly with 85–90% A prominent qualitative feature of the memory
accuracy (Butters et al., 1988; Flicker et al., 1991; deficit of patients with AD is an enhanced ten-
Knopman & Ryberg, 1989; Morris et al., 1991; dency to produce intrusion errors (Butters et al.,
Tröster et al., 1993; Welsh et al., 1991). 1987; Fuld et al., 1982; Jacobs et al., 1990). That is,
The abnormally rapid forgetting shown by patients will incorrectly produce old, previously
patients with AD suggests that their memory learned information during the attempt to recall
disorder may be due to ineffective consolida- new material. The abnormal production of intru-
tion of information. This possibility is supported sion errors has been interpreted as increased
by studies that have shown to-be-remembered sensitivity to interference or deficient inhibitory
information is not accessible after a delay even processes in patients with AD. Although intru-
if retrieval demands are reduced by the use of sion errors are not a pathognomonic sign of AD
recognition testing (e.g., Delis et al., 1991), and (Jacobs et al., 1990), their occurrence can be a use-
by studies that demonstrate an abnormal serial ful adjunct to other memory measures (e.g., total
position effect in the episodic memory perfor- recall, recognition memory, rate of forgetting)
mance of patients with AD (Bayley et al., 2000; in developing clinical algorithms for detecting
Capitani et al., 1992; Carlesimo et al., 1995; early AD and differentiating it from other types
Greene et al., 1996; Massman et al., 1993; Wilson of dementia (Delis et al., 1991).
et al., 1983). In these latter studies, patients with Patients with AD often exhibit a severe deficit
early AD consistently show a reduction of the in the ability to remember past events that were
primacy effect (i.e., recall of words from the successfully remembered prior to the onset of
beginning of a list), suggesting that they cannot the disease (i.e., retrograde amnesia). The ret-
effectively transfer information from primary rograde amnesia of AD is temporally graded
memory (i.e., a passive, time-dependent, limited with memories from the distant past better
capacity store that allows the most recent items retained than memories from the more recent
to be better recalled than other items) to second- past (Beatty et al., 1988; Hodges et al., 1993;
ary memory (an actively accessed, long-lasting Kopelman, 1989; Sagar et al., 1988; Wilson et al.,
store that allows early list items that received 1981). This pattern of loss is similar to that of
the greatest amount of processing to be better patients with circumscribed amnesia (Albert
recalled than other items), or that they cannot et al., 1979; Squire et al., 1989) and has been
maintain information in secondary memory attributed to the interruption of a long-term con-
after its successful transfer. This deficit has led to solidation process (e.g., shift ing memory from
the development of several effective clinical tests an episodic to a semantic form) that is critically
for early AD that distinguish between primary dependent upon the hippocampal-diencephalic
(or short-term) and secondary (or long-term) memory system (Cermak, 1984; Squire, 1987;
memory (e.g., California Verbal Learning Test– Zola-Morgan & Squire, 1990; but see Nadel &
2nd edition [CVLT-II]; Delis et al., 2000; Buschke Moscovitch, 1997). However, AD patients are
selective reminding procedure; Buschke, 1973). often impaired even for the most remote time
Impaired encoding ability may also adversely periods, which may result from a combination
affect AD patients’ performance on episodic of the episodic and semantic memory deficits
memory tests (Martin et al., 1985). Semantic that they suffer (for review, see Salmon, 2000).
encoding procedures (Goldblum et al., 1998) or Although the episodic memory impairment
the use of materials that allow the use of semantic described above is usually the earliest and most
encoding strategies (for review, see Backman & prominent feature of the dementia of AD, a
Small, 1998) are less effective in improving epi- number of higher-order cognitive processes
sodic memory performance in patients with become affected as the neuropathology spreads
AD than in normal elderly individuals. This beyond medial temporal lobe structures to the
semantic encoding deficit is targeted by several association cortices of the temporal, frontal, and
clinical memory tests that effectively differenti- parietal lobes (Braak & Braak, 1991). For exam-
ate between mildly demented AD patients and ple, semantic memory that underlies general
normal elderly individuals (e.g., Buschke et al., knowledge and language is often disturbed rela-
1997; Knopman & Ryberg, 1989). tively early in the course of AD (for reviews, see
The Neuropsychology of Dementia 167

Hodges & Patterson, 1995; Nebes, 1989; Salmon subjects on tests that required set-shift ing, self-
et al., 1999). This disturbance is evident in AD monitoring, or sequencing, but not on tests
patients’ reduced ability to recall overlearned that required cue-directed attention or verbal
facts (e.g., the number of days in a year; Norton problem solving. The executive function defi-
et al., 1997), and in their impairment on tests cits associated with AD are also evident on dif-
of confrontation naming (Bayles & Tomoeda, ficult problem-solving tests such as the Tower of
1983; Bowles et al., 1987; Hodges et al., 1991; London puzzle (Lange et al., 1995) and the mod-
Huff et al., 1986; Martin & Fedio, 1983), verbal ified Wisconsin Card Sorting Task (Bondi et al.,
fluency (Butters et al., 1987; Martin & Fedio, 1993), on tests of relational integration (Waltz
1983; Monsch et al., 1994), and semantic catego- et al., 2004), and on various other clinical neu-
rization (Aronoff et al., 2006; Chan et al., 1998). ropsychological tests such as the Porteus Maze
In addition, the spontaneous speech of patients Task, Part B of the Trail-Making Test, and the
with AD is frequently vague, empty of content Raven Progressive Matrices Task (Grady et al.,
words, and fi lled with indefinite phrases and 1988; for review, see Duke & Kaszniak, 2000).
circumlocutions (Nicolas et al., 1985). Some aspects of attention are often impaired
The semantic memory deficit exhibited by relatively early in the course of AD. Attention
patients with AD may reflect the loss of semantic deficits in mildly demented AD patients have
knowledge for particular items or concepts dur- been shown by using dual-processing tasks,
ing the course of the disease. Studies that have tasks that require the disengagement and
probed for knowledge of particular concepts shifting of attention, and working memory
across different modes of access and output (e.g., tasks that are dependent upon the control of
fluency, confrontation naming, sorting, word- attentional resources (for reviews, see Duke &
to-picture matching, definition generation) Kaszniak, 2000; Parasuraman & Haxby, 1993;
showed that patients with AD were significantly Perry & Hodges, 1999). The ability to focus and
impaired on all measures of semantic memory, sustain attention is usually only affected in later
and when a particular stimulus item was missed stages of the disease, as was shown by the essen-
(or correctly identified) in one task, it was likely tially normal performance of mildly demented
to be missed (or correctly identified) in other AD patients on the Attention/Concentration
tasks that accessed the same information in a Index of the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised
different way (Chertkow & Bub, 1990; Hodges (WMS-R), a measure derived from performance
et al., 1992). A true loss of semantic knowl- on tests of digit span (forward and backward),
edge (rather than deficient retrieval) in mildly visual memory span (forward and backward),
demented patients with AD was also implicated and mental control (Butters et al., 1988).
in a longitudinal study by Norton and colleagues Patients with AD often exhibit deficits in
(1997) that showed year-to-year consistency in visuospatial abilities that are apparent on visuo-
the items missed during progressive decline on constructional tasks such as the Block Design
a test of general knowledge (i.e., the Number Test (Mohr et al., 1990; Pandovani et al., 1995;
Information Test) that had minimal language Villardita, 1993), the Clock Drawing Test (for
demands (also see Salmon et al., 1999). review, see Freedman et al., 1994), or copying
In addition to memory and language deficits, a complex figure (Locascio et al., 1995; Mohr
patients with early AD often exhibit deficits in et al., 1990; Pandovani et al., 1995; Villardita,
“executive” functions responsible for concurrent 1993). Deficits are also often apparent on tasks
mental manipulation of information, concept that require visuoperception and visual orienta-
formation, problem solving, and cue-directed tion, such as the Judgment of Line Orientation
behavior (Perry & Hodges, 1999). A study by test (Ska et al., 1990), the Money Road Map Test
Lefleche and Albert (1995) demonstrated that (Locascio et al., 1995), and the Hooper Visual
the ability to perform concurrent manipulation Orientation Test (Paxton et al., 2007). These
of information appears to be particularly vul- visuospatial deficits are usually not apparent
nerable in early AD. In this study, very mildly in the very earliest stages of AD and may have
demented patients with AD were significantly little to contribute to the differentiation of early
impaired relative to elderly normal control dementia from normal aging (e.g., Locascio et al.,
168 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

1995; Storandt et al., 1984). However, visuospa- showed a typical course for the disease. Receiver
tial abilities decline over time in AD and may Operating Characteristic (ROC) curve analy-
be useful for tracking the progression of the dis- ses showed excellent sensitivity and specificity
ease (see Paxton et al., 2007). for the detection of very mild AD for learning
Taken together, the neuropsychological and delayed recall measures from the California
research findings reviewed above suggest that Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) (sensitivity:
AD usually results in a specific pattern of cog- 95–98%, specificity: 88–89%), the category flu-
nitive deficits that can help differentiate the dis- ency test (sensitivity: 96%, specificity: 88%), and
ease from normal aging. Prominent deficits in Part B of the Trail-Making Test (sensitivity: 85%,
episodic memory (e.g., rapid forgetting), certain specificity: 83%). A diagnostic model obtained
executive functions (e.g., cognitive set-shifting), using a nonparametric recursive partitioning
and semantic knowledge characterize the dis- procedure (Classification Tree Analysis) showed
ease early on, and are thought to have clinical that a combination of performance on the cate-
utility for early detection of AD. This was con- gory fluency test (a measure of semantic mem-
firmed in a study that directly compared the abil- ory and executive function) and the delayed
ity of a number of sensitive measures of learning recall measure of the Visual Reproduction Test
and memory, executive abilities, language, and accurately classified 96% of the patients with
visuospatial abilities to differentiate between 98 AD and 93% of the elderly normal control sub-
patients with mild AD (i.e., scored ≥ 24 on the jects, a level of accuracy higher than achieved
Mini-Mental State Exam) and 98 gender-, age-, with any individual cognitive measure; similar
and education-matched normal control subjects correct classification rates were seen when the
(Salmon et al., 2002). The diagnosis of AD was Dementia Rating Scale was allowed in the analy-
verified in each of the AD patients by subsequent sis (see Figure 8–3). These findings substantiate
autopsy or longitudinal clinical evaluations that the typical pattern of deficits usually observed

Is Category Fluency < 40? Is Category Fluency < 40?

Yes No Yes No

Normal control Normal control


Model Model
96% Sensitivity 95% Sensitivity
93% Specificity 94% Specificity

Is VR < 9? Is DRS < 133?

Yes No Yes No

Normal control Normal control

Alzheimer’s disease Alzheimer’s disease

Note: VR = Visual Reproduction Delayed Recall; DRS = Dementia Rating Scale

Figure 8–3. Two classification and regression tree models that were maximally effective in differentiating
patients with mild Alzheimer’s disease from healthy older adults. The variables retained in the models included
a measure of category fluency (i.e., sum of animals, fruits, and vegetables) and either global cognition (i.e., DRS
total score) or delayed recall (i.e., Visual Reproduction Test raw score). (Adapted from Salmon et al., 2002).
The Neuropsychology of Dementia 169

in AD and attest to the clinical utility of a thor-


Impact of Aging on the
ough neuropsychological evaluation for early
Neuropsychological Detection
diagnosis.
of Alzheimer’s Disease
There is a growing body of experimental
evidence to indicate that the cortical neuro- The boundaries between normal age-related
pathology of AD results in the loss of effective cognitive change and early signs of AD are par-
interaction between distinct and relatively intact ticularly difficult to delineate in very elderly
cortical information-processing systems (e.g., individuals (e.g., over the age of 80). This is
DeLacoste & White, 1993). Because the neurofi- because many of the early structural and func-
brillary tangle pathology of AD has a strong pre- tional changes of AD overlap with changes
dilection for cortical layers (e.g., Layer-III and that occur in normal aging. Normal aging is
Layer-V) and cell types (e.g., mid-size pyrami- associated with mild brain atrophy on struc-
dal neurons) that support connections between tural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI; Jack
functionally related cortical association areas, et al., 1998; Jernigan et al., 2001; Pfefferbaum
it effectively disconnects the hippocampus et al. 1994), decreased hemodynamic response
from neocortex (e.g., Hyman et al., 1984) and on functional MR imaging (D’Esposito et al.,
disrupts corticocortical pathways that connect 1999), reduced synaptic density (Masliah et al.,
cortical association areas (for review, see Hof 1993), and increased white matter abnormalities
& Morrison, 1999). This disconnection is evi- (Guttman et al., 1998; Jernigan et al., 2001; Salat
dent in marked abnormalities in the interre- et al., 1999). These brain changes are thought
gional pattern of blood-flow activation elicited to mediate age-related decline in information-
during the performance of cognitive tasks (e.g., processing speed, executive functions, learning
Delbeuck et al., 2003; Grady et al., 2001; Haxby efficiency, and effortful retrieval (Corey-Bloom
et al., 1985), and reduced coherence (i.e., syn- et al., 1996; Desgranges et al., 1998; Grady et al.,
chronization) between electroencephalography 1995; Gunning-Dixon & Raz, 2000; Kaszniak &
(EEG) signals measured at different scalp sur- Newman, 2000; Schacter et al., 1996; Ylikoski
face electrode sites that correspond to neocorti- et al., 1993). Because normal aging detrimen-
cal association areas that must work in concert tally affects many of the same cognitive abili-
during integrative cognitive tasks (e.g., cross- ties affected by AD (see the previous section),
modal stimulus processing) (e.g., Dunkin et al., the prominence of specific deficits related to
1995; Hogan et al., 2003; Jelic et al., 1996; Knott AD may be much less evident in the Very-Old
et al., 2000; Stevens et al., 2001). Behaviorally, than in the Young-Old, especially after perfor-
corticocortical disconnectivity in patients with mance is standardized to the age-appropriate
AD is demonstrated by an impaired ability to normal cohort. This may result in a less distinct
“bind” distinct visual stimulus features that and somewhat atypical cognitive deficit profi le
are effectively processed in different cortical associated with AD in the Very-Old compared
streams (i.e., motion and color; Festa et al., to Young-Old.
2005), to identify objects through the integra- This possibility was confirmed in a study
tion of perceptual, lexical, and semantic repre- that directly compared the neuropsychologi-
sentations mediated by distinct cortical regions cal test performance of AD patients who were
(Della Sala et al., 2000; Dobkins & Albright, over the age of 80 (Very-Old) or below the age
1998; Foster et al., 1999; Freedman & Oscar- of 70 (Young-Old) (Bondi et al., 2003). Despite
Berman, 1997; Kurylo et al., 1996; Lakmache achieving similar raw scores on all neuropsy-
et al., 1998; Tales et al., 2002; Tippett et al., chological measures, the Young-Old and Very-
2003), or to perform cross-hemispheric integra- Old AD patients differed in the severity and
tion of information discretely processed in left- pattern of cognitive deficits they exhibited in
or right-hemisphere cortical regions (Golob relation to their age-appropriate control groups
et al., 2001). These results suggest that behav- (see Figure 8–4). Analysis of composite age-ap-
ioral evidence of cortical disconnectivity may propriate z-scores in various cognitive domains
have potential as a cognitive marker for detect- showed that Young-Old AD patients were gener-
ing and tracking progression of AD. ally more impaired than Very-Old patients and
170 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

0 Healthy elderly
Average impairment score (z-score)

–1

–2

–3 AD age > 80

–4 AD age < 70

–5

–6

–7
Language Visuospatial Executive Memory savings

Figure 8–4. The average composite impairment score achieved by Alzheimer’s disease (AD) patients older
than age 80 or younger than age 70 in the cognitive domains of language, visuospatial abilities, executive func-
tions, and memory (savings scores). The presented scores are z-scores referenced to the patient groups’ respec-
tive age-appropriate healthy elderly control cohort. (Adapted from Bondi et al., 2003).

had a typical AD profi le. That is, they exhibited (2003) showed that this was not the driving fac-
deficits in executive functions and the reten- tor in their study because the variance associ-
tion of episodic memories (i.e., savings scores) ated with the various cognitive scores did not
that were greater than their deficits in other differ between the older and younger normal
cognitive domains. By contrast, Very-Old AD control groups.
patients exhibited a similar level of impairment These and similar results clearly indicate
across all cognitive domains so that their deficit that normal aging can significantly impact
profi le lacked the disproportionate saliency of the severity and pattern of neuropsychological
memory and executive function deficits typical deficits associated with early AD and reduce
of the disease. the saliency of the deficit profi le as a diagnostic
It is interesting to note that the distinct cogni- marker of the disease. This finding has impor-
tive profiles exhibited by Young-Old and Very- tant clinical implications because it identifies
Old AD patients actually reflected differences in the significant risk of false-negative diagnostic
the respective age-matched normative cohorts. errors in very elderly AD patients if the clini-
Although the raw scores of the younger and cian expects to see the typical deficit pattern
older AD patients were similar, the older con- characteristic of younger AD patients.
trol group performed significantly worse than
the younger control group on nearly all cogni-
Neuropsychological Detection of
tive tests, with the largest differences apparent
“Prodromal” Alzheimer’s Disease
on tests of memory, executive functions, and
category fluency. Thus, the better z-scores of the It is now commonly accepted that the neurode-
Very-Old AD patients compared to the Young- generative changes of AD begin well before the
Old AD patients are a function of lower mean clinical manifestations of the disease become
performance in the older control group on tests apparent (e.g., Katzman, 1994). As the patho-
of cognitive abilities that are vulnerable to nor- logic changes of AD gradually accumulate,
mal age-related decline. Although increased a threshold for the initiation of the clinical
variability in test performance related to nor- symptoms of the disease is eventually reached.
mal aging could also impact the z-scores of the Once this threshold is crossed, cognitive defi-
Very-Old AD patients, Bondi and colleagues cits associated with AD become evident and
The Neuropsychology of Dementia 171

Normal cognition has focused largely on this aspect of cognition.


Indeed, a number of prospective longitudinal
Preclinical period studies of cognitive function in nondemented
performance

older adults have shown that a subtle decline


Cognitive

MCI in episodic memory often occurs prior to the


emergence of the obvious cognitive and behav-
Dementia ioral changes required for a clinical diagnosis of
AD (Bondi et al., 1994; Fuld et al., 1990; Grober
& Kawas, 1997; Howieson et al., 1997; Jacobs
Age et al., 1995; Tschanz et al., 2006). In some cases,
Figure 8–5. Modification of the chronic dis-
decline in episodic memory becomes appar-
ease model of Alzheimer’s disease first proposed by
ent many years prior to the onset of dementia
Katzman (1976). The solid line (—) represents a typi-
(Backman et al., 2001; Bondi et al., 1999; Kawas
cal trajectory of cognitive decline for most individuals
et al., 2003; Linn et al., 1995; Schaie et al., 2005;
who do not carry risk factors for the disease. The dot-
Small et al., 2000). These and similar findings
ted line (. . . .) represents a trajectory of decline for indi-
led to the development of formal criteria for
viduals who have the same degree of neuropathologic
Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI; Peterson
changes and contributing causes as those depicted in
et al., 1995), a predementia condition in elderly
the solid line but who have less brain reserve capacity
individuals that is characterized by both sub-
perhaps from poorer neural development or intercon-
jective and objective memory impairment that
nectivity. The dashed line (– –) represents individuals
occurs in the face of relatively preserved general
with the same relative brain reserve capacity as those
cognition and functional abilities (for reviews,
depicted in the solid line but who have a genetic or
see Albert & Blacker, 2006; Collie & Maruff,
environmental predisposition to AD.
2000; Peterson et al., 2001).
The course of episodic memory change dur-
ing the prodromal phase of AD has been the
gradually worsen in parallel with continued focus of a number of studies (Backman et al.,
neurodegeneration. When the cognitive deficits 2001; Chen et al., 2001; Rubin et al., 1998; Small
become global and severe enough to interfere et al., 2000; Storandt et al., 2002). These stud-
with normal social and occupational func- ies suggest that memory performance may be
tioning, established criteria for dementia and poor but stable a number of years prior to the
a clinical diagnosis of AD are met. It is clear development of the dementia syndrome in
from this sequence of events that subtle cogni- those with AD, and then decline rapidly in the
tive decline is likely to occur in a patient with period immediately preceding the dementia
AD well before the clinical diagnosis can be diagnosis. Consistent with this plateau model of
made with any certainty. Identification of the decline, Small et al. (2000) and Backman et al.
cognitive changes that occur during this “pro- (2001) found that episodic memory was mildly
dromal” phase of the disease might provide a impaired 6 years prior to dementia onset, but
way to reliably detect AD in its earliest stages, changed little over the next 3 years. In con-
when potential disease-modifying treatments trast, Chen et al. (2001) and Lange et al. (2002)
might be most effective (Thal, 1999). Because of showed a significant and steady decline in epi-
the importance of this goal, the attempt to iden- sodic memory beginning about 3 years prior
tify prodromal cognitive changes of AD is one to the dementia diagnosis in individuals with
of the most active areas of neuropsychological prodromal AD. These results indicate that an
research (see Figure 8–5). abrupt decline in memory in an elderly individ-
In light of neuropathological evidence that ual might better predict the imminent onset of
the earliest changes of AD usually occur in the dementia than poor but stable memory ability.
medial temporal lobe structures that are known Such a plateau model (i.e., mild but stable epi-
to be critical for episodic memory (Braak & sodic memory decline followed by more abrupt
Braak, 1991), it is not surprising that the search decline in the years proximal to diagnosis) was
for prodromal cognitive markers of the disease validated in a large-scale study by Smith et al.
172 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

(2007), who found that the plateau was evident Table 8–1. Summary of the percentage of studies
on tests of episodic memory, but not on tests of (N = 73) documenting specific cognitive domains
other cognitive domains. affected in the prodromal period of Alzheimer’s
Although the search for cognitive changes in disease.
prodromal AD has largely focused on episodic Neuropsychological Percentage of Studies
memory (see also position paper by Dubois Domain with Affected Domain
et al., 2007), several recent reviews and meta- Attention 71
analyses suggest that there is largely nonspecific Verbal Learning 57
cognitive decline in the 2 to 3 years preceding Verbal Memory 50
a dementia diagnosis. Although a decline in Executive Functions 44
episodic memory is consistently found in these Processing Speed 43
General Cognition 38
studies, they also often reveal additional defi- Language 33
cits in executive functions, perceptual speed, Visual Learning 29
verbal ability, visuospatial skill, and attention Visual Memory 28
during the prodromal phase of AD (Backman Visuospatial Abilities 26
et al., 2004, 2005; Storandt et al., 2006; Twamley Praxis 17
Motor Speed 17
et al., 2006). This widespread decline in cog- Working Memory 12
nitive abilities mirrors evidence that multiple
brain regions (e.g., medial and lateral temporal Notes: The cognitive domains most consistently associated
with prodromal AD were attention (e.g., 71% of studies in
lobes, frontal lobes, anterior cingulate cortex) which it was assessed), verbal learning and memory, execu-
are impaired in prodromal AD (Albert et al., tive functions, processing speed, and language.
2001; Small et al., 2003) (see Table 8–1). Source: Adapted from Twamley et al. (2006).
Several studies suggest that measures of
semantic knowledge (i.e., vocabulary, naming,
category fluency) decline during the prodromal a study that demonstrated that language tasks
period of AD, even though they are relatively were predictive of AD pathology observed 6
independent of episodic memory impairment years later (Powell et al., 2006).
(Koenig et al., 2007) and not particularly sus- A study by Jacobson and colleagues (2002)
ceptible to normal age-related decline (Mickes identified asymmetric cognitive profi les as a
et al. 2007; see also Cuetos et al., 2007; Powell possible prodromal marker of AD in at-risk
et al., 2006). Thus, semantic memory impair- older adults. Based upon prior research showing
ment may be a promising cognitive marker for lateralized cognitive deficits (e.g., greater verbal
prodromal detection of AD in at-risk elderly than visuospatial deficits, or vice versa) in sub-
individuals. This possibility is supported by groups of mildly demented patients with AD,
the results of a recent neuropsychological study these investigators suggested that inconsistent
that showed that both semantic and episodic findings of cognitive markers in at-risk groups
memory declined rapidly in a 3 year prodromal occur because subgroups of subjects have asym-
period progressing to AD, whereas decline in metric deficits that cannot be appreciated with
executive functions was not especially promi- the use of a single test. To test this hypothesis,
nent (Mickes et al., 2007; see Figure 8–6). Based Jacobson and colleagues compared 20 cogni-
upon these findings, Mickes et al. suggested that tively normal elderly adults who were in a pro-
cognitive functions subserved by the medial and dromal phase of AD (i.e., they were diagnosed
lateral temporal lobes (episodic memory and with AD approximately 1 year later) and 20 age-
semantic knowledge, respectively) are substan- and education-matched normal control subjects
tially more impaired than cognitive functions on a number of individual cognitive test scores
subserved by the frontal lobes (executive func- and a derived score that reflected the absolute
tioning) in early AD. In addition, these results difference between verbal and visuospatial abil-
are consistent with a report of decreased seman- ity (i.e., a measure of cognitive asymmetry).
tic access in nondemented older adults at risk Although the groups performed similarly on
for AD owing to the presence of the APOE ε4 individual cognitive tests of memory, language,
allele (Rosen et al., 2005), and with the results of and visuospatial ability, the prodromal AD
The Neuropsychology of Dementia 173

–1
z score

–2

Episodic memory
Executive functions
Semantic knowledge

–3
n–2 n–1 n
Year

Figure 8–6. Aggregate control-referenced z-scores for prodromal AD patients on tests of episodic memory,
semantic memory, and executive functions. Their neuropsychological profi les were examined 1 year (n–1) and
2 years (n–2) prior to the first year of their non-normal diagnosis (referred to as year n). (Adapted from Mickes
et al., 2007).

patients were more likely than normal control patterns of preserved and impaired cognitive
subjects to exhibit evidence of cognitive asym- abilities. Knowledge of these differences might
metry in either the verbal or visuospatial direc- lead to better understanding of the neurobio-
tion. These results suggest that a subgroup of logical basis of various cognitive disorders, have
prodromal AD patients have asymmetric cog- important implications for the neurobiological
nitive changes that are obscured when cogni- basis of normal cognition, and improve differ-
tive scores are averaged over the entire group. ential diagnosis of various neurodegenerative
Additional evidence of cognitive asymmetry in disorders. The remaining sections will review
prodromal AD or in elderly individuals at risk similarities and differences in the cognitive
for AD has been shown in subsequent studies deficits of AD and those of other age-related
that compared auditory versus spatial attention causes of dementia including HD, DLB, FTD,
(Jacobson et al., 2005a), verbal versus design flu- and VaD.
ency (Houston et al., 2005), global versus local
item processing (Jacobson et al., 2005b), and
Alzheimer’s Disease vs. Huntington’s
response inhibition versus cognitive flexibility
Disease
(Wetter et al., 2005). Consideration of these
nonmemory changes in conjunction with subtle Many aspects of cognition are affected in qual-
declines in episodic memory may improve the itatively distinct ways by AD and HD. One
ability to detect AD in its earliest stages. distinction is evident in the nature of the epi-
sodic memory deficits associated with the two
disorders. As described in the section above,
Differentiating Alzheimer’s
the dementia of AD is usually characterized
Disease from Other Dementias
by a severe deficit in episodic memory that has
Dementia can arise from a wide variety of been attributed to ineffective consolidation (i.e.,
etiologically and neuropathologically distinct storage) of new information (Salmon, 2000). In
disorders that give rise to somewhat different contrast, the dementia of HD is usually
174 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

characterized by mild to moderate memory deficient, causing the remote memory deficit
impairment that appears to result from a gen- to be equally distributed across decades. This
eral deficit in the ability to initiate and carry out interpretation is bolstered by an analysis of cued
the systematic retrieval of otherwise success- retrieval in a remote memory task that indi-
fully stored information (Butters et al., 1985, cated a preferential cueing benefit for patients
1986; Moss et al., 1986). These differences were with HD compared to patients with AD (Sadek
demonstrated in a study by Delis and colleagues et al., 2004).
(1991) that directly compared AD and HD A number of qualitative differences exist in
patients on a rigorous test of verbal learning and the language and semantic knowledge deficits
memory (i.e., the CVLT). Despite comparable exhibited by patients with AD and HD. For
immediate and delayed recall deficits (based on example, patients with AD usually exhibit a
age-corrected normative data), patients with AD significant confrontation naming deficit (e.g.,
were just as impaired on the recognition trial Bayles & Tomoeda, 1983), and the greatest
as they were on the free recall trials, whereas proportion of their naming errors are seman-
patients with HD were less impaired on the rec- tically based (e.g., superordinate errors such
ognition trial than on free recall trials. The ben- as calling a “camel” an “animal”). In con-
efit of recognition testing for the HD patients trast, patients with HD usually have little dif-
suggests that their memory impairment is ficulty with confrontation naming (Folstein
attenuated when the need for effortful, strategic et al., 1990; Hodges et al., 1991), and the naming
retrieval is reduced (Butters et al., 1985, 1986), a errors they make tend to be perceptually based
benefit not shared by patients with AD. In addi- (e.g., calling a “pretzel” a “snake”; Hodges et al.,
tion, patients with AD exhibited significantly 1991). Differences also exist in the pattern of
faster forgetting over the 20-minute delay inter- verbal fluency deficits associated with the two
val than did patients with HD (also see Butters disorders. Patients with HD are severely and
et al., 1988; Troster et al., 1993). Patients with AD equivalently impaired on both letter fluency
retained less than 20% of the initially acquired (i.e., generate words that begin with the letters
information, whereas those with HD retained a “F,” “A,” or “S”) and category fluency (i.e., gen-
near-normal 70%. These distinct deficit patterns erate exemplars of animals, fruits, or vegeta-
are consistent with the notion that information bles) tasks, whereas patients with AD are more
is not effectively consolidated by patients with impaired on category fluency than on letter flu-
AD because of early damage to medial temporal ency tasks (Butters et al., 1987; Monsch et al.,
lobe structures (e.g., hippocampus, entorhinal 1994; for reviews, see Henry et al., 2004, 2005).
cortex), whereas information can be successfully Studies of the temporal dynamics of retrieval
stored but not effectively retrieved by patients from semantic memory during the letter and
with HD, presumably owing to disruption of category fluency tasks provide some informa-
frontostriatal circuits (although some residual tion about the nature of the loss that underlies
memory deficit is apparent even when retrieval these distinct deficit patterns (Rohrer et al.,
demands are reduced; Brandt et al., 1992; for 1995, 1999). Patients with AD achieved a lower-
review, see Montoya et al., 2006). than-normal mean latency consistent with the
The ineffective retrieval exhibited by patients idea that they effectively draw exemplars from
with HD on tests of episodic memory is also a semantic set that is abnormally small due to a
evident in their performance on tests of remote loss of semantic knowledge. Patients with HD,
memory. Whereas patients with AD show a in contrast, had a higher-than-normal mean
severe and temporally graded retrograde amne- response latency consistent with the notion that
sia, patients with HD exhibit a relatively mild they have a normal semantic set size, but draw
retrograde amnesia that equally affects all time exemplars abnormally slowly due to ineffective
periods (Albert et al., 1981; Beatty et al., 1988; retrieval.
Sadek et al., 2004). Presumably, episodic mem- The view that AD and HD differentially impact
ory that was acquired in the past is successfully the integrity of the structure and organization
stored and retained over time by these patients, of semantic memory was directly examined
but retrieval of this information is generally by Chan and colleagues using cluster analysis
The Neuropsychology of Dementia 175

and multidimensional scaling techniques to to learn and retain certain motor and cognitive
statistically model a spatial representation of skills such as pursuit rotor tracking, adaptation-
the degree of association between concepts in mediated weight biasing, visual prism adapta-
semantic memory (for review, see Chan et al., tion, serial reaction time sequences, reading
1998). The degree of association between the mirror-reversed text, complex problem solving
various exemplars in the category “animals” (e.g., Tower of Hanoi puzzle), and probabilis-
was estimated from their proximate position tic classification learning (Gabrieli et al., 1997;
when generated in a verbal fluency task, or from Heindel et al., 1989; for review, see Heindel &
the frequency with which they were paired in Salmon, 2001). Taken together, these findings
a triadic comparison task. Modeling showed indicate that there is a dissociation in the neural
that the network of semantic associations for substrates that support various forms of implicit
patients with HD was virtually identical to that memory. Priming appears to be mediated by the
of control subjects, whereas that of patients with basal forebrain and association cortices dam-
AD was characterized by less consistency and aged in AD, whereas motor and cognitive skill
weaker and more concrete associations (i.e., size learning is largely mediated by the neostriatal
was emphasized rather than domesticity). Thus, structures (particularly the caudate nucleus)
AD appears to be characterized by a decline damaged in HD.
in the structure and organization of semantic Distinct aspects of attention, working mem-
knowledge that does not occur in HD. Because ory, and executive functions are differentially
the categorization task placed little demand on affected in AD and HD. In general, a deficit
retrieval processes, the results also support the in attention is more salient in HD than in AD
possibility that HD is characterized by a gen- (e.g., Butters et al., 1988). The attention deficit in
eral retrieval deficit that is not influenced by the HD is characterized by difficulty in shift ing or
demands various tasks place on the structure of allocating attention (Hanes et al., 1995; Lange
semantic memory. et al., 1995; Lawrence et al., 1996), particu-
Although memory is usually thought of as larly when attentional shifts must be internally
a conscious process in which an individual regulated (Sprengelmeyer et al., 1995). Several
explicitly attempts to remember a specific bit of studies have shown, for example, that the abil-
information, there are some forms of memory ity to effectively shift attention between stimu-
that occur without conscious awareness (i.e., lus dimensions (e.g., from color to shape) in a
implicit memory) and without dependence upon visual discrimination task is impaired in mod-
the medial temporal lobe structures important erately to severely demented HD patients, but
for explicit episodic memory (Schacter, 1987; not in patients with AD or in mildly demented
Squire, 1987). Two forms of implicit memory HD patients (Lange et al., 1995; Lawrence et al.,
include priming, a phenomenon in which the 1996).
ability to identify or to generate a particular Alzheimer’s disease and HD differentially
stimulus is enhanced simply through prior expo- affect working memory (Baddeley, 1986). Early
sure to an identical or associated stimulus, and in the course of HD, working memory deficits
motor and cognitive skill learning that occurs are apparent in the ability to maintain infor-
with repeated practice. These forms of implicit mation in temporary memory buffers (e.g., as
memory are differentially impaired in AD and evidenced by poor digit span performance),
HD. Mildly demented patients with AD, but not to inhibit irrelevant information, and to use
those with HD, are impaired on various priming strategic aspects of memory (e.g., planning,
tasks including lexical (word stem completion) organization) to enhance free recall (Butters
priming, semantic paired-associate priming, et al., 1978; Caine et al., 1977; Lange et al.,
and priming to enhance the identification of 1995; Lawrence et al., 1996). Working memory
fragmented pictures (see Fleischman & Gabrieli, deficits are relatively mild in early AD and are
1998, for review), whereas motor learning is primarily characterized by a disruption of the
relatively spared (Eslinger & Damasio, 1986). In central executive with sparing of the phonolog-
contrast, mildly demented patients with HD, but ical loop and visuospatial scratchpad (Baddeley
not those with AD, are impaired in the ability et al., 1991; Collette et al., 1999). It is only in the
176 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

later stages of AD that all aspects of working study that examined the ability of AD and HD
memory become compromised (Baddeley et al., patients to mentally rotate representations of
1991; Collette et al., 1999). objects (Lineweaver et al., 2005). Patients with
One of the most salient aspects of the demen- HD were significantly slower than normal con-
tia associated with HD is impairment of vari- trol subjects in performing mental rotation, but
ous “executive” functions involved in planning were as accurate as controls in making the rota-
and problem solving. Patients with HD usually tion and reporting the correct side of the tar-
exhibit deficits in goal-directed behavior, the get. Patients with AD, in contrast, performed
ability to generate multiple response alterna- the mental rotation as quickly as controls, but
tives, the capacity to resist distraction and main- were significantly impaired in making an accu-
tain response set, and the cognitive flexibility rate rotation and reporting the correct side of
needed to evaluate and modify behavior (for the target. These results were interpreted as
review, see Brandt & Bylsma, 1993). These defi- showing that HD patients can effectively per-
cits have been documented with a wide variety form mental rotation of visual representations,
of tests that assess executive functions, including but suffer a general bradyphrenia (i.e., slowed
the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (Paulsen et al., thinking) that parallels the bradykinesia that
1995; Peinemann et al., 2005; Pillon et al., 1991; characterizes the disorder. Conversely, patients
Ward et al., 2006), the Stroop Test (Peinemann with AD are impaired in performing mental
et al., 2005; Ward et al., 2006), the Tower of rotation, perhaps due to extrapersonal visual
London Test (Lange et al., 1995), the Gambling orientation deficits secondary to neocortical
Decision Making task (Stout et al., 2001), and damage in regions involved in processing visual
tests of verbal concept formation (Hanes et al., motion (e.g., the middle temporal gyrus).
1995). Similar deficits in executive functions Another study that directly compared visu-
occur in AD (for reviews, see Duke & Kaszniak, ospatial abilities in patients with AD or HD
2000; Perry & Hodges, 1999), but few studies examined their ability to draw and copy clocks
have directly compared this aspect of cognition (Rouleau et al, 1992). The two patient groups
in the two disorders. Further research is needed were impaired on both conditions of this task,
to determine if specific aspects of executive dys- but patients with AD were significantly worse
function are more common in one dementia syn- in the draw-to-command condition than in
drome than the other, and to establish whether the copy condition, whereas patients with HD
or not this facet of cognitive impairment can were equally impaired in both conditions. A
differentiate between AD and HD. qualitative analysis of the types of errors pro-
Visuospatial processing deficits occur in duced showed that HD patients tended to make
both AD (for review, see Cronin-Golomb & graphic, visuospatial, and planning errors in
Amick, 2001) and HD (Brandt & Butters, 1986; both the command and copy conditions consis-
Brouwers et al., 1984; Bylsma et al., 1992; Caine tent with planning and motor deficits mediated
et al., 1986; Josiassen et al., 1983; Lawrence by frontal-subcortical dysfunction. In contrast,
et al., 2000; Ward et al., 2006). In one of the few AD patients tended to make conceptual or
studies to directly compare visuospatial per- semantically based errors (e.g., drawing a face
formance in the two disorders, Brouwers and without numbers, or hands) in the command
colleagues (1984) showed that AD and HD had condition but not the copy condition, consis-
differential impacts on personal and extraper- tent with a deficit in accessing knowledge of the
sonal orientation abilities. Patients with AD, attributes and meaning of a clock due to neo-
but not those with HD, were impaired on tests cortical damage in regions supporting semantic
of visuoconstructional ability that required knowledge.
extrapersonal orientation (e.g., copying a com-
plex figure). Patients with HD, but not those
Alzheimer’s Disease vs. Dementia with
with AD, were impaired on visuospatial tasks
Lewy Bodies (DLB)
that required personal orientation (e.g., the
Money Road Map Test). This dissociation The dementia syndromes associated with AD
was supported by the results of a more recent and DLB are quite similar and include insidious
The Neuropsychology of Dementia 177

onset of cognitive decline with early involve- tests of visual attention, size and form discrimi-
ment of memory (Hansen et al., 1990; Hansen & nation, and visual figure-ground segregation. A
Galasko, 1992; McKeith et al., 1996). Mild par- study of visual search processes (Cormak et al.,
kinsonism (e.g., bradykinesia, rigidity, masked 2004) showed that DLB patients were more
facies), recurrent and well-formed visual hal- impaired than AD patients in the ability to per-
lucinations, and fluctuations in attention or form serial search that required feature integra-
alertness (Cercy & Bylsma, 1997; Galasko et al., tion (i.e., detect a single red target circle within
1996; Hansen et al., 1990; McKeith et al., 1996; arrays of 2, 8, or 16 green circles and red squares
Merdes et al., 2003) are more prevalent in DLB distracters) and in “preattentive” parallel search
than in AD, and are the basis for consensus cri- processes that usually elicit the “popout” phe-
teria designed to help clinically diagnose DLB nomenon (i.e., detecting a single red target cir-
and distinguish it from AD (McKeith et al., cle within arrays of 2, 8, or 16 green distractor
1996, 2005). Unfortunately, these clinical fea- circles). Performance on the parallel search task
tures are not ubiquitous in DLB and occur with provided relatively good sensitivity (85%) and
only about 50% frequency at any time during specificity (87%) for distinguishing patients
the course of the disorder (Merdes et al., 2003). with DLB from those with AD.
Thus, patients found to have DLB at autopsy The disproportionately severe visuospatial
have often been clinically diagnosed as hav- deficits exhibited by patients with DLB may be
ing probable or possible AD during life (e.g., related to occipital cortex dysfunction, which
Hansen et al., 1990; Merdes et al., 2003). does not usually occur in patients with AD. A
A number of studies have compared the neu- number of studies have shown hypometabolism
ropsychological deficits associated with DLB and decreased blood flow in primary visual and
and AD. These studies have consistently shown visual association cortex in DLB but not in AD
that the most salient difference between the two (e.g., Minoshima et al., 2001). Neuropathologic
disorders is disproportionately severe visuospa- studies have identified white matter spongiform
tial and visuoconstructive deficits in DLB. This change with coexisting gliosis in the occipi-
distinction has been shown using tests of visual tal cortex of patients with DLB (Higuchi et al.,
perception (Calderon et al., 2001; Lambon Ralph 2000), and in some cases deposition of Lewy
et al., 2001; Mori et al., 2000), visual search bodies is also observed (e.g., Gomez-Tortosa
(Cormak et al., 2004), drawing simple and com- et al., 1999). In contrast, AD pathology in the
plex two-dimensional figures (Aarsland et al., occipital cortex is rare. The relationship between
2003; Connor et al., 1998; Galasko et al., 1996; the structural and metabolic abnormalities in
Gnanalingham et al., 1996,1997; Hansen et al., the occipital cortex of patients with DLB and
1990; Noe et al., 2003; Salmon et al., 1996), their prominent visuospatial deficits remains
and construction of three-dimensional objects unknown.
(Hansen et al., 1990; Shimomura et al., 1998). In addition to disproportionate visuospatial
Calderon and colleagues (2001), for exam- deficits, patients with DLB often have greater
ple, found that DLB patients performed worse deficits in attention and executive functions
than AD patients on tests of fragmented letter than patients with AD. Studies have shown
identification, discrimination of “real” objects that DLB patients perform worse than equally
from nonobjects, and segregation of overlap- demented AD patients on measures of attention
ping figures. These particularly severe deficits such as the WAIS-R Digit Span subtest (Hansen
in visuospatial and visuoperceptual abilities et al., 1990), the Cancellation Test (Noe et al.,
were apparent even though the DLB patients 2004), and a computer-based visual search
performed significantly better than the patients task that assesses the ability to focus attention
with AD on a verbal memory test and at the (Sahgal et al., 1992b). Patients with DLB are
same level on tests of semantic memory (also see also more impaired than AD patients on verbal
Lambon Ralph et al., 2001). Similar results were fluency tests that require initiation and system-
obtained by Mori and colleagues (2000), who atic retrieval from semantic memory (Aarsland
found that DLB patients performed significantly et al., 2003; Ballard et al., 1996; Connor et al.,
worse than equally demented AD patients on 1998; Galasko et al., 1998; Hansen et al., 1990),
178 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

paired-associates learning tests (Galloway et al., studies using neuropathologic (Lippa et al.,
1992), delayed matching-to-sample tests (Sahgal 1998) or magnetic resonance imaging (Barber
et al., 1992a), and spatial working memory tasks et al., 2001; Hashimoto et al., 1998) procedures
that assess both spatial memory and the ability have shown that the medial temporal lobe struc-
to use an efficient search strategy (Sahgal et al., tures important for memory (e.g., hippocampus,
1995). Abstract reasoning abilities assessed entorhinal cortex, parahippocampal gyrus) are
by the Raven Colored Progressive Matrices more severely affected in AD than in DLB. This
(Shimomura et al., 1998) and the WAIS-R may account for the poorer retention (i.e., con-
Similarities subtest (Galasko et al., 1998) are solidation) exhibited by the AD patients. The
more impaired in patients with DLB than in greater recognition memory performance of
those with AD. the DLB patients compared to patients with AD
The prominent attention and executive func- suggests that they may have a particular deficit
tion deficits associated with DLB are similar in the ability to initiate and carry out system-
to those that occur in patients with basal gan- atic retrieval of successfully stored information.
glia dysfunction that interrupts frontostriatal This retrieval deficit may be mediated by the
circuits (e.g., patients with HD). In DLB, these frontostriatal damage that occurs in DLB but
circuits may be disrupted by substantia nigra not in AD.
pathology that interrupts dopaminergic projec- The results of the studies reviewed above
tions to the striatum, and by direct neocortical indicate that visuospatial, attention, and exec-
Lewy body pathology in the association areas of utive function deficits are more prominent in
the frontal lobes. In addition, AD pathology may DLB than AD, whereas memory impairment
be superimposed upon the Lewy body pathol- is greater in AD than in DLB. These distinct
ogy in the frontal cortex of patients with DLB. deficit patterns have been confirmed in a num-
This combination of pathologic processes may ber of recent studies that compared clinically
induce the disproportionately severe executive diagnosed or autopsy-confirmed DLB and AD
function and attention deficits that characterize patient groups on relatively extensive batteries
the disease. of neuropsychological tests (Ferman et al., 2006;
Although deficits in visuospatial abili- Guidi et al., 2006; Johnson et al., 2005; Kraybill
ties, executive functions, and attention are et al., 2005; Stavitsky et al., 2006) (see Figure 8–7).
usually greater in patients with DLB than in The robustness of these deficit patterns (par-
equally demented patients with AD, memory ticularly of the disproportionate visuospatial
is often more impaired in patients with AD. deficits in DLB) may have important clinical
Furthermore, qualitative differences in impaired utility in distinguishing between AD and DLB
memory processes are evident in the two dis- in mildly demented patients (Tiraboschi et al.,
orders. These differences were highlighted in a 2006).
study that directly compared the performances
of patients with autopsy-confirmed DLB (all
Alzheimer’s Disease vs. Frontotemporal
with concomitant AD pathology consistent with
Dementia
Lewy body variant of AD) and patients with
autopsy-confirmed AD on the CVLT and the AD and FTD are clinically similar and diffi-
WMS-R Logical Memory Test (Hamilton et al., cult to distinguish during life (Mendez et al.,
2004). Despite equivalent deficits in their ability 1993; Varma et al., 1999). Indeed, Mendez and
to learn new verbal information on these tests, colleagues (1993) found that 86% of autopsy-
the DLB patients exhibited better retention and confirmed FTD patients had been clinically mis-
better recognition memory than patients with diagnosed with AD during life. Some success
AD (also see Ballard et al., 1996; Calderon et al., has been achieved in differentiating between
2001; Connor et al., 1998; Heyman et al., 1999; FTD and AD on the basis of behavioral symp-
Salmon et al., 1996; Shimomura et al., 1998; toms (e.g., Barber et al., 1995; Bozeat et al., 2000;
Walker et al., 1997). These differences are con- Kertesz et al., 2000; Mendez et al., 1998; Miller
sistent with the particular pathological changes et al., 1997), but this success has been limited.
that occur in the two disorders. A number of These findings have led some investigators to
The Neuropsychology of Dementia 179

Visual reproduction copy Block design

20 45
40

15 * 35
30
Mean Score

25
10
20 *

15
5
10
5
0 0
LBV AD NC LBV AD NC

Boston Naming Test Logical Memory Savings Score

30 100
90
25
80
70
20
Mean Score

60
15 50
40
10
30 *
20
5
10
0 0
LBV AD NC LBV AD NC
* DLB significantly different from AD

Figure 8–7. The average scores achieved by normal control (NC) subjects (n = 24), patients with Alzheimer’s
disease (AD; n = 24), and patients with dementia with Lewy bodies (all with the Lewy body variant (LBV) of
AD; n = 24) on several neuropsychological tests. The LBV patients were disproportionately impaired com-
pared to the AD patients on tests of visuospatial ability (Visual Reproduction Copy and Block Design), but less
impaired on tests of verbal memory (Logical Memory Savings Score). The patient groups did not differ on a test
of confrontation naming (Boston Naming Test) (Adapted from Hamilton et al., 2004).

propose that FTD and AD might be differenti- visuospatial and constructional abilities were
ated on the basis of the nature and severity of found to be less impaired in FTD than in AD
their cognitive deficits. Unfortunately, studies in some studies (Elfgren et al., 1994; Mendez
that directly compare neuropsychological defi- et al., 1996), but not others (Frisoni et al., 1995;
cits in FTD and AD have provided inconsistent Lindau et al., 1998; Mathuranath et al., 2000;
results. A number of studies showed a greater Pachana et al., 1996). Several studies found bet-
verbal fluency deficit in FTD than in AD (Frisoni ter visuospatial memory in FTD than in AD
et al., 1995; Lindau et al., 1998; Mathuranath (Frisoni et al., 1995; Pachana et al., 1996), but
et al., 2000), but others did not replicate this others failed to replicate this finding (Binetti et
result (Binetti et al., 2000; Pasquier et al., al., 2000; Lindau et al., 1998; Thomas-Anterion
1995; Thomas-Anterion et al., 2000). Similarly, et al., 2000). Reduced verbal output, stereotyped
180 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

language, and echolalia are observed clinically because semantic category fluency requires a
in FTD (Johanson & Hagberg, 1989; Miller et al., search through semantic memory and is criti-
1997; Neary et al., 1988), but these characteris- cally dependent upon an adequate knowledge
tics have not been formally compared in FTD of the physical and/or functional attributes
and AD patients. There appears to be little or that define a particular semantic category. In
no difference between FTD and AD patients contrast, letter fluency requires the use of pho-
on measures of confrontation naming (Binetti nemic cues to guide retrieval and may thus
et al., 2000; Mendez et al., 1996; Pachana et al., require greater effort and more active strategic
1996; Thomas-Anterion et al., 2000). search than semantic category fluency. Results
Several studies that examined profi les of cog- showed that FTD patients performed worse
nitive deficits associated with FTD and AD dem- than AD patients overall, and showed that
onstrated a subtle difference in the patterns of FTD patients had similar impairment in let-
deficits engendered by the two disorders (Forstl ter and semantic category fluency, whereas AD
et al., 1996; Starkstein et al., 1994). In these patients had greater impairment in semantic
studies, patients with FTD had a more severe fluency than letter fluency. A measure of the
deficit in executive functions than in other cog- disparity between letter and semantic fluency
nitive abilities, whereas AD patients had execu- (the Semantic Index) correctly classified 26 of
tive dysfunction that was proportional to their 32 AD patients (82%) and 12 of 16 FTD patients
deficits in language and visuospatial abilities, (75%). Interestingly, the misclassified FTD sub-
and less prominent than their episodic memory jects all had clinical presentations of progressive
deficit. These results were confirmed by a more nonfluent aphasia (Mesulam, 1982) or Semantic
recent study that directly compared neuropsy- Dementia (i.e., severe naming and word com-
chological deficit profi les in autopsy-confirmed prehension impairments in the context of flu-
FTD and AD patients who were matched for ent speech; Hodges et al., 1992; Snowden et al.,
level of dementia (i.e., MMSE scores) at the time 1989). When these cases were excluded and the
of testing (Rascovsky et al., 2002). Rascovsky analyses repeated, dissociations were apparent
and colleagues (2002) found that FTD patients in letter worse than semantic fluency for the FTD
performed significantly worse than AD patients patients and semantic worse than letter fluency
on word generation tasks sensitive to frontal for the AD patients. In addition, the Semantic
lobe dysfunction (i.e., letter and category flu- Index now correctly classified 90% of FTD and
ency tests), but significantly better on tests of AD patients. These unique patterns of fluency
memory (i.e., Mattis DRS Memory subscale) deficits may be indicative of differences in the
and visuospatial abilities (i.e., Block Design and relative contribution of frontal lobe-mediated
Clock Drawing tests) sensitive to dysfunction of retrieval deficits and temporal-lobe-mediated
medial temporal and parietal association cor- semantic deficits in FTD and AD.
tices, respectively (see Figure 8–8). A logistic Similar levels of discriminability were
regression model using letter fluency, Mattis observed in a number of additional studies that
DRS Memory subscale, and the Block Design attempted to differentiate between FTD and AD
test provided good discriminability between the on the basis of tests of executive functions, visu-
groups, correctly classifying approximately 86% ospatial abilities, and memory (Elfgren et al.,
of FTD and AD patients (91% of AD patients 1994; Gregory et al., 1997; Libon et al., 2007;
and 77% of FTD patients). Lipton et al., 2005). For example, Grossman
In a subsequent study, Rascovsky and col- et al. (2007) achieved 88% diagnostic accuracy
leagues (2007) sought to determine whether or in differentiating between autopsy-confirmed
not autopsy-confirmed FTD and AD patients FTD and AD patients on the basis of clinical
who were matched for level of dementia at the and neuropsychological features. These results
time of testing differed in the pattern of deficits indicate that distinct cognitive profi les are
they exhibited on letter and semantic category associated with FTD and AD, and suggest that
fluency tasks. Although both verbal fluency cognitive assessment may provide useful infor-
tasks engage frontal-lobe-mediated executive mation when making the differential diagnosis
processes, distinct patterns were hypothesized of FTD.
The Neuropsychology of Dementia 181

20 20 35
18 18
30
16 16
14 14 25

Mean words
Mean words

Mean score
12 12 20
10 10
15
8 8
6 6 10
4 4
5
2 2
0 0 0
AD FTD AD FTD AD FTD
Letter fluency Category fluency Block design

2.5
18
16
2
14
12
Mean score

1.5
Mean score

10

1 8
6

0.5 4
2
0 0
AD FTD AD FTD
Clock: command MDRS memory

Figure 8–8. Age- and education-adjusted means (and SEM) for the number of words produced in the let-
ter and category fluency tasks and a scores achieved on the WISC-R Block Design test, command condition
of the Clock Drawing Test and Mattis Dementia Rating Scale (MDRS) memory subscale by patients with
Frontotemporal dementia (FTD) (black bars) or Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) (clear bars). Patients with FTD per-
formed significantly worse than those with AD on verbal fluency tasks, but significantly better on tests of
memory and visuospatial abilities. All group differences are significant at p < .05. (Adapted from Rascovsky
et al., 2002).

episodic memory (particularly delayed recall)


Alzheimer’s Disease vs. Vascular (Desmond, 2004; Graham et al., 2004; Kertesz
Dementia & Clydesdale, 1994; Lafosse et al., 1997; Lamar
Studies of the neuropsychological deficits asso- et al., 1997). These studies also suggest that exec-
ciated with VaD have primarily focused on dif- utive dysfunction is usually the most prominent
ferentiating between subcortical VaD and AD deficit in subcortical VaD, presumably because
(see also chapter by Brown in this volume). For subcortical pathology interrupts frontosubcor-
the most part, these studies show that patients tical circuits that mediate this aspect of cogni-
with subcortical VaD are more impaired than tion. Consistent with this possibility, Price and
those with AD on tests of executive functions, colleagues (2005) found that VaD patients with
whereas patients with AD are more impaired a significant volume of white matter abnormal-
than those with subcortical VaD on tests of ity on imaging exhibited a profi le of greater
182 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

executive and visuoconstructional deficits than especially apparent in episodic memory (par-
impairment of memory and language abilities. ticularly delayed recall), semantic knowledge,
Most of the studies that have shown distinct and some aspects of executive functions. There
cognitive profi les in subcortical VaD and AD is also emerging evidence that the cortical neu-
employed clinically diagnosed patients with- ropathology of AD causes a loss of functional
out autopsy confirmation of diagnosis. The connectivity that adversely affects interaction
lack of autopsy confirmation may have allowed between distinct and relatively intact cortical
some degree of diagnostic misclassification information-processing systems. This loss may
since AD and VaD often overlap in their clini- cause, for example, an impaired ability to “bind”
cal presentations. In a study that avoided this distinct visual stimulus features that are effec-
potential confound, Reed and colleagues (2007) tively processed in different cortical streams
found that patients with autopsy-confirmed AD (i.e., motion and color). The qualitatively dis-
had a deficit in episodic memory (both verbal tinct pattern of cognitive impairment associated
and nonverbal memory) that was significantly with early AD appears to be less salient in very
greater than their executive function deficit. In old patients than in younger patients when they
contrast, patients with autopsy-confirmed sub- are compared to their age-appropriate cohort.
cortical VaD had a deficit in executive functions This finding may be driven in large part by
that was greater than their deficit in verbal (but the age-associated decline in certain cognitive
not nonverbal) episodic memory. An analysis abilities (e.g., speed of information-processing,
of individual patient profi les showed that 71% retrieval processes) that occurs with normal
of AD patients exhibited a profi le with mem- healthy aging. Progress has been made in iden-
ory impairment more prominent than execu- tifying cognitive markers of “prodromal” AD
tive dysfunction, whereas only 45% of patients that are apparent before the development of
with subcortical VaD exhibited a profi le with the dementia syndrome. Decline in episodic
more prominent executive dysfunction than memory is usually the earliest cognitive change
memory impairment. Interestingly, relatively that occurs during the prodromal stage of AD,
severe cerebrovascular disease at autopsy was but decline in other cognitive domains, and
often not associated with clinically significant asymmetry in cognitive abilities (e.g., high
cognitive decline. When the profile analysis was verbal vs. low visuospatial performance), may
restricted to those patients who exhibited sig- also predict the imminent onset of dementia.
nificant cognitive impairment at their clinical Neuropsychological research has also identified
assessment, the distinction between subcortical patterns of cognitive impairment that might
VaD and AD patients was more pronounced. A help distinguish AD from other neuropatho-
low memory profile was exhibited by 79% of AD logically distinct neurodegenerative disorders
patients (5% with a low executive profile) and a such as HD, DLB, FTD, and VaD. Knowledge
low executive profile by 67% of subcortical VaD of these differences is clinically important for
patients (0% with a low memory profile). The distinguishing among various causes of demen-
results suggest that relatively distinct cognitive tia, and provides useful information about the
deficit profi les might be clinically useful in dif- brain–behavior relationships that mediate cog-
ferentiating between subcortical VaD and AD, nitive abilities affected by various neurodegen-
but additional research with autopsied patients erative diseases.
is needed to further define the deficit profi le
that will be most useful in this regard.
Conclusions and Future Directions
Clinical and experimental neuropsychological
Chapter Summary
research has made great strides in differentiat-
A wealth of neuropsychological research has ing between the cognitive changes that signal
shown that the cognitive deficits associated with the onset of a dementia syndrome and those
AD are distinct from age-associated cognitive that occur as a normal consequence of aging.
decline. Quantitative and qualitative differences Many of the basic cognitive processes that are
between early AD and normal aging effects are adversely affected by AD have been identified,
The Neuropsychology of Dementia 183

and the prodromal cognitive changes that pre- (e.g., DLB, FTD), and to determine how these
dict the subsequent development of dementia profiles can be incorporated with other clinical
are being uncovered. Considerable progress has features to improve the accuracy of differential
also been made in delineating different patterns diagnosis in very mildly demented individu-
of relatively preserved and impaired cogni- als. Accurate early diagnosis is a particularly
tive abilities that distinguish between AD and important goal since the various neurodegener-
other age-associated neurodegenerative disor- ative disorders are likely to respond differently
ders. Understanding the cognitive distinctions to potential treatments for dementia that are in
between these disorders can aid in the devel- development. Finally, the strides made in under-
opment of better differential diagnosis and standing the neuropsychology of dementia need
reveal the nature of brain–behavior relation- to be linked to those made in neuroimaging
ships underlying memory, language, executive research with consonant goals of early detec-
functions, and other cognitive abilities that are tion and maximization of intervention efforts.
affected. This approach will lead to better understanding
The search for antecedent markers of demen- of the brain–behavior relationships underlying
tia is predicated on the notion that significant dementia.
neural dysfunction and cell death occurs well in
advance of the clinical diagnosis. It is impera-
tive to reliably identify individuals prior to the Acknowledgments
development of significant clinical symptoms in Preparation of this chapter was supported by
order to begin treatments that might halt or slow funds from NIA grants P50 AG005131 (MWB,
disease progression. Therefore, the identification DPS), RO1 AG012674 (MWB), K24 AG026431
and validation of subtle cognitive abnormalities (MWB), and RO1 AG012963 (DPS) to the
(e.g., poor delayed recall performance, cogni- University of California San Diego and the
tive asymmetry) for prodromal diagnosis of Veterans Medical Research Foundation, P30
AD remains an extremely important goal. The AG019610 (AWK) to the Arizona Alzheimer’s
role of cortical disconnectivity in producing the Consortium, and by grant IIRG-07–59343
specific pattern of cognitive deficits that occurs (MWB) from the Alzheimer’s Association. The
in early AD has only begun to be assessed, but authors wish to thank their many collabora-
the identification of cognitive processes that are tors at the UCSD Alzheimer’s Disease Research
particularly vulnerable to the effects of cortical Center and the Arizona Alzheimer’s Disease
disconnectivity might provide a useful cognitive Core Center.
marker for the disease and a means by which to
assess the effects of interventions that specifi-
cally target cortical function (e.g., the NMDA References
receptor antagonist memantine). Clearly, addi- Aarsland, D., Litvan, I., Salmon, D. P., Galasko,
tional research is needed in this area. D., Wentzel-Larsen, T., & Larsen, P. (2003).
Despite intense research focus, it remains dif- Performance on the dementia rating scale in
ficult to estimate rate of cognitive decline in AD Parkinson’s disease with dementia and dementia
and other age-related neurodegenerative dis- with Lewy bodies: Comparison with progres-
eases, although there is promising evidence that sive supranuclear palsy and Alzheimer’s disease.
certain aspects of current cognitive performance Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry,
can predict subsequent rate of global cognitive 74, 1215–1220.
decline in patients with AD (e.g., Chan et al., Albert, M. L., Feldman, R. G., & Willis, A. L. (1974).
The “subcortical dementia” of progressive supra-
1995). Further research is needed to confirm this
nuclear palsy. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery
possibility and to generalize it to other neurode- and Psychiatry, 37, 121–130.
generative disorders such as DLB and FTD (e.g., Albert, M. S., & Blacker, D. (2006). Mild cognitive
see Wicklund et al., 2007). In addition, there is impairment and dementia. Annual Review of
a continuing need to identify differences in the Clinical Psychology, 2, 379–388.
profiles of cognitive deficits associated with AD Albert, M. S., Butters, N., & Brandt, J. (1981).
and other age-related neurodegenerative diseases Development of remote memory loss in patients
184 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

with Huntington’s disease. Journal of Clinical and Bachman, D. L., Wolf, P. A., Linn, R., Knoefel, J. E.,
Experimental Neuropsychology, 3, 1–12. Cobb, J. L., Belanger, A. J., et al. (1992). Prevalence
Albert, M. S., Butters, N., & Levin, J. (1979). of dementia and probable senile dementia of
Temporal gradients in the retrograde amnesia of the Alzheimer type in the Framingham study.
patients with alcoholic Korsakoff disease. Archives Neurology, 42, 115–119.
of Neurology, 36, 211–216. Bachman, D. L., Wolf, P. A., Linn, R., Knoefel, J. E.,
Albert, M. S., Moss, M. B., Tanzi, R., & Jones, K. Cobb, J. L., Belanger, A. J., et al. (1993). Incidence
(2001). Preclinical prediction of AD using neu- of dementia and probable Alzheimer’s disease in
ropsychological tests. Journal of the International a general population: The Framingham study.
Neuropsychological Society, 7, 631–639. Neurology, 43, 515–519.
Albin, R. L. (1993). Antagonistic pleiotropy, muta- Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford:
tion accumulation, and human genetic disease. Claredon Press.
Genetica, 91, 279–286. Baddeley, A. D., Bressi, S., Della Sala, S., Logie, R., &
Alexander, D. M., Williams, L. M., Gatt, J. M., Spinnler, H. (1991). The decline of working mem-
Dobson-Stone, C., Kuan, S. A., Todd, E. G., et al. ory in Alzheimer’s disease: A longitudinal study.
(2007). The contribution of apolipoprotein E alleles Brain, 114, 2521–2542.
on cognitive performance and dynamic neural Ballard, C., Patel, A., Oyebode, F., & Wilcock,
activity over six decades. Biological Psychology, 75, G. (1996). Cognitive decline in patients with
229–238. Alzheimer’s disease, vascular dementia and senile
Alexander, G. E., DeLong, M. R., & Strick, P. L. dementia of the Lewy body type. Age and Ageing,
(1986). Parallel organization of functionally seg- 25, 209–213.
regated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Band, G. P., Ridderinkhof, K. R., & Segalowitz, S.
Annual Review of Neuroscience, 9, 357–381. (2002). Explaining neurocognitive aging: Is one
Alzheimer, A. (1907). Über eine eigenartige factor enough? Brain and Cognition, 49, 259–267.
Erkrankung der Hirnrinde. Allg Z Psychiatr Barber, R., McKeith, I. G., Ballard, C., Gholkar, A.,
Psychol-Gerichtl Med, 64, 146–148. & O’Brien, J. T. (2001). A comparison of medial
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and lateral temporal lobe atrophy in demen-
and statistical manual of mental sisorders, 4th ed. tia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer’s disease:
Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association. Magnetic resonance imaging volumetric study.
Andreasen, N., Blennow, K., Sjodin, C., Winblad, Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 12,
B., & Svardsudd, K. (1999). Prevalence and inci- 198–205.
dence of clinically diagnosed memory impair- Barber, R., Snowden, J. S., & Craufurd, D. (1995).
ments in a geographically defined general Frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s dis-
population in Sweden. The Pitea Dementia Project. ease: Retrospective differentiation using infor-
Neuroepidemiology, 18, 144–155. mation from informants. Journal of Neurology,
Aronoff, J. M., Gonnerman, L. M., Almor, A., Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 59, 61–70.
Arunachalam, S., Kempler, D., & Andersen, E. Barnes, D. E., Yaffe, K., Satariano, W. A., & Tager,
S. (2006). Information content versus relational I. B. (2003). A longitudinal study of cardiorespi-
knowledge: Semantic deficits in patients with ratory fitness and cognitive function in healthy
Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 44, 21–35. older adults. Journal of the American Geriatrics
Backman, L., Jones, S., Berger, A. K., Laukka, E. J., & Society, 51(4), 459–465.
Small, B. J. (2004). Multiple cognitive deficits dur- Bartzokis, G. (2004). Age-related myelin breakdown:
ing the transition to Alzheimer’s disease. Journal A developmental model of cognitive decline and
of Internal Medicine, 256, 195–204. Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiology of Aging, 25,
Backman, L., Jones, S., Berger, A. K., Laukka, E. J., 5–18.
& Small, B. J. (2005). Cognitive impairment in Bayles, K. A., Boone, D. R., Tomoeda, C. K., Slauson,
preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: A meta-analysis. T. J., & Kaszniak, A. W. (1989). Differentiating
Neuropsychology, 19, 520–531. Alzheimer’s patients from the normal elderly and
Backman, L., & Small, B. J. (1998). Influences of stroke patients with aphasia. Journal of Speech and
cognitive support on episodic remembering: Hearing Disorders, 54, 74–87.
Tracing the process of loss from normal aging to Bayles, K. A., & Kaszniak, A. W. (1987).
Alzheimer’s disease. Psychology and Aging, 13, Communication and cognition in normal aging
267–276. and dementia. Boston: College Hill/Little-Brown.
Backman, L., Small, B. J., & Fratiglioni, L. (2001). Bayles, K. A., & Tomoeda, C. K. (1983). Confrontation
Stability of the preclinical episodic memory deficit naming impairment in dementia. Brain and
in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain, 124, 96–102. Language, 19, 98–114.
The Neuropsychology of Dementia 185

Bayley, P. J., Salmon, D. P., Bondi, M. W., Bui, B. K., Braak, H., & Braak, E. (1991). Neuropathological
Olichney, J., Delis, D. C., et al. (2000). Comparison staging of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta
of the serial position effect in very mild Alzheimer’s Neuropathologica, 82, 239–259.
disease, mild Alzheimer’s disease, and amnesia Brandt, J., & Butters, N. (1986). The neuropsychology
associated with electroconvulsive therapy. Journal of Huntington’s disease. Trends in Neuroscience, 9,
of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 118–120.
290–298. Brandt, J., & Bylsma, F. W. (1993). The dementia
Beatty, W. W., Salmon, D. P., Butters, N., Heindel, of Huntington’s disease. In R. W. Parks, R. F.
W. C., & Granholm, E. L. (1988). Retrograde Zec, and R. S. Wilson (Eds.), Neuropsychology of
amnesia in patients with Alzheimer’s disease or Alzheimer’s disease and other dementias (pp. 265–
Huntington’s disease. Neurobiology of Aging, 9, 282). New York: Oxford University Press.
181–186. Brandt, J., Corwin, J., & Krafft, L. (1992). Is verbal rec-
Binetti, G., Locascio, J. J., Corkin, S., Vonsattel, J. ognition memory really different in Huntington’s
P., & Growdon, J. H. (2000). Differences between and Alzheimer’s disease? Journal of Clinical and
Pick disease and Alzheimer disease in the clinical Experimental Neuropsychology, 14, 773–784.
appearance and rate of cognitive decline. Archives Brouwers, P., Cox, C., Martin, A., Chase, T., & Fedio,
of Neurology, 57, 225–232. P. (1984). Differential perceptual-spatial impair-
Bloss, C. S., Delis, D. C., Salmon, D. P., & Bondi, M. ment in Huntington’s and Alzheimer’s dementias.
W. (2008). APOE genotype is associated with left- Archives of Neurology, 41, 1073–1076.
handedness and visuospatial skills in children. Brun, A., Englund, B., Gustafson, L., Passant, U.,
Neurobiology of Aging [Epub ahead of print]. Mann, D.M.A, Neary, D., & Snowden, J.S. (1994).
Bondi, M. W., Houston, W. S., Salmon, D. P., Corey- Clinical and neuropathological criteria for fron-
Bloom, J., Katzman, R., Thal, L. J., et al. (2003). totemporal dementia. Journal of Neurology,
Neuropsychological deficits associated with Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 57, 416–418.
Alzheimer’s disease in the very old: Discrepencies Brun, A. (1987). Frontal lobe degeneration of the
in raw vs. standardized scores. Journal of the non-Alzheimer type I. Neuropathology. Archives
International Neuropsychological Society, 9, Gerontology and Geriatrics, 6, 193–208.
783–795. Buckner, R. L. (2004). Memory and executive func-
Bondi, M. W., Monsch, A. U., Butters, N., Salmon, tion in aging and AD: Multiple factors that cause
D. P., & Paulsen, J. S. (1993). Utility of a modified decline and reserve factors that compensate.
version of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in Neuron, 44, 195–208.
the detection of dementia of the Alzheimer type. Buschke, H. (1973). Selective reminding for analy-
Clinical Neuropsychologist, 7, 161–170. sis of memory and learning. Journal of Verbal
Bondi, M. W., Monsch, A. U., Galasko, D., Butters, Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 543–550.
N., Salmon, D. P., & Delis, D. C. (1994). Preclinical Buschke, H., Sliwinski, M. J., Kuslansky, G., & Lipton,
cognitive markers of dementia of the Alzheimer R. B. (1997). Diagnosis of early dementia by the
type. Neuropsychology, 8, 374–384. double memory test. Neurology, 48, 989–997.
Bondi, M. W., Salmon, D. P., Galasko, D., Thomas, R. Butters, N., Granholm, E., Salmon, D. P., Grant, I.,
G., & Thal, L. J. (1999). Neuropsychological func- & Wolfe, J. (1987). Episodic and semantic mem-
tion and apolipoprotein E genotype in the preclin- ory: A comparison of amnesic and demented
ical detection of Alzheimer’s disease. Psychology patients. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
and Aging, 14, 295–303. Neuropsychology, 9, 479–497.
Borenstein, A. R., Copenhaver, C. I., & Mortimer, J. Butters, N., Salmon, D. P., Cullum, C. M., Cairns,
A. (2006). Early-life risk factors for Alzheimer dis- P., Troster, A. I., Jacobs, D., et al. (1988).
ease. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, Differentiation of amnesic and demented patients
20, 63–72. with the Wechsler Memory Scale—Revised.
Bowles, N. L., Obler, L. K., & Albert, M. L. (1987). Clinical Neuropsychologist, 2, 133–148.
Naming errors in healthy aging and dementia of Butters, N., Sax, D. S., Montgomery, K., & Tarlow, S.
the Alzheimer type. Cortex, 23, 519–524. (1978). Comparison of the neuropsychological defi-
Bozeat, S., Gregory, C. A., Lambon Ralph, M. A., cits associated with early and advanced Huntington’s
& Hodges, J. R. (2000). Which neuropsychiatric disease. Archives of Neurology, 35, 585–589.
and behavioral features distinguish frontal and Butters, N., Wolfe, J., Granholm, E., & Martone, M.
temporal variants of frontotemporal dementia (1986). An assessment of verbal recall, recognition
and Alzheimer’s disease? Journal of Neurology, and fluency abilities in patients with Huntington’s
Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 69, 178–186. disease. Cortex, 22, 11–32.
186 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Butters, N., Wolfe, J., Martone, M., Granholm, E., disease: A prospective community study. Archives
& Cermak, L. S. (1985). Memory disorders asso- of General Psychiatry, 58, 853–858.
ciated with Huntington’s disease: Verbal recall, Chertkow, H., & Bub, D. (1990). Semantic memory
verbal recognition and procedural memory. loss in dementia of Alzheimer’s type. Brain, 113,
Neuropsychologia, 23, 729–743. 397–417.
Bylsma, F. W., Brandt, J., & Strauss, M. E. (1992). Chui, H. C. (2007). Subcortical ischemic vascular
Personal and extrapersonal orientation in dementia. Neurologic Clinics, 25, 717–740.
Huntington’s disease patients and those at risk. Chui, H. C., Mack, W., Jackson, J. E., Mungas, D.,
Cortex, 28, 113–122. Reed, B. R., Tinklenberg, J., et al. (2000). Clinical
Caine, E. D., Bamford, K. A., Schiffer, R. B., Shoulson, criteria for the diagnosis of vascular dementia: A
I., & Levy, S. (1986). A controlled neuropsycho- multicenter study of comparability and interrater
logical comparison of Huntington’s disease and reliability. Archives of Neurology, 57, 191–196.
multiple sclerosis. Archives of Neurology, 43, Chui, H. C., Victoroff, J., Margolin, D., Jagust, W.,
249–254. Shankle, R., & Katzman, R. (1992). Criteria for the
Caine, E. D., Ebert, M. H., & Weingartner, H. (1977). diagnosis of ischemic vascular dementia proposed
An outline for the analysis of dementia: The mem- by the State of California Alzheimer’s Disease
ory disorder of Huntington’s disease. Neurology, Diagnostic Treatment Centers. Neurology, 42,
27, 1087–1092. 473–480.
Calderon, J., Perry, R. J., Erzinclioglu, S. W., Berrios, Churchill, J. D., Galvez, R., Colcombe, S., Swain, R.
G. E., Dening, T. R., & Hodges, J. R. (2001). A., Kramer, A. F., & Greenought, W. T. (2002).
Perception, attention, and working memory are Exercise, experience, and the aging brain.
disproportionately impaired in dementia with Neurobiology of Aging, 23, 941–955.
Lewy bodies compared with Alzheimer’s disease. Collette, F., Van der Linden, M., Bechet, S., &
Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, Salmon, E. (1999). Phonological loop and central
70, 157–164. executive functioning in Alzheimer’s disease.
Capitani, E., Della Sala, S., Logie, R., & Spinnler, Neuropsychologia, 37, 905–918.
H. (1992). Recency, primacy, and memory: Collie, A., & Maruff, P. (2000). The neuropsychology
Reappraising and standardising the serial position of preclinical Alzheimer’s disease and mild cogni-
curve. Cortex, 28, 315–342. tive impairment. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral
Carlesimo, G. A., Sabbadini, M., Fadda, L., & Review, 24, 365–374.
Caltagirone, C. (1995). Different components in Connor, D. J., Salmon, D. P., Sandy, T. J., Galasko, D.,
word-list forgetting of pure amnesics, degenera- Hansen, L. A., & Thal, L. (1998). Cognitive pro-
tive demented and healthy subjects. Cortex, 31, fi les of autopsy-confirmed Lewy body variant vs.
735–745. pure Alzheimer’s disease. Archives of Neurology,
Cercy, S. P., & Bylsma, F. W. (1997). Lewy body 55, 994–1000.
and progressive dementia: A critical review Corder, E. H., Saunders, A. M., Strittmatter, W. J.,
and meta-analysis. Journal of the International Schmechel, D. E., Gaskell, P. C., Small, G. W., et al.
Neuropsychological Society, 3, 179–194. (1993). Gene dose of apolipoprotein E type 4 allele
Cermak, L. S. (1984). The episodic/semantic distinc- and the risk of Alzheimer’s disease in late onset
tion in amnesia. In L. R. Squire and N. Butters families. Science, 261, 921–923.
(Eds.), The neuropsychology of memory (pp. 52–62). Corey-Bloom, J., Wiederholt, W. C., Edelstein, S.,
New York: Guilford Press. Salmon, D. P., Cahn, D., & Barrett-Connor, E.
Chan, A. S., Salmon, D. P., Butters, N., & Johnson, (1996). Cognitive and functional status of the old-
S. (1995). Semantic network abnormality pre- est old. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society,
dicts rate of cognitive decline in patients with 44, 671–674.
Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of the International Cormak, F., Gray, A., Ballard, C., & Tovee, M. J.
Neuropsychological Society, 1, 297–303. (2004). A failure of “pop-out” in visual search tasks
Chan, A. S., Salmon, D. P., & Butters N. (1998). in dementia with Lewy bodies as compared to
Semantic network abnormalities in patients with Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s disease. International
Alzheimer’s disease. In R. W. Parks, D. S. Levine, Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 19, 763–772.
and D. L. Long (Eds.), Fundamentals of neural Cronin-Golomb, A., & Amick, M. (2001). Spatial
network modeling (pp. 381–393). Cambridge, MA: abilities in aging, Alzheimer’s disease, and
MIT Press. Parkinson’s disease. In F. Boller and S. F. Cappa
Chen, P., Ratcliff, G., Belle, S. H., Cauley, J. A., (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology, 2nd edi-
DeKosky, S. T., & Ganguli, M. (2001). Patterns of tion, (Vol 6): Aging and Dementia (pp. 119–143).
cognitive decline in presymptomatic Alzheimer Amsterdam: Elsevier.
The Neuropsychology of Dementia 187

Corsellis, J. A. N., Bruton, C. J., & Freeman-Browne, Dubois, B., Feldman, H. H., Jacova, C., DeKosky, S. T.,
D. (1973). The aftermath of boxing. Psychological Barberger-Gateau, P., Cummings, J., et al. (2007).
Medicine, 3, 270–303. Research criteria for the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s
Cuetos, R., Arango-Lasprilla, J. C., Uribe, C., disease: revising the NINCDS-ADRDA criteria.
Valencia, C., & Lopera, F. (2007). Linguistic Lancet Neurology, 6, 734–746.
changes in verbal expression: A preclinical marker Duke, L. M., & Kaszniak, A. W. (2000). Executive
of Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of the International control functions in degenerative dementias: A
Neuropsychological Society, 13, 433–439. comparative review. Neuropsychology Review, 10,
Cummings, J. L., & Benson, D. F. (1992). Dementia: 75–99.
A clinical approach. Boston: Butterworth- Dunkin, J. J., Osato, S., & Leuchter, A. F. (1995).
Heinemann. Relationships between EEG coherence and
De Lacoste, M., & White, C. L. (1993). The role of cor- neuropsychological tests in dementia. Clinical
tical connectivity in Alzheimer’s disease patho- Electroencephalography, 26, 47–59.
genesis: A review and model system. Neurobiology Elfgren, C., Brun, A., Gustafson, L., Johanson,
of Aging, 14, 1–16. A., Minthon, L., Passant, U., et al. (1994).
Delbeuck, X., Van der Linden, M., & Collette, F. Neuropsychological tests as discriminators
(2003). Alzheimer’s disease as a disconnection between dementia of Alzheimer’s type and
syndrome? Neuropsychology Review, 13, 79–92. Frontotemporal dementia. International Journal
Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B. of Geriatric Psychiatry, 9, 635–642.
A. (2000). The California Verbal Learning Test-II. Eslinger, P. J., & Damasio, A. R. (1986). Preserved
New York: Psychological Corp. motor learning in Alzheimer’s disease:
Delis, D. C., Massman, P. J., Butters, N., Salmon, Implications for anatomy and behavior. Journal of
D. P., Cermak, L. S., & Kramer, J. H. (1991). Neuroscience, 6, 3006–3009.
Profi les of demented and amnesic patients on the Eslinger, P. J., Damasio, A. R., Benton, A. L., & Van
California verbal learning test: Implications for Allen, M. (1985). Neuropsychologic detection
the assessment of memory disorders. Psychological of abnormal mental decline in older persons.
Assessment, 3, 19–26. Journal of the American Medical Association, 253,
Della Sala, S., Kinnear, P., Spinnler, H., & Stangalino, 670–674.
C. (2000). Color-to-figure matching in Alzheimer’s Ferman, T. J., Smith, G. E., Boeve, B. F., Graff-Radford,
disease. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 15, N. R., Lucas, J. A., et al. (2006). Neuropsychological
571–585. differentiation of dementia with Lewy bodies from
Desgranges, B., Baron, J. C., & Eustache, F. (1998). normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Clinical
The functional neuroanatomy of episodic mem- Neuropsychologist, 20, 623–636.
ory: The role of the frontal lobes, the hippocam- Festa, E., Insler, R. Z., Salmon, D. P., Paxton,
pal formation, and other areas. Neuroimage, 8, J., Hamilton, J. M., & Heindel, W. C. (2005).
198–213. Neocortical disconnectivity disrupts sensory inte-
Desmond, D. W. (2004). The neuropsychology of gration in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology,
vascular cognitive impairment: Is there a specific 19, 728–738.
cognitive impairment? Journal of the Neurological Fleischman, D. A., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (1998).
Sciences, 226, 3–7. Repetition priming in normal aging and
D’Esposito, M., Zarahn, E., Aguirre, G. K., & Rypma, Alzheimer’s disease: A review of findings and the-
B. (1999). The effect of nromal aging on the cou- ories. Psychology and Aging, 13, 88–119.
pling of neural activity to the BOLD hemody- Flicker, C., Ferris, S. H., & Reisberg, B. (1991). Mild
namic response. NeuroImage, 10, 6–14. cognitive impairment in the elderly: Predictors of
Dobkins, K. R., & Albright, T. D. (1998). The influ- dementia. Neurology, 41, 1006–1009.
ence of chromatic information on visual motion Folstein, S. E., Brandt, J., & Folstein, M. F. (1990).
processing in the primate visual system. In T Huntington’s disease. In J. L. Cummings (Ed.),
Watanabe (Ed.), High-level motion processing- Subcortical dementia (pp. 87–107). New York:
computational, neurobiological and psychophysi- Oxford University Press.
cal perspectives (pp. 53–94). Cambridge: MIT. Förstl, H., Besthorn, C., Geiger-Kabisch, C., Sattel,
Dong, M. J., Peng, B., Lin, X. T., Zhao, J., Zhou, Y. R., H., & Schreitter-Gasser, U. (1996). Frontal lobe
& Wang, R. H. (2007). The prevalence of demen- degeneration and Alzheimer’s disease: A con-
tia in the People’s Republic of China: A systematic trolled study on clinical fi ndings, volumetric brain
analysis of 1980–2004 studies. Age and Ageing, 36, changes and quantitative electroencephalography
619–624. data. Dementia, 7, 27–34.
188 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Foster, J. K., Behrmann, M., & Stuss, D. T. (1999). Comparison with Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s
Visual attention deficits in Alzheimer’s dis- diseases. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and
ease: Simple versus conjoined feature search. Psychiatry, 62, 243–252.
Neuropsychology, 13, 223–245. Goldblum, M., Gomez, C., Dalla Barba, G., Boller, F.,
Fratiglioni, L., & Wang, H. X. (2007). Brain reserve Deweer, B., Hahn, V., et al. (1998). The influence of
hypothesis in dementia. Journal of Alzheimer’s semantic and perceptual encoding on recognition
Disease, 12, 11–22. memory in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychologia,
Fratiglioni, L., Winblad, B., & von Strauss, E. (2007). 36, 717–729.
Prevention of Alzheimer’s disease and dementia. Golob, E. J., Miranda, G. G., Johnson, J. K., & Starr,
Major finding from the Kungsholmen project. A. (2001). Sensory cortical interactions in aging,
Physiology and Behavior, 92, 98–104. mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer’s dis-
Freedman, M., Leach, L., Kaplan, E., Winocur, G., ease. Neurobiology of Aging, 22, 755–763.
Shulman, K. I., & Delis, D. C. (1994). Clock draw- Gomez-Tortosa, E., Newell, K., Irizarry, M. C.,
ing: A neuropsychological analysis. New York: Albert, M., Growdon, J. H., & Hyman, B. T. (1999).
Oxford University Press. Clinical and quantitative pathologic correlates
Freedman, M., & Oscar-Berman, M. (1997). of dementia with Lewy bodies. Neurology, 53,
Breakdown of cross-modal function in dementia. 1284–1291.
Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Gómez-Tortosa, E., Irizarry, M. C., Gómez-Isla, T.,
Neurology, 10, 102–106. & Hyman, B. T. (2000). Clinical and neuropatho-
Frisoni, G. B., Pizzolato, G., Geroldi, C., Rossato, A., logical correlates of dementia with Lewy bodies.
Bianchetti, A., & Trabucchi, M. (1995). Dementia Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 920,
of the frontal type: Neuropsychological and 99[TC 9–15.
-HMPAO] SPECT features. Journal of Geriatric Grady, C. L., & Craik, F. I. (2000). Changes in
Psychiatry and Neurology, 8, 42–48. memory processing with age. Current Opinion in
Fuld, P. A., Katzman, R., Davies, P., & Terry, R. D. Neurobiology, 10, 224–231.
(1982). Intrusions as a sign of Alzheimer dementia: Grady, C. L., Furey, M. L., Pietrini, P., Horwitz, B.,
Chemical and pathological verification. Annals of & Rapoport, S. I. (2001). Altered brain functional
Neurology, 11, 155–159. connectivity and impaired short-term memory in
Fuld, P. A., Masur, D. M., Blau, A. D., Crystal, H., Alzheimer’s disease. Brain, 124, 739–756.
& Aronson, M. K. (1990). Object-memory evalu- Grady, C. L., Haxby, J. V., Horwitz, B., Sundaram, M.,
ation for prospective detection of dementia in Berg, G., Schapiro, M., et al. (1988). Longitudinal
normal functioning elderly: Predictive and nor- study of the early neuropsychological and cerebral
mative data. Journal of Clinical and Experimental metabolic changes in dementia of the Alzheimer
Neuropsychology, 12, 520–528. type. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Gabrieli, J. D. E., Stebbins, G. T., Singh, J., Neuropsychology, 10, 576–596.
Willingham, D. B., & Goetz, C. G. (1997). Intact Grady, C. L., Mcintosh, A. R., Horwitz, B., Maisog,
mirror-tracing and impaired rotary-pursuit skill J. M., Ungerleider, L. G., et al. (1995). Age-related
learning in patients with Huntington’s disease: reductions in human recognition memory due to
Evidence for dissociable memory systems in skill impaired encoding. Science, 269, 218–221.
learning. Neuropsychology, 11, 272–281. Graham, N. L., Emery, T., & Hodges, J. R. (2004).
Galasko, D., Katzman, R., Salmon, D. P., Thal, L. J., Distinct cognitive profi les in Alzheimer’s dis-
& Hansen, L. (1996). Clinical and neuropathologi- ease and subcortical vascular dementia. Journal
cal findings in Lewy body dementias. Brain and of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 75,
Cognition, 31, 166–175. 61–71.
Galloway, P. H., Sahgal, A., McKeith, I. G., Lloyd, Greene, J. D. W., Baddeley, A. D., & Hodges, J. R.
S., Cook, J. H., Ferrier, I. N., et al. (1992). Visual (1996). Analysis of the episodic memory deficit in
pattern recognition memory and learning deficits early Alzheimer’s disease: Evidence from the doors
in senile dementias of Alzheimer and Lewy body and people test. Neuropsychologia, 34, 537–551.
types. Dementia, 3, 101–107. Greenwood, P. M. (2000). The frontal aging hypoth-
Gnanalingham, K. K., Byrne, E. J., & Thornton, A. esis evaluated. Journal of the International
(1996). Clock-face drawing to differentiate Lewy Neuropsychological Society, 6, 705–726.
body and Alzheimer type dementia syndromes. Gregory, C. A., Orrell, M., Sahkian, B., & Hodges,
Lancet, 347, 696–697. J. (1997). Can frontotemporal dementia and
Gnanalingham, K. K., Byrne, E. J., Thornton, A., Alzheimer’s disease be differentiated using a brief
Sambrook, M. A., & Bannister, P. (1997). Motor battery of tests? International Journal of Geriatric
and cognitive function in Lewy body dementia: Psychiatry, 12, 357–383.
The Neuropsychology of Dementia 189

Grober, E., & Kawas, C. (1997). Learning and reten- and whole-brain atrophy in dementia with Lewy
tion in preclinical and early Alzheimer’s disease. bodies. Neurology, 51, 357–362.
Psychology and Aging, 12, 183–188. Haxby, J. V., Duara, R., Grady, C. L., Cutler, N. R., &
Grossman, M., Libon, D. J., Forman, M. S., Massimo, Rapoport, S. I. (1985). Relations between neurop-
L., Wood, E., Moore, P., et al. (2007). Distinct sychological and cerebral metabolic asymmetries
antemortem profi les in patients with pathologi- in early Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Cerebral
cally defined frontotemporal dementia. Archives Blood Flow and Metabolism, 5, 193–200.
of Neurology, 64(11), 1601–1609. Hedden, T., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2004). Insights into the
Guidi, M., Paciaroni, L., Paolini, S., DePadova, S., ageing mind: A view from cognitive neuroscience.
& Scarpino, O. (2006). Differences and similari- Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 5, 87–97.
ties in the neuropsychological profi le of dementia Heindel, W. C., & Salmon, D. P. (2001). Cognitive
with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer’s disease in the approaches to the memory disorders of demented
early stage. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, patients. In H. E. Adams and P. B. Sutker (Eds.),
248, 120–123. Comprehensive handbook of psychopathology,
Gunning-Dixon, F. M., & Raz, N. (2000). The cog- 2nd Edition (pp. 841–878). New York: Kluwer/
nitive correlates of white matter abnormali- Academic-Plenum.
ties in normal aging: A quantitative review. Heindel, W. C., Salmon, D. P., Shults, C. W., Walicke,
Neuropsychology, 14, 224–232. P. A., & Butters, N. (1989). Neuropsychological
Guskiewicz, K. M., Marshall, S. W., Bailes, J., evidence for multiple implicit memory systems:
McCrea, M., Cantu, R. C., Randolph, C., et al. A comparison of Alzheimer’s, Huntington’s,
(2005). Association between recurrent concus- and Parkinson’s disease patients. Journal of
sion and late-life cognitive impairment in retired Neuroscience, 9, 582–587.
professional football players. Neurosurgery, 57(4), Henry, J. D., Crawford, J. R., & Phillips, L. H.
719–726. (2004). Verbal fluency performance in demen-
Guttman, C. R., Jolesz, F. A., Kikinis, R., Killiany, tia of the Alzheimer’s type: A meta-analysis.
R. J., Moss, M. B., Sandor, T., et al. (1998). White Neuropsychologia, 42, 1212–1222.
matter changes with normal aging. Neurology, 50, Henry, J. D., Crawford, J. R., & Phillips, L. H. (2005).
972–978. A meta-analytic review of verbal fluency deficits
Hamilton, J. M., Salmon, D. P., Galasko, D., Delis, in Huntington’s disease. Neuropsychology, 19,
D. C., Hansen, L. A., et al. (2004). A comparison 243–252.
of episodic memory deficits in neuropathologi- Heyman, A., Fillenbaum, G. G., Gearing, M.,
cally-confirmed Dementia with Lewy Bodies and Mirra, S. S., Welsh-Bohmer, K. A., Peterson, B.,
Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of the International et al. (1999). Comparison of Lewy body variant of
Neuropsychological Society, 10, 689–697. Alzheimer’s disease with pure Alzheimer’s disease:
Han, S. D., & Bondi, M. W. (2008). Revision of the Consortium to establish a registry for Alzheimer’s
apolipoprotein E compensatory mechanism disease, part XIX. Neurology, 52, 1839–1844.
hypothesis. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 4, 251–254. Higuchi, M., Tashiro, M., Arai, H., Okamura, N.,
Han, S. D., Drake, A. I., Cessante, L. M., Jak, A. J., Hara, S., et al. (2000). Glucose hypometabo-
Houston, W. S., Delis, D. C., et al. (2007). APOE and lism and neuropathological correlates in brains
Recovery from TBI in a U.S. Military Population: of dementia with Lewy bodies. Experimental
Evidence for a Compensatory Mechanism? Journal Neurology, 162, 247–256.
of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 78, Hodges, J. R., & Graham, N. L. (2001). Vascular
1103–1108. dementias. In J. R. Hodges (Ed.), Early onset
Hanes, K. R., Andrewes, D. G., & Pantelis, C. (1995). dementia: A multidisciplinary approach (pp. 319–
Cognitive flexibility and complex integration 337). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
in Parkinson’s disease, Huntington’s disease, Hodges, J. R., & Patterson, K. (1995). Is semantic
and schizophrenia. Journal of the International memory consistently impaired early in the course
Neuropsychological Society, 1, 545–553. of Alzheimer’s disease? Neuroanatomical and diag-
Hansen, L. A., & Galasko, D. (1992). Lewy body dis- nostic implications. Neuropsychologia, 33, 441–459.
ease. Current Opinion in Neurology, 5, 889–894. Hodges, J. R., Salmon, D. P., & Butters, N. (1991). The
Hansen, L., Salmon, D., Galasko, D., Masliah, E., nature of the naming deficit in Alzheimer’s and
Katzman, R., et al. (1990). The Lewy body variant Huntington’s disease. Brain, 114, 1547–1558.
of Alzheimer’s disease: A clinical and pathologic Hodges, J. R., Salmon, D. P., & Butters, N. (1992).
entity. Neurology, 40, 1–8. Semantic memory impairment in Alzheimer’s
Hashimoto, M., Kitagaki, H., Imamura, T., Hirono, disease: Failure of access or degraded knowledge?
N., Shimomura, T., et al. (1998). Medial temporal Neuropsychologia, 30, 301–314.
190 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Hodges, J. R., Salmon, D. P., & Butters, N. (1993). Jacobs, D., Salmon, D. P., Tröster, A. I., & Butters,
Recognition and naming of famous faces in N. (1990). Intrusion errors in the figural memory
Alzheimer’s disease: A cognitive analysis. of patients with Alzheimer’s and Huntington’s
Neuropsychologia, 31, 775–788. disease. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 5,
Hof, P. R., & Morrison, J. H. (1999). The cellular basis 49–57.
of cortical disconnection in Alzheimer’s disease Jacobs, D. M., Sano, M., Dooneief, G., Marder, K.,
and related dementing conditions. In R. D. Terry, Bell, K. L., & Stern, Y. (1995). Neuropsychological
R. Katzman, K. L. Bick, & S. S. Sisodia (Eds.), detection and characterization of preclinical
Alzheimer disease (pp. 207–232). New York: Raven. Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 45, 957–962.
Hogan, M. J., Swanwick, G. R. J., Kaiser, J., Rowan, M., Jacobson, M. W., Delis, D. C., Lansing, A., Houston,
& Lawlor, S. (2003). Memory-related EEG power W. S., Olsen, R., Wetter, S., et al. (2005a).
and coherence reductions in mild Alzheimer’s dis- Asymmetries in global and local processing in
ease. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 49, elderly with the APOE e4 allele. Neuropsychology,
147–163. 19, 822–829.
Houston, W. S., Delis, D. C., Lansing, A., Cobell, Jacobson, M. W., Delis, D. C., Bondi, M. W., &
C., Jacobson, M. W., Salmon, D. P., et al. (2005). Salmon, D. P. (2005b). Asymmetry in auditory and
Executive function asymmetry in older adults spatial attention span in normal elderly genetically
genetically at risk for Alzheimer’s disease: Verbal at risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Clinical
versus design fluency. Journal of the International and Experimental Neuropsychology, 27, 240–253.
Neuropsychological Society, 11, 863–870. Jacobson, M. W., Delis, D. C., Bondi, M. W., &
Howieson, D. B., Dame, A., Camicioli, R., Sexton, G., Salmon, D. P. (2002). Do neuropsychological
Payami, H., & Kaye, J. A. (1997). Cognitive mark- tests detect preclinical Alzheimer’s disease?:
ers preceding Alzheimer’s dementia in the healthy Individual-test versus cognitive discrepancy anal-
oldest old. Journal of the American Geriatric yses. Neuropsychology, 16, 132–139.
Society, 45, 84–89. Jelic, V., Shigeta, M., Julin, P., Almkvist, O., Winblad,
Hubacek, J. A., Pitha, J., Skodova, Z., Adamkova, V., B., & Wahlund, L. O. (1996). Quantitative elec-
Lanska, V., & Poledne, R. (2001). A possible role troencephalography power and coherence in
of apolipoprotein E polymorphism in predisposi- Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impair-
tion to higher education. Neuropsychobiology, 43, ment. Dementia, 7, 314–323.
200–203. Jellinger, K. A. (2004). Head injury and dementia.
Huff, F. J., Becker, J. T., Belle, S. H., Nebes, R. D., Current Opinion in Neurology, 17, 719–723.
Holland, A. L., & Boller, F. (1987). Cognitive defi- Jernigan, T. J., Archibald, S. L., Fennema-Notestine,
cits and clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease. C., Gamst, A. C., Stout, J. C., Bonner, J., et al.
Neurology, 37, 1119–1124. (2001). Effects of age on tissues and regions of the
Huff, F. J., Corkin, S., & Growdon, J. H. (1986). cerebrum and cerebellum. Neurobiology of Aging,
Semantic impairment and anomia in Alzheimer’s 22, 581–594.
disease. Brain and Language, 28, 235–249. Johanson, A., & Hagberg, B. (1989). Psychometric
Hultsch, D. F., Hertzog, C., Small, B. J., & Dixon, R. characteristics in patients with frontal lobe degen-
A. (1999). Use it or lose it: Engage lifestyle as a buf- eration of non-Alzheimer’s type. Archives of
fer of cognitive decline in aging. Psychology and Gerontology and Geriatrics, 8, 129–137.
Aging, 14(2), 245–263. Johnson, D. K., Morris, J. C., & Galvin, J. E. (2005).
Hyman, B. T., Damasio, A. R., Van Hoesen, G. W., Verbal and visuospatial deficits in dementia with
& Barnes, C. L. (1984). Alzheimer’s disease: Cell Lewy bodies. Neurology, 65, 1232–1238.
specific pathology isolates the hippocampal for- Jordan, B. D., Relkin, N. R., Ravdin, L. D., Jacobs, A.
mation. Science, 225, 1168–1170. R., Bennett, A., & Gandy, S. (1997). Apolipoprotein
Ince, P. G., Perry, E. K., & Morris, C. M. (1998). E epsilon4 associated with chronic traumatic brain
Dementia with Lewy bodies: A distinct non-Al- injury in boxing. Journal of the American Medical
zheimer dementia syndrome? Brain Pathology, 8, Association, 278, 136–140.
299–324. Jorm, A. F., Korten, A. E., & Henderson, A. S.
Ineichen, B. (1987). Measuring the rising tide. How (1987). The prevalence of dementia: A quantita-
many dementia cases will there be by 2001? British tive integration of the literature. Acta Psychiatrica
Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 193–200. Scandinavica, 76, 465–479.
Jack, C. R., Petersen, R. C., Xu, Y. C., Waring, S. Josiassen, R. C., Curry, L. M., & Mancall, E. L. (1983).
C., O’Brien, P. C., et al. (1998). Medial temporal Development of neuropsychological deficits in
atrophy on MRI in normal aging and very mild Huntington’s disease. Archives of Neurology, 40,
Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 49, 786–794. 791–796.
The Neuropsychology of Dementia 191

Kaszniak, A. W., & Newman, M. C. (2000). Toward Knopman, D. S., Mastri, A. R., Frey, W., Sung, J. H.,
a neuropsychology of cognitive aging. In S. H. & Rustan, T. (1990). Dementia lacking distinctive
Qualls and N. Abeles (Eds.), Psychology and the histologic features: A common non-Alzheimer
aging revolution: How we adapt to longer life (pp. degenerative dementia. Neurology, 40, 251–256.
43–67). Washington, DC: American Psychological Knopman, D. S., & Ryberg, S. (1989). A verbal mem-
Association. ory test with high predictive accuracy for demen-
Kaszniak, A. W., Wilson, R. S., Fox, J. H., & Stebbins, tia of the Alzheimer type. Archives of Neurology,
G. T. (1986). Cognitive assessment in Alzheimer’s 46, 141–145.
disease: Cross-sectional and longitudinal perspec- Knott, V., Mohr, E., Mahoney, C., & Ilivitsky, V.
tives. Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences, (2000). Electroencephalographic coherence in
13, 420–423. Alzheimer’s disease: Comparisons with a control
Katzman, R. (1994). Apolipoprotein E and Alzheimer’s group and population norms. Journal of Geriatric
disease. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 4, Psychiatry and Neurology, 13, 1–8.
703–707. Koenig, P., Smith, E. E., Moore, P., Glosser, G., &
Katzman, R. (1993). Education and the prevalence of Grossman, M. (2007). Categorization of novel
dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 43, animals by patients with Alzheimer’s disease and
13–20. corticobasal degeneration. Neuropsychology, 21,
Kawas, C. H., & Katzman, R. (1999). Epidemiology 193–206.
of dementia and Alzheimer disease. In R. D. Terry, Kopelman, M. D. (1989). Remote and autobiographi-
R. Katzman, K. L. Bick, and S. S. Sisodia (Eds.), cal memory, temporal context memory and fron-
Alzheimer disease (2nd ed., pp. 95–116). New York: tal atrophy in Korsakoff and Alzheimer patients.
Raven Press. Neuropsychologia, 27, 437–460.
Kawas, C. H., Corrada, M. M., Brookmeyer, R., Kosaka, K., Yoshimura, M., Ikeda, K., & Budka,
Morrison, A., Resnick, S. M., Zonderman, A. B., H. (1984). Diff use type of Lewy body disease:
et al. (2003). Visual memory predicts Alzheimer’s Progressive dementia with abundant cortical Lewy
disease more than a decade before diagnosis. bodies and senile changes of varying degree: A
Neurology, 60, 1089–1093. new disease? Clinical Neuropathology, 3, 185–192.
Keltikangas-Jarvinen, L., Raikkonen, K., & Kramer ,M. (1986). Trends of institutionalization and
Lehtimaki, T. (1993). Dependence between apoli- prevalence of mental disorders in nursing homes.
poprotein E phenotypes and temperament in chil- In M. S. Harper and B. D. Lebowitz (Eds.), Mental
dren, adolescents, and young adults. Psychosomatic illness in nursing homes: Agenda for research (pp.
Medicine, 55, 155–163. 7–26). Rockville, MD: National Institute of Mental
Kertesz, A., Davidson, W., & Munoz, D. G. (1999). Health (DHHS Publication No. (ADM) 86–1459).
Clinical and pathological overlap between fronto- Kramer, A. F., & Erickson, K. I. (2007). Capitalizing
temporal dementia, primary progressive aphasia on cortical plasticity: Influence of physical activ-
and corticobasal degeneration: The Pick com- ity on cognition and brain function. Trends in
plex. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(8), 342–348.
10(suppl), 46–49. Kramer, A. F., Bherer, L., Colcombe, S. J., Dong, W., &
Kertesz, A., Hudson, L., Mackenzie, I. R. A., & Greenough, W. T. (2004). Environmental influences
Munoz, D. G. (1994). The pathology and nosol- on cognitive and brain plasticity during aging.
ogy of primary progressive aphasia. Neurology, 44, The Journals of Gerontology. Series A, Biological
2065–2072. Sciences and Medical Sciences, 59, 940–957.
Kertesz, A., Nadkarni, N., Davidson, W., & Thomas, Kraybill, M. L., Larson, E. B., Tsuang, D. W., Teri, L.,
A. W. (2000). The Frontal Behavioral Inventory in McCormick, W. C., et al. (2005). Cognitive differ-
the differential diagnosis of frontotemporal demen- ences in dementia patients with autopsy-verified
tia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological AD, Lewy body pathology, or both. Neurology, 64,
Society, 6, 460–468. 2069–2073.
Kertesz, A., & Clydesdale, S. (1994). Kurylo, D. D., Corkin, S., Rizzo, J. F., & Growdon,
Neuropsychological deficits in vascular dementia J. H. (1996). Greater relative impairment of object
vs. Alzheimer’s disease: Frontal lobe deficits prom- recognition than of visuospatial abilities in
inent in vascular dementia. Archives of Neurology, Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology, 10, 74–81.
51, 1226–1231. Lafosse, J. M., Reed, B. R., Mungas, D., Sterling, S.
Knopman, D. S., DeKosky, S. T., Cummings, J. L., B., Wahbeh, H., & Jagust, W. J. (1997). Fluency
Chui, H., Corey-Bloom, J., Relkin, N., et al. (2001). and memory differences between ischemic
Practice parameter: Diagnosis of dementia (an vascular dementia and Alzheimer’s disease.
evidence-based review). Neurology, 56, 1143–1153. Neuropsychology, 11, 514–522.
192 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Lakmache, Y., Lassonde, M., Gauthier, S., Frigon, the International Neuropsychological Society, 11,
J-Y., & Lepore, F. (1998). Interhemispheric dis- 30–39.
connection syndrome in Alzheimer’s disease. Linn, R. T., Wolf, P. A., Bachman, D. L., Knoefel,
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, J. E., Cobb, J. L., et al. (1995). The “preclinical
95, 9042–9046. phase” of probable Alzheimer’s disease. Archives
Lamar, M., Podell, K., Carew, T. G., Cloud, B. S., of Neurology, 52, 485–490.
Resh, R., et al. (1997). Perseverative behavior in Lippa, C. F., Johnson, R., & Smith, T. W. (1998). The
Alzheimer’s disease and subcortical ischemic vas- medial temporal lobe in dementia with Lewy bod-
cular dementia. Neuropsychology, 11, 523–534. ies: A comparative study with Alzheimer’s disease.
Lambon Ralph, M. A., Powell, J., Howard, D., Annals of Neurology, 43, 102–106.
Whitworth, A. B., Garrard, P., & Hodges, J. R. Lipton, A. M., Ohman, K. A., Womack, K. B., Hynan,
(2001). Semantic memory is impaired in both L. S., Ninman, E. T., & Lacritz, L. H. (2005).
dementia with Lewy bodies and dementia of Subscores of the FAB differentiate frontotempo-
Alzheimer’s type: A comparative neuropsy- ral lobar degeneration from AD. Neurology, 65,
chological study and literature review. Journal 726–731.
of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 70, Locascio, J. J., Growdon, J. H., & Corkin, S. (1995).
149–156. Cognitive test performance in detecting, stag-
Lange, K. L., Bondi, M. W., Salmon, D. P., Galasko, D., ing, and tracking Alzheimer’s disease. Archives of
Delis, D. C., Thomas, R. G., et al. (2002). Decline in Neurology, 52, 1087–1099.
verbal memory during preclinical Alzheimer’s dis- Mathuranath, P. S., Nestor, P. J., Berrios, G. E.,
ease: Examination of the effect of APOE genotype. Rakowicz, W., & Hodges, J. R. (2000). A brief cog-
Journal of the International Neuropsychological nitive test battery to differentiate Alzheimer’s dis-
Society, 8, 943–955. ease and frontotemporal dementia. Neurology, 55,
Lange, K. W., Sahakian, B. J., Quinn, N. P., Marsden, 1613–1620.
C. D., & Robbins, T. W. (1995). Comparison of Martin, A., Brouwers, P., Cox, C., & Fedio, P. (1985).
executive and visuospatial memory function in On the nature of the verbal memory deficit in
Huntington’s disease and dementia of Alzheimer Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Language, 25,
type matched for degree of dementia. Journal 323–341.
of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 58, Martin, A., & Fedio, P. (1983). Word production and
598–606. comprehension in Alzheimer’s disease: The break-
Lawrence, A. D., Sahakian, B. J., Hodges, J. R., down of semantic knowledge. Brain and Language,
Rosser, A. E., Lange, K. W., & Robbins, T. W. 19, 124–141.
(1996). Executive and mnemonic functions in Masliah, E., Mallory, M., Hansen, L. A., DeTeresa,
early Huntington’s disease. Brain, 119, 1633–1645. R., & Terry, R. D. (1993). Quantitative synaptic
Lawrence, A. D., Watkins, L. H., Sahakian, B. J., alterations in the human neocortex during normal
Hodges, J. R., & Robbins, T. W. (2000). Visual aging. Neurology, 43, 192–197.
object and visuospatial cognition in Huntington’s Massman, P. J., Delis, D. C., & Butters, N. (1993). Does
disease: Implications for information processing impaired primacy recall equal impaired long-term
in corticostriatal circuits. Brain, 123, 1349–1364. storage?: Serial position effects in Huntington’s
Lefleche, G., & Albert, M. S. (1995). Executive disease and Alzheimer’s disease. Developmental
function deficits in mild Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsycholy, 9, 1–15.
Neuropsychology, 9, 313–320. Mayeux, R., Ottman, R., Maestre, G., Ngai, C., Tang,
Libon, D. J., Xie, S. X., Moore, P., Farmer, J., Antani, M-X., Ginsberg, H., et al. (1995). Synergistic effects
S., et al. (2007). Patterns of neuropsychologi- of traumatic head injury and apolipoprotein ε4 in
cal impairment in frontotemporal dementia. patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 45,
Neurology, 68, 369–375. 555–557.
Lindau, M., Almkvist, O., Johansson, S. E., & McHugh, P. R., & Folstein, M. F. (1975). Psychiatric
Wahlund, L. O. (1998). Cognitive and behavioral symptoms of Huntington’s chorea: A clinical and
differentiation of frontal lobe degeneration of the phenomenologic study. In D. F. Benson, and D.
non-Alzheimer’s type and Alzheimer’s Disease. Blumer (Eds.), Psychiatric aspects of neurological
Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 9, disease (pp. 267–285). New York: Raven Press.
205–213. McKeith, I. G., Dickson, D. W., Lowe, J., Emre, M.,
Lineweaver, T. T., Salmon, D. P., Bondi, M. W., O’Brien, J. T, et al. (2005). Diagnosis and man-
& Corey-Bloom, J. (2005). Distinct effects of agement of dementia with Lewy bodies: Third
Alzheimer’s disease and Huntington’s disease report of the DLB consortium. Neurology, 65,
on performance of mental rotation. Journal of 1863–1872.
The Neuropsychology of Dementia 193

McKeith, I. G., Galasko, D., Kosaka, K., Perry, E., Mohr, E., Litvan, I., Williams, J., Fedio, P., & Chase,
Dickson, D., et al. (1996). Clinical and pathologi- T. N. (1990). Selective deficits in Alzheimer and
cal diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB): Parkinson dementia: Visuospatial function.
Report of the Consortium on Dementia with Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences, 17,
Lewy Bodies (CDLB) International Workgroup. 292–297.
Neurology, 47, 1113–1124. Mondadori, C. R. A., de Quervain, D. J-F., Buchmann,
McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., Katzman, A., Mustovic, H., Wollmer, M. A., Schmidt, C. F.,
R., Price, D., & Stadlan, E.M. (1984). Clinical et al. (2007). Better memory and neural efficiency
diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: Report of the in young apolipoprotein E epsilon4 carriers.
NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices Cerebral Cortex, 17, 1934–1947.
of Department of Health and Human Services Monsch, A. U., Bondi, M. W., Butters, N. Paulsen,
Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology, 34, J.S., Salmon, D.P., Brugger, P., et al. (1994). A
939–944. comparison of category and letter fluency in
Mendez, M. F., Cherrier, M., Perryman, K., Alzheimer’s disease and Huntington’s disease.
Pachana, N., Miller, B., & Cummings, J. L. (1996). Neuropsychology, 8, 25–30.
Frontotemporal dementias versus Alzheimer’s Montoya, A., Pelletier, M., Menear, M., Duplessis, E.,
disease: Differential cognitive features. Neurology, Richer, F., & Lepage, M. (2006). Episodic memory
47, 1189–1194. impairment in Huntington’s disease: A meta-
Mendez, M. F., Perryman, K. M., Miller, B. L., & analysis. Neuropsychologia, 44, 1984–1994.
Cummings, J. L. (1998). Behavioral differences Mori, E., Shimomura, T., Fujimori, M., Hirono, N.,
between frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s Imamura, T., et al. (2000). Visuoperceptual impair-
disease: A comparison on the BEHAVE-AD rating ment in dementia with Lewy bodies. Archives of
scale. International Psychogeriatrics 10, 155–162. Neurology, 57, 489–493.
Mendez, M. F., Selwood, A., Mastri, A. R., & Frey, Morris, J. C., McKeel, D. W., Storandt, M., Rubin, E.
W. H. (1993). Pick’s disease versus Alzheimer’s H., Price, J. L., et al. (1991). Very mild Alzheimer’s
disease: A comparison of clinical characteristics. disease: Informant-based clinical, psychometric,
Neurology, 43, 289–292. and pathologic distinction from normal aging.
Mesulam, M. M. (1982). Slowly progressive apha- Neurology, 41, 469–478.
sia without generalized dementia. Annals of Mortimer, J. A. (1988). Do psychosocial risk fac-
Neurology, 30, 69–72. tors contribute to Alzheimer’s disease? In A. S.
Merdes, A. R., Hansen, L. A., Jeste, D. V., Galasko, Henderson and J. H. Henderson (Eds.), Etiology of
D., Hofstetter, C. R., et al. (2003). Influence of dementia of the Alzheimer’s type (pp. 39–52). New
Alzheimer pathology on clinical diagnostic accu- York: John Wiley and Sons.
racy in dementia with Lewy bodies. Neurology, 60, Mortimer, J. A., van Duijn, C. M., Chandra, V.,
1586–1590. Fratglioni, L., Graves, A. B., Heyman, A., et al.
Mickes, L., Wixted, J. T., Fennema-Notestine, (1991). Head trauma as a risk factor for Alzheimer’s
C., Galasko, D., Bondi, M. W., Thal, L. J., et al. disease: A collaborative re-analysis of case-control
(2007). Progressive impairment on neuropsycho- studies. International Journal of Epidemiology,
logical tasks in a longitudinal study of preclini- 20(suppl.2), S28–S35.
cal Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology, 21(6), Moss, M. B., Albert, M. S., Butters, N., & Payne, M.
696–705. (1986). Differential patterns of memory loss among
Miech, R. A., Breitner, J. C., Zandi, P. P., Khachaturian, patients with Alzheimer’s disease, Huntington’s
A. S., Anthony, J. C., & Mayer, L. (2002). Incidence disease and alcoholic Korsakoff ’s syndrome.
of AD may decline in the early 90s for men, later Archives of Neurology, 43, 239–246.
for women: The Cache County study Neurology, Nadel, L., & Moscovitch, M. (1997). Memory con-
58, 209–218. solidation, retrograde amnesia and the hippocam-
Miller, B. L., Ikonte, C., Ponton, M., Levy, M., Boone, pal complex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 7,
K., Darby, A., et al. (1997). A study of the Lund- 217–227.
Manchester research criteria for frontotemporal Neary, D. (1999). Overview of frontotempo-
dementia: Clinical and single-photon emission ral dementias and the consensus applied.
CT correlations. Neurology, 48, 937–942. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders,
Minoshima, S., Foster, N. L., Sima, A., Frey, K. A., 10(suppl), 6–9.
Albin, R. L., & Kuhl, D. E. (2001). Alzheimer’s dis- Neary, D., Snowden, J. S., Gustafson, L. Passant, U.,
ease versus dementia with Lewy bodies: Cerebral Stuss, D., Black S., et al. (1998). Frontotemporal
metabolic distinction with autopsy confirmation. lobar degeneration: A consensus on clinical diag-
Annals of Neurology, 50, 358–365. nostic criteria. Neurology, 51, 1546–1554.
194 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Nebes, R. (1989). Semantic memory in Alzheimer’s Peinemann, A., Schuller, S., Pohl, C., Jahn, T.,
disease. Psychological Bulletin 106, 377–394. Weindl, A., & Kassubek, J. (2005). Executive dys-
Nicholas, M., Obler, L., Albert, M., & Helm- function in early stages of Huntington’s disease
Estabrooks, N. (1985). Empty speech in Alzheimer’s is associated with striatal and insular atrophy: A
disease and fluent aphasia. Journal of Speech and neuropsychological and voxel-based morphomet-
Hearing Research, 28, 405–410. ric study. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 239,
Noe, E., Marder, K., Bell, K. L., Jacobs, D. M., Manly, 11–19.
J. J., & Stern, Y. (2003). Comparison of demen- Perry, R. J., & Hodges, J. R. (1999). Attention and
tia with Lewy bodies to Alzheimer’s disease and executive deficits in Alzheimer’s disease: A critical
Parkinson’s disease with dementia. Movement review. Brain 122, 383–404.
Disords, 19, 60–67. Perry, R. H., Irving, D., Blessed, G., Fairbairn, A., &
Norton, L. E., Bondi, M. W., Salmon, D. P., & Perry, E. K. (1990). Senile dementia of the Lewy
Goodglass, H. (1997). Deterioration of generic body type: A clinically and neuropathologi-
knowledge in patients with Alzheimer’s dis- cally distinct form of Lewy body dementia in the
ease: Evidence from the Number Information elderly. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 95,
Test. Journal of Clinical and Experimental 119–139.
Neuropsychology, 19, 857–866. Petersen, R. C., Doody, R., Kurz, A., Mohs, R. C.,
O’Sullivan, M., Jones, D. K., Summers, P. E., Morris, Morris, J. C., et al. (2001). Current concepts in
R. G., Williams, S. C., & Markus, H. S. (2001). mild cognitive impairment. Archives of Neurology,
Evidence for cortical “disconnection” as a mecha- 58, 1985–1992.
nism of age-related cognitive decline. Neurology, Petersen, R. C., Smith, G. E., Ivnik, R. J., Tangalos,
57, 632–638. E. G., Schaid, D. J., et al. (1995). Apolipoprotein
Pachana, N. A., Boone, K. B., Miller, B. L., Cummings, E status as a predictor of the development of
J. L., & Berman, N. (1996). Comparison of neu- Alzheimer’s disease in memory-impaired individ-
ropsychological functioning in Alzheimer’s dis- uals. Journal of the American Medical Association,
ease and frontotemporal dementia. Journal of 273, 1274–1278.
the International Neuropsychological Society, 2, Pfefferbaum, A., Adalsteinsson, E., & Sullivan,
505–510. E. (2005). Frontal circuitry degradation marks
Pandovani, A., Di Piero, V., Bragoni, M., Iacoboni, healthy adult aging: Evidence from diff usion ten-
M., Gualdi, G. G., & Lenzi, G. L. (1995). Patterns of sor imaging. Neuroimage, 26, 891–899.
neuropsychological impairment in mild demen- Pfefferbaum, A., Mathalon, D. H., Sullivan, E. V.,
tia: A comparison between Alzheimer’s disease Rawles, J. M., Zipursky, R. B., & Lim, K. O. (1994). A
and multi-infarct dementia. Acta Neurologica quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study of
Scandinavica, 92, 433–442. changes in brain morphology from infancy to late
Parasuraman, R., & Haxby, J. V. (1993). Attention adulthood. Archives of Neurology, 51, 874–887.
and brain function in Alzheimer’s disease. Pick, A. (1892). Uber die Beziehungen der senilen
Neuropsychology, 7, 242–772. Hirnatrophie zur Aphasie. Prag. Med. Wschr. 17,
Park, H. L., O’Connell, J. E., & Thomson, R. G. (2003). 165–167.
A systematic review of cognitive decline in the Pillon, B., Dubois, B., Ploska, A., & Agid, Y. (1991).
general elderly population. International Journal Severity and specificity of cognitive impairment
of Geriatric Psychiatry, 18, 1121–1134. in Alzheimer’s, Huntington’s, and Parkinson’s
Pasquier, F., Lebert, F., Grymonprez, L., & Petit, H. diseases and progressive supranuclear palsy.
(1995). Verbal fluency in dementia of frontal lobe Neurology, 41, 634–643.
type and dementia of Alzheimer type. Journal Powell, M. R., Smith, G. E., Knopman, D. S., Parisi,
of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 58, J. E., Boeve, B. F., Petersen, R. C., et al. (2006).
81–84. Cognitive measures predict pathologic Alzheimer
Paulsen, J. S., Salmon, D. P., Monsch, A. U., disease. Archives of Neurology, 63, 865–868.
Butters, N., Swenson, M., & Bondi, M. W. (1995). Price, C. C., Jefferson, A. L., Merino, J. G., Heilman,
Discrimination of cortical from subcortical K. M., & Libon, D. J. (2005). Subcortical vascular
dementias on the basis of memory and problem- dementia: Integrating neuropsychological and
solving tests. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51, neuroradiologic data. Neurology, 65, 376–382.
48–58. Rascovsky, K., Salmon, D. P., Ho, G. J., Galasko, D.,
Paxton, J. L., Peavy, G. M., Jenkins, C., Rice, V. Peavy, G. M., Hansen, L. A., et al. (2002). Cognitive
A., Heindel, W. C., & Salmon, D. P. (2007). profi les differ in autopsy-confirmed fronto-tempo-
Deterioration of visual–perceptual ability in ral dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology,
Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex 43, 967–975. 58, 1801–1808.
The Neuropsychology of Dementia 195

Rascovsky, K., Salmon, D. P., Hansen, L. A., Thal, L. Sahgal, A., Galloway, P. H., McKeith, I. G.,
J., & Galasko, D. (2007). Disparate phonemic and Edwardson, J. A., & Lloyd, S. (1992b). A compara-
semantic fluency deficits in autopsy-confirmed tive study of attentional deficits in senile demen-
frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. tias of Alzheimer and Lewy body types. Dementia,
Neuropsychology, 21, 20–30. 3, 350–354.
Raz, N. (2005). The aging brain observed in vivo: Sahgal, A., Galloway, P. H., McKeith, I. G., Lloyd, S.,
Differential changes and their modifiers. In R. Cook, J. H., Ferrier, N., et al. (1992a). Matching-to-
Cabeza, L. Nyberg, and D. Park (Eds.), Cognitive sample deficits in patients with senile dementias of
neuroscience of aging: Linking cognitive and the Alzheimer and Lewy body types. Archives of
cerebral aging (pp. 19–57). New York: Oxford Neurology, 49, 1043–1046.
University Press. Sahgal, A., McKeith, I. G., Galloway, P. H., Tasker,
Reed, B. R., Mungas, D. M., Kramer, J. H., Ellis, W., N., & Steckler, T. (1995). Do differences in visu-
Vinters, H. V., Zarow, C., et al. (2007). Profi les ospatial ability between senile dementias of the
of neuropsychological impairment in autopsy- Alzheimer and Lewy body types reflect differences
defined Alzheimer’s disease and cerebrovascular solely in mnemonic function? Journal of Clinical
disease. Brain, 130(Pt3), 731–739. and Experimental Neuropsychology, 17, 35–43.
Rohrer, D., Salmon, D. P., Wixted, J. T., & Paulsen, J. Salat, D. H., Kaye, J. A., & Janowsky, J. S. (1999).
S. (1999). The disparate effects of Alzheimer’s dis- Prefrontal gray and white matter volumes in
ease and Huntington’s disease on the recall from healthy aging and Alzheimer disease. Archives of
semantic memory. Neuropsychology, 13, 381–388. Neurology, 56, 338–344.
Rohrer, D., Wixted, J. T., Salmon, D. P., & Butters, Salmon, D. P. (2000). Disorders of memory in
N. (1995). Retrieval from semantic memory and Alzheimer’s disease. In L. S. Cermak (Ed.),
its implications for Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Handbook of neuropsychology, 2nd edition (Vol.
Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and 2): Memory and its disorders (pp. 155–195).
Cognition, 21, 1–13. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Roman, G. C., Tatemichi, T. K., Erkinjuntti, T., Salmon, D. P., & Bondi, M. W. (1999). Neuropsychology
Cummings, J.L., Masdeu, J.C., Garcia, J.H., et al. of Alzheimer’s disease. In R. D. Terry, R. Katzman,
(1993). Vascular dementia: Diagnostic criteria for K. L. Bick, and S. S. Sisodia (Eds.), Alzheimer dis-
research studies. Report of the NINDS-AIREN ease (pp. 39–56). Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams
International Workshop. Neurology, 43, 250–260. and Wilkens.
Rosen, V. M., Sunderland, T., Levy, J., Harwell, Salmon, D. P., Butters, N., & Chan, A. S. (1999). The
A., McGee, L., Hammond, C., et al. (2005). deterioration of semantic memory in Alzheimer’s
Apolipoprotein E and category fluency: Evidence disease. Canadian Journal of Experimental
for reduced semantic access in healthy normal Psychology, 53, 108–117.
controls at risk for developing Alzheimer’s dis- Salmon, D. P., Galasko, D., Hansen, L. A.,
ease. Neuropsychologia, 43, 647–658. Masliah, E., Butters, N., Thal, L. J., et al. (1996).
Rouleau, I., Salmon, D. P., Butters, N., Kennedy, C., Neuropsychological deficits associated with dif-
& McGuire, K. (1992). Quantitative and qualita- fuse Lewy body disease. Brain and Cognition, 31,
tive analyses of clock drawings in Alzheimer’s and 148–165.
Huntington’s disease. Brain and Cognition, 18, Salmon, D. P., Heindel, W. C., & Lange, K. L. (1999).
70–87. Differential decline in word generation from pho-
Rubin, E. H., Storandt, M., Miller, J. P., Kinscherf, D. nemic and semantic categories during the course of
A., Grant, E. A., Morris, J. C., et al. (1998). A pro- Alzheimer’s disease: Implications for the integrity
spective study of cognitive function and onset of of semantic memory. Journal of the International
dementia in cognitively healthy elders. Archives of Neuropsychological Society, 5, 692–703.
Neurology, 55, 395–401. Salmon, D. P., Thomas, R. G., Pay, M. M., Booth,
Sadek, J. R., Johnson, S. A., White, D. A., Salmon, A., Hofstetter, C. R., Thal, L. J., et al. (2002).
D. P., Taylor, K. I., Delapena, J. H., et al. (2004). Alzheimer’s disease can be accurately diagnosed
Retrograde amnesia in dementia: Comparison of in very mildly impaired individuals. Neurology,
HIV-associated dementia, Alzheimer’s disease, 59, 1022–1028.
and Huntington’s disease. Neuropsychology, 18, Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory
692–699. of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological
Sagar, H. J., Cohen, N. J., Sullivan, E. V., Corkin, S., & Review, 103, 403–428.
Growdon, J. H. (1988). Remote memory function Saunders, A. M., Strittmatter, W. J., Schmechel, D.,
in Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s disease. St. George-Hyslop, P. H., Perick-Vance, M. A., Joo,
Brain, 111, 525–539. S. H., et al. (1993). Association of apolipoprotein
196 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

E allele e4 with late-onset familial and sporadic Starkstein, S. E., Migliorelli, R., Teson, A., Sabe, L.,
Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 43, 1467–1472. Vázquez, S., Turjanski, M., et al. (1994). Specificity
Schacter, D. L. (1987). Implicit memory: History and of changes in cerebral blood in patients with frontal
current status. Journal of Experimental Psychology. lobe dementia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery
Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 3, 501–517. and Psychiatry, 57, 790–796.
Schacter, D. L., Savage, C. R., Alpert, N. M., Rauch, S. Stavitsky, K., Brickman, A. M., Scarmeas, N.,
L., & Albert, M. S. (1996). The role of hippocampus Torgan, R. L., Tang, M-X., et al. (2006). The pro-
and frontal cortex in age-related memory changes: gression of cognition, psychiatric symptoms, and
A PET study. NeuroReport, 7, 1165–1169. functional abilities in dementia with Lewy bodies
Schaie, K. W., Caskie, G. I. L., Revell, A. J., Willis, S. and Alzheimer’s disease. Archives of Neurology, 63,
L., Kaszniak, A. W., & Teri, L. (2005). Extending 1450–1456.
neuropsychological assessments into the primary Stern, Y., Alexander, G. E., Prohovnik, I., & Mayeux,
mental ability space. Aging, Neuropsychology, and R. (1992). Inverse relationship between educa-
Cognition, 12, 245–277. tion and parietotemporal perfusion deficit in
Serretti, A., Olgiati, P., & De Ronchi, D. (2007). Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 32, 371–375.
Genetics of Alzheimer’s disease: A rapidly evolving Stern, Y., Gurland, B., Tatemichi, T. K., Tang, M.
field. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 12, 73–92. X., Wilder, D., & Mayeux, R. (1994). Influence
Shimomura, T., Mori, E., Yamashita, H., Imamura, of education and occupation on the incidence
T., Hirono, N., et al. (1998). Cognitive loss in of Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of the American
dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer dis- Medical Association, 271, 1004–1010.
ease. Archives of Neurology, 55, 1547–1552. Stevens, A., Kircher, T., Nickola, M., Bartels, M.,
Ska, B., Poissant, A., & Joanette, Y. (1990). Line ori- Rosellen, N., & Wormstall, H. (2001). Dynamic
entation judgement in normal elderly and sub- regulation of EEG power and coherence is lost
jects with dementia of Alzheimer’s type. Journal early and globally in probable DAT. European
of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 12, Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience,
695–702. 251, 199–204.
Small, B. J., Fratiglioni, L., Viitanen, M., Winblad, Storandt, M., Grant, E.A, Miller, J. P., & Morris, J. C.
B., & Bäckman, L. (2000). The course of cogni- (2006). Longitudinal course and neuropathologic
tive impairment in preclinical Alzheimer disease: outcomes in original vs revised MCI and in pre-
Three- and 6-year follow-up of a population-based MCI. Neurology, 67(3), 467–473.
sample. Archives of Neurology, 57, 839–844. Storandt, M., Botwinick, J., Danziger, W. L., Berg,
Small, B. J., Mobly, J. L., Laukka, E. J., Jones, S., & L., & Hughes, C. P. (1984). Psychometric differen-
Backman, L. (2003). Cognitive deficits in pre- tiation of mild senile dementia of the Alzheimer
clinical Alzheimer’s disease. Acta Neurologica type. Archives of Neurology, 41, 497–499.
Scandinavica Supplementum, 179, 29–33. Storandt, M., Grant, E. A., Miller, J. P., & Morris,
Smith, G. E., Pankratz, V. S., Negash, S., Machulda, J. C. (2002). Rates of progression in mild cogni-
M. M., Petersen, R. C., Boeve, B. F., et al. (2007). tive impairment and early Alzheimer’s disease.
A plateau in pre-Alzheimer memory decline: Neurology, 59, 1034–1041.
Evidence for compensatory mechanisms? Stout, J. C., Rodawalt, W. C., & Siemers, E. R. (2001).
Neurology, 69, 133–139. Risky decision making in Huntington’s disease.
Smyer, M. A. (1988). The nursing home community. Journal of the International Neuropsychological
In M. A. Smyer, M. D. Cohn, and D. Brannon Society, 7, 92–101.
(Eds.), Mental health consultation in nursing homes Strittmatter, W. J., Saunders, A. M., Schmechel, D.,
(pp. 1–23). New York: New York University Press. Pericak-Vance, M., Enghild, J., Salvesen, G. S., et
Snowden, J. S., Goulding, P. J., & Neary, D. (1989). al. (1993). Apolipoprotein-E—high-avidity bind-
Semantic dementia: A form of circumscribed cere- ing to B-amyloid and increased frequency of type
bral atrophy. Behavioural Neurology, 2, 167–182. 4 allele in late-onset familial Alzheimer disease.
Sprengelmeyer, R., Lange, H., & Homberg, V. (1995). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of
The pattern of attentional deficits in Huntington’s the United States of America, 90, 9649–9653.
disease. Brain, 118, 145–152. Tales, A., Butler, S. R., Fossey, J., Gilchrist, I. D., Jones,
Squire, L. R. (1987). Memory and brain. New York: R. W., & Troscianko, T. (2002). Visual search in
Oxford University Press. Alzheimer’s disease: A deficiency in processing
Squire, L. R., Haist, F., & Shimamura, A. P. (1989). The conjunctions of features. Neuropsychologia, 40,
neurology of memory: Quantitative assessment 1849–1857.
of retrograde amnesia in two groups of amnesic Thal, L. J. (1999). Clinical trials in Alzheimer dis-
patients. Journal of Neuroscience, 9, 828–839. ease. In R. Terry, R. Katzman, K. L. Bick, and S.
The Neuropsychology of Dementia 197

S. Sisodia (Eds.), Alzheimer disease (pp. 423–439). Vonsattel, J. P., & Di Figlia, M. (1998). Huntington dis-
Philadelphia: Raven Press. ease. Journal of Neuropathology and Experimental
Thomas-Anterion, C., Jacquin, K., & Laurent, B. Neurology, 57, 369–384.
(2000). Differential mechanisms of impairment Walker, Z., Allen, R., Shergill, S., & Katona, C. (1997).
of remote memory in Alzheimer’s and frontotem- Neuropsychological performance in Lewy body
poral dementia. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. British Journal
Disorders, 11, 100–106. of Psychiatry, 170, 156–158.
Tippett, L. J., Blackwood, K., & Farah, M. J. (2003). Waltz, J. A., Knowlton, B. J., Holyoak, K. J., Boone,
Visual object and face processing in mild-to-mod- K. B., Back-Madruga, C., et al. (2004). Relational
erate Alzheimer’s disease: From segmentation to integration and executive function in Alzheimer’s
imagination. Neuropsychologia, 41, 453–468. disease. Neuropsychology, 18, 296–305.
Tiraboschi, P., Hansen, L. A., Alford, M., Merdes, Wancata, J., Börjesson-Hanson, J., Ostling, S.,
A., Masliah, E., Thal, L. J., et al. (2002). Early and Sjögren, K., & Skoog, I. (2007). Diagnostic criteria
widespread cholinergic losses differentiate demen- influence dementia prevalence. American Journal
tia with Lewy bodies from Alzheimer disease. of Geriatrci Psychiatry, 15, 1034–1045.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 946–951. Ward, J., Sheppard, J. M., Shpritz, B., Margolis,
Tiraboschi, P., Salmon, D. P., Hansen, L. A., Hofstetter, R. L., Rosenblatt, A., & Brandt, J. (2006). A
C. R., Thal, L. J., & Corey-Bloom, J. (2006). What four-year study of cognitive functioning in
best differentiates Lewy body from Alzheimer’s dis- Huntington’s disease. Journal of the International
ease in early-stage dementia? Brain, 129, 729–735. Neuropsychological Society, 12, 445–454.
Troster, A. I., Butters, N., Salmon, D. P., Cullum, C. Weder, N. D., Aziz, R., Wilkins, K., & Tampi, R.
M., Jacobs, D., Brandt, J., et al. (1993). The diag- R. (2007). Frontotemporal dementias: A review.
nostic utility of savings scores: Differentiating Annals of General Psychiatry, 6, 1–15.
Alzheimer’s and Huntington’s diseases with Welsh, K., Butters, N., Hughes, J., Mohs, R., &
the logical memory and visual reproduction Heyman, A. (1991). Detection of abnormal mem-
tests. Journal of Clinical and Experimental ory decline in mild cases of Alzheimer’s disease
Neuropsychology, 15, 773–788. using CERAD neuropsychological measures.
Tschanz, J. T., Welsh-Bohmer, K. A., Lyketsos, C. Archives of Neurology, 48, 278–281.
G., Corcoran, C., Green, R. C., Hayden, K., et al. West, R. L. (1996). An application of prefrontal cortex
(2006). Conversion to dementia from mild cogni- function theory to cognitive aging. Psychological
tive disorder: The Cache county study. Neurology, Bulletin, 120, 272–292.
67, 229–234. Wetter, S., Delis, D. C., Houston, W. S., Jacobson,
Twamley, E. W., Ropacki, S. A. L., & Bondi, M. W. M. W., Lansing, A., Cobell, K., et al. (2005).
(2006). Neuropsychological and neuroimaging Deficits in inhibition and flexibility are associ-
changes in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Journal ated with the APOE e4 allele in nondemented
of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12, older adults. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
707–735. Neuropsychology, 27, 943–952.
van Duijn, C. M., Stijnen, T., & Hofman, A. (1991). Whitehouse, P. J., Price, D. L., Struble, R. G., Clark, A.
Risk factors for Alzheimer’s disease: Overview of W., Coyle, J. T., & DeLong, M. R. (1982). Alzheimer’s
the EURODEM collaborative re-analysis of case- disease and senile dementia: Loss of neurons in the
control studies. EURODEM Risk Factors Research basal forebrain. Science, 215, 1237–1239.
Group. International Journal of Epidemiology, Wicklund, A. H., Rademaker, A., Johnson, N., Weitner,
20(Suppl. 2), S4–S12. B. B., & Weintraub, S. (2007). Rate of cognitive
van Praag, H., Shubert, T., Zhao, C., & Gage, F. change measured by neuropsychologic test perfor-
H. (2005). Exercise enhances learning and hip- mance in 3 distinct dementia syndromes. Alzheimer
pocampal neurogenesis in aged mice. Journal of Disease and Associated Disorders, 21, S70–S78.
Neuroscience, 25(38), 8680–8685. Wilhelmsen, K. C., Lynch, T., Pavlou, E., Higgins,
Varma, A. R., Snowden, J. S., Lloyd, J. J., Talbot, P. R., M., & Nygaard, T. G. (1994). Localization of dis-
Mann, D. M. A., & Neary, D. (1999). Evaluation inhibition dementia parkinsonism amyotrophy
of the NINCDS-ADRDA criteria in the differen- complex to 17q21–22. American Journal of Human
tiation of Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal Genetics, 55, 1159–1165.
dementia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Williams, G. C. (1957). Pleiotropy, natural selection,
Psychiatry, 66, 184–188. and the evolution of senescence. Evolution, 11(4),
Villardita, C. (1993). Alzheimer’s disease compared 398–411.
with cerebrovascular dementia. Acta Neurologica Wilson, R. S., Mendes de Leon, C. F., Barnes, L. L.,
Scandinavica, 87, 299–308. Schneider, J.A., Bienias, J.L., Evans, D.A., et al.
198 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

(2002). Participation in cognitively stimulating changes in healthy elderly persons correlate with
activities and risk of incident Alzheimer disease. attention and speed of mental processing. Archives
Journal of the American Medical Association, of Neurology, 50, 818–824.
287(6), 742–748. Zandi, P. P., Carlson, M. C., Plassman, B. L., Welsh-
Wilson, R. S., Bacon, L. D., Fox, J. H., & Kaszniak, Bohmer, K. A., Mayer, L. S., Steffens, D. C., et al.
A. W. (1983). Primary and secondary memory in (2002). Hormone replacement therapy and inci-
dementia of the Alzheimer type. Journal of Clinical dence of Alzheimer disease in older women: The
Neuropsychology, 5, 337–344. Cache County Study. Journal of the American
Wilson, R. S., Kaszniak, A. W., & Fox, J. H. (1981). Medical Association, 288, 2123–2129.
Remote memory in senile dementia. Cortex, 17, Zhang, M., Katzman, R., Salmon, D. P., Jin, H., Cai, G.,
41–48. Wang, Z., et al. (1990). The prevalence of dementia
Wright, R. O., Hu, H., Silverman, E. K., Tsaih, and Alzheimer’s disease (AD) in Shanghai, China:
S. W., Schwartz, J., Bellinger, D., et al. (2003). Impact on age, gender and education. Annals of
Apolipoprotein E genotype predicts Bayley scales Neurology, 27, 428–437.
infant development score. Pediatric Research, 54, Zola-Morgan, S., & Squire, L. R. (1990). The primate
819–825. hippocampal formation: Evidence for a time-
Ylikoski, R., Ylikoski, A., Erkinjuntti, T., Sulkava, limited role in memory storage. Science, 250,
R., Raininko, R., & Tilvis, R. (1993). White matter 288–290.
9

Neuropsychological Aspects of Parkinson’s


Disease and Parkinsonism
Susan McPherson and Jeffrey Cummings

Parkinson’s disease (PD) is one of the most Definitions and Classification


common neurological diseases, affecting
Parkinson’s disease must be distinguished from
approximately 350 per 100,000 individuals.
the less specific clinical syndrome of parkinson-
Approximately 95% of incident cases of PD
ism. Parkinsonism refers to a cluster of motor
occur after the age of 60 making it a common
symptoms consisting of slowness of movement
disorder among the elderly (de Lau & Breteler,
(bradykinesia), difficulty initiating movement
2006). At least one study has projected that the
(akinesia), hypokinetic speech, masked facies,
number of cases of PD will increase to between
muscular rigidity, a shuffling or unsteady gait,
8.7 and 9.2 million patients by 2030 (Dorsey
abnormal posture, and disturbances in equi-
et al., 2007). Behavioral and neuropsychologi-
librium. Tremor may or may not be present.
cal disorders are common in PD and other par-
Parkinson’s disease is a neurodegenerative dis-
kinsonian syndromes. Patients with PD may
order consisting of symptoms of parkinsonism
have dementia, depression, anxiety, apathy, and
plus a pill-rolling type of resting tremor. PD
a variety of drug-related or surgically induced
typically improves with dopaminergic treat-
behavioral alterations.
ment. An absence or paucity of tremor and poor
Significant advances have been made in
treatment response help differentiate non-PD
understanding the pathophysiology of PD.
parkinsonian syndromes from PD (Stacy &
Although depletion of dopamine in the sub-
Jankovic, 1992).
stantia nigra and basal ganglia has been recog-
nized as responsible for the motor disturbances
of the disease, evidence suggests that changes
Classification of Parkinsonism
in the basal ganglia and frontal–subcortical
circuits also play a role in the associated cog- Parkinsonism can be divided into four sub-
nitive abnormalities and behavioral changes groups: primary, secondary, multiple system
(Taylor & Saint-Cyr, 1992; Zgaljardic et al., degeneration, and other neurodegenerative
2006). conditions (Table 9–1). Primary or idiopathic
The purpose of this chapter is to provide an PD accounts for majority of cases of par-
overview of the neuropsychological and psy- kinsonism. Young onset disease (age 21–40)
chiatric features of parkinsonian syndromes, affects 5%–10% of patients and, although onset
including PD, progressive supranuclear palsy is rare before the age of 30, there is a juvenile
(PSP), and multiple system atrophies (MSA). form of the disease with onset before age 21
The clinical characteristics, pathology, treat- (Samii et al., 2004). Secondary parkinsonism
ment and neuropsychological deficits associ- may occur in a variety of conditions including
ated with each disorder are described. infections (encephalitis, slow virus), exposure

199
200 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Table 9–1. Classifications of Parkinsonism prevalence of PD in industrialized countries has


been estimated at 0.3% of the population and
Primary (idiopathic) parkinsonism 1% in persons over age 60 (Nussbaum & Ellis,
Parkinson’s disease
Juvenile Parkinson’s disease
2003). The estimated annual incidence rates
Secondary parkinsonism (number of new cases per year) range from 12 to
Drug-induced (antipsychotic agents, antiemetics) 15 per 100,000 for all age groups with a signif-
Infections (postencephalitic, slow virus, human icant rise to 160 per 100,000 for persons above
immunodeficiency virus) age 65 (Hirtz et al., 2007). Recent epidemiolog-
Vascular (lacunar state)
Toxins (MPTPa, manganese, carbon monoxide)
ical surveys suggest that the disease affects men
Metabolic (parathyroid, hypothyroidism) slightly more than women (Baldereschi et al.,
Trauma (pugilistic encephalopathy) 2000; Lai et al., 2003). Community-based stud-
Tumor, normal pressure hydrocephalus ies of PD have varied with some suggesting that
Multiple System Degeneration (parkinsonian plus the highest rates of the disease are observed
syndromes)
Multiple system degeneration-parkinsonism
in Europe and North America, with mark-
(formerly striatonigral degeneration) edly lower rates in Japan, China, and Africa
Multiple system degeneration-cerebellar (Albin, 2006). Although PD appears to occur
(formerly olivopontocerebellar atrophy) at a lower prevalence rate in Blacks and Asians
Multiple system degeneration-autonomic as compared with Whites, differences in these
(formerly Shy Drager syndrome)
Other neurodegenerative disorders
rates may reflect differences in response rates,
Progressive supranuclear palsy survival and case-ascertainment rather than
Lewy body dementia real differences across ethnic groups (de Lau &
Wilson’s disease Breteler, 2006).
Rigid Huntington’s disease
Parkinsonian-ALS dementia complex
Idiopathic calcification of the basal ganglia Clinical Characteristics
(Fahr’s disease)
a
Movement disturbance is the hallmark of PD
1-methyl-4-phenyl-1, 2, 3, 6-tetrahydrophyridine (MPTP)
and the primary motor symptoms include aki-
nesia, bradykinesia, rigidity, loss of associated
movements, tremor, and neuro-ophthalmic
to toxins, drugs (particularly antipsychotic abnormalities. Tremor is probably the best-
medications), or trauma (pugilistic encephalop- recognized clinical feature in PD, but other
athy). There also exists a vascular form of the motor disturbances may present as the initial
disease, usually caused by lacunar infarctions sign. The primary symptom of PD, bradykine-
of the basal ganglia and characterized by mark- sia or hypokinesia, accounts for many of the
edly impaired gait with relatively spared upper characteristic features of the disease including
body motor functioning (Rektor et al., 2006). difficulty with initiation of movement, expres-
Multiple system degenerations account for the sionless face (masked facies), loss of associated
second largest number of parkinsonian plus movements, and micrographia. Muscular rigid-
disorders and include the parkinsonian (stria- ity involves the upper and lower limbs, produc-
tonigral degeneration), cerebellar (olivoponto- ing a lead-pipe rigidity to passive manipulation.
cerebellar atrophy) and autonomic (Shy Drager The combination of tremor and rigidity bestows
syndrome) forms of the disease. Other degener- a ratchet or cogwheel character to the rigidity.
ative causes of parkinsonism include conditions Increase in rigidity also results in the postural
such as PSP, Lewy body dementia, the rigid changes seen in PD characterized by slight flex-
form of Huntington’s disease, Wilson’s disease, ion of ankles, knees, hips, elbows, back, and neck
and idiopathic basal ganglia calcification. and poor balance, leading to an increased risk
of falls (Samii et al., 2004). Although hypokine-
Parkinson’s Disease sia is frequently associated with rigidity, the two
can be dissociated and the psychomotor retar-
Parkinson’s disease is ranked among the most dation of PD is not a result of rigidity. The char-
common chronic neurological disorders. The acteristic tremor of the disease is a pill-rolling
Neuropsychological Aspects of Parkinson’s Disease and Parkinsonism 201

alternating contraction of opposing muscles, on chromosome 4 has been linked to autosomal


usually with a frequency of 4–6 cycles per sec- dominant familial disease (Polymeropoulos
ond (cps) and is the first symptom in 70% of et al., 1997). The PARK2 locus which encodes
patients. The tremor typically develops in one the protein parkin, a key enzyme in the ubiq-
upper extremity and may spread to all four uitin-proteosome system, has been linked to
limbs, face, and tongue (Mendez & Cummings, juvenile onset PD (Kitada et al., 1998). An addi-
2003). This tremor is most apparent when the tional loci (PARK5) have been linked to ubiq-
patient is in alert repose, is absent or minimal uitin-pathways by encoding ubiquitin terminal
when the patient is relaxed or asleep, and is hyrolase L1 (UCHl1). Three loci have focused
exacerbated by stress. In addition to the resting on mitochondrial dysfunction, due to muta-
tremor, a minority of PD patients may exhibit tions in the proteins PINK1, DJ-1, and LRRK2.
an action or postural tremor of 6–12 cps. Mutations in the DJ-1 gene (PARK7) have been
Neuro-ophthalmologic changes and auto- associated with autosomal recessive early-on-
nomic disturbances also occur in PD. Volitional set PD and may be linked to oxidative stress
upgaze and convergence are impaired. Rapid (Bonifati et al., 2003). Mutations of the PARK8
volitional eye movements tend to be fragmented (LRRK2) locus produce dominantly inherited
into multiple saccades and pursuit movements are PD and may contribute to a small number of
broken into a series of small saccadic steps (cog- cases of typical late-onset disease (Albin, 2006).
wheel eye movements) (Mendez & Cummings, While the above gene loci have been associated
2003). Autonomic disturbances often associ- with some forms of the disease, the relationship
ated with PD include prominent gastrointestinal of these genes to PD is far from conclusive, and
symptoms, orthostatic hypotension, constipa- they appear to account for only a small number
tion, impotence, and esophageal spasm (Jost, of cases.
2003). Sleep disturbances are also common and
arise from a variety of causes including noc-
turnal stiffness, nocturia, depression, restless
Pathology
leg syndrome, and rapid eye movement (REM)
sleep behavior disorder (Stacy, 2002). Characteristic neuropathological changes in PD
include neuron loss in the substantia nigra and
other brain stem nuclei (locus ceruleus, dorsal
vagal nucleus, and sympathetic ganglia) and the
Genetics
presence of Lewy bodies in some of the remain-
Studies on the genetics of PD have produced ing neurons of the involved nuclei. Lewy bodies
mixed results. While some studies have sug- are intracellular structures made up of pro-
gested that only 15% of patients with PD have tein and sphingomyelin and consist of loosely
a first degree relative with the disease (Payami packed fi laments in an outer zone surrounding
et al., 1994), more recent studies have suggested a granular core. The principal protein of Lewy
that individuals with PD are two to three times bodies is α-synuclein. Although found in high-
more likely to have a first degree relative with est concentrations in the substantia nigra and
the disease (Marder et al., 2003). Twin studies locus ceruleus, they also occur in the amygdala,
have suggested low concordance rates in late- cortex, nucleus basalis, dorsal vagal nucleus,
onset PD but increased concordance rates in autonomic ganglia, and hypothalamus (Braak
earlier onset PD (Tanner et al., 1999). Recent et al., 2003).
genetic investigation has resulted in the discov- Parkinson’s disease is characterized neuro-
ery of genes and/or genetic loci associated with chemically by the loss of dopaminergic projec-
PD. Nine loci in PD have been identified and tions from the substantia nigra pars compacta to
five genes for Mendelian PARK loci have been the basal ganglia and associated brain regions.
cloned. These include α-synuclein (PARK1); Dopamine content is markedly diminished in
parkin (PARK2), DJ-1 (PARK7), PINK1 the caudal putamen. Dopamine is also depleted
(PARK6), and LRRK2 (PARK8) (Hardy et al., in the caudate, frontal lobe cortex, and medial
2006). The α-synuclein mutation (PARK1) found temporal lobes. Other neurotransmitters and
202 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

modulators are decreased in PD including nor- increasing side effects of confusion, hallucina-
epinephrine, glutamate decarboxylase, gamma tion, and insomnia often observed in patients’
amino butyric acid (GABA), methionine-en- taking selegiline. Results of a trial evaluating
kephalin, cholestokinin, and serotonin and its the efficacy and safety of rasageline as both
metabolite 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid, albeit monotherapy and adjunctive therapy suggested
none to the extent of dopamine (Mendez & that when compared with patients on placebo,
Cummings, 2003). Dopamine receptor densi- patients treated with rasageline as monotherapy
ties are unaffected. Clinical signs of PD are evi- reported only 3% more symtoms of depression
dent when 80% of striatal dopamine and 50% of as compared with patients on placebo. When
nigral neurons are lost (Fearnley & Lees, 1991). used as adjunctive therapy cognitive and behav-
Cholinergic deficits are prominent in PD with ioral adverse events occurred only 1.6% of the
dementia and are associated with cognitive time (Elmer et al., 2006) as compared with
and neuropsychiatric symptoms (Bohnen et al., patients on placebo.
2003; Tiraboschi et al., 2000). Selegiline and rasageline are MAO-B inhibi-
tors that reduce the generation of hydroxyl
radicals. Free radicals interfere with the main-
Treatment
tenance of cell membranes and lead to cellular
death; reduction of radical presence prevents
Medications
cell death and may allow partially injured neu-
Treatment of PD includes agents used to relieve rons to recover. Although consensus is lacking
symptoms and those that may slow progres- among neuroscientists, administration of sele-
sion and preserve neurons. The current main- giline or rasageline may mitigate cell death and
stay of PD therapy is levodopa, a precursor to slows disease progression.
dopamine that readily crosses the blood–brain One of the newest classes of drugs for the
barrier and, once in the substantia nigra, is con- treatment of PD are the peripherally acting
verted into dopamine. Levodopa is typically catechol-O-methlytransferase (COMT) inhib-
administered in conjunction with the dopa- itor-entacapone. Administration of a COMT
decarboxylase inhibitor carbidopa, to mini- inhibitor such as entacapone concomitantly
mize peripheral side effects. Levodopa has been with levodopa leads to more stable plasma levo-
found to be effective in alleviating the motor dopa concentrations through out the day and to
symptoms of PD in at least 60% of patients for fewer daily fluctuations (Brooks & Sagar, 2003).
a period of 1–4 years. Unfortunately, levodopa Studies with entacapone have shown the drug
produces many adverse effects including nau- to be effective in increasing the “on” time in
sea, choreoathetotic dyskinesias, postural hypo- patients with fluctuations and improving activi-
tension, and palpitations. Psychiatric side effects ties of daily living in patients without significant
such as sleep disturbances, hallucinations, delu- “on–off ” fluctuations (Brooks & Sagar, 2003).
sions, anxiety, delirium, and hypomania are Studies on rivastigmine, an inhibitor of
not uncommon (Mendez & Cummings, 2003). both acetycholinesterase and butyrylcholin-
Promipaxole and ropinerole are dopamine esterase, have been shown to have meaningful
receptor agonists that also relieve parkinsonian treatment benefits in patients with Lewy body
symptoms and have side effects similar to levo- dementia (McKeith et al., 2000) and in patients
dopa. It is common to use a receptor agonist in with dementia associated with PD (Aarsland
conjunction with levodopa to minimize late- et al., 2002; Giladi et al., 2003). Rivastigmine
occurring on–off symptoms. produced moderate improvements on global
Rasageline (N-propargyl-R-aminoindan) ratings of dementia, attention and executive
mesylate is a selective, irreversible, second gen- functions, and behavioral symptoms in patients
eration monoamine oxidase (MAO-B) inhibitor with dementia associated with PD. Higher rates
that has shown effectiveness in early PD when of nausea, vomiting, and tremor were observed
given as an adjunct to levodopa (Parkinson (Emre et al., 2004). Rivastigmine has been
Study Group, 2002). Treatment with rasageline approved for use in PD dementia by the U.S.
has been shown to reduce “off ” time without Food and Drug Administration.
Neuropsychological Aspects of Parkinson’s Disease and Parkinsonism 203

Surgical Interventions dysphagia, abulia, and aphonia and is no longer


recommended (Ghika et al., 1999).
Pallidotomy
Surgical treatment for PD began over 60 years Deep Brain Stimulation
ago when thalamotomy was performed to Deep brain stimulation (DBS) involves high-
reduce contralateral tremor (Burchiel, 1995) frequency, bilateral stimulation of deep brain
and pallidotomy improved motor symptoms targets including thalamus, globus pallidus,
(Guridi & Lozano, 1997). With the introduction and subthalamic nucleus (STN) and is consid-
of medications in the 1960s, the use of surgi- ered an effective treatment for advanced PD
cal intervention was abandoned until the 1990s (Limousin et al., 1998) with noted benefits on
when surgical intervention was reintroduced to motor function (Volkmann, 2004) and quality
improve bradykinesia, rigidity, and dyskinesias of life (Diamond & Jankovic, 2005). Eligibility
(Hallett & Litvan, 1999). for DBS relies on the recommendations of
The cognitive outcome associated with uni- the Core Assessment Program for Surgical
lateral pallidotomy has produced mixed results. Interventional Therapies in Parkinson’s disease
Some studies have shown no evidence of cog- (CAPSIT-PD; Defer et al., 1999) and requires
nitive impairment (Cahn et al., 1998; Fukuda clinically diagnosed PD, a disease duration of
et al., 2000; Perrine et al., 1998; Soukup et al., greater than 5 years, age <70, refractory motor
1997) while others have reported improvement complications, Hoehn-Yahr scale ≥3 (bilateral
in memory (Lacritz et al., 2000). Other studies disease with postural impairment) and severe
have reported declines in phonemic and seman- motor disability among other criteria. Exclusion
tic verbal fluency particularly after left-sided criteria include dementia and poorly controlled
pallidotomy in right-handed patients (Alegret psychiatric disorders.
et al., 2003; Kubu et al., 2000; Masterman et al., The cognitive effects of STN stimulation
1998; Obwegeser et al., 2000; Riordan et al., have been investigated with majority of studies
1997; Trepanier et al., 1998; York et al., 2003). revealing decline in both semantic and phone-
Impairments have been noted in verbal learn- mic fluency following STN (Parsons et al., 2006).
ing and memory, particularly after left-sided The effects of STN on other aspects of cogni-
pallidotomy (Lombardi et al., 2000; Riordan tion reveal inconsistent results ranging from
et al., 1997). Changes in visuospatial functions no global cognitive deterioration (Funkiewiez
and visual memory have shown either improve- et al., 2004), minimal effects on cognition
ments (Obwegeser et al., 2000; Riordan et al., (Ardouin et al., 1999) or mild changes in atten-
1997) or declines (Trepanier et al., 1998) fol- tion, verbal fluency (Morrison et al., 2004),
lowing right-sided surgery. Studies on execu- executive functions, verbal memory, and spa-
tive functions have also produced mixed results tial functions (Alegret et al., 2001; Daniele et al.,
with some studies reporting no changes in 2003; Dujardin et al., 2001; Smeding et al., 2006;
executive functions (Cahn et al., 1998; Demakis Trepanier et al., 2000). Studies on STN in older
et al., 2002; Kubu et al., 2000; Uitti et al., 2000) patients have shown decreases in memory, men-
or slight improvements (Jahanshahi et al., 2002; tal speed, and fluency (Saint-Cyr et al., 2000)
Rettig et al., 2000) and other studies reporting as well as global cognitive decline or behavior
declines in working memory and/or executive changes (Dujardin et al., 2001).
impairments (de Bie et al., 2001; Obwegeser
et al., 2000; Stebbins et al., 2000; Trepanier et al.,
1998). One recent study showed an isolated def- Neuropsychological Aspects of PD
icit on one measure of executive functions only
(Wisconsin Card Sorting Test) marked by an Patients with PD commonly exhibit deficits in
increase in the percentage of perseverative errors cognitive abilities; in many cases, these deficits
and a decrease in the conceptual responses per- are confined to specific functions such as loss
centage (Olzak et al., 2006). Bilateral pallidomy of cognitive flexibility, reduced ability for learn-
has been associated with cognitive deficits, ing and recall of information, and psychomotor
204 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

slowing. The following section presents the pat- literature regarding retrieval deficits may be
tern of neuropsychologic deficits exhibited by secondary to task difficulty stating that some
PD patients without overt dementia. recognition tasks may not be sufficiently chal-
lenging to reveal deficits in early-stage PD
patients (Whittington et al., 2006).
Attention
Simple attentional skills, as assessed by a
Language
patient’s ability to repeat a series of digits or
register a short list of words, is generally unim- Compared with other areas of cognitive func-
paired in patients with PD, even in patients tioning, language skills are relatively spared in
with severe motor dysfunction. Performance on PD, particularly vocabulary. There is somewhat
attentional tasks that demand speeded cognitive greater controversy in the literature concerning
processing or require the patient to internally confrontation naming with investigations find-
guide their attentional resources are generally ing mixed results.
impaired (Pahwa et al., 1998; Ridenour & Dean, Although the literature concerning perfor-
1999). Deficits in divided attention have been mance of PD patients on tasks of verbal fluency,
suggested and are attributed to the inability to as assessed by asking the patient to generate a
fi lter out interference and ignore distractions list of words according to some predetermined
(Claus & Mohr, 1996). criteria (e.g., letters or semantic categories), has
produced mixed findings, the preponderance of
literature suggests that both semantic and pho-
Memory
nemic fluency are impaired in PD patients with-
Non-demented patients with PD exhibit defi- out dementia (Bayles et al., 1993; Raskin et al.,
cits in the areas of paired associates, verbal 1992; Troster et al., 1998). It has been suggested
list learning, recall of brief prose passages and that poor performance on tasks of verbal flu-
recall measures involving reproduction of ency, secondary to deficits in executive function
complex designs. These deficits are indepen- has usually been attributed to frontal/execu-
dent of anti-parkinsonian medications (Brown tive deficits (e.g., set shifting or strategy initia-
& Marsden, 1990; Cooper et al., 1993). Patients tion) and not to a breakdown of lexical stores
are sensitive to the effects of proactive interfer- (Zgaljardic et al., 2003).
ence (Massman et al., 1990). While free recall of Deficits have been reported in comprehen-
newly learned information is impaired, mem- sion of complex commands and grammar and
ory for remote information has been found to decreased syntactic complexity in spontaneous
be unimpaired. Studies have also suggested that speech. Difficulties in complex sentence pro-
prospective memory, which involves remember- cessing have been reported to be present in non-
ing to execute a planned action at some point in demented PD patients (Grossman, 1999; Skeel
the future, is impaired in PD (Katai et al., 2003; et al., 2001) and have been attributed to limita-
Whittington et al., 2006). These results suggest tions in working memory and processing speed
that a specific profi le of memory deficit exists in and the need for greater recruitment of other
PD patients without overt dementia, as opposed cortical regions to compensate for depleted
to a more pervasive decline in memory function working memory resources (Grossman et al.,
in patients with overt dementia. 2003).
Controversy exists regarding retrieval of
information or recognition memory in PD.
Visuospatial Skills
While some studies suggest that retrieval
is intact in PD (Breen, 1993; Emre, 2003; Assessment of visuospatial skills is difficult
Ivory et al., 1999), other studies have found in patients with profound motor impairment.
impaired recognition in non-demented PD Even when the effects of motor slowing are
patients (Barnes et al., 2003; Stebbins et al., taken into account, visuospatial and visuocon-
1999; Woods & Troster, 2003). Some investiga- structive skills are among the most frequently
tors have suggested that the inconsistencies in reported cognitive disturbances associated with
Neuropsychological Aspects of Parkinson’s Disease and Parkinsonism 205

PD (Brown & Marsden, 1990). Studies of visu- achieve significantly fewer categories, with dis-
ospatial functions have found that PD patients agreement among researchers on the frequency
exhibit deficits in visual analysis and synthesis and type of errors. Deficits in abstract reason-
(as assessed by performance on embedded fig- ing, cognitive flexibility, accuracy of categori-
ures tasks), visual discrimination and match- zation, and problem solving are also prominent
ing, and pattern completion. Patients with PD (Hodgson et al., 2002; Maddox & Filoteo, 2001;
exhibited impaired performance on tasks of Tomer et al., 2002).
constructional praxis even after the speed com- Several authors have suggested that dis-
ponent had been eliminated to minimize motor turbances in frontal systems abilities and set
demands. Visuospatial dysfunction may be aptitude may be the underlying mechanism
secondary to demands of visuospatial tasks on accounting for the difficulties manifested by PD
executive functions such as planning and shift- patients in the areas of memory, visuospatial
ing of attention (Bondi et al., 1993; Raskin et al., skills, language, and perception (Levin et al.,
1992). 1992) and a recent study has attempted to link
Not all aspects of visuospatial functioning are the executive dysfunction in PD to frontal sub-
impaired in PD. Studies have suggested that PD cortical circuit dysfunction (Zgaljardic et al.,
patients were as accurate as control subjects in 2006). Findings suggested that dysfunction of
making right/left judgments even on measures the dorsolateral prefrontal circuit (DLPFC), as
requiring mental rotation (Brown & Marsden, defined by neuropsychological measures, influ-
1990). Variable findings have been reported on ences the overall executive impairment found
graphomotor visuoconstructive tasks, such as in non-demented PD patients.
design copying, with some investigators report-
ing impairment and others finding no differ-
Issues in Neuropsychology of PD
ences between PD patients and controls on
either simple or complex design copying. Although it is evident from the studies cited
above that patients with PD may exhibit changes
in cognitive abilities very early in the course of
Executive Functions
the disease, additional factors occurring in the
The term executive function refers to a group of course of the illness are likely to further impair
cognitive skills involved in the initiation, plan- the cognitive abilities of the PD patient. The
ning, and monitoring of goal-directed behav- following section will consider several of these
iors. Executive functions include the ability to factors: disease severity, the effect of “on–off ”
establish and maintain set; shift from one set to periods on cognitive functions, and the interac-
another; form concepts and reason abstractly; tion of depression and anxiety with intellectual
use feedback to monitor behavior; program function.
sequential motor activities; develop strategies
to learn and copy complex figures; and exert
Early versus Late PD
emotional self-control and maintain socially
appropriate behavior. Assessment of patients who have not yet begun
Impaired performance on tasks of execu- treatment with anti-parkinsonian medications
tive functions are among the earliest cognitive provides a clearer understanding of the specific
deficits observed in patients with PD. Patients impact of the disease on cognitive functioning.
without overt dementia have been found to Results of studies assessing recently diagnosed
exhibit deficits in areas of planning, set shifting PD patients before treatment was begun with
and initiation of responses (Cools et al., 2001a, anti-parkinsonian medications indicated that
2001b; Hozumi et al., 2000). Researchers have deficits in early PD are likely to be confined to
found consistently poor performance by PD the three areas of psychomotor slowing, loss of
patients, as compared with matched controls, cognitive flexibility, and mildly reduced learn-
on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), a ing and recall. Other studies have suggested
task of concept formation and set shift ing abil- that deficits in frontally mediated measures
ity with most studies reporting that PD patients including attention and executive functions
206 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

are among the earliest deficits (Dujardin et al., with impaired performance on frontal/exec-
1999; Owen et al., 1992). Untreated, newly diag- utive neuropsychological tests leading to the
nosed PD patients showed deficits in immediate suggestion that dopamine might not be directly
recall of verbal material, language production associated with nonmotor dysfunction but may
and semantic fluency, set-formation, cognitive be operative in an indirect fashion (Zgaljardic
sequencing and working memory, and visuomo- et al., 2003).
tor construction, but they were unimpaired in
immediate memory span, long-term forgetting, Neuropsychiatric Features of PD
naming, comprehension, and visual perception
(Cooper et al., 1991). Depression
Findings of deficits in memory, attention, and
executive functions were reported in newly diag- Depression ranks as one of most common psy-
nosed patients who were treated with levodopa chiatric complications of PD and has been
as compared with normal controls (Muslimovic estimated to occur in approximately 40% of
et al., 2005). In contrast, patients with moderate patients (Aarsland et al., 1999). In a system-
to severe PD who have been under treatment for atic review of the available literature on PD and
long periods of time often exhibit more marked depression, Cummings (1992) found no con-
neuropsychological impairments including def- sistent relationships between depressive symp-
icits in psychomotor speed, visuospatial ability, toms and patient’s current age, age at onset of
and both verbal and nonverbal learning and PD, or duration of PD. Although studies have
recall. identified a past history of depression as a risk
factor for a major mood disorder after the onset
of PD, other investigators have not found a fam-
Cognitive Changes Associated ily history of psychiatric illness, or more spe-
with “On–Off” Periods cifically family history of mood disorder, to
The “on–off ” phenomenon in PD refers to be a risk factor (Leentjens et al., 2002; Mayeux
abrupt fluctuations between relatively mild et al., 1992). There is no consistently identified
motor disability or chorea (“on”) and relatively relationship between depression and patient
severe motor disability (“off ”) that occur after gender. Depression is characterized by promi-
chronic levodopa therapy and includes end- nent anxiety and less self-punative ideation, as
of-dose deterioration as well as unpredictable compared with idiopathic depression and lower
shifts between mobile or dyskinetic “on” states suicide rates as seen in the general population
and akinetic “off ” states unrelated to medi- (Lauterbach, 2004). Dementia has been found
cation schedules. Prior literature comparing to be more common among those with greater
cognition in “on” versus “off ” states found no cognitive impairment or dementia (Aarsland
significant differences in the areas of attention, et al., 1996).
simple reaction time, verbal fluency, visuospa- The pathophysiology of depression in PD
tial abilities, executive functions, immediate has been attributed to a complex combination
recall for short stories and paired associates, of abnormalities in the dopaminergic, norad-
immediate and delayed recall of nonverbal renergic, and serotonergic transmitters systems
memory, and delayed recall and recognition on (Mayeux, 1990) with an allelic variation in sero-
a list learning task. Significant differences were tonin transporters suggested as a predisposing
noted between on/off patients on tests of supras- factor to mood disorders (Mossner et al., 2001).
pan list learning, delayed recall of short stories
and paired associates, and choice reaction time,
Anxiety
with optimal results when dopaminergic sta-
tus was congruent during learning and later Anxiety and panic have been noted in up to
retrieval, indicating a state-dependent memory 40% of PD patients. Anxiety after initiation of
effect (Delis & Massman, 1992). Other stud- levodopa treatment has been reported, with
ies have reported that dopamine withdrawal patients experiencing apprehension, nervous-
in a sample of patients with PD was associated ness, irritability, feeling of impending disaster,
Neuropsychological Aspects of Parkinson’s Disease and Parkinsonism 207

palpitations, hyperventilation, and insomnia study has suggested that gambling is associated
(Factor et al., 1995). Several studies have sug- with the use of medications ropinirole, per-
gested that anxiety symptoms may precede the golide, and pramipexole (Lu et al., 2006) with
diagnosis (Lauterbach & Duvoisin, 1991; Shiba one study suggesting that the disorder occurs
et al., 2000) with an exacerbation of symptoms mainly in patients taking pramipexole (Dodd
following initiation of treatment with levodopa. et al., 2005).
Reports have suggested that pergolide and sele- Anatomically, pathological gambling in PD
giline may be more likely to induce anxiety than has been hypothesized to be linked to the ven-
other parkinsonian agents (Cummings, 1991). tral striatium and ventral prefrontal cortex, both
Anxiety has also been linked to “off ” periods of which have been implicated in reinforcement
and was correlated with severity of symptoms learning and reward processing (Ardouin et al.,
and disease duration (Siemers et al., 1993). 2006; Reuter et al., 2005). Over stimulation of
In contrast to the above findings, a few studies D3 receptors of the limbic areas of the brain
have reported that anxiety in PD is not related have been suggested as another pharmacologi-
to duration of treatment or levodopa level and cal substrate of gambling behavior (Dodd et al.,
may be related to coexistence of depression 2005) with the development of pathological
(Stein et al., 1990). Given the high frequency of gambling in PD patients attributed to overstim-
depression in PD patients in general, it has been ulation of dopamine receptors receiving projec-
hypothesized that the presence of anxiety in tions from relatively spared ventral tegmental
PD patients is a concomitant of depression, and area (Cools et al., 2003).
may not be related to pathophysiologic changes
associated with PD (Henderson et al., 1992).
Sleep Disorders
Sleep disorders are common in PD, affecting
Psychosis
60%–98% of patients (Adler & Thorpy, 2005)
Psychotic symptoms have been estimated to and occur as a result of a combination of fac-
occur in up to 50% of PD patients, generally tors including the disease itself, mood disorders
occurring in patients usually those treated with and pain (Ferreri et al., 2006). Sleep disorders
anti-parkinsonian agents. Drug-induced psy- include insomnia and hypersomnia as well as
chosis is more common with dopamine ago- parasomnias, including vivid dreams, sleep
nists than with L-dopa and is the leading cause terror disorder, REM sleep disorder including
of nursing home placement and produces a REM behavior disorder (RBD). REM behavior
greater impact on quality of life and caregiver disorder is a syndrome in which patients act out
burden than does motor disability (Lauterbach, dreams with kicking, grabbing, yelling, shout-
2004). Visual hallucinations, misidentifications ing, and falling or jumping out of bed (Ferreri
and paranoid delusions are common and may et al., 2006). At least one study suggested that
result from a combination of anti-parkinsonian RBD may proceed the onset of motor symptoms
medications, frontal lobe dysfunction, and in PD in up to 33% of patients (Postuma et al.,
REM sleep intrusions during the day (Mendez 2006).
& Cummings, 2003).
Dementia Associated with PD
Gambling
Dementia affects approximately 40% of patients
Pathological gambling has been reported in PD with PD, and incidence in PD patients is up to
on dopaminergic treatment (Dodd et al., 2005; six times that in healthy adults (Emre, 2003).
Driver-Dunckley et al., 2003; Molina et al., The prevalence of dementia in PD ranges from
2000) and is attributed to dopamine dysregula- 107 to 187 per 100,000 (Mayeux et al., 1995).
tion syndrome that is defined as compulsive use Prevalence of dementia among PD patients
of dopaminergic treatment with secondary cog- below age 50 has been noted to be zero while
nitive and behavioral disturbances (Giovannoni the prevalence in patients above age 80 was 69%
et al., 2000; Lawrence et al., 2003). At least one (Mayeux et al., 1992). Risk factors for dementia
208 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

include advanced age at onset of motor symp- and is considered more severe than in patients
toms, duration of disease, akinetic-rigid syn- with AD with similar dementia severity (Levin
drome, early occurrence of levodopa related et al., 1991; Stern et al., 1993). Deficits in ver-
confusion or psychosis, depression, and poor bal fluency are more severe than in patients
cognitive test scores, particularly on measures with AD (Stern et al., 1993). Naming difficul-
of verbal fluency (Emre, 2003). ties, decreased information content of sponta-
Parkinson’s disease, the dementia associated neous speech, and impaired comprehension of
with Parkinson’s disease (PDD), and dementia complex sentences have been reported in both
with Lewy bodies (DLB) are presently referred demented and non-demented patients with PD
to as the “Lewy body diseases” (LBD) (Lippa although to a lesser extent than in patients with
et al., 2007). The clinical manifestations of PDD AD (Grossman et al., 1992). One study compar-
and DLB reflect a common pathogenesis, pre- ing patients with PDD, DLB, and AD reported
sumably related to misfolding and aggregation the presence of a subcortical-type cognitive
of α-synuclein, leading to the conclusion by a impairment profile with marked executive,
recent DLB/PDD workgroup that the two dis- visuoconstructive, and attentional impairment,
eases share many features (Lippa et al., 2007). but relatively less marked memory impairment
This workgroup accepted the classification of in majority of the sample of subjects with PDD
PDD and DLB based on the timing of the onset and DLB as compared with AD (Janvin et al.,
of dementia in relation to the onset of motor 2006). However, 26% of the PDD sample had a
symptoms (Lippa et al., 2007). When the onset pattern of cortical cognitive impairment, sim-
of cognitive symptoms precedes motor symp- ilar to most of the AD patients, with relatively
toms, a diagnosis of DLB is appropriate, and more severe memory impairment.
when the onset of the cognitive symptoms fol-
lows motor symptoms a diagnosis of PDD is
Dementia with Lewy Bodies
appropriate.
Dementia with lewy bodies is considered to be
the second most common subgroup of degener-
Parkinson’s Disease Dementia
ative dementias (McKeith et al., 2005) account-
The clinical features of dementia associated ing for 15%–35% of all dementias after AD
with PDD includes a dysexecutive system in (Geser et al., 2005). The core features of DLB
which impairment of executive functions is include fluctuations in cognition with pro-
the primary feature and is more extensive and nounced variation in attention and alertness,
severe than in patients with PD without demen- recurrent visual hallucinations that are typi-
tia. Deficits in attention and concentration are cally well formed and detailed, and the sponta-
marked by slowed reaction time and vigilance neous features of parkinsonism (McKeith et al.,
and fluctuations, similar to those found in 2005). Other features suggestive of DLB include
patients with DLB (Ballard et al., 2002). Studies REM sleep behavior disorder, severe neurolep-
regarding memory deficits in PDD have sug- tic sensitivity, and low dopamine transporter
gested that deficits in new learning and memory uptake in the basal ganglia as demonstrated by
are present but are less severe than those seen imaging. The cognitive profi le of DLB is marked
in patients with Alzheimer’s disease (Pillon by substantial attentional deficits and executive
et al., 1991; Stern et al., 1993). Although con- and visuospatial dysfunction (Calderon et al.,
troversy exists regarding the existence of a 2001; Collerton et al., 2003). In terms of cogni-
retrieval deficit in PDD, early studies suggested tive performance, preservation of confrontation
that patients with PDD benefited from semantic naming, short- and medium-term recall with
cuing (Pillon et al., 1991). As discussed above, intact recognition, and greater impairment on
executive dysfunction deficits are evident in the verbal fluency, visual perception, and perfor-
early stages of PD, with deficits in concept for- mance tasks helps to differentiate DLB from
mation, problem solving, set shifting, and the AD (Connor et al., 1998; Mormont et al., 2003;
ability to benefit from environmental cuing. Walker et al., 1997). The presence of visual hal-
Visuospatial dysfunction is also noted in PDD lucinations and visuospatial/constructional
Neuropsychological Aspects of Parkinson’s Disease and Parkinsonism 209

dysfunction also helps differentiate DLB from loss and norepinephrine depletion in the locus
AD in the earliest stages (Tiraboschi et al., coeruleus of patients with PDD and other stud-
2006). The neuropathological changes associ- ies finding no difference in concentrations of
ated with the DLB are discussed below. norepinephrine in cerebral neocortex and hip-
pocampus between patients with PD and PDD
(Emre, 2003). Neuronal loss in raphe nuclei and
Neurobiology and Neuropathology
reduced serotonin concentrations in the stri-
of Dementia in Parkinson’s Disease
atopallidal complex, hippocampus, and frontal
The neurobiological alterations associated with cortex have been described in PD but without
PDD include deficits in dopamine, monoam- differences between demented and non-de-
ines, and acetylcholine as well as neuropath- mented PD patients (Emre, 2003).
ological changes including the coexistence of
an Alzheimer-type dementia and Lewy body
Cholinergic Deficit
pathology.
Substantial evidence exists for a cholinergic def-
icit due to degeneration of ascending cholinergic
Dopaminergic Deficits
pathways contributing to cognitive impairment
Dopaminergic deficit is the main neuro- in patients with PD. Cholinergic deficits associ-
chemical impairment in PD and is linked to ated with degeneration of the basal forebrain is
the motor symptoms of the disease. Marked prominent in PDD and more severe than corti-
reductions of dopamine have been noted par- cal cholinergic losses in other dementias such
ticularly in the putamen and caudate nuclei. as AD, vascular or frontotemporal dementia
Other areas of dopamine reduction include the (Lippa et al., 1999; Perry et al., 1994). Level of
substantia nigra, nucleus accumbens, lateral cognitive impairment and presence of dementia
hypothalamus, ventral tegmental area, frontal has been described in patients with a decrease
lobe, cingulate gyrus, entorhinal cortex, and in cholinergic innervation of the cerebral cor-
hippocampus. tex and severe cellular loss in the basal nucleus
Evidence suggests that dopaminergic deficit of Meynert (Emre, 2003). Preliminary results
is not alone responsible for the dementia in PD. from a recent autopsy study of patients with
Although decrease in striatal dopamine concen- PDD suggested that in patients with onset of
trations are the same in PD and PDD, decrease dementia more than 10 years after PD, the mor-
in dopamine concentrations is greater in neo- phologic changes associated with DLB were less
cortical areas in PDD than PD suggesting a role pronounced and the more prominent finding
for degeneration of mesocortical dopaminergic was a marked cholinergic deficit (Aarsland et al.,
systems in the development of dementia (Emre, 2005). Cholinergic deficits have been linked to
2003). While experimental studies have sug- key clinical symptoms such as attentional dys-
gested that treatment with levodopa improves function, fluctuation in alertness, and visual
some of the cognitive deficits of PDD, clinical hallucinations, and may be an early marker of
evidence suggests that levodopa treatment does pathology.
not improve cognition suggesting that the dopa-
minergic deficit is not the main neurochemical
Alzheimer’s Disease Pathology
impairment responsible for dementia in PDD
(Emre, 2003). Several investigators have reported that neu-
ropathological changes associated with AD,
namely senile plaques and neurofibrillary
Monoaminergic Deficit
tangles, are commonly present in the brains of
Involvement of ascending noradrenergic and demented PD patients. Concurrent incipient
serotoninergic pathways has also been sug- AD with fully developed PD has been suggested
gested as the cause of cognitive impairment in as a cause of impaired cognition in some cases
PD. Research has produced mixed findings with of PDD. Presence of moderate to severe demen-
some studies reporting more severe neuronal tia in PD patients has been found to correlate
210 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

highly with AD pathology, including the corti- in the temporal cortex (Harding & Halliday,
cal neuropathological changes of AD (Jellinger 2001). Increased distribution of Lewy bodies
et al., 2002). Recent studies suggest that AD type in the temporal lobe has been associated with
changes are highly specific to PDD, but lack sen- well-formed visual hallucinations in all patients
sitivity. Although Lewy bodies have the greatest (Harding et al., 2002).
correlation with dementia, ß-amyloid load in
the brain is thought to be greater in DBL as com-
Treatment of Parkinson’s Disease
pared with PDD. Lippa and colleagues (2007)
Dementias
have suggested that the Pittsburgh Compound
B (PIB)—PET is potentially valuable for deter- Treatment of PDD includes pharmacological
mining ß-amyloid load (Klunk et al., 2004) and treatments for the motor, neuropsychiatric, and
may aid in determining ß-amyloid burden asso- cognitive symptoms. Levodopa has been used
ciated with PDD and DLB (Lippa et al., 2007). for the motor disorder of both DLB and PDD.
Studies using glucose PET or single pho- The use of anticholinergic medications should be
ton emission computed tomography (SPECT) avoided. Improvement in visual hallucinations,
have been used to assess functional changes in delusions, anxiety, behavioral disturbance, and
PD with dementia and compare PDD to AD. cognition can be achieved with the use of cho-
Occipital hypoperfusion was closely correlated linesterase inhibitors (CHEIs; McKeith et al.,
with PDD in one study as compared with fron- 2005). Although open label studies have gener-
tal, parietal and temporal perfusion (Matsui ally demonstrated effectiveness of the available
et al., 2005), while measurement of medial CHEIs, donepezil, rivastigmine, and galan-
occipital regional cerebral blood flow discrim- tamine in PDD and DLB, placebo controlled
inated DLB from AD in another study (Shimizu data from a large trial are available only for
et al., 2005) suggesting that occipital hypoper- rivastigmine (Emre et al., 2004). Rivastigmine
fusion may be a useful tool in differentiating produced improvement on measures of cogni-
PDD and DLB from AD. Other studies have tion, function, and behavior. Apathy is common
shown globally reduced metabolic activity or a in PDD and may improve with CHEIs. Atypical
disproportionate involvement of the temporo- antipsychotics may be used but only very cau-
parieto-occipital junction regions (Kawabata tiously due to severe neuroleptic sensitivity and
et al., 1991; Spampinato et al., 1991). Frontal the use of traditional antipsychotics should be
and parietal hypoperfusion have been reported avoided (McKeith et al., 2005). As with PD,
in SPECT studies of demented PDD patients depression is common in both PDD and DLB
(Bissessur et al., 1997; Sawada et al., 1992). and can be treated with non-anticholinergic
antidepressants such as the selective seroto-
nergic reuptake inhibitors (SSRI) or serotonin–
Lewy Body Pathology
norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors (SNRI).
Diff use cortical Lewy body pathology is
commonly found in patients with PDD and cor-
relates with the severity of dementia (Aarsland
Differential Diagnosis of Parkinsons
et al., 2005; Hurtig et al., 2000; Mattila et al.,
Disease and Parkinsonian Syndromes
2000). Lewy body densities in temporal neocor-
tex have been reported to correlate with cogni-
Alzheimer’s Disease
tive impairment in patients with PD pathology
and studies using α-synuclein antibodies as a The cognitive deficits associated with AD are
marker of Lewy bodies suggested that fron- discussed in the chapter by Bondi et al. As
tal cortical Lewy bodies were associated with described above, the cognitive changes in PD
greater cognitive decline independent of AD are distinct from AD with the onset of defi-
pathology (Hurtig et al., 2000; Mattila et al., cits in attention, executive abilities, and spa-
2000). Cortical density of Lewy bodies did not tial skills as hallmarks (See Table 9–2). Several
distinguish PDD from DLB, although PDD neuropsychological differences exist between
patients had higher densities of Lewy bodies patients with PD and patients with AD. Simple
Neuropsychological Aspects of Parkinson’s Disease and Parkinsonism 211

Table 9–2. Clinical Distinction between Parkinson’s Disease with Dementia (PDD),
and Alzheimer’s Disease (AD)
PDD AD
Neuropsychological Features
Attention
Mental Speed Impaired Intact/slight declines
Simple attention Fluctuations Intact
Language skills
Naming Intact Impaired
Phonemic Fluency Impaired Intact
Semantic Fluency Impaired Impaired
Memory Retrieval deficit Storage deficit
Visuospatial Prominent deficits Mild to moderate deficits
Executive Functions Prominent deficits Mild to moderate deficits
Neuropsychiatric Features
PD motor features Present early Absent early
Neuroleptic sensitivity Present Uncommon
Autonomic dysfunction Common Uncommon
disorder Hallucinations/Delusions Common
Uncommon
REM sleep behavior
Common Absent

attentional skills are intact in AD while patients is the most prominent deficit. (See Table 9–2 for
with PD may show fluctuations in the level of neurocognitive and behavioral comparisons).
alertness. In AD, certain language skills are ini-
tially intact (e.g., phonemic fluency, reading) Parkinsonian Plus Syndromes. There are sev-
with more pervasive early deficits in naming, eral hypokinetic neurodegenerative disorders
word-finding ability and language compre- that share clinical similarities with PD. However,
hension. In PD, language changes include dys- patients with these disorders typically exhibit
arthic speech and difficulty with both semantic additional neurological abnormalities and thus
and phonemic fluency. Memory is impaired in these syndromes are referred to as multiple sys-
patients with both PD and AD, although the tem atrophies or parkinson-plus syndromes.
impairment in AD is more pronounced, with
AD patients performing much more poorly on
Progressive Supranuclear Palsy
tasks of delayed recall and recognition and PD
patients performing better on measures of rec- Progressive supranuclear palsy is an uncommon
ognition or retrieval. Visuospatial deficits are disease with a prevalence rate of 5 per 100,000
exhibited by both PD and AD patients, even (Nath et al., 2001). Age at onset is typically in
when tasks do not involve motor speed or man- the sixth or seventh decade and progresses to
ual dexterity (Levin et al., 1992). In addition, death in 5–10 years. The disease is sporadic and
PD patients tend to exhibit visuospatial deficits occurs more commonly in males than females.
earlier in the course of the disease as compared Patients with PSP tend to have an astonished or
to AD. PD and AD patients alike exhibit poor worried facial expression secondary to rigidity
performance on tasks of cognitive flexibility. and hypertonicity of the facial muscles.
However, in PD poor performance frontal tests Postural instability with frequent falls, usu-
such as those involving planning, sequencing ally backward, is among the most disabling fea-
multiple steps, and set shifting is often the sole tures. Loss of volitional downward gaze is one
or most striking deficit, as compared with AD, of the most important distinguishing features
where memory (learning, accelerated forgetting) of PSP and is one of the earliest manifestations
212 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

of the disease. The patient’s eyes deviate upward Multiple System Atrophy. Multiple system
when the head is tilted forward indicating that atrophy refers to a group of sporadic neurode-
oculocephalic reflexes remain intact. Upward generative disorders of adult onset that share the
gaze is progressively impaired and volitional common feature of parkinsonism. The disease
horizontal gaze is also lost as the disease is characterized by a combination of parkinso-
progresses. Clinicopathological studies have nian, cerebellar, autonomic, and cortical spi-
reported early postural instability with falls nal (pyramidal) signs and symptoms with cell
and supranuclear gaze palsy as the most reliable loss and gliosis extensively in the basal ganglia,
means of differentiating PSP from other par- inferior olives, pons, and cerebellum. On neu-
kinsonian disorders (Collins et al., 1995; Litvan ropathological examination, glial cytoplasmic
et al., 1997). In contrast to the stooped posture, inclusions consisting primarily of α-synuclein
short and shuffling steps, narrow base, and have been described, suggesting that the disease
flexed knees seen in patients with PD, patients belongs to the group of α-synucleinopathies
with PSP assume an erect or hypererect posture (Dickson et al., 1999). Estimates of the preva-
and have a stiff and broad-based gait (Mendez lence of MSA vary from 1.9 to 4.9 per 100,000
& Cummings, 2003). Patients with PSP develop (Vanacore, 2005). Approximately 90% of
profound bradykinesia during the course of the patients with MSA have REM sleep behavior
illness that resembles PD. Rigidity in the PSP disorder, which has been correlated with nigros-
patient is more evident in the midline struc- triatal dopaminergic demise (Gilman et al.,
tures such as the neck and trunk, as opposed to 2003). Six of the following features have been
the limbs (e.g., axial rigidity). Both resting and noted to reliably diagnose MSA: sporadic adult
action tremor are unusual. Whereas patients onset, autonomic signs, parkinsonism, cerebel-
with PD often exhibit substantial improvement lar features, pyramidal signs, lack of response to
when treated with levodopa, patients with PSP levodopa treatment, lack of cognitive dysfunc-
show little response. Speech in PSP is character- tion, and lack of downward gaze palsy (Litvan
ized by a monotonous, hypernasal, low-pitched, et al., 1998).
spastic dysarthria (Stacy & Jankovic, 1992), and Relatively recent consensus criteria (Gilman
patients may progress to total anarthria or mut- et al., 1999) have been developed and have pro-
ism in late stages (Mendez & Cummings, 2003). posed new terminology to replace the terms
Changes in personality, including apathy, irri- striatonigral degeneration (SND), sporadic
tability, and labile mood may be among the first olivopontocerebellar atrophy (sOPCA), and Shy
features of PSP. Drager syndrome (SDS), previously considered
Neuropathologic changes associated with distinct diagnostics subtypes of MSA. The des-
the disease include brain stem atrophy with ignation MSA-P has been delineated for those
neuronal loss, neurofibrillary tangles and glial patients with the onset of parkinsonism; MSA-C
inclusions in the brain stem, basal ganglia and is used when MSA begins with cerebellar ataxia,
diencephalon (Litvan et al., 1996). The neuro- and MSA-A is used for patients with autonomic
fibrillary tangles of PSP are composed of four disturbances.
repeat tau and have a different distribution from
those of AD (Avila et al., 2004). Performance Multiple System Atrophy-Parkinsonism.
on measures of information processing speed, Multiple system atrophy-parkinsonism
abstract reasoning, executive functions, (MSA-P) is the most common of the MSA
retrieval and procedural learning, category flu- subtypes accounting for 40%–66% of cases in
ency and naming have been noted to be worse various studies (Gilman, 2006). This term has
in PSP than PD (Cordato et al., 2006; Soliveri been used to replace the old term striatonigral
et al., 2000). Patients are three times more likely degeneration. The parkinsonian features of
to develop dementia than patients with idio- bradykinesia, rigidity, gait unsteadiness, and
pathic PD. Use of cholinesterase inhibitors has hypokinetic speech are hallmarks of MSA-P.
not proven efficacious for treatment of cognitive Improvement in rigidity, bradykinesia, and
dysfunction (Lauterbach, 2004). postural instability with use of levodopa has
Neuropsychological Aspects of Parkinson’s Disease and Parkinsonism 213

been noted in only half of the cases with MSA-P reclassified as a form of spinocerebellar ataxia
(Hughes et al., 1992; Parati et al., 1993), and a (SCA) The disease is slowly progressive and may
lack of reponsiveness to levodopa has been used have a 25-year course.
to differentiate PD from striatonigral degener- Relatively mild cognitive deficits have
ation. A significant number of patients show been reported in autosomal dominant OPCA
iron deposition appearing as low T2 signal in (dOPCA), particularly frontal lobe dysfunction
the putamen (Macia et al., 2001). Diagnosis (Botez-Marquard & Botez, 1993). Berent and
is confirmed only upon postmortem exami- colleagues (2002) have suggested that motor
nation, which is significant for neuronal loss dysfunction appears to account for discrepan-
in the putamen and caudate nucleus (O’Brien cies on cognitive tasks between OPCA patients
et al., 1990). Lewy bodies and neurofibrillary and normal controls. While subjects with OPCA
tangles are not common. did not differ from normal controls on measures
Neuropsychological studies have suggested without motor demands such as working math-
mild memory and executive impairments ematical problems without paper and pencil,
(Pillon et al., 1995) and significant impair- defining a list or words or identifying the essen-
ments of visuospatial organization in MSA-P tial missing piece in a picture, subjects did dif-
(Testa et al., 1993). Patients with MSA-P per- fer from normal controls on three subtests with
formed more poorly than PD patients on motor demands such as constructing blocks
frontal attention measures (Meco et al., 1996) to match design and quickly copying symbols
and on measures of verbal fluency and visual paired with numbers. At least one recent study
search (Soliveri et al., 2000). Patients with (Bürk et al., 2006) suggested that MSA-C sub-
MSA-P exhibited greater difficulties on tasks jects performed more poorly on measures of
of learning, recognition, and verbal fluency as verbal memory (story recall and list learning)
compared to controls and patients with both and category and letter fluency as compared
sporadic and autosomal dominant form of with normal controls.
OPCA (Berent et al., 2002).
Multiple System Atrophy-Autonomic.
Multiple System Atrophy-Cerebellar. The term MSA-A refers to those cases in
The cerebellar disorder of MSA-C includes which the disease presents with parkinson-
ataxia of gait, limb movements and speech, and ism and prominent autonomic disturbance.
ocular motor disorders including gaze-evoked Abnormalities of function of the autonomic ner-
nystagmus, overshoot dysmetria, and cycadic vous system including impotence, orthostatic
intrusions into smooth pursuit movements. hypotension, and urinary incontinence appear
Patients often complain of imbalance, unstead- early in the clinical course. Other symptoms
iness of gait, and deterioration of handwriting may include anhidrosis, pupillary changes, and
and other fine motor skills. Despite recommen- decreased tearing. An akinetic, rigid parkin-
dations by the consensus conference on MSA, sonian syndrome develops several years after
the term olivopontocerebellar atrophy (OPCA) the onset of the autonomic dysfunction. Some
continues to be used in the literature and is patients may present with associated cerebel-
applied to disorders characterized clinically by lar, upper motor neuron, and/or lower motor
ataxic gait, hypotonia, upper and lower motor neuron findings. The disease is more common
neuron signs, limb unsteadiness, and dysarthria. in men and symptoms first begin in the sixth
MSA-C accounts for approximately 10%–15% of decade of life. Pathological changes include cell
cases of MSA. The presence of ophthalmoplegia loss and gliosis in the pigmented brain stem
and pyramidal tract signs is used clinically to nuclei, striatum, pontine structures, and the
distinguish OPCA from pure cerebellar atrophy. intermediolateral cell column of the spinal cord
Onset of OPCA is usually between ages 30 and (Gilman, 2006). Autonomic failure has been
50 years and may be sporadic or inherited in an correlated with loss of catecholaminergic neu-
autosomal dominant pattern (dOPCA), with the rons in the reticular formation of the rostroven-
autosomal dominant form of the disease now trolateral medulla (Benarroch et al., 2000).
214 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Summary of the intellectual disturbances documented in


PD and other basal ganglia syndromes can be
Parkinson’s Disease is among the most common ascribed to interruption of frontal–subcortical
neurological illnesses affecting the elderly. It is circuit function. The high frequency of cogni-
characterized by motor symptoms including tive deficits, mood changes, anxiety, and per-
slowness of movement and difficulty initiating sonality changes among patients with PD and
movement, masked facies, muscular rigidity, other movement disorders indicate that the
shuffling or unsteady gait, stooped posture, basal ganglia play critical roles in human intel-
disturbances in equilibrium, and tremor. The lectual and emotional function.
use of levodopa continues to be the mainstay
of treatment for PD. Other pharmacological
therapies include the use of selegiline, rasage- Acknowledgment
line, and entacapone. Stimulation of the STN is Supported in part by the Department of Veterans
considered an effective treatment for advanced Affairs and National Institute on Aging grants
PD with noted benefits on motor function and 1P30 AG10123, AG11325–02, P50 AGi6570.
quality of life.
Neuropsychologic deficits in non-demented
PD patients include moderate impairment in References
verbal and nonverbal memory for both imme- Aarsland, D., Laake, K., Larsen, J. P., & Janvin, C.
diate and delayed recall, with recent controversy (2002). Donepezil for cognitive impairment in
regarding performance on tasks of recogni- Parkinson’s disease: A randomised controlled
tion. Language functions are generally spared. study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and
Visuospatial and visuoconstructive skills are Psychiatry, 72(6), 708–712.
among the most frequently cited deficits in Aarsland, D., Larsen, J. P., Lim, N. G., Janvin, C.,
Karlsen, K., Tandberg, E., et al. (1999). Range
PD, even when the effects of motor slowing are
of neuropsychiatric disturbances in patients
taken into account. Executive functions are the with Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neurology,
earliest and most consistently impaired abili- Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 67(4), 492–496.
ties. Depression is the most common psychi- Aarsland, D., Perry, R., Brown, A., Larsen, J. P., &
atric syndrome in PD; anxiety and apathy are Ballard, C. (2005). Neuropathology of dementia in
also frequently observed. The dementia associ- Parkinson’s disease: A prospective, community-
ated with PD is considered to be one of the Lewy based study. Annals of Neurology, 58(5), 773–776.
body diseases and shares pathological features Aarsland, D., Tandberg, E., Larsen, J. P., & Cummings,
of misfolding and aggregation of α-synuclein J. L. (1996). Frequency of dementia in Parkinson
with Lewy body dementia suggesting that the disease. Archives of Neurology, 53(6), 538–542.
two diseases are similar. Rivastigmine has been Adler, C. H., & Thorpy, M. J. (2005). Sleep issues in
Parkinson’s disease. Neurology, 64(12 Suppl 3),
approved for treatment of the dementias asso-
S12–S20.
ciated with PD. Neurodegenerative disorders Albin, R. L. (2006). Parkinson’s disease: Background,
that share clinical similarities with PD include diagnosis, and initial management. Clinics in
DLB, PSP, and the various subtypes of MSA. Geriatric Medicine, 22(4), 735–751, v.
Disorders that affect the head of the caudate Alegret, M., Junque, C., Valldeoriola, F., Vendrell, P.,
nucleus or thalamus produce the syndrome of Pilleri, M., Rumia, J., et al. (2001). Effects of bilat-
subcortical dementia with executive function eral subthalamic stimulation on cognitive func-
abnormalities, memory loss, slowed cognition, tion in Parkinson disease. Archives of Neurology,
and mood and personality changes. The cau- 58(8), 1223–1227.
date nucleus, globus pallidus, and thalamus Alegret, M., Valldeoriola, F., Tolosa, E., Vendrell, P.,
are member structures of frontal–subcortical Junque, C., Martinez, J., et al. (2003). Cognitive
effects of unilateral posteroventral pallidotomy:
circuits that link these subcortical nuclei with
A 4-year follow-up study. Movement Disorders,
specific regions of the frontal lobe. Disruption 18(3), 323–328.
of the circuits results in behavioral abnormal- Ardouin, C., Pillon, B., Peiffer, E., Bejjani, P.,
ities and cognitive deficits similar to those Limousin, P., Damier, P., et al. (1999). Bilateral sub-
occurring with frontal lobe dysfunction. Many thalamic or pallidal stimulation for Parkinson’s
Neuropsychological Aspects of Parkinson’s Disease and Parkinsonism 215

disease affects neither memory nor executive func- Bonifati, V., Rizzu, P., Squitieri, F., Krieger, E.,
tions: A consecutive series of 62 patients. Annals of Vanacore, N., van Swieten, J. C., et al. (2003).
Neurology, 46(2), 217–223. DJ-1 (PARK7), a novel gene for autosomal reces-
Ardouin, C., Voon, V., Worbe, Y., Abouazar, sive, early onset parkinsonism. Journal of the
N., Czernecki, V., Hosseini, H., et al. (2006). Neurological Sciences, 24(3), 159–160.
Pathological gambling in Parkinson’s dis- Botez-Marquard, T., & Botez, M. I. (1993). Cognitive
ease improves on chronic subthalamic nucleus behavior in heredodegenerative ataxias. European
stimulation. Movement Disorders, 21(11), Neurology, 33(5), 351–357.
1941–1946. Braak, H., Del Tredici, K., Rub, U., de Vos, R. A.,
Avila, J., Lucas, J. J., Perez, M., & Hernandez, F. Jansen Steur, E. N., & Braak, E. (2003). Staging of
(2004). Role of tau protein in both physiologi- brain pathology related to sporadic Parkinson’s
cal and pathological conditions. Physiological disease. Neurobiology of Aging, 24(2), 197–211.
Reviews, 84(2), 361–384. Breen, E. K. (1993). Recall and recognition
Baldereschi, M., Di Carlo, A., Rocca, W. A., Vanni, memory in Parkinson’s disease. Cortex, 29(1),
P., Maggi, S., Perissinotto, E., et al. (2000). 91–102.
Parkinson’s disease and parkinsonism in a longi- Brooks, D. J., & Sagar, H. (2003). Entacapone is ben-
tudinal study: Two-fold higher incidence in men. eficial in both fluctuating and non-fluctuating
ILSA Working Group. Italian Longitudinal Study patients with Parkinson’s disease: A randomised,
on Aging. Neurology, 55(9), 1358–1363. placebo controlled, double blind, six month study.
Ballard, C. G., Aarsland, D., McKeith, I., O’Brien, J., Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry,
Gray, A., Cormack, F., et al. (2002). Fluctuations 74(8), 1071–1079.
in attention: PD dementia vs DLB with parkinson- Brown, R. G., & Marsden, C. D. (1990). Cognitive
ism. Neurology, 59(11), 1714–1720. function in Parkinson’s disease: From description
Barnes, J., Boubert, L., Harris, J., Lee, A., & David, A. to theory. Trends in Neuroscience, 13(1), 21–29.
S. (2003). Reality monitoring and visual halluci- Burchiel, K. J. (1995). Thalamotomy for movement
nations in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia, disorders. Neurosurgery Clinics of North America,
41(5), 565–574. 6(1), 55–71.
Bayles, K. A., Trosset, M. W., Tomoeda, C. K., Burk, K., Daum, I., & Rub, U. (2006). Cognitive func-
Montgomery, E. B., Jr., & Wilson, J. (1993). tion in multiple system atrophy of the cerebellar
Generative naming in Parkinson disease type. Movement Disorders, 21(6), 772–776.
patients. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Cahn, D. A., Sullivan, E. V., Shear, P. K., Heit, G., Lim,
Neuropsychology, 15(4), 547–562. K. O., Marsh, L., et al. (1998). Neuropsychological
Benarroch, E. E., Schmeichel, A. M., & Parisi, J. E. and motor functioning after unilateral ana-
(2000). Involvement of the ventrolateral medulla in tomically guided posterior ventral pallidotomy.
parkinsonism with autonomic failure. Neurology, Preoperative performance and three-month fol-
54(4), 963–968. low-up. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and
Berent, S., Giordani, B., Gilman, S., Trask, C. L., Behavioral Neurology, 11(3), 136–145.
Little, R. J., Johanns, J. R., et al. (2002). Patterns of Calderon, J., Perry, R. J., Erzinclioglu, S. W., Berrios,
neuropsychological performance in multiple sys- G. E., Dening, T. R., & Hodges, J. R. (2001).
tem atrophy compared to sporadic and hereditary Perception, attention, and working memory are
olivopontocerebellar atrophy. Brain Cognition, disproportionately impaired in dementia with
50(2), 194–206. Lewy bodies compared with Alzheimer’s disease.
Bissessur, S., Tissingh, G., Wolters, E. C., & Scheltens, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry,
P. (1997). rCBF SPECT in Parkinson’s disease 70(2), 157–164.
patients with mental dysfunction. Journal of Claus, J. J., & Mohr, E. (1996). Attentional deficits in
Neural Transmission Supplement, 50, 25–30. Alzheimer’s, Parkinson’s, and Huntington’s diseases.
Bohnen, N. I., Kaufer, D. I., Ivanco, L. S., Lopresti, B., Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 93(5), 346–351.
Koeppe, R. A., Davis, J. G., et al. (2003). Cortical Collerton, D., Burn, D., McKeith, I., & O’Brien,
cholinergic function is more severely affected in J. (2003). Systematic review and meta-analysis
parkinsonian dementia than in Alzheimer dis- show that dementia with Lewy bodies is a visual–
ease: An in vivo positron emission tomographic perceptual and attentional–executive dementia.
study. Archives of Neurology, 60(12), 1745–1748. Dementia and Geriatric Cognivite Disorders, 16(4),
Bondi, M. W., Kaszniak, A. W., Rapcsak, S. Z., & 229–237.
Butters, M. A. (1993). Implicit and explicit memory Collins, S. J., Ahlskog, J. E., Parisi, J. E., & Maraganore,
following anterior communicating artery aneu- D. M. (1995). Progressive supranuclear palsy:
rysm rupture. Brain Cognition, 22(2), 213–229. Neuropathologically based diagnostic clinical
216 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

criteria. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Defer, G. L., Widner, H., Marie, R. M., Remy, P., &
Psychiatry, 58(2), 167–173. Levivier, M. (1999). Core assessment program for
Connor, D. J., Salmon, D. P., Sandy, T. J., Galasko, surgical interventional therapies in Parkinson’s
D., Hansen, L. A., & Thal, L. J. (1998). Cognitive disease (CAPSIT-PD). Movement Disorders, 14(4),
profi les of autopsy-confirmed Lewy body variant 572–584.
vs pure Alzheimer disease. Archives of Neurology, Delis, D. C., & Massman, P. J. (1992). The effects of
55(7), 994–1000. dopamine fluctuation on cognition and affect. In
Cools, R., Barker, R. A., Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins, T. S. J. Huber & J. L. Cummings (Eds.), Parkinson’s
W. (2001a). Enhanced or impaired cognitive func- disease: Neurobehavioral aspects. New York:
tion in Parkinson’s disease as a function of dopa- Oxford. <AQ: Please provide page range.>
minergic medication and task demands. Cerebral Demakis, G. J., Mercury, M. G., Sweet, J. J., Rezak,
Cortex, 11(12), 1136–1143. M., Eller, T., & Vergenz, S. (2002). Motor and
Cools, R., Barker, R. A., Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins, cognitive sequelae of unilateral pallidotomy in
T. W. (2001b). Mechanisms of cognitive set flex- intractable Parkinson’s Disease: Electronic mea-
ibility in Parkinson’s disease. Brain, 124(Pt 12), surement of motor steadiness is a useful outcome
2503–2512. measure. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Cools, R., Barker, R. A., Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins, T. Neuropsychology, 24(5), 655–663.
W. (2003). L-Dopa medication remediates cognitive Diamond, A., & Jankovic, J. (2005). The effect of deep
inflexibility, but increases impulsivity in patients brain stimulation on quality of life in movement
with Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia, disorders. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and
41(11), 1431–1441. Psychiatry, 76(9), 1188–1193.
Cooper, J. A., Sagar, H. J., Jordan, N., Harvey, N. S., & Dickson, D. W., Liu, W., Hardy, J., Farrer, M., Mehta,
Sullivan, E. V. (1991). Cognitive impairment in early, N., Uitti, R., et al. (1999). Widespread alterations
untreated Parkinson’s disease and its relationship of alpha-synuclein in multiple system atrophy.
to motor disability. Brain, 114(Pt 5), 2095–2122. American Journal of Pathology, 155(4), 1241–1251.
Cooper, J. A., Sagar, H. J., & Sullivan, E. V. (1993). Dodd, M. L., Klos, K. J., Bower, J. H., Geda, Y. E.,
Short-term memory and temporal ordering Josephs, K. A., & Ahlskog, J. E. (2005). Pathological
in early Parkinson’s disease: Effects of disease gambling caused by drugs used to treat Parkinson
chronicity and medication. Neuropsychologia, disease. Archives of Neurology, 62(9), 1377–1381.
31(9), 933–949. Dorsey, E. R., Constantinescu, R., Thompson, J.
Cordato, N. J., Halliday, G. M., Caine, D., & Morris, P., Biglan, K. M., Holloway, R. G., Kieburtz, K.,
J. G. (2006). Comparison of motor, cognitive, et al. (2007). Projected number of people with
and behavioral features in progressive supra- Parkinson disease in the most populous nations,
nuclear palsy and Parkinson’s disease. Movement 2005 through 2030. Neurology, 68(5), 384–386.
Disorders, 21(5), 632–638. Driver-Dunckley, E., Samanta, J., & Stacy, M. (2003).
Cummings, J. L. (1991). Behavioral complications of Pathological gambling associated with dopamine
drug treatment of Parkinson’s disease. Journal of agonist therapy in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology,
the American Geriatrics Society, 39(7), 708–716. 61(3), 422–423.
Cummings, J. L. (1992). Depression and Parkinson’s Dujardin, K., Defebvre, L., Krystkowiak, P., Blond,
disease: A review. American Journal of Psychiatry, S., & Destee, A. (2001). Influence of chronic bilat-
149(4), 443–454. eral stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus on
Daniele, A., Albanese, A., Contarino, M. F., cognitive function in Parkinson’s disease. Journal
Zinzi, P., Barbier, A., Gasparini, F., et al. (2003). of Neurology, 248(7), 603–611.
Cognitive and behavioural effects of chronic stim- Dujardin, K., Degreef, J., Rogelet, P., Defebvre, L.,
ulation of the subthalamic nucleus in patients & Destee, A. (1999). Impairment of the supervi-
with Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neurology, sory attentional system in early untreated patients
Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 74(2), 175–182. withParkinson’s disease. Journal of Neurology,
de Bie, R. M., Schuurman, P. R., Bosch, D. A., de 246, 783–788.
Haan, R. J., Schmand, B., & Speelman, J. D. (2001). Elmer, L., Schwid, S., Eberly, S., Goetz, C., Fahn, S.,
Outcome of unilateral pallidotomy in advanced Kieburtz, K., et al. (2006). Rasagiline-associated
Parkinson’s disease: Cohort study of 32 patients. motor improvement in PD occurs without worsen-
Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, ing of cognitive and behavioral symptoms. Journal
71(3), 375–382. of the Neurological Sciences, 248(1–2), 78–83.
de Lau, L. M., & Breteler, M. M. (2006). Epidemiology Emre, M. (2003). Dementia associated with
of Parkinson’s disease. The Lancet Neurology, 5(6), Parkinson’s disease. The Lancet Neurology, 2(4),
525–535. 229–237.
Neuropsychological Aspects of Parkinson’s Disease and Parkinsonism 217

Emre, M., Aarsland, D., Albanese, A., Byrne, E. dysregulation in patients with Parkinson’s dis-
J., Deuschl, G., De Deyn, P. P., et al. (2004). ease on dopamine replacement therapies. Journal
Rivastigmine for dementia associated with of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 68(4),
Parkinson’s disease. New England Journal of 423–428.
Medicine, 351(24), 2509–2518. Grossman, M. (1999). Sentence processing in
Factor, S. A., Molho, E. S., Podskalny, G. D., & Parkinson’s disease. Brain Cognition, 40(2),
Brown, D. (1995). Parkinson’s disease: Drug- 387–413.
induced psychiatric states. Advances in Neurology, Grossman, M., Carvell, S., Stern, M. B., Gollomp, S.,
65, 115–138. & Hurtig, H. I. (1992). Sentence comprehension
Fearnley, J. M., & Lees, A. J. (1991). Ageing and in Parkinson’s disease: The role of attention and
Parkinson’s disease: Substantia nigra regional memory. Brain and Language, 42(4), 347–384.
selectivity. Brain, 114(Pt 5), 2283–2301. Grossman, M., Cooke, A., DeVita, C., Lee, C., Alsop,
Ferreri, F., Agbokou, C., & Gauthier, S. (2006). D., Detre, J., et al. (2003). Grammatical and resource
Recognition and management of neuropsychiatric components of sentence processing in Parkinson’s
complications in Parkinson’s disease. Canadian disease: An fMRI study. Neurology, 60(5), 775–781.
Medical Association Journal, 175(12), 1545–1552. Guridi, J., & Lozano, A. M. (1997). A brief history of
Fukuda, M., Kameyama, S., Yoshino, M., Tanaka, pallidotomy. Neurosurgery, 41(5), 1169–1180.
R., & Narabayashi, H. (2000). Neuropsychological Hallett, M., & Litvan, I. (1999). Evaluation of surgery
outcome following pallidotomy and thalamotomy for Parkinson’s disease: A report of the Therapeutics
for Parkinson’s disease. Stereotactic and Functional and Technology Assessment Subcommittee of the
Neurosurgery, 74(1), 11–20. American Academy of Neurology. The Task Force
Funkiewiez, A., Ardouin, C., Caputo, E., Krack, on Surgery for Parkinson’s Disease. Neurology,
P., Fraix, V., Klinger, H., et al. (2004). Long term 53(9), 1910–1921.
effects of bilateral subthalamic nucleus stimu- Harding, A. J., Broe, G. A., & Halliday, G. M. (2002).
lation on cognitive function, mood, and behav- Visual hallucinations in Lewy body disease relate
iour in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neurology, to Lewy bodies in the temporal lobe. Brain, 125(Pt
Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 75(6), 834–839. 2), 391–403.
Geser, F., Wenning, G. K., Poewe, W., & McKeith, I. Harding, A. J., & Halliday, G. M. (2001). Cortical
(2005). How to diagnose dementia with Lewy bod- Lewy body pathology in the diagnosis of dementia.
ies: State of the art. Movement Disorders, 20(Suppl Acta Neuropathologica (Berlin), 102(4), 355–363.
12), S11–S20. Hardy, J., Cai, H., Cookson, M. R., Gwinn-Hardy,
Ghika, J., Ghika-Schmid, F., Fankhauser, H., Assal, K., & Singleton, A. (2006). Genetics of Parkinson’s
G., Vingerhoets, F., Albanese, A., et al. (1999). disease and parkinsonism. Annals of Neurology,
Bilateral contemporaneous posteroventral palli- 60(4), 389–398.
dotomy for the treatment of Parkinson’s disease: Henderson, R., Kurlan, R., Kersun, J. M., & Como,
Neuropsychological and neurological side effects. P. (1992). Preliminary examination of the comor-
Report of four cases and review of the literature. bidity of anxiety and depression in Parkinson’s
Journal of Neurosurgery, 91(2), 313–321. disease. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical
Giladi, N., Shabtai, H., Gurevich, T., Benbunan, B., Neurosciences, 4(3), 257–264.
Anca, M., & Korczyn, A. D. (2003). Rivastigmine Hirtz, D., Thurman, D. J., Gwinn-Hardy, K.,
(Exelon) for dementia in patients with Parkinson’s Mohamed, M., Chaudhuri, A. R., & Zalutsky, R.
disease. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 108(5), (2007). How common are the “common” neuro-
368–373. logic disorders? Neurology, 68(5), 326–337.
Gilman, S. (2006). Parkinsonian syndromes. Clinics Hodgson, T. L., Tiesman, B., Owen, A. M., &
in Geriatric Medicine, 22(4), 827–842, vi. Kennard, C. (2002). Abnormal gaze strategies
Gilman, S., Koeppe, R. A., Chervin, R. D., Consens, during problem solving in Parkinson’s disease.
F. B., Little, R., An, H., et al. (2003). REM sleep Neuropsychologia, 40(4), 411–422.
behavior disorder is related to striatal monoamin- Hozumi, A., Hirata, K., Tanaka, H., & Yamazaki,
ergic deficit in MSA. Neurology, 61(1), 29–34. K. (2000). Perseveration for novel stimuli in
Gilman, S., Low, P. A., Quinn, N., Albanese, A., Ben- Parkinson’s disease: An evaluation based on
Shlomo, Y., Fowler, C. J., et al. (1999). Consensus event-related potentials topography. Movement
statement on the diagnosis of multiple system Disorders, 15(5), 835–842.
atrophy. Joural of Neurological Sciences, 163(1), Hughes, A. J., Colosimo, C., Kleedorfer, B., Daniel, S.
94–98. E., & Lees, A. J. (1992). The dopaminergic response
Giovannoni, G., O’Sullivan, J. D., Turner, K., Manson, in multiple system atrophy. Journal of Neurology,
A. J., & Lees, A. J. (2000). Hedonistic homeostatic Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 55(11), 1009–1013.
218 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Hurtig, H. I., Trojanowski, J. Q., Galvin, J., Ewbank, pallidotomy in intractable Parkinson’s disease.
D., Schmidt, M. L., Lee, V. M., et al. (2000). Brain Cognition, 42(3), 364–378.
Alpha-synuclein cortical Lewy bodies correlate Lai, B. C., Schulzer, M., Marion, S., Teschke, K., &
with dementia in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology, Tsui, J. K. (2003). The prevalence of Parkinson’s
54(10), 1916–1921. disease in British Columbia, Canada, estimated by
Ivory, S. J., Knight, R. G., Longmore, B. E., & using drug tracer methodology. Parkinsonism and
Caradoc-Davies, T. (1999). Verbal memory in non- Related Disorders, 9(4), 233–238.
demented patients with idiopathic Parkinson’s Lauterbach, E. C. (2004). The neuropsychiatry
disease. Neuropsychologia, 37(7), 817–828. of Parkinson’s disease and related disorders.
Jahanshahi, M., Rowe, J., Saleem, T., Brown, R. G., Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 27(4),
Limousin-Dowsey, P., Rothwell, J. C., et al. (2002). 801–825.
Striatal contribution to cognition: Working mem- Lauterbach, E. C., & Duvoisin, R. C. (1991). Anxiety
ory and executive function in Parkinson’s disease disorders in familial parkinsonism. American
before and after unilateral posteroventral pallido- Journal of Psychiatry, 148(2), 274.
tomy. Journal of Cognitive Neurosciences, 14(2), Lawrence, A. D., Evans, A. H., & Lees, A. J. (2003).
298–310. Compulsive use of dopamine replacement therapy
Janvin, C. C., Larsen, J. P., Salmon, D. P., Galasko, in Parkinson’s disease: Reward systems gone awry?
D., Hugdahl, K., & Aarsland, D. (2006). Cognitive The Lancet Neurology, 2(10), 595–604.
profi les of individual patients with Parkinson’s Leentjens, A. F., Lousberg, R., & Verhey, F. R. (2002).
disease and dementia: Comparison with demen- Markers for depression in Parkinson’s disease.
tia with lewy bodies and Alzheimer’s disease. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 106(3), 196–201.
Movement Disorders, 21(3), 337–342. Levin, B. E., Llabre, M. M., Reisman, S., Weiner,
Jellinger, K. A., Seppi, K., Wenning, G. K., & Poewe, W. J., Sanchez-Ramos, J., Singer, C., et al. (1991).
W. (2002). Impact of coexistent Alzheimer pathol- Visuospatial impairment in Parkinson’s disease.
ogy on the natural history of Parkinson’s disease. Neurology, 41(3), 365–369.
Journal of Neural Transmission, 109(3), 329–339. Levin, B. E., Tomer, R., & Rey, G. J. (1992). Cognitive
Jost, W. H. (2003). Autonomic dysfunctions in idio- impairments in Parkinson’s disease. Neurologic
pathic Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neurology, Clinics, 10(2), 471–485.
250(Suppl 1), I28–I30. Limousin, P., Krack, P., Pollak, P., Benazzouz,
Katai, S., Maruyama, T., Hashimoto, T., & Ikeda, A., Ardouin, C., Hoff mann, D., et al. (1998).
S. (2003). Event based and time based prospec- Electrical stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus
tive memory in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of in advanced Parkinson’s disease. New England
Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 74(6), Journal of Medicine, 339(16), 1105–1111.
704–709. Lippa, C. F., Duda, J. E., Grossman, M., Hurtig, H.
Kawabata, K., Tachibana, H., & Sugita, M. (1991). I., Aarsland, D., Boeve, B. F., et al. (2007). DLB
Cerebral blood flow and dementia in Parkinson’s and PDD boundary issues: Diagnosis, treatment,
disease. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and molecular pathology, and biomarkers. Neurology,
Neurology, 4(4), 194–203. 68(11), 812–819.
Kitada, T., Asakawa, S., Hattori, N., Matsumine, Lippa, C. F., Smith, T. W., & Perry, E. (1999).
H., Yamamura, Y., Minoshima, S., et al. (1998). Dementia with Lewy bodies: Choline acetyltrans-
Mutations in the parkin gene cause autoso- ferase parallels nucleus basalis pathology. Journal
mal recessive juvenile parkinsonism. Nature, of Neural Transmission, 106(5–6), 525–535.
392(6676), 605–608. Litvan, I., Booth, V., Wenning, G. K., Bartko, J. J.,
Klunk, W. E., Engler, H., Nordberg, A., Wang, Y., Goetz, C. G., McKee, A., et al. (1998). Retrospective
Blomqvist, G., Holt, D. P., et al. (2004). Imaging application of a set of clinical diagnostic criteria for
brain amyloid in Alzheimer’s disease with the diagnosis of multiple system atrophy. Journal
Pittsburgh Compound-B. Annals of Neurology, of Neural Transmission, 105(2–3), 217–227.
55(3), 306–319. Litvan, I., Campbell, G., Mangone, C. A., Verny, M.,
Kubu, C. S., Grace, G. M., & Parrent, A. G. (2000). McKee, A., Chaudhuri, K. R., et al. (1997). Which
Cognitive outcome following pallidotomy: The clinical features differentiate progressive supra-
influence of side of surgery and age of patient nuclear palsy (Steele–Richardson–Olszewski
at disease onset. Journal of Neurosurgery, 92(3), syndrome) from related disorders? A clinico-
384–389. pathological study. Brain, 120(Pt 1), 65–74.
Lacritz, L. H., Cullum, C. M., Frol, A. B., Dewey, R. Litvan, I., Hauw, J. J., Bartko, J. J., Lantos, P. L.,
B., Jr., & Giller, C. A. (2000). Neuropsychological Daniel, S. E., Horoupian, D. S., et al. (1996).
outcome following unilateral stereotactic Validity and reliability of the preliminary NINDS
Neuropsychological Aspects of Parkinson’s Disease and Parkinsonism 219

neuropathologic criteria for progressive supra- based investigation of Parkinson’s disease with
nuclear palsy and related disorders. Journal of and without dementia. Relationship to age and
Neuropathology and Experimental Neurology, gender. Archives of Neurology, 49(5), 492–497.
55(1), 97–105. Mayeux, R., Marder, K., Cote, L. J., Denaro, J.,
Lombardi, W. J., Gross, R. E., Trepanier, L. L., Lang, Hemenegildo, N., Mejia, H., et al. (1995). The fre-
A. E., Lozano, A. M., & Saint-Cyr, J. A. (2000). quency of idiopathic Parkinson’s disease by age,
Relationship of lesion location to cognitive out- ethnic group, and sex in northern Manhattan,
come following microelectrode-guided palli- 1988–1993. American Journal of Epidemiology,
dotomy for Parkinson’s disease: Support for the 142(8), 820–827.
existence of cognitive circuits in the human pal- McKeith, I., Del Ser, T., Spano, P., Emre, M., Wesnes,
lidum. Brain, 123(Pt 4), 746–758. K., Anand, R., et al. (2000). Efficacy of rivastigmine
Lu, C., Bharmal, A., & Suchowersky, O. (2006). in dementia with Lewy bodies: A randomised,
Gambling and Parkinson disease. Archives of double-blind, placebo-controlled international
Neurology, 63(2), 298. study. Lancet, 356(9247), 2031–2036.
Macia, F., Yekhlef, F., Ballan, G., Delmer, O., & McKeith, I. G., Dickson, D. W., Lowe, J., Emre, M.,
Tison, F. (2001). T2-hyperintense lateral rim and O’Brien, J. T., Feldman, H., et al. (2005). Diagnosis
hypointense putamen are typical but not exclusive and management of dementia with Lewy bodies:
of multiple system atrophy. Archives of Neurology, Third report of the DLB Consortium. Neurology,
58(6), 1024–1026. 65(12), 1863–1872.
Maddox, W. T., & Filoteo, J. V. (2001). Striatal con- Meco, G., Gasparini, M., & Doricchi, F. (1996).
tributions to category learning: Quantitative Attentional functions in multiple system atrophy
modeling of simple linear and complex nonlinear and Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neurology,
rule learning in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 60(4), 393–398.
Journal of the International Neuropsychological Mendez, M. F., & Cummings, J. L. (2003). Dementia:
Society, 7(6), 710–727. A clinical approach, Third Ed. Boston: Butterworth-
Marder, K., Levy, G., Louis, E. D., Mejia-Santana, Heinemann.
H., Cote, L., Andrews, H., et al. (2003). Familial Molina, J. A., Sainz-Artiga, M. J., Fraile, A., Jimenez-
aggregation of early- and late-onset Parkinson’s Jimenez, F. J., Villanueva, C., Orti-Pareja, M.,
disease. Annals of Neurology, 54(4), 507–513. et al. (2000). Pathologic gambling in Parkinson’s
Massman, P. J., Delis, D. C., Butters, N., Levin, B. E., & disease: A behavioral manifestation of pharma-
Salmon, D. P. (1990). Are all subcortical dementias cologic treatment? Movement Disorders, 15(5),
alike? Verbal learning and memory in Parkinson’s 869–872.
and Huntington’s disease patients. Journal of Mormont, E., Laurier-Grymonprez, L., Baisset-
Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 12(5), Mouly, C., & Pasquier, F. (2003). The profi le of
729–744. memory disturbance in early Lewy body demen-
Masterman, D., DeSalles, A., Baloh, R. W., Frysinger, tia differs from that in Alzheimer’s disease. Revue
R., Foti, D., Behnke, E., et al. (1998). Motor, cog- Neurologique (Paris), 159(8–9), 762–766.
nitive, and behavioral performance following uni- Morrison, C. E., Borod, J. C., Perrine, K., Beric, A., Brin,
lateral ventroposterior pallidotomy for Parkinson M. F., Rezai, A., et al. (2004). Neuropsychological
disease. Archives of Neurology, 55(9), 1201–1208. functioning following bilateral subthalamic
Matsui, H., Udaka, F., Miyoshi, T., Hara, N., Tamura, nucleus stimulation in Parkinson’s disease. Archives
A., Oda, M., et al. (2005). N-isopropyl-p- 123I iodo- of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19(2), 165–181.
amphetamine single photon emission computed Mossner, R., Henneberg, A., Schmitt, A., Syagailo,
tomography study of Parkinson’s disease with Y. V., Grassle, M., Hennig, T., et al. (2001). Allelic
dementia. Internal Medicine, 44(10), 1046–1050. variation of serotonin transporter expression is
Mattila, P. M., Rinne, J. O., Helenius, H., Dickson, associated with depression in Parkinson’s disease.
D. W., & Roytta, M. (2000). Alpha-synuclein- Molecular Psychiatry, 6(3), 350–352.
immunoreactive cortical Lewy bodies are asso- Muslimovic, D., Post, B., Speelman, J. D., & Schmand,
ciated with cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s B. (2005). Cognitive profi le of patients with newly
disease. Acta Neuropathologica (Berlin), 100(3), diagnosed Parkinson disease. Neurology, 65(8),
285–290. 1239–1245.
Mayeux, R. (1990). Depression in the patient with Nath, U., Ben-Shlomo, Y., Thomson, R. G., Morris,
Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, H. R., Wood, N. W., Lees, A. J., et al. (2001). The
51 Suppl, 20–23; discussion 24–25. prevalence of progressive supranuclear palsy
Mayeux, R., Denaro, J., Hemenegildo, N., Marder, K., (Steele–Richardson–Olszewski syndrome) in the
Tang, M. X., Cote, L. J., et al. (1992). A population- UK. Brain, 124(Pt 7), 1438–1449.
220 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Nussbaum, R. L., & Ellis, C. E. (2003). Alzheimer’s diseases and progressive supranuclear palsy.
disease and Parkinson’s disease. New England Neurology, 41(5), 634–643.
Journal of Medicine, 348(14), 1356–1364. Pillon, B., Gouider-Khouja, N., Deweer, B., Vidailhet,
O’Brien, C., Sung, J. H., McGeachie, R. E., & Lee, M., Malapani, C., Dubois, B., et al. (1995).
M. C. (1990). Striatonigral degeneration: Clinical, Neuropsychological pattern of striatonigral degen-
MRI, and pathologic correlation. Neurology, 40(4), eration: Comparison with Parkinson’s disease and
710–711. progressive supranuclear palsy. Journal of Neurology,
Obwegeser, A. A., Uitti, R. J., Lucas, J. A., Witte, R. J., Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 58(2), 174–179.
Turk, M. F., & Wharen, R. E., Jr. (2000). Predictors Polymeropoulos, M. H., Lavedan, C., Leroy, E., Ide,
of neuropsychological outcome in patients fol- S. E., Dehejia, A., Dutra, A., et al. (1997). Mutation
lowing microelectrode-guided pallidotomy for in the alpha-synuclein gene identified in fami-
Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neurosurgery, lies with Parkinson’s disease. Science, 276(5321),
93(3), 410–420. 2045–2047.
Olzak, M., Laskowska, I., Jelonek, J., Michalak, M., Postuma, R. B., Lang, A. E., Massicotte-Marquez, J.,
Szolna, A., Gryz, J., et al. (2006). Psychomotor & Montplaisir, J. (2006). Potential early markers of
and executive functioning after unilateral poster- Parkinson disease in idiopathic REM sleep behav-
oventral pallidotomy in patients with Parkinson’s ior disorder. Neurology, 66(6), 845–851.
disease. Journal of Neurological Sciences, 248(1–2), Raskin, S. A., Borod, J. C., & Tweedy, J. R. (1992).
97–103. Set-shift ing and spatial orientation in patients
Owen, A. M., James, M., Leigh, P. N., Summers, B. with Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Clinical and
A., Marsden, C. D., Quinn, N. P., et al. (1992). Experimental Neuropsychology, 14(5), 801–821.
Fronto-striatal cognitive deficits at different Raskin, S. A., Sliwinski, M., & Borod, J. C. (1992).
stages of Parkinson’s disease. Brain, 115(Pt 6), Clustering strategies on tasks of verbal fluency
1727–1751. in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 30(1),
Pahwa, R., Paolo, A., Troster, A., & Koller, W. (1998). 95–99.
Cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Rektor, I., Rektorova, I., & Kubova, D. (2006).
European Journal of Neurology, 5(5), 431–441. Vascular parkinsonism—an update. Journal of
Parati, E. A., Fetoni, V., Geminiani, G. C., Soliveri, Neurological Sciences, 248(1–2), 185–191.
P., Giovannini, P., Testa, D., et al. (1993). Response Rettig, G. M., York, M. K., Lai, E. C., Jankovic,
to L-DOPA in multiple system atrophy. Clinical J., Krauss, J. K., Grossman, R. G., et al. (2000).
Neuropharmacology, 16(2), 139–144. Neuropsychological outcome after unilateral pal-
Parkinson Study Group. (2002). A controlled trial of lidotomy for the treatment of Parkinson’s disease.
rasagiline in early Parkinson Disease. The TEMPO Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry,
study. Archives of Neurology, 59, 1937–1943. 69(3), 326–336.
Parsons, T. D., Rogers, S. A., Braaten, A. J., Woods, Reuter, J., Raedler, T., Rose, M., Hand, I., Glascher,
S. P., & Troster, A. I. (2006). Cognitive sequelae J., & Buchel, C. (2005). Pathological gambling
of subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation is linked to reduced activation of the mesolim-
in Parkinson’s disease: A meta-analysis. Lancet bic reward system. Nature Neuroscience, 8(2),
Neurology, 5(7), 578–588. 147–148.
Payami, H., Larsen, K., Bernard, S., & Nutt, J. (1994). Ridenour, T. A., & Dean, R. S. (1999). Parkinson’s
Increased risk of Parkinson’s disease in parents disease and neuropsychological assessment.
and siblings of patients. Annals of Neurology, International Journal of Neuroscience, 99(1–4),
36(4), 659–661. 1–18.
Perrine, K., Dogali, M., Fazzini, E., Sterio, D., Kolodny, Riordan, H. J., Flashman, L. A., & Roberts, D. W.
E., Eidelberg, D., et al. (1998). Cognitive function- (1997). Neurocognitive and psychosocial corre-
ing after pallidotomy for refractory Parkinson’s lates of ventroposterolateral pallidotomy surgery
disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and in Parkinson’s disease. Neurosurgery Focus, 2(3),
Psychiatry, 65(2), 150–154. e7.
Perry, E. K., Haroutunian, V., Davis, K. L., Levy, R., Saint-Cyr, J. A., Trepanier, L. L., Kumar, R., Lozano,
Lantos, P., Eagger, S., et al. (1994). Neocortical A. M., & Lang, A. E. (2000). Neuropsychological
cholinergic activities differentiate Lewy body consequences of chronic bilateral stimulation of
dementia from classical Alzheimer’s disease. the subthalamic nucleus in Parkinson’s disease.
Neuroreport, 5(7), 747–749. Brain, 123(Pt 10), 2091–2108.
Pillon, B., Dubois, B., Ploska, A., & Agid, Y. (1991). Samii, A., Nutt, J. G., & Ransom, B. R. (2004).
Severity and specificity of cognitive impairment Parkinson’s disease. Lancet, 363(9423),
in Alzheimer’s, Huntington’s, and Parkinson’s 1783–1793.
Neuropsychological Aspects of Parkinson’s Disease and Parkinsonism 221

Sawada, H., Udaka, F., Kameyama, M., Seriu, N., Stebbins, G. T., Gabrieli, J. D., Shannon, K. M., Penn,
Nishinaka, K., Shindou, K., et al. (1992). SPECT R. D., & Goetz, C. G. (2000). Impaired frontostri-
findings in Parkinson’s disease associated with atal cognitive functioning following posteroven-
dementia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and tral pallidotomy in advanced Parkinson’s disease.
Psychiatry, 55(10), 960–963. Brain Cognition, 42(3), 348–363.
Shiba, M., Bower, J. H., Maraganore, D. M., Stein, M. B., Heuser, I. J., Juncos, J. L., & Uhde,
McDonnell, S. K., Peterson, B. J., Ahlskog, J. E., T. W. (1990). Anxiety disorders in patients
et al. (2000). Anxiety disorders and depressive dis- with Parkinson’s disease. American Journal of
orders preceding Parkinson’s disease: A case-con- Psychiatry, 147(2), 217–220.
trol study. Movement Disorders, 15(4), 669–677. Stern, Y., Richards, M., Sano, M., & Mayeux, R. (1993).
Shimizu, S., Hanyu, H., Kanetaka, H., Iwamoto, T., Comparison of cognitive changes in patients with
Koizumi, K., & Abe, K. (2005). Differentiation of Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s disease. Archives of
dementia with Lewy bodies from Alzheimer’s dis- Neurology, 50(10), 1040–1045.
ease using brain SPECT. Dementia and Geriatric Tanner, C. M., Ottman, R., Goldman, S. M.,
Cognitive Disorders, 20(1), 25–30. Ellenberg, J., Chan, P., Mayeux, R., et al. (1999).
Siemers, E. R., Shekhar, A., Quaid, K., & Dickson, Parkinson disease in twins: An etiologic study.
H. (1993). Anxiety and motor performance in Journal of the American Medical Association,
Parkinson’s disease. Movement Disorders, 8(4), 281(4), 341–346.
501–506. Taylor, A. E., & Saint-Cyr, J. A. (1992). Executive
Skeel, R. L., Crosson, B., Nadeau, S. E., Algina, J., function. In S. J. Huber & J. L. Cummings (Eds.),
Bauer, R. M., & Fennell, E. B. (2001). Basal ganglia Parkinsons disease: Neurobehavioral aspects (pp.
dysfunction, working memory, and sentence com- 74–85). New York: Oxford University Press.
prehension in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Testa, D., Fetoni, V., Soliveri, P., Musicco, M.,
Neuropsychologia, 39(9), 962–971. Palazzini, E., & Girotti, F. (1993). Cognitive and
Smeding, H. M., Speelman, J. D., Koning-Haanstra, motor performance in multiple system atrophy and
M., Schuurman, P. R., Nijssen, P., van Laar, T., Parkinson’s disease compared. Neuropsychologia,
et al. (2006). Neuropsychological effects of bilat- 31(2), 207–210.
eral STN stimulation in Parkinson disease: A con- Tiraboschi, P., Hansen, L. A., Alford, M., Sabbagh,
trolled study. Neurology, 66(12), 1830–1836. M. N., Schoos, B., Masliah, E., et al. (2000).
Soliveri, P., Monza, D., Paridi, D., Carella, F., Genitrini, Cholinergic dysfunction in diseases with Lewy
S., Testa, D., et al. (2000). Neuropsychological fol- bodies. Neurology, 54(2), 407–411.
low up in patients with Parkinson’s disease, stria- Tiraboschi, P., Salmon, D. P., Hansen, L. A., Hofstetter,
tonigral degeneration-type multisystem atrophy, R. C., Thal, L. J., & Corey-Bloom, J. (2006). What
and progressive supranuclear palsy. Journal of best differentiates Lewy body from Alzheimer’s
Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 69(3), disease in early-stage dementia? Brain, 129(Pt 3),
313–318. 729–735.
Soukup, V. M., Ingram, F., Schiess, M. C., Bonnen, J. Tomer, R., Fisher, T., Giladi, N., & Aharon-Peretz,
G., Nauta, H. J., & Calverley, J. R. (1997). Cognitive J. (2002). Dissociation between spontaneous and
sequelae of unilateral posteroventral pallidotomy. reactive flexibility in early Parkinson’s disease.
Archives of Neurology, 54(8), 947–950. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, & Behavioral
Spampinato, U., Habert, M. O., Mas, J. L., Bourdel, M. Neurology, 15(2), 106–112.
C., Ziegler, M., de Recondo, J., et al. (1991). (99mTc)- Trepanier, L. L., Kumar, R., Lozano, A. M., Lang, A.
HM-PAO SPECT and cognitive impairment in E., & Saint-Cyr, J. A. (2000). Neuropsychological
Parkinson’s disease: A comparison with demen- outcome of GPi pallidotomy and GPi or STN deep
tia of the Alzheimer type. Journal of Neurology, brain stimulation in Parkinson’s disease. Brain
Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 54(9), 787–792. Cognition, 42(3), 324–347.
Stacy, M. (2002). Sleep disorders in Parkinson’s dis- Trepanier, L. L., Saint-Cyr, J. A., Lozano, A. M., &
ease: Epidemiology and management. Drugs and Lang, A. E. (1998). Neuropsychological conse-
Aging, 19(10), 733–739. quences of posteroventral pallidotomy for the
Stacy, M., & Jankovic, J. (1992). Differential diagnosis treatment of Parkinson’s disease. Neurology, 51(1),
of Parkinson’s disease and the parkinsonism plus 207–215.
syndromes. Neurologic Clinics, 10(2), 341–359. Troster, A. I., Fields, J. A., Testa, J. A., Paul, R. H.,
Stebbins, G. T., Gabrieli, J. D., Masciari, F., Monti, Blanco, C. R., Hames, K. A., et al. (1998). Cortical
L., & Goetz, C. G. (1999). Delayed recognition and subcortical influences on clustering and
memory in Parkinson’s disease: A role for working switching in the performance of verbal fluency
memory? Neuropsychologia, 37(4), 503–510. tasks. Neuropsychologia, 36(4), 295–304.
222 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Uitti, R. J., Wharen, R. E., Duff y, J. R., Lucas, J. A., dementia in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of the
Schneider, S. L., Rippeth, J. D., et al. (2000). Unilateral International Neuropsychological Society, 9(1),
pallidotomy for Parkinson’s disease: Speech, motor, 17–24.
and neuropsychological outcome measurements. York, M. K., Levin, H. S., Grossman, R. G., Lai, E.
Parkinsonism & Related Disorders, 6(3), 133–143. C., & Krauss, J. K. (2003). Clustering and switch-
Vanacore, N. (2005). Epidemiological evidence ing in phonemic fluency following pallidotomy
on multiple system atrophy. Journal of Neural for the treatment of Parkinson’s disease. Journal
Transmission, 112(12), 1605–1612. of Clinical & Experimental Neuropsychology, 25(1),
Volkmann, J. (2004). Deep brain stimulation for 110–121.
the treatment of Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Zgaljardic, D. J., Borod, J. C., Foldi, N. S., & Mattis,
Clinical Neurophysiology, 21(1), 6–17. P. (2003). A review of the cognitive and behavioral
Walker, Z., Allen, R. L., Shergill, S., & Katona, C. L. sequelae of Parkinson’s disease: Relationship to
(1997). Neuropsychological performance in Lewy frontostriatal circuitry. Cognitive & Behavioral
body dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. British Neurology, 16(4), 193–210.
Journal of Psychiatry, 170, 156–158. Zgaljardic, D. J., Borod, J. C., Foldi, N. S., Mattis,
Whittington, C. J., Podd, J., & Stewart-Williams, P. J., Gordon, M. F., Feigin, A., et al. (2006). An
S. (2006). Memory deficits in Parkinson’s dis- examination of executive dysfunction associ-
ease. Journal of Clinical & Experimental ated with frontostriatal circuitry in Parkinson’s
Neuropsychology, 28(5), 738–754. disease. Journal of Clinical & Experimental
Woods, S. P., & Troster, A. I. (2003). Prodromal Neuropsychology, 28(7), 1127–1144.
frontal/executive dysfunction predicts incident
10

Huntington’s Disease
Jason Brandt

For the second edition of Neuropsychological life, the dementia is not confounded by the
Assessment in Neuropsychiatric Disorders, this brain changes associated with normal aging.
author co-wrote a chapter on Huntington’s Finally, HD is inherited as an autosomal dom-
disease (HD) with his mentor, colleague, and inant trait, and the availability of a defi nitive
friend, Nelson Butters. In 1995, shortly before genetic test allows for the identification of pre-
the book’s publication, Nelson’s illustrious symptomatic individuals. Th is affords us the
career in neuropsychology was cut short by his unique opportunity to study the evolution of
premature death from amyotrophic lateral scle- a neuropsychiatric syndrome from its latent
rosis. The current chapter, a summary of the (presymptomatic) stage, through its 10–20
neuropsychology of HD and an update on what year course, to the postmortem examination
we have learned in the ensuing years, is dedi- of brain tissue.
cated to Nelson’s memory. HD presents clinically with a triad of symp-
toms (Folstein, 1989; Harper, 1996; Hayden,
1981). First, and most prominently, there is
A Brief Overview of the Syndrome
the movement disorder. HD is characterized
Among the neuropsychiatric disorders dis- by the onset, typically in one’s thirties or for-
cussed in this volume, HD provides some of ties, of involuntary choreiform and athetoid
the most fertile conditions for neuropsycho- (jerking and writhing) movements, as well as
logical investigation. There are several reasons impairments of voluntary action (David et al.,
for this. First, the disease is not difficult to 1987; Folstein et al., 1983; Georgiou et al.,
diagnose from history (especially family his- 1997; Hefter et al., 1987). Second, HD patients
tory) and clinical examination. The discovery develop a dementia syndrome that has served
of the specific genetic mutation responsible for as the prototype for subcortical dementia
this illness in 1993 allows for diagnostic con- (Brandt et al., 1988; McHugh & Folstein, 1975;
fi rmation. Second, the neuropathology of HD Paulsen et al., 1995). As will be discussed in
is relatively stereotyped and well understood. more detail below, HD patients do not develop
HD is a disease primarily of the basal ganglia, frank aphasia, amnesia, agnosia, or apraxia
with death of particular neuronal popula- (but cf. Shelton & Knopman, 1991) but rather
tions in the head of the caudate nucleus and suffer from marked impairments in aspects
the putamen being most prominent. Th ird, of attention and executive control (including
HD causes a dementia syndrome that is more retrieval from episodic memory and knowledge
selective than that seen in Alzheimer’s dis- stores) and reduced information-processing
ease (AD) or other cortical dementias. Fourth, speed (Brandt, 1991; Brandt & Butters, 1986;
since the disease typically has onset in mid- Brandt & Bylsma, 1993). Third, patients

223
224 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

usually develop emotional disturbances, with The Dementia of Huntington’s


depression, irritability, and apathy being Disease
most common (Burns et al., 1990; Chatterjee
Early studies of the dementia of HD typically
et al., 2005; Folstein et al., 1983; Paulsen et al.,
compared samples of early- or mid-course
2001a, 2005). Importantly, multiple studies
HD patients to neurologically normal sub-
have found that cognitive impairment and emo-
jects (either persons unrelated to HD patients
tional/behavioral symptoms are major sources
or asymptomatic, at-risk offspring of patients)
of functional impairment in HD, often more so
(Bamford et al., 1995; Butters et al., 1978; Fedio
than the movement disorder (Bamford et al.,
et al., 1979; Josiassen et al., 1982; Sax et al.,
1995; Brandt et al., 1984; Hamilton et al., 2003;
1983; Strauss & Brandt, 1986). Not surpris-
Mayeux et al., 1986; Nehl et al., 2004; Rothlind
ingly, these studies typically reported impair-
et al., 1993).
ments on a wide variety of neuropsychological
Although the hereditary nature of HD was
tests. A meta-analysis by Zakzanis (1998) of
recognized by George Huntington in 1872, it
36 published studies found that HD patients
was not until Gusella and colleagues discovered
performed more poorly than neurologically
a restriction fragment length polymorphism
normal subjects in all seven of the neuropsy-
(RFLP) linked to HD in 1983 that its genetic
chological domains examined. Tests of delayed
locus was identified (Gusella et al., 1983). This
recall (e.g., Visual Reproductions-II and Logical
discovery led to the development of a presymp-
Memory-II from the Wechsler Memory Scale-
tomatic genetic test for persons at risk who had
Revised) and memory acquisition (e.g., learning
“informative” pedigrees (Brandt et al., 1989;
variables from the California Verbal Learning
Meissen et al., 1988). Ten years later, the precise
Test) yielded the largest effect sizes, followed by
mutation was discovered (Huntington’s Disease
tests of cognitive flexibility and abstraction (e.g.,
Collaborative Research Group, 1993), allowing
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test [WCST], Tower
for a vastly improved, definitive genetic test.
of London, Controlled Oral Word Association
HD was found to be caused by the pathologi-
Test). Tests of “verbal skill” (e.g., Wechsler Adult
cal expansion of a trinucleotide repeat sequence
Intelligence Scale-Revised [WAIS-R] verbal
at chromosomal locus 4p16.3. The sequence
subtests, Boston Naming Test, National Adult
of DNA bases at this location—cytosine, ade-
Reading Test) yielded the smallest effect sizes.
nine, and guanine (CAG)—is repeated between
One limitation of this meta-analysis is that sev-
10 and 35 times on healthy chromosomes. On
eral effect size computations were based on data
chromosomes from HD patients, this sequence
from a single published study.
expands to more than 36 CAG repeats. There
Other studies examining the neuropsycho-
is now very good evidence that the size of the
logical profi le of HD have taken a comparative
expansion is negatively correlated with age of
approach, juxtaposing the test performances
clinical onset (Andresen et al., 2006; Andrew
of HD patients to those of patients with other
et al., 1993; Claes et al., 1995; Duyao et al., 1993;
neurological disorders. A major challenge for
Kieburtz et al., 1994; Stine et al., 1993). Cases
such studies is the necessity of matching patient
of juvenile onset HD, for example, almost
groups for stage of disease or dementia severity;
always have CAG repeat lengths greater than
none of the methods for doing this is entirely
60 (Duyao et al. 1993; Nance & Myers, 2001;
satisfactory. Three studies (Brandt et al., 1988;
Ranen et al., 1995). In contrast, people who have
Paulsen et al., 1995; Salmon et al., 1989) com-
expansions in the range of 36–39 typically have
pared groups of HD patients and AD patients,
such late onset that some live normal lifespans
matched on either the Mini-Mental State Exam
without ever developing signs or symptoms.
(Folstein et al., 1975) or the Dementia Rating
Furthermore, there is mounting evidence that
Scale (Mattis, 1988). The mean profi les of
longer repeat length is correlated with more
these groups on those same instruments were
rapid progression of symptoms (Brandt et al.,
compared. The results are extremely consis-
1996; Illarioshkin et al., 1994; Mahant et al.,
tent: across levels of severity, HD patients have
2003; Rosenblatt et al., 1998; but c.f. Kieburtz
greater impairment on tests of attention, mental
et al., 1994).
Huntington’s Disease 225

tracking, and generativity than equivalently number of spoken letters or spoken numerals in
demented patients with AD. HD patients are tape-recorded sequences of letters and numer-
much less impaired than their AD counterparts als of increasing length. Schretlen et al. (1996)
on items assessing orientation, naming, and reported that HD patients of average intellect,
new learning/memory. all of whom scored 27 or higher on the Mini-
In one study using the comparative approach Mental State Examination (MMSE), display very
(Brandt et al., 2004), 21 patients with HD were severe impairments on this task when compared
compared to 31 patients with primary cere- to normal subjects. This deficit may be due to
bellar degenerations using neuropsychologi- impairment in auditory working memory (i.e.,
cal tests assessing five domains (motor, verbal, maintaining information in active storage and
spatial, memory, and executive). HD and cere- operating on it for a single trial only) as much as
bellar degeneration are both conditions which an impairment in selective attention.
produce mid-life onset of a movement disorder, Müller and colleagues (2002) studied HD
and the groups studied had equivalently severe patients and neurologically normal control sub-
motor impairment. The HD patients displayed jects on attention tasks chosen to assess three
more pervasive and more severe cognitive defi- components of a specific attentional model (van
cits, with largest effect sizes (compared to nor- Zomeran & Brouwer, 1994): selection, intensity,
mal subjects) in the spatial and the executive and supervisory control. Impairments were
domains (see Figure 10–1). found in all three domains, with the most severe
and pervasive deficits in intensity. Unlike nor-
mal subjects, HD patients did not have shorter
Attention
latencies in a cued than an uncued reaction
Although attention is often described as par- time task. The authors suggest that this repre-
ticularly impaired in HD, there have been sents a specific deficit in “extrinsic alertness.”
surprisingly few empirical studies specifically A somewhat different conclusion was reached
investigating attentional phenomena in this dis- by Sprengelmeyer and colleagues (1995). They
order. One reason may be that many aspects of reported that HD patients were able to maintain
attention can also be conceptualized as aspects attention when provided with external cues, but
of working memory or executive control. For fail to do so without cues.
example, the Brief Test of Attention (Schretlen, Also using a reaction-time paradigm, Stout
1997) requires patients to keep track of the et al. (2001b) demonstrated that patients with

0.5
Cerebellar (N=31)
Huntington (N=21)
0.4
Effect size (eta-squared)

0.3

0.2

0.1

0
Motor Verbal Spatial Memory Executive

Figure 10–1. Magnitude of cognitive deficits in five domains among patients with Huntington’s disease
(N = 21) and patients with cerebellar degeneration (N = 31). Effect sizes (partial eta-squared) for each domain
are derived from analyses of variance comparing performance of patient groups to normal control subjects on
constituent neuropsychological tests. (From Brandt et al., 2004. Used with permission.)
226 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

HD fail to show negative priming. Whereas are usually attributed to disorganized semantic
normal subjects and Parkinson’s disease (PD) networks, HD patients perform poorly due
patients had elevated reaction times when to faulty retrieval mechanisms and strategies
responding to targets that shared features with (Chan et al., 1993; Randolph et al., 1993; Rohrer
distractors from previous trials, this was not the et al., 1999).
case in HD. The authors speculate that the neo- Two frequently studied characteristics of
striatum, but not the substantia nigra, plays a word list generation performance are cluster-
critical role in selective attention by altering the ing (i.e., producing successive words from the
salience of distractor stimuli. same subcategory) and switching among clus-
A specific impairment in shifting attentional ters. Clustering on letter-cued word genera-
set has been described by some investigators tion tasks is often impaired in temporal lobe
as prominent in early HD (Aron et al., 2003; disorders, whereas reduced switching has been
Lawrence et al., 1996). In contrast, no impair- found in patients with frontal-lobe pathology
ment in attentional shift ing was observed by (Troyer et al., 1998). Rich and colleagues (1999)
Filoteo et al. (1995) using a global–local atten- examined word generation to initial letter cues
tion task (e.g., deciding whether a “3” was pre- in 72 patients with HD and 41 healthy partici-
sent when shown a large numeral “1” made up pants and found reduced switching but normal
of smaller “3”s). Rather, Filoteo and colleagues clustering in the patients. This was interpreted
found that HD patients, but not PD patients, had as reflecting dysfunction in cerebral circuitry
abnormally long reaction times on inconsistent linking prefrontal cortex to the striatum. In
trials (a large “1” made up of small “3”s) relative addition, switching, but not clustering, corre-
to consistent trials (a large “3” made up of small lated inversely with disease severity, as mea-
“3”s) (Roman et al., 1998). Thus, only the HD sured by both the Quantified Neurological
patients were unusually vulnerable to distrac- Examination (QNE) (Folstein et al., 1983) and
tion from the unattended hierarchical level in the MMSE. Furthermore, annual follow-ups
the global–local paradigm. Maintaining atten- over 5 years revealed a monotonic decrease in
tional focus was a problem for HD patients, but switching over time, whereas clustering perfor-
shifting when appropriate was not. mance remained stable. Neurologically normal
control participants performed uniformly over
time on both measures. These results are con-
Language
sistent with a progressive reduction in cogni-
The most significant obstacle to communica- tive flexibility in HD, attributed to disruption
tion with HD patients is their difficulty with of frontal–subcortical circuits traversing the
motor control necessary for intelligible speech. caudate nucleus.
Language per se is only mildly affected. Patients
make minor syntactic errors (Murray & Lenz,
Spatial Cognition
2001) and have some difficulty comprehend-
ing implied meanings (Murray & Stout, 1999). In addition to their prominent deficits in
The few errors that HD patients make on tests retrieval from memory and aspects of executive
of visual confrontation naming tend to be control, HD patients also have very substantial
due to perceptual misinterpretations rather impairments in spatial abilities (Brandt et al.,
than impaired access to semantic information 2004, 2005; Bylsma et al., 1992; Mohr et al.,
(Hodges et al., 1991). 1991). The severity of these spatial deficits may be
One of the most consistently reported neuro- overlooked or underappreciated, since patients’
psychological impairments in HD, and one that difficulties with visuomotor and visuographic
appears very early in the illness, is in the abil- tasks are often seen as understandable conse-
ity to rapidly generate lists of words conform- quences of their movement disorder. However,
ing to specific rules (“verbal fluency”) (Butters HD patients also have difficulty on purely
et al., 1986; Henry et al., 2005; Ho et al., 2002; perceptual, entirely nonmotor spatial tasks
Randolph et al., 1993). Whereas the perfor- (Fedio et al., 1979; Gómez-Tortosa et al., 1996;
mance failures of AD patients on these tasks Lawrence et al., 2000) and in situations where
Huntington’s Disease 227

the motor impairment has been experimentally cortical zone important for spatial localization
or statistically controlled. For example, Brandt (Saint-Cyr, 2003).
et al. (2004) reported that patients with HD had
more profound spatial deficits than patients
Memory
with equivalently severe movement disorders
due to cerebellar degeneration. Across studies, impaired learning and retrieval
Mohr and colleagues (1997) addressed the of new episodic memories are the most fre-
specificity of the spatial deficits in HD. They quently observed cognitive deficits in HD
reported that HD and PD patients share cer- (Zakzanis, 1998). The wordlist and narrative
tain visuospatial processing deficits, but only learning impairments of HD patients have been
HD patients were impaired on map reading, particularly well described (Butters et al., 1986,
directional sense, and adjusting their targeted 1987; Delis et al., 1991; Hodges et al., 1990;
movements to alterations in their body posi- Pillon et al., 1993). On list-learning tasks, defi-
tions. This was interpreted as reflecting a spe- cient immediate and delayed free recall with
cific impairment in person-centered spatial preservation of yes/no or forced-choice rec-
judgment (see also Bylsma et al., 1992). Davis ognition is often taken as the sine qua non of
et al. (2003) reported that the processing and subcortical dementia (Brandt & Munro, 2001).
memory of both egocentric and allocentric spa- However, several studies (Brandt et al., 1992;
tial information is impaired in HD, but only Kramer et al., 1988; Lang et al., 2000), including
the former (person-centered) is correlated with a recent meta-analysis (Montoya et al., 2006),
measures of disease severity. have questioned whether this pattern really
Lawrence and colleagues (2000) undertook a does characterize the memory impairment of
detailed analysis of visual object and visuospa- HD. In addition, several studies have raised the
tial cognition in early HD. They administered possibility that the verbal memory difficulties
the Visual Object and Space Perception (VOSP) of HD patients are actually secondary to one or
Battery (Warrington & James, 1991) as well as a more disorders of executive control (Lawrence
number of computerized tasks of visual memory et al., 1996; Pillon et al., 1993).
(discussed below) to 19 HD patients and age- Less research has addressed the visuospatial
and NART-matched normal control subjects. memory impairment of HD patients. Hodges
The only VOSP task on which the patient group and colleagues (1990) found that HD patients
performed abnormally was the object decision who performed as poorly as AD patients on the
subtest. This task requires subjects to indicate immediate recall on the Visual Reproductions
which of four silhouettes is of a real object, but subtest of the Wechlser Memory Scale performed
naming or otherwise identifying the object significantly better than AD patients on delayed
is not necessary. The HD patients were also recall. This suggests that rate of forgetting is rel-
impaired in both accuracy and reaction time atively normal in HD, a conclusion reached ear-
on simultaneous trials (i.e., 0-second delay) of lier by Martone and colleagues (1986) and later
a matching-to-sample task. This suggests that at by Davis and collaborators (1999).
least some of HD patients’ failures on tests of Lawrence et al. (2000) observed that self-
visual pattern and visual spatial memory may ordered spatial working memory is impaired in
be due to perceptual or executive factors. early HD while visual object working memory
The neural mechanisms underlying impaired is not. They argued that this difference is pri-
spatial cognition in HD is not fully understood. marily due to the greater load the former task
It has been suggested that one of the functions places on the development and implementation
of the basal ganglia is to update information of search strategies. These same authors also
on spatial relationships from context-relevant observed that the learning of both visuospatial
sensory input (Lawrence et al., 1998, 2000). In patterns and spatial positions was impaired in
addition, the caudate nucleus receives signif- HD but, as in other studies, rate of forgetting
icant inputs from the inferior parietal sulcus was normal. In addition, HD patients were
(Yeterian & Pandya, 1993). Thus, the HD stri- impaired in the simultaneous condition of a
atum may be deprived of critical input from a match-to-sample task, leading the authors to
228 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

9
8
7

Number correct
6
5
4
3 Minimally disabled
2 Mildly disabled
1 Moderately disabled
0
Item Location Item Location Item Location
Alzheimer’s Huntington’s Parkinson’s

Figure 10–2. Delayed recall of objects (items) and their positions in space (locations) in Alzheimer’s disease,
Huntington’s disease, and Parkinson’s disease patients, stratified by level of functional disability. (From Brandt
et al., 2005. Used with permission.)

speculate that the primary visuospatial memory They are clearly impaired in their ability to
deficit in HD was not in memory per se, but learn the pursuit rotor task (Butters, 1984;
rather in response selection. Heindel et al., 1988) and in learning to adjust
Recently, Brandt et al. (2005) reported that their movements to compensate for changes
memory for the location of objects in space is in their body positions (Potegal, 1971) or the
more impaired than memory for the objects displacement of visual stimuli (Paulsen et al.,
themselves in HD. Reminiscent of Lawrence 1993). These deficits are seen very early in the
et al.’s (2000) finding in working memory, this disease, even when baseline motor performance
selective impairment was found across levels is controlled experimentally or statistically.
of disease severity, and was not seen in AD or Whereas HD patients are impaired in learn-
PD (see Figure 10–2). It was speculated that this ing the pursuit rotor task, they learn a mirror-
reflects a disruption of parietal lobe’s input to reversed tracing task as well as nonpatients
the caudate nucleus in HD. (Gabrieli et al., 1997). This has led to the pro-
posal that the neostriatal structures that degen-
erate in HD are essential for the learning of
Motor Learning and Other Forms of
repetitive motor sequences (as in the pursuit
Nondeclarative Memory
rotor), but not for the learning of new mappings
The basal ganglia of primates have extensive between visual cues and movements (as in the
reciprocal connections to the motor and pre- mirror-tracing task) (Willingham et al., 1996).
motor areas of the frontal lobe (Alexander et al., Studies using the serial reaction task to inves-
1986), regions that have been implicated in the tigate procedural memory have yielded mixed
planning of movements as well as their execu- results, with some finding performance deficits
tion. These corticostriatal connections, as well in HD (Knopman & Nissen, 1991; Kim et al.,
as the striatum’s intrinsic connectivity, have led 2004; Willingham & Koroshetz, 1993) and oth-
to the appreciation that the basal ganglia are ers not, at least under some conditions (Brandt,
involved not solely in motor execution, but in 1994a; Brown et al., 2001). Differences in how
motor learning as well (Graybiel et al., 1994). task data are analyzed are responsible for some
Patients with HD are, in fact, impaired in of the differences among studies.
acquiring many different motor and sensori- Knowlton and colleagues (1996) extended
motor skills (Bondi & Kaszniak, 1991; Heindel the nondeclarative learning impairment of HD
et al., 1988; Knopman & Nissen, 1991; Martone from the sensorimotor realm to the conceptual
et al., 1984; Willingham & Koroshetz, 1993). realm. They showed that HD patients are poor
Huntington’s Disease 229

at learning a probabilistic classification task planning and working memory than on tests
that, it is argued, requires the type of gradual, of risk-taking and decision making. In fact,
trial-by-trial habit acquisition, often outside of Watkins and coworkers (2000) found just that.
awareness, that is the province of the caudate Twenty patients with early HD were impaired
nucleus. In contrast, HD patients were normal on the one-touch version of the Tower of London
in learning an artificial grammar, a task pro- test of planning, while they performed like neu-
posed to be more dependent on neocortical rologically normal subjects on a decision-mak-
mechanisms. Filoteo et al. (2001) also found ing task that involved predicting outcomes, and
HD patients to be impaired in learning differ- wagering on them, based on their probabilities
ent types of categorization rules. of occurrence. However, Stout and colleagues
(2001a) found HD patients (but not PD patients)
to be impaired on the Iowa Gambling Task, the
Executive Control
deficit related to difficulties learning the win/
The executive functions—those “general pur- lose contingencies. Furthermore, Campbell and
pose control mechanisms that modulate the colleagues (2004) showed that the autonomic
operation of various cognitive subprocesses and reaction (skin conductance response) displayed
thereby regulate the dynamics of human cog- by normal subjects upon making a disadvanta-
nition” (Miyake et al., 2000)—have tradition- geous response that costs them money is absent
ally been attributed to the prefrontal cortex. in HD patients. These authors invoke Damasio’s
However, they are now appreciated as emerging somatic marker hypothesis (Damasio, 1996;
from the integrated activity of cortico-thalamo- Tranel et al., 2000) and suggest that the fronto-
striatal circuits, with the caudate nucleus play- striatal dysfunction of HD renders these patients
ing a particularly prominent role. unable to use physiological cues of success and
As described earlier, brief selective attention failure to guide decision making.
and working memory tasks, often embedded in
mental status exams, are sensitive to impair-
Emotional Processing
ments in executive control in HD (Brandt et al.,
1988; Paulson et al., 1995; Rothlind & Brandt, Even relatively early in the illness, HD patients
1993). In addition, well-known clinical tests of have difficulty identifying emotions expressed
executive functioning, including the WCST, in vocal prosody (Speedie et al., 1990) and in
Stroop Color-Word Test, and the Tower of facial expressions (Jacobs et al., 1995; Montagne
London Test, are all impaired in HD patients et al., 2006). One of the most provocative find-
(Paulsen et al., 1996; Watkins et al., 2000), as are ings in recent years is that these patients may
research tasks such as random number genera- have a selective impairment in the perception
tion (Ho et al., 2004) and the CANTAB execu- of disgust. With few exceptions (e.g., Milders
tive tasks (Lawrence et al., 1996). et al., 2003), studies have found that patients
There is growing evidence that the executive with HD are more impaired in the recognition
functions can be behaviorally and neuroana- of facial expressions of disgust than expres-
tomically dissociated. For example, lesions of sions of fear, anger, and sadness (Hayes et al.,
the orbitofrontal and ventromedial prefrontal 2007; Sprengelmeyer et al., 1996, 1997; Wang
cortex appear to selectively impair performance et al., 2003). In several studies (Gray et al., 1997;
on a gambling-type test of decision making, Hennenlotter et al., 2004; Sprengelmeyer et al.,
while lesions of the dorsolateral frontal cor- 2006), this deficit has been found even in pre-
tex impair a problem-solving task requiring symptomatic persons carrying the HD muta-
strategy development and working memory tion. Hayes and colleagues (2007) maintain
(Tower of London; Bechara et al., 1998; Rogers that this deficit in the perception of facial dis-
et al., 1998, 1999). Since the caudate nucleus gust represents a more general impairment in
receives its most prominent afferent projec- the experience of disgust, as it extends to the
tions from the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, it labeling of disgusting sounds, the classifica-
might be expected that HD patients would be tion of disgust-evoking pictures, the hedonic
impaired earlier, or more severely, on tests of ratings of disgusting odors, and knowledge of
230 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

the situational determinants of disgust. These appreciable change over time on the WCST,
investigators posit that the deficits reflect probably due to the very strong practice effect
dysfunction in the insula, a cortical zone on this task (Basso et al., 1999). Similar findings
with prominent connections to the striatum were reported by Bachoud-Lévi and colleagues
(Chikama et al., 1997) and which serves as the (2001) using an overlapping test battery.
somatosensory cortex for the olfactory, gus- The role of CAG repeat length in account-
tatory, and visceral senses. In support of this ing for rate of decline was examined by Brandt
supposition, Hennenlotter et al. (2004) reported and colleagues (1996). At study entry, those
reduced BOLD responses from the left insula HD patients with long mutations were youn-
on fMRI among presymptomatic HD mutation ger and had earlier onsets, but they were no
carriers during the processing of facial expres- more neurologically or cognitively impaired
sions of disgust. This selective deficit in disgust than those with short mutations. However, the
processing in HD stands in contrast to the rela- long-mutation group had greater decline over
tively selective deficit in fear processing among a 2-year period on a cognitive factor reflecting
patients with amygdala pathology (Adolphs global mental ability. In a much larger sample
et al., 1999). from 43 sites participating in the Huntington’s
Study Group, Mahant et al. (2003) examined
the correlates of cognitive and functional pro-
Longitudinal Studies of
gression among 1,026 patients followed for
Huntington’s Disease
an average of 2.7 years. Younger age of onset
Although HD is a progressive disorder, there was associated with more rapid decline on the
have been relatively few studies of the course SDMT, but the effect was not significant for the
of its cognitive impairment. In an early study, Controlled Word Association Test or the Stroop
Hodges et al. (1990) studied 14 HD patients, 14 Color-Word Test (the only other tests adminis-
AD patients, and 14 normal elderly subjects on tered). Younger onset was also associated with
two occasions, separated by 1 year. The only faster functional decline. It was assumed by the
neuropsychological task on which HD patients authors that longer CAG repeat lengths under-
displayed greater decline over time than AD lie this more pernicious disease course in those
patients was letter-guided wordlist generation with earlier onset, but no repeat length data are
(verbal fluency), a finding ascribed to more included in this report.
rapidly progressive retrieval deficits in HD. In one of the more comprehensive recent
Snowden and colleagues (2001) reported that studies, Ward and colleagues (2006) studied
relatively automatic tasks with few cognitive the course of cognitive performance of 70 HD
demands (e.g., letter fluency, word-reading and patients over 4 years (i.e., five annual assess-
color-naming trials of Stroop task) declined ments). Using random effects modeling, Ward
more over 1 year than those making greater and collaborators found that, as in many previ-
cognitive demands (e.g., Stroop interference ous studies, the effects of time (visit) on test per-
trial, WCST, Road Map Test of Directional formance were modest. Statistically significant
Sense). The results were interpreted as reflect- declines were found on the Trail-Making Test,
ing the greater degeneration of striatal nuclei Brief Test of Attention, Controlled Oral Word
than associative neocortex. However, even for Association Test, Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-
the most sensitive tests, the absolute change in Revised (HVLT-R), word-reading and color-
performance over the 1-year study period was naming trials of the Stroop task, Developmental
extremely modest. Ho and colleagues (2003) Test of Visual-Motor Integration, and WAIS-R
reported decline on all the attention and execu- Block Design, but there was no significant dec-
tive function tests administered to HD patients line in the WCST, interference trial of the Stroop
annually for 3–6 years. Timed psychomotor task or WAIS-R Vocabulary. Neurologic dys-
tests (e.g., part A of the Trail-Making Test, function (i.e., QNE scores), both at baseline and
and the word-reading and color-naming trials at each subsequent visit, was the most potent pre-
of the Stroop task, but not the SDMT) were dictor of cognitive performance. In this study,
particularly sensitive to change. There was no CAG repeat length was not an independent
Huntington’s Disease 231

predictor of cognitive decline once the effect of While some studies admitted only persons
neurologic severity was considered. with no, or only very minimal, nonspecific
neurological signs (Hahn-Barma et al., 1998;
Paulsen et al., 2001b), others included subjects
Studies of Presymptomatic
with “major signs consistent with HD” (Foroud
Huntington’s Disease
et al., 1995; Kirkwood et al., 2000; Solomon
Since the discovery of the trinucleotide expan- et al., 2007; Witjes-Ané et al., 2003). When
sion responsible for HD, there have been many the samples in the latter studies are limited to
attempts to determine whether people who carry those who are entirely free of even minor motor
the mutation, but are not yet clinically ill, can abnormalities, the cognitive impairments often
be distinguished from those without the muta- disappear (Foroud et al., 1995; Solomon et al.,
tion. Recent brain imaging studies have dem- 2007). Other factors likely contributing to dis-
onstrated structural and/or metabolic changes crepant research fi ndings include differences in
in the striatum or cerebral cortex in such indi- the variety and sensitivity of the specific cogni-
viduals (Aylward et al., 2004; Ciarmiello et al., tive tests employed and differences in statistical
2006; Feigin et al., 2001; Harris et al., 1999; power to detect group differences (primarily
Paulsen et al., 2004; Reading et al., 2004, 2005; due to sample size).
Reynolds et al., 2002; Rosas et al., 2005; van Brandt et al. (2002) analyzed the results of
Oostrom et al., 2005). In addition, subtle ocu- detailed cognitive testing from 203 neurolog-
lomotor and other movement peculiarities and ically normal offsprings of HD patients who
neuropsychiatric changes have been reported in were subsequently tested for the HD muta-
some mutation-positive individuals prior to the tion. Seventy-five subjects had ≥37 CAG repeats
onset of clinical signs and symptoms sufficient (mutation-positive) and 128 had ≤30 repeats.
for diagnosis (Blekher et al., 2006; Foroud et al., The mutation-positive cases tended to have one
1995; Golding et al., 2006; Hinton et al., 2007; or two very minor neurologic abnormalities, but
Kirkwood et al., 1999, 2000; Snowden et al., the QNE scores of the two groups did not dif-
2002). fer significantly and both remained well within
The search for changes in cognition among normal limits. Neuropsychological tests that are
neurologically normal offsprings of HD very sensitive to early HD failed to detect a sig-
patients carrying the gene mutation has met nificant difference between mutation-positive
with mixed results. Several investigations have and mutation-negative groups. Furthermore,
found no differences in neuropsychological test among mutation-positive cases, those with
performance between those with and with- shorter mutations (37–43 repeats) and those with
out the CAG expansion (Brandt et al., 2002; longer mutations (≥44 repeats) did not differ on
Campodonico et al., 1996, 1998; de Boo et al., any of the 12 neuropsychological test variables.
1997, 1999). Other investigations have found Proximity to clinical onset of HD was estimated
performance deficits associated with the HD for the mutation-positive cases by computing the
mutation. Impairments have been reported in difference between chronological age and pro-
reaction time, psychomotor and processing jected age at onset. Projected age at onset was
speed (Kirkwood et al., 1999; Lemiere et al., calculated from CAG repeat length and age at
2002; Paulsen et al., 2001b; Snowden et al., 2002; onset of the affected parent, based on a previ-
Witjes-Ané et al., 2003), spatial analysis and ously derived regression equation (Campodonico
constructional praxis (Rosenberg et al., 1995), et al., 1996). Whereas the close-to-onset subgroup
verbal fluency (Lawrence et al., 1998), verbal (mean = 4.0 years) and far-from-onset subgroup
memory (Hahn-Barma et al., 1998; Solomon (mean = 13.1 years) did not differ in QNE score,
et al., 2007), visuospatial memory (Wahlin statistically significant differences were found
et al., 2007), mental arithmetic (Kirkwood on 7 of the 12 neuropsychological test variables.
et al., 1999, 2000), and the perception of disgust The close-to-onset group was most severely
(Gray et al., 1997), among others. Much of the impaired on the WAIS-R Block Design subtest
discrepancy in findings among studies appears (p = .006) and the SDMT (p = .009), as well as on
to hinge on the definition of “asymptomatic.” all three trials of the Stroop Color-Word Test and
232 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

nondominant hand performance of the Grooved developed HD symptoms and were diagnosed
Pegboard Test. (i.e., converted) after an average of 7.9 years.
Snowden et al. (2002) also found that per- These converters did not differ on any neuro-
formance on psychomotor tasks begins a grad- psychological variable from the mutation-posi-
ual decline several years prior to clinical onset, tive cases who did not convert during the study
while memory performance declines precipi- period (N = 82) or those who tested negative for
tously around symptom onset. A similar con- the HD mutation (N = 134). However, the con-
clusion can be drawn from the recent report verters who developed diagnosable HD soon
by Solomon and colleagues (2007). Studying a after genetic testing (between 1.8 and 8.6 years
very large cohort from the Huntington’s Study after baseline, with an average of 3.7 years) per-
Group, they compared 51 presymptomatic per- formed more poorly than those who converted
sons with the HD mutation to 423 without the later (between 8.7 and 18.3 years, with an aver-
mutation on the HVLT-R (Brandt & Benedict, age of 11.8 years) on the WCST. Although the
2001). Impairments were found on total learn- groups have equivalent very low (i.e., normal)
ing (sum of words correctly recalled on three scores on the QNE, the “early” converters made
free-recall trials) and recognition discrimina- more perseverative and nonperseverative errors
tion (hits minus false-positives) only among and were overall less efficient in their problem
those mutation-positives with at least minor, solving. While the presence of deficits in selec-
nonspecific motor abnormalities. Those who tive domains of nonverbal cognition may por-
were absolutely symptom-free did not differ tend earlier emergence of symptoms among
from mutation-negatives. Those with the genetic those with the huntingtin mutation, the data are
mutation who were judged closer to onset and not yet sufficiently robust for use in counseling
those who had smaller striatal volumes tended individual persons at risk.
to have poorer HVLT-R scores. Beyond the scope of this chapter, but cer-
Most studies that have reported cognitive tainly of great importance to clinicians working
impairment in presymptomatic HD have found with HD patients and their families, is the rich
it in mutation-positive persons who were judged psychological literature on the factors influenc-
to be close to onset (e.g., Brandt et al., 2002; ing the decision of whether to take the presymp-
Paulsen et al., 2004; Wahlin et al., 2007). While tomatic test (Codori et al., 1994; Quaid et al.,
informative, studies using estimated years to 1987; Quaid & Morris, 1993), the ethical issues
onset provide less definitive evidence of pre- raised by such testing (Bates, 1981; Brandt,
clinical cognitive changes than longitudinal 1994b), and the potentially life-altering conse-
investigations of persons who actually develop quences of presymptomatic diagnosis (Codori
symptoms of clinical HD (i.e., “convert”) dur- & Brandt, 1994; Codori et al., 1997; Wiggins
ing the study period. Paulsen et al. (2001b) ana- et al., 1992). What we have already learned from
lyzed the limited neuropsychological test data the study of this relatively rare neurodegen-
available from the 36 centers participating in erative disorder will continue to serves as an
the Huntington’s Study Group. They found important model as the “new genetics” plays an
that 70 of 260 mutation-positive subjects con- ever-increasing role in the practice of medicine
verted to HD within 2 years of baseline evalua- and in neuropsychology.
tion. These participants performed more poorly
on the SDMT and all three trials of the Stroop
Acknowledgment
Color-Word Test (but not a verbal fluency test)
than those who did not convert within 2 years. Preparation of this chapter was supported by
Brandt et al. (2008) studied longitudinally grant NS 16375 from the National Institute of
237 people at risk who were clinically normal Neurological Disorders and Stroke.
when first examined. Each participant under-
went a quantified neurological exam, testing for
References
the CAG repeat expansion, and neuropsycho-
logical testing. Twenty-one of these individuals Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Hamann, S., Young, A. W.,
tested positive for the mutation, subsequently Calder, A. J., Phelps, E. A., et al. (1999). Recognition
Huntington’s Disease 233

of facial emotion in nine individuals with Brandt, J. (1991). Cognitive impairments in


bilateral amygdala damage. Neuropsychologia, 37, Huntington’s disease: Insights into the
1111–1117. neuropsychology of the striatum. In F. Boller &
Alexander, G. E., DeLong, M. R., & Strick, P. L. J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology
(1986). Parallel organization of functionally seg- (pp. 241–264). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
regated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Brandt, J. (1994a). Cognitive investigations in
Annual Review of Neuroscience, 9, 357–381. Huntington’s disease. In L. S. Cermak (Ed.),
Andresen, J. M., Gayán, J., Djoussé, L., Roberts, S., Neuropsychological explorations of memory
Brocklebank, D., Cherny, S. S., et al. (2006). The and cognition: Essays in honor of Nelson Butters
relationship between CAG repeat length and age (pp. 135–146). New York: Plenum Press.
of onset differs for Huntington’s disease patients Brandt, J. (1994b). Ethical considerations in genetic
with juvenile onset and adult onset. Annals of testing: An empirical study of presymptomatic
Human Genetics, 70, 1–7. diagnosis of Huntington’s disease. In K. W. M.
Andrew, S. E., Goldberg, Y. P., Kremer, B., Telenius, Fulford, J. Soskice, & G. Gillett (Eds.), Medicine
H., Theilmann, J., Adam, S., et al. (1993). The and moral reasoning (pp. 41–59). Cambridge:
relationship between trinucleotide (CAG) repeat Cambridge University Press.
length and clinical features of Huntington’s dis- Brandt, J., & Benedict, R. H. B. (2001). The Hopkins
ease. Nature Genetics, 4, 398–403. Verbal Learning Test—Revised professional man-
Aron, A. R., Watkins, L., Sahakian, B. J., Monsell, ual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment
S., Barker, R. A., & Robbins, T. W. (2003). Task- Resources, Inc.
set switching deficits in early-stage Huntington’s Brandt, J., & Butters, N. (1986). The neuropsychology
disease: Implications for basal ganglia function. of Huntington’s disease. Trends in Neuroscience, 9,
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 629–642. 118–120.
Aylward, E. H., Sparks, B. F., Field, K. M., Yallapragada, Brandt, J., & Bylsma, F. W. (1993). The demen-
V., Shpritz, B. D., Rosenblatt, A., et al. (2004). tia of Huntington’s disease. In R. W. Parks,
Onset and rate of striatal atrophy in preclinical R. F. Zec, & R. W. Wilson (Eds.), Neuropsychology
Huntington disease. Neurology, 63, 66–72. of Alzheimer’s disease and other dementias
Bachoud-Lévi, A. C., Maison, P., Bartolomeo, P., (pp. 265–282). New York: Oxford University Press.
Boissé, M. F., Dalla Barba, G., Ergis, A. M., et al. Brandt, J., Bylsma, F. W., Gross, R., Stine, O. C., Ranen,
(2001). Retest effects and cognitive decline in lon- N., & Ross, C. A. (1996). Trinucleotide repeat
gitudinal follow-up of patients with early HD. length and clinical progression in Huntington’s
Neurology, 56, 1052–1058. disease. Neurology, 46, 527–531.
Bamford, K. A., Caine, E. D., Kido, D. K., Cox, C., Brandt, J., Corwin, J., & Krafft, L. (1992). Is verbal rec-
& Shoulson, I. (1995). A prospective evaluation ognition memory really different in Alzheimer’s
of cognitive decline in early Huntington’s dis- and Huntington’s disease? Journal of Clinical and
ease: Functional and radiographic correlates. Experimental Neuropsychology, 14, 773–784.
Neurology, 45, 1867–1873. Brandt, J., Folstein, S. E., & Folstein, M. F. (1988).
Basso, M. R., Bornstein, R. A., & Lang, J. M. (1999). Differential cognitive impairment in Alzheimer
Practice effects on commonly used measures of disease and Huntington’s disease. Annals of
executive function across twelve months. Clinical Neurology, 23, 555–561.
Neuropsychologist, 13, 283–292. Brandt, J., Inscore, A., Ward, J., Shpritz, B.,
Bates, M. (1981). Ethics of provocative test for Rosenblatt, A., Margolis, R. L., et al. (2008).
Huntington’s disease. New England Journal of Neuropsychological deficits in Huntington’s dis-
Medicine, 304, 175–176. ease gene carriers and predictors of early “con-
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Anderson, S. version.” Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical
W. (1998). Dissociation of working memory from Neurosciences, in press.
decision making within the human prefrontal Brandt, J., Leroi, I., O’Hearn, E., Rosenblatt, A., &
cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 428–437. Margolis, R. L. (2004). Cognitive impairments
Blekher, T., Johnson, S. A., Marshall, J., White, K., in cerebellar degeneration: a comparison with
Hui, S., Weaver, M., et al. (2006). Saccades in Huntington’s disease. Journal of Neuropsychiatry
presymptomatic and early stages of Huntington and Clinical Neurosciences, 16, 176–184.
disease. Neurology, 67, 394–399. Brandt, J., & Munro, C. (2001). Memory in sub-
Bondi, M. W., & Kaszniak, A. W. (1991). Implicit cortical dementia. In A. Baddeley, B. Wilson, &
and explicit memory in Alzheimer’s disease M. Kopelman (Eds.), Handbook of memory dis-
and Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Clinical and orders, 2nd ed. (pp. 591–614). London: Oxford
Experimental Neuropsychology, 13, 339–358. University Press.
234 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Brandt, J., Quaid, K. A, Folstein, S. E., Garber, P. A., the International Neuropsychological Society, 10,
Maestri, N. E., Abbott, M. H., et al. (1989). 239–245.
Presymptomatic diagnosis of delayed-onset dis- Campodonico, J. R., Aylward, E., Codori, A. M.,
ease with linked DNA markers: The experience Young, C., Krafft, L., Magdalinski, M., et al. (1998).
in Huntington’s disease. Journal of the American When does Huntington’s disease begin? Journal of
Medical Association, 261, 3108–3114. the International Neuropsychological Society, 4,
Brandt, J., Shpritz, B., Codori, A. M., Margolis, R., & 467–473.
Rosenblatt, A. (2002). Neuropsychological mani- Campodonico, J. R., Codori, A. M., & Brandt, J.
festations of the genetic mutation for Huntington’s (1996). Neuropsychological stability over two years
disease in presymptomatic individuals. Journal of in asymptomatic carriers of the Huntington’s dis-
the International Neuropsychological Society, 8, ease mutation. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery,
918–924. and Psychiatry, 61, 621–624.
Brandt, J. Shpritz, B., Munro, C. A., Marsh, L., & Chan, A. S., Butters, N., Paulsen, J. S., Salmon, D.
Rosenblatt, A. (2005). Differential impairment of P., Swenson, M. R., & Maloney, L. T. (1993). An
spatial location memory in Huntington’s disease. assessment of the semantic network in patients
Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, with Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Cognitive
76, 1516–1519. Neuroscience, 5, 254–261.
Brandt, J., Strauss, M. E., Larus, J., Jensen, B., & Chatterjee, A., Anderson, K. E., Moskowitz, C. B.,
Folstein, S. E. (1984). Clinical correlates of demen- Hauser, W. A., & Marder, K. S. (2005). A com-
tia and disability in Huntington’s disease. Journal parison of self-report and caregiver assessment of
of Clinical Neuropsychology, 6, 401–412. depression, apathy, and irritability in Huntington’s
Brown, R. G., Redondo-Verge, L., Chacon, J. R., disease. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical
Lucas, M. L., & Channon, S. (2001). Dissociation Neurosciences, 17, 378–383.
between intentional and incidental sequence Chikama, M., McFarland, N. R., Amaral, D. G., &
learning in Huntington’s disease. Brain, 124, Haber, S. N. (1997). Insular cortical projections to
2188–2202. functional regions of the striatum correlate with
Burns, A., Folstein, S., Brandt, J., & Folstein, M. cortical cytoarchitechtonic organization in the pri-
(1990). Clinical assessment of irritability, aggres- mate. Journal of Neuroscience, 1997, 17, 9686–9705.
sion, and apathy in Huntington’s and Alzheimer’s Ciarmiello, A., Cannella, M., Lastoria, S., Simonelli,
disease. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, M., Frati, L., Rubinsztein, D. C., et al. (2006).
178, 20–26. Brain white-matter volume loss and glucose hypo-
Butters, N. (1984). The clinical aspects of memory metabolism precede the clinical symptoms of
disorders: Contributions from experimental stud- Huntington’s disease. Journal of Nuclear Medicine,
ies of amnesia and dementia. Journal of Clinical 47, 215–222.
Neuropsychology, 6, 17–36. Claes, S., Van Zand, K., Legius, E., Dom, R., Malfroid,
Butters, N., Granholm, E., Salmon, D. P., Grant, I., M., Baro, F., et al. (1995). Correlations between
& Wolfe, J. (1987). Episodic and semantic mem- triplet repeat expansion and clinical features in
ory: A comparison of amnesic and demented Huntington’s disease. Archives of Neurology, 113,
patients. Journal of Clinical and Experimental 749–753.
Neuropsychology, 9, 479–497. Codori, A. M., & Brandt, J. (1994). Psychological costs
Butters, N., Sax, D., Montgomery, K., & Tarlow, and benefits of predictive testing for Huntington’s
S. (1978). Comparison of the neuropsychologi- disease. American Journal of Medical Genetics
cal deficits associated with early and advanced (Neuropsychiatric Genetics), 54, 174–184.
Huntington’s disease. Archives of Neurology, 35, Codori, A. M., Hanson, R., & Brandt, J. (1994). Self-
585–589. selection in predictive testing for Huntington’s
Butters, N., Wolfe, J., Granholm, E., & Martone, M. disease. American Journal of Medical Genetics
(1986). An assessment of verbal recall, recognition (Neuropsychiatric Genetics), 54, 167–173.
and fluency abilities in patients with Huntington’s Codori, A. M., Slavney, P. R., Young, C.,
disease. Cortex, 22, 11–31. Miglioretti, D. L., & Brandt, J. (1997). Predictors
Bylsma, F. W., Brandt, J., & Strauss, M. E. (1992). of Psychological adjustment to genetic testing
Personal and extrapersonal orientation in for Huntington’s disease. Health Psychology, 16,
Huntington’s disease and those at risk. Cortex, 28, 36–50.
113–122. Damasio, A. R. (1996). The somatic marker hypoth-
Campbell, M. C., Stout, J. C., & Finn, P. R. (2004). esis and the possible functions of the prefrontal
Reduced autonomic responsiveness to gambling cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal
task losses in Huntington’s disease. Journal of Society of London (Biol.), 1413–1420.
Huntington’s Disease 235

David, A. S., Jeste, D. V., Folstein, M. F., & Folstein, Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975).
S. E. (1987). Voluntary movement dysfunction in “Mini-Mental State”: A practical method for grad-
Huntington’s disease and tardive dyskinesia. Acta ing the cognitive state of patients for the clinician.
Neurologica Scandinavica, 75, 130–139. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 2, 189–198.
Davis, J. D., Filoteo, J. V., Kesner, R. P., & Roberts, J. W. Folstein, S. E. (1989). Huntington’s disease: A disor-
(2003). Recognition memory for hand positions der of families. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins
and spatial locations in patients with Huntington’s University Press.
disease: Differential visuospatial memory impair- Folstein, S. E., Abbott, M. H., Chase, G. A., Jensen,
ment? Cortex, 39, 239–253. B. A., & Folstein, M. F. (1983). The association of
Davis, J. D., Filoteo, J. V., Rilling, L. M., & Roberts, affective disorder with Huntington’s Disease in a
J. W. (1999). Rate of forgetting for visual spatial case series and in families. Psychological Medicine,
information in patients with Huntington’s disease. 13, 537–542.
Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 14, 635–636. Folstein, S. E., Jensen, B., Leigh, R. J., & Folstein, M. F.
de Boo, G. M., Tibben, A., Hermans, J., Jennekens- (1983). The measurement of abnormal move-
Schinkel, A., Maat-Kievit, A., & Roos, R. A. C. ment: Methods developed for Huntington dis-
(1999). Memory and learning are not impaired ease. Neurobehavioral Toxicology and Teratology,
in presymptomatic individuals with an increased 5, 605–609.
risk of Huntington’s disease. Journal of Clinical Foroud, T., Siemers, E., Kleindorfer, B. S., Bill, D. J.,
and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21, 831–836. Hodes, M. E., Norton, J. A., et al. (1995). Cognitive
de Boo, G. M., Tibben, A., Lanser, J. B., Jennekens- scores in carriers of Huntington’s disease gene
Schinkel, A., Hermans, J., Maat-Kievit, A., et al. compared to noncarriers. Annals of Neurology, 37,
(1997). Early cognitive and motor symptoms in 657–664.
identified carriers of the gene for Huntington dis- Gabrieli, J. D., Stebbins, G. T., Singh, J., Willingham,
ease. Archives of Neurology, 54, 1353–1357. D. B., & Goetz, C. G. (1997). Intact mirror-trac-
Delis, D. C., Massman, P. J., Butters, N., Salmon, D. P., ing and impaired rotary-pursuit skill learning
Cermak, L. S., & Kramer, J. H. (1991). Profi les of in patient’s with Huntington’s disease: Evidence
demented and amnesic patients on the California for dissociable memory systems in skill learning.
Verbal Learning Test: Implications for the Neuropsychology, 11, 272–281.
assessment of memory disorders. Psychological Georgiou, N., Phillips, J. G., Bradshaw, J. L.,
Assessment, 3, 19–26. Cunnington, R., & Chiu, E. (1997). Impairments
Duyao, M. P., Ambrose, C. M., & Myers, R. H. (1993). of movement kinematics in patients with
Trinucleotide repeat length: instability and age of Huntington’s disease: A comparison with and
onset in Huntington’s disease. Nature Genetics, 4, without a concurrent task. Movement Disorders,
387–392. 12, 386–396.
Fedio, P., Cox, C. S., Neophytides, A., Canal-Frederick, Golding, C. V., Danchaivijitr, C., Hodgson, T. L.,
G., & Chase, T. N. (1979). Neuropsychological Tabrizi, S. J., & Kennard, C. (2006). Identification
profi le of Huntington’s disease. In T. N. Chase, of an oculomotor biomarker of preclinical
N. S. Wexler, & A. Barbeau (Eds.), Advances Huntington disease. Neurology, 67, 485–487.
in neurology, Vol. 23: Huntington’s disease Gómez-Tortosa, E., del Barrio, A., Barroso, T., &
(pp. 239–255). New York: Raven Press. García Ruiz, P. J. (1996). Visual processing dis-
Feigin, A., Leenders, K. L., Moeller, J. R., Missimer, orders in patients with Huntington’s disease and
J., Kuenig, G., Spetsieris, P., et al. (2001). Metabolic asymptomatic carriers. Journal of Neurology, 243,
network abnormalities in early Huntington’s dis- 286–292.
ease: An 18F fluorodeoxyglucose PET study. Journal Gray, J. M., Young, A. W., Barker, W. A., Curtis, A.,
of Nuclear Medicine, 42, 1591–1595. & Gibson, D. (1997). Impaired recognition of dis-
Filoteo, J. V., Delis, D. C., Roman, M., Demedura, gust in Huntington’s disease carriers. Brain, 120,
T., Ford, E., Butters, N., et al. (1995). Visual atten- 2029–2038.
tion and perception in patients with Huntington’s Graybiel, A. M., Aosaki, T., Flaherty, A. W., &
disease: Comparisons with other subcortical Kimura, M. (1994). The basal ganglia and adaptive
and cortical dementias. Journal of Clinical and motor control. Science, 265, 1826–1831.
Experimental Neuropsychology, 17, 654–667. Gusella, J. F., Wexler, N. S., Conneally, P. M., Naylor,
Filoteo, J. V., Maddox, W. T., & Davis, J. D. (2001). A S. L., Anderson, M. A., Tanzi, R. E., et al. (1983).
possible role of the striatum in linear and nonlin- A polymorphic DNA marker genetically linked to
ear category learning: Evidence from patients with Huntington’s disease. Nature, 306, 234–238.
Huntington’s disease. Behavioral Neuroscience, Hahn-Barma, V., Deweer, B., Durr, A., Dode, C.,
115, 786–798. Feingold, J., Pillon, B., et al. (1998). Are cognitive
236 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

changes the first symptoms of Huntington’s disease? phonemic and semantic clustering and switching.
A study of gene carriers. Journal of Neurology, Neuropsychologia, 40, 1277–1284.
Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 64, 172–177. Hodges, J. R., Salmon, D. P., & Butters, N. (1990).
Hamilton, J. M., Salmon, D. P. Corey-Bloom, J., Differential impairment of semantic and episodic
Gamst, A., Paulsen, J. S., Jerkins, S., et al. (2003). memory in Alzheimer’s and Huntington’s dis-
Behavioral abnormalities contribute to func- eases: A controlled prospective study. Journal
tional decline in Huntington’s disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 53,
of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 74, 1089–1095.
120–122. Hodges, J. R., Salmon, D. P., & Butters, N. (1991). The
Harper, P. S. (1996). Huntington disease. London: nature of the naming deficit in Alzheimer’s and
W.B. Saunders. Huntington’s disease. Brain, 114, 1547–1558.
Harris, G. J., Codori, A. M., Lewis, R. F., Schmidt, E., Huntington’s Disease Collaborative Research
Bedi, A., & Brandt, J. (1999). Reduced basal gan- Group. (1993). A novel gene containing a trinu-
glia blood flow and volume in presymptomatic, cleotide repeat that is expanded and unstable
gene-tested persons at risk for Huntington’s dis- on Huntington’s disease chromosomes. Cell, 72,
ease. Brain, 122, 1667–1678. 971–983.
Hayden, M. R. (1981). Huntington’s chorea. New Illarioshkin, S. N., Igarashi, S., Onodera, O.,
York: Springer-Verlag. Markova, E. D., Nikolskaya, N. N., Tanaka, H.,
Hayes, C. J., Stevenson, R. J., & Coltheart, M. et al. (1994). Trinucleotide repeat length and rate
(2007). Disgust and Huntington’s disease. of progression of Huntington’s disease. Annals of
Neuropsychologia, 45, 1135–1151. Neurology, 36, 630–635.
Hefter, H., Hömberg, V., Lange, H. W., & Freund, Jacobs, D. H., Shuren, J., & Heilman, K. M. (1995).
H. (1987). Impairment of rapid movement in Impaired perception of facial identity and facial
Huntington’s disease. Brain, 110, 585–612. affect in Huntington’s disease. Neurology, 46,
Heindel, W. C., Butters, N., & Salmon, D. P. (1988). 1217–1218.
Impaired learning of a motor skill in patients with Josiassen, R. C., Curry, L., Roemer, R. A., DeBease,
Huntington’s disease. Behavioral Neuroscience, C., & Mancall, E. L. (1982). Patterns of intellectual
102, 141–147. deficit in Huntington’s disease. Journal of Clinical
Hennenlotter, A., Schroeder, U., Erhard, P., Haslinger, Neuropsychology, 4, 173–183.
B., Stahl, R., Weindl, A., et al. (2004). Neural corre- Kieburtz, K., MacDonald, M., Shih, C., Feigin,
lates associated with impaired disgust processing A., Steinberg, K., Bordwell, K., et al. (1994).
in pre-symptomatic Huntington’s disease. Brain, Trinucleotide repeat length and progression of ill-
127, 1446–1453. ness in Huntington’s disease. Journal of Medical
Henry, J. D., Crawford, J. R., & Phillips, L. H. (2005). Genetics, 31, 872–874.
A eta-analytic review of verbal fluency deficits Kim, J. S., Reading, S. A. J., Brashers-Krug, T.,
in Huntington’s disease. Neuropsychology, 19, Calhoun, V. D., Ross, C. A., & Pearlson, G. D.
243–252. (2004). Functional MRI study of a serial reac-
Hinton, S. C., Paulsen, J. S., Hoff man, R. G., tion time task in Huntington’s disease. Psychiatry
Reynolds, N. C., Zimbelman, J. L., & Rao, S. M. Research: Neuroimaging, 131, 23–30.
(2007). Motor timing variability increases in pre- Kirkwood, S. C., Siemers, E., Stout, J. C., Hodes, M.
clinical Huntington’s disease patients as estimated E., Conneally, P. M., Christian, J C., et al. (1999).
onset of motor symptoms approaches. Journal of Longitudinal cognitive and motor changes among
the International Neuropsychological Society, 13, presymptomatic Huntington disease gene carri-
539–543. ers. Archives of Neurology, 56, 563–568.
Ho, A. K., Sahakian, B. J., Brown, R. G., Barker, R. Kirkwood, S. C., Siemers, E., Hodes, M. E., Conneally,
A., Hodges, J. R., Ane, M. N., et al. (2003). Profi le P. M., Christian, J. C., & Foroud, T. (2000). Subtle
of cognitive progression in early Huntington’s dis- changes among presymptomatic carriers of the
ease. Neurology, 61, 1702–1706. Huntington’s disease gene. Journal of Neurology,
Ho, A. K., Sahakian, B. J., Robbins, T. W., & Barker, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 69, 773–779.
R. A. (2004). Random number generation in Knopman, D., & Nissen, M. J. (1991). Procedural
patients with symptomatic and presymptomatic learning is impaired in Huntington’s disease:
Huntington’s disease. Cognitive and Behavioral Evidence from the serial reaction time task.
Neurology, 17, 208–212. Neuropsychologia, 29, 245–254.
Ho, A. K., Sahakian, B. J., Robbins, T. W., Barker, R. A., Knowlton, B. J., Squire, L. R., Paulsen, J. S., Swerdlow,
Rosser, A. E., & Hodges, J. R. (2002). Verbal fluency N. R., Swenson, M., & Butters, N. (1996).
in Huntington’s disease: A longitudinal analysis of Dissociations within nondeclarative memory
Huntington’s Disease 237

in Huntington’s disease. Neuropsychology, 10, and phenominological study. In D. F. Benson &


538–548. D. Blumer (Eds.), Psychiatric aspects of neurologic
Kramer, J. H., Delis, D. C., Blusewicz, M. J., Brandt, disease (pp. 267–286). New York: Grune and
J., Ober, B. A., & Strauss, M. (1988). Verbal Stratton.
memory errors in Alzheimer’s and Huntington’s Meissen, G., Myers, R., Mastromauro, C., Koroshetz,
dementias. Developmental Neuropsychology, W., Klinger, K., Farrer, L. et al. (1988). Predictive
4, 1–15. testing for Huntington’s disease with use of a
Lang, C. J. G., Majer, M., Balan, P., & Reischies, F. M. linked DNA marker. New England Journal of
(2000). Recall and recognition in Huntington’s Medicine, 318, 535–542.
disease. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 15, Milders, M., Crawford, J. R., Lamb, A., & Simpson,
361–371. S. A. (2003). Differential deficits in expression
Lawrence, A. D., Hodges, J. R., Rosser, A. E., Kershaw, recognition in gene-carriers and patients with
A., ff rench-Constant, C., Rubinsztein, D. C., et al. Huntington’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 41,
(1998). Evidence for specific cognitive deficits 1484–1492.
in preclinical Huntington’s disease. Brain, 121, Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki,
1329–1341. A. H., & Howerter, A. (2000). The unity and diver-
Lawrence, A. D., Sahakian, B. J., Hodges, J. R., Rosser, sity of executive functions and their contributions
Lange, K. W., & Robbins, T. W. (1996). Executive to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable
and mnemonic functions in early Huntington’s analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.
disease. Brain, 119, 1633–1645. Mohr, E., Brouwers, P., Claus, J. J., Mann, U. M.,
Lawrence, A. D., Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. Fedio, P., & Chase, T. N. (1991). Visuospatial
(1998). Cognitive functions and corticostriatal cir- cognition in Huntington’s disease. Movement
cuits: insights from Huntington’s disease. Trends Disorders, 6, 127–132.
in Cognitive Science, 2, 379–388. Mohr, E., Claus, J. J., Jules, J., & Brouwers, P. (1997).
Lawrence, A. D., Watkins, L. H. A., Sahakian, B. J., Basal ganglia disease and visuospatial cogni-
Hodges, J. R., & Robbins, T. W. (2000). Visual tion: Are these disease-specific impairments?
object and visuospatial cognition in Huntington’s Behavioural Neurology, 10, 67–75.
disease: Implications for information processing Montagne, B., Kessels, R. P. C., Kammers, M. P. M.,
in corticostraial circuits. Brain, 123, 1349–1369. Kingma, E., de Haan, E. H. F., Roos, R. A. C., et al.
Lemiere, J., Decruyenaere, M., Evers-Kiebooms, G., (2006). Perception of emotional facial expres-
Vandenbussche, E., & Dom, R. (2002). sions at different intensities in early-symptomatic
Longitudinal study evaluating neuropsycholog- Huntington’s disease. European Neurology, 55,
ical changes in so-called asymptomatic carriers 151–154.
of the Huntington’s disease mutation after 1 year. Montoya, A., Pelletier, M., Menear, M., Duplessis, E.,
Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 106, 131–141. Richer, F., & Lepage, M. (2006). Episodic memory
Mahant, N., McCusker, E. A., Byth, K., Graham, S., & impairment in Huntington’s disease: A meta-
the Huntington Study Group. (2003). Huntington’s analysis. Neuropsychologia, 44, 1984–1994.
disease: Clinical correlates of disability and pro- Müller, S. V., Jung, A., Preinfalk, J., Kolbe, H., Ridao-
gression. Neurology, 61, 1085–1092. Alonso, M., Dengler, R., et al. (2002). Disturbance
Martone, M., Butters, N., & Trauner, D. (1986). Some of “extrinsic alertness” in Huntington’s dis-
analyses of forgetting of pictorial material in ease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
amnesic and demented patients. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 24, 517–526.
and Experimental Neuropsychology, 8, 161–178. Murray, L. L., & Lenz, L. P. (2001). Productive syntax
Martone, M., Butters, N., Payne, M., Becker, J., & abilities in Huntington’s and Parkinson’s diseases.
Sax, D. S. (1984). Dissociations between skill Brain and Cognition, 46, 213–219.
learning and verbal recognition in amnesia and Murray, L. L., & Stout, J. C. (1999). Discourse com-
dementia. Archives of Neurology, 41, 965–970. prehension in Huntington’s and Parkinson’s
Mattis, S. (1988). Dementia Rating Scale Professional diseases. American Journal of Speech-Language
Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Pathology, 8, 137–148.
Resources, Inc. Nance, M. A., & Myers, R. H. (2001). Juvenile onset
Mayeux, R., Stern, Y., Herman, A., Greenbaum, L., Huntington’s disease—clinical and research per-
& Fahn, S. (1986). Correlates of early disability spectives. Mental Retardation and Developmental
in Huntington disease. Annals of Neurology, 20, Disabilities Research Reviews, 7, 153–157.
727–731. Nehl, C., Paulsen, J. S., & the Huntington Study
McHugh, P. R., & Folstein, M. F. (1975). Psychiatric Group. (2004). Cognitive and psychiatric aspects
syndromes of Huntington’s chorea: A clinical of Huntington’s disease contribute to functional
238 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

capacity. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, Ranen, N. G., Stine, O. C., Abbott, M. H., Sherr, M.,
192, 72–74. Codori, A. M., Franz, M. L., et al. (1995).
Paulsen, J. S., Butters, N., Salmon, D. P., Heindel, Anticipation and instability of IT-15 (CAG)
W. C., & Swenson, M. R. (1993). Prism adapta- repeats in parent–offspring pairs with Huntington
tion in Alzheimer’s and Huntington’s disease. disease. American Journal of Human Genetics, 57,
Neuropsychology, 7, 73–81. 593–602.
Paulsen, J. S., Como, P., Rey, G., Bylsma, F., Jones, Reading, S., Dziorny, A. C., Peroutka, L. A., Schrieber,
R., Saint-Cyr, J. et al. (1996). The clinical util- M., Gourley, L. M., Yallapragada, V., et al. (2004).
ity of the Stroop test in a multicenter study of Functional brain changes in presymptomatic
Huntington’s disease. Journal of the International Huntington’s disease. Annals of Neurology, 55,
Neuropsychological Society, 2, 35. 879–883.
Paulsen, J. S., Nehl, C., Hoth, K. F., Kanz, J. E., Reading, S. A. J., Yassa, M. A., Bakker, A., Dziorny, A. C.,
Benjamin, M., Conybeare, R., et al. (2005). Gourley, L. M., Yallapragada, V., et al. (2005).
Depression and stages of Huntington’s dis- Regional white matter change in pre-symptomatic
ease. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Huntington’s disease: A diff usion tensor imaging
Neurosciences, 17, 496–502. study. Psychiatric Research: Neuroimaging, 140,
Paulsen, J. S., Ready, R. E., Hamilton, J. M., Mega, M. S., 55–62.
& Cummings, J. L. (2001a). Neuropsychiatric aspects Reynolds, N. C., Jr., Hellman, R. S., Tikofsky, R. S.,
of Huntington’s disease. Journal of Neurology, Prost, R. W., Mark, L. P., Elejalde, B. R., et al.
Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 71, 310–314. (2002). Single photon emission computerized
Paulsen, J. S., Zhao, H., Stout, J. C., Brinkman, R. R., tomography (SPECT) in detecting neurodegen-
Guttman, M., Ross, C. A., et al. (2001b). Clinical eration in Huntington’s disease. Nuclear Medicine
markers of early disease in persons near onset of Commununications, 23, 13–18.
Huntington’s disease. Neurology, 57, 658–662. Rich, J. B., Troyer, A. K., Bylsma, F. W., & Brandt, J.
Paulsen, J. S., Zimbelman, J. L., Hinton, S. C., (1999). Longitudinal analysis of phonemic clus-
Langbehn, D. R., Leveroni, C. L., Benjamin, M. L., tering and switching during word list genera-
et al. (2004). fMRI biomarker of early neuronal tion in Huntington’s disease. Neuropsychology,
dysfunction in presymptomatic Huntington’s 13, 525–531.
Disease. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 25, Rogers, R. D., Owen, A. M., Middleton, H. C.,
1715–1721. Williams, E. J., Pickard, J. D., Sahakian, B. J., et al.
Paulsen, J. S., Butters, N., Sadek, J. R., Johnson, S. A., (1999). Choosing between small, likely rewards
Salmon, D. P., Swerdlow, N. R., et al. (1995). and large, unlikely rewards activates inferior and
Distinct cognitive profi les of cortical and subcor- orbital prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience,
tical dementia in advanced illness. Neurology, 45, 20, 9029–9038.
951–956. Rogers, R. D., Sahakian, B. J., Hodges, J. R., Polkey,
Pillon, B., Deweer, B., Agid, Y., & Dubois, B. (1993). C. E., Kennard, C., & Robbins, T. W. (1998).
Explicit memory in Alzheimer’s, Huntington’s, Dissociating executive mechanisms of task con-
and Parkinson’s diseases. Archives of Neurology, trol following frontal lobe damage and Parkinson’s
50, 374–379. disease. Brain, 121, 815–842.
Potegal, M. (1971). A note on spatial-motor deficits Rohrer, D., Salmon, D. P., Wixted, J. T., & Paulsen, J. S.
in patients with Huntington’s disease: A test of a (1999). The disparate effects of Alzheimer’s disease
hypothesis. Neuropsychologia, 9, 233–235. and Huntington’s disease on semantic memory.
Quaid, K. A., Brandt, J., & Folstein, S. E. (1987). The Neuropsychology, 13, 381–388.
decision to be tested for Huntington’s disease. Roman, M. J., Delis, D. C., Filoteo, J. V., Demedura,
Journal of the American Medical Association, 257, T. L., Paulsen, J., Swerdlow, N. R., et al. (1998). Is
3362. there a “subcortical” profi le of attentional dysfunc-
Quaid, K. A., & Morris, M. (1993). Reluctance tion? A comparison of patients with Huntington’s
to undergo predictive testing: The case of and Parkinson’s diseases on a global–local focused
Huntington’s disease. American Journal of Medical attention task. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Genetics, 45, 41–45. Neuropsychology, 20, 873–884.
Randolph, C., Braun, A. R., Goldberg, T. E., & Rosas, H. D., Hevelone, N. D., Zaleta, A. K., Greve,
Chase, T. (1993). Semantic fluency in Alzheimer’s, D. N., Salat, D. H., & Fischl, B. (2005). Regional
Parkinson’s and Huntington’s disease: dissociation cortical thinning in preclinical Huntington dis-
of storage and retrieval failures. Neuropsychology, ease and its relationship to cognition. Neurology,
7, 82–88. 65, 745–747.
Huntington’s Disease 239

Rosenberg, N. K., Sorensen, S. A., & Christensen, A. L. in Huntington’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 45,
(1995). Neuropsychological characteristics of 1767–1776.
Huntington’s disease carriers: a double blind Speedie, L. J., Brake, N., Folstein, S. E., & Heilman,
study. Journal of Medical Genetics, 32, 600–604. K. M. (1990). Comprehension of prosody in
Rosenblatt, A., Margolis, R. L., Becher, M. W., Huntington’s disease. Journal of neurology,
Aylward, E., Franz, M. L., Sherr, M., et al. (1998). Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 53, 607–610.
Does CAG repeat number predict rate of patho- Sprengelmeyer, R., Lange, H., & Homberg, V. (1995).
logic changes in Huntington’s disease? Annals of The pattern of attentional deficits in Huntington’s
Neurology, 44, 708–709. disease. Brain, 118, 145–152.
Rothlind, J. C., & Brandt, J. (1993). A brief assess- Sprengelmeyer, R., Schroeder, U., Young, A. W.,
ment of frontal and subcortical functions in & Epplen, J. T. (2006). Disgust in pre-clinical
dementia. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Huntington’s disease: A longitudinal study.
Neurosciences, 5, 73–77. Neuropsychologia, 44, 518–533.
Rothlind, J. C., Bylsma, F. W., Peyser, C., Folstein, S. E., Sprengelmeyer, R., Young, A. W., Calder, A. J.,
& Brandt, J. (1993). Cognitive and motor correlates Karnat, A., Lange, H., Hömberg, V., et al. (1996).
of everyday functioning in early Huntington’s dis- Loss of disgust: Perception of faces and emotions
ease. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 181, in Huntington’s disease. Brain, 119, 1647–1665.
194–199. Sprengelmeyer, R., Young, A. W., Sprengelmeyer, A.,
Saint-Cyr, J. A. (2003). Frontal-striatal circuit func- Calder, A. J. Rowland, D., Perrett, D.,
tions: Context, sequence, and consequence. et al. (1997). Recognition of facial expres-
Journal of the International Neuropsychological sions: Selective impairment of specific emo-
Society, 9, 103–128. tions in Huntington’s disease. Cognitive
Salmon, D. P., Kwo-on-Yuen, P. F., Heindel, W. C., Neuropsychology, 14, 839–879.
Butters, N., & Thal, L. J. (1989). Differentiation of Stine, O. C., Pleasant, N., Franz, M. L., Abbott, M. H.,
Alzheimer’s disease and Huntington’s disease with Folstein, S. E., & Ross, C. A. (1993). Correlation
the Dementia Rating Scale. Archives of Neurology, between onset age of Huntington’s disease and the
46, 1204–1208. length of the trinucleotide repeat in IT-15. Human
Sax, D. S., O’Donnell, B., Butters, N., Menzer, L., Molecular Genetics, 2, 1547–1549.
Montgomery, K., & Kayne, H. L. (1983). Computed Stout, J. C., Rodawalt, W. C., & Siemers, E. R. (2001a).
tomographic, neurologic, and neuropsychologic Risky decision making in Huntington’s disease.
correlates of Huntington’s disease. International Journal of the International Neuropsychological
Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 21–36. Society, 7, 92–101.
Schretlen, D. (1997). Brief Test of Attention Stout, J. C., Wylie, S. A., Simone, P. M., & Siemers,
Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological E. R. (2001b). Influence of competing distractors
Assessment Resources, Inc. on response selection in Huntington’s disease and
Schretlen, D., Brandt, J., & Bobholz, J. H. (1996). Parkinson’s disease. Cognitive Neuropsychology,
Validation of the Brief Test of Attention in patients 18, 643–653.
with Huntington’s disease and amnesia. The Strauss, M. E., & Brandt, J. (1986). Attempt at
Clinical Neuropsychologist 10, 90–95. pre-clinical identification of Huntington’s dis-
Shelton, P. A., & Knopman, D. S. (1991). Ideomotor ease using the WAIS. Journal of Clinical and
apraxia in Huntington’s disease. Archives of Experimental Neuropsychology, 8, 210–218.
Neurology, 48, 35–41. Tranel, D., Bechara, A., & Damasio, A. R. (2000).
Snowden, J., Craufurd, D., Griffiths, H., Thompson, Decision making and the somatic marker hypoth-
J., & Neary, D. (2001). Longitudinal evaluation esis. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The new cogni-
of cognitive disorder in Huntington’s disease. tive neurosciences, 2nd edition (pp. 1047–1061).
Journal of the International Neuropsychological Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Society, 7, 33–44. Troyer, A. K., Moscovitch, M., Winocur, G., Alexander,
Snowden, J. S., Craufurd, D., Thompson, J., & M. P., & Stuss, D. (1998). Clustering and switch-
Neary, D. (2002). Psychomotor, executive, and ing on verbal fluency: The effects of focal frontal
memory function in preclinical Huntington’s and temporal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia, 36,
disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental 449–504.
Neuropsychology, 24, 133–145. van Oostrom, J. C. H., Maguire, R. P., Verschuuren-
Solomon, A. C., Stout, J. C., Johnson, S. A., Langbehn, Bemelmans, C. C., Veenma-van der Duin, L.,
D. R., Aylward, E. A., Brandt, J., et al. (2007). Verbal Pruim, J., Roos, R. A. C., et al. (2005). Striatal
episodic memory declines prior to diagnosis dopamine D2 receptors, metabolism, and volume
240 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

in preclinical Huntington disease. Neurology, 65, Wiggins, S., Whyte, P., Huggins, M., Adams, S.,
941–943. Theilmann, J., Bloch, M., et al. (1992). The psy-
van Zomeran, A. H., & Brouwer, W. H. (1994). chological consequences of predictive testing for
Clinical neuropsychology of attention. New York: Huntington’s disease. New England Journal of
Oxford University Press. Medicine, 327, 1401–1405.
Wahlin, T. B. R., Lundin, A., & Dear, K. (2007). Willingham, D. B., & Koroshetz, W. J. (1993). Evidence
Early cognitive deficits in Swedish gene carriers of of dissociable motor skills in Huntington’s disease
Huntington’s disease. Neuropsychology, 21, 31–44. patients. Psychobiology, 21, 173–182.
Wang, K., Hoosain, R., Yang, R. M., Meng, Y., & Willingham, D. B., Koroshetz, W. J., & Peterson,
Wang, C. Q. (2003). Impairment of recognition of E. W. (1996). Motor skills have diverse neu-
disgust in Chinese with Huntington’s or Wilson’s ral bases: Spared and impaired skill acquisition
disease. Neuropsychologia, 41, 527–537. in Huntington’s disease. Neuropsychology, 10,
Ward, J., Sheppard, J. M., Shpritz, B., Margolis, R. L., 315–321.
Rosenblatt, A., & Brandt, J. (2006). A four-year Witjes-Ané, M. N., Vegter-van der Vlis, M., van
prospective study of cognitive functioning in Vugt, J. P., Lanser, J. B., Hermans, J., Zwinderman,
Huntington’s disease. Journal of the International A. H., et al. (2003). Cognitive and motor func-
Neuropsychological Society, 12, 445–454. tioning in gene carriers for Huntington’s disease:
Warrington, K. K., & James, M. (1991). Visual object A baseline study. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and
and space perception battery. Bury St. Edmunds, Clinical Neurosciences, 15, 7–16.
U.K.: Thames Valley Test Company. Yeterian, E. H., & Pandya, D. N. (1993). Striatal con-
Watkins, L. H., Rogers, R. D., Lawrence, A. D., nections of the parietal association cortices in rhe-
Sahakian, B. J., Rosser, A. E., & Robbins, T. W. sus monkeys. Journal of Comparative Neurology,
(2000). Impaired planning but intact deci- 332, 175–197.
sion-making in early Huntington’s disease: Zakzanis, K. K. (1998). The subcortical dementia
Implications for specific frontal-striatal pathol- of Huntington’s disease. Journal of Clinical and
ogy. Neuropsychologia, 38, 1112–1125. Experimental Neuropsychology, 20, 565–578.
11

The Neuropsychiatry and Neuropsychology


of Gilles de la Tourette Syndrome
Mary M. Robertson and Andrea E. Cavanna

In this chapter, we review the neuropsychiat- 2004). Of importance is that these studies were
ric and neuropsychological literature on Gilles worldwide (Sweden [two], United Kingdom,
de la Tourette Syndrome (GTS), which has Japan, United States of America [two], Taiwan
expanded considerably over the last few years. [two], Italy, mainland China), and they were
Comorbid attention-deficit hyperactivity disor- similar in that they were conducted in main-
der (ADHD), obsessive-compulsive behaviors stream schools, used standard multistaged
(OCB) and/or disorder (OCD), and depression, methods, with both observations and ques-
are encountered more frequently, and will be tionnaires about pupils, as well as obtaining
discussed in more detail. information from parents and/or teachers. It
is acknowledged that in majority of the “cases”
identified, the symptoms are probably mild and
Definition, Epidemiology,
undiagnosed, fulfi lling current diagnostic crite-
Historical Notes
ria but unlikely to cause distress or impairment.
The current internationally recognized diagnos- The prevalence of GTS in special educational
tic criteria for GTS include the presence of mul- populations, such as those individuals with
tiple motor tics and one or more vocal (phonic) mental retardation and/or learning difficulties
tics, both of which must exceed a year’s dura- or autistic spectrum disorders, is even higher
tion (ICD-10, World Health Organization, 1992; (e.g., Baron-Cohen et al., 1999; Eapen et al.,
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental 1997; Kurlan et al., 2001).
Disorders, 4th edition-Text Revision [DSM- The first clear medical description of GTS was
IV-TR], American Psychiatric Association, made in 1825, when Itard reported the medical
2000). The anatomical location, number, fre- history of a French noblewoman, Marquise de
quency, complexity and severity of the tics char- Dampierre (Itard, 1825), whose case was then
acteristically change over time. GTS was once re-documented by Georges Gilles de la Tourette
considered to be very uncommon, but to date in 1885 when he described nine cases of GTS,
no less than ten definitive studies have docu- emphasizing the triad of multiple tics, coprolalia
mented remarkably consistent findings and (unprovoked, inappropriate swearing), and echo-
suggested a prevalence of between 0.46% and lalia (imitating the speech of others) (Gilles de
1.76% for youngsters between the ages of 5 and la Tourette, 1885). Of note is that Marquise also
18 years (Comings et al., 1990; Hornsey et al., manifested symptoms of OCD in addition to a tic
2001; Kadesjo & Gillberg, 2000; Khalifa & von disorder. We will return to discuss the association
Knorring, 2003; Kurlan et al., 2001; Lanzi et al., between GTS and OCD later in the chapter.
2004; Nomoto & Machiyama, 1990; Wang & A convincing case has been made (McHenry,
Kuo, 2003; Wong & Lau, 1992; Zheng et al., 1967; Murray, 1979) that Dr. Samuel Johnson

241
242 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

suffered from GTS with multiple motor tics and certainly more extensive investigation of cop-
a variety of vocal tics including “ejaculations per handling is not necessary. Standardized
of the Lord’s prayer,” sounds like the clucking schedules are mandatory for research and
of a hen and a whale exhaling (Boswell, 1867; are useful for accurately diagnosing GTS and
McHenry, 1967; Murray, 1979). He also exhib- assessing the response to medication. There
ited echolalia, mild self-injurious behavior. It are many standardized rating scales or sched-
has been suggested that Dr. Johnson suffered ules that help in the more accurate description
from severe OCD in addition to his motor of symptoms, including the National Hospital
and vocal tics. He felt impelled to measure his Interview Schedule (Robertson & Eapen, 1996),
footsteps, perform complex gestures when he the Yale Global Tic Severity Scale (Leckman
crossed a threshold, and involuntarily touch et al., 1989), the MOVES Scale (Gaff ney, 1994),
specific objects (Murray, 1979). The history of the Hopkins Motor and Vocal Tic Severity Scale
GTS has been elegantly reviewed in the schol- (Walkup et al., 1992), the Tourette’s syndrome
arly exposition by Kushner (1995). videotaped scale (Goetz et al., 1987), and the
Diagnostic Confidence Index (Robertson et al.,
1999), which specifically highlight the phe-
Neurology
nomenological characteristics of tics, including
In a large group of patients with GTS exam- suppression, rebound, suggestibility, waxing
ined by Shapiro et al. (1978), subtle neurolog- and waning course, and premonitory urges. For
ical deficits were found in 57%, and 20% were implementing majority of these scales, familiar-
left-handed or ambidextrous. Most (78%) had ity with Tourette’s syndrome, as well as train-
minor motor asymmetry and 20% had chorea ing by an expert, is important. Many of these
or choreoathetoid movements. Other abnor- scales help not only in accurate diagnosis (e.g.,
malities included posturing, poor coordination, National Hospital Interview Schedule) but also
nystagmus, reflex asymmetry, and unilateral give a likelihood of diagnosis (e.g., Diagnostic
Babinski reflexes. In contrast, Lees et al. (1984), Confidence Index), indicate severity (e.g., Yale
using a standardized handedness questionnaire Global Tic Severity Scale, MOVES Scale), and
(Annett, 1970), found 87% of their sample of 53 can be used in research protocols (e.g., Tourette’s
patients to be right-handed, and other inves- syndrome videotaped scale).
tigators have found minor nonspecific neu-
rological abnormalities only in a few patients
Etiology
(Caine et al., 1988; Erenberg et al., 1986; Lees
et al., 1984; Regeur et al., 1986; Robertson et al., Current etiological theories for GTS include (1)
1988). Abnormalities documented have included genetic influences, (2) infections, (3) pre- and/
chorea, dystonia, torticollis, dysphonia, dys- or perinatal difficulties, (4) psychosocial stress,
diadochokinesia, postural abnormalities, reflex and (5) androgen exposure. Originally, how-
asymmetry, and motor incoordination. ever, the etiology of GTS was considered to be
psychological and indeed even psychoanalytical
(e.g., Ferenczi, 1921; Mahler, 1949), but over the
Assessment
last few years large families were documented
Assessment of patients with GTS requires a (e.g., Kurlan et al., 1986; McMahon et al.,
thorough personal and family history, as well 1996; Robertson & Cavanna, 2007; Robertson &
as mental state and neurological examina- Gourdie, 1990; Walkup et al., 1996) with many
tions. In terms of clinical diagnosis, special related people being affected by tic or obses-
investigations are not useful if the assessor sive-compulsive symptomatology, suggesting a
is experienced with the syndrome. Although familial nature.
the exclusion of Wilson’s disease with serum With regard to genetics, complex segrega-
assay of copper and caeruloplasmin is consid- tion analyses of family history data provided
ered mandatory, in the authors’ experience, no strong evidence that the mode of transmission
patient presenting with this distinctive picture was compatible with an autosomal dominant
of GTS has actually had Wilson’s disease and model (Curtis et al., 1992; Eapen et al., 1993),
The Neuropsychiatry and Neuropsychology of Gilles de la Tourette Syndrome 243

but subsequent segregation analyses suggested (ABGAs) in some patients with GTS, supporting
that the mode of inheritance may be more com- a role of GABHS and basal ganglia autoimmu-
plex (Pauls, 2006). While the mode of inher- nity in GTS (see Leonard & Swedo, 2001; Rizzo
itance is not simple, it is clear that GTS has a et al., 2006), while other groups have failed to
significant genetic basis and that some individ- confirm these findings (Loiselle et al., 2004;
uals with GTS, Chronic Multiple Tics (CMT), Luo et al., 2004). It seems unlikely that GABHS
and/or OCD manifest variant expressions of the infections directly cause GTS, but it may well
same genetic susceptibility factors (Pauls, 2003). be that individuals inherit a susceptibility to
Recently dopamine receptor D2 gene polymor- GTS and to the way they react to some infec-
phisms were shown to be associated with GTS tions. Thus, most authors suggest that there is
in some individuals (Lee et al., 2005). There an association between streptococcal infections
have now been five complete genome scans and GTS in a subgroup of patients.
reported in GTS and regions of interest have Leckman (2003 [review]) outlined the poten-
been shown to be on chromosomes 2, 4, 5, 7, 8, tial role of pre- and perinatal events in the path-
10, 11, 13, 17, 18, 19, as well as suggestions that ogenesis of GTS, suggesting that the mothers of
the DRD4 and MAO-A genes may also confer an children with tics were more likely to have expe-
increased risk for developing GTS (Barr et al., rienced a complication during pregnancy than
1999; Robertson, 2004 [review]) but no signif- the mothers of children who did not have tics,
icant linkage results have been obtained and that the severity of maternal life stress during
replicated. Most recently, Abelson et al. (2005) pregnancy (including severe nausea and/or vom-
reported the association of GTS with the gene iting during the first trimester) are risk factors
SLITRK1 on chromosome 13q31.1 in a small for developing tic disorders, and that premature
number of individuals with GTS, which is the low-birth-weight children as well as those with
first indication of a gene being involved in some low Apgar scores and more frequent maternal
cases of GTS. Thus, the genetics of GTS is com- prenatal visits were associated with having GTS
plex and there is almost certainly genetic as well (see also Leckman et al., 1990; Lees et al., 1984;
as clinical heterogeneity. Santangelo et al., 1994). A rigorous controlled
Deng et al. (2006) and Keen-Kim et al. (2006), study by Burd et al. (1999) involving 92 cases
on the other hand, have provided evidence that and 460 matched controls identified one cate-
mutations in the SLITRK1 gene probably are a gorical variable (trimester prenatal care begun)
rare cause of GTS and tests designed to detect and three continuous variables (Apgar score at
variants in the SLITRK1 gene do not have diag- 5 minutes, month prenatal began and number
nostic utility in clinical practice. of prenatal visits) as potential risk factors for the
Neuroimmunological theories operating via development of GTS. However, in a more recent
the process of molecular mimicry have become study, when 25 GTS children were compared to
of interest in the etiopathogenesis of GTS over 25 healthy controls, there was a nonsignificant
the last decade. Swedo et al. (1998) described increase with regards to the “reduced optimal-
a group of 50 children with OCD and tic dis- ity score” (a measure which takes into account
orders, designated as Pediatric Autoimmune pregnancy, obstetrical, and other relevant medi-
Neuropsychiatric Disorders Associated with cal records) in the pre-, peri- or neonatal periods
Streptococcal (group A beta-hemolytic strepto- (Khalifa & von Knorring, 2005).
coccal [GABHS] infections), or PANDAS. The Finally, clinical experience suggests an asso-
best evidence for a relationship between GTS ciation between stressful life events and fluctua-
and streptococcal infections comes from the tions in symptom severity of tic disorder patients
study by Mell et al. (2005). These authors found (see e.g., Hoekstra et al., 2004; Lin et al., 2007),
that patients with GTS, OCD, or tic disorder whilst Leckman and his group have raised the
were more likely than controls to have had prior hypothesis that androgen exposure (“prena-
streptococcal infection in the 3 months before tal masculinization of the brain”) may also be
onset date. A few other controlled studies have important in the etiopathogenesis of GTS and
found laboratory evidence of GABHS infections tic-related disorders (Alexander & Peterson,
and/or increased antibasal ganglia antibodies 2004; Peterson et al., 1994, 1998).
244 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Thus, the etiology of GTS is much more com- and single-photon emission computed tomog-
plex than previously recognized, with complex raphy (SPECT) have, with general consistency,
genetic mechanisms, some infections possibly reported hypometabolism or low regional cere-
having effects, and pre- and perinatal difficul- bral blood flow (rCBF) in basal ganglia regions
ties also affecting the phenotype. in subjects with GTS. Likewise, radioligand
studies have reported functional abnormali-
ties within the dopaminergic neurotransmit-
Neuroimaging and
ter system in the striatum (Butler et al., 2006;
Pathophysiology
Peterson, 2001). Event-related neuroimaging
The previous two decades have seen the birth studies using functional MRI (Bohlhalter et al.,
and maturation of neuroimaging investigations 2006) and PET (Lerner et al., 2007; Stern et al.,
of GTS, aimed at identifying the cerebral bases 2000) techniques investigated the neural cir-
of the disorder and to define the neural systems cuits involved in tic generation in patients with
that modulate or compensate for the presence of GTS. These studies consistently reported activa-
the core symptoms. The results of these investiga- tion in a brain network of paralimbic areas such
tions, combined with prior clinical and preclinical as anterior cingulate and insular cortex, supple-
studies have helped generate specific hypotheses mentary motor area, and parietal operculum
of the neural systems involved in GTS. predominantly before tic onset. In contrast, at
Investigations of the structure of the brain the beginning of tic action, significant activi-
using computed tomography (CT) scans ties were found in sensorimotor areas including
(Robertson et al., 1988) and magnetic reso- thalamus, putamen, caudate, primary motor
nance imaging (MRI) scans (Chase et al., 1986) cortex, superior parietal lobule bilaterally, and
essentially had not revealed any abnormali- cerebellum.
ties, until 1993, when two independent stud- Mink (2006 [review]) has recently presented
ies showed reduced basal ganglia volume with a model for the pathophysiology of tics, based
MRI (Peterson et al., 1993; Singer et al., 1993). on our understanding of basal ganglia function
In the largest neuroimaging study in GTS to and connectivity, in conjunction with known
date, Peterson et al. (2003) demonstrated that features of anatomical organization and dopa-
the caudate nucleus volumes were significantly mine neurotransmission. According to this
smaller in children and adults with GTS, while model, clusters of striatal neurons (matrisomes)
lenticular nucleus volumes were also smaller become abnormally active in inappropriate con-
in adults with GTS and in children who had texts leading to inhibition of the internal globus
comorbid OCD. Bloch et al. (2005) demon- pallidus or the substantia nigra (pars reticulata)
strated that the volumes of caudate nucleus cor- neurons that would normally be active to sup-
related significantly and inversely with severity press unwanted movements. The inhibition of
of tics and OCD in early adulthood and it was these neurons would then disinhibit thalamo-
suggested that caudate volumes could therefore cortical circuits, thus leading to the production
predict the future severity of GTS. Furthermore, of tics. Activity-dependent dopamine effects
recent MRI studies (Plessen et al., 2004, 2006) would inappropriately reinforce these activity
showed that the corpus callosum is smaller in patterns leading to stereotyped repetition. The
children and youngsters with GTS compared overactive striatal neuronal clusters may change
with healthy control subjects, resulting in an under various influences, so that the produced
altered interhemispherical white matter con- movements change over time. Although directly
nectivity. On the other hand, an increase in testable, this model requires confirmation
the corpus callosum cross-sectional area has through a valid animal model of tics or higher
been documented in adult patients with GTS resolution functional imaging techniques.
(e.g., Moriarty et al., 1997), thus suggesting In summary, the basal ganglia have consis-
plastic changes in response to long-lasting tic tently been implicated in the pathophysiology
symptomatology. of GTS. Nearly all PET and SPECT studies have
Metabolism and blood flow-related stud- demonstrated reduced metabolism or blood
ies using positron emission tomography (PET) flow to the basal ganglia in subjects with GTS
The Neuropsychiatry and Neuropsychology of Gilles de la Tourette Syndrome 245

relative to controls. Reduced flow and metab- movements including touching, hitting, jump-
olism are seen most frequently in the ventral ing, smelling of the hands or objects, spitting,
striatum and, within the striatum, most often kicking, stamping, squatting, and a variety of
in the left hemisphere. The radioligand stud- abnormalities of gait (Jankovic, 2001; Leckman,
ies have also implicated the basal ganglia in 2002; Robertson, 2000; Singer, 2005).
the pathophysiology of GTS, although with The onset of vocal tics is usually later than that
less consistency than have the metabolism of the motor tics, with a mean age of 7–11 years.
and blood flow studies. The lack of consistency The usual utterances include grunting, cough-
could be due at least in part to the complexity ing, throat-clearing, barking, yelping, snorting,
of the systems regulating dopamine metabo- explosive utterances, screaming, humming,
lism and dopamine receptor density. Therefore, hissing, clicking, colloquial emotional exclama-
the blood flow and metabolism studies, along tions, and inarticulate sounds. Coprolalia (the
with the radioligand findings, quite strongly uttering of obscenities) is reported in approx-
implicate pathophysiology of the basal ganglia imately one-third of patients and usually has
portions of the CSTC circuits, especially in or a mean age of onset of 14 years. Copropraxia
around the caudate nucleus portions of the ven- (the making of obscene gestures) is reported in
tral striatum. The basal ganglia are conduits 3%–21% of GTS patients. Echophenomena (the
for information-processing streams that serve imitation of sounds, words, or actions of oth-
multiple and diverse functions. The ventral ers) occur in 11%–44% of patients (Jankovic,
striatum, and particularly the ventral caudate 2001; Leckman, 2002; Robertson, 2000; Singer,
nucleus, tends to subserve temporolimbic and 2005).
orbitofrontal portions of CSTC circuitry. These Premonitory feelings or sensations precede
regions are thought to subserve, among other motor and vocal tics in as much as 90% of
things, impulse control, reward contingencies, patients; these may be localized sensations or
and executive functions—all of which are behav- discomforts (Banaschewski et al., 2003; Kwak
ioral systems that have been hypothesized to be et al., 2003; Leckman et al., 1993; Woods et al.,
dysfunctional in GTS -related psychopathology. 2005). Tics characteristically fluctuate (wax
The other CSTC system in which regions differ and wane), may be suppressed voluntarily, are
frequently between GTS and healthy controls is suggestible, and persist during sleep (Jankovic,
that involving the association cortices, particu- 1997). In addition to changing in severity over
larly the frontal, parietal, and superior tempo- weeks and months, both motor and phonic tics
ral regions, all of which have been consistently arise in bouts (minutes to hours) over the course
implicated in attentional functioning. Clearly of a day (Leckman, 2002).
more investigations are needed to clarify the Tics and vocalizations are characteristically
relationship between the cortical and basal gan- aggravated by anxiety, stress, boredom, fatigue,
glia findings and GTS symptoms (Frey & Albin, and excitement; while sleep, alcohol, orgasm,
2006). fever, relaxation, and concentration lead to tem-
porary disappearance of symptoms (Robertson,
2000). The influence of psychosocial stress on
Clinical Characteristics
tic expression has been thoroughly investigated
The clinical characteristics of individuals with in a prospective longitudinal study by Hoekstra
GTS appear to be independent of culture, as they et al. (2004). However, the significance of these
occur with some degree of uniformity irrespec- factors for a causal model of GTS remains
tive of the country of origin. The age of onset of unclear.
GTS symptoms ranges from 2 to 15 years, with a The long-term outcome of GTS was first sug-
mean of 7 years being commonly reported. The gested to be lifelong, illustrated perhaps best by
most frequent initial symptoms are tics involv- the marquise de Dampierre, described by both
ing the eyes (such as eye blinking), head nod- Itard (1825) and Gilles de la Tourette (1885),
ding, and facial grimacing. GTS is often referred who was still ticking in her elderly years. More
to as a tic disorder, but patients with the syn- recent follow-up studies of GTS (Bloch et al.,
drome usually exhibit a wide variety of complex 2006; Coffey et al., 2000; Erenger et al., 1987;
246 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Golden, 1984; Leckman et al., 1998) suggest that Gilles de la Tourette, Guinon, and Grasset, illus-
tics begin around 5–6 years, are worse at 10–12 trating early documentation of psychopathol-
years and by the age of 18 years many tic symp- ogy, with special reference to OCD. Subsequent
toms are reduced. In adulthood, a fortunate work has confirmed a range of disturbances in
one-third of children with GTS will be symp- patients with GTS (Corbett et al., 1969; Morphew
tom-free; another one-third will have only mild & Sim, 1969). OCD, ADHD, and affective disor-
symptom severity that does not require medi- ders—particularly depression—often represent
cal attention or pharmacological treatment. the problems for which the patient is referred to
Both Coffey et al. (2000) and Bloch et al. (2005) a physician (Robertson, 2000). In addition, anti-
showed that the psychopathology such as OCD social behavior, inappropriate sexual activity,
increase with increasing age. However, substan- exhibitionism, aggressive behavior, discipline
tial long-term follow-up studies are still needed problems, sleep disturbances, and self-injurious
to document the exact course of GTS. behaviors (SIBs) are found in a substantial per-
Majority of studies agree that GTS occurs centage of clinic GTS populations (Robertson,
three to four times more commonly in males 2000; Robertson et al., 1988, 1989). Kurlan et al.
than in females and that it is found in all social (1996) described nonobscene socially inappro-
classes, although Asam (1982) and Robertson priate behaviors in GTS that can be associated
et al. (1988) reported respectively that over 60% in one-third of patients with social difficulties
of their GTS cohorts failed to attain their paren- and seem to be related to impulse discontrol.
tal social class. Thus, although GTS is found It has been suggested (Robertson, 2000) that
in all social classes, some studies suggest that it may be useful to clinically subdivide TS into
GTS patients may well underachieve socially (1) “pure TS,” consisting primarily and almost
(Robertson et al., 1988; Sandor et al., 1990). solely of motor and phonic tics; (2) “full blown
TS” that includes coprophenomena (producing
swear words and rude gestures), echophenom-
Associated Psychopathology
ena (copying other people’s actions and words),
and Comorbidities
and paliphenomena (repeating one’s own
In an elegant epidemiological study by Khalifa actions and words); (3) “TS-plus” (originally
and von Knorring (2005), the prevalence of GTS coined by Packer, 1997), in which an individual
was 0.6% of youngsters between 7 and 15 years also has significant obsessive-compulsive symp-
old. One important finding in this study was that toms or OCD, ADHD, and SIB (e.g., Cavanna
only 8% of the GTS patients had no other diag- et al., 2006a, 2008). Others with severe psycho-
nosis, while as many as 36% had three or more pathology (e.g., depression, anxiety, personal-
other diagnoses. In a large clinic-based multi- ity disorders, and other difficult and antisocial
center study, Freeman et al. (2000) documented behaviors) may also be included in this group.
that only 12% of GTS patients had no other psy-
chopathology. Thus, both in epidemiological
ADHD and GTS
and clinical settings, the finding is remarkably
consistent that only 8%–12% of individuals with Both GTS and ADHD are common neurode-
GTS have no other psychopathology. velopmental disorders which tend to occur
Gilles de la Tourette (1899), acknowledging frequently as comorbid conditions, often
the writings of Guinon (1886), suggested that accompanied by other psychiatric disorders,
“tiqueurs” nearly always had associated psy- such as OCD and mood disorders. In general, it
chiatric disorders, especially multiple phobias, is important that a thorough assessment is con-
arithmomania and agoraphobia, habits disor- ducted to ensure that the youngster has in fact
ders, hypochondriasis, and enuresis. Grasset both GTS and ADHD. Some youngsters with
(1890) also referred to the obsessions and pho- GTS are so fidgety with their tics, and are try-
bias of patients. To him these were an accom- ing to suppress their tics, that they may appear
paniment of the tic disorder, representing to have poor concentration. ADHD and TS
“psychical” tics. Robertson and Reinstein (1991) have a close relationship (Robertson, 2006b), as
translated, for the first time, the writings of demonstrated by the high rate of comorbidity:
The Neuropsychiatry and Neuropsychology of Gilles de la Tourette Syndrome 247

as many as 60%–80% of GTS probands have those with ADHD only, on the indices of dis-
comorbid ADHD (Freeman et al., 2000; Zhu ruptive behaviors. Studies further showed that
et al., 2006). These data suggest a shared, but still children with GTS+ADHD evidenced more
unknown, neurobiological basis, and the clini- internalizing behavior problems and poorer
cal spectrum of the two disorders tends to over- social adaptation than children with GTS-only
lap. Whether tic disorders plus ADHD reflect a or controls. Of importance is that youngsters
separate entity and not merely two-coexisting with GTS-only were not significantly different
disorders, as suggested by Gillberg et al. (2004), from unaffected controls on most measures of
is still controversial. externalizing behaviors and social adaptation,
There have been both suggestions and refuta- but did have more internalizing symptoms.
tions that the two disorders are genetically related Only in one study (Rizzo et al., 2007), the GTS-
(e.g., Knell & Comings, 1993; Pauls et al., 1988). only group obtained higher scores than con-
Others have shown that there may be two types trols on “delinquent behavior,” which is to some
of GTS with ADHD: those in whom ADHD is extent in contrast to these previous findings. In
independent of GTS and those in whom ADHD summary, individuals with GTS-only appear
is secondary to GTS. Another possibility is that to be different to those with GTS+ADHD, and
‘‘pure’’ ADHD and GTS+ADHD are different this clearly has major management and prog-
phenomenologically, but the exact relationship nostic implications. In general, the ADHD
is unclear (for review see Robertson, 2006b). symptoms encountered in “pure” or “primary”
The work of the Rothenberger group has exam- ADHD are similar to the ADHD symptoms
ined the additive effect in detail. Their first study encountered in patients with GTS (Robertson,
(Yordanova et al., 1996) indicated no additive 2006b). The increased prevalence of ADHD
effect at the psychophysiological level, while the among individuals with GTS is not restricted
second (Yordanova et al., 1997) only partially to clinic populations, but has also been identi-
supported the additive model. The third (Moll fied in epidemiological and community studies
et al., 2001) did provide evidence for additive (Apter et al., 1993; Hornsey et al., 2001; Khalifa
effects at the level of motor system excitabil- & von Knorring, 2005). Thus, it appears that
ity, while their latest supported the notion that the ADHD observed in people with GTS is not
GTS+ADHD was indeed a separate nosological merely a result of referral or ascertainment bias.
entity (Yordanova et al., 2006). Thus, it would In the Apter et al. (1993) epidemiological study,
appear that the additive model is becoming ADHD in the GTS cases identified was signif-
more convincing, but these results need to be icantly elevated above the population point
replicated. prevalence of ADHD in the Israeli general pop-
Relatively recently, some research groups ulation at the time of the study.
have separated individuals with GTS into sub- Robertson (2006b) has recently reviewed the
groups, specifically separating those with and relationships between GTS and ADHD par-
without ADHD, demonstrating significant dif- ticularly as far as treatment is concerned (see
ferences. Thus, they have examined cohorts of also Robertson & Eapen, 1992) and suggested
children including children with GTS-only, that when a patient has GTS+ADHD, the cli-
and comparing them with other groups such nician should first assess which symptoms are
as GTS+ADHD, ADHD only, and unaffected the most problematic, and attempt to treat the
controls (e.g., Carter et al., 2000; Rizzo et al., target symptoms.
2007; Sherman et al., 1998; Spencer et al. 1998;
Stephens & Sandor, 1999; Sukhodolsky et al.,
OCD and GTS
2003; Tabori-Kraft, 2007). Overall, these studies
generally indicated that youngsters with GTS- Kinnear-Wilson, a neurologist, like Gilles de
only did not differ from unaffected controls la Tourette himself, acknowledged a relation-
on many ratings, including aggression, delin- ship between tics and OCD: “no feature is more
quency and/or conduct difficulties. By contrast, prominent in tics than its irresistibility . . . The
children with GTS+ADHD scored significantly element of compulsion links the condition
higher than unaffected controls and similar to intimately to the vast group of obsessions and
248 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

fi xed ideas” (Kinnear-Wilson, 1927). Modern integral to GTS (Robertson et al., 1988), they
literature confirmed that OCD and GTS are are both clinically and statistically different to
intimately related, although the percentage of those encountered in “pure” or “primary” OCD
patients with GTS who also show OCD varies (e.g., Baer, 1994; Cavanna et al., 2006b; Frankel
from as low as 11% in some reports (Kelman, et al., 1986; George et al., 1993; Leckman et al.,
1965) to as high as 80% in others (Yaryura- 1994, 2003; Miguel et al., 1997, 2000; Mula et al.,
Tobias et al., 1981). Cummings and Frankel 2008).
(1985) commented on similarities between Frankel et al. (1986) reported that the U.S.
GTS and OCD, including age of onset; life- and U.K. patients with GTS had signifi-
long course; waxing and waning of symptoms; cantly higher obsessional scores on a specially
involuntary, intrusive, ego-alien behavior and designed inventory when compared to controls.
experiences; occurrence in the same families; The obsessional items endorsed by GTS subjects
and worsening with depression and anxiety. changed with increasing age, in that younger
Montgomery et al. (1982) further suggested that patients endorsed more items to do with impulse
the obsessive-compulsive symptoms increase in control, whereas older subjects endorsed items
frequency with the duration of GTS. to do with were more concerned with checking,
OCD bears strong similarities to GTS, with arranging and fear of contamination. Cluster
the suggestion that certain obsessive-compul- analysis of the inventory responses comparing
sive symptoms (OCS) or behaviors (OCB) form OCD and GTS groups revealed a group of seven
an alternative phenotypic expression of GTS questions that were preferentially endorsed by
(Eapen et al., 1993; Pauls et al., 1986a, 1986b). GTS patients (blurting obscenities, counting
A family study by Pauls et al. (1986b) demon- compulsions, impulsions to hurt oneself) and
strated that the frequency of OCD in the absence eleven questions elicited high scores from OCD
of tics among first-degree relatives was signifi- patients (ordering, arranging, routines, rituals,
cantly elevated in families of both GTS+OCD touching one’s body, obsessions about people
and GTS–OCD probands, and that these rates hurting each other). George et al. (1993) dem-
were increased over estimates of the general onstrated that patients with GTS+OCD had
population and a control sample of adoptive significantly more violent, sexual and sym-
relatives. Studies examining the relationship metrical obsessions and more touching, blink-
between age of onset of OCD in probands and ing, counting, and self-damaging compulsions,
their affected relatives have found that child- compared to patients with OCD-alone who had
hood onset OCD is a highly familial disorder more obsessions concerning dirt or germs and
and that these early onset-cases may represent more compulsions relating to cleaning; the sub-
a valid subgroup, with higher genetic loading jects who had both disorders (i.e., GTS+OCD)
and shared vulnerability with chronic tic dis- reported that their compulsions arose spon-
orders (Rosario-Campos et al., 2005). Bellodi taneously, whereas the subjects with OCD-alone
et al. (1992) suggested that earlier age of onset reported that their compulsions were frequently
is associated with greater familiality. Further preceded by cognitions (George et al., 1993).
support for this notion comes from the study Thus from a clinical point of view, there are dif-
of Hemmings et al. (2004), who found a clini- ferences which in later studies seemed related
cal association between early age of onset and to family history. Other groups have found
an increased frequency of tics and related dis- similar results in that there were phenome-
orders, and a genetic association between early nological differences between the repetitive
onset OCD and the dopamine receptor type 4 behaviors encountered in GTS and OCD (e.g.,
gene (DRD4) suggesting a role for dopaminer- Miguel et al., 1997). Moreover, patients with
gic system in the development of early-onset GTS and OCD reported that their compulsions
OCD, unlike the serotonergic system that is arose spontaneously or de novo, whereas those
implicated in adult OCD (see also Robertson, with OCD-alone reported that their compul-
1995). sions were frequently preceded by cognitions.
Several studies have documented that Cavanna et al. (2006b) compared the nature of
although the OCB encountered in GTS are the OCS in two GTS populations, GTS–OCD
The Neuropsychiatry and Neuropsychology of Gilles de la Tourette Syndrome 249

and GTS+OCD, and reported that the first evidence to suggest that the reverse was true,
group of patients showed a significantly higher i.e. that GTS is more common in patients with
prevalence of “impulsive” and harming repeti- a main diagnosis of major depressive disorder
tive behaviors. More recently, Mula et al. (2008) (Eapen et al., 2001).
compared the phenomenology of the obsessional Depression in GTS is highly likely to be
thoughts between patients with GTS+OCD and multifactorial in origin, as is the depression in
matched patients diagnosed with temporal non-GTS populations (e.g., Winokur, 1997).
lobe epilepsy and OCD. The authors found that What exactly are the contributory factors of the
violent and symmetrical themes were prepon- depression in patients with GTS? GTS can be
derant in the first group, whilst fear of contami- a distressing condition, particularly if tics are
nation and religious/philosophical ruminations moderate to severe. Thus, depression in GTS
were more frequent in the second group. patients could be explained, at least in part, by
Taken together, these data indicate that GTS the fact that sufferers have a chronic, socially
and OCD are somehow intertwined, and that disabling and stigmatizing disease. Moreover,
specific OCS or OCB are likely to be integral to it has been clearly demonstrated that chil-
GTS (Hounie et al., 2006 [reviews]; Robertson, dren who have been bullied at school may also
2000). become depressed (Bond et al., 2001; Salmon
et al., 1998) and some children with GTS are
bullied, teased and given pejorative nicknames,
Depression and GTS
and thus the depression may result from that
Depression has long been found in association (Robertson, 2000, 2006a).
with GTS (e.g., Montgomery et al., 1982). There is Another reason, which might play a part
now good evidence from controlled and uncon- in the etiology of depression in clinic GTS
trolled studies recently reviewed by Robertson patients, is that in GTS, comorbidity with OCD
(2006a) and Robertson and Orth (2006) to sup- is high (see this chapter). The most common
port the view that affective disorders are com- complication of OCD ranging from 13% to 75%
mon in patients with GTS, with a lifetime risk of is depression (Perugi et al., 2002), and in a large
10%, and prevalence of between 1.8% and 8.9%. collaborative study, major depressive disor-
In specialist clinics, patients with GTS, depres- der was the most common comorbid disorder
sion or depressive symptomatology was found in OCD patients, occurring in 38% of subjects
to occur in between 13% and 76% of 5295 indi- (Perugi et al., 1999). Next, ADHD is common
viduals. In controlled studies in clinical settings in GTS (see this chapter) and ADHD has been
embracing 758 GTS patients, the patients were shown to have a high comorbidity with depres-
significantly more depressed than controls in all sion (e.g., Milberger et al., 1995), and thus many
but one instance. In community and epidemio- GTS patients could be depressed because of the
logical studies, depression in GTS individuals comorbidity with ADHD (Robertson, 2006b).
was evident in 1/5 investigations. Clinical cor- The depression in clinic GTS patients may
relates of the depression in clinical patients with also be due to the side effects of both typical and
GTS appear to be: tic severity and duration, the atypical neuroleptic medications. Depression
presence of echophenomena and coprophenom- has been reported with, for example, haloperi-
ena, premonitory sensations, sleep disturbances, dol, pimozide, fluphenazine, tiapride, sulpiride,
OCD, SIB, aggression, childhood conduct dis- and risperidone, as well as tetrabenazine, the
order, and possibly ADHD (Robertson, 2006a; calcium antagonist flunarizine, mecamylamine,
Robertson & Orth, 2006). The depression in and clonidine (Robertson, 2000).
people with GTS has been shown to result in Finally, the high levels of depression in some
a reduced Quality of Life (Elstner et al., 2001), of these studies may reflect the fact that patients
and may lead to hospitalization and even sui- that attend specialist clinics (including patients
cide in a few people (Robertson, 2006a). Taken with GTS) often have more than one problem/
together, the literature indicates that depressive disorder; this may introduce ascertainment bias.
symptoms, and even major depressive disorder, This view would be supported by some of the
are common in GTS. In contrast, there is no epidemiological studies, in which individuals
250 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

with GTS were not rated as having depressed increased rates of tic disorders, OCS/OCB, and
mood when compared to non-GTS subjects or anxiety symptoms in children who had a first-
controls. For instance, the study of Sukhodolsky degree relative with GTS.
et al. (2003) demonstrated that the “pure” GTS In summary, it seems that anxiety is common
subjects (i.e., those without comorbidity) were in clinic GTS patients, but its exact relationship
not more depressed than the controls. to GTS is yet unclear.
In summary, the etiology of depression in
GTS is highly likely multifactorial, as in primary
Rage and Explosive Outbursts
depressive illness, and less likely to be caused by
a single etiological factor. The precise phenom- Explosive outbursts, aggression, “rage attacks”
enology and natural history of depression in are being more frequently recognized and then
the context of GTS deserves more research, as documented in people with GTS (Budman
well as its contribution to the GTS phenotype(s). et al., 1998, 2000; Stephens & Sander, 1999). It
Similar to OCD, the phenomenology of depres- appears that there is a provocation, an extensive
sive symptoms may differ between GTS patients reaction to the provocation, and remorse after-
and those with major depressive disorder. In wards. These outbursts are related probably to
depressed GTS patients, this may help address impulsivity and may benefit from SSRI treat-
not only factors of particular relevance to the ment (Bruun & Budman, 1998).
etiology of their depression and thus improve
its recognition but also treatment and outcome.
Personality Disorders and Other
With regard to psychotic disorders, in partic-
Neuropsychiatric Disorders
ular schizophrenia, it is generally accepted that
there is no association between psychosis and GTS To date, there has been only one investigation of
apart from a few isolated case reports (Burd & personality disorder in GTS, but it has impor-
Kerbeshian, 1984; Takeuchi et al., 1986). On tant clinical implications, and therefore the
the other hand, a recent study by Cavanna et al. results will be discussed briefly. Robertson et al.
(2007) reported a relatively high prevalence of (1997) examined 39 adult GTS patients of mod-
schizotypal personality disorder, especially in erate severity and 34 age- and sex-matched con-
association with OCD comorbidity. Finally, trols using the Structured Clinical Interview for
there have been suggestions (Berthier et al., DSM-III-R Personality Disorders II (SCID-II),
1998; Kerbeshian et al., 1995) that bipolar disor- plus a self-rated scale for personality disorders.
der and GTS could be related; however, numbers Results showed that 64% of GTS patients had
are small and it is likely that the relationship one or more DSM-III-R personality disorders,
is with other comorbid conditions, rather than compared with only 16% of control subjects,
with GTS per se. which was highly statistically significant. The
explanation for this increase in personality dis-
order may well be the result of the long-term
Anxiety Disorders
outcome of childhood ADHD, referral bias, or
Anxiety has also been found in substantial pro- because of other childhood psychopathology.
portions of GTS cohorts (e.g., Erenberg et al., Other neuropsychiatric disorders associated
1987; Robertson et al., 1988), and GTS patients with GTS include autistic spectrum disorders
have been shown to have increased anxiety (Baron-Cohen et al., 1999), people with learning
when compared to control populations (e.g., difficulties, disability and “mental retardation”
Robertson et al., 1993, 1997). In the large mul- (Eapen et al., 1997; Kurlan et al., 2001), reading
ticenter study by Freeman et al. (2000), as high disabilities, dyspraxia, dyslexia, and dyscalculia
as 18% of the 3500 patients had comorbid anx- (Comings & Comings, 1987).
iety disorders. Thibert et al. (1995) examined
responses to a mailed questionnaire and showed
Family Psychopathology
that the GTS patients with comorbid OCS/OCB
scored higher on social anxiety than did the Gilles de la Tourette (1899) noted that the fam-
general population. Carter et al. (1994) found ily history of patients with GTS was almost
The Neuropsychiatry and Neuropsychology of Gilles de la Tourette Syndrome 251

invariably “loaded for nervous disorder.” coprolalia, and SIB only reached high levels in
Samuel Johnson’s father also appeared to suffer individuals with comorbidity; whereas males
from depression, “a general sensation of gloomy were more likely than females to have comorbid
wretchedness,” and it is thought to be from disorders (Freeman et al., 2000). Moreover, peo-
him that Dr. Johnson “inherited . . . a vile melan- ple with GTS have been shown to be more prone
choly” (Boswell, 1867). to depression, and the severity of this increases
Montgomery et al. (1982) found that 70% of with the duration of GTS, possibly as a conse-
30 first-degree relatives of 15 patients with GTS quence of having a stigmatizing disorder. OCD,
satisfied Feighner criteria for psychiatric ill- on the other hand, which is also remarkably
ness, the most common diagnoses being unipo- common in people with GTS, appears to be an
lar depression, OC illness, and panic disorder. integral part of the syndrome. A recent factor
Subsequently, Green and Pitman (1986), Pauls analytic study on a large U.K. GTS pedigree
et al. (1986a, 1986b), and Robertson and Gourdie supports the view that OCB represent a pheno-
(1990) found the rate of OCB/OCD significantly type of the putative GTS gene(s) (Robertson &
higher among relatives of patients with GTS Cavanna, 2007). The authors’ suggestions as to
than in control populations. Robertson et al. the relationships between GTS and other vari-
(1988) reported that 48% of 90 probands had a ous heterogeneous psychopathologies are sum-
positive family history of psychiatric illness, of marized in Table 11–1.
which the most common disorders were depres- To date, very few studies have investigated
sion, schizophrenia, and OCD. More recently quality of life (QOL) in patients with GTS
Khalifa and von Knorring (2005) described (Bernard et al. 2006; Elstner et al., 2001; Al
25/4479 (0.6%) of GTS youngsters in a com- Faqih, 2007). According to these preliminary
munity sample and compared them to healthy investigations, patients with GTS show signifi-
controls. They showed that more parents and cantly worse QOL than the general population.
siblings of GTS children had an increased prev- Specifically, correlates of reduced QOL include
alence of OCD, ADHD, and depression. Eighty comorbid OCD and ADHD, rather than tic
percent had a first-degree relative with a psy- severity.
chiatric disorder. While it is acknowledged that Both the DSM and the ICD criteria have dic-
these studies are not controlled, it would appear tated that GTS is a unitary condition. However,
that there is increased psychiatric morbidity in recent studies using hierarchical cluster analy-
the relatives of patients with GTS, and that OCD sis (Mathews et al., 2007) and principal compo-
is the most common disorder found in these nent factor analysis (Alsobrook & Pauls, 2002;
relatives (see also Robertson & Cavanna, 2007). Robertson & Cavanna, 2007) have demon-
As suggested earlier, this implicates a common strated that there may be more than one GTS
genetic basis to these disorders. phenotype. There are also early suggestions that
at least some of the many suggested etiologies
may result in different phenotypes. For exam-
Neuropsychiatry: Preliminary
ple, in the Alsobrook and Pauls (2002) study,
Conclusions
three out of four factors were heritable. Martino
The clinical and epidemiological studies et al. (2007) showed that ABGA positive GTS
reviewed in this chapter suggest that associated individuals had significantly less ADHD. Hyde
behavioral problems are common in people et al. (1992, 1994) showed for monozygotic twins
with GTS, so that it seems reasonable to view concordant for tics but discordant for severity,
this condition as a psychopathological spec- the smaller birth weight twin had more severe
trum disorder (e.g., Cavanna et al., 2008). GTS and more abnormal EEGs. With regard
As mentioned before, the largest investiga- to psychopathology, Eapen et al. (2004) con-
tion to date, embracing 3500 clinic patients with ducted a principal component factor analysis
GTS worldwide, demonstrated that at all ages, and demonstrated two factors. Thus, both the
88% of individuals had reported comorbidity. phenotype of the “tic-part” of GTS and the psy-
The most common was ADHD, followed by chopathology of GTS are not homogeneous uni-
OCD. Anger control problems, sleep difficulties, tary entities. There are many causes and many
252 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Table 11–1. Suggested Relationships between Psychopathology and GTS


Neuropsychiatric disorder Relationship with GTS
OCD Generally suggested as an integral part of and geneti-
cally related to GTS
ADHD Common in GTS and possibly genetically linked in
some cases
Depression Multifactorial/possibly due to comorbidity with OCD
and ADHD, rather than to GTS per se
Dysphoria, anxiety, cognitive impairment, school Secondary to medication in some cases
phobia
Schizophrenia Uncommon, the association is by chance
Autistic spectrum disorder, rage attacks, learning Relationship is unknown and more research is needed
disability, mental retardation, anorexia nervosa
Huntington’s disease, Parkinson’s disease, Wilson’s No relationship
disease
Bipolar disorder Few cases described, probably related to comorbidities
Explosive attacks, temper tantrums, “rage” attacks Related to impulsivity, possibly distinct entity
Note: Only 8%–12% of GTS individuals in epidemiological and clinical settings have “pure” tics and no other diagnosis.
Abbreviations: GTS, Gilles de la Tourette syndrome; OCD, Obsessive-compulsive disorder; ADHD, attention-deficit hyper-
activity disorder.
Source: Adapted from Robertson, 2004.

phenotypes. Only time will tell whether or not Incagnoli & Kane, 1981; Shapiro et al., 1974).
future studies demonstrate further etiological– These studies show a relatively consistent set of
phenotypic relationships. findings: patients with GTS, as a group, have IQ
scores in the normal range (Corbett et al., 1969;
Randolph et al., 1993), although GTS and men-
Neuropsychology
tal handicap can of course coexist (Golden &
We turn now to the neuropsychology of GTS. Greenhill, 1981). In a proportion of the patients
In our review of this, we restrict our discussion with overall IQs in the normal range, statisti-
to cognition in these patients. This term is both cally significant discrepancies between verbal
broad enough to include the major psychologi- and performance IQ on the Wechsler Scales
cal processes, and yet focuses enough to exclude have been found (50% in the Shapiro et al.
such psychological systems as emotion. As will [1974] study). For most patients, performance
become evident, the neuropsychology of GTS IQ is significantly worse than verbal IQ, but
has hardly begun. We suggest that a scientific significant discrepancies in the opposite direc-
theory of this disorder must ultimately aim tion are also sometimes found. Schuerholz et al.
to give a thorough account of how cognition (1996), however, did not report any discrepancy
and emotion link with biology and behavior between the verbal and performance IQ when
in patients with this condition. The following comparing patients with GTS-only and patients
paragraphs review the studies addressing the with GTS and comorbid ADHD. Sherman et al.
main cognitive domains in patients with GTS, (1998) compared 21 GTS-only subjects, 14 GTS
namely intelligence, language, attention and + ADHD, and 18 controls on the Wechsler Full
executive functions, memory, and visuomotor Scale IQ. The GTS + ADHD group (IQ 99.2)
skills. performed significantly worse than the con-
trols (IQ 112.8) and the GTS-only group (106.5).
They did not report discrepancies of more than
Intelligence
15 points between the verbal and performance
More intelligence testing has been carried out scale.
in GTS than almost any other kind of cogni- Within the subtests, Coding, written
tive investigation. A range of IQ tests have been Arithmetic, and copying tasks (as measured,
used, a key one being the Wechsler Scales (e.g., for example, in the Aphasia Screening Test of
The Neuropsychiatry and Neuropsychology of Gilles de la Tourette Syndrome 253

the Halstead–Reitan Battery), seem to cluster not systematically studied. Apart from this set
as a set of severe deficits. Incagnoli and Kane of symptoms, some studies also report a history
(1981) suggest this may represent dysfunction of speech and language problems, even prior to
of “nonconstructional visuopractic abilities” the onset of GTS (O’Quinn & Thompson, 1980),
(p. 168), although the relationship between this though again, the precise nature of these early
postulated cognitive deficit in visuographic language problems remains unspecified.
skills and the symptomatology of the disorder Given the centrality of language abnormali-
remains to be fully explored. Furthermore, ties in these patients, it is somewhat surprising
although Incagnoli and Kane stress specifically how few psycholinguistic studies of GTS have
on nonconstructional visuopractic deficits, been carried out. Ludlow et al. (1982) studied 54
deficits have also been found in some construc- patients and 54 normal controls by recording 10
tional abilities, such as those measured by the minutes of conversation and picture description
Block Design or Object Assembly subtests of the from each subject. These authors found that,
Wechsler Scales (Bornstein et al., 1983; Shapiro from the recordings of spontaneous speech,
et al., 1974). These differences may reflect devel- majority of tics were produced at the beginning
opmental changes, since Shapiro et al. (1974) or end of speech clause production. Tics thus
found deficits on the Block Design subtest in do not seem to interfere randomly with speech
adults with GTS, but not in children. but are produced in synchrony with overall
Overall, GTS patients appear to perform in speech rhythms, mostly at speech boundaries.
the normal range on tests of general intelligence Motor tics, on the other hand, do not respect
(see also Como, 2001 [review]). The authors who word boundaries (Frank, 1978). Second, Ludlow
reported discrepancies between verbal and per- et al. (1982) used a range of language tests on
formance IQ have noted that they are more com- the same patients, all part of the Neurosensory
mon in older GTS subjects (Shapiro et al., 1974; Center Comprehensive Examination of Aphasia
Sutherland et al., 1982). This is in accordance (NCCEA), a standardized instrument. These
with the findings by Bornstein et al. (1983) and tests included assessments of Visual Naming,
Schuerholz et al. (1996) that neuropsychological Description of Use, Word Fluency, Sentence
abnormalities are more common in older GTS Construction, Sentence Repetition, Reading,
patients and can become more prominent as the Writing, and Copying. Significant differences
disorder progresses. between the groups appeared only on language
expression, writing (expressive and dictation),
and copying. The latter findings are consistent
Language
with the visuographic deficits revealed using
Vocal tics are, of course, a major diagnostic intelligence tests, reviewed earlier.
symptom of GTS. Not all vocal tics are clearly In the Ludlow et al. (1982) study, reading was
linguistic, however. Some vocal tics, for exam- also poorer than normal, but this just failed to
ple, are nonverbal (e.g., throat-clearing, barking, reach statistical significance. Although the stud-
or snorting). However, clear verbal tics make up ies of intelligence reviewed earlier suggested
some 35% of vocal tics (Ludlow et al., 1982), and that arithmetic skills are more impaired in GTS
these include coprolalia (unprovoked swear- than either reading or spelling skills (Bornstein
ing), palilalia (frequent reiteration of syllables), et al., 1983), there are nevertheless a number of
jargon (production of strings of meaningless other studies reporting high levels of reading
syllables), word-tics (interjection of words that problems. Comings and Comings (1987), for
are meaningful but are not part of speech), and example, found severe reading difficulties (dys-
echolalia (repetition of heard speech). Lees et al. lexia) in 26.8% of their sample, in contrast to
(1984) estimate that coprolalia and echolalia only 4.2% of their control group.
occur in about 40%–50% of patients with GTS. Sutherland et al. (1982) report that perfor-
Interestingly, Rickards (2001) reported signing mance was low on a Word Fluency Test (naming
coprolalia and attempts to disguise as a man as many objects, and then animals, as possible
with prelingual deficits. Most of these language in one minute, and then alternating between
abnormalities have simply been described, but giving color and bird names as often as possible
254 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

in 1 minute). This was not explained by any for the ensuing school and learning difficulties
gross anomaly such as abnormal speech later- that are reported in a proportion of patients,
alization, as a dichotic listening task confirmed although this explanation is not by itself suffi-
a normal left hemisphere dominance for words. cient to account for the pattern of specific defi-
It is reasonable to surmise that the difficulties cits (e.g., in visuospatial abilities), unless one
on the Word Fluency Test might be related to posits attentional deficits that are specific to, for
impairments in “frontal systems,” rather than example, the visual modality. Some evidence
to purely linguistic factors, given the difficulties that this may be the case comes from a study by
that patients with nonfocal frontal lesions have Bornstein et al. (1983), which reported that their
on such tests (Shallice, 1988). patients with GTS scored in the normal range on a
Brookshire et al. (1994) compared GTS test of auditory attention span (from the Wechsler
patients and controls on the Vocabulary test of Scales, mean scaled score = 9.6), but in the below
the WISC-R and the Oral Fluency Test. Their average range on a test of visual attention span
results showed that the GTS group scored sig- (using the Knox Cube Test). This pattern once
nificantly lower on both tests than the control again confirms the dysfunction in the visuospatial
group. Schuerholz et al. (1998) compared a and visuomotor systems reviewed earlier.
GTS-only, GTS + ADHD, and a control group, A further study by Channon et al. (1992)
showing that the GTS + ADHD group was sig- showed that mild deficits were found using “com-
nificantly slower than the GTS-only group on plex” tests of attention, such as serial addition,
the Rapid Automatized Naming, whereas in block sequence span (forwards), the trail-mak-
the 1996 study the groups did not differ on this ing test, and a letter cancellation vigilance task.
measure. On the Letter Word Fluency task, the Given the very high proportion of GTS patients
GTS-only subjects scored significantly lower who show attentional deficits, and given that
when compared to the GTS + ADHD. De Nil attention is one of the most well-studied pro-
et al. (2005) analyzed the frequency and type cesses in experimental psychology, with a range
of speech disfluencies in 69 children diagnosed of paradigms readily available (e.g., Allport,
with GTS and compared their results with sim- 1987), there is a clear need for more controlled
ilar speech data from a control group of 27 indi- experiments in this area. Important questions
viduals. Self-report data on fluency difficulties that remain to be considered include specifying
did not reveal significant group differences; how attention deficits in patients with GTS dif-
however, detailed analysis of fluency during fer or compare to those in children without GTS
reading and spontaneous speech revealed an but with ADHD (Taylor, 1985); and whether the
overall higher level of more typical (normal) behavioral problems that are common in GTS
disfluencies in the GTS group. No overall dif- are directly related to attentional deficits, or (as
ferences in less typical (stuttering) disfluen- Wilson et al., 1982, found) to other factors such
cies were observed between the two groups of as severity of tics and level of IQ, or to both.
children. According to Como (2001) executive deficits
The reviewed studies show that the linguis- are also common among GTS patients and can
tic skills of GTS patients are relatively intact. in some cases be more debilitating than the tics
The most frequently reported deficit is on the characterizing the syndrome. Sutherland et al.
Fluency Tests, which can be accounted for by (1982) first reported that GTS patients per-
impairments in executive functions, rather than formed significantly worse on the Newcombe
by linguistic factors entirely. Word Fluency Test and on the copy of the
Rey Complex Figure compared to controls.
Schuerholz et al. (1996) found that GTS-only
Attention and Executive Functions
group performed significantly better on the
As mentioned earlier, there is now little doubt copy part of the Rey Complex Figure than the
that GTS is associated strongly with attentional control and GTS + ADHD groups. On the other
difficulties. Most studies that have looked at the hand, Bornstein (1991) and Bornstein and Yang
overlap between GTS and ADHD have suggested (1991) found no impairment on the Wisconsin
that the attentional deficits may be a key reason Card Sorting Test (WCST). Bornstein et al.
The Neuropsychiatry and Neuropsychology of Gilles de la Tourette Syndrome 255

(1983) revealed an interesting pattern with the Given that the symptoms of GTS seem to
GTS patients performing poorly on the Knox involve those systems that are central to the con-
Cube Test, a measure of visual attention span. trol of action, it is relevant to enquire about the
Channon et al. (1992) administered a number functioning of the so-called frontal systems that
of tests measuring attention to GTS subjects are held to control action (and to be impaired in
and controls. Compared to the control group, patients with nonfocal frontal lesions). In cog-
GTS patients performed significantly worse nitive psychology the main mechanism that has
on the Corsi Block Test forward, cancellation been described is the Supervisory Attentional
task, mental arithmetic task, and Trail Making System (or the SAS: Shallice, 1988). To under-
B. Bornstein (1991), however, found no impair- stand the SAS, we must first mention what
ment on the Trail Making B among the TS sub- Shallice calls the Contention Scheduling System
jects, compared to published norms. Ozonoff (CSS), which responds to external stimuli and
et al. (1994) used the Go-NoGo Task and H&S repeats relevant, routine actions. The SAS over-
task (in which participants are asked to classify sees the CSS, by activating or inhibiting alter-
eight different stimulus types, which appear on a native schemata. The primary function of the
computer screen, as either Hs or Ss) to assess the SAS is to respond to novelty. Shallice (1988)
executive functions in GTS-only patients and proposed that the SAS is impaired in patients
matched controls. The results revealed no signif- with frontal lobe syndrome, resulting in disin-
icant differences between the groups on neither hibition of actions.
the Go-NoGo tasks nor H&S task. The authors Previous work has shown that children with
presented two explanations for these results. GTS are impaired in so-called intention edit-
First, it was possible that the Go-NoGo task was ing, a process required whenever two or more
too easy for the GTS-only and that a measure intentions are activated simultaneously, only
with greater cognitive demands might have dis- one of which can be executed into action. A
tinguished between the groups. Alternatively, study by Baron-Cohen et al. (1994) used two
there are many modalities of inhibition, and tasks: in the first one they asked the children
GTS may involve a failure of motor inhibition to simultaneously open one hand while closing
but not deficits in cognitive inhibition. A study the other, and then switch to doing this in the
by Sherman et al. (1998) compared GTS-only, opposite pattern, carrying on this simultaneous
GTS + ADHD, and a control group on a con- alternation for 10 trials (this is Luria’s Hand
tinuous performance test. Results indicated that Alternation Task). The second task involved
the performance of the GTS-only and control playing the Yes and No Game, in which the
group was within the normal limits on mea- children could answer a question in any way
sures of attention (number of hits), however the they liked, so long as they did not answer with
GTS-only performed significantly better than the words “Yes” or “No.” On both tasks, chil-
the GTS + ADHD group. Georgiou et al. (1995) dren with GTS were severely impaired, relative
tested the ability of GTS patients to respond to to normal control groups. These deficits suggest
relevant (congruent) and irrelevant (incongru- the existence of a mechanism in the normal case
ent) stimuli. In this study protocol, the sub- (the Intention Editor) that has failed to develop
jects had to respond to visuospatial stimuli on normally in children with GTS (see Baron-
a computer screen. Compared to controls, GTS Cohen & Moriarty, 1995; Baron-Cohen &
patients were more sensitive to irrelevant stimu- Robertson, 1995).
lus information, and showed difficulty in inhib- The reviewed studies of executive function
iting an inappropriate response. The authors ability in GTS patients reveal somewhat incon-
concluded that the nature of GTS patients’ sistent findings. However, in those studies
attentional difficulties might reside in an inabil- that have grouped GTS patients according to
ity to effectively shift and/or change cognitive comorbid conditions such as ADHD or OCD,
sets. This inability to change cognitive sets can executive dysfunctions appear to be more com-
be related to basal ganglia-frontal disturbances mon in GTS accompanied by psychiatric dis-
often seen in GTS, resulting in attentional prob- orders (Schuerholz et al., 1996, 1998; Sherman
lems and response inhibition. et al., 1998). However, since few studies to date
256 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

have excluded GTS patients with comorbid the text being read, poor recall of stories may
conditions, the question arises if the execu- simply reflect distraction.
tive dysfunctions are to be considered artefacts
of comorbid conditions in GTS rather then
specific to GTS itself. Visuomotor Skills
Motor tics are, of course, another pathogno-
Memory monic symptom of this disorder. The range of
motor tics is very wide, from mild blinking to
Only a few studies have focused purely or in
conspicuous flailing of limbs. Perhaps more
depth on memory functions in GTS. Ludlow
unusual, though, are motor phenomena that
et al. (1982) report that patients with GTS score
have specific content. Echopraxia is a clear
in the normal range on tests of auditory memory
example of this, where a patient involuntarily
(Sentence Repetition, Digit Repetition Forwards,
repeats (“echoes”) the actions of another per-
and Digit Repetition Backwards). This intact
son immediately after observing them. Lees
auditory memory system (at least as regards
et al. (1984) report that echopraxia occurs in
short-term memory) contrasts with Sutherland
21% of patients with GTS. Here is Gilles de la
et al.’s (1982) report that copying and drawing
Tourette’s own description of this extraordi-
from memory is impaired in GTS, as is delayed
nary phenomenon:
recall of visual material (the Wechsler Memory
Figures), relative to both mentally handicapped S. is in the courtyard of the Salpetrière in his usual
and schizophrenic controls groups. A study way. He is moving about, making a few contortions
by Randolph et al. (1993) using the California and a few strange sounds, all of which is his ordi-
Verbal Memory Test and Rey Complex Figure nary practice. Another patient approaches him and
found no difference between performance of decides to imitate one of his stranger movements,
which consists of lift ing the right arm and leg and
severe and less severe GTS patients. Brookshire
stomping with the left foot, a peculiar position that
et al. (1994) used the Verbal and Nonverbal one can clearly see is apt to cause a loss of balance. At
Selective Reminding Test and reported that the the same time, he mimics one of our patient’s more
GTS group performed in a similar way as the bizarre and characteristic utterances. Soon S., who
control group on both the verbal and nonverbal has been rather calm, starts to imitate the screams
part. Likewise, controlled studies by Channon and strange mannerisms of his fellow patient, and
et al. (2003, 2006) and Crawford et al. (2005) does it so vigorously that he falls, fortunately not
found no evidence of impairment in implicit hurting himself. Hospital guards have to stop this
aspects of memory and learning. More recently, game, which could become dangerous. This inci-
Lavoie et al. (2007) demonstrated a specific dent, sadly, has become the source of many cruel epi-
nonverbal deficit in two cohorts of patients with sodes where other patients take sadistic advantage
of S.’s irresistible imitative compulsion. (Gilles de la
GTS and chronic tic disorder who completed
Tourette, in Goetz & Klawans [1982], pp. 7–8)
the Rey–Osterreich Complex Figure and the
California Verbal Learning Test. The notion of an “imitative compulsion” dra-
Whether the memory deficits that have been matically conveys how these movements might
occasionally identified represent pure memory be triggered simply by the sight of another
problems, or are secondary to the attentional person’s movements, the resulting imitation
deficits reviewed above, remains to be estab- being entirely automatic and involuntary.
lished. Since many of the studies that have Copropraxia (the gestural equivalent of copro-
reported memory impairments in GTS have not lalia), like echopraxia, is another motor tic with
divided the patients into groups according to a content, and again suggests a single neurologi-
comorbid condition, it is hard to say whether cal or cognitive deficit may underlie symptoms
the impairment is due to GTS solely or to a at both levels, despite earlier arguments against
comorbid condition. For example, Sutherland this claim. Copropraxia remains to be studied
et al. (1982) speculate that because of the urge in more detail.
many patients report to repeat what they hear, Bornstein et al. (1983) first reported general
which must distract them from the meaning of motor deficits as measured by the Grooved
The Neuropsychiatry and Neuropsychology of Gilles de la Tourette Syndrome 257

Pegboard Test (an index of visuomotor coordi- of subjects, which may mask important neu-
nation). Golden (1984) reports that half of his ropsychological differences that may exist
patients actually showed superior motor skills, between subgroups. For example, some stud-
whereas the others showed impaired perfor- ies have lumped together both old and young
mance. On the other hand, Bornstein and Yang patients, or those with tic-related symptoms/
(1991) did not find a significant difference on comorbid conditions and those without, those
the performance of medicated and unmedicated with an early and a late onset of GTS, and even
GTS patients on the Grooved Pegboard Test or those who are receiving medication and those
Trail Making A and B. Randolph et al. (1993) who are not. Th is variation, in our opinion,
divided twin pairs into twins with less severe prevents a careful comparison of homogenous
and more severe GTS and found that the more subgroups within the disorder. It is hoped that
severe TS group performed significantly better future research in this area will adopt a more
on the Purdue Pegboard Test, but only with the rigorous experimental approach, drawing on
dominant hand. Brookshire et al. (1994) did not methods available in experimental psychol-
find a significant difference between the GTS ogy, and defi ne patient groups more tightly, so
group and controls on tests requiring visual- that if subgroups do exist, these can be better
motor skills such as the Block Design and understood. Such data are needed in order to
Object Assembly of the WISC. However, the gain a better insight into the neuropsychologi-
two groups differed significantly on the Coding cal status of GTS.
of WISC, and the GTS group tended to perform
worse (nonsignificantly) than the controls on
Summary
the Grooved Pegboard Test. Finally, in a study
by Schultz et al. (1998), GTS patients scored In this chapter, we have reviewed some of the
significantly lower than the control group on neuropsychiatric and neuropsychological
all three conditions dominant, nondominant, studies of GTS. The major finding in the neu-
bimanual) of the Purdue Pegboard Test. The ropsychiatry field is that there is a high rate of
question of whether the motor tics in this con- psychiatric comorbidities in GTS, the most
dition reflect abnormalities in the planning of commonly encountered being OCD, ADHD,
action, or in the control of action, is currently and affective disorders (particularly depres-
unknown. sion). The exact relationships between psycho-
pathology and GTS are complex, and far from
being fully elucidated. However, in recent years
Neuropsychology: Preliminary
a few lines of evidence have started to emerge.
Conclusions
For instance, it is now generally accepted that
The neuropsychology studies reviewed in OCB/OCD is unlikely to be a consequence of
this chapter suggested that the level of per- the disorder and instead appears to be an inte-
formance of GTS patients is not indicative of gral part of it, probably etiologically/genetically
grossly impaired neuropsychological func- related.
tion. GTS individuals score in the average With regard to neuropsychology, it appears
or above average range on intelligence tasks, that the overall level of performance of GTS
and on most other measures they are close to patients is not indicative of grossly impaired
normal means. Moreover, where impairments neuropsychological function. They tend to score
have been reported (especially attentional in the average range on intelligence tasks and
deficits), they are in most instances linked to most other cognitive measures. However, small
a comorbid condition (e.g., ADHD). However, sample size, lack of control groups, and ascer-
it is important to underscore that the neuro- tainment bias, represent significant limitations
psychology of GTS is still incomplete, in part, of most neuropsychological studies conducted
because there have been insufficient experi- so far. Further studies on specific subgroups
mental studies of key psychological systems, of patients are needed in order to answer the
and in part because the studies that have been question of how cognitive deficits relate to GTS
carried out have tended to include a wide mix symptoms.
258 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

In conclusion, it appears that GTS can no Baer, L. (1994). Factor analysis of symptom subtypes
longer be thought of simply as a motor disorder, of obsessive compulsive disorder and their relation
and, most importantly, that GTS is no longer a to personality and tic disorders. Journal of Clinical
unitary condition, as it was previously thought. Psychiatry, 55, S18–S23.
Banaschewski, T., Woerner, W., & Rothenberger,
This change in perspective brings us closer to
A. (2003). Premonitory sensory phenomena and
Georges Gilles de la Tourette’s original view of
suppressibility of tics in Tourette syndrome:
this disorder. Developmental aspects in children and ado-
lescents. Developmental Medicine and Child
Neurology, 45, 700–703.
Acknowledgments Baron-Cohen, S., Cross, P., Crowson, M., &
Robertson, M. (1994). Can children with Gilles
The authors wish to thank the Tourettes Action -
de la Tourette syndrome edit their intentions?
U.K. and the Tourette Syndrome Association -
Psychological Medicine, 24, 29–40.
U.S.A. for their continuing support and Baron-Cohen, S., & Moriarty, J. (1995). Developmental
encouragement. dysexecutive syndrome: Does it exist? A neuro-
psychological perspective. In M. Robertson & V.
Eapen (Eds.), Movement and allied disorders in
References
childhood (pp. 305–316). New York: John Wiley
Abelson, J. F., Kwan, K. Y., O’Roak, B. J., Baek, D. and Sons, Ltd.
Y., Stillman A. A., Morgan, T. M., et al. (2005). Baron-Cohen, S., & Robertson, M. (1995). Children
Sequence variants in SLITRK1 are associated with with either autism, Gilles de la Tourette syndrome,
Tourette’s Syndrome. Science, 310, 317–320. or both: Mapping cognition to specific syndromes.
Al Faqih, S. S. (2007). Determinants of quality of Neurocase, 1, 101–104.
life in Gilles de la Tourette Syndrome. Movement Baron-Cohen, S., Scahill, V., Izaguirre, J., Hornsey,
Disorders, 22, S281. H., & Robertson, M. M. (1999). The prevalence
Alexander, G. M., & Peterson, B. S. (2004). Testing of Gilles de la Tourette Syndrome in children
the prenatal hormone hypothesis of tic-related and adolescents with autism: A large scale study.
disorders: Gender identity and gender role behav- Psychological Medicine, 29, 1151–1159.
ior. Developmental Psychopathology, 16, 407–420. Barr, C. L., Wigg, K. G., Pakstis, A. J., Kurlan, R., Pauls,
Allport, A. (1987). Selection for action: Some behav- D., Kidd, K. K., et al. (1999). Genome scan for link-
ioral and neurophysiological considerations of age to Gilles de la Tourette Syndrome. American
attention and action. In H. Heurer & A. Sanders Journal of Human Genetics, 88, 437–445.
(Eds.), Perspectives on perception and action Bellodi, L., Sciuto, G., Diaferia, G., Ronchi, P., &
(chap. 15, pp. 395–419). Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Smeraldi, E. (1992). Psychiatric disorders in the
Erlbaum. families of patients with obsessive-compulsive
Alsobrook, J. P. 2nd, & Pauls, D. L. (2002). A factor disorder. Psychiatry Research, 42, 111–120.
analysis of tic symptoms in Gilles de la Tourette’s Bernard, B. A., Stebbins, G. T., Siegel, S., Schultz, T.
syndrome. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, M., Hays, C., Morrissey, M. J., et al. (2006). The
291–296. impact of co-morbidities on quality of life in Gilles
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic de la Tourette Syndrome. Neurology, 66(Suppl 2),
and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders IV-text A365–A366.
revision (DSM-IV-TR). Washington, DC: American Berthier, M. L., Kulisevsky, J., & Campos, V. M.
Psychiatric Association. (1998) Bipolar disorder in adult patients with
Annett, M. (1970). A classification of hand prefer- Tourette’s syndrome: A clinical study. Biological
ences by association analysis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 43, 364–370.
Psychology, 61, 303–321. Bloch, M. H., Leckman, J. F., Zhu, H., & Peterson,
Apter, A., Pauls, D. L., Bleich, A., Zohar, A. H., Kron, B. S. (2005). Caudate volumes in childhood pre-
S., Ratzoni, G., et al. (1993). An epidemiologic dict symptom severity in adults with Tourette syn-
study of Gilles de la Tourette’s syndrome in Israel. drome. Neurology, 65, 1253–1258.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 734–738. Bloch, M. H., Peterson, B. S., Scahill, L., Otka, J.,
Asam, U. (1982). A follow-up study of Tourette syn- Katsovich, L., Zhang, H., et al. (2006). Adulthood
drome. In A. J. Friedhoff & T. N. Chase (Eds.), outcome of tic and obsessive-compulsive symp-
Gilles de la Tourette Syndrome, advances in neu- tom severity in children with Tourette syndrome.
rology, vol. 35 (pp. 285–286). New York: Raven Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine,
Press. 160, 65–69.
The Neuropsychiatry and Neuropsychology of Gilles de la Tourette Syndrome 259

Bohlhalter, S., Goldfine, A., Matteson, S., Garraux, emotional adjustment in children affected with
G., Hanakawa, T., Kansaku, K., et al. (2006). Gilles de la Tourette’s syndrome: Associations
Neural correlates of tic generation in Tourette syn- with ADHD and family functioning. Journal of
drome: An event-related functional MRI study. Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 215–23.
Brain, 129(Pt 8), 2029–2037. Carter, A. S., Pauls, D. L., Leckman, J. F., & Cohen, D.
Bond, L., Carlin, J. B., Thomas, L., Rubin, K., & J. (1994). A prospective longitudinal study of Gilles
Patton, G. (2001). Does bullying cause emotional de la Tourette syndrome. Journal of the American
problems? A prospective study of young teenagers. Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 33,
British Medical Journal, 323, 480–484. 377–385.
Bornstein, R. A. (1991). Neuropsychological per- Cavanna, A. E., Monaco, F., Mula, M., Robertson,
formance in adults with Tourette Syndrome. M. M., & Critchley, H. D. (2006a). Uneven
Psychiatry Research, 37, 229–236. focal shoe deterioration in Tourette syndrome.
Bornstein, R. A., King, G., & Carroll, A. (1983). Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 2,
Neuropsychological abnormalities in Gilles de 587–588.
la Tourette´s Syndrome. Journal of Nervous and Cavanna, A. E., Robertson, M. M., & Critchley,
Mental Disease, 171, 497–502. H. D. (2007). Schizotypal personality traits in
Bornstein, R. A., & Yang, V. (1991). Neuropsychological Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Acta Neurologica
performance in medicated and unmedicated Scandinavica, 116, 385–391.
patients with TS. American Journal of Psychiatry, Cavanna, A. E., Servo, S., Monaco, F., & Robertson,
148, 468–471. M. M. (2008). More than tics: The behavioral spec-
Boswell, J. (1867). The life of Samuel Johnson. London: trum of Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Journal
George Routledge and Sons. of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, In
Brookshire, B. L., Butler, I. J., Ewing-Cobbs, L., & Press.
Fletcher J. M. (1994). Neuropsychological charac- Cavanna, A. E., Strigaro, G., Martino D., Robertson,
teristics of children with TS: Evidence for a non- M. M., & Critchley, H. D. (2006b). Compulsive
verbal learning disability. Journal of Clinical and behaviours in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome.
Experimental Neuropsychology, 16, 289–302. Confinia Neuropsychiatrica, 1, 37–40.
Bruun, R., & Budman, C. (1998). Paroxetine as Channon, S., Flynn, D., & Robertson, M. (1992).
a treatment for anger attacks in patients with Attentional deficits in Gilles de la Tourette
Tourette syndrome. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, syndrome. Neuropsychiatry, Neurology, and
59, 581–588. Behavioral Neurology, 5, 170–177.
Budman, C., Bruun, R., Park, K., Lesser, M., & Channon, S., Gunning, A., Frankl, J., & Robertson,
Olsom, M. (2000). Explosive outbursts in children M. M. (2006). Tourette’s syndrome (TS): Cognitive
with Tourette’s disorder. Journal of the American performance in adults with uncomplicated TS.
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, Neuropsychology, 20, 58–65.
1270–1276. Channon, S., Pratt, P., & Robertson, M. M. (2003).
Budman, C., Park, K., Olson, M., & Bruun, R. Executive function, memory, and learning
(1998). Rage attacks in children and adolescents in Tourette’s syndrome. Neuropsychology, 17,
with Tourette syndrome: A pilot study. Journal of 247–254.
Clinical Psychiatry, 59, 576–580. Chase, T. N., Geoff rey, V., Gillespie, M., & Burrows,
Burd, L., & Kerbeshian, J. (1984). Gilles de la G. H. (1986). Structural and functional stud-
Tourette’s syndrome and bipolar disorder. Archives ies of Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Revue
of Neurology, 41, 1236. Neurologique (Paris), 142, 851–855.
Burd, L., Severud, R., Klug, M. G., & Kerbeshian, J. Coffey, B. J., Biederman, J., Geller, D. A., Spencer, T.,
(1999). Prenatal and perinatal risk factors for Tourette Park, K. S., Shapiro, S. J., et al. (2000). The course
disorder. Journal of Perinatal Medicine, 27, 295–302. of Tourette’s disorder: A literature review. Harvard
Butler, T., Stern, E., & Silbersweig, D. (2006). Review of Psychiatry. 8, 192–198.
Functional neuroimaging of Tourette syndrome: Comings, D. E., & Comings, B. G. (1987). A con-
Advances and future directions. Advances in trolled study of Tourette’s syndrome. 1. Attention-
Neurology, 99, 115–129. deficit disorder, learning disorders, and school
Caine, E. D., McBride, M. C., Chiverton, P., Bamford, problems. American Journal of Human Genetics,
K. A., Rediess, S., & Shiao, J. (1988). Tourette 41, 701–741.
syndrome in Monroe county school children. Comings, D. E., Himes, J. A., & Comings, B. G.
Neurology, 38, 472–475. (1990). An epidemiologic study of Tourette’s syn-
Carter, A. S., O’Donnell, D. A., Schultz, R. T., Scahill, drome in a single school district. Journal of Clinical
L., Leckman, J. F., & Pauls, D. L. (2000). Social and Psychiatry, 51, 463–469.
260 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Como, P. G. (2001). Neuropsychological function in Ferenczi, S. (1921). Psychoanalytic observations on


Tourette syndrome. Advances in Neurology, 85, tic. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 2,
103–111. 1–30.
Corbett, J., Mathews, A., Connell, P., & Shapiro, D. Frank, S. (1978). Psycholinguistic findings in Gilles de
(1969). Tics and Gilles de la Tourette’s syndrome: la Tourette’s syndrome. Journal of Communication
A follow-up study and critical review. British Disorders, 11, 349–363.
Journal of Psychiatry, 115, 1229–1241. Frankel, M., Cummings, J. L., Robertson, M. M.,
Crawford, S., Channon, S., & Robertson M. M. Trimble, M. R., Hill, M. A., & Benson, D. F.
(2005). Tourette’s syndrome: performance on (1986). Obsessions and compulsions in Gilles de la
tests of behavioural inhibition, working memory Tourette’s syndrome. Neurology, 36, 378–382.
and gambling. Journal of Child Psychology and Freeman, R. D., Fast, D. K., Burd, L., Kerbeshian, J.,
Psychiatry, 46, 1327–1336. Robertson, M. M., & Sandor P. (2000). An interna-
Cummings, J. L., & Frankel, M. (1985). Gilles de la tional perspective on Tourette syndrome: selected
Tourette syndrome and the neurological basis of findings from 3,500 individuals in 22 countries.
obsessions and compulsions. Biological Psychiatry, Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 42,
20, 1117–1126. 436–447.
Curtis, D., Robertson, M. M., & Gurling, H. M. (1992). Frey, K. A., & Albin, R. L. (2006). Neuroimaging of
Autosomal dominant gene transmission in a large Tourette syndrome. Journal of Child Neurology, 21,
kindred with Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. 672–677.
British Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 845–849. Gaff ney, G. R. (1994). The MOVES: a self-rating
De Nil, L. F., Sasisekaran, J., Van Lieshout, P. H., scale for Tourette’s syndrome. Journal of Child
& Sandor, P. (2005). Speech disfluencies in indi- Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 4, 269–280.
viduals with Tourette syndrome. Journal of George, M. S., Trimble, M. R., Ring, H. A., Sallee,
Psychosomatic Research, 58, 97–102. F. R., & Robertson, M. M. (1993). Obsessions in
Deng, H., Le, W. D., Xie, W. J., & Jankovic, J. (2006). obsessive compulsive disorder with and without
Examination of the SLITRK1 gene in Caucasian Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. American Journal
patients with Tourette syndrome. Acta Neurologica of Psychiatry, 150, 93–97.
Scandinavica, 114, 400–402. Georgiou, N., Bradshaw, J. L., Phillips, J. G., Bradshaw,
Eapen, V., Fox-Hiley, P., Banerjee, S., & Robertson, J. A., & Chiu, E. (1995). The Simon effect and atten-
M. (2004). Clinical features and associated psy- tion deficits in Gilles de la Tourette Syndrome and
chopathology in a Tourette syndrome cohort. Acta Huntington´s disease. Brain, 118, 1305–1318.
Neurologica Scandinavica, 109, 255–260. Gillberg, I. C., Rasmussen, P., Kadesjo, B., Soderstrom,
Eapen, V., Laker, M., Anfield, A., Dobbs, J., & H., Rastam, M., Johnson, M., et al. (2004).
Robertson, M. M. (2001). Prevalence of tics and Co-existing disorders in ADHD-implications for
Tourette syndrome in an inpatient adult psychia- diagnosis and intervention. European Child and
try setting. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, Adolescent Psychiatry, 13, 80–89.
26, 417–420. Gilles de la Tourette, G. (1885). Etude sur une affec-
Eapen, V., Pauls, D., & Robertson, M. (1993). tion nerveuse caracterisee par de l’incoordination
Evidence for autosomal dominant transmission in motrice accompagnee d’echolalie et de copralalie.
Tourette’s syndrome. British Journal of Psychiatry, Archives of Neurology, 9, 19–42; 158–200.
163, 593–596. Gilles de la Tourette, G. (1899). La maladie des tics
Eapen, V., Robertson, M. M., Zeitlin, H., & Kurlan, convulsifs. La Semaine Medicale, 19, 153–156.
R. (1997). Gilles de la Tourette’s Syndrome in Goetz, C., & Klawans, H. (1982). Gilles de la Tourette’s
special education. Journal of Neurology, 244, syndrome. In A. Friedhoff & T. Chase (Eds.), Gilles
378–382. de la Tourette’s syndrome, advances in neurology,
Elstner, K., Selai, C. E., Trimble, M. R., & Robertson, vol. 35 (pp. 1–16). New York: Raven Press.
M. M. (2001). Quality of Life (QOL) of patients with Goetz, C. G., Tanner, C. M., Wilson, R. S., &
Gilles de la Tourette’s syndrome. Acta Psychiatrica Shannon, K. M. (1987). A rating scale for Gilles
Scandinavica, 103, 52–59. de la Tourette’s syndrome: Description, reliability,
Erenberg, G., Cruse, R. P., & Rothner, A. D. (1986). and validity data. Neurology, 37, 1542–4.
Tourette syndrome: an analysis of 200 pediatric Golden, G. S. (1984). Psychologic and neuropsycho-
and adolescent cases. Cleveland Clinic Quarterly, logic aspects of Tourette’s syndrome. Neurologic
53, 127–131. Clinics, 21, 91–102.
Erenberg, G., Cruse, R. P., & Rothner, A. D. (1987). Golden, G. S., & Greenhill, L. (1981). Tourette syn-
The natural history of Tourette syndrome: A fol- drome in mentally retarded children. Mental
low-up study. Annals of Neurology, 22, 383–385. Retardation, 19, 17–19.
The Neuropsychiatry and Neuropsychology of Gilles de la Tourette Syndrome 261

Grasset, J. (1890). Lecons sur un cas de maladie des school children. Journal of the American Academy
tics et un cas de tremblement singulier de la tete et of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 548–555.
des membres gauches. Archives of Neurology, 20, Keen-Kim, D., Mathews, C. A., Reus, V. I., Lowe, T.
27–45; 187–211. L., Herrera, L. D., Budman, C. L., et al. (2006).
Green, R. C., & Pitman, R. K. (1986). Tourette syn- Overrepresentation of rare variants in a specific
drome and obsessive-compulsive disorder. In M. ethnic group may confuse interpretation of asso-
A. Jenike, L. Baer, & W. O. Minichiello (Eds.), ciation analyses. Human Molecular Genetics, 15,
Obsessive-compulsive disorders: Theory and 3324–3328.
management (pp. 147–164). Littleton, MA: PSG Kelman, D. H. (1965). Gilles de la Tourette’s disease
Publishing Company. in children: A review of the literature. Journal of
Guinon, G. (1886). Sur la maladie des tics convulsifs. Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 6, 219–226.
Revue de Medicine, 6, 50–80. Kerbeshian, J., Burd, L., & Klug, M. G. (1995).
Hemmings, S. M., Kinnear, C. J., Lochner, C., Comorbid Tourette’s disorder and bipolar disor-
Niehaus, D. J., Knowles, J. A., Moolman-Smook, der: an etiologic perspective. American Journal of
J. C., et al. (2004). Early- versus late-onset obses- Psychiatry, 152, 1646–1651.
sive-compulsive disorder: investigating genetic Khalifa, N., & von Knorring, A. L. (2003). Prevalence
and clinical correlates. Psychiatry Research, 30, of tic disorders and Tourette syndrome in a Swedish
175–182. school population. Developmental Medicine and
Hoekstra, P. J., Steenhuis, M. P., Kallenberg, C. G., & Child Neurology, 45, 315–319.
Minderaa, R. B. (2004). Association of small life Khalifa, N., & von Knorring, A. L. (2005). Tourette
events with self reports of tic severity in pediat- syndrome and other tic disorders in a total popu-
ric and adult tic disorder patients: A prospective lation of children: Clinical assessment and back-
longitudinal study. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, ground. Acta Paediatrica, 94, 1608–1614.
65, 426–431. Kinnear-Wilson, S. A. (1927). Tics and child condi-
Hornsey, H., Banerjee, S., Zeitlin, H., & Robertson, tions. Journal of Neurology and Psychopathology,
M. M. (2001). The prevalence of Tourette syndrome 8, 93–109.
in young people inmainstream schools. Journal of Knell, E. R., & Comings, D. E. (1993). Tourette’s
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 1035–1039. syndrome and attention-deficit hyperactivity
Hounie, A. G., do Rosario-Campos, M. C., Diniz, J. disorder: evidence for a genetic relationship. The
B., Shavitt, R. G., Ferrao, Y. A., Lopes, A. C., et al. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 54, 331–337.
(2006). Obsessive-compulsive disorder in Tourette Kurlan, R., Behr, J., Medved, L., Shoulson, I.,
syndrome, Advances in Neurology, 99, 22–38. Pauls, D., Kidd, J. R., et al. (1986). Familial
Hyde, T. M., Aaronson, B. A., Randolph, C., Rickler, Tourette’s syndrome: Report of a large pedigree
K. C., & Weinberger, D. R. (1992). Relationship of and potential for linkage analysis. Neurology,
birth weight to the phenotypic expression of Gilles 36, 772–776.
de la Tourette’s syndrome in monozygotic twins. Kurlan, R., Daragjati, C., Como, P., McDermott, M.
Neurology, 42(3 Pt 1), 652–658. P., Trinidad, K. S., Roddy, S., et al. (1996). Non-
Hyde, T. M., Emsellem, H. A., Randolph, C., obscene complex socially inappropriate behavior
Rickler, K. C., & Weinberger, D. R. (1994). in Tourette’s syndrome. Journal of Neuropsychiatry
Electroencephalographic abnormalities in mono- and Clinical Neurosciences, 8, 311–317.
zygotic twins with Tourette’s syndrome. British Kurlan, R., Mc Dermott, M. P., Deeley, C., Como, P.
Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 811–817. G., Brower, C., Eapen, S., et al. (2001). Prevalence
Incagnoli, T., & Kane, R. (1981). Neuropsychological of tics in school children and association with
functioning in Gilles de la Tourette’s syndrome. placement in special education. Neurology, 57,
Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 3, 165–169. 1383–1388.
Itard, J. M. G. (1825). Memoire sur quelques fonc- Kushner, H. I. (1995). Medical fictions: the case of
tions involuntaires des appareils de la locomotion the cursing marquise and the (re)construction of
de la prehension et de la voix. Archives of General Gilles de la Tourette’s Syndrome. Bulletin of the
Medicine, 8, 385–407. History of Medicine, 69, 224–254.
Jankovic, J. (1997). Phenomenology and classification Kwak, C., Dat Vuong, K., & Jankovic, J. (2003).
of tics. In J. Jankovic (Ed.), Neurologic clinics (pp. Premonitory sensory phenomenon in Tourette’s
267–275). Philadelphia: WH Saunders Company. syndrome. Movement Disorders, 18(12),
Jankovic, J. (2001). Tourette’s syndrome. New 1530–1533.
England Journal of Medicine, 345, 1184–1192. Lanzi, G., Zambrino, C. A., Termine, C., Palestra,
Kadesjo, B., & Gillberg, C. (2000). Tourette’s disor- M., Ferrari Ginevra, O., Orcesi, S., et al. (2004).
der: epidemiology and comorbidity in primary Prevalence of tic disorders among primary school
262 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

students in the city of Pavia, Italy. Archives of International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology,


Disesase in Childhood, 89, 45–47. 4, 191–198.
Lavoie, M. E., Thibault, G., Stip, E., & O’Connor, Lerner, A., Bagic, A., Boudreau, E. A., Hanakawa, T.,
K. P. (2007). Memory and executive functions in Pagan, F., Mari, Z., et al. (2007). Neuroimaging
adults with Gilles de la Tourette syndrome and of neuronal circuits involved in tic generation in
chronic tic disorder. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, patients with Tourette syndrome. Neurology, 68,
12, 165–181. 1979–1987.
Leckman, J. F. (2002). Tourette’s syndrome. Lancet, Lin, H., Katsovich, L., Ghebremichael, M., Findley,
16, 1577–1586. D. B., Grantz, H., Lombroso, P. J., et al. (2007).
Leckman, J. F. (2003). In search of the pathophysi- Psychosocial stress predicts future symptom sever-
ology of Tourette syndrome. In M. A. Bedard, Y. ities in children and adolescents with Tourette
Agid, S. Chouinard, S. Fahn, A. D. Korczyn, & syndrome and/or obsessive-compulsive disorder.
P. Lesperance (Eds.), Mental and behavioral dys- Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48,
function in movement disorders. (pp. 467–476) 157–166.
Humana Press, Totowa, New Jersey. Loiselle, C. R., Lee, O., Moran, T. H., & Singer, H.
Leckman, J. F., Dolnansky, E. S., Hardin, M. T., S. (2004). Striatal microinfusion of Tourette syn-
Clubb, M., Walkup, J. T., Stevenson, J., et al. (1990). drome and PANDAS sera: Failure to induce behav-
Perinatal factors in the expression of Tourette’s ioral changes. Movement Disorders, 19, 371–374.
syndrome: An exploratory study. Journal of the Ludlow, C., Polinsky, R., Caine, E., Bassich, C., &
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Ebert, M. (1982). Language and speech abnormal-
Psychiatry, 29, 220–226. ities in Tourette’s syndrome. In A. Friedhoff and
Leckman, J. F., Grice, D. E., Barr, L. C., de Vries, A. T. Chase (Eds.), Gilles de la Tourette’s Syndrome,
L., Martin, C., Cohen, D. J., et al. (1994–1995). Tic- Advances in Neurology, vol. 35 (pp. 351–362). New
related vs. non-tic-related obsessive compulsive York: Raven Press.
disorder. Anxiety, 1, 208–215. Luo, F., Leckman, J. F., Katsovich, L., Findley,
Leckman, J. F., Pauls, D. L., Zhang, H., Rosario- D., Grantz, H., Tucker, D. M., et al. (2004).
Campos, M. C., Katsovich, L., Kidd, K. K., et al. Prospective longitudinal study of children with tic
(2003). Obsessive-compulsive symptom dimen- disorders and/or obsessive-compulsive disorder:
sions in affected sibling pairs diagnosed with Relationship of symptom exacerbations to newly
Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. American Journal acquired streptococcal infections. Pediatrics, 113,
of Medical Genetics B Neuropsychiatric Genetics, e578–e585.
116, 60–68. Mahler, M. S. (1949). Psychoanalytic evaluations of
Leckman, J. F., Riddle, M., & Harden, M. (1989). tics: A sign and symptom in psychopathology.
The Yale global tic severity scale: initial testing Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 3–4, 279.
of a clinician-rated scale of tic severity. Journal of Martino, D., Defazio, G., Church, A. J., Dale, R. C.,
the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Giovannoni, G., Robertson, M. M., et al. (2007).
Psychiatry, 28, 566–573. Antineuronal antibody status and phenotype anal-
Leckman, J. F., Walker, D. E., & Cohen, D. J. (1993). ysis in Tourette’s syndrome. Movement Disorders,
Premonitory urges in Tourette’s syndrome. 22, 1424–1429.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 98–102. Mathews, C. A., Jang, K. L., Herrera, L. D., Lowe,
Leckman, J. F., Zhang, H., Vitale, A., Lahnin, F., T. L., Budman, C. L., Erenberg, G., et al. (2007).
Lynch, K., Bondi, C., et al. (1998). Course of tic Tic symptom profiles in subjects with Tourette
severity in Tourette’s syndrome. Pediatrics, 102, Syndrome from two genetically isolated popula-
14–19. tions. Biological Psychiatry, 61, 292–300.
Lee, C. C., Chou, I. C., Tsai, C. H., Wang, T. R., Li, T. McHenry, L. C., Jr. (1967). Samuel Johnson’s tics and
C., & Tsai, F. J. (2005). Dopamine receptor D2 gene gesticulations. Journal of the History of Medicine,
polymorphisms are associated in Taiwanese chil- 22, 152–168.
dren with Tourette Syndrome. Pediatric Neurology, McMahon, W. M., van de Wetering, B. J., Filloux,
33, 272–276. F., Betit, K., Coon, H., & Leppert, M. (1996).
Lees, A., Robertson, M., Trimble, M., & Murray, N. Bilineal transmission and phenotypic variation
(1984). A clinical study of Gilles de la Tourette’s of Tourette’s disorder in a large pedigree. Journal
syndrome in the United Kingdom. Journal of of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 47, 1–8. Psychiatry, 35, 672–680.
Leonard, H. L., & Swedo, S. E. (2001). Paediatric Mell, L. K., Davis, R. L., & Owens, D. (2005).
autoimmune neuropsychiatric disorders asso- Association between streptococcal infection
ciated with streptococcal infection (PANDAS). and obsessive-compulsive disorder, Tourette’s
The Neuropsychiatry and Neuropsychology of Gilles de la Tourette Syndrome 263

syndrome, and tic disorder. Pediatrics, 116, Ozonoff, S., Strayer, D. L., McMahon, W. M., &
56–60. Filloux, F. (1994). Executive function abilities in
Miguel, E. C., Baer, L., Coffey, B. J., Rauch, S. L., autism and Tourette syndrome: An information
Savage, C. R., O’Sullivan, R. L., et al. (1997). processing approach. Journal of Child Psychology
Phenomenological differences appearing with and Psychiatry, 35, 1015–1032.
repetitive behaviours in obsessive-compulsive dis- Packer, L. E. (1997). Social and educational resources
order and Gilles de la Tourette’s syndrome. British for patients with Tourette syndrome. Neurologic
Journal of Psychiatry, 170, 140–145. Clinics, 15, 457–473.
Miguel, E. C., do Rosario-Campos, M. C., Prado, Pauls, D. L. (2003). An update on the genetics of Gilles
H. S., do Valle, R., Rauch, S. L., Coffey, B. J., et al. de la Tourette Syndrome. Journal of Psychosomatic
(2000). Sensory phenomena in obsessive-compul- Research, 55(1), 7–12.
sive disorder and Tourette’s disorder. Journal of Pauls, D. L. (2006). A genome-wide scan and fi ne
Clinical Psychiatry, 61, 150–156. mapping in Tourette syndrome families. Advances
Milberger, S., Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Murphy, in Neurology, 99, 130–135.
J., & Tsuang, M. T. (1995). Attention deficit hyper- Pauls, D. L., Cohen, D. J., Kidd, K. K., & Leckman, J.
activity disorder and comorbid disorders: Issues F. (1988). Tourette syndrome and neuropsychiatric
of overlapping symptoms. American Journal of disorders: Is there a genetic relationship? American
Psychiatry, 152, 1793–1799. Journal of Human Genetics, 43, 206–217.
Mink, J. W. (2006). Neurobiology of basal ganglia Pauls, D. L., Leckman, J., Towbin, K. E., Zahner,
and Tourette syndrome: Basal ganglia circuits and G. E., & Cohen, D. J. (1986a). A possible genetic
thalamocortical outputs. Advances in Neurology, relationship exists between Tourette’s syn-
99, 89–98. drome and obsessive-compulsive disorder.
Moll, G. H., Heinrich, H., Trott, G. E., Wirth, S., Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 22, 730–733.
Bock, N., & Rothenberger A. (2001). Children with Pauls, D. L., Towbin, K. E., Leckman, J. F., Zahner, G.
comorbid attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder E., & Cohen, D. J. (1986b). Gilles de la Tourette’s
and tic disorder: Evidence for additive inhibi- syndrome and obsessive compulsive disorder.
tory deficits within the motor system. Annals of Archives of General Psychiatry, 43, 1180–1182.
Neurology, 49, 393–396. Perugi, G., Akiskal, H. S., Ramacciotti, S., Nassini, S.,
Montgomery, M. A., Clayton, P. J., & Friedhoff, Toni, C., Milanfranchi, A., et al. (1999). Depressive
A. J. (1982). Psychiatric illness in Tourette syn- comorbidity of panic, social phobic, and obses-
drome patients and first-degree relatives. In A. sive-compulsive disorders re-examined: Is there
J. Friedhoff and T. N. Chase (Eds.), Gilles de la a bipolar II connection? Journal of Psychiatric
Tourette Syndrome, Advances in Neurology, vol. 35 Research, 33, 53–61.
(pp. 335–340). New York: Raven Press. Perugi, G., Toni, C., Frare, F., Traverso, M. C.,
Moriarty, J., Varma, A. R., Stevens, J., Fish, M., Hantouche, E., & Akiskal, H. S. (2002). Obsessive-
Trimble, M. R., & Robertson, M. M. (1997). A vol- compulsive-bipolar comorbidity: A systematic
umetric MRI study of Gilles de la Tourette’s syn- exploration of clinical features and treatment
drome. Neurology, 49, 410–415. outcome. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 63,
Morphew, J. A., & Sim, M. (1969). Gilles de la 1129–1134.
Tourette’s syndrome: A clinical and psychopatho- Peterson, B. S. (2001). Neuroimaging studies of
logical study. British Journal of Medical Psychology, Tourette syndrome: a decade of progress. In D.
42, 293–301. J. Cohen, C. G. Goetz, and J. Jankovic (Eds.),
Mula M., Cavanna, A. E., Critchley, H. D., Robertson, Tourette Syndrome. (pp. 179–196) Philadelphia:
M. M., & Monaco, F. (2008). Phenomenology of Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
obsessive compulsive disorder in patients with Peterson, B. S., Leckman, J. F., Scahill, L., Naftolin,
temporal lobe epilepsy and Gilles de la Tourette F., Keefe, D., Charest, N. J., et al. (1994). Steroid
syndrome. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical hormones and Tourette’s syndrome: Early experi-
Neurosciences, 20, 223–226. ence with antiandrogen therapy. Journal of Clinical
Murray, T. J. (1979). Dr. Samuel Johnson’s movement Psychopharmacology, 14, 131–135.
disorders. British Medical Journal, 1, 1610–1614. Peterson, B. S., Riddle, M., & Cohen, D. (1993).
Nomoto, F., & Machiyama, Y. (1990) An epidemi- Reduced basal ganglia volumes in Tourette’s syn-
ological study of tics. Journal of Psychiatry and drome using 3D reconstruction techniques from
Neurology, 44, 649–655. MRI. Neurology, 43, 941–949.
O’Quinn, A., & Thompson, R. (1980). Tourette’s Peterson, B. S., Thomas, P., Kane, M. J., Scahill, L.,
syndrome: An expanded review. Pediatrics, 66, Zhang, H., Bronen, R., et al. (2003). Basal gan-
420–424. glia volumes in patients with Gilles de la Tourette
264 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

syndrome. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, tions, and implications. Journal of Psychosomatic
415–424. Research, 61, 349–358.
Peterson, B. S., Zhang, H., Anderson, G. M., & Robertson, M. M. (2006b). Attention deficit hyperac-
Leckman, J. F. (1998). A double-blind, placebo- tivity disorder, tics and Tourette’s syndrome: The
controlled, crossover trial of an antiandrogen in relationship and treatment implications. European
the treatment of Tourette’s syndrome. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 15, 1–11.
Clinical Psychopharmacology, 18, 324–331. Robertson, M. M., Banerjee, S., Fox-Hiley, P. J., &
Plessen, K. J., Gruner, R., Lundervold, A., Hirsch, J. Tannock, C. (1997). Personality disorder and
G., Xu, D., Bansal, R., et al. (2006). Reduced white psychopathology in Tourette’s syndrome: A con-
matter connectivity in the corpus callosum of trolled study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 171,
children with Tourette syndrome. Journal of Child 283–286.
Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 1013–1022. Robertson, M. M., Banerjee, S., Kurlan, R., Cohen, D.
Plessen, K. J., Wentzel-Larsen, T., Hugdahl, K., J., Leckman, J. F., McMahon, W., et al. (1999). The
Feineigle, P., Klein, J., Staib, L. H., et al. (2004). Tourette syndrome diagnostic confidence index:
Altered interhemispheric connectivity in individ- Development and clinical associations. Neurology,
uals with Tourette’s disorder. American Journal of 53, 2108–2112.
Psychiatry, 161, 2028–2037. Robertson, M. M., & Cavanna, A. E. (2007). The
Randolph, C., Hyde, T. M., Gold, J. M., Goldberg, Gilles de la Tourette syndrome: A principal com-
T. E., & Weinberger, D. R. (1993). TS in monozy- ponent factor analytic study of a large pedigree.
gotic twins. Relationship of tic severity to neuro- Psychiatric Genetics, 17, 143–152.
spychological functions. Archives of Neurology, 50, Robertson, M. M., Channon, S., Baker, J., & Flynn,
725–728. D. (1993). The psychopathology of Gilles de la
Regeur, L., Pakkenberg, B., Fog, R., & Pakkenberg, Tourette syndrome: a controlled study. British
H. (1986). Clinical features and long term treat- Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 114–117.
ment with pimozide in 65 patients with Gilles Robertson, M. M., & Eapen, V. (1992). Pharmacologic
de la Tourette’s syndrome. Journal of Neurology, controversy of CNS stimulants in Gilles de la
Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 49, 791–795. Tourette’s syndrome. Clinical Neuropharmacology,
Rickards, H. (2001). Signing coprolalia and attempts 15, 408–425.
to disguise in a man with prelingual deafness. Robertson, M. M., & Eapen, V. (1996). The National
Movement Disorders, 16, 790–791. Hospital Interview Schedule for the assessment
Rizzo, R., Curatolo, P., Gulisano, M., Virzi, of Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. International
M., Arpino, C., & Robertson, M. M. (2007). Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 6,
Disentangling the effects of Tourette syndrome 203–226.
and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder on Robertson, M. M., & Gourdie, A. (1990). Familial
cognitive and behavioral phenotypes. Brain and Tourette’s syndrome in a large British pedigree:
Development, 29, 413–420. Associated psychopathology, severity of Tourette’s
Rizzo, R., Gulisano, M., Pavone, P., Fogliari, F., & and potential for linkage analysis. British Journal
Robertson, M. M. (2006). Increased antistreptoc- of Psychiatry, 156, 515–521.
cal antibody titres and anti-basal ganglia antibod- Robertson, M. M., & Orth, M. (2006). Behavioral
ies in Tourette’s syndrome: A controlled study. and affective disorders in Tourette syndrome.
Journal of Child Neurology, 21, 747–753. Advances in Neurology, 99, 39–60.
Robertson, M. M. (1995). The relationship between Robertson, M. M., & Reinstein, D. Z. (1991).
Gilles de la Tourette’s syndrome and obses- Convulsive tic disorder. Georges Gilles de la
sive compulsive disorder. Journal of Serotonin Tourette, Guinon, and Grasset on the phenomenol-
Research, Suppl. 1, 49–62. ogy and psychopathology of Gilles de la Tourette
Robertson, M. M. (2000). Invited Review. Tourette syndrome. Behavioral Neurology, 4, 29–56.
Syndrome, associated conditions and the com- Robertson, M. M., Trimble, M. R., & Lees, A. J. (1988).
plexities of treatment. Brain, 123, 425–462. The psychopathology of the Gilles de la Tourette
Robertson, M. M. (2004). Tourette Syndrome; an syndrome: A phenomenological analysis. British
update. Psychiatry 111, The Medicine Publishing Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 383–390.
Company Group (ed Littlewood RL). Psychiatry, Robertson, M. M., Trimble, M. R., & Lees, A. J.
3(8), 3–7. (1989). Self-injurious behavior and the Gilles de
Robertson, M. M. (2006a). Mood disorders and la Tourette syndrome. A clinical study and review
Gilles de la Tourette’s syndrome: An update on of the literature. Psychological Medicine, 19,
prevalence, etiology, comorbidity, clinical associa- 611–625.
The Neuropsychiatry and Neuropsychology of Gilles de la Tourette Syndrome 265

Rosario-Campos, M. C., Leckman, J. F., Curi, M., Spencer, T., Biederman, J., Harding, M., O’Donnell,
Quadrano, S., Katsovich, L., Miguel, E. C., et al. D., Wilens, T., Faraone, S., et al. (1998).
(2005). A family study of early-onset obsessive- Disentangling the overlap between Tourette’s dis-
compulsive disorder. American Journal of Medical order and ADHD. Journal of Child Psychology and
Geneticts. Part B, Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 136, Psychiatry, 39, 1037–1044.
92–97. Stephens, R. J., & Sandor, P. (1999). Aggressive behav-
Salmon, G., James, A., & Smith, D. M. (1998). iour in children with Tourette syndrome and
Bullying in schools: Self reported anxiety, depres- comorbid attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder
sion, and self esteem in secondary school children. and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Canadian
British Medical Journal, 317, 924–925. Journal of Psychiatry, 44, 1036–1042.
Sandor, P., Musisi, S., Moldofsky, H., & Lang, A. Stern, E., Silbersweig, D. A., Chee, K. Y., Holmes,
(1990). Tourette syndrome: A follow-up study. A., Robertson, M. M., Trimble, M., et al. (2000).
Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 10, A functional neuroanatomy of tics in Tourette
197–199. syndrome. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57,
Santangelo, S. L., Pauls, D. L., Goldstein, J. M., 741–748.
Faraone, S. V., Tsuang, M. T., & Leckman, J. F. Sukhodolsky, D. G., Scahill, L., Zhang, H., Peterson,
(1994). Tourette’s syndrome: What are the influ- B. S., King, R. A., Lombroso, P. J., et al. (2003).
ences of gender and comorbid obsessive-compul- Disruptive behavior in children with Tourette’s
sive disorder? Journal of the American Academy of syndrome: Association with ADHD comorbidity,
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 33, 795–804. tic severity, and functional impairment. Journal
Schuerholz, L. J., Baumgardner T. L., Singer, S. S., Reiss, of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
A. L., & Denckla, M. B. (1996). Neuropsychological Psychiatry, 42, 98–105.
Status of Children with Tourette’s syndrome with Sutherland, R., Kolb, B., Schoel, W., Whishaw, I., &
and without attention deficit hyperactivity disor- Davies, D. (1982). Neuropsychological assessment
der. Neurology, 46, 958–965. of children and adults with Tourette’s syndrome: A
Schuerholz, L. J., Singer, H. S., & Denckla, M. B. comparison with learning difficulties and schizo-
(1998). Gender study of neuropsychological and phrenia. In A. Friedhoff and T. Chase (Eds.), Gilles
neuromotor function in children with Tourette de la Tourette’s Syndrome, Advances in Neurology,
syndrome with and without attention-deficit vol. 35 (pp. 311–322). New York: Raven Press.
hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child Neurology, Swedo, S. E., Leonard, H. L., Garvey, M., Mittleman,
13, 277–282. B., Allen, A. J., Perlmutter, S., et al. (1998).
Shallice, T. (1988). From neuropsychology to neural Pediatric autoimmune neuropsychiatric disorders
structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. associated with streptococcal infections: Clinical
Shapiro, A., Shapiro, E., & Clarkin, J. (1974). Clinical description of the first 50 cases. American Journal
psychological testing in Tourette’s syndrome. of Psychiatry, 155, 264–271.
Journal of Personality Assessment, 38, 464–478. Tabori-Kraft, J. (2007). Clinical features of children
Shapiro, A. K., Shapiro, E. S., Bruun, R. D., & Sweet, with tic disorders: A population based study of chil-
R. D. (1978). Gilles de la Tourette Syndrome. New dren aged 9–11. International Scientific Symposium
York: Raven Press. on Tourette Syndrome Lillehammer, Norway, 66.
Sherman, E. M., Shepard, L., Joschko, M., & Takeuchi, K., Yamashita, M., Morikiyo, M., Takeda,
Freeman, R. D. (1998). Sustained attention and N., Morita, K., Tamura, T., et al. (1986). Gilles de
impulsivity in children with Tourette syndrome: la Tourette’s syndrome and schizophrenia. Journal
Comorbidity and confounds. Journal of Clinical of Nervous and Mental Disease, 174, 247–248.
and Experimental Neuropsychology, 20, 644–657. Taylor, E. (1985). Syndromes of overactivity and
Schultz, R. T., Carter, A. S., Gladstone, M., Scahill, L., attention deficit. In M. Rutter and L. Hersov
Leckman, J. F., Peterson, B. S., et al. (1998). Visual- (Eds.), Child and adolescent psychiatry: Modern
motor functioning in children with Tourette syn- approaches/II (pp. 424–443). Oxford/Boston:
drome. Neuropsychology, 12, 134–145. Blackwells Scientific Publications.
Singer, H. S. (2005). Tourette’s syndrome: From Thibert, A. L., Day, H. I., & Sandor, P. (1995).
behaviour to biology. Lancet Neurology, 4, Self-concept and self-consciousness in adults
149–159. with Tourette syndrome. Canadian Journal of
Singer, H. S., Reiss, A., Brown, J., Aylward, E. H., Shih, Psychiatry, 40, 35–39.
B., Chee, E., et al. (1993). Volumetric MRI changes Walkup, J. T., LaBuda, M. C., Singer, H. S., Brown,
in basal ganglia of children with Tourette’s syn- J., Riddle, M. A., & Hurko, O. (1996). Family
drome. Neurology, 43, 950–956. study and segregation analysis of Tourette
266 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

syndrome: Evidence for a mixed model of inher- World Health Organization. (1992). International
itance. American Journal of Human Genetics, 59, classification of diseases and health-related
684–693. problems/X. Geneva: World Health Organization.
Walkup, J. T., Rosenberg, L. A., Brown, J., & Singer, Yaryura-Tobias, J. A., Neziroglu, F., Howard, S., &
H. S. (1992). The validity of instruments measur- Fuller, B. (1981). Clinical aspects of Gilles de la
ing tic severity in Tourette’s syndrome. Journal of Tourette syndrome. Journal of Orthomolecular
the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 10, 263–268.
Psychiatry, 31, 472–477. Yordanova, J., Dumais-Huber, C., & Rothenberger,
Wang, H. S., & Kuo, M. F. (2003). Tourette’s syn- A. (1996). Coexistence of tics and hyperactivity in
drome in Taiwan: An epidemiological study children: No additive at the psychophysiological
of tic disorders in an elementary school at level. International Journal of Psychophysiology,
Taipei County. Brain Development, 25(Suppl 1), 21, 121–133.
29–31. Yordanova, J., Dumais-Huber, C., Rothenberger,
Wilson, R. S., Garron, D.C., Tanner, C. M., & Klawans, A., & Woerner, W. (1997). Frontocortical activity
H. L. (1982). Behavior disturbance in children in children with comorbidity of tic disorder and
with Tourette syndrome. In A. J. Friedhoff and T. attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Biological
N. Chase (Eds.), Gilles de la Tourette’s syndrome, Psychiatry, 41, 585–594.
advances in neurology, vol. 35 (pp. 329–334). New Yordanova, J., Heinrich, H., Kolev, V., & Rothenberger,
York: Raven Press. A. (2006). Increased event-related theta activity as
Winokur, G. (1997). All roads lead to depression: a psychophysiological marker of comorbidity in
Clinically homogeneous, etiologically heteroge- children with tics and attention-deficit/hyperac-
neous. Journal of Affective Disorders, 45, 97–108. tivity disorders. Neuroimage, 32, 940–955.
Woods, D. W., Piacentini, J., Himle, M. B., & Chang, Zheng, R. Y., Jin, R., Xu, H. Q., Huang, W. W., Chen,
S. (2005). Premonitory Urge for Tics Scale (PUTS): H., Shao, B., et al. (2004). Study on the prevalence
Initial psychometric results and examination of the of tic disorders in schoolchildren aged 16 years old
premonitory urge phenomenon in youths with tic in Wenzhou. Zhongua Liu Xing Bing Za Zhi, 25,
disorders. Journal of Developmental Behavioural 745–747.
Pediatrics, 26, 397–403. Zhu, Y., Leung, K. M., Liu, P. Z., Zhou, M., & Su, L.
Wong, C. K., & Lau, J. T. (1992). Psychiatric morbid- Y. (2006). Comorbid behavioral problems in TS
ity in a Chinese primary school in Hong Kong. are persistently correlated with the severity of tic
Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, symptoms. Australian and New Zealand Journal of
26, 459–466. Psychiatry, 40, 67–73.
12

The Neuropsychology of Epilepsy


Henry A. Buchtel and Linda M. Selwa

Seizures, Stigma, Localization,


cause the onset of a particular seizure is beyond
and Treatment
the scope of this chapter. An excellent review
Epilepsy was defined in 2005 by the International can be found in Dichter (1998). In general, epi-
League Against Epilepsy as “a disorder of lepsy results from hyperexcitability in neuronal
the brain characterized by an enduring pre- circuits, resulting from genetic, structural, or
disposition to generate epileptic seizures and biochemical alterations in fundamental neu-
by the neurological, cognitive, psychological ronal function. Basic molecular mechanisms
and social consequences of this condition” of this hyperexcitability are still being studied.
(Fisher et al., 2005). This new interpretation of Likely factors that are critical to epileptogenesis
the illness emphasizes the importance of the in various types of epilepsy include dysfunction
neuropsychological and behavioral aspects of in sodium, calcium, or potassium channels in
epilepsy. For many years the behavioral mani- excitable membranes, abnormalities in neuro-
festations of acute seizures, which can include transmitter receptors or transport molecules,
fear, inattention, anger, or mood changes, have and changes in trophic substances that deter-
attracted the attention of behavioral neurolo- mine cellular growth and plasticity. Specific
gists, psychiatrists, and neuropsychologists. examples of potential cellular influences of epi-
In recent years, the importance of cognitive lepsy include enhanced calcium currents and
and psychological assessments has been rec- downregulated potassium currents in mesial
ognized in the evaluation of epilepsy surgery temporal lobe epilepsy, which is likely to be
patients. Social factors have been recognized influenced by the effects of GABA metabotropic
as significant determinants of the quality of life receptors in the cortex and glutamate (AMPA
of epilepsy patients. This chapter will provide and Kainate) receptors in cortical and hippo-
a brief description of the brain substrates that campal neurons. Neuronal plasticity and inter-
predispose to epilepsy and the classification of connection are also critical for initiation and
epilepsy syndromes. Information about cogni- propagation of many types of seizures and these
tive, social, and behavioral issues for patients relationships can be altered through any com-
with epilepsy will be provided, and psycholog- bination of acquired brain abnormalities and
ical syndromes that can mimic epilepsy will be genetic predispositions.
described. Finally, the assessment of and treat- The genetics of epilepsy is complex and only
ment options for patients with epilepsy will be a small proportion of epilepsies have been char-
reviewed. acterized genetically. Most individuals with
A review of the brain abnormalities causing epilepsy do not have a first-degree relative with
epilepsy and the specific biochemical events that epilepsy, and when a family history is present,

267
268 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

a simple Mendelian pattern is not present. treatment, mental and emotional disability, and
However, if an individual has epilepsy with- social dysfunction is substantial.
out any obvious precipitating cause (e.g., head The incidence of epilepsy (new cases in a par-
injury), the chance that a sibling (3.6%) or off- ticular time period) has been estimated to be
spring (10.6%) will have epilepsy is higher than around 0.5 to 1.0 per 1000 per year, meaning that
in the general population (1.7%). The risk of in a group of 1,000,000 individuals there would
epilepsy is not greater in more distant relatives be about 500–1000 new cases each year. Data
(nieces, nephews, and grandchildren). The evi- from studies in Great Britain indicate that most
dence that the apparent heritability is genetic seizure disorders begin in the age range 0–4
rather than, for example, environmental, comes years (0.75/1000). The next most common ages
from twin studies. If one of a pair of monozy- of onset are 15–19 years (0.53/1000), 10–14 years
gotic twins has epilepsy, there is about a 49% (0.44/1000), and 5–9 years (0.33/1000). All other
probability that the other one will also have epi- age groups have incidence rates below 0.23/1000.
lepsy. If the twins are dizygotic, the concordance Males and females appear to be equally affected
rate is considerably lower, about 16% (Kjeldsen and there are more left-handers with epilepsy
et al., 2003). Same-sex dizygotic twins have than would be expected by chance.
a higher concordance rate than different-sex The diagnosis of epilepsy is made by combin-
twins (13% versus 5.5%, Kjeldsen et al., 2003). ing a witnessed description of the behavioral
When both twins have epilepsy, it is almost manifestations of brain electrical abnormali-
always of the same kind, that is, partial versus ties and the results of diagnostic tests, including
generalized (94%, Berkovic et al., 1998). EEG (electroencephalography) and structural
Prevalence and incidence: Estimates of the imaging with MRI. The differential diagnosis of
prevalence of epilepsy in the general popula- epilepsy includes syncope, parasomnias, com-
tion, that is, the ratio of individuals affected to plicated migraines, and metabolic abnormal-
those unaffected, have ranged from 2 per 1000 ities including hyper/hypoglycemia. The EEG
to 50 per 1000. The variability may reflect the is abnormal in 60% of patients with epilepsy
different definitions of what constitutes epi- between seizures. During an epileptic seizure,
lepsy (Is there a minimum seizure frequency?, if consciousness is impaired the EEG will dem-
Are febrile seizures included?, Is the diagnosis onstrate clear abnormalities. In those with psy-
accurate?, and so forth). On average, the most chological episodes resembling seizures, there
accurate estimates suggest that the prevalence are clear distinguishing clinical features, and
is around 5 per 1000, meaning that in a city the EEG is normal during the episodes despite
of 1,000,000 there would be 5000 individuals apparent loss of consciousness. Essentially, epi-
with seizures. Thus, in the United States, with lepsy is an illness characterized by repetitive
a population just above 300,000,000, there are seizures. Some seizures are symptomatic of
approximately 1.5 million individuals with epi- brief systemic changes that are not dependent
lepsy. Some studies have estimated the preva- on specific localized neuronal abnormalities
lence to be as high as 50 million, with more than in the brain. Seizures can occur due to drug
300,000 children under the age of 14 years and intoxication, alcohol withdrawal, or glucose or
more than 500,000 individuals over the age of electrolyte imbalances, and these episodes are
65. With 6.6 billion individuals in the world, not considered to be epileptic in the same sense
approximately 33 million will have epilepsy. that a spontaneous predisposition to recurrent
Some estimates are as high as 50 million. Twenty stereotyped episodes are. Patients with epi-
to thirty percent of these will have intractable lepsy have a lasting neuronal predisposition to
seizures (uncontrollable with current medi- abnormal electrical discharges that remains
cal treatments). Over a lifetime, 3%–5% of all even when acute episodes are controlled with
individuals will have at least one seizure; of medications.
those who have a seizure, approximately 12% In social terms, the stigma and loss of inde-
will develop chronic epilepsy. The cost of this pendence that occurs as a result of the disease
common debilitating illness in terms of medical are often as or more detrimental to overall
The Neuropsychology of Epilepsy 269

quality of life than the cerebral dysfunction A second classification dimension is based
that has caused the episodes. The involuntary on the brain regions felt to be responsible for
contractions, unusual vocalizations and behav- the generation of the epileptogenic zone. If the
ior, incontinence, and tongue biting that occur epilepsy involves seizures that start in one cor-
during many seizures result in fear and disgust tical structure and spread to a variable degree
in uneducated observers and lead to a substan- to other structures, it is referred to as a partial
tial social stigma. In many cultures, those with epilepsy (formerly called focal seizures). The
seizures are considered to be deranged or dan- individual may experience unusual emotional,
gerous, and in some it is even felt that their con- cognitive, sensory, motor, or autonomic phe-
dition may be infectious. In ancient Greece, these nomena that reflect the area of onset of indi-
patients were felt to be possessed, and epilepsy vidual seizures. Examples of focal seizures that
was called the “sacred disease” (see Tempkin, can occur with this type of epilepsy include
1971 for an excellent historical review). While those with olfactory auras, prominent emo-
the attitudes of modern society has improved, tional changes, visual symptoms, or involun-
epilepsy patients are often denied employment tary motor movements. Reflex epilepsies are
in public positions and are not allowed to drive generally partial seizure disorders triggered by
or operate many kinds of machinery. Seizures activation of the irritable epileptogenic zone
are hidden as much as possible; the social iso- (e.g., Heschel’s gyrus seizures can begin after
lation of individuals with this disorder means exposure to specific trigger sounds). Idiopathic
that many in the population have never seen a partial epilepsies include benign epilepsy with
seizure. centrotemporal spikes (BECTS) and benign
Classification: The most recent discussion occipital epilepsy. The most common refractory
from the International League Against Epilepsy symptomatic partial epilepsy in adults is mesial
about the best scheme for classification and temporal epilepsy.
terminology of seizures occurred in 2001 If the entire brain is involved at the outset of
(Engel, 2001). We will attempt to provide a con- ictal events or in the generation of the seizures,
cise summary of the most current terminology the condition is classified as generalized epi-
about the types of seizures and the epileptic syn- lepsy. Some patients with generalized epilepsies
dromes that exist. In general, seizure types refer can have both partial onset seizures and gener-
to the behavioral phenomenology (semiology) alized seizures. Most patients with generalized
and EEG findings in particular events. Epilepsy epilepsy have generalized electrographic dis-
syndromes refer to a more specific individual charges, and most lose consciousness with the
diagnosis that takes into account the cause of majority of their seizures, but exceptions exist
the seizures, semiology and EEG findings, and in cases where the generator of the seizures is
likely prognostic outcome. Further information felt to represent a global process. Idiopathic
is available in books and helpful Internet sites generalized epilepsies include absence epilepsy,
listed in Appendix 1. In terms of epilepsy types, epilepsy with generalized tonic-clonic sei-
seizures with known structural abnormalities zures (GTCs), and juvenile myoclonic epilepsy.
or clear preceding brain injury are generally Symptomatic generalized epilepsies include
referred to as symptomatic. Examples are sei- Lennox-Gastaut syndrome and seizures in
zures arising from mesial temporal lobe sclero- patients with early onset injuries.
sis, neuronal migrational errors in the cerebral In the case of partial seizures, if the episode
cortex, stroke, head injury, anoxia, tumors, or progresses to amnesia for the event or impair-
cerebral palsy. If the cause is not known, the ment or loss of consciousness, it is referred to as
epilepsy is idiopathic or cryptogenic. The former a complex partial seizure. If this initial period
term is used when no identifiable cause of the is followed by a typical GTC, the event is called
seizures can be found. The latter term is used a “secondarily generalized partial seizure.”
when the clinicians think that there is an abnor- Generalized seizure types include GTC, tonic
mality in brain structure but it has not yet been seizures, atonic seizures, myoclonic seizures,
identified. and absence seizures.
270 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Most of these seizures are brief: GTCs usually seizures (Hermann et al., 2006). The lack of
last between 60 and 90 seconds. When an indi- improvement was seen as a decline from expec-
vidual seizure lasts longer than 30 minutes, or tation. However, it is also possible that these
repeated seizures continue for 30 minutes with- patients do not benefit from practice effects and
out resumption of normal behavior, the event is simply perform at the same level over multiple
termed “status epilepticus.” Status epilepticus assessments.
with generalized convulsive movements has
a mortality of over 20% in several U.S. series,
Neuropsychometric Testing in
with the worst prognosis in older patients with-
Epilepsy
out a prior diagnosis of epilepsy. The sequelae of
status epilepticus include cognitive, cerebellar,
Choosing the Right Interictal Tests
or even motor deficits.
for Localization and Evaluation of the
In adults partial epilepsies are most common,
Patient
with temporal lobe epilepsy accounting for the
majority of those with partial onset seizures. In There are at least four primary reasons for
adults over 50, stroke is the most common cause assessing cognitive and noncognitive abilities
of epilepsy, with hemorrhagic strokes resulting in patients with epilepsy. Given the difficulties
in the highest incidence of seizures. Idiopathic with normal education in children with sei-
generalized epilepsies account for roughly 30% zures, it is often valuable to carry out baseline
of seizure disorders, with absence epilepsies intelligence and memory testing early in the
more common in children and adolescents. child’s school career in order to maximize the
Idiopathic epilepsy is generally more amenable chance that the child’s educational curriculum
to treatment with antiepileptic drugs (AEDs) is tailored to his or her cognitive abilities. A sec-
than symptomatic generalized epilepsies. ond reason for assessing and monitoring cogni-
There is a widespread perception that hav- tive abilities is to determine whether changes in
ing seizures is bad for the brain. Clinicians fre- medication regimen have affected intellectual
quently see some of their medically refractory or attentional abilities. Third, the neuropsy-
patients (patients whose seizures cannot be chological profi le of patients with seizures can
controlled by medication) deteriorate over the provide indications of when to consider sei-
years, and it is natural to assume that the sei- zures of psychogenic origin; see below for fur-
zures themselves are the cause. Obvious reasons ther discussion of this distinction. Finally, some
could be prolonged periods of apnea during patients have medically intractable seizures and
the seizure and the increased area of the brain may be considered for surgical relief. If so, it is
with abnormal neuronal discharges caused by important to carry out a full neuropsychologi-
kindling. A less obvious cause is falling during cal assessment as a baseline before surgery and
seizures, leading to multiple head injuries. An to determine whether the pattern of deficits is
alternative is that the brain disease that causes in correspondence with other sources of evi-
the seizures is progressive in some cases. It is dence for the lateralization and localization
also possible that the perceived deterioration of the suspected epileptogenic focus. The tests
is rare and that the vast majority of patients and procedures for each of these four purposes
remain stable cognitively despite the continua- will vary, but in each case up-to-date intelli-
tion of their seizures. Data on both sides of this gence and memory testing is clearly indicated.
argument have been published. Helmstaedter Objective sensory and motor testing is also
et al. (2003) followed 249 patients (147 surgi- clearly needed, and the use of a dynamometer
cally treated and 102 medically treated) and and tapping instrument are used in psychomet-
reported that chronic uncontrolled seizures ric evaluations in order to have objective num-
led to memory decline, consistent with pro- bers that are reliable. The minimum testing
gressive medial temporal lobe atrophy. Most allowable can be inferred from the test battery
recently, a study has been published showing used in a recently completed study of the ben-
that the expected practice effect of repeat cogni- efits from early surgical interventions (ERSET);
tive testing is absent in patients with intractable this had a 90-minute neuropsychological test
The Neuropsychology of Epilepsy 271

battery agreed upon by neuropsychologists in problems do not accompany these verbal mem-
18 different sites around the United States. This ory deficits, the dysfunction is probably mesial
abbreviated battery consisted of the following alone, without involvement of the neocortex.
measures: WAIS-III or WISC-III Vocabulary, Visual–spatial and somatosensory disorders
Block Design, and Digit Span; Rey Auditory are usually indicative of parietal dysfunction,
Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT); Boston Naming and abnormal scores on motor and executive
Test (60 item) (BNT); Controlled Oral Word function tasks usually indicate a disturbance
Association Test (COWAT); Brief Visuospatial of functions of anterior brain regions. Many
Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R); Trail- patients have neurocognitive deficits in more
Making Test (A & B) (TMT); Grooved Pegboard than one area, indicating that the process or
(full board, one trial): and the Multiple Ability injury causing their seizures has affected more
Self-Report Questionnaire (MASQ). The last than one part of the brain. This does not mean
measure is used to assess subjective changes in that there are multiple epileptogenic foci, but if
cognition by asking questions about the exis- the individual is being considered for epilepsy
tence and frequency of real-life cognitive fail- surgery, the best candidates for successful con-
ures (e.g., “I forget important things I was told trol of seizures after surgery are those whose
just a few days ago.”). neurocognitive deficits are restricted to the
At the University of Michigan, as at many region that has been identified as abnormal by
other centers evaluating patients for seizure other means (EEG, imaging).
surgery, we add to this list a number of cog-
nitive, sensory, motoric, and mood measures.
The Wada Test
We include the other WAIS-III subtests for
full Verbal and Performance IQ measures, the One of the most fruitful procedures performed
Wechsler Memory Scale-III, Grip Strength and during the evaluation for seizure surgery is a
Finger Oscillation, the expanded Halstead– test during which a neuroradiologist injects
Reitan Neuropsychological Inventory (Tapping, an anesthetic (sodium amobarbital or metho-
Tactual Performance Test, Speech Perception, hexital) into the internal carotid artery of one
Seashore Rhythm Test, Category Test), Aphasia hemisphere and then the other. This temporar-
Screening, and Trail-Making Test (A&B). To ily inactivates large portions of the hemisphere
assess mood and personality variables, we on the side of the injection, either directly or
give the MMPI-2, Miale-Holsopple Sentence indirectly (e.g., visual cortex is deafferented by
Completion, the Washington Psychosocial interruption of neuronal activity in the lateral
Seizure Inventory, and a Self-Rating Question- geniculate). This allows the clinicians to deter-
naire. mine whether language abilities are on the side
Inferences about the location(s) of neurocog- being injected and to allow an assessment of
nitive dysfunction are based on the pattern of the likelihood that surgery on that side would
strengths and weaknesses in the results of the lead to postsurgery amnesia. The test frequently
neuropsychological assessment. The examiner goes by the name intracarotid amobarbital pro-
first has to take into account educational and cedure (IAP), but the increasing use of metho-
psychosocial influences on this pattern. Once hexital (Brevital; see Buchtel et al., 2002) has
this is done, the presence or absence of lan- led to the adoption of a more generic term, the
guage deficits, visual–spatial problems, exec- Wada test, which honors its inventor and leaves
utive function, and motor deficits can be used unstated the drug used. The procedure was
to identify regions of abnormal functioning. developed initially at the Montreal Neurological
Naming problems are usually seen when the Institute, where the neurologist Juhn Wada was
epileptogenic focus involves the functions of a fellow in the 1950s. In addition to its clinical
the language-dominant hemisphere (usually usefulness, this procedure has allowed testing
left hemisphere in right-handers and most of of various cognitive abilities when only one
left-handers); verbal memory problems are usu- hemisphere is working. The patient receives
ally seen with mesial temporal lobe focus in a cerebral angiogram in order to know where
the language-dominant hemisphere; if naming the drug is going to go. Then a small quantity of
272 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

sodium amobarbital or methohexital is injected and a Wada test or cortical mapping procedure
slowly while the clinicians monitor motor (either intraoperative or extraoperative) can
and cognitive abilities. Expected neurologi- help to define safe margins of resection that
cal changes include contralateral hemiplegia, will not impair memory, language, or motor
hemianopsia, and loss of tactile sensation. If function.
the language hemisphere is injected, the patient In patients with other types of lesions,
stops talking and then after some time may including vascular anomalies (arteriovenous
speak but with paraphasias and naming dif- malformations [AVMs], venous angiomas,
ficulties. Understanding what the examiner etc.), dysembryoplastic neuroepithelial tumor
is saying is usually, but not invariably, absent. (DNET), or cortical dysplasia including hamar-
Several minutes after injection of the anesthetic, tomas, resections can also be tailored to presur-
the patient will begin to recover motor and sen- gical tests of ictal onset and functional testing of
sory functions, and, if the language hemisphere the most electrically active regions to determine
was injected, language functions will return to safe margins of resection when these structures
baseline. In addition to assessment of language are determined to be related to the ictal onset
functions during the action of the drug, the zone.
clinician will present new information to the For those patients with mesial temporal scle-
patient for later recall in order to determine rosis, one of the most common indications for
whether the noninjected hemisphere is capa- epilepsy surgery for those refractory to medica-
ble of forming new memories on its own. When tions, neuropsychometric testing, and the Wada
the nonlanguage hemisphere is injected, speech test can be important in verifying the area of
may be slurred because of contralateral facial seizure onset. These tests can be strong corrob-
weakness, but the person can follow commands orating features that identify the lateralization
using the unaffected hand and can understand of the most epileptogenic area, and can provide
speech and talk. Patients are typically unaware information about the likelihood of memory
of the neurological and neuropsychological or language deficits after surgery. For instance,
deficits during the test, although there are occa- if visuospatial memory is significantly more
sional exceptions, usually when the language impaired than verbal memory, it is likely that
hemisphere is injected. The reason patients are the seizure onset is in the right (or nondomi-
sometimes aware of right-sided hemiplegia and nant) hemisphere. If verbal memory is impaired,
language problems has never been established it may be a bit more likely that seizures begin in
with certainty, but one theory is that the right the dominant mesial temporal region, but this
hemisphere has more awareness of what is hap- association is less strong and therefore less spe-
pening on the right side of the body than the left cific. The degree of verbal memory difficulty
hemisphere has of the left side. It is also possi- can help predict the amount of initial deficit
ble, though less likely, that the loss of language after resection of the dominant mesial temporal
and the use of the dominant hand is more strik- structures—those with better verbal skills tend
ing and therefore more likely to be remembered to lose more functional ability. The Wada test
afterwards. is critical in these patients to the localization
of language and memory function, and usually
serves as a final check to ensure that the out-
Surgery
come of the proposed surgery is anticipated and
Sometimes the seizure is the first manifestation any modifications in the surgical plan can be
of the presence of a brain tumor. Even when made. Up to 75% of those with mesial tempo-
resection of the tumor is the most appropri- ral sclerosis and concordant neuropsychometric
ate option, localization of the ictal onset zone testing, EEG, and clinical findings can go on to
can help to tailor the resection to provide the have complete control of all seizures that might
highest chance of seizure freedom after the affect consciousness after appropriate resective
lesion is removed. Neuropsychometric testing procedures.
can be particularly helpful in cases where the In patients who are refractory to medical
lesion affects the mesial temporal structures, management and who have no clear lesions on
The Neuropsychology of Epilepsy 273

imaging (usually epilepsy protocol MRI), surgi- during which a pacemaker-like device in the
cal treatment may still be possible. In this case, chest wall is attached to an electrode wind-
a localizing neuropsychological profi le may be ing around the vagus nerve. Th is device helps
even more helpful. Combined with a positron reduce seizure frequency in up to 30%–40% of
emission tomography (PET) scan to identify those who have it implanted; the rate of com-
possible areas of hypometabolism, a single pho- plete seizure freedom after implantation is less
ton emission computed tomography (SPECT) than 1%, and this procedure is usually per-
scan to localize the ictal onset zone as precisely formed when other surgical options are lim-
as possible (see Figure 12–1; see also the color ited. An additional benefit of VNS treatment is
version in the color insert section), evidence of that the patient and family are provided with a
focal language, visuospatial, or memory dys- magnetic device that can activate higher stim-
function can be very valuable. The Wada test is ulation settings and, in some cases, abort a sei-
also useful in this context, as in other lesional zure before it spreads, causes falls, and leads to
epilepsies. The likelihood of success in these status epilepticus. In patients with prolonged
cases depends on the focality of the onset zone, seizures or clusters of seizures, this feature can
the clarity of EEG and semiological findings, be very valuable.
and the absence of a functionally important cor-
tex in the area of resection. After careful evalu-
Nonepileptic Seizures
ation, the patient is usually quoted a 30%–50%
likelihood that consciousness-altering seizures Some patients who appear to have refractory
will be controlled, depending on the individual epilepsy on initial history and evaluation are
factors identified. actually suffering from a conversion disorder
Finally, another type of surgical procedure with paroxysmal events of interrupted behav-
that can be helpful in the treatment of epi- ior or motor function that will not respond to
lepsy is the insertion of a vagal nerve stim- treatment of electrical hyperirritability because
ulator (VNS). Th is is a simple procedure electrophysiological function in the brain is, in

Figure 12–1. Ictal SPECT scan of a 19-year-old patient with unusual episodes of fidgeting, mumbling, and
rubbing her legs. She has bilateral epileptiform activity, but the SPECT scan shows clear right temporal hyper-
perfusion during a seizure (radiological convention: right is on the left). Neuropsychological test results showed
that she had a relatively low IQ (full-scale IQ = 68), and had difficulty learning and remembering both verbal
and pictorial information. The findings suggested bilateral deficits without clear lateralizing significance.
274 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

fact, normal. The vast majority of seizures have experienced. The history can provide some help-
an organic basis, and mental health workers ful clues to prompt early consideration of NES.
should assume that a patient with seizures has Table 12–1 shows typical differences between
no control over their frequency, duration, or psychogenic and epileptic seizures.
form. However, some paroxysmal events that The most reliable historical features for dif-
have been presumed to be epileptic are actu- ferentiating seizures from NES in several series
ally psychological and may respond to psy- are descriptions of motionless unresponsiveness
chotherapy or psychoactive medications. In that last over 20–30 minutes. In this situation,
the vast majority of cases, patients with events the likelihood of NES is very high, but moni-
that suddenly appear to impair consciousness toring on an inpatient epilepsy unit remains
without electrographic correlates (nonepilep- the gold standard of diagnosis. Unfortunately,
tic seizures, NES) do not have any conscious authors have described an average duration of
knowledge of their behavior, and therefore the illness before accurate diagnosis of up to 7 years,
event should not be considered an example and the prognosis for remission of the events is
of malingering. These episodes almost always best for those who can be diagnosed in the first
occur in the setting of a substantial childhood year of illness, and for patients with a younger
trauma, with between 90% and 100% of these age at onset (Reuber & Elger, 2003).
patients having a history of physical or sexual Other neuropsychological features that dis-
abuse in various series. When the seizure is tinguish NES patients from those with epilepsy
considered to be nonorganic in origin, it has include sometimes a display of less effort on
sometimes been called a “pseudoseizure” or psychometric testing, a high incidence of eleva-
“hysterical” seizure, though these terms are tions in scales 1 and 3 of the MMPI (Hs and Hy;
generally discouraged because their pejora- Wilkus & Dodrill, 1989), and a variety of differ-
tive connotations may interfere with effective ent personality patterns. In one study, the per-
communication with the individual and his/ sonality types included groups of borderline,
her family. avoidant, and overly controlled personalities in
In some cases, EEGs can remain normal NES patients (Reuber et al., 2004). Treatments
when the patients with epilepsy do not lose con- with various types of dialectical and behavioral
sciousness if the focus is too deep in the brain therapies have been attempted, and group ther-
to be discerned using scalp electrodes. In some apy is also gaining popularity.
of those cases, a SPECT scan can be helpful in The diagnosis of nonepileptic seizures is also
identifying a focal area of increased metabo- critical because of significant comorbidities.
lism during the ictal event. Nonepileptic psy- In one series, 52% met the criteria for depres-
chogenic seizures are only diagnosed when sion, 39% had suicidal ideation, and 20% had
the patient clearly loses consciousness, respon- attempted suicide (Ettinger et al., 1999). These
siveness to the environment, and/or is amnestic patients have very refractory episodes of loss
for information presented during the episode. of consciousness with many series indicating
Although some individuals with nonepileptic remission rates between 40% and 50%, and
seizures also have organically determined epi- treatment remains difficult (LaFrance, Alper,
leptic seizures, the use of tight diagnostic cri- Babcock, Barry, Benbadis, Caplan, Gates,
teria has shown that the coexistence of PNES Jacobs, Kanner, Martin, Rundhaugen, Stewart &
and genuine epilepsy is not very common. Roy Vert (for the NES Treatment Workshop par-
Martin and colleagues (Martin et al., 2003; see ticipants), 2006). Up to 18% of patients with
also Benbadis et al., 2001) studied 1590 patients NES have been treated for status epilepticus
who received a definitive diagnosis after video (Dworetzky et al., 2006).
monitoring of their spells. Of these, 514 (32.3%)
were diagnosed with PNES, and only 29 (5.3%)
Social Issues (Vocational Counseling,
of them also had epilepsy. The majority of
Driving, Legal, etc.)
these patients are diagnosed only after video-
EEG monitoring allows their physicians to be Particularly important for the mental health
certain of the nature of each type of episode clinician is the increased risk for serious
The Neuropsychology of Epilepsy 275

Table 12–1. Distinguishing Characteristics of Epileptic and Nonepileptic Seizuresa


Epileptic seizure Nonepileptic seizure
Duration of GTC is 90 seconds, partial seizures NES can last between seconds and hours.
up to a few minutes Events that last longer than 10 minutes are suspicious
for NES
Nearly always stereotyped description of seizure Some have a more diverse range of ictal phenomena, but
onset may also have stereotyped spells
Almost never includes long periods of motionless Up to 30%–40% of events consist of long periods of
unresponsiveness motionless unresponsiveness
Eyes often open Eyes often closed
Incontinence and tongue biting in 30% of seizures Incontinence and tongue biting more rare, but may
occur
Ictal cries often occur after seizure is well If an ictal cry occurs, it is often the first manifestation
established of the event
Often rising epigastric sensations in aura Very rare rising epigastric sensation
Status epilepticus not frequent Often very long seizures (up to hours)
Bilateral synchronous tonic and clonic jerks, with Asynchronous movements of the extremities more
rare bicycling movements in frontal epilepsy common
Head and body slow version of 90 degrees or more More than 90 degree turning of body is very rare, head
are common shaking (“no”) may be seen
Fine amplitude tremor is rare Tremor is fairly common
Directed violence does not occur except in the Bystanders may be hit or kicked
postictal state
With bilateral tonic-clonic seizure, no speech Massive jerks together with speech
After tonic-clonic seizures, minutes of coma Consciousness after massive jerks
Seldom crying afterwards Often crying afterwards
Usually some benefit from anticonvulsants No or inconsistent reaction to AEDs
Sometimes occurs during sleep Rarely occurs during sleep, most often occurs in public
places and MD offices
Paucity of other medical problems History of many other drugs taken, and often many
other illnesses
a
The presence of characteristics in the nonepileptic column cannot be taken as sufficient evidence that the seizures have
a psychogenic origin. However, a preponderance of characteristics in this column would certainly raise the question that
spells are nonorganic in origin.

depression (around 5% across all kinds of epi- of the accumulative effects of multiple injuries
lepsy; 3–4 times the national average). Among sustained during seizures.
persons with epilepsy, suicide is listed as cause Still important but not directly life threaten-
of death in 7%–22% of deaths. Studies since the ing, childhood epilepsy also negatively affects
turn of the century have consistently shown educational achievement and, regardless of the
that the life expectancy of persons with epilepsy age of onset, impacts on the person’s eventual
is shorter than in the rest of the population; the employment level. About a third of children with
death rate of children with epilepsy 0–5 years of epilepsy receive special educational support,
age is 1.3 times greater than that of the general and IQ increases linearly as a function of age of
population; from 5 to 24 years of age, the rate onset (from 83 for adults whose seizures began
is 6.6 times greater; from 25 to 45 years of age, in infancy to 102 for those with adult onset).
the rate is 3.7 times greater. During the period Equally serious, and probably not unrelated to
when persons with epilepsy were typically insti- the person’s educational experiences, one’s abil-
tutionalized, as many as 50% of them died of ity to find employment is greatly reduced by
causes either directly or indirectly associated epilepsy. In a 1973 survey, almost half of per-
with their seizures. Among those whose deaths sons with epilepsy reported that they had been
were directly attributed to seizures, around turned down for a job because of their epilepsy,
12% died of status epilepticus (a continuous and 30% reported that they had lost at least one
and uncontrollable seizure); many others died job because of seizures. Various studies over
276 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

this century have shown the unemployment cushion or folded piece of clothing beneath the
rate among working-age persons with epilepsy person’s head can reduce the chance of a head
is between 2 and 7 times the rate of unemploy- injury, and turning the head to one side so the
ment in the general population. In general, saliva can escape is a good idea. It is useless to
an employer has the right to ask a prospective try to interrupt the seizure; when the seizure
employee if he/she has any medical condition is over, the person may need to rest or sleep.
that will interfere with successful carrying Seizures usually stop within several minutes,
out of the duties, so reduction in employment but if the seizure continues for 10 minutes or
opportunities may not be entirely the product more, or if seizures follow in succession with-
of a prejudice against epilepsy itself. Loss of the out a period of complete recovery, then medical
ability to drive a car is mentioned by most per- attention should be sought (this could signal the
sons with epilepsy as a major loss (the seizure- beginning of status epilepticus). Focal (simple
free period before driving again differs from partial) seizures do not require any action on
place to place; for example, in Michigan it is the part of observers. A person having a com-
6 months while in Ontario, Canada, it is 1 year). plex partial seizure should not be restrained
Pregnancy and genetic aspects: There have unless the person is placing himself/herself in
been reported cases of birth defects for women danger. As in the case of a generalized seizure,
who use AEDs. In the overall population, the hot or sharp objects should be removed. Partial
rate of birth defects is 2%–3%, and the rate is seizures sometimes progress to a generalized
slightly higher (0.5%) for women with epilepsy seizure (“secondarily generalized”), so further
who are not taking medication. Women taking precautions may be necessary.
a single AED have a risk of about 6%–7%, with
some medications being more problematic than
Pharmacological Treatment
others. Taking several AEDs increases the risk
even more. Unfortunately, seizure frequency A wide variety of medications have proven
may go up during pregnancy, so the need for useful in the treatment of epilepsy. The arma-
AEDs may even increase. In some cases, the per- mentarium of available agents has expanded
son’s physician may feel that the risks of preg- steadily, and each AED medication has a spe-
nancy are too great for the mother and child, cific set of indications and possible pitfalls.
and recommend that pregnancy be avoided. First, the type of epilepsy must be carefully
Finally, some AEDs reduce the effectiveness of characterized in order to determine the most
oral birth control pills. As mentioned below, effective medication. Primary generalized epi-
the genetics of epilepsy suggest that children lepsies, usually generic syndromes beginning
may inherit a predisposition to epilepsy, but not in childhood or adolescence, typically respond
epilepsy itself. This means that, for example, a much more completely to valproic acid than to
head injury would more likely lead to a seizure other agents. Levetiracetam, topiramate, and
disorder if the person has close relatives with zonisamide have also been documented to have
epilepsy. some efficacy for these patients. On the other
Persons with epilepsy frequently suffer hand, several of the medications designed
because people do not know what to do when for partial epilepsies can worsen the fre-
they see a seizure occur. It was once believed quency and duration of generalized seizures—
that a soft object should be inserted between these include carbamazepine, tiagabine, and
the teeth to prevent biting the tongue during oxcarbamazepine.
a seizure. This is now highly discouraged. The Carbamazepine is the most widely prescribed
proper response, if any, depends on the kind of medication for partial epilepsies in developed
seizure. In general, only a person having a GTC countries, but several other medications have
needs attention, and in this case the greatest documented substantial efficacy as add-on or
help consists of remaining calm, helping the adjunctive therapy, including lamotrigine, topi-
person gently to the floor, and loosening any ramate, and levetiracetam. Several of the newer
tight clothing. Hot or sharp objects that could agents developed for partial epilepsy have been
cause harm should be moved away. Placing a found to have relatively weak antiepileptic
The Neuropsychology of Epilepsy 277

properties and are currently even more widely each precipitate psychosis in a small percentage
prescribed for treatment of pain—these include of patients. Topiramate and Zonisamide can
gabapentin and pregabalin (Lyrica). cause weight loss, while weight gain is a fairly
AEDs are often selected because of their common side effect of valproic acid, especially
associated side effects and how those relate at higher doses.
to the wishes and predispositions of patients. The ability to titrate a medication rapidly can
Lamotrigine seems to have very little in the way be a significant advantage in certain circum-
of cognitive effects—in that sense it is perhaps stances—in new frequent or prolonged seizures,
the best tolerated medication, but it can lead to phenytoin and levetiracetam can each be rap-
a life-threatening rash if titrated up too rapidly. idly titrated to therapeutic doses, whereas with
Topiramate now has well-documented negative many of the other newer AEDs, gradual intro-
effects on verbal memory in some patients, and duction is advisable.
most AEDs can cause sedation and attention Please see Table 12–2 for usual indications,
problems. Levetiracetam and topiramate can doses, and side effects.

Table 12–2. Comparison of the Important Characteristics of New Antiepileptic Agents


Medication and Effective in Effective in Usual side effects Special cautions
usual dose range partial epilepsy generalized
epilepsy
Phenytoin ++ ++ Sedation, hirsuitism, Supplement vitamin
(Dilantin) gingival hyperplasia D to prevent
300–500 mg/d osteoporosis, check
LFTs
Carbamazepine ++ + (Avoid in Blurred vision, Check CBC and
(Tegretol) absence) imbalance, fatigue SGOT/PT regularly
600–1600 mg/d
Phenobarbital + ++ Sedation, dizziness Strong P 450 inducer
(Luminal)
90–150 mg/d
Valproic Acid + +++ Weight gain, tremor, May cause
(Depakote) sedation thrombocytopenia
1500–3000 or liver problems,
mg/d regular blood work
needed
Lamotrigine ++ +? Headache, fatigue Stevens Johnson
(Lamictal) rash if started too
300–500 mg/d rapidly
Levetiracetam ++ ++ Anxiety, depression, Occasionally causes
(Keppra) edema, tremor psychosis
1500–3000 mg/d
Topiramate ++ ++ Selective verbal Occasionally causes
(Topamax) memory psychosis
200–400 mg/d impairment, renal
stones
Gabapentin + Edema, tremor, Used also in pain
(Neurontin) sedation syndromes, very
1200–3600 mg/d safe
Zonisamide + ++ (Useful in Kidney stones, weight Rash can be severe
(Zonegran) absence) loss
200–400 mg/d
Oxcarbamazepine + (May worsen Sedation, dizziness, May cause
(Trileptal) absence) diplopia hyponatremia
1200–2400 mg/d
Tiagabin + (May worsen Sedation, dizziness Spike wave stupor
(Gabatril) absence) reported in some
8–32 mg/d gen epi pts
278 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Appendix 1 • With hyperkinetic automatisms


• With focal negative myoclonus
International League against Epilepsy: http:// • With inhibitory motor seizures
www.ilae-epilepsy.org • Gelastic seizures
Epilepsy Foundation: • Hemiclonic seizures
http://www.epilepsyfoundation.org/about/ • Secondarily generalized seizures
American Epilepsy Society: http://www.aesnet.org/ • Reflex seizures in focal epilepsy syndromes
Support Groups (one among many): http:// Continuous seizure types
www.geocities.com/epilepsy911/ Generalized status epilepticus
• Generalized tonic-clonic status epilepticus
Appendix 2 • Clonic status epilepticus
• Absence status epilepticus
Seizure types (from http://www.ilae-epilepsy.
• Tonic status epilepticus
org)
• Myoclonic status epilepticus
EPILEPTIC SEIZURE TYPES
Focal status epilepticus
Self-limited seizure types
• Epilepsia partialis continua of Kojevnikov
Generalized seizures
• Aura continua
• Tonic-clonic seizures (includes variations • Limbic status epilepticus (psychomotor status)
beginning with a clonic or myoclonic phase) • Hemiconvulsive status with hemiparesis
• Clonic seizures
• Without tonic features References
• With tonic features
Benbadis, S.R., Agrawal, V. & Tatum, W.O. (2001).
• Typical absence seizures
How many patients with psychogenic non-
• Atypical absence seizures epileptic seizures also have epilepsy? Neurology,
• Myoclonic absence seizures 57, 915–917.
• Tonic seizures Berkovic, S. F., Howell, R. A., Hay, D. A., &
• Spasms Hopper, J. L. (1998). Epilepsies in twins: Genetics
• Myoclonic seizures of the major epilepsy syndromes. Annals of
• Massive bilateral myoclonus Neurology, 43, 435–445.
• Eyelid myoclonia Buchtel, H. A., Passaro, E., Selwa, L. M., Deveikis,
• Without absences J., & Gomez-Hassan, D. (2002). Sodium metho-
• With absences hexital (Brevital) as anesthetic in the Wada Test.
• Myoclonic atonic seizures Epilepsia, 43, 1056–1061.
Dichter, M. A. (1998). In J. Engel and T. A. Pedley
• Negative myoclonus
(Eds.), Epilepsy: A comprehensive textbook
• Atonic seizures (Chapters 21–25). Philadelphia : Lippincott-Raven.
• Reflex seizures in generalized epilepsy Dworetzky, B. A., Mortati, K. A., Rossetti, A. O.,
syndromes Vaccaro, B., Nelson, A., & Edward B. Bromfield E. B.
• Seizures of the posterior neocortex (2006). Clinical characteristics of psychogenic
• Neocortical temporal lobe seizures nonepileptic seizure status in the long-term moni-
• Focal seizures toring unit. Epilepsy and Behavior, 9, 335–338.
• Focal sensory seizures Engel, J. (2001). A proposed diagnostic scheme for
• With elementary sensory symptoms (e.g., people with epileptic seizures and epilepsy: Report
occipital and parietal lobe seizures) of the ILAE task force on classification and termi-
• With experiential sensory symptoms (e.g., nology. Epilepsia, 42, 796–803.
Ettinger, A., Devinsky, O., Weisbrot, D. M.,
temporoparieto-occipital junction seizures)
Ramakrishna, R. K., & Goyal, A. (1999). A compre-
• Focal motor seizures hensive profi le of clinical psychiatric and psycho-
• With elementary clonic motor signs social characteristics of patients with psychogenic
• With asymmetrical tonic motor seizures nonepileptic seizures. Epilepsia, 40, 1292–1298.
(e.g., supplementary motor seizures) Fisher, R. S., van Emde Boas, W., Blume, W., Elger, C.,
• With typical (temporal lobe) automatisms Genton, P., Lee, P., et al.. (2005). Epileptic sei-
(e.g., mesial temporal lobe seizures) zures and epilepsy: Defi nitions proposed by the
The Neuropsychology of Epilepsy 279

International League Against Epilepsy (ILAE) Martin, R., Burneo, J. G., Prasad, A., Powell, T., Faught,
and the International Bureau for Epilepsy (IBE). E., Knowlton, R., Mendez, M. & Kuzniecky, R.
Epilepsia, 46, 470–472. (2003). Frequency of epilepsy in patients with
Helmstaedter, C., Kurthen, M., Lux, S., Reuber, M., & psychogenic seizures monitored by video-EEG.
Elger, C. E. (2003). Chronic epilepsy and cogni- Neurology, 61, 1791–1792.
tion: A longitudinal study in temporal lobe epi- Reuber, M., Pukrop, R., Bauer, J., Derfuss, R. &
lepsy. Annals of Neurology, 54, 425–432. Elger, C.E. (2004). Multidimensional analy-
Hermann, B. P., Seidenberg, M., Dow, C., Jones, J., sis of personality in pateints with NES. Journal
Rutecki, P., Bhattacharya, A., et al. (2006). Cogni- of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 75,
tive prognosis in chronic temporal lobe epilepsy. 743–748.
Annals of Neurology, 60, 80–87. Reuber, M., & Elger, C. E. (2003). Psychogenic non-
Kjeldsen, M. J., Corey, L. A., Christensen, K., & Friis, epileptic seizures: Review and update. Epilepsy
M. L. (2003). Epileptic seizures and syndromes in and Behavior, 4, 205–216.
twins: the importance of genetic factors. Epilepsy Temkin, O. (1971). The falling sickness: A history of
Research, 55, 137–146. epilepsy from the Greeks to the beginnings of mod-
LaFrance, W.C., Alper, K., Babcock, D., Barry, J.J., ern neurology, 2nd ed. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins
Benbadis, S., Caplan, R., Gates, J., Jacobs, M., Kanner, University Press.
A., Martin, R., Rundhaugen, L., Stewart, R., & Vert , Wilkus, R. J., & Dodrill, C. B. (1989). Factors affect-
C., (for the NES Treatment Workshop participants) ing the outcome of MMPI and neuropsychological
(2006). Nonepileptic seizures treatment workshop assessments of psychogenic and epileptic seizure
summary. Epilepsy and Behavior, 8, 451–461. patients. Epilepsia, 30, 339–347.
13

The Neuropsychology of Multiple Sclerosis


Allen E. Thornton and Vanessa G. DeFreitas

Multiple sclerosis (MS) is one of the most the myelin covering neuronal axons and the oli-
common neurological disorders of the cen- godendrocytes that produce myelin. The result
tral nervous system (CNS), affecting approx- is widespread demyelination of axons and the
imately 250,000–400,000 individuals in the formation of plaques by astrocytes, which slows
United States and 2.5 million people worldwide or blocks the conduction of nerve impulses.
(Anderson et al., 1992; www.nationalmsso- Although MS primarily affects the myelin sheath
ciety.org). Although its etiology has not been that covers axons, there does appear to be some
clearly established, evidence indicates that evidence of axonal loss (Keegan & Noseworthy,
MS involves autoimmune inflammatory pro- 2002). Symptom attenuation appears to result
cesses and results in widespread demyelin- from the clearing of edema, partial remyelina-
ation of axons and the formation of sclerotic tion of axons, and a redistribution of sodium
plaques (Keegan & Noseworthy, 2002). Unlike channels along demyelinated axon segments
many other degenerative diseases, MS typically (Noseworthy et al., 2000). Following increased
begins during early adulthood, with a mean age illness progression, however, damage to some
of onset approximating 30 years (Vukusic & axons may be irreversible, and the production
Confavreux, 2001). Given its early onset and of myelin by oligodendrocytes may become
accruement of CNS injury throughout a nearly exhausted (Noseworthy et al., 2000).
normal life span (Sadovnick et al., 1992), it is Although MS lesions can be found in any
not surprising that MS can significantly alter myelinated area of the CNS, the distribution of
one’s quality of life (Murray, 2005). plaques is not altogether random. Regions pref-
erentially damaged in MS include the white mat-
ter surrounding the ventricles, corpus callosum,
Neuropathology
optic nerves, brainstem, cerebellum, and spi-
While the precise mechanism(s) triggering the nal cord (Ge, 2006; Lucchinetti & Parisi, 2006;
autoimmune response in MS remains unclear, Noseworthy et al., 2000). MS lesions predomi-
T lymphocytes appear to be crucial in the nantly affect white matter, but lesions also appear
development of MS lesions (Compston & Coles, in the gray–white matter junctions and in the
2002; Keegan & Noseworthy, 2002; Noseworthy gray matter itself (Kidd et al., 1999). Preferential
et al., 2000; Rinker II et al., 2006). Specifically, damage includes cortical sulci and the cingu-
these cells cross the blood–brain barrier from late, temporal, insular, and cerebellar cortices
the circulatory system and penetrate the CNS. (Lucchinetti & Parisi, 2006). As illustrated in
The T cells encounter the proteins of the myelin Figure 13–1 (see also the color figure in the color
and secrete cytokines that in turn recruit mac- insert section), periventricular and frontoparietal
rophages and microglia that attack and damage lesions are very common in MS and these lesions

280
The Neuropsychology of Multiple Sclerosis 281

Periventricular Frontal lesions


lesions Enhancing
lesion

Parietal lesions

Figure 13–1. Axial MRI scans of a 42-year-old female with relapsing—remitting MS and an Expanded
Disability Status Scale of 3.5. The patient’s disease duration is 11 years and she is not being managed with any
disease modifying therapy. From left to right: T2-weighted, FLAIR and T1-weighted post-gadolinium contrast
images. Periventricular hyperintense lesions and frontoparietal lesions are noted on the T2 and FLAIR images;
a gadolinium-enhancing lesion is also apparent. (Images are courtesy of Cornelia Laule, PhD, Department of
Radiology, University of British Columbia MRI Research Centre).

are associated with neurocognitive impairments the afternoon and following exercise (Ford et al.,
(e.g., Sperling et al., 2001). Brain atrophy also 1998; Noseworthy et al., 2000). Cortical signs
occurs in MS and begins early. It occurs at a rate such as aphasia, apraxia, and recurrent seizures
of 0.6%–1.0% annually, and manifests as enlarged are only rarely experienced (Noseworthy et al.,
ventricles and decreased brain volume (Bermel 2000).
& Bakshi, 2006; Ge, 2006). Wallerian degenera-
tion, or the degeneration of axons that become
detached from their cell bodies, may contribute Diagnosis
to tissue loss (Bermel & Bakshi, 2006).
In general, the diagnosis of MS is based on a detailed
clinical history, findings of clinical abnormalities
Disease Manifestation, Course,
observed on neurological examination (described
and Diagnosis
above), and objective evidence of pathology
obtained from magnetic resonance imaging (MRI;
Symptoms
see Figure 13–1), cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) analy-
MS involves a spectrum of neurological symp- sis, or visual evoked potentials (VEP) (Keegan &
toms that reflect the presence and distribution of Noseworthy, 2002; McDonald et al., 2001; Miller,
damage in the CNS and vary considerably across 2006). The addition of these radiological and labo-
individuals (Compston & Coles, 2002; Feinstein, ratory investigations is invaluable in the detection
1999). Common initial symptoms of MS include of MS neuropathology. MRI, in particular, is
vertigo, numbness or tingling in the limbs, gait helpful in identifying the presence of CNS lesions
or balance disturbance, limb weakness, loss of (Ge, 2006), and abnormalities in the CSF analysis
vision in one or both eyes, and diplopia (i.e., dou- (e.g., an elevated immunoglobin G [IgG] index,
ble vision) (Beatty, 1996; Joy & Johnston, 2001; mildly elevated CSF white blood cell count, or
Vukusic & Confavreux, 2001). Other symptoms the identification of unique oligoclonal IgG
such as spasticity, Lhermitte’s sign (i.e., tin- bands) support the presence of an inflammatory
gling that radiates down the arms, back, or neck process (Joy & Johnston, 2001). Furthermore,
evoked by neck flexion), facial numbness, dysar- findings of delayed but well-preserved VEP
thria, pain, sexual dysfunction, and loss of bowel waveforms are consistent with the slowed nerve
or bladder control are also commonly observed impulses seen in demyelinated axons (Joy &
(Miller, 2006; Mohr et al., 2004). Moreover, both Johnston, 2001; McDonald et al., 2001).
mental and physical fatigue occurs in the major- Until recently, the diagnosis of MS was based
ity of persons with MS, and is generally worse in on the Poser classification system (Poser et al.,
282 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

1983). Specifically, the Poser criteria divided MS demonstrating high sensitivity (83%) and spec-
patients into those with definite MS and those ificity (83%) (Dalton et al., 2002).
with probable MS, and further subdivided these
categories into clinical and laboratory supported.
Course
Criteria for clinically definite MS included either
(1) two attacks and clinical evidence of two sep- A striking feature of MS is that symptom mani-
arate lesions, or (2) two attacks, clinical evidence festations vary markedly across persons with
of one lesion, and more objective evidence of a MS and fluctuate over time within an individ-
second separate lesion. A diagnosis of laboratory- ual. Accordingly, widely accepted subtypes of
supported definite MS was given in individuals MS have been established on the basis of clinical
with (1) two attacks, either clinical or objective course (Feinstein, 1999; Joy & Johnston, 2001;
evidence of one lesion, and CSF IgG or oligoclo- Lublin & Reingold, 1996; Miller, 2006). More
nal bands, (2) one attack, clinical evidence of two specifically, approximately 80%–85% of persons
separate lesions, and IgG or oligoclonal bands, with MS initially suffer from relapsing-remitting
or (3) one attack, clinical evidence of one lesion, MS (RRMS), which is characterized by acute epi-
objective evidence of a second, separate lesion, sodes of neurological deterioration interspersed
and CSF IgG or oligoclonal bands. Clinically with full or partial recovery between relapses,
probable MS was defined as having (1) two attacks and a lack of disease progression during periods
and clinical evidence of one lesion, (2) one attack of recovery. Of these individuals, approximately
and clinical evidence of two separate lesions, or 10%–20% experience benign MS, with only a few
(3) one attack, clinical evidence of one lesion, and relapses and minimal impairment or disability
objective evidence of a second, separate lesion. 15 years after onset. However, at least 50% of
Finally, a diagnosis of laboratory-supported prob- individuals with an initial relapsing-remitting
able MS was given if the patient had two attacks course will eventually develop secondary-pro-
and evidence of IgG or oligoclonal bands. gressive MS (SPMS), which is characterized by
In 2001, the International Panel on the steady worsening of symptoms over time with or
Diagnosis of MS further refined the recom- without occasional relapses, minor remissions,
mended diagnostic criteria for MS (McDonald or plateaus. In contrast, 10%–15% of individu-
et al., 2001). According to the McDonald criteria, als suffer from primary-progressive MS (PPMS),
a diagnosis of MS is typically given if there have which is marked by chronic disease progres-
been two or more distinct attacks consistent with sion that begins at onset with only occasional
MS, as well as objective evidence of at least two and transient plateaus or minor improvements.
CNS lesions (i.e., in the cerebral white matter, cer- Although rare, 5% of individuals with initial
ebellum, optic nerves, brain stem, or spinal cord) PPMS will go on to develop progressive-relapsing
that are separated in time and space. However, MS, which is characterized by a progressive dis-
a diagnosis of MS can also be given when there ease onset followed by distinct acute exacerba-
has been only one attack as long as it is accom- tions and continual disease progression between
panied by objective evidence of at least two CNS relapses. Also rare is malignant MS, whereby the
lesions that are disseminate in time and space. disease progresses rapidly, leading to significant
Moreover, the physician must determine that disability and even death in a relatively short
there is “no better explanation” for the observed period of time. It is important to note that, until
abnormalities before giving a diagnosis of MS. recently, PPMS and SPMS were classified under
Failure to meet the diagnosis of MS results in the the same category (i.e., chronic progressive MS;
classification of either not MS or possible MS (i.e., CPMS) (Montalban & Rio, 2006), and therefore
when a patient clinically presents as having MS much of the research in this area has employed
but has not yet been diagnostically evaluated, the CPMS course.
or if the evidence of the diagnostic examination
is inconclusive). The Panel also recommends
Prognosis
against the use of the terms clinically definite MS
and probable MS. The utility of the McDonald Although the prognosis of MS is quite variable,
diagnostic criteria has since been established, there is evidence that numerous demographic
The Neuropsychology of Multiple Sclerosis 283

and clinical factors predict overall out- risk of their birthplace only if they relocate
come (Compston & Coles, 2002; Kantarci & when at least 15 years of age (Kurtzke, 2000).
Weinshenker, 2001; Keegan & Noseworthy, Viral exposure may increase susceptibility to
2002; Naismith et al., 2006; Noseworthy et al., MS, as persons who develop MS contract more
2000; Tremlett & Devonshire, 2006). Indicators childhood infections after age 6 than healthy
of relatively good prognosis include being individuals and show increased levels of anti-
female, winter birth, younger onset age, pri- bodies of several viruses in the CSF (Marrie,
marily sensory and visual disturbances, com- 2004). Although conclusive evidence regard-
plete recovery from relapses, few relapses, and ing the association between specific viruses and
longer intervals between relapses. On the other susceptibility to MS is generally limited, past
hand, being male or African American, having research has shown that infectious mononucle-
primarily motor and cerebellar disturbances, osis appears to be a risk factor (Thacker et al.,
incomplete recovery from relapses, frequent 2006), Chlamydia pneumoniae is more common
relapses during the initial years, a progressive in persons who develop MS (Bagos et al., 2006),
course at onset, and short intervals between and there is an increased risk of relapse follow-
relapses predict poorer prognosis. Moreover, ing infection (Rutschmann et al., 2002).
relapses have also been associated with increas- Influenza immunization does not appear to
ing number of stressful life events and limited increase the risk of relapse (Rutschmann et al.,
social support (Brown et al., 2006; Mohr et al., 2002). Other potential risk factors include expo-
2004). Although life expectancy in MS is only sure to organic solvents and reduced vitamin
slightly reduced as a result of disease-related D and sun exposure (Kantarci & Wingerchuk,
factors, there is an increased rate of suicide in 2006).
afflicted individuals (Sadovnick et al., 1992).
Neurocognition in Multiple
Etiology and Risk Factors Sclerosis
While the precise etiology remains unknown, Neurocognitive dysfunction in MS is common,
it is generally accepted that MS results from an with estimates of impairment approximat-
interaction between genetic predisposition and ing 40%–50% in community-based samples
environmental factors. Familial studies demon- (Brassington & Marsh, 1998; Rao et al., 1991a).
strate concordance rates for monozygotic twins These deficits are meaningful and are one of a
that approximate 30%–35%, while those for dizy- few disease manifestations predictive of voca-
gotic twins and non-twin siblings are between tional status (Benedict et al., 2005a, 2006b; Rao
3% and 5% (Hillert & Masterman, 2001; Pryse- et al., 1991b). Furthermore, MS-related deficits
Phillips & Sloka, 2006; Sadovnick et al., 1993). have a negative influence on several compo-
Similarly, as genetic relatedness becomes more nents of daily functioning, including social and
distant (e.g., parents, second-degree relatives, avocational activities (Higginson et al., 2000;
adopted siblings), the risk of developing MS is Rao et al., 1991b).
further reduced (Compston, 1999; Ebers et al., The heterogeneous neurocognitive manifes-
1995). Genetic linkage studies have also shown tations of MS precludes there being a typical
that several genes can predispose one to devel- pattern of impairment. Indeed, the variability
oping MS (GAMES and Transatlantic Multiple in deficits expression is striking even in persons
Sclerosis Genetics Cooperative, 2003). with RRMS and minimal disability (Ryan et al.,
The risk of developing MS is two to three 1996; also see Hannay et al., 2004). For instance,
times greater for females than males, and is based upon the fift h percentile impairment cut-
more prevalent in Caucasians (Joy & Johnston, off, 35% of a sample of 177 persons with RRMS
2001; Pryse-Phillips & Sloka, 2006). Prevalence showed no impairments. However, 33% of the
rates of MS increase as geographic latitude sample exhibited impairment on three or more
becomes more extreme at the northern and tests, with 5% of individuals appearing rel-
southern hemispheres, and those who migrate atively globally impaired (Ryan et al., 1996).
from a high to low risk region maintain the Often cognitive impairments emerge early in
284 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

the disease (Schulz et al., 2006; Zivadinov et al., that the effect estimates are presented so that
2001) and longitudinal work suggests that per- larger values represent greater impairment in
sons with MS may frequently experience both a persons with MS. The estimates are also comple-
broadening of their initial impairments and the mented with results from relevant single-sample
development of new impairments over longer studies. Furthermore, when available (Nocentini
durations (Amato et al., 2001; Bergendal et al., et al., 2006; Rao et al., 1991a), observations of
2007). Indeed, the prevalence rates of signifi- the frequency of persons with MS exhibiting
cant neurocognitive impairment show remark- impairment below the fifth percentile of healthy
able increases, even early in the disease (Amato individuals are noted for a given neurocognitive
et al., 2001; Zivadinov et al., 2001). Recent work domain. As illustrated in Table 13–2, the fre-
suggests that information-processing speed quency of these significantly impaired individu-
may be particularly vulnerable to this decline als is useful to consider given the heterogeneity
over time, especially in persons with SPMS of the manifestations of the disorder.
(Bergendal et al., 2007).
Several comprehensive quantitative reviews
Intelligence
have elucidated the “average” neurocogni-
tive deficit severities across various domains. Intelligence as indexed by contemporary IQ
Although in both single-sample studies and measures is composed of a multitude of disso-
quantitative reviews it might seem counterintu- ciable abilities. The most frequently employed
itive to consider the aggregated neurocognitive IQ measures are derived from the Wechsler
impairments in a heterogeneous disorder such as Adult Intelligence Scales (WAIS; Kaufman &
MS, quantitative reviews are valuable in that they Lichtenberger, 2006). On these scales, per-
yield information regarding the breadth of dys- sons suffering from MS exhibit greater defi-
function that the disease entails, as well as iden- cits in Performance Intelligence (PIQ) than
tifying lesser or less frequent impairments that Verbal Intelligence (VIQ). As illustrated in
may go unrecognized in individuals. Relative to Table 13–1, quantitative summaries of the lit-
single-sample studies, quantitative reviews cap- erature reveal that PIQ is robustly impacted
ture a broader spectrum of the disorder in terms (d = 0.77), approaching the conventional defi-
of clinical factors, research settings, and method- nition of large (Cohen, 1988). There is perhaps
ologies. Consequently, a more complete picture some attenuation of the effect in the Prakash
of the impairment MS entails is elucidated. et al. (2008) study (d = 0.60), which is com-
The effect size statistic most often used in prised of only RRMS samples. VIQ is more
meta-analyses is essentially an aggregate mea- moderately affected. Nonetheless, Table 13–2
sure of Cohen’s d and represents the magnitude indicates that 10% of a large sample of persons
of the differences in neurocognitive performance with RRMS suffering minimal neurological
between two groups of participants. This statistic disability (Expanded Disability Status Scale;
indexes the strength of impairment in MS relative EDSS) were significantly impaired in fluid
to a control group, typically composed of healthy intelligence (Raven Progressive Matrices;
individuals. The extent of distributional overlap RPM), whereas, approximately 20% of indi-
between persons with MS and controls is also viduals with a variety of courses and relatively
signified by d (Cohen, 1988). For instance, what severe disability exhibit fairly broad intellec-
conventionally is categorized as a large-effect size tual impairments.
of d = 0.80, corresponds to a 53% overlap (47%
nonoverlap) in the distributions of two groups on
Verbal Abilities
a given performance measure (Cohen, 1988).
Regardless of variation in methodology, the Modest-to-moderate losses are often observed
extant meta-analytic studies (Henry & Beatty, on select and narrower verbal ability measures,
2006; Prakash et al., 2008; Thornton & Raz, 1997; which are consistent with the aforementioned
Wishart & Sharpe, 1997; Zakzanis, 2000) pro- overall VIQ effect. Table 13–1 illustrates that on
duce fairly consistent neurocognitive results that these select subtests, estimates are always less than
are selectively summarized in Table 13–1. Note 0.50. In relatively severely disabled community
The Neuropsychology of Multiple Sclerosis 285

participants the frequency of MS-related impair- Digits Modalities Test (SDMT). Processing-
ments across WAIS Verbal subscales range from speed reductions are associated with an effect
8% to 19% (see Table 13–2). size of approximately one standard deviation
In terms of neurocognitive measures of lan- on these tests (only 45% joint MS-control dis-
guage functioning, moderate effects size esti- tributional overlap). In a less impaired sample
mates are apparent in confrontation naming of RRMS, 43% of persons exhibited significant
(Table 13–1). In contrast, marked reductions impairments on the oral version of the SDMT
in verbal fluency are evident with effect mag- (see Table 13–2). Interestingly, impairments on
nitudes occasionally approximating 1.00. Table the Trail-Making Tests (TMT) appear relatively
13–2 illustrates that in these select samples attenuated compared with other processing-
approximately 20% of persons with MS exhibit speed measures (see Table 13–1). The TMT, the
significant fluency deficits relative to controls. Digit Symbol subtest, and the SDMT tax visual
Often these deficits are conceptualized as exec- search and visuomotor speed significantly; how-
utive in nature. Meta-analytic findings suggest ever, the latter two tests place greater demands
that fluency deficits may be greater in CPMS on memory (Laux & Lane, 1985; Lezak et al.,
and when disability is greater (Henry & Beatty, 2004; Strauss et al., 2006), perhaps contributing
2006; Zakzanis, 2000). Although longer dura- to the differential impairment.
tion of illness, increased age, and greater dis- Effect estimates capturing the loss of pro-
ability level mitigated the association between cessing efficiency with measures of reaction
CPMS and reduced fluency (Henry & Beatty, time are moderate to large (d = 0.65) even in
2006), these confounded illness features are RRMS samples (see Prakash et al. 2008: Table
fundamental to CPMS. 13–1). In single-sample studies, deficits in
information-processing speed have also been
reported on various laboratory tasks, includ-
Visuospatial and Constructional Skills
ing reaction time, response inhibition, visual
Overall, basic visuoperceptual and construc- search, psychomotor speed and speeded arith-
tional abilities are moderately affected by MS metic (Archibald & Fisk, 2000; Denney et al.,
despite the fact that these skills, like many 2005; Kail, 1998; Kujala et al., 1994; Rao et al.,
measures, are partially reliant on primary sen- 1989c). For instance, impairment in divided,
sory and/or motor functions (see Table 13–1). focused, and sustained attention has been noted
In a community sample of persons with MS, in a hospital catchment sample of persons with
12%–19% of individuals showed significant MS who experienced fairly severe disability
impairments on various tasks of visual discrim- (De Sonneville et al., 2002). Executive aspects
ination, visuospatial integration, and on the of attentional regulation were also impaired in
more complex task of facial recognition match- this sample and deficits increased with greater
ing (see Table 13–2). In a less disabled sample task complexity, suggesting that motor dys-
of RRMS, 14%–16% of persons exhibited sig- function itself was not solely responsible for the
nificant impairments on visual copying tasks. impairments. Overall, participants in this study
Importantly, visuoperceptual and construc- exhibited a 40% slowing in processing speed
tional tasks often contain multiple operations relative to healthy persons.
that may contribute to these deficits, including Tests of attention and psychomotor speed
motor skills, perceptual abilities, and executive often involve visual and motor components that
processes (Hannay et al., 2004). may contribute to poor task performance in MS
(Benedict et al., 2002b; Bruce et al., 2007). The
Sternberg Memory Scanning Test (Sternberg,
Information Processing
1969) produces response speed measures that
Marked impairments are evident in informa- are minimally contaminated by motor and sen-
tion-processing speed in persons with MS. sory deficits (Gontkovsky & Beatty, 2006). In
These losses are most robust on visuomotor this paradigm subjects are presented with a set
scanning tasks, including the Digit Symbol of items to hold in short-term memory (STM).
subtest from the WAIS scales and the Symbol- A probe item is then presented, and the subject
Table 13–1. Effect Size Estimates from Meta-analytic Reviews.
Wishart and Sharpe (1997) Thornton and Raz (1997) Zakzanis (2000) Henry and Beatty (2006) Prakash et al. (2008)
Cognitive Domain d k OL d k OL d k OL d k OL d k OL

Intellectual Abilities
Verbal (VIQ) 0.47 9 69% - - - 0.50 8 67% 0.37 11 74% 0.30 9 79%
Performance (PIQ) 0.77 5 54% - - - - - - - - - 0.60 6 62%
Verbal & Language Abilities
Information (WAIS) - - - - - - 0.23 5 83% - - - 0.08 13 94%
Comprehension (WAIS) - - - - - - 0.30 5 79% - - - - - -
Similarities (WAIS) 0.32 7 78% - - - 0.43 7 71% - - - 0.28 7 80%
Vocabulary (WAIS) - - - - - - 0.40 7 73% - - - - - -
Semantic Fluency - - - - - - 0.99 7 45% 0.93 16 47% - - -
Phonemic Fluency 0.68 19 58% - - - 0.78 18 54% 0.93 16 47% - - -
Verbal Fluency (omnibus) - - - - - - - - - - - - 0.69 26 57%
Boston Naming Test 0.39 7 73% - - - 0.54 8 65% 0.45 11 70% 0.34 20 76%
Visuospatial & Construction
Skills
Block Design - - - - - - 0.50 6 67% - - - - - -
Visuoperceptual 0.43 11 71% - - - - - - - - - 0.55 11 64%
Visuoconstructional 0.52 19 66% - - - - - - - - - 0.54 22 65%
Attention & Working
Memory
Digit/Spatial Span - - - 0.35 22 76% - - - - - - 0.43 27 71%
Digit Span (total) 0.45 19 70% - - - - - - - - - - - -
Digit Span Forward 0.63 6 60% - - - 0.37 15 74% - - - - - -
Digit Span Backward - - - - - - 0.42 13 72% - - - - - -
Arithmetic (WAIS) - - - - - - 0.33 6 77% - - - - - -
Verbal Working Memory - - - 0.72 11 56% - - - - - - 0.52 85 66%
Trail-Making Test—Form A - - - - - - 0.30 5 79% - - - - - -
Trail-Making Test—Form B 0.65 10 59% - - - 0.41 6 72% - - - - - -
Processing Speed (SDMT and/ 0.93 9 47% - - - 1.03 8 44% 0.90 6 48% - - -
or Digit Symbol)
Processing Efficiency - - - - - - - - - - - - 0.65 13 59%
(Reaction Time)
Verbal LTM
List/Pairs Learning (multiple 0.82 19 52% - - - - - - - - - - - -
trials)
Immediate Recall (single 0.80 12 53% - - - - - - - - - - - -
trial)
Delayed Recall 0.68 16 58% - - - - - - - - - 0.78 44 54%
Free Recall - - - 0.81 32 52% - - - - - - - - -
Cued Recall - - - 0.69 13 57% - - - - - - - - -
Recognition - - - 0.54 13 65% - - - - - - 0.49 17 67%
Nonverbal LTM
List/Pairs Learning 0.93 8 47% - - - - - - - - - - - -
(multitrial)
Immediate Recall 0.65 9 59% - - - - - - - - - 0.52 39 66%
Delayed Recall 0.39 10 73% - - - - - - - - - 0.55 14 64%
Free Recall - - - 0.55 12 64% - - - - - - - - -
Cued Recall - - - 0.75 9 55% - - - - - - - - -
Executive/Conceptual Skills
WCST (pers. errors and/or 0.43 9 71% - - - 0.57 11 63% 0.52 8 66% - - -
responses)
WCST (categories) - - - - - - 0.52 10 66% 0.52 9 66%
WCST (sorting and shift ing) - - - - - - - - - - - - 0.35 25 76%
Halstead Category Test 0.45 5 70% - - - - - - - - - - - -

Notes: d = Cohen’s d; k = the number of contributing observations; OL = approximate percentage of overlap of the distributions; WAIS = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Test; SDMT =
Symbol-Digits Modalities Test; LTM = long term memory; WCST = Wisconsin Card Sorting Task. To facilitate comparisons, all effect estimates have been converted to d when given
in the original article as r (see Friedman, 1968). Also for Beatty et al. (2006), with the exception of fluency tasks, the effects reported were not based upon a comprehensive review
of the literature. Additionally, the Prakash et al. (2008) meta-analysis was restricted to RRMS samples; the other estimates illustrated were derived from MS samples that were not
restricted to a particular course. The WM d of Thornton and Raz (1997) is based upon the Constant Trigrams and Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test; the WM d of Prakash et al.,
2008 is based upon the Digit Span (backward) test, the Sternberg Test, the n-Back Test, and the Letter-Number Sequencing Test (WAIS-III).
TABLE 13–2. Frequency Estimates of Cognitive Impairment in MS
Nocentini et al. (2006) Rao et al. (1991a) n = 39
n = 461 RRMS. EDSS: RRMS; 19 CPMS; 42 CSMS.
M= 2.6; SD = 1.3 EDSS: M = 4.1; SD = 2.2
Cognitive domain % Impaired % Impaired
Overall Intellectual Abilities
WAIS-R Verbal Intelligence - 21
Raven Progressive Matrices 10 19
Verbal and Language Abilities
Information (WAIS) - 8
Comprehension (WAIS) - 18
Similarities (WAIS) - 19
Vocabulary (WAIS) - 16
Phonemic Fluency 19 22
Boston Naming Test - 9
Oral Comprehension - 9
Visuospatial & Construction Skills
Hooper Visual Organization Test - 13
Judgment of Line Orientation Test - 14
Visual Form Discrimination Test - 12
Facial Recognition Test - 19
Visual Design Copy 14–16 -
Attention and Working Memory
Digit Span Forward (WAIS) - 8
Digit Span Backward (WAIS) - 7
Arithmetic (WAIS) - 15
Sternberg Memory Scanning Task - 11
Paced Auditory Serial Addition - 22–25
Test
Stroop Interference Test - 12
Symbol Digit Modalities Test (oral) 43 -
Memory
Selective Reminding Test - -
Long-term storage - 22
Consistent long-term retrieval - 31
RAVLT (sum of trials 1–5) 25 -
RAVLT (delayed trial) 23 -
Prose Recall - 25
7/24 Spatial Recall Test - 31
Immediate Visual Memory (recog.) 3 -
President’s Test - 10
Executive/Conceptual Skills
Card Sorting (preservations) 14 8
Card Sorting (categories) 10 13
Halstead Category Test - 14
Notes: The “percent impaired” is the percentage of persons with MS that fall at or below the fi ft h percentile
of healthy persons. EDSS = Expanded Disability Status Scale; RRMS = relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis;
CPMS = chronic progressive multiple sclerosis; CSMS = chronic-stable multiple sclerosis; WAIS = Wechsler
Adult Intelligence Scale; RAVLT = Rey Auditory-Verbal Learning Test.

288
The Neuropsychology of Multiple Sclerosis 289

must judge whether the probe falls within the There is ample evidence supporting WM
memory set. Subjects are administered a num- impairments in persons with MS that are most
ber of item sets that vary in size (e.g., 1–6 dig- apparent when rapid information process-
its). Findings from healthy individuals indicate ing is required. Verbal WM, as operational-
that as the STM set size is increased, there is a ized by the Paced Auditory Serial Addition
corresponding linear increase in the latency to Test (PASAT) and Consonant Word Trigrams
judge whether an item falls within the memory tasks (see Ozakbas et al., 2004), is substan-
set (Sternberg, 1969). tially diminished in persons with MS (d = 0.72,
A few studies have evaluated the see Thornton & Raz, 1997; Table 13–1). Table
speed of STM search using the Sternberg 13–2 indicates that in a community sample,
Paradigm in participants with MS rela- 22%–25% of persons with MS are significantly
tive to healthy control subjects (Archibald & impaired on the PASAT task, which involves
Fisk, 2000; Rao et al., 1989c, 1991a). In a com- both processing speed and WM (see Demaree
munity sample, 11% of individuals with MS et al., 1999). More moderate WM deficits are
showed significant slowing in memory scan- apparent in samples comprised of only RRMS
ning (see Table 13–2) and this finding was sub- patients (d = 0.52, see Prakash et al., 2008; Table
sequently replicated at a university-associated 13–1), when using a compilation of tests that
MS research clinic. In contrast, one study failed tax WM and processing speed to a lesser degree
to detect any significant MS-control difference (e.g., Digit Span (backward), Letter-Number
in memory search efficiency (Litvan et al., Sequencing). Indeed, the extent to which a task
1988a). However, the sample size of the study makes demands upon WM appears to be cru-
was small, which may have contributed to the cial. On the n-back task, an experimental WM
null finding. procedure that requires identifying whether the
More generally, MS-associated deficits in current stimulus matches a previous stimulus
information-processing speed have often been presented a given number of trials back, persons
associated with both clinical and pathologi- with RRMS exhibit increasing impairment at
cal aspects of the disease. Compared to RRMS, higher complexity levels even after accounting
information-processing inefficiency appears to for simple motor speed (e.g., Parmenter et al.,
be greatest in persons with progressive MS, and 2007a).
in those with greater disability levels and longer The impairments discussed above reflect a
illness duration (De Sonneville et al., 2002). general characterization of WM; however, WM
Other research has suggested that both higher has been more precisely formulated into a mul-
lesion load, and particularly reduced brain ticomponent theory (Baddeley, 1986, 1992). In
volume, are associated with attention ineffi- Baddeley’s model, a set of limited-capacity slave
ciency and increased response speed variability subsystems process different types of informa-
(Lazeron et al., 2006). tion, which are controlled by a central exec-
utive supervisor. The subsystems include the
visuospatial sketchpad and phonological loop.
Short-term and Working Memory
The latter comprises two components: (1) the
Modest-to-moderate deficits are seen in less phonological store, which holds speech-based
complex aspects of short-term and working information for a very brief duration, and (2)
memory (WM) such as memory span and men- articulatory control, which refreshes the phono-
tal arithmetic (see Table 13–1). Table 13–2 indi- logical store through subvocal rehearsal. Under
cates that only 7%–8% of community-residing dual-task conditions, the limited capacity cen-
persons with MS experienced significant Digit tral executive controller appears degraded in
Span impairments; whereas, 15% of this sam- MS relative to healthy controls (D’Esposito et al.,
ple showed impairments in Arithmetic, which 1996). Articulatory control processes within the
is more apt to challenge WM processes. These phonological slave subsystems also appear to be
processes involve manipulating, integrating, diminished in persons with MS, as suggested by
and simultaneously holding items in STM while an exaggerated word length effect (Litvan et al.,
processing information. 1988b; Rao et al., 1993; Ruchkin et al., 1994). In
290 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

contrast, the phonological store itself appears and colleagues (2004) discuss potential expla-
intact (Litvan et al., 1988b). nations for the processing speed observations
Disease course and underlying brain pathol- in MS. From the perspective of the Relative
ogy clearly influences the expression of neu- Consequence Model, fundamental processing-
rocognitive deficits, particularly impairments speed impairment may underlie deficits on
involving processing speed and WM capacities other more complex neurocognitive tasks (e.g.,
(e.g., Archibald & Fisk, 2000; Sperling et al., WM) when a critical threshold of processing-
2001). WM deficits are relatively greater in per- speed impairment is reached. This is contrasted
sons with a progressive MS course (Thornton & with the Independent Consequence Model,
Raz, 1997). Although patients with either RRMS which holds that processing-speed deficits and
or SPMS often exhibit impairments in process- WM impairments may arise from independent
ing speed, progressive MS is associated with sources (DeLuca et al., 2004). While support has
increased risk for WM impairments. There is an been generated for both of these models (DeLuca
apparent precipitous increase in STM and WM et al., 2004; Demaree et al., 1999; Lengenfelder
impairments in samples comprised of CPMS et al., 2006), there is nevertheless substantial
participants relative to samples comprised of evidence suggesting that information-process-
RRMS participants (see Thornton & Raz, 1997; ing speed may be a fundamental deficit in MS
Zakzanis, 2000). This latter observation does (e.g., De Sonneville et al., 2002). Deficits in
not appear to be attributable to greater disabil- information-processing speed appear common
ity levels in the progressive MS participants in MS generally (e.g., ¼ of persons with RRMS
(Archibald & Fisk, 2000). experienced such deficits); whereas WM deficits
DeLuca and colleagues (2004) have made a are relatively rare in RRMS but become increas-
similar observation regarding the relative prev- ingly common with a progressive MS course.
alence of impairment in WM and information- Processing speed and WM capacity also
processing speed in participants seen at a tertiary appear related to performance deficits on other
referral center. Information-processing speed more complex neurocognitive tasks. Whether
was significantly impaired (fift h percentile cut- these abilities are mechanisms for more com-
off ) in 22% of individuals with RRMS and 77% plex neurocognitive deficits or markers for such
of persons with SPMS. In contrast, only 6% of deficits remains unclear. Nonetheless, reduced
persons with RRMS were significantly impaired WM capacity and decreased processing speed
in WM, whereas 32% of the participants with are frequently associated with impairments
SPMS showed WM deficits. Importantly, these in long-term memory (LTM; DeLuca et al.,
associations might be proxies for the underly- 1994; Gaudino et al., 2001; Litvan et al., 1988b;
ing neuropathology. For example, greater total Thornton et al., 2002). In addition, greater vul-
lesion load and the specific loads in the frontal nerability to retrograde interference (RI) is
and parietal cortex have been associated with apparent in persons with MS, which presumably
poorer performance on the PASAT and SDMT entails WM processes (Griffiths et al., 2005).
and lesion load was marginally greater in SPMS Likewise, attenuated processing speed is associ-
compared to RRMS (Sperling et al., 2001). ated with poor performance on a variety of other
Indeed, there has been interest in distin- cognitive tasks (Denney et al., 2004, 2005).
guishing WM deficits from those of infor-
mation processing speed in persons with MS.
Long-Term Memory
Interestingly, when PASAT-type tasks, which
are noted to involve WM and processing speed, Over the last several decades, MS-related LTM
are set at slower presentation speeds, the recall deficits have been observed in both clinical
accuracy of most individuals with MS is equal (Carroll et al., 1984; Grant et al., 1984; Rao
to that of healthy participants (Demaree et al., et al., 1984) and community-based samples
1999; Lengenfelder et al., 2006). This finding (Rao et al., 1989b, 1991a). While a small sub-
is argued to be consistent with a fundamental group of persons with MS exhibit amnesia and
deficit in speed of information processing over dementia (Beatty et al., 1996; Fischer, 1988; Rao
WM accuracy (DeLuca et al., 2004). DeLuca et al., 1984), generally persons with MS have the
The Neuropsychology of Multiple Sclerosis 291

capacity to learn new information (Beatty et al., this evidence is indirect because it is observed
1988; DeLuca et al., 1994; Minden et al., 1990; during the rapid access of highly consolidated
Rao et al., 1984). information, not in retrieving recently encoded
Whether persons with MS exhibit a reduc- long-term declarative memories.
tion in their rate of information acquisition In selective reminding paradigms, an incon-
is less obvious. On multitrial lists, individu- sistent recall pattern in persons with MS is
als with MS recall fewer items on each trial; observed relative to that of healthy individuals
the overall impact on recall corresponds to a (Rao et al., 1989b, 1991a, 1993). This recall incon-
large-effect estimate (see Table 13–1, List and sistency occurs after items have presumably
Pairs Learning). This observation confounds been entered into long-term storage, which sug-
raw recall with learning rate, as normal recall gests that retrieval falters. Table 13–2 indicates
gains may occur after poor initial trial recall. that 31% of a community-based sample of per-
MS-related deficits in learning have also been sons with MS showed significant impairments
reported in paradigms that involve a tally of in their LTM retrieval consistency. A retrieval-
the number of trials required to meet a cri- focused interpretive framework has also been
terion performance level (e.g., DeLuca et al., applied to the pattern of recognition and recall
1994; Grant et al., 1984; Heaton et al., 1985). performance in persons with MS. Recognition
However, these measures are also sensitive to is thought to minimize demands on retrieval
both raw recall and learning rate. Alternatively, and capture the encoding and storage processes
the rate of recall improvement (learning slope) (Grafman et al., 1990). The meta-analytic find-
over multiple trials gauges acquisition more ings in Table 13–1 indicate that verbal LTM rec-
directly. Observations suggest that the overall ognition is moderately affected in MS. In the
learning slope is relatively intact in group stud- verbal domain, this effect is attenuated relative
ies of individuals with MS (Beatty et al., 1989b; to the more robust losses in free recall.
Jennekens-Schinkel et al., 1990; Rao et al., 1984, Consistent with the retrieval-focused inter-
but also see Beatty & Gange, 1977). pretation of MS-related memory, dysfunction
Typically, persons with MS do not show a is evidence of preserved encoding. For instance,
pattern of rapid forgetting; rather deficits are encoding that involves semantic as opposed to
apparent at the time of immediate recall (e.g., perceptual processing enhances LTM perfor-
van den Burg et al., 1987). Indeed, Table 13–1 mance in MS (Carroll et al., 1984). Like healthy
indicates that effect estimates for immediate individuals, persons with MS also recall highly
and delayed recall are fairly consistent. This lack imaginable words more readily than difficult to
of differential forgetting suggests that attention, imagine words (Caine et al., 1986). Nevertheless,
WM, encoding, and/or retrieval processes are encoding may also be a source of memory fail-
candidate operations underlying LTM impair- ure. When persons with MS use semantic study
ment in MS. strategies, a loss in the distinctiveness of infor-
Long-term declarative memory impairment mation within the memory trace is observed
in MS has often been conceptualized as primar- (Carroll et al., 1984). Individuals with MS also
ily involving attenuated effortful retrieval of have difficulty utilizing contextual information
information, while encoding and storage appear presented during encoding to retrieve infor-
relatively preserved (Caine et al., 1986; Grafman mation later (Thornton et al., 2002) and they
et al., 1990; Rao et al., 1989b, 1993). For example, exhibit reduced semantic clustering, which
the recall pattern generated by persons with MS might reflect poor semantic encoding (Arnett
during verbal fluency and selective reminding et al., 1997; Diamond et al., 1997).
tasks (Buschke, 1973; Buschke & Fuld, 1974) Interference may also contribute to compro-
implicates retrieval as a viable mechanism mised LTM encoding in MS. Proactive interfer-
in the LTM impairment in MS. Semantic and ence (PI) occurs when prior learning impedes
lexical fluency induce retrieval processes in retention of subsequent materials (Postman &
accessing memory stores (Grafman et al., 1990; Underwood, 1973). Versions of the Wicken’s par-
Rao et al., 1989b). As noted earlier, verbal flu- adigm (Wickens, 1970) have been used to study
ency is markedly impaired in MS; nevertheless, interference in MS. In this paradigm, semantically
292 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

related items administered across multiple learn- not necessarily the only source of LTM impair-
ing trials is associated with a decline in recall as ment. Further, in individuals with MS a variety
interference increases. Recall recovers (“release” of recall patterns emerge, with a small minority
from PI) with the introduction of a new semantic of persons meeting a strict definition of retrieval
category. Increased PI is associated with poorer deficit (Beatty et al., 1996). Operations that tax
WM ability (Blusewicz et al., 1996), while atten- either encoding and retrieval processes appear
uated release from PI has been related to mne- central to the deficits observed. Memory tax-
monic and executive deficits (Cermak et al., 1974; ing cognitive operations deployed during LTM
Randolph et al., 1992; Squire, 1982). warrant further research attention as they likely
Individuals with MS generally demon- contribute to the impairment patterns observed
strate normal accumulation and release from on traditional clinical memory tests.
PI on Wicken’s-type paradigms (Beatty et al.,
1989a; Johnson et al., 1998; Rao et al., 1993),
Implicit and Procedural Memory
even though they often exhibit WM and exec-
utive impairments and lesions to frontoparietal Contrasting the broad impairments noted in
regions (see Figure 13–1; e.g., Foong et al., 1997; declarative LTM, findings have consistently
Parmenter et al., 2007b; Sperling et al., 2001). indicated relatively normal abilities in non-
But the Wicken’s paradigm may be insensitive to declarative memory. In an earlier study, three
interference abnormalities (Dobbs et al., 1989). tasks of implicit memory were administered
Recently, our laboratory revisited interference to a community-based sample of persons with
in MS, using the California Verbal Learning MS and healthy individuals (Rao et al., 1993).
Test (CVLT; Delis et al., 1987; Kramer & Delis, On a degraded word-priming task, individu-
1991), and replicated findings suggesting that als with MS identified words in fewer trials
individuals with MS are no more susceptible than healthy controls, while exhibiting explicit
to accumulation of PI than healthy individu- memory impairment for these words. In addi-
als (Griffiths et al., 2005). In contrast, attenu- tion, MS and control participants derived equal
ated release from PI was observed, which might RT benefits from the repeated serial presenta-
reflect a loss in the semantic distinctiveness of tion of stimuli on a four-choice RT test. Finally,
LTM (Carroll et al., 1984). This loss may also be repeated stimulus exposure facilitated per-
manifested in the reduction of semantic catego- formance equally in MS patient and control
rization during recall (Arnett et al., 1997). subjects on an implicit reversed-mirror word-
In addition, we evaluated RI, which refers to reading task. More severely disabled persons
the decrement in retention of prior learning by with MS also perform well on nondeclarative
subsequent learning (Postman & Underwood, memory tasks. For example, regardless of dis-
1973). Findings suggested that persons with MS ability level MS participants perform normally
have increased susceptibility to RI (Griffiths on word stem-priming and pursuit motor-learn-
et al., 2005). These results appear to be germane ing tasks (Beatty et al., 1990; Scarrabelotti &
to ultimate LTM, as accumulated RI predicted Carroll, 1999; Seinela et al., 2002). In summary,
long-delay free recall in MS, but not in healthy when memory is based upon presumably non-
participants. We speculate that increased RI in conscious performance measures, persons with
MS may reflect a reduction in their ability to reg- MS benefit from having had previous exposure
ister distinctiveness in memory. Persons with to the stimulus material to a degree that is sim-
MS might readily register gist (general) infor- ilar to that of healthy individuals.
mation, rather than contextual and semantic
details (Goldstein et al., 1992; Thornton et al.,
Remote Memory
2002). Consequently, information may be more
vulnerable to interference from new entries that Persons with MS experience remote memory
activate similar general target attributes. impairment. For example, the ability to iden-
In summary, it appears that an increased vul- tify famous individuals from various decades is
nerability to LTM retrieval failure is indicated by impaired in persons with MS, with deficits that
aggregated group studies, but this breakdown is are consistent across the decades (Beatty et al.,
The Neuropsychology of Multiple Sclerosis 293

1988, 1989b). Nonetheless, these participants the coordination of cognition itself by means
recalled past US presidents in order as readily as of regulatory processes (see Fernandez-Duque
healthy individuals, which might arise from a et al., 2000).
lesser difficulty level for this test relative to iden- Traditional executive functioning tasks have
tifying famous faces (Beatty et al., 1988, 1989b; been used in the investigation of neuropsy-
Paul et al., 1997). Information on both tests is of chological deficits in MS. The most commonly
an impersonal nature; consequently, it has been employed tests are the Wisconsin Card Sorting
noted that conclusions are constrained by the Test (WCST) and Halstead Category Test
presumption that MS and healthy participants (HCT), both of which require abstract concept
had acquired the relevant information and were formation, mental flexibility, and response to
equally familiar with it in their past (Paul et al., feedback. Quantitative integrations of this liter-
1997). This limitation is overcome by autobio- ature suggest that the WCST and the HCT are
graphic memory paradigms (Kenealy et al., moderately affected by MS, with some sugges-
2000; Paul et al., 1997). tion that samples containing only RRMS might
The Autobiographical Memory Interview be more mildly impacted (i.e., Prakash et al.,
(AMI) evaluates semantic and episodic autobio- 2008; see Table 13–1). Estimates of the number
graphical memory by verifying memories of individuals with MS who are significantly
through the use of collateral interview of signifi- impaired on card sorting tasks range from 8%
cant others. Two past studies using this protocol to 14% (see Table 13–2). It has been suggested
have produced divergent findings, but the MS that these difficulties primarily reflect a fun-
participant samples were substantially different. damental MS-related weakness in identifying
In a sample of relatively mildly disabled persons sorting concepts (Beatty & Monson, 1996), but
with MS, findings revealed semantic, not epi- this issue has by no means been resolved.
sodic, autobiographic impairments (Paul et al., In addition to deficits on tasks of sorting
1997). Specifically, impairment was limited to and set shifting, persons with MS generally
generic (semantic) facts such as recalling the exhibit impairment on tests that require plan-
name of the high school attended. Participants ning a sequence of moves to reach a goal, while
with MS did not exhibit autobiographic inhibiting prepotent incorrect responses (e.g.,
impairments of personal memory episodes. tower tasks; Arnett et al., 1997; Foong et al.,
Furthermore, autobiographical knowledge of 1997). Particularly remarkable are MS-related
semantic facts was disrupted equally across all deficits are on the Stroop task, which requires
periods. In contrast, 60% of individuals with both information-processing speed and prepo-
more severe and long-standing MS exhibited tent response inhibition (Deloire et al., 2005;
autobiographical memory deficits, with impair- Foong et al., 1997; Rao et al., 1991a; also see
ment in both episodic and semantic autobio- Vitkovitch et al., 2002). Recently, Prakash et al.
graphical knowledge (Kenealy et al., 2000). (2008) reported meta-analytic observation from
These impairments were associated with intel- RRMS that indicated surprisingly large Stoop-
lectual level and conformed to Ribet’s law, with related deficits of d = 0.79 in these samples.
deficits being greater for more recent events. However, for both the tower task and the Stroop
test, processing speed may be fundamental to
the underpinnings of these MS-related deficits
Executive Functions and Metacognition
(Denney et al., 2004, 2005).
Perhaps the most intangible of all neu- Persons with MS exhibit a variety of other def-
rocognitive abilities are executive functions. icits presumed to reflect executive dysfunction.
Fundamentally, these abilities entail planning, For instance, temporal order memory impair-
monitoring, and adaptive/flexible respond- ment has been reported in MS (Armstrong
ing to novel or emergent circumstances (Lezak et al., 1996; Beatty & Monson, 1991a; but also
et al., 2004; Luria, 1966; Strauss et al., 2006). see Arnett et al., 1997). In addition, impair-
Metacognitive capacities are closely related ments have been observed in the novel sequenc-
to executive functions, as the former involves ing of pictorial information (Beatty & Monson,
both knowledge regarding cognition as well as 1994). Both temporal memory and cognitive
294 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

sequencing difficulties are related to poor per- MS (Canellopoulou & Richardson, 1998).
formance in various other cognitive domains, Metamemory and metacognition refers to the
including traditional executive tasks (Beatty & knowledge an individual has about their cog-
Monson, 1991a, 1994). nitive capabilities as well as the control over
Researchers have also examined the accuracy cognitive processes (Fernandez-Duque et al.,
of feeling-of-knowing (FOK) judgments in MS 2000). Awareness of one’s abilities and their
participants (Beatty & Monson, 1991b). In this limitations and the implementation/control of
case, FOK judgments were predictions made by relevant cognitive processes may improve per-
the subject as to whether an item that was not formance when cognitive demands are exten-
recalled would be subsequently recognized. In sive. Importantly, degradation in these oversight
persons with MS, poor FOK accuracy on select and control aspects of cognition in MS likely
measures was associated with impairment contributes to the losses observed in other abili-
on “frontal lobe tasks” and LTM recognition. ties. Depression and depressive attitudes appear
Relative to healthy individuals, persons with to play an important role in the accuracy of
MS also overestimate the occurrence of infre- metamemory judgments in MS (Randolph et al.,
quently presented words and underestimate 2001, 2004; Taylor, 1990). Increased depression
the occurrence of frequently presented words often emerges as a significant associate of per-
(Grafman et al., 1991). Additionally, MS par- ceived compared with objective cognitive dys-
ticipants have difficulty in cognitive estimation. function (Lovera et al., 2006). The relationships
Specifically, they show limitations in their abil- between metamemory and executive function,
ity to use known information to make accurate and metamemory and depression, are mediated
abstractions that generate a solution to a novel via depressive attitudes, suggesting that depres-
problem (e.g., “What is the average length of a sion and depressive attitudes further intensifies
man’s (or women’s) spine?”; Foong et al., 1997). memory complaints (Randolph et al., 2004).
Finally, persons with MS benefit from the use of Finally, the ability to remember to complete
imagery mnemonics, but most notably when the a task in the future appears compromised in
mnemonics are experimenter-generated versus patient with MS. This prospective memory abil-
self-generated (Canellopoulou & Richardson, ity requires not only remembering an intention
1998). MS patients apparently abandon the use at a future time but retrospectively recalling
of self-generated mnemonics (i.e., at follow-up) the required task (Cohen et al., 2001). Although
when explicit directions were absent. prior findings have suggested deficits in ret-
More generally, these findings are related rospective over prospective memory in MS
to the concept of insight. Findings are equivo- (Bravin et al., 2000), more recent research that
cal regarding how insightful persons with MS matched persons with MS and controls on the
are regarding their true memory and cogni- retrospective recall of information (as well as on
tive abilities. Although individuals with MS aspects of STM) indicates that these prospective
may be as accurate as informants in predict- deficits extend beyond retrospective impair-
ing their memory (Randolph et al., 2001), other ments (Rendell et al., 2007).
observations indicate that memory predictions
made by persons with MS occasionally appear
Risk Factors Associated with
degraded (Higginson et al., 2000; Taylor, 1990).
Neurocognitive Dysfunction
An earlier report revealed that, relative to con-
trols, persons with MS exhibited reduced cor- Generally, greater disability levels, a progres-
respondence between their subjective memory sive course, and the presence of depression
judgments and their true memory (Beatty & and fatigue are all modestly associated with
Monson, 1991b); nonetheless, there are positive increased MS-related neurocognitive impair-
associations between memory complaints and ments (e.g., Arnett, 2005; Beatty et al., 1988,
objective measures of memory and cognition 1989b; De Sonneville et al., 2002; Gaudino et al.,
(Randolph et al., 2001, 2004). 2001; Grossman et al., 1994; Heaton et al., 1985;
Regardless, the above findings clearly imply Hildebrandt et al., 2006; Huijbregts et al., 2006;
a deficit in the metacognition of persons with Kessler et al., 1992; Lynch et al., 2005; Thornton &
The Neuropsychology of Multiple Sclerosis 295

Raz, 1997; Wallin et al., 2006; Zakzanis, 2000). spectroscopy, diff usion tensor imaging) may
In addition, active disease processes are associ- further explicate variations in neurocognition in
ated with increased neurocognitive impairment, MS (Comi et al., 2001; Merelli & Casoni, 2000).
particularly on attention-demanding tasks Finally, neurocognitive deficits in MS appear
(Foong et al., 1998; Grant et al., 1984). Further, to emerge when physiological compensatory
it is important to recognize that impaired scores capacity is exceeded, as evaluated by activa-
on tasks involving motor and sensory function- tion patterns during cognitive tasks on func-
ing may partially reflect primary deficits in these tional MRI (fMRI; Cader et al., 2006; Cifelli &
areas rather than in cognition (Benedict et al., Matthews, 2002; Penner et al., 2007; Staffen
2002b; Bruce et al., 2007). et al., 2002; Wishart et al., 2004). The ability
Neurocognitive dysfunction in MS is strongly of individuals with MS to reorganize cortical
associated with total lesion burden (e.g., Benedict network activations to compensate for underly-
et al., 2006a; Lazeron et al., 2005, 2006; Rao et al., ing structural brain damage may contribute to
1989a; Rovaris et al., 2000). The distribution of the variability in the severity of neurocognitive
lesions in MS has also been related to select neu- manifestations in persons with MS (Rocca &
rocognitive deficits that are cogent within estab- Filippi, 2007). Deficits may be most pronounced
lished neuropsychological frameworks (Arnett on tasks that both tax cognitive abilities and
et al., 1994; Huber et al., 1992; Lazeron et al., 2005; exceed the general compensatory capacity of
Sperling et al., 2001). Recently, the role of brain individuals with MS (Penner et al., 2007).
atrophy has been recognized as an important
feature associated with the expression of neu-
Emotional Functioning in MS
rocognitive impairments in MS (Benedict et al.,
2004, 2006a; Lanz et al., 2007; Lazeron et al., Evidence of emotional disturbances is apparent
2006; Parmenter et al., 2007b). Longitudinally, in a considerable proportion of persons with MS.
progression of brain atrophy has been identified Major depression, in particular, has a lifetime
as a significant predictor in the development of prevalence of approximately 50% in persons
physical disability and neurocognitive dysfunc- with MS, following diagnosis (Sadovnick et al.,
tion in early MS, whereas changes in brain lesion 1996), which is notably higher than that seen in
volumes were not (Zivadinov et al., 2001). the general population (American Psychiatric
Regional lobar atrophy has been linked to Association, 1994; Dalton & Heinrichs, 2005;
impairments in a variety of neurocognitive func- Schubert & Foliart, 1993). A recent quantitative
tions (Benedict et al., 2002a; Lazeron et al., 2005, review comparing the depression scores of per-
2006) and temporal lobar atrophy was recently son with MS to those of healthy persons indicate
found to be specifically predictive of MS-related differences in excess of one standard deviation
declarative LTM impairment (Benedict et al., that favor greater depression in persons with
2005b; Hildebrandt et al., 2006). However, for MS (Dalton & Heinrichs, 2005). Interestingly,
both regional atrophy and localized lesion vol- in RRMS samples the effect estimates observed
umes, neurocognitive dysfunction is often also are somewhat less (d = 0.70) and apparently
strongly associated with total brain pathology. equivalent to that of anxiety (d = 0.69; Prakash
This raises the possibility that the selective asso- et al., 2008).
ciations are proxies for overall pathology (Foong It has been suggested that depression in MS
et al., 1997; Merelli & Casoni, 2000). can result from either an emotional reaction
Despite the covariation between neurocogni- to having the disease or as a result of neuropa-
tion and neuropathology, a significant portion of thology (Mohr et al., 2001; Zorzon et al., 2001).
the pattern and severity of deficits remain unex- In terms of neuropathological underpinnings,
plained by conventional imaging techniques. extensive lesions in the left medial inferior pre-
One possibility is that these techniques fail to frontal region in conjunction with left anterior
capture pathology in tissue that appears nor- temporal lobe atrophy were recently found to
mal (Merelli & Casoni, 2000). Thus, once fully be particularly strong predictors of depression,
established, some of the newer techniques (e.g., accounting for 42% of the variance in depression
magnetization transfer, magnetic resonance scores (Feinstein et al., 2004). Other studies also
296 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

suggested that pathology within the cortex and disability (Feinstein & Feinstein, 2001; Feinstein
white matter tracks are associated with depres- et al., 1997) and lesions in the cerebro-ponto-
sion and psychiatric disorders in MS (Bakshi cerebellar pathways (Parvizi et al., 2001).
et al., 2000; Benedict et al., 2004; Berg et al., 2000; Executive deficits in MS have been specifi-
Honer et al., 1987; Pujol et al., 2000; Zorzon et al., cally associated with neuropsychiatric aspects
2001). Temporal cortex lesions have been related of the disorder. Persons with MS exhibiting a
to poor cognitive-behavioral treatment response neurocognitive pattern consistent with fron-
in depressed persons with MS (Mohr et al., tal lobe pathology (impairments on executive
2003). Furthermore, attenuated treatment gains tasks), also tend to exhibit increased eupho-
at a 6-month follow-up were related to greater ria, impaired empathy, and reduced altruism.
total lesion volume, mediated through neuropsy- Relative to the reports of collateral informants,
chological functioning. These findings indicate persons with MS overestimate their conscien-
the complex relationships between cognition, tiousness and empathy toward others and expe-
depression, and emotional recovery. rience increased neuroticism, suggesting that
Depression is associated with greater risk these individuals suffer a loss of insight into
for MS-related cognitive impairment, partic- their personality alterations (Benedict et al.,
ularly on tasks involving effort, WM, process- 2001). Interestingly, euphoria and disinhibition
ing speed, and executive abilities (Arnett, 2005; are strongly associated with brain atrophy and
Arnett et al., 1999a, 1999b, 2001; Demaree et al., lesion burden (Benedict et al., 2004).
2003; Feinstein, 2006; Gilchrist & Creed, 1994;
Landro et al., 2004; Maor et al., 2001; Thornton &
Summary
Raz, 1997; Wallin et al., 2006). Given the fact
that many of the neurological symptoms of MS MS is a heterogeneous disease that affects indi-
overlap with vegetative symptoms of depression, viduals differentially over a spectrum of phys-
the multisymptomatic nature of depression and ical, emotional, and neurocognitive domains.
its association with neurocognition has been The manifestations of the disease vary both
scrutinized more closely (Arnett, 2005). In this within an individual over time and between
longitudinal work, negative evaluative symp- individuals. This likely reflects the fact that the
toms (e.g., inferiority feelings, worthlessness) pathological changes of the central nervous
were strongly and most consistently associ- system in persons with MS are dynamic, wide-
ated with neurocognitive impairment, relative spread, and distinctive.
to mood (e.g., sad, glum) and vegetative (e.g., Five prior meta-analytical reviews have pro-
exhausted, poor appetite) symptoms (Arnett, vided a wealth of consistent data suggesting
2005). Furthermore, poor coping style inter- that the most pronounced MS-related neuro-
acted with depression as their joint occurrence psychological deficits emerge in the areas of
increased the likelihood of cognitive impair- verbal fluency, information processing speed,
ment (Arnett et al., 2002). and in aspects of WM and LTM. Deficits in all
There is also an increased lifetime prevalence of these areas are conventionally characterized
rate of bipolar disorder in MS that is approxi- as moderate to large. These prominent deficits
mately 13 times higher than that of the general emerge when cognitive effort and process-
population (Feinstein, 1999). The lifetime preva- ing speed are critical to effective performance
lence rate of any anxiety disorder is elevated and (see D’Esposito et al., 1996; Lengenfelder et al.,
approximates 36%, with panic disorder, obses- 2006). Additionally, impairments appear to be
sive compulsive disorder, and generalized anxi- amplified by capacity-reducing conditions such
ety disorder among the most common (Korostil as depression (Arnett et al., 1999b, 2001), and
& Feinstein, 2007). Anxiety appears to be more when compensatory capacity (as evaluated by
reactive in nature, and not strongly related to activation patterns during cognitive tasks on
MRI-revealed brain pathology (Zorzon et al., fMRI) has been exceeded (Cader et al., 2006;
2001). Approximately 8%–10% of persons with Cifelli & Matthews, 2002; Staffen et al., 2002;
MS experience pathological laughing or cry- Wishart et al., 2004). Deficits may be most
ing, which appears to be related to more severe pronounced on tasks that both tax cognitive
The Neuropsychology of Multiple Sclerosis 297

abilities and exceed the general compensatory Arnett, P. A. (2005). Longitudinal consistency of the
capacity of individuals with MS. relationship between depression symptoms and
There are several potential areas in which cognitive functioning in multiple sclerosis. CNS
interventions might mitigate the neurocogni- Spectrums, 10(5), 372–382.
Arnett, P. A., Higginson, C. I., & Randolph, J. J. (2001).
tive impairments experienced by persons with
Depression in multiple sclerosis: Relationship
MS. Specifically, treatments to alleviate depres-
to planning ability. Journal of the International
sion and improve coping appear to be prom- Neuropsychological Society, 7(6), 665–674.
ising. Techniques to retrain brain function by Arnett, P. A., Higginson, C. I., Voss, W. D., Bender,
establishing viable compensatory networks W. I., Wurst, J. M., & Tippin, J. M. (1999a).
that overcome impairments may also be fruit- Depression in multiple sclerosis: Relationship
ful (Penner et al., 2006; Penner et al., 2007). to working memory capacity. Neuropsychology,
Furthermore, curtailing disease progression 13(4), 546–556.
and enhancing cognition through pharmaco- Arnett, P. A., Higginson, C. I., Voss, W. D., Randolph,
logical interventions and rehabilitation might J. J., & Grandey, A. A. (2002). Relationship between
reduce the ultimate neurocognitive deficits coping, cognitive dysfunction and depression
in multiple sclerosis. Clinical Neuropsychologist,
incurred, but clearly more evidence supporting
16(3), 341–355.
these treatments is crucial (Pierson & Griffith,
Arnett, P. A., Higginson, C. I., Voss, W. D., Wright,
2006). B., Bender, W. I., Wurst, J. M., et al. (1999b).
Depressed mood in multiple sclerosis: Relationship
Acknowledgment to capacity-demanding memory and attentional
functioning. Neuropsychology, 13(3), 434–446.
The authors wish to acknowledge Donna Lang Arnett, P. A., Rao, S. M., Bernardin, L., Grafman,
for consultation on neuroimaging aspects of J., Yetkin, F. Z., & Lobeck, L. (1994). Relationship
this paper and Wendy Loken Thornton for her between frontal lobe lesions and Wisconsin Card
comments on an earlier version of this manu- Sorting Test performance in patients with multi-
script. We also thank the meta-analytic authors ple sclerosis. Neurology, 44(3), 420–425.
Arnett, P. A., Rao, S. M., Grafman, J., Bernardin, L.,
who provided additional information to assist
Luchetta, T., Binder, J. R., et al. (1997). Executive
in effect size reporting.
functions in multiple sclerosis: An analysis of tem-
poral ordering, semantic encoding, and planning
References abilities. Neuropsychology, 11(4), 535–544.
Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory. Oxford:
Amato, M. P., Ponziani, G., Siracusa, G., & Sorbi, S. Clarendon Press.
(2001). Cognitive dysfunction in early-onset mul- Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memory. Science,
tiple sclerosis: A reappraisal after 10 years. Archives 255(5044), 556–559.
of Neurology, 58(10), 1602–1606. Bagos, P. G., Nikolopoulos, G., & Ioannidis, A.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic (2006). Chlamydia pneumoniae infection and the
and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). risk of multiple sclerosis: A meta-analysis. Multiple
Washington, DC: Author. Sclerosis, 12(4), 397–411.
Anderson, D. W., Ellenberg, J. H., Leventhal, C. M., Bakshi, R., Czarnecki, D., Shaikh, Z. A., Priore, R.
Reingold, S. C., Rodriguez, M., & Silberberg, D. L., Janardhan, V., Kaliszky, Z., et al. (2000). Brain
H. (1992). Revised estimate of the prevalence of MRI lesions and atrophy are related to depres-
multiple sclerosis in the United States. Annals of sion in multiple sclerosis. Neuroreport, 11(6),
Neurology, 31(3), 333–336. 1153–1158.
Archibald, C. J., & Fisk, J. D. (2000). Information Beatty, P. A., & Gange, J. J. (1977). Neuropsychological
processing efficiency in patients with multiple aspects of multiple sclerosis. Journal of Nervous
sclerosis. Journal of Clinical and Experimental and Mental Disease, 164(1), 42–50.
Neuropsychology, 22(5), 686–701. Beatty, W. W. (1996). Multiple sclerosis. In R. L.
Armstrong, C., Onishi, K., Robinson, K., D’Esposito, Adams, O. A. Parsons, J. L. Culbertson, & S. J.
M., Thompson, H., Rostami, A., et al. (1996). Serial Nixon (Eds.), Neuropsychology for clinical practice:
position and temporal cue effects in multiple scle- Etiology, assessment, and treatment of common
rosis: Two subtypes of defective memory mecha- neurological disorders (pp. 225–242). Washington,
nisms. Neuropsychologia, 34(9), 853–862. DC: American Psychological Association.
298 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Beatty, W. W., Goodkin, D. E., Beatty, P. A., & Benedict, R. H., Cookfair, D., Gavett, R., Gunther, M.,
Monson, N. (1989a). Frontal lobe dysfunction and Munschauer, F., Garg, N., et al. (2006b). Validity
memory impairment in patients with chronic pro- of the Minimal Assessment of Cognitive Function
gressive multiple sclerosis. Brain and Cognition, in Multiple Sclerosis (MACFIMS). Journal of the
11(1), 73–86. International Neuropsychological Society, 12(4),
Beatty, W. W., Goodkin, D. E., Monson, N., & Beatty, 549–558.
P. A. (1989b). Cognitive disturbances in patients Benedict, R. H., Fischer, J. S., Archibald, C. J.,
with relapsing remitting multiple sclerosis. Arnett, P. A., Beatty, W. W., Bobholz, J., et al.
Archives of Neurology, 46(10), 1113–1119. (2002b). Minimal neuropsychological assessment
Beatty, W. W., Goodkin, D. E., Monson, N., & Beatty, P. of MS patients: A consensus approach. Clinical
A. (1990). Implicit learning in patients with chronic Neuropsychologist, 16(3), 381–397.
progressive multiple sclerosis. International Journal Benedict, R. H. B., Priore, R. L., Miller, C.,
of Clinical Neuropsychology, 12(3), 166–172. Munschauer, F. E., & Jacobs, L. (2001). Personality
Beatty, W. W., Goodkin, D. E., Monson, N., Beatty, disorder in multiple sclerosis correlates with cog-
P. A., & Hertsgaard, D. (1988). Anterograde and nitive impairment. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and
retrograde amnesia in patients with chronic pro- Clinical Neurosciences, 13(1), 70–76.
gressive multiple sclerosis. Archives of Neurology, Benedict, R. H., Wahlig, E., Bakshi, R., Fishman,
45(6), 611–619. I., Munschauer, F., Zivadinov, R., et al. (2005a).
Beatty, W. W., & Monson, N. (1991a). Memory for Predicting quality of life in multiple sclerosis:
temporal order in multiple sclerosis. Bulletin of the Accounting for physical disability, fatigue, cog-
Psychonomic Society, 29(1), 10–12. nition, mood disorder, personality, and behav-
Beatty, W. W., & Monson, N. (1991b). Metamemory ior change. Journal of the Neurological Sciences,
in multiple sclerosis. Journal of Clinical and 231(1), 29–34.
Experimental Neuropsychology, 13(2), 309–327. Benedict, R. H., Weinstock-Guttman, B., Fishman,
Beatty, W. W., & Monson, N. (1994). Picture and I., Sharma, J., Tjoa, C. W., & Bakshi, R. (2004).
motor sequencing in multiple sclerosis. Journal of Prediction of neuropsychological impairment in
Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 16(2), multiple sclerosis: Comparison of conventional
165–172. magnetic resonance imaging measures of atrophy
Beatty, W. W., & Monson, N. (1996). Problem solving and lesion burden. Archives of Neurology, 61(2),
by patients with multiple sclerosis: Comparison 226–230.
of performance on the Wisconsin and California Benedict, R. H., Zivadinov, R., Carone, D. A.,
Card Sorting Tests. Journal of the International Weinstock-Guttman, B., Gaines, J., Maggiore,
Neuropsychological Society, 2(2), 134–140. C., et al. (2005b). Regional lobar atrophy pre-
Beatty, W. W., Wilbanks, S. L., Blanco, C. R., Hames, dicts memory impairment in multiple sclero-
K. A., Tivis, R., & Paul, R. H. (1996). Memory dis- sis. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 26(7),
turbance in multiple sclerosis: Reconsideration of 1824–1831.
patterns of performance on the selective remind- Berg, D., Supprian, T., Thomae, J., Warmuth-Metz,
ing test. Journal of Clinical and Experimental M., Horowski, A., Zeiler, B., et al. (2000). Lesion
Neuropsychology, 18(1), 56–62. pattern in patients with multiple sclerosis and
Benedict, R. H., Bakshi, R., Simon, J. H., Priore, depression. Multiple Sclerosis, 6(3), 156–162.
R., Miller, C., & Munschauer, F. (2002a). Frontal Bergendal, G., Fredrikson, S., & Almkvist, O.
cortex atrophy predicts cognitive impairment in (2007). Selective decline in information process-
multiple sclerosis. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and ing in subgroups of multiple sclerosis: An 8-year
Clinical Neurosciences, 14(1), 44–51. longitudinal study. European Neurology, 57(4),
Benedict, R. H., Bruce, J. M., Dwyer, M. G., 193–202.
Abdelrahman, N., Hussein, S., Weinstock- Bermel, R. A., & Bakshi, R. (2006). The measurement
Guttman, B., et al. (2006a). Neocortical atrophy, and clinical relevance of brain atrophy in multiple
third ventricular width, and cognitive dysfunc- sclerosis. Lancet Neurology, 5(2), 158–170.
tion in multiple sclerosis. Archives of Neurology, Blusewicz, M. J., Kramer, J. H., & Delmonico, R. L.
63(9), 1301–1306. (1996). Interference effects in chronic alcoholism.
Benedict, R. H. B., Carone, D. A., & Bakshi, R. (2004). Journal of the International Neuropsychological
Correlating brain atrophy with cognitive dysfunc- Society, 2(2), 141–145.
tion, mood disturbances, and personality disorder Brassington, J. C., & Marsh, N. V. (1998).
in multiple sclerosis. Journal of Neuroimaging, Neuropsychological aspects of multiple sclerosis.
14(Suppl 3), 36S–45S. Neuropsychology Review, 8(2), 43–77.
The Neuropsychology of Multiple Sclerosis 299

Bravin, J. H., Kinsella, G. J., Ong, B., & Vowels, L. Compston, A. (1999). The genetic epidemiology of
(2000). A study of performance of delayed inten- multiple sclerosis. Philosophical Transactions of the
tions in multiple sclerosis. Journal of Clinical and Royal Society of London, 354(1390), 1623–1634.
Experimental Neuropsychology, 22(3), 418–429. Compston, A., & Coles, A. (2002). Multiple sclerosis.
Brown, R. F., Tennant, C. C., Sharrock, M., Lancet, 359(9313), 1221–1231.
Hodgkinson, S., Dunn, S. M., & Pollard, J. D. Dalton, C. M., Brex, P. A., Miszkiel, K. A., Hickman,
(2006). Relationship between stress and relapse in S. J., MacManus, D. G., Plant, G. T., et al. (2002).
multiple sclerosis: Part II. Direct and indirect rela- Application of the new McDonald criteria to
tionships. Multiple Sclerosis, 12(4), 465–475. patients with clinically isolated syndromes sug-
Bruce, J. M., Bruce, A. S., & Arnett, P. A. (2007). Mild gestive of multiple sclerosis. Annals of Neurology,
visual acuity disturbances are associated with per- 52(1), 47–53.
formance on tests of complex visual attention in Dalton, E. J., & Heinrichs, R. W. (2005). Depression
MS. Journal of the International Neuropsychological in multiple sclerosis: A quantitative review of the
Society, 13(3), 544–548. evidence. Neuropsychology, 19(2), 152–158.
Buschke, H. (1973). Selective reminding for analy- De Sonneville, L. M. J., Boringa, J. B., Reuling, I. E.
sis of memory and learning. Journal of Verbal W., Lazeron, R. H. C., Ader, H. J., & Polman, C. H.
Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12(5), 543–550. (2002). Information processing characteristics in
Buschke, H., & Fuld, P. A. (1974). Evaluating storage, subtypes of multiple sclerosis. Neuropsychologia,
retention, and retrieval in disordered memory and 40(11), 1751–1765.
learning. Neurology, 24(11), 1019–1025. Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B. A.
Cader, S., Cifelli, A., Abu-Omar, Y., Palace, J., & (1987). The California Verbal Learning Test. San
Matthews, P. M. (2006). Reduced brain func- Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
tional reserve and altered functional connectiv- Deloire, M. S., Salort, E., Bonnet, M., Arimone,
ity in patients with multiple sclerosis. Brain, 129, Y., Boudineau, M., Amieva, H., et al. (2005).
527–537. Cognitive impairment as marker of diff use brain
Caine, E. D., Bamford, K. A., Schiffer, R. B., Shoulson, abnormalities in early relapsing remitting multi-
I., & Levy, S. (1986). A controlled neuropsychologi- ple sclerosis. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery,
cal comparison of Huntington’s disease and multi- and Psychiatry, 76(4), 519–526.
ple sclerosis. Archives of Neurology, 43(3), 249–254. DeLuca, J., Barbieri-Berger, S., & Johnson, S. K. (1994).
Canellopoulou, M., & Richardson, J. T. (1998). The nature of memory impairments in multiple
The role of executive function in imagery mne- sclerosis: Acquisition versus retrieval. Journal of
monics: Evidence from multiple sclerosis. Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 16(2),
Neuropsychologia, 36(11), 1181–1188. 183–189.
Carroll, M., Gates, R., & Roldan, F. (1984). DeLuca, J., Chelune, G. J., Tulsky, D. S., Lengenfelder,
Memory impairment in multiple sclerosis. J., & Chiaravalloti, N. D. (2004). Is speed of process-
Neuropsychologia, 22(3), 297–302. ing or working memory the primary information
Cermak, L. S., Butters, N., and Moreines, J. (1974). processing deficit in multiple sclerosis? Journal of
Some analyses of the verbal encoding deficit of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 26(4),
alcoholic Korsakoff patients. Brain and Language, 550–562.
1(2), 141–150. Demaree, H. A., DeLuca, J., Gaudino, E. A., &
Cifelli, A., & Matthews, P. M. (2002). Cerebral plas- Diamond, B. J. (1999). Speed of information
ticity in multiple sclerosis: Insights from fMRI. processing as a key deficit in multiple sclero-
Multiple Sclerosis, 8(3), 193–199. sis: Implications for rehabilitation. Journal of
Cohen, A. I., West, R., & Craik, F. I. M. (2001). Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 67(5),
Modulation of the prospective and retrospective 661–663.
components of memory for intentions in youn- Demaree, H. A., Gaudino, E., & DeLuca, J. (2003).
ger and older adults. Aging, Neuropsychology, and The relationship between depressive symptoms
Cognition, 8(1), 1–13. and cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 8(3), 161–171.
behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Denney, D. R., Lynch, S. G., Parmenter, B. A., &
Lawrence Erlbaum. Horne, N. (2004). Cognitive impairment in
Comi, G., Rovaris, M., Leocani, L., Martinelli, V., & relapsing and primary progressive multiple scle-
Filippi, M. (2001). Clinical and MRI assessment of rosis: Mostly a matter of speed. Journal of the
brain damage in MS. Neurological Sciences, 22(8), International Neuropsychological Society, 10(7),
S123–S127. 948–856.
300 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Denney, D. R., Sworowski, L. A., & Lynch, S. G. (2005). Ford, H., Trigwell, P., & Johnson, M. (1998). The
Cognitive impairment in three subtypes of multi- nature of fatigue in multiple sclerosis. Journal of
ple sclerosis. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, Psychosomatic Research, 45(1), 33–38.
20(8), 967–981. Friedman, H. (1968). Magnitude of experimen-
D’Esposito, M., Onishi, K., Thompson, H., Robinson, tal effect and a table for its rapid estimation.
K., Armstrong, C., & Grossman, M. (1996). Psychological Bulletin, 70(4), 245–251.
Working memory impairments in multiple scle- GAMES, & Transatlantic Multiple Sclerosis
rosis: Evidence from a dual-task paradigm. Genetics Cooperative. (2003). A meta-analysis
Neuropsychology, 10(1), 51–56. of whole genome linkage screens in multiple
Diamond, B. J., DeLuca, J., Kim, H., & Kelley, S. M. sclerosis. Journal of Neuroimmunology, 143(1),
(1997). The question of disproportionate impair- 39–46.
ments in visual and auditory information process- Gaudino, E. A., Chiaravalloti, N. D., DeLuca, J., &
ing in multiple sclerosis. Journal of Clinical and Diamond, B. J. (2001). A comparison of memory
Experimental Neuropsychology, 19(1), 34–42. performance in relapsing-remitting, primary
Dobbs, A. R., Aubrey, J. B., & Rule, B. G. (1989). Age- progressive and secondary progressive, multiple
associated release from proactive interference: A sclerosis. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and
review. Canadian Psychology, 30(3), 588–595. Behavioral Neurology, 14(1), 32–44.
Ebers, G. C., Sadovnick, A. D., & Risch, N. J. (1995). Ge, Y. (2006). Multiple sclerosis: The role of MR
A genetic basis for familial aggregation in multiple imaging. American Journal of Neuroradiology,
sclerosis. Canadian Collaborative Study Group. 27(6), 1165–1176.
Nature, 377(6545), 150–151. Gilchrist, A. C., & Creed, F. H. (1994). Depression,
Feinstein, A. (1999). The clinical neuropsychia- cognitive impairment and social stress in multiple
try of multiple sclerosis. New York: Cambridge sclerosis. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38(3),
University Press. 193–201.
Feinstein, A. (2006). Mood disorders in multiple Goldstein, F. C., McKendall, R. R., & Haut, M. W.
sclerosis and the effects on cognition. Journal of (1992). Gist recall in multiple sclerosis. Archives of
the Neurological Sciences, 245(1), 63–66. Neurology, 49(10), 1060–1064.
Feinstein, A., & Feinstein, K. (2001). Depression Gontkovsky, S. T., & Beatty, W. W. (2006). Practical
associated with multiple sclerosis. Looking methods for the clinical assessment of informa-
beyond diagnosis to symptom expression. Journal tion processing speed. International Journal of
of Affective Disorders, 66(2), 193–198. Neuroscience, 116(11), 1317–1325.
Feinstein, A., Feinstein, K., Gray, T., & O’Connor, P. Grafman, J., Rao, S., Bernardin, L., & Leo, G. J.
(1997). Prevalence and neurobehavioral correlates (1991). Automatic memory processes in patients
of pathological laughing and crying in multiple with multiple sclerosis. Archives of Neurology,
sclerosis. Archives of Neurology, 54(9), 1116–1121. 48(10), 1072–1075.
Feinstein, A., Roy, P., Lobaugh, N., Feinstein, K., Grafman, J., Rao, S. M., & Litvan, I. (1990). Disorders
O’Connor, P., & Black, S. (2004). Structural brain of memory. In S. M. Rao (Ed.), Neurobehavioral
abnormalities in multiple sclerosis patients with aspects of multiple sclerosis (pp. 102–117). New
major depression. Neurology, 62(4), 586–590. York: Oxford University Press.
Fernandez-Duque, D., Baird, J. A., & Posner, M. I. Grant, I., McDonald, W. I., Trimble, M. R., Smith,
(2000). Executive attention and metacognitive reg- E., & Reed, R. (1984). Deficient learning and
ulation. Consciousness and Cognition, 9, 288–307. memory in early and middle phases of multiple
Fischer, J. S. (1988). Using the Wechsler Memory sclerosis. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and
Scale - Revised to detect and characterize mem- Psychiatry, 47(3), 250–255.
ory deficits in multiple sclerosis. The Clinical Griffiths, S. Y., Yamamoto, A., Boudreau, V. G.,
Neuropsychologist, 2(2), 149–172. Ross, L. K., Kozora, E., & Thornton, A. E. (2005).
Foong, J., Rozewicz, L., Quaghebeur, G., Davie, C. A., Memory interference in multiple sclerosis. Journal
Kartsounis, L. D., Thompson, A. J., et al. (1997). of the International Neuropsychological Society,
Executive function in multiple sclerosis: The role 11(6), 737–746.
of frontal lobe pathology. Brain, 120(1), 15–26. Grossman, M., Armstrong, C., Onishi, K., Thompson,
Foong, J., Rozewicz, L., Quaghebeur, G., Thompson, H., Schaefer, B., Robinson, K., et al. (1994).
A. J., Miller, D. H., & Ron, M. A. (1998). Patterns of cognitive impairment in relapsing-
Neuropsychological deficits in multiple scle- remitting and chronic progressive multiple scle-
rosis after acute relapse. Journal of Neurology, rosis. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and
Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 64(4), 529–532. Behavioral Neurology, 7(3), 194–210.
The Neuropsychology of Multiple Sclerosis 301

Hannay, H. J., Howieson, D. B., Loring, D. W., Fischer, Kail, R. (1998). Speed of information processing in
J. S., & Lezak, M. D. (2004). Neuropathology patients with multiple sclerosis. Journal of Clinical
for neuropsychologists. In M. D. Lezak, D. B. and Experimental Neuropsychology, 20(1), 98–106.
Howieson, D. W. Loring, H. J. Hannay, and J. S. Kantarci, O., & Wingerchuk, D. (2006). Epidemiology
Fischer (Eds.), Neuropsychological Assessment (4th and natural history of multiple sclerosis: New
ed., pp. 157–285). New York: Oxford University insights. Current Opinion in Neurology, 19(3),
Press. 248–254.
Heaton, R. K., Nelson, L. M., Thompson, D. S., Burks, Kantarci, O. H., & Weinshenker, B. G. (2001).
J. S., & Franklin, G. M. (1985). Neuropsychological Prognostic factors in multiple sclerosis. In S. D.
findings in relapsing-remitting and chronic-pro- Cook (Ed.), Handbook of multiple sclerosis (3rd
gressive multiple sclerosis. Journal of Consulting ed., pp. 449–463). New York: Marcel Dekker Inc.
and Clinical Psychology, 53(1), 103–110. Kaufman, A. S., & Lichtenberger, E. O. (2006).
Henry, J. D., & Beatty, W. W. (2006). Verbal fluency Assessing adolescent and adult intelligence (3rd
deficits in multiple sclerosis. Neuropsychologia, ed.). New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc.
44(7), 1166–1174. Keegan, B. M., & Noseworthy, J. H. (2002). Multiple
Higginson, C. I., Arnett, P. A., & Voss, W. D. (2000). sclerosis. Annual Review of Medicine, 53,
The ecological validity of clinical tests of memory 285–302.
and attention in multiple sclerosis. Archives of Kenealy, P. M., Beaumont, G. J., Lintern, T., & Murrell,
Clinical Neuropsychology, 15(3), 185–204. R. (2000). Autobiographical memory, depression
Hildebrandt, H., Hahn, H. K., Kraus, J. A., Schulte- and quality of life in multiple sclerosis. Journal of
Herbrüggen, A., Schwarze, B., & Schwendemann, Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 22(1),
G. (2006). Memory performance in multiple scle- 125–131.
rosis patients correlates with central brain atro- Kessler, H. R., Cohen, R. A., Lauer, K., & Kausch,
phy. Multiple Sclerosis, 12(4), 428–436. D. F. (1992). The relationship between disability
Hillert, J., & Masterman, T. (2001). The genetics of and memory dysfunction in multiple sclerosis.
multiple sclerosis. In S. D. Cook (Ed.), Handbook The International Journal of Neuroscience, 62(1),
of multiple sclerosis (3rd ed., pp. 33–66). New York: 17–34.
Marcel Dekker Inc. Kidd, D., Barkhof, F., McConnell, R., Algra, P. R.,
Honer, W. G., Hurwitz, T., Li, D. K., Palmer, M., & Allen, I. V., & Revesz, T. (1999). Cortical lesions in
Paty, D. W. (1987). Temporal lobe involvement in multiple sclerosis. Brain, 122, 17–26.
multiple sclerosis patients with psychiatric disor- Korostil, M., & Feinstein, A. (2007). Anxiety disor-
ders. Archives of Neurology, 44(2), 187–190. ders and their clinical correlates in multiple scle-
Huber, S. J., Bornstein, R. A., Rammohan, K. W., rosis patients. Multiple Sclerosis, 13(1), 67–72.
Christy, J. A., Chakeres, D. W., & McGhee Jr., R. Kramer, J. H., & Delis, D. C. (1991). Interference
B. (1992). Magnetic resonance imaging correlates effects on the California Verbal Learning Test:
of executive function impairments in multiple A construct validation study. Psychological
sclerosis. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Assessment, 3(2), 299–302.
Behavioral Neurology, 5(1), 33–36. Kujala, P., Portin, R., Revonsuo, A., & Ruutiainen, J.
Huijbregts, S. C., Kalkers, N. F., de Sonneville, L. M., (1994). Automatic and controlled information pro-
de Groot, V., & Polman, C. H. (2006). Cognitive cessing in multiple sclerosis. Brain, 117, 1115–1126.
impairment and decline in different MS sub- Kurtzke, J. F. (2000). Multiple sclerosis in time
types. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 245(1), and space--geographic clues to cause. Journal of
187–194. Neurovirology, 6(Suppl 2), S134–S140.
Jennekens-Schinkel, A., van der Velde, E. A., Sanders, Landro, N. I., Celius, E. G., & Sletvold, H. (2004).
E. A., & Lanser, J. B. (1990). Memory and learn- Depressive symptoms account for deficient infor-
ing in outpatients with quiescent multiple scle- mation processing speed but not for impaired
rosis. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 95(3), working memory in early phase multiple sclerosis
311–325. (MS). Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 217(2),
Johnson, S. K., DeLuca, J., Diamond, B. J., & Natelson, 211–216.
B. H. (1998). Memory dysfunction in fatiguing ill- Lanz, M., Hahn, H. K., & Hildebrandt, H. (2007).
ness: Examining interference and distraction in Brain atrophy and cognitive impairment in mul-
short-term memory. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, tiple sclerosis: A review. Journal of Neurology,
3(4), 269–285. 254(Suppl 2), II43–II48.
Joy, J. E., & Johnston, R. B. (Eds.). (2001). Multiple Laux, L. F., & Lane, D. M. (1985). Information pro-
sclerosis: Current status and strategies for the future. cessing components of substitution test perfor-
Washington, DC: National Academy Press. mance. Intelligence, 9(2), 111–136.
302 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Lazeron, R. H. C., Boringa, J. B., Schouten, M., Marrie, R. A. (2004). Environmental risk factors in
Uitdehaag, B. M. J., Bergers, E., Lindeboom, J., multiple sclerosis aetiology. Lancet Neurology,
et al. (2005). Brain atrophy and lesion load as 3(12), 709–718.
explaining parameters for cognitive impairment McDonald, W. I., Compston, A., Edan, G., Goodkin,
in multiple sclerosis. Multiple Sclerosis, 11(5), D., Hartung, H. P., Lublin, F. D., et al. (2001).
524–531. Recommended diagnostic criteria for multiple
Lazeron, R. H. C., de Sonneville, L. M. J., Scheltens, sclerosis: Guidelines from the International Panel
P., Polman, C. H., & Barkhof, F. (2006). Cognitive on the Diagnosis of Multiple Sclerosis. Annals of
slowing in multiple sclerosis is strongly associated Neurology, 50(1), 121–127.
with brain volume reduction. Multiple Sclerosis, Merelli, E., & Casoni, F. (2000). Prognostic factors in
12(6), 760–768. multiple sclerosis: Role of intercurrent infections
Lengenfelder, J., Bryant, D., Diamond, B. J., Kalmar, and vaccinations against influenza and hepatitis
J. H., Moore, N. B., & DeLuca, J. (2006). Processing B. Neurological Sciences, 21(4), S853–S856.
speed interacts with working memory effi- Miller, A. E. (2006). Clinical features. In S. D. Cook
ciency in multiple sclerosis. Archives of Clinical (Ed.), Handbook of multiple sclerosis (4th ed., pp.
Neuropsychology, 21(3), 229–238. 153–178). New York: Taylor and Francis.
Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., Loring, D. W., Hannay, Minden, S. L., Moes, E. J., Orav, J., Kaplan, E., &
H. J., & Fischer, J. S. (2004). Neuropsychological Reich, P. (1990). Memory impairment in multi-
Assessment (4th ed.). New York: Oxford University ple sclerosis. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Press. Neuropsychology, 12(4), 566–586.
Litvan, I., Grafman, J., Vendrell, P., & Martinez, J. M. Mohr, D., Hart, S., Julian, L., Cox, D., & Pelletier, D.
(1988a). Slowed information processing in multiple (2004). Association between stressful life events
sclerosis. Archives of Neurology, 45(3), 281–285. and exacerbation in multiple sclerosis: A meta-
Litvan, I., Grafman, J., Vendrell, P., Martinez, J. M., analysis. British Medical Journal, 328(7442),
Junque, C., Vendrell, J. M., et al. (1988b). Multiple 731–733.
memory deficits in patients with multiple sclerosis. Mohr, D. C., Boudewyn, A. C., Goodkin, D. E.,
Exploring the working memory system. Archives Bostrom, A., & Epstein, L. (2001). Comparative
of Neurology, 45(6), 607–610. outcomes for individual cognitive-behavior ther-
Lovera, J., Bagert, B., Smoot, K. H., Wild, K., Frank, apy, supportive-expressive group psychotherapy,
R., Bogardus, K., et al. (2006). Correlations of and sertraline for the treatment of depression
Perceived Deficits Questionnaire of Multiple in multiple sclerosis. Journal of Consulting and
Sclerosis Quality of Life Inventory with Beck Clinical Psychology, 69(6), 942–949.
Depression Inventory and neuropsychologi- Mohr, D. C., Epstein, L., Luks, T. L., Goodkin, D.,
cal tests. Journal of Rehabilitation Research and Cox, D., Goldberg, A., et al. (2003). Brain lesion
Development, 43(1), 73–82. volume and neuropsychological function pre-
Lublin, F. D., & Reingold, S. C. (1996). Defi ning the dict efficacy of treatment for depression in mul-
clinical course of multiple sclerosis: Results of an tiple sclerosis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
international survey. National Multiple Sclerosis Psychology, 71(6), 1017–1024.
Society (USA) Advisory Committee on Clinical Montalban, X., & Rio, J. (2006). Interferons and cog-
Trials of New Agents in Multiple Sclerosis. nition. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 245(1),
Neurology, 46(4), 907–911. 137–140.
Lucchinetti, C. F., & Parisi, J. E. (2006). Pathology: Murray, T. J. (2005). Multiple sclerosis: The history of
What may it tell us? In S. D. Cook (Ed.), Handbook a disease. New York: Demos Medical Pub.
of multiple sclerosis (4th ed., pp. 113–152). New Naismith, R. T., Trinkaus, K., & Cross, A. H. (2006).
York: Taylor and Francis. Phenotype and prognosis in African-Americans
Luria, A. R. (1966). Higher cortical functions in man with multiple sclerosis: A retrospective chart
(2nd ed.). New York: Basic Books, Inc. review. Multiple Sclerosis, 12(6), 775–781.
Lynch, S. G., Parmenter, B. A., & Denney, D. R. Nocentini, U., Pasqualetti, P., Bonavita, S.,
(2005). The association between cognitive impair- Buccafusca, M., De Caro, M. F., Farina, D., et al.
ment and physical disability in multiple sclerosis. (2006). Cognitive dysfunction in patients with
Multiple Sclerosis, 11(4), 469–476. relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis. Multiple
Maor, Y., Olmer, L., & Mozes, B. (2001). The rela- Sclerosis, 12(1), 77–87.
tion between objective and subjective impairment Noseworthy, J. H., Lucchinetti, C., Rodriguez, M.,
in cognitive function among multiple sclerosis & Weinshenker, B. G. (2000). Multiple sclerosis.
patients: The role of depression. Multiple Sclerosis, The New England Journal of Medicine, 343(13),
7(2), 131–135. 938–952.
The Neuropsychology of Multiple Sclerosis 303

Ozakbas, S., Ormeci, B., Akdede, B. B., Alptekin, Pujol, J., Bello, J., Deus, J., Cardoner, N., Martí-
K., & Idiman, E. (2004). Utilization of the audi- Vilalta, J. L., & Capdevila, A. (2000). Beck
tory consonant trigram test to screen for cog- Depression Inventory factors related to demyelin-
nitive impairment in patients with multiple ating lesions of the left arcuate fasciculus region.
sclerosis: Comparison with the Paced Auditory Psychiatry Research, 99(3), 151–159.
Serial Addition Test. Multiple Sclerosis, 10(6), Randolph, C., Gold, J. M., Carpenter, C. J., Goldberg, T.
686–689. E., & Weinberger, D. R. (1992). Release from proac-
Parmenter, B. A., Shucard, J. L., & Shucard, D. W. tive interference: Determinants of performance and
(2007a). Information processing deficits in multi- neuropsychological correlates. Journal of Clinical
ple sclerosis: A matter of complexity. Journal of the and Experimental Neuropsychology, 14(5), 785–800.
International Neuropsychological Society, 13(3), Randolph, J. J., Arnett, P. A., & Freske, P. (2004).
417–423. Metamemory in multiple sclerosis: Exploring
Parmenter, B. A., Zivadinov, R., Kerenyi, L., Gavett, affective and executive contributors. Archives of
R., Weinstock-Guttman, B., Dwyer, M. G., et al. Clinical Neuropsychology, 19(2), 259–279.
(2007b). Validity of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Randolph, J. J., Arnett, P. A., & Higginson, C. I. (2001).
and Delis–Kaplan Executive Function System Metamemory and tested cognitive functioning in
(DKEFS) sorting tests in multiple sclerosis. Journal multiple sclerosis. The Clinical Neuropsychologist,
of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 15(3), 357–368.
29(2), 215–223. Rao, S. M., Grafman, J., DiGiulio, D., Mittenberg,
Parvizi, J., Anderson, S. W., Martin, C. O., Damasio, W., Bernardin, L., Leo, G. J., et al. (1993). Memory
H., & Damasio, A. R. (2001). Pathological laugh- dysfunction in multiple sclerosis: Its relation to
ter and crying: A link to the cerebellum. Brain, working memory, semantic encoding, and implicit
124(9), 1708–1719. learning. Neuropsychology, 7(3), 364–374.
Paul, R. H., Blanco, C. R., Hames, K. A., & Beatty, Rao, S. M., Hammeke, T. A., McQuillen, M. P.,
W. W. (1997). Autobiographical memory in Khatri, B. O., & Lloyd, D. (1984). Memory distur-
multiple sclerosis. Journal of the International bance in chronic progressive multiple sclerosis.
Neuropsychological Society, 3(3), 246–251. Archives of Neurology, 41(6), 625–631.
Penner, I. K., Kappos, L., Rausch, M., Opwis, K., & Rao, S. M., Leo, G. J., Bernardin, L., & Unverzagt,
Radu, E. W. (2006). Therapy-induced plasticity of F. (1991a). Cognitive dysfunction in multiple
cognitive functions in MS patients: Insights from sclerosis. I. Frequency, patterns, and prediction.
fMRI. Journal of Physiology, 99(4), 455–462. Neurology, 41(5), 685–691.
Penner, I. K., Opwis, K., & Kappos, L. (2007). Rao, S. M., Leo, G. J., Ellington, L., Nauertz, T.,
Relation between functional brain imaging, cog- Bernardin, L., & Unverzagt, F. (1991b). Cognitive
nitive impairment and cognitive rehabilitation dysfunction in multiple sclerosis. II. Impact on
in patients with multiple sclerosis. Journal of employment and social functioning. Neurology,
Neurology, 254(Suppl 2), II53–II57. 41(5), 692–696.
Pierson, S. H., & Griffith, N. (2006). Treatment Rao, S. M., Leo, G. J., Haughton, V. M., St Aubin-
of cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Faubert, P., & Bernardin, L. (1989a). Correlation
Behavioural Neurology, 17, 53–67. of magnetic resonance imaging with neuropsy-
Poser, C. M., Paty, D. W., Scheinberg, L., McDonald, chological testing in multiple sclerosis. Neurology,
W. I., Davis, F. A., Ebers, G. C., et al. (1983). 39(2), 161–166.
New diagnostic criteria for multiple sclero- Rao, S. M., Leo, G. J., & St. Aubin-Faubert, P. (1989b).
sis: Guidelines for research protocols. Annals of On the nature of memory disturbance in multi-
Neurology, 13, 227–231. ple sclerosis. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Postman, L., & Underwood, B. J. (1973). Critical Neuropsychology, 11(5), 699–712.
issues in interference theory. Memory and Rao, S. M., St. Aubin-Faubert, P., & Leo, G. J.
Cognition, 1(1), 19–40. (1989c). Information processing speed in patients
Prakash, R.S., Snook E.M., Lewis, J.M., Motl, R.W., with multiple sclerosis. Journal of Clinical and
& Kramer, A.F. (2008). Cognitive impairments Experimental Neuropsychology, 11(4), 471–477.
in relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis: A meta- Rendell, P. G., Jensen, F., & Henry, J. D. (2007).
analysis. Multiple Sclerosis, 14(9), 1250–1261. Prospective memory in multiple sclerosis. Journal
Pryse-Phillips, W., & Sloka, S. (2006). Etiopathogenesis of the International Neuropsychological Society,
and epidemiology: Clues to etiology. In S. D. Cook 13(3), 410–416.
(Ed.), Handbook of multiple sclerosis (4th ed., pp. Rinker II, J. R., Naismith, R. T., & Cross, A. H.
1–40). New York: Taylor and Francis. (2006). Multiple sclerosis: An autoimmune disease
304 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

of the central nervous system? In S. D. Cook (Ed.), A longitudinal study. Archives of Neurology, 58(1),
Handbook of multiple sclerosis (4th ed., pp. 95–112). 115–121.
New York: Taylor and Francis. Squire, L. R. (1982). Comparisons between forms of
Rocca, M. A., & Filippi, M. (2007). Functional MRI amnesia: Some deficits are unique to Korsakoff ’s
in multiple sclerosis. Journal of Neuroimaging, syndrome. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
17(Suppl 1), 36S–41S. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 8(6), 560–571.
Rovaris, M., Filippi, M., Minicucci, L., Iannucci, G., Staffen, W., Mair, A., Zauner, H., Unterrainer, J.,
Santuccio, G., Possa, F., et al. (2000). Cortical/ Niederhofer, H., Kutzelnigg, A., et al. (2002).
subcortical disease burden and cognitive impair- Cognitive function and fMRI in patients with
ment in patients with multiple sclerosis. American multiple sclerosis: Evidence for compensatory
Journal of Neuroradiology, 21(2), 402–408. cortical activation during an attention task. Brain,
Ruchkin, D. S., Grafman, J., Krauss, G. L., Johnson 125(6), 1275–1282.
Jr., R., Canoune, H., & Ritter, W. (1994). Event- Sternberg, S. (1969). Memory-scanning: Mental pro-
related brain potential evidence for a verbal work- cesses revealed by reaction-time experiments.
ing memory deficit in multiple sclerosis. Brain, American Scientist, 57(4), 421–457.
117(2), 289–305. Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S., & Spreen, O. (2006).
Rutschmann, O. T., McCrory, D. C., Matchar, D. B., A compendium of neuropsychological tests:
& Immunization Panel of the Multiple Sclerosis Administration, norms, and commentary (3rd ed.).
Council for Clinical Practice Guidelines (2002). New York: Oxford University Press.
Immunization and MS: A summary of published Taylor, R. (1990). Relationships between cognitive
evidence and recommendations. Neurology, test performance and everyday cognitive diffi-
59(12), 1837–1843. culties in multiple sclerosis. The British Journal of
Ryan, L., Clark, C., Klonoff, H., Li, D., & Paty, D. Clinical Psychology, 29, 251–253.
(1996). Patterns of cognitive impairment in relaps- Thacker, E. L., Mirzaei, F., & Ascherio, A. (2006).
ing-remitting multiple sclerosis and their relation- Infectious mononucleosis and risk for multiple
ship to neuropathology on magnetic resonance sclerosis: A meta-analysis. Annals of Neurology,
images. Neuropsychology, 10(2), 176–193. 59(3), 499–503.
Sadovnick, A. D., Armstrong, H., Rice, G. P., Bulman, Thornton, A. E., & Raz, N. (1997). Memory impair-
D., Hashimoto, L., Paty, D. W., et al. (1993). A pop- ment in multiple sclerosis: A quantitative review.
ulation-based study of multiple sclerosis in twins: Neuropsychology, 11(3), 357–366.
Update. Annals of Neurology, 33(3), 281–285. Thornton, A. E., Raz, N., & Tucker, K. A. (2002).
Sadovnick, A. D., Ebers, G. C., Wilson, R. W., & Paty, Memory in multiple sclerosis: Contextual
D. W. (1992). Life expectancy in patients attend- encoding deficits. Journal of the International
ing multiple sclerosis clinics. Neurology, 42(5), Neuropsychological Society, 8(3), 395–409.
991–994. Tremlett, H. L., & Devonshire, V. A. (2006).
Sadovnick, A. D., Remick, R. A., Allen, J., Swartz, E., Does the season or month of birth influence
Yee, I. M. K., Eisen, K., et al. (1996). Depression disease progression in multiple sclerosis?
and multiple sclerosis. Neurology, 46(3), 628–632. Neuroepidemiology, 26(4), 195–198.
Scarrabelotti, M., & Carroll, M. (1999). Memory dis- van den Burg, W., van Zomeren, A. H., Minderhoud,
sociation and metamemory in multiple sclerosis. J. M., Prange, A. J., & Meijer, N. S. (1987). Cognitive
Neuropsychologia, 37(12), 1335–1350. impairment in patients with multiple sclerosis and
Schubert, D. S., & Foliart, R. H. (1993). Increased mild physical disability. Archives of Neurology,
depression in multiple sclerosis patients. A meta- 44(5), 494–501.
analysis. Psychosomatics, 34(2), 124–130. Vitkovitch, M., Bishop, S., Dancey, C., & Richards, A.
Schulz, D., Kopp, B., Kunkel, A., & Faiss, J. H. (2006). (2002). Stroop interference and negative priming in
Cognition in the early stage of multiple sclerosis. patients with multiple sclerosis. Neuropsychologia,
Journal of Neurology, 253(8), 1002–1010. 40(9), 1570–1576.
Seinela, A., Hamalainen, P., Koivisto, M., & Vukusic, S., & Confavreux, C. (2001). The natural
Ruutiainen, J. (2002). Conscious and unconscious history of multiple sclerosis. In S. D. Cook (Ed.),
uses of memory in multiple sclerosis. Journal of Handbook of multiple sclerosis (3rd ed., pp. 433–
the Neurological Sciences, 198(1), 79–85. 447). New York: Marcel Dekker Inc.
Sperling, R. A., Guttmann, C. R. G., Hohol, M. J., Wallin, M. T., Wilken, J. A., & Kane, R. (2006). Cognitive
Warfield, S. K., Jakab, M., Parente, M., et al. (2001). dysfunction in multiple sclerosis: Assessment,
Regional magnetic resonance imaging lesion bur- imaging, and risk factors. Journal of Rehabilitation
den and cognitive function in multiple sclerosis: Research and Development, 43(1), 63–72.
The Neuropsychology of Multiple Sclerosis 305

Wallin, M. T., Wilken, J. A., Turner, A. P., Williams, Zakzanis, K. K. (2000). Distinct neurocognitive
R. M., & Kane, R. (2006). Depression and multiple profi les in multiple sclerosis subtypes. Archives of
sclerosis: Review of a lethal combination. Journal Clinical Neuropsychology, 15(2), 115–136.
of Rehabilitation Research and Development, 43(1), Zivadinov, R., Sepcic, J., Nasuelli, D., De Masi, R.,
45–62. Bragadin, L. M., Tommasi, M. A., et al. (2001). A
Wickens, D. D. (1970). Encoding categories of words: longitudinal study of brain atrophy and cognitive
An empirical approach to meaning. Psychological disturbances in the early phase of relapsing-re-
Review, 77(1), 1–15. mitting multiple sclerosis. Journal of Neurology,
Wishart, H., & Sharpe, D. (1997). Neuropsychological Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 70(6), 773–780.
aspects of multiple sclerosis: A quantitative Zorzon, M., de Masi, R., Nasuelli, D., Ukmar, M.,
review. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Mucelli, R. P., Cazzato, G., et al. (2001). Depression
Neuropsychology, 19(6), 810–824. and anxiety in multiple sclerosis. A clinical and
Wishart, H. A., Saykin, A. J., McDonald, B. C., MRI study in 95 subjects. Journal of Neurology,
Mamourian, A. C., Flashman, L. A., Schuschu, 248(5), 416–421.
K. R., et al. (2004). Brain activation patterns
associated with working memory in relapsing-
remitting MS. Neurology, 62(2), 234–238.
14

Cerebrovascular Disease
Gregory G. Brown, Ronald M. Lazar, and Lisa Delano-Wood

Cerebrovascular Disease Readers might wish to consult Kales et al.


(2005) for a discussion of CVD and depression.
Although early Greek physicians described Although we do not thoroughly review studies
stroke, it was not until 1658 that Johanne Jacob of the neuropsychological effects of treatment,
Wepfer attributed the symptoms of stroke to we comment on cerebrovascular treatment in
disorders of circulation (Benton, 1991; Clarke, discussions of neuropsychological outcome.
1963). In his Observationses Anatomicae, Wepfer
was the first author to show that stroke could
The Distribution of Cerebral
be caused by occlusion of cerebral vessels or by
Vessels and Their Collaterals
subdural or intracerebral hemorrhage (Benton,
1991). Wepfer’s distinction between cerebrovas- The topography of major brain arteries and their
cular occlusive disease and cerebral hemorrhage connections determine the location of brain
remains a central clinical distinction in the dif- dysfunction caused by a cerebrovascular event.
ferential diagnosis of stroke (Wiebe-Velazquez Paired carotid and vertebral arteries bring blood
& Hachinski, 1991). As the pathophysiologi- to the brain from the heart (Figure 14–1). Each
cal basis of stroke has become more refined, common carotid artery divides into external
the disorders of the content of blood, primary and internal segments at about the upper border
blood diseases, and abnormalities of blood ves- of the thyroid cartilage (Gilroy & Meyer, 1969,
sels underlying the neuropsychological abnor- p. 478). At the level of the optic nerve, the inter-
malities of cerebrovascular disease (CVD) have nal carotid artery divides into the anterior and
become increasingly clarified. This chapter is a middle cerebral arteries. The vertebral arteries
review of CVD for neuropsychologists who wish ascend through the spinal vertebrae and join in
to do clinical work or research with patients the lower pons to form the basilar artery. At the
who have experienced stroke, transient ische- Circle of Willis at the medial base of the cere-
mic attack, ischemic white matter disease, or bral hemispheres, the basilar artery divides
vascular dementia (VaD). We limit our review to form the posterior cerebral arteries, which
to studies that highlight neuropsychological ascend to provide blood to the inferior tem-
aspects of CVD per se, and do not review stud- poral gyrus, portions of the occipital lobe, and
ies that use stroke patients to draw conclusions parts of the superior parietal lobule (Sasaki &
about localization of function. Although we Kassell, 1990). Whereas the carotid arteries and
do not thoroughly review the effects of stroke the anterior and middle cerebral arteries supply
on affective brain systems, we comment on the blood to the forebrain, the vertebrobasilar cir-
impact of CVD on emotional functioning as culation and the posterior cerebral arteries sup-
part of broader neuropsychological syndromes. ply the brain stem, the cerebellum, much of the

306
Cerebrovascular Disease 307

thalamus, the occipital lobes, and the posterior of the pattern and severity of damage in stroke
medial temporal lobes. (Nilsson et al., 1979; Toole, 1990, p. 9).
Thomas Willis (1664) was the first author
to describe the functional significance of cere-
Ischemic Stroke
brovasculature connections as collateral paths
of blood flow when a single artery is blocked Definitions of stroke require the rapid onset of
(Meyer & Hierons, 1962). One path, the Circle nonconvulsive neurological deficits (Victor &
of Willis, connects the anterior and posterior Ropper, 2001). How focal the symptoms must
cerebral arteries at the base of the brain (Figures be to be called a stroke varies among experts,
14–1 and 14–3). Additional connections result with some authorities considering sudden and
from the joining of small branches of the ante- nonconvulsive loss of consciousness a form of
rior, middle, and posterior cerebral arteries as stroke, while other authorities emphasizing the
they pass through the leptomeninges (Sasaki & focality of the neurological deficit (Victor &
Kassell, 1990). Although the external carotid Ropper, 2001; Wiebe-Velazquez & Hachinski,
artery generally provides blood to tissue exter- 1991). Ischemic stroke occurs whenever the
nal to the skull and the internal carotid artery blood flow through arterial branches is blocked
provides blood to brain tissue, the external and sufficiently to cause cell death (Victor & Ropper,
internal carotid arteries form connections at sev- 2001). Common causes of arterial occlusion are
eral points, most prominently through branches embolism—the blockage of a blood vessel by an
of the ophthalmic artery, which arises from abnormal particle circulating in the blood—and
the internal carotid artery (Osborn, 1980, pp. thrombosis, i.e. blood clots (Victor & Ropper,
78–85). Examples of these connections included 2001, p. 870). Although stroke has traditionally
the anastomoses between the anterior branch of been defined by neurological symptoms, mod-
the superficial temporal artery, a branch of the ern brain imaging has greatly improved the
external carotid system, and the supraorbital identification of ischemic infarction (see defi-
and frontal branches of the ophthalmic artery nition below). Fluid-attenuated inversion recov-
(Pick & Howden, 1977). Individual variation in ery (FLAIR) magnetic resonance (MR) images
collateral supply is an important determinant are highly T2-weighted and have shown greater

Anterior communicating A.

L. Middle cerebral A. R. Anterior cerebral A.

L. Anterior choroidal A.
R. Ophthalmic A.
L. Internal carotid A. Posterior communicating A.
L. Posterior cerebral A.
Basilar A. R. External carotid A.

L. Common carotid A.
L. Vertebral A. R. Common carotid A. R. Vertebral A.

L. Subclavian A. R. Brachiocephalic A.

Arotic arch

Figure 14–1. A schematic diagram of the major vessels supplying blood to the brain. The polygon shaped
like a baseball field at the top of the diagram is the circle of Willis. (Reproduced with permission from Brown
et al., 1986.)
308 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

sensitivity to the detection of infarction than min. Blood brings glucose and oxygen to the
conventional T2-weighted magnetic resonance brain, while dispersing the heat and metabolic
images (Shafer et al., 2006). Diff usion-weighted products of cerebral activity (Toole, 1990, pp.
images along with maps of the apparent diff u- 28–29). In natural states, the brain can toler-
sion coefficient have especially advanced the ate only a brief cessation of blood flow before
detection of tissue abnormality within the first 6 neural functioning is disrupted. Cerebral ische-
hours of cerebral ischemia (Shafer et al., 2006). mia occurs when blood flow to a brain region
is reduced sufficiently to alter neural metabo-
lism and function (Ginsberg, 1997). When the
Epidemiology
duration and severity of ischemia produces
Studies since 1990, completed primarily in permanent functional and structural changes,
Australia, New Zealand, and Europe, have found infarction occurs. In some animal stroke mod-
a median incidence of ischemic stroke of about 5 els and in human stroke, the ischemic region
cases per 1000 per year for populations 45 years typically has a gradient of viability that varies
and older (Feigin et al., 2003). The incidence of with CBF. At the core is a locus where blood
stroke is greatly influenced by age with median flow is not more than 10%–20% of controls,
incidence worldwide less than 2 cases per 1000 neural tissue becomes isoelectric and destined
per year between ages 45 and 54 and increasing to progress to infarction (Ginsberg, 1997; Welch
10-fold to 20 cases per 1000 per year for individu- & Levine, 1991). Surrounding the ischemic core
als 85 years or older (Feigin et al., 2003). The inci- is a penumbral region where CBF is 20%–40%
dence of stroke in the Ukraine—12 per 1000 per of control values. Neural tissue in the penum-
year—is among the highest worldwide (Mihalka bral region is isoelectric, and conditions fluctu-
et al., 2001). Ischemic stroke accounts for ate between those favoring and those opposing
67%–80% of all new strokes (Feigin et al., 2003). tissue viability (Astrup et al., 1981; Meyer et al.,
Comprising about 9.7% of all deaths, stroke is 1962). Although residual perfusion in the pen-
the second most common, single cause of death umbra is probably sufficient to maintain near-
worldwide behind heart disease (Paul et al., normal concentrations of molecular energy, it
2007). Stroke mortality rate is higher in devel- is insufficient to maintain normal lactate levels,
oped than underdeveloped countries (Paul et al., adequate functioning of ionic pumps, and neu-
2007). Depending on the location of the study, rotransmitter synthesis (Welch & Levine, 1991).
studies have found that ischemic stroke causes Surrounding the ischemic zone is a hyperemic
death in 12%–20% of cases within one month of region where viable neural tissue retains its nor-
the event (Feigin et al., 2003). Considering the mal electrical activity and where CBF is higher
medical treatments and lifestyle interventions than control states (Welch & Barkley, 1986).
available to manage stroke risk factors, stroke Protecting viable neurons in penumbral and
and cardiovascular disease are the most com- hyperemic regions from progressing to irrevers-
mon causes of preventable mortality and seri- ible injury is one focus of current stroke drug
ous long-term disability among middle-aged development (Fisher, 2006).
and older adults (http://www.ninds.nih.gov/ Diminished molecular energy appears to be
news_and_events/press_releases/pressrelease_ the stimulus for the cascade of events leading
may_stroke_050801.htm). to ischemic infarction; bioenergetic failure,
however, is not itself a sufficient cause of tis-
sue death. For example, brain infarction does
Pathophysiology
not develop during hypoglycemic coma despite
The brain is about 2% of total body weight, the occurrence of bioenergetic failure (Siesjo
yet receives 15%–20% of the oxygenated blood & Smith, 1997). The list of candidate causes of
pumped from the heart (Sokoloff, 1997). neuronal death following ischemia include aci-
Mean gray matter cerebral blood flow (CBF) dosis, abnormal calcium influx into nerve cells,
is approximately 80 ml/100 gm/min or about free radical production, glutamate-mediated
three to four times the CBF of white matter excitotoxicity, dysregulated lipid metabolism,
(22 ml/100 gm/min) (Fieschi & Rosiers, 1976), nitric oxide neurotoxicity, and activation of
yielding a mean CBF of about 60 ml/100 gm/ proteolytic enzymes, to name some leading
Cerebrovascular Disease 309

candidates. The primer edited by Welch et al. functioning have been frequently shown to be
(1997) can be consulted for reviews of each of more sensitive to brain dysfunction than tests of
these potential causes of ischemic neuronal specific neuropsychological domains and might
death. Zemke and colleagues provide a current be especially useful in detecting subtle cerebro-
review of the mechanisms of ischemic cell death vascular impairment (Hom, 1991). The extent to
from the viewpoint of naturally occurring pro- which this traditional view can be confirmed in
tective factors (Zemke et al., 2004). stroke groups with homogenous arteriographic
lesions remains to be tested.
Given the large variety of ways that stroke
The Behavioral Effects of
might impair cognitive functioning, how
Ischemic Stroke
should stroke patients be evaluated? Recently
Strokes often produce focal or multifocal neu- an NINDS (National Institute of Neurological
robehavioral deficits and disrupt general adap- Disorders and Stroke) and Canadian Stroke
tive neuropsychological functions (Hom, 1991; Network consensus panel recommended a bat-
Victor & Ropper, 2001). Up to 65% of patients tery of brief tests that measure executive func-
experience the new onset or worsening of tion and attention, visuospatial performance,
cognitive deficits following stroke (Donovan language and lexical retrieval, memory and
et al., 2008; Jin et al., 2006). The prevalence of learning, neuropsychiatric and depressive
poststroke cognitive impairment is especially symptoms, and premorbid status (Hachinski
high among individuals more than 75 years of et al., 2006). The consensus group recommended
age (Ballard et al., 2003). A thorough review specific tasks within each domain after review-
of the focal neuropsychological syndromes ing candidate tasks for quality of standardiza-
produced by stroke would require a discussion tion, psychometric properties (e.g., reliability
of much of the wide-ranging localization liter- and lack of ceiling and flow effects), brevity
ature. As an alternative to a broad review, we and portability, cost and ease of use, domain
refer readers to the literature on the impact specificity, cross-cultural generalizability, and
of stroke on the specific neuropsychological previous use among patients with vascular
domains of memory (Snaphaan & de Leeuw, cognitive impairment (VCI; Hachinski et al.,
2007), language (Salter et al., 2006), and visuo- 2006). After considering these test selection
perceptual function (Hildebrandt et al., 1999). criteria, the consensus working group recom-
We discuss executive functions in the following mended test batteries that vary in duration and
sections. assessment focus (see Table 14–1). The 5-minute
In a traditional view, stroke impairs perfor- battery was intended for use in epidemiologi-
mance on two types of general adaptive func- cal studies, especially those studies involving
tioning measures (Hom & Reitan, 1990). The assessment over the telephone. The 60-min-
first type is composed of indices, such as the ute battery was developed to provide reliable
Halstead Impairment Index and the Average information about the integrity of functioning
Impairment Index, that sum over scores in specific neuropsychological domains. The
obtained from separate tests or from individual 30-minute battery was designed to clinically
items. These indices measure the additive as well screen patients suspected of having vascular
as synergistic effects of impairment of discrete cognitive deficit (Hachinski et al., 2006).
functions. The second type of index, such as the Although the tests selected by the consensus
Halstead Category Test, the Trail-Making Test, group have been individually validated as sensi-
especially part B, and the Digit Symbol subtest tive measures of vascular cognitive impairment,
from the Wechsler intelligence scales, reflect the the usefulness of these tests as components
integrated, interactive behavior of several brain of an integrated test battery remains to be
systems (Reitan & Davison, 1974). Perhaps determined.
because stroke can have remote as well as focal
effects, both additive and integrative indices of
Transient Ischemia
general adaptive function are diminished by
lateralized stroke (Fujishima et al., 1974; Hom A sudden focal neurological symptom pre-
& Reitan, 1990). General measures of adaptive sumably due to vascular disease, limited to the
310 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Table 14–1. NINDS–Canadian Stroke Network Neuropsychological Test Protocols


Neuropsychological doman & Targeted battery length (minutes)
recommended tests 60 30 5

Executive/Activation
Animal Naming (semantic fluency) Recommended Recommended Supplemental
Controlled Oral Word Association Test Recommended Recommended
WAIS-III Digit Symbol Coding Recommended Recommended
Trail-Making Test: Parts A and B Recommended Supplemental Supplemental
Montreal Cognitive Assessment 1-Letter Recommended
Verbal Fluency
Learning Strategies from Hopkins Recommended
Verbal Learning Test
Simple and Choice Reaction Time Future Development
Self-Regulation and Metacognition Future Development
Language/Lexical Retrieval
Boston Naming Test (2nd ed.)a Recommended
Visuospatial
Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure—Copy Recommended
Condition
Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure— Supplemental
Memory Condition
Memory/Learning
Hopkins Verbal Learning Test—Revised Recommended Recommended
California Verbal Learning Test—2 Alternative
Boston Naming Test Recognition Future
Development
Digit Symbol Coding Incidental Supplemental
Learning
Montreal Cognitive Assessment 5-Word Recommended
Test
Neuropsychiatric/Depressive Symptoms
Neuropsychiatric Inventory— Recommended Recommended
Questionnaire Version
Center for Epidemiological Studies— Recommended Recommended
Depression Scale
Premorbid Status
Informant Questionnaire for Cognitive Recommended
Decline in the Elderlya
Mental Status
Mini-Mental State Examination Supplemental Supplemental Supplemental
Orientation
Cognitive Assessment 6-Item Orientation Recommended
Notes: See Hachinski, et al., 2006 for test references and suggested standardization sources. Remaining tests from the
Montreal Cognitive Assessment could be administered as part of the 5-minute battery. The Mini-Mental State Examination
should be administered on a different day, from the 5-minute test, or at least 1-hour later.
a
Short form.
Source: Adapted from Hachinski et al. (2006).

eye or to a brain region perfused by a specific neurological symptoms associated with TIAs
artery and resolving within 24 hours, has tra- resolve within 30–60 minutes raised concerns
ditionally been diagnosed as a transient ische- about the legitimacy of the 24-hour criterion
mic attack (TIA) (“A Classification and Outline among critics of this definition (Albers et al.,
of Cerebrovascular Diseases,” 1975). The 2002; Pessin et al., 1977). Modern imaging
long-standing observation that the majority of methods and current drug treatments for acute
Cerebrovascular Disease 311

infarction have rendered the 24-hour duration be available prior to a patient’s TIA, it is difficult
criterion obsolete (Albers et al., 2002). The cur- to dissociate the neurobehavioral effects of TIA
rent definition of TIA, advanced by the TIA from the effects of stroke-risk factors or from
Working Group, defines TIA as a brief episode the impact of previous ischemic events.
of neurological dysfunction due to retinal or Nonetheless, TIAs are not benign and require
focal brain ischemia with clinical symptoms rapid evaluation and management to prevent
usually lasting less than one hour and occur- stroke and subsequent neuropsychological mor-
ring in the absence of evidence of acute infarc- bidity (Johnston & Hill, 2004).
tion (Albers et al., 2002). If imaging studies
show characteristic signs of acute brain infarc-
Outcome of Stroke
tion, the diagnosis of stroke is made regard-
less of symptom duration (Albers et al., 2002). House et al. (1990) found in a community-based
This definition focuses clinical attention on the study that 26% of patients with first-ever stroke
investigation of the causes of ischemia rather displayed impaired mental status on the Mini-
than symptom duration. The new definition of Mental State Examination (MMSE) 1 month fol-
TIA is also compatible with neuropsychological lowing stroke. The percentage of patients with
evidence of cognitive deficits that persist after impaired mental status dropped to 21 at the
their clinical symptoms subside (Brown et al., 6-month follow-up and remained at this level
1996a). Although the TIA Working Group rec- at the 12-month evaluation as well. Tatemichi
ommended diff usion-weighted magnetic reso- and colleagues (1994) investigated memory, ori-
nance imaging to investigate structural brain entation, verbal function, visuospatial ability,
changes, a negative diff usion-weighted image abstract reasoning, and attention in a sample of
does not necessarily rule out persistent neuronal 227 patients studied 3 months following stroke.
dysfunction. Lazar and colleagues, for exam- These investigators defined cognitive failure as
ple, have observed the reemergence of resolved a score at or less than the fift h percentile of a
hemiparesis and aphasia after administration of stroke-free control group after adjusting all test
a benzodiazepine to TIA patients with negative scores for demographic factors. If cognitive
diff usion-weighted images (Lazar et al., 2003). impairment was defined as failure on 4 or more
The extent to which neuropsychological evalu- of the 17 test items, 35.2% of the stroke patients
ations provide additional information to imag- and 3.8% of controls experienced cognitive
ing studies when attempting to rule out subtle impairment. Dependent living status was more
but persistent brain dysfunction among TIA than twice as likely if cognitive impairment was
patients remains an open research question. present, even after controlling for age and degree
Patients with TIAs are at high risk of stroke in of physical impairment (Tatemichi et al., 1994).
the days and weeks following the episode. A meta- In a longitudinal study, Ballard and colleagues
analysis of 11 very well-screened studies of stroke studied 115 stroke survivors more than 74 years
risk following a TIA found that 3.5%, 8.0%, and of age who were without dementia 3 months
9.2% of TIA patients experience stroke, respec- poststroke (Ballard et al., 2003). Patients were
tively, 3, 30, and 90 days after the episode (Wu evaluated 3 months and again 15 months fol-
et al., 2007). In the three studies where patients lowing stroke with a neuropsychological battery
had face-to-face evaluations with medical per- measuring memory, attention, executive per-
sonnel three months following TIA, the risk of formance, and language (Ballard et al., 2003).
early stroke was found to be even higher: 9.9%, At the 12-month follow-up, 9% of older stroke
13.4%, and 17.3% at 2, 30, and 90 days, respec- patients significantly deteriorated in global cog-
tively (Wu et al., 2007). Half of patients receiving nition, memory, and attention. Level of expres-
neuropsychological assessments following TIAs sive language performance and vigilance at 3
have neuropsychological deficits (e.g., Bakker et months predicted general cognitive decline at
al., 2003). These deficits are usually mild and are 12 months. Delayed improvement of cognitive
often not easily recognizable as a specific neu- functioning was observed in 50% of poststroke
ropsychological syndrome (Bakker et al., 2003). patients with significant gains seen in orienta-
Because it is rare for neuropsychological data to tion, language expression, memory, abstract
312 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

thinking, perception, and executive function assessment of premorbid functioning. Instead, the
(Ballard et al., 2003). The largest improvements functional outcome panel specifically listed limb
were seen on composite measures of mental sta- parixs and the instrumental language domains,
tus, where gains among improvers were equal reading, writing, and numeric calculation as neu-
to two-thirds of the baseline standard devia- ropsychological domains useful in predicting
tion. A study of a younger stroke cohort with functional outcome (Donovan et al., 2008).
a mean age of 60 years found that about 30%
of patients who had mild cognitive impairment
Vascular Dementia
between 0 and 6 months after stroke normal-
ized their cognitive function by 12–18 months Despite the considerable degree of accuracy in
(Tham et al., 2002). A third study found delayed diagnosing Alzheimer’s disease (AD), the clin-
improvement in only 10% of stroke patients ical differentiation of vascular disease remains
(mean age, 70 years) between 3 and 15 months one of the most difficult and challenging issues
after stroke (Desmond et al., 1996). The neurobi- for neuropsychologists and neurologists in
ological processes underlying delayed cognitive clinical settings. In the sections that follow, an
impairment or delayed improvement remain overview of the neuroanatomical and neuropsy-
unknown, although correlates of global cogni- chological aspects of vascular dementia (VaD)
tive impairment following stroke include older will be provided. Additionally, the newer con-
age; low education attainment; prestroke cog- cept of VCI will be presented with an emphasis
nitive impairment; the severity, location, and on vascular risk factors, WML pathology, and
recurrence of stroke; neuroradiological features the clinical differentiation of vascular-related
such as extent of white matter lesions (WMLs) cognitive syndromes.
and medial temporal lobe atrophy; and medi-
cal comorbidities, including diabetes mellitus,
Epidemiology
myocardial infarction, atrial fibrillation, and
pneumonia (Cherubini & Senin, 2003). VaD is the second most common form of demen-
Investigators have used measures of general tia in the United States, and its prevalence is
brain function and indices of specific neurobe- expected to rise dramatically, especially as the
havioral domains to study the neuropsycholog- aging population continues to expand at unprec-
ical prediction of functional outcome following edented rates (Statistical Abstract of the United
stroke (Barker-Collo & Feigin, 2006). In samples States, 2006: The National Data Book, 2005). VaD
of patients poststroke, the MMSE total score is emerging as a major public health concern and
at admission has been found to predict motor increased attention is currently being focused on
impairment at discharge (Ozdemir et al., 2001) and this particular dementia syndrome for many rea-
to correlate highly with anosognosia (Wagner & sons, including (1) increasing prevalence of VaD,
Cushman, 1994). More specific neuropsycholog- particularly in eastern populations such as Japan,
ical domains predictive of dependent functional China, and Russia where VaD appears to be more
outcome include sustained attention (Robertson common than AD (Fratiglioni et al., 1999); (2)
et al., 1997), praxis and emotional control (Sundet greater risk for increased short-term morbidity,
et al., 1988), and memory (Goldstein et al., 2001; mortality, and institutionalization associated
MacNiel & Lichtenberg, 1997). A recent expert with VaD versus AD (Bennett, 2001); and (3)
panel has made recommendations about which indications that VaD might be more amenable
neuropsychological domains should be assessed than other dementia types to early intervention
when assessing functional outcome from stroke given that vascular risk factors such as hyper-
(Donovan et al., 2008). Like the NINDS and tension and diabetes are better established and
Canadian Stroke Network consensus panel, the are more likely to be modifiable (Bowler, 2002;
functional outcome panel recommended assess- Rockwood et al., 2003).
ment of the domains of executive, visuospatial,
language, memory, and emotional functioning.
Clinicopathological Classification
The functional outcome panel did not separate
learning from memory, as did the Stroke panel, VaD can arise from either ischemic or hemor-
nor did the outcome panel specifically list the rhagic damage to the brain and includes several
Cerebrovascular Disease 313

different subtypes (Roman, 2002). Since there conceptualization of VaD as characterized by a


are numerous neuropathologic mechanisms greater degree of frontal-subcortical dysfunc-
that can lead to VaD, clinical and neuropsycho- tion than in AD of comparable severity (Lafosse
logical sequelae are known to be heterogeneous et al., 1997; Libon et al., 1997).
in nature. Depending on several factors, includ- Current DSM-IV criteria indicate that a diag-
ing size, location, and type of cerebral dam- nosis of VaD is warranted in the context of cog-
age, common neurologic signs and symptoms nitive and functional loss in combination with
include hemiparesis, visual field deficits and evidence of stroke. However, studies have shown
visual disturbances, hemisensory loss, brain that, in contrast with AD, VaD appears to be
stem abnormalities, and sensory and/or motor more heterogeneous in origin, pathogenesis, and
symptoms. Given the different subgroupings clinical course (Erkinjuntti, 2000; Rockwood
of VaD, it has been somewhat challenging to et al., 1999). In addition, approximately 40% of
identify homogeneous populations for neuro- patients with VaD do not evidence stroke, and
psychological studies. Most research to date has only 40% of patients with VaD have such focal
focused on multi-infarct dementia and, although signs (Bowler, 2002). Meyer et al. (2002) argue
there is some overlap between the major sub- that VaD can be separated into two groups: one
groups, efforts to distinguish them according to group characterized predominantly by multi-
dementia onset have clinical merit. For exam- infarct, strategic-infarct, or intracranial hem-
ple, early identification of insidious-onset VaD orrhage, with abrupt onset (in line with the
(subcortical VaD) introduces an opportunity traditional view of VaD); and the other form
for clinical interventions such as control of vas- of VaD caused by subcortical small-vessel dis-
cular risk factors that may minimize, arrest, or ease, with an insidious onset mirroring the
even reverse cognitive deterioration (Bowler & clinical course of AD. Indeed, recent research
Hachinski, 2003). has demonstrated that subcortical small-vessel
disease appears to be an important etiology
for VaD (Chui, 2001; Erkinjuntti et al., 2000),
Diagnostic Considerations
with prevalence rates ranging from 36% to 67%
In contrast to AD—which is characterized by (Cummings, 1994; Erkinjuntti, 1987). Slow pro-
a slow, degenerative progression of cognitive gression of neuropsychological deficits in sub-
and functional capacity—VaD has been tradi- cortical small-vessel disease is thought to be
tionally thought of as having an abrupt onset linked to gradually progressive microvascular
of dementia symptoms, followed by stepwise brain changes. Specifically, it has been pos-
deterioration of cognitive performance reflect- ited that hypertensive arteriolar lipohyalinosis
ing neurological signs and symptoms consis- (cerebral microangiopathy) involving small
tent with focal brain lesions (Roman, 1999). penetrating vessels underlies neuropathologi-
VaD is thought to primarily affect subcorti- cal changes that promote the clinical syndrome
cal and frontal-lobe functions (Bowler, 2002; VaD (Chui, 2001). These neuropathological
Rockwood et al., 2003), and patients with this changes are thought to be associated with var-
dementia type have been described in the lit- ious vascular risk factors (i.e., hypertension,
erature to exhibit early executive impairment, hyperlipidemia, and diabetes), which are pre-
slowed psychomotor functioning, and frequent sent well before VaD becomes diagnosable and
depressive features (Libon et al., 2001; Naarding may cause progressive cognitive impairment
et al., 2003). Frontal-subcortical circuits have over time (Bowler & Hachinski, 2003; Meyer
been an area of recent research interest (Bigler et al., 2002).
et al., 2003; Libon et al., 2001) and the specific
circuits disrupted in VaD appear to include
Problems with the DSM-IV Vascular
the dorsolateral prefrontal neuronal circuit
Dementia Diagnosis
mediating executive functioning, the orbito-
frontal circuit mediating emotional lability, and More recently, the traditional DSM-IV diag-
the anterior cingulate circuit responsible for nosis of VaD has been challenged as problem-
motivation and initiation (Cummings, 1994). atic, and the foundations for the diagnosis are
Moreover, imaging studies lend support to the under scrutiny (Hachinski, 1994; Nolan et al.,
314 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

1998; Rockwood et al., 2000). Indeed, it has of Diseases (ICD-10), State of California
been noted that the diagnosis of VaD is clouded Alzheimer’s Disease Diagnostic and Treatment
by many false perceptions; specifically, these Centers (ADDTC), and the National Institute
assumptions are (1) that the course of VaD is for Neurological Disorders and Stroke–
chronic and progressive, (2) that a large stroke Association Internationale pour la Recherche
or many strokes must precede VaD, and (3) that et l’Enseignement en Neurosciences (NINDS-
VaDs are quite rare (Brust, 1988). Contrary to AIREN). Unfortunately, the criteria are not
current DSM-IV criteria, focal neurological interchangeable and can lead to a fivefold dif-
signs, sudden onset, stepwise progression, and ference in the frequency of classifying VaD
relationship to known stroke(s) are argued to (Chui et al., 2000; Gold et al., 2002; Wetterling
be unnecessary (Bowler & Hachinski, 2003; et al., 1996). A recent retrospective analysis
Erkinjuntti & Rockwood, 2001; Sachdev & Looi, using four classification systems for the clini-
2003). cal diagnosis of VaD indicated that classifica-
Recently, the DSM-IV criteria for VaD have tion varied widely as a function of the criteria
been criticized as “Alzheimerized” (Sachdev & used: patients diagnosed with VaD using one
Looi, 2003) and thus inappropriate since the cri- set of criteria were not similarly diagnosed
teria were based primarily on AD (Hachinksi, using other criteria (Cosentino et al., 2004). In
1994; McKhann et al., 1984). First, there is a contrast to the ICD-10, which allows for a dif-
requirement of memory impairment, which is ferentiation of vascular dementia into subtypes
restrictive when applied to VaD, since recent (i.e., VaD of acute onset, multi-infarct demen-
research has shown that memory impairment tia, subcortical VaD, and mixed or unspecified
may not be an early or most salient feature of types), most commonly employed diagnos-
this dementia type (Bowler & Hachinski, 2003; tic schemes do not allow for subtyping and
Cosentino et al., 2004; Garrett et al., 2004). require evidence of significant ischemic brain
Second, while individuals with AD typically injury on structural neuroimaging (i.e., multi-
evidence significant deficits on language and ple infarcts). It is thus argued that the problems
semantic knowledge tasks, patients with VaD inherent in the diagnosis of VaD may be asso-
rarely demonstrate any such deficits (Cosentino ciated with inadequate detection and diagnosis
et al., 2004; Garrett et al., 2004). Instead, some (Bowler & Hachinski, 2003). Indeed, the cur-
studies have shown that, in contrast to AD, VaD rent criteria may fail to identify patients with
is often characterized by dramatic impairment significant cognitive loss caused by CVD, and
on tests of executive functioning, or higher- the emphasis on dementia may underestimate
order thinking, encompassing domains such the burden of disease associated with early VaD
as cognitive flexibility, planning, and inhi- (Hachinski, 1992, 1994).
bition (Cosentino et al., 2004; Garrett et al., Failing to identify early VaD may detract
2004). Unfortunately, the application of the from the focus on prevention, which may have
“Alzheimerized” definition to VaD diagno- negative consequences with respect to treatment
sis may lead to diagnoses of VaD at a relatively since, in contrast to AD, vascular risk factors are
advanced state, or its not being diagnosed at all treatable, and strokes can be prevented (Bowler
(Bowler & Hachinski, 2003; Rockwood et al., & Hachinski, 2003; Erkinjuntti et al., 2000).
2000; Erkinjuntti et al., 2000). Such patients will likely not have the opportu-
The diagnosis of VaD is also problematic nity for secondary preventive measures to delay
because there is currently no agreed-upon or stop their progression to dementia (Bowler,
set of criteria (Chui et al., 2000; Gold et al., 1993; Bowler & Hachinski, 2003; Erkinjuntti &
2002). Several diagnostic criteria are in wide- Hachinski, 1993; Hachinski, 1991, 1992, 1994).
spread use, including cutting scores based on Calling for an overhaul of current VaD criteria,
the Hachinski Ischemic Score (HIS) and qual- Bowler (2002) fervently argued that VaD reflects
itative criteria based on the Diagnostic and an “outmoded concept” and urged for increased
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM- attention to be focused on subtler forms of vas-
IIIR, DSM-IV), International Classification cular-related impairment.
Cerebrovascular Disease 315

developed dementia over 5 years. Additionally,


Neuropsychological Functioning
Meyer et al. (2002) attempted to answer the
along the Continuum of
question of whether a mild cognitive impair-
Cerebrovascular Disease
ment (MCI) stage precedes some cases of VaD
by longitudinally following 291 volunteer sub-
Vascular Cognitive Impairment
jects over one to seven years. Subjects were
Although VaD has been the subject of increased initially screened with the MMSE and tested
clinical research (Rockwood et al., 2003), far annually with neuroimaging (MRI or CT).
less is known about the factors that contribute Follow-up of the 73 subjects who were diag-
to the development of dementia among people nosed with MCI occurred every 3–6 months,
with chronic CVD compared to AD. Research and results indicated that, of the 27 subjects who
has shown that the total burden of cognitive developed VaD within 7 years, roughly 56% had
impairment of cerebrovascular origin may be prodromal MCI. Similarly, Ballard et al. (2003)
very high. Specifically, 78% of elderly patients at showed that approximately 9% of their MCI
autopsy have evidence of CVD as do over 80% of sample developed VaD over 1 year and almost
those who are demented (Bowler & Hachinski, half of the patients developed dementia over 5
2002) and about 9 in 1000 people aged 85 years years of follow-up. Taken together, these find-
or more may develop VaD each year (Bowler & ings suggest that individuals with cognitive dif-
Hachinski, 2003). Just as subtle cognitive ficulties secondary to CVD are at increased risk
impairment may be present several years before for further cognitive decline and conversion
the clinical diagnosis of AD (Masur et al., 1994; to dementia. Indeed, it appears that MCI may
Meguro et al., 2001), such understated vascular- represent a clinically heterogeneous group of
related cognitive deficits may be evident in early individuals who are at elevated risk for demen-
VaD. The crucial question is whether this com- tia that is not solely restricted to AD. Thus, the
mon and important problem can be detected concept of VCI appears to be clinically useful
and halted before significant cognitive loss has and relevant.
occurred. Hachinski (1994) and others (Bowler,
It has been argued that vascular-related cog- 2000; Bowler et al., 1999; Devasenapathy &
nitive impairment (VCI) be used to describe the Hachinski, 2000; Garrett, et al., 2004) proposed
early stage of VaD (Bowler, 2002; Erkinjuntti & that VCI exists on a continuum that comprises
Rockwood, 2001; Hachinski, 1994). However, three primary stages: brain-at-risk (R-CVD);
currently, no universally accepted criteria have vascular cognitive impairment—no dementia
been formalized (mostly due to a relative lack of (VaCIND); and VaD. The first stage—R-CVD—
data with respect to early cognitive loss), and it consists of individuals with cardiovascular or
has been suggested that, until formal criteria are other system disease processes who are at risk
proposed, the criteria for VCI should be wide for developing CVD (with no clinically signif-
ranging and broad (Bowler & Hachinski, 2003). icant cognitive or functional impairments).
Specifically, Bowler and Hachinski (2002, 2003) VaCIND is the first hypothesized clinical stage
indicate that individuals should be considered preceding VaD (Hachinski, 1994) and is analo-
for VCI when there is any evidence of CVD (with gous to MC) (Bowler & Hachinski, 2003). Like
or without known risk factors or neuroimaging MCI, individuals with VaCIND do not have sig-
findings) in the presence of associated cogni- nificant impairment in their ability to complete
tive impairment that does not reach the level of basic activities of daily living.
dementia (Bowler & Hachinski, 2003). Chui et al. (2003) have conceptualized VCI
Using these broad criteria, preliminary data with finer gradations within the course of VaD.
appear to support the validity of this concept Their stages include brain at risk (presence of
of VCI (Ballard et al., 2003; Meyer et al., 2002; vascular risk factors); early ischemic brain injury,
Wentzel et al., 2001). For example, Wentzel et al. but clinically asymptomatic; early symptomatic,
(2001) recently reported that 50% of the sub- but prior to diagnosis (cognitive decline by neu-
jects in their sample who met criteria for VCI ropsychological testing); clinical diagnosis of
316 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

VCI without dementia (VaCIND); clinical diag- functioning) are most disturbed during the late
nosis of VaD; advanced dementia (severe VaD); phases of VaD raises the possibility that mild
and death. According to Chui et al. (2003) and changes in these cognitive functions may occur
others (Bowler & Hachinski, 2003; Inzitari et during the early stages of this dementia process.
al., 2003; Rockwood et al., 2003), detection and However, it remains unclear whether clinically
attempts at intervention do not typically occur meaningful changes are evident among patients
until individuals reach stage five or six of Chui in early stages of VaD. Furthermore, it is of
et al.’s (2003) VaD continuum, mostly due to the clinical interest to determine if the difficulties
restrictive nature of the current diagnostic crite- on measures of frontal-lobe functioning exist
ria for VaD, as discussed above. Unfortunately, in the context of preserved function in other
adequate intervention at these stages is futile cognitive domains such as memory. As noted
given that excessive cerebrovascular damage has by Garrett et al. (2004), a pattern of strengths
typically occurred at these later stages. While and weaknesses on cognitive measures may
research on vascular-related cognitive impair- prove to be a critical determinant of diagnos-
ment (VCI) is in its infancy and only a few studies tic differentiation between the various stages of
have been conducted to date, VaCIND is thought vascular-related cognitive impairment, as well
to be the best established concept for identify- as distinguishing vascular etiologies from other
ing at-risk patients with CVD (Stephens et al., dementing conditions. Thus, the identification
2004). Empirical support for the clinical concept of a pattern of neuropsychological changes
has begun to develop—particularly in Asia and associated with the early stage of VaD (i.e., Va
Europe—with a strong interest and emphasis on CIND) would greatly benefit diagnosis and
the early phases of the disease process. facilitate intervention (Garrett et al., 2004).

Outcome of Vascular
Neuropsychological Impairment in
Cognitive Impairment
Vascular Cognitive Impairment
VCI is associated with an increased risk for
A greater understanding of neuropsychologi-
adverse outcomes. Rockwood et al. (2000) found
cal impairment during the early stages of VaD
that a failure to consider VCI underestimates
might improve the early detection of neuro-
the prevalence of impairment and the associ-
psychological deficits and justify interventions
ated risk for adverse outcomes. Specifically, this
to prevent or slow progression to dementia
study compared the rates of adverse outcomes
(Bowler, 2000; Devasenapathy & Hachinski,
for older patients with (1) no cognitive impair-
2000; Garret et al., 2004). However, to date, the
ment, (2) VCI, and (3) probable AD. After
neuropsychological profi le of VaCIND has not
reassessment 5 years later, VCI was the most
received significant study. It has been shown
prevalent form of cognitive impairment among
that neurocognitive difficulties associated with
older adults aged 65–84 years. Most strikingly,
cardiac disease and mild CVD include reduced
rates of institutionalization for those with VCI
information-processing speed and reduced cog-
were similar to that of those with VaD, and the
nitive flexibility (Kilander et al., 1998; Waldstein
mortality rate for VCI patients was similar to
et al., 2001). Additionally, recent neuroimaging
that of patients with AD. Data such as these
studies conclude that the magnitude of cognitive
support the view that increased attention on
impairment among patients with mild cardio-
subtle vascular-related cognitive impairment is
vascular disease is significantly associated with
important and clinically relevant. Moreover, the
neuropathological changes secondary to vas-
criteria that require the diagnosis of dementia
cular damage (Raz et al., 2003). As will be dis-
likely underestimate the prevalence and burden
cussed in further detail, research has suggested
of vascular cognitive disease (Hachinski, 1994;
that cardiac risk factors can be associated with
Rockwood et al., 1994).
cognitive and neuropathological changes in the
absence of frank stroke or dementia (DeCarli
White Matter Lesion Pathology
et al., 2001; Swan et al., 1998).
The finding that cognitive functions medi- In 1986, Hachinski coined the term “leukoaraio-
ated by frontostriatal circuits (i.e., executive sis” to describe abnormal cerebral white matter
Cerebrovascular Disease 317

changes as seen on CT scans (Hachinski et al., they represent a pathognomonic sign for brain
1986). At about the same time, Awad, Spetzler, disease (DeCarli & Scheltens, 2002).
Hodak, Awad, and Carey (1986) characterized
these white matter changes as seen on MRI as
Vascular Cognitive Impairment and
“incidental subcortical lesions.” Initially termed
White Matter Lesions
“UBOs,” or Unidentified Bright Objects, these
changes have more recently been termed WMLs The contribution of WML to cognitive impair-
or hyperintensities. They are typically found in ment is receiving increased attention. Recent
deep cerebral areas and as cappings on the lat- research has shown that 33%–97% of cases of
eral ventricles (Drayer, 1988) and are identified VaD demonstrate extensive WML (Campbell &
as signal hyperintensities on T2- and proton den- Coffey, 2001; Pohjasvaara et al., 2000; Wetterling
sity-weighted magnetic resonance (MR) scans. et al., 1996) and less extensive WMLs are thought
The precise etiology of WML is debated since to be evident in VCI (Erkinjuntti & Rockwood,
numerous potential causes have been identified 2001; Pohjasvaara et al., 2000). Bowler and
(Brown, 2000; Chui, 2001). For example, it is Hachinski (2003) have reported that there is a
thought that WML can be caused by either arte- need for research to focus on WML in terms of
riosclerosis causing direct occlusion of small their association with subtle forms of cognitive
arteries or by partial occlusion of small arter- impairment, as well as how best to define the
ies in combination with cardiovascular failure borders between normal aging, VCI, and VaD
(orthostatic hypotension, carotid sinus sensitiv- (Bowler & Hachinski, 2003).
ity, and congestive heart failure) (Chui, 2001). In The presence of vascular risk factors or
addition, WML appear to be attributed to nor- cerebrovascular events in general may be suf-
mal aging processes, including increased fluid ficient for a diagnosis of VCI (Erkinjuntti &
in the white matter around the anterior horns Rockwood, 2001). Although current criteria
of the lateral ventricles and enlarged perivascu- require evidence of stroke for a diagnosis of
lar spaces (Munoz et al., 1993). Moreover, WML VaD, some studies show that silent infarcts and
have been posited to be caused by subclinical WMLs are relevant and may represent early
ischemia secondary to cerebral hypoperfusion cases of VaD, or VCI (Erkinjuntti & Rockwood,
(Brun, 2003). Indeed, the cerebral white mat- 2001). Indeed, it has been shown that evidence
ter is the least irrigated compartment of the of ischemic disease on neuroimaging (in the
brain, and thus it may be more vulnerable to the absence of stroke and atrophy) appear to be
effects of ischemia and hypoperfusion (Brown, associated with cognitive impairment (Bowler,
2000). Interestingly, the prefrontal brain regions 2002). In fact, some researchers (Emery et al.,
appear to be most vulnerable given lower vaso- 1994; Phillips & Mate-Kole, 1997) have argued
dilatory capacity versus white matter found in that WML represent the beginning stage of a
other brain regions (Brown, 2000). series of pathological processes that precede tis-
WML have been related to physical illnesses sue infarction and suggest the term “pre-infarct
such as diabetes mellitus (Longstreth et al., state” to describe cognitive changes in patients
2000), hypertension (de Leeuw et al., 2002), who evidence WML, regardless of whether they
stroke or myocardial infarction, atrial fibril- evidence cerebral infarcts. Current research is
lation and carotid artery atherosclerosis (de beginning to focus on more subtle lesions, and
Leeuw et al., 2000). WML reach their highest how they may be associated with neuropsycho-
prevalence in patients who have VaD (Smith logical impairment (Garrett et al., 2004; Raz
et al., 2000) and depression (Kumar et al., 2000). et al., 2003; Stephens et al., 2004). According to
There is little controversy that WML increases Sachdev and Looi (2003), more clearly defined
with the aging process (Gunning-Dixon & differentiation of VaD from AD is likely to be
Raz, 2000; Pantoni & Garcia, 1995; Schmidt greatly influenced by the extent of WML in the
et al., 1999), and a threshold effect for cogni- two groups.
tive decline has been hypothesized to be pre- In a longitudinal study of 27 individuals with
sent (Libon et al., 2008). However, the debate MCI by Wolf et al. (2000), it was demonstrated
over identified WML centers on their clini- that subjects who developed dementia after 2–3
cal significance, and it is still unclear whether years had more severe WMLs. In addition, there
318 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

was an inverse relationship between WML and cognitive loss (Garrett et al., 2004). Consistent
the degree of temporal lobe atrophy in those with deficits often found in VaD, older individ-
who progressed to dementia during the fol- uals with numerous WMLs appear to perform
low-up interval; thus, high WML severity was worse on tests of speed and visuospatial func-
associated with lesser degree of temporal lobe tion, even after controlling for age (Cosentino
atrophy and higher global cognitive perfor- et al., 2004; Garrett et al., 2004). Additionally,
mance, as measured by SIDAM (Structured while few studies have examined the distri-
Interview for the Diagnosis of Dementia of bution of WML and their effect on cognitive
Alzheimer Type). This illustrates how subcorti- function, there is some evidence that the extent
cal vascular impairment and cortical degenera- and pattern of WML is important in influenc-
tive dementia seem to differ in their course and ing cognitive performance (Raz et al., 2003).
clinical picture. Specifically, it appears likely Specifically, associated deficits appear to be
that those with greater levels of WMLs and less of a frontal-subcortical nature, affecting neu-
medial temporal lobe atrophy may progress to ropsychological functions such as speed of
VaD, whereas those with little to no WML and processing and executive functioning; this rela-
evidence of significant medial temporal lobe tionship appears to be stronger with respect
atrophy may more likely progress to AD. Thus, to deep white matter lesions (DWML) versus
WML appear to play a role in the dementia pro- periventricular (PVL) WML (Garrett et al.,
cess and may accelerate decline in individuals 2004; Raz et al., 2003), although data are
with mild cognitive impairment. preliminary.
Recent studies have noted that the neuropsy-
chological impairment sometimes seen in asso-
Neuropsychological Deficits Associated
ciation with WML is similar to other types of
with White Matter Lesions
subcortical dementing illnesses (Giovannetti-
Early neuropathological research involving Carew et al., 1997; Libon et al., 2001). Libon et al.
WMLs was quite mixed. While some studies (1997) found a dissociation between tests of ver-
pointed to a relationship between such lesions bal declarative memory and executive systems
and neuropsychological functioning (Almkvist functioning; patients with VaD obtained lower
et al., 1992; Breteler et al., 1994a, 1994b), many scores and made more perseverative errors on
found no relationship after controlling for age executive systems tests, but exhibited less for-
(Hunt et al., 1989; Tupler et al., 1992). It has getting, obtained higher tests scores on delayed
been suggested that earlier research was ham- free/cued recall and recognition test condi-
pered by many factors, including small sample tions, and made fewer intrusion errors than
sizes (Mirsen et al., 1991), the use of inadequate AD patients. This study also found that the pat-
and insensitive tests that are rarely used with tern of neuropsychological impairment seen in
older populations (Mirsen et al., 1991), differ- dementia associated with subcortical WML was
ences in subject settings (Hershey et al., 1987; more similar to other subcortical dementing
Rao et al., 1989), and the use of CT scans, which illnesses, such as Parkinson’s disease (PD) and
are not very sensitive to WML (Diaz et al., Huntington’s disease, than cortical dementing
1991; Hershey et al., 1987; Miyao et al., 1992). illnesses such as AD. Similar findings have been
However, not all of the negative studies can be reported by Doody et al. (1998).
dismissed as resulting from these limitations In a more recent study by Libon and colleagues
(See Hunt et al.’s elegant 1989 neuropsychologi- (2001), subjects were assessed with regards to
cal and magnetic resonance imaging study). WML quantity independent of clinical diagno-
Contrary to previous studies, more recent sis. Individuals with a diagnosis of either vas-
reports have demonstrated that WMLs appear cular or Alzheimer’s dementia were placed in
to disrupt cognitive functioning and thus may groups representing either minimal-mild WML
have clinical significance (Breteler et al., 1994a, or significant WML. After being compared to
1994b; Garrett et al., 2004). Indeed, although patients with PD, it was found that neuropsy-
the effects are often subtle and mild, there has chological testing failed to distinguish between
been a link between early VCI and WMLs and those with PD and those with significant WML;
Cerebrovascular Disease 319

additionally, those with significant WML dem- Study Group, 1999; Stapf et al., 2001) with a
onstrated disproportionate impairment on tests missing capillary bed, and instead are con-
of visuoconstruction and executive systems nected by fistulas and characterized by shunt-
functioning, whereas patients with little WML ing of blood from artery to vein (Bambakidis
showed greater impairment on tests of declar- et al., 2001; Klimo et al., 2007; O’Brien et al.,
ative memory and semantic knowledge. These 2006). This shunting mechanism causes hyper-
findings point to a pattern of cognitive impair- tension within the AVM and in the draining
ment associated with significant WML as dis- vein (The Arteriovenous Malformation Study
tinctly different when compared to AD. Group, 1999; Iwama et al., 2002; Loring, 1999)
A cautionary observation about these more and hypotension in the surrounding and feed-
recent studies is that they typically have been ing vessels (The Arteriovenous Malformation
performed by researchers working in stroke Study Group, 1999), with little apparent clini-
clinics or in stroke centers. Consequently, the cal effect (Diehl et al., 1994; Fogarty-Mack et al.,
base rates for CVD in these settings will be 1996; Mast et al., 1995; Murphy, 1954).
greater than in the general population. AVMs are often asymptomatic and go unde-
tected unless there is a clinical event (such as
hemorrhage or seizure). Unlike other brain
Cerebrovascular Anomalies
lesions, the AVM itself often does not cause cog-
The neuropsychological literature discussed nitive dysfunction. This phenomenon has been
above primarily involved patients with brain explained by Lazar and his colleagues (Lazar
ischemia. This section focuses on the neuro- et al., 1997, 2000), who proposed that brain
psychological correlates of brain hemorrhage reorganization was due to the chronic nature of
associated with vascular anomalies, especially the AVM. Rather, it is usually a hemorrhage that
cerebral aneurysms and brain arteriovenous is responsible for functional/cognitive changes
malformations (AVMs). seen in patients with AVM.

Brain Arteriovenous Malformations Neuropathology and Pathophysiology


AVMs of the brain are lesions composed of The central point of abnormal development in
coiled batches of blood vessels located in any the AVM is the nidus (Loring, 1999), which is the
part of the brain (cortical, subcortical, dural, densest region of the arteries and veins. The feed-
or brain stem) (see Figure 14–2). They are made ing arteries can include branches off the main
up of a complex tangle of abnormal veins and cerebral arteries (e.g., MCA, PCA, ACA), carotid
arteries (The Arteriovenous Malformation or vertebral arteries, or the choroidal arteries

A B C

Figure 14–2. Cerebral angiogram of an arteriovenous malformation. (a) Early arterial phase (b) Arterial
phase (c) Venous phase (Reprinted with permission from Brown, G. G., Spicer, K. B., and Malik, G., 1991.)
320 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

from the subcortical regions (The Arteriovenous Hemorrhage related to AVMs account for
Malformation Study Group, 1999). Both paren- approximately 1%–2% of all strokes (Furlan et
chymal and dural AVMs have the potential to al., 1979; Gross et al., 1984; Hashimoto et al.,
cause a more focal deficit (similar to that seen in 2004; Perret & Nishioka, 1966), and intracranial
stroke) while dural AVMs have also been shown hemorrhage (ICH) is the most frequent symp-
to cause a more global dementia-like syndrome tom of AVM, (Brown et al., 1990; Graf et al.,
(Festa et al., 2004; Hurst et al., 1998; Ito et al., 1983; Ondra et al., 1990).
1995; Matsuda et al., 1999; Tanaka et al., 1999). Nonhemorrhagic seizures co-occur in
Subcortical (deep) AVMs have been associ- 16%–53% of the AVM population (The
ated with deficits such as neglect and memory Arteriovenous Malformation Study Group,
disturbance (Buklina, 2001, 2002). 1999; Hofmeister et al., 2000) and are the sec-
ond most common presentation. The Columbia
AVM Database demonstrated 49% of AVM-
Demographics and Epidemiology
related seizure activity to be generalized ton-
AVMs are an uncommon vascular phenom- ic-clonic, 22% are focal, 22% are focal with
enon occurring in approximately 4.3% of the secondary generalization, 4% are complex par-
population based on 4530 consecutive autopsies tial, and 4% are not classified (Choi & Mohr,
(McCormick & Rosenfield, 1973). Pathology 2005; Hofmeister et al., 2000). Most epilepto-
data have shown that approximately 12% of genic AVMs are superficial and supratento-
AVMs are symptomatic (Hashimoto et al., 2004; rial, and are fed by the middle cerebral artery
McCormick, 1978), and researchers report that (Turjman et al., 1995).
approximately 0.1%–1% of the population
will have a symptomatic AVM annually (The
Neurological/Neuropsychological
Arteriovenous Malformation Study Group,
Deficits
1999; Brown et al., 1996b; Hofmeister et al., 2000;
Mohr et al., 2004; Redekop et al., 1998; Singer Neurological deficit is reported in the AVM pop-
et al., 2006). Females account for 45%–51% of ulation with varying frequencies (1.3%–48%),
AVM cases (Hofmeister et al., 2000). The mean with reversible deficits significantly more
age of diagnosis is 31.2 years, with significant common (8%) than persistent deficits (7%)
variability in reporting across clinical sites. It (Hofmeister et al., 2000; Mast et al., 1995; Wenz
was reported that 69% to 74% of patients had et al., 1998). Some researchers have demon-
AVM location in eloquent (functional) brain strated significant deficits in neuropsycholog-
regions and the majority (52%–59%) showed ical functioning of patients, some studies of
deep venous drainage (Hofmeister et al., 2000). which unfortunately combine ruptured and
As for AVM size, 38% were small (<3 cm), 55% unruptured AVMs (Baker et al., 2004; Mahalick
were medium (3–6 cm), and 7% were large et al., 1993; Marshall et al., 2003; Steinvorth et
AVMs (>6 cm) (Hofmeister et al., 2000). al., 2002; Wenz et al., 1998). Wenz et al. found
While AVMs are considered to be congeni- that AVM patients (both with and without hem-
tal or developmental, there are currently no in orrhage) demonstrated below-normal perfor-
utero reports of AVM, suggesting that it may mance on tests of general IQ (24% of patients),
not be due to embryonic vessel development as attention (34% of patients), and memory (48%
once theorized (Stapf et al., 2001). Rather, they of patients). Mahalick et al. also demonstrated
are likely formed during the late fetal or imme- significantly lower performance on tests of neu-
diate postpartum periods (Stapf et al., 2001), ropsychological functioning for AVM patients
although the mechanism is still unknown. as compared to normals, but again did not
Most researchers classify AVM clinical presen- covary for prior hemorrhage. In another study
tation into two major categories, hemorrhagic combining ruptured and unruptured AVM,
and nonhemorrhagic, which can present with it was found that AVM patients were again
identical symptoms (e.g., headache, seizure, or significantly below normal on tests of intel-
neurological deficit) with one significant differ- ligence, memory, and attention (Steinvorth
entiation: a bleed (Stapf et al., 2006). et al., 2002).
Cerebrovascular Disease 321

Researchers have demonstrated improvement malformations and account for 1% of all strokes
in neuropsychological functioning postsurgery (Festa et al., 2004; Hurst et al., 1998; Kurl et al.,
(Malik et al., 1996; Wenz et al., 1998). Cognitive 1996; Newton & Cronqvist, 1969). Dural AV fis-
improvements after AVM treatment have been tulas (while located in the dura and not directly
attributed to improved CBF and reduction of associated with eloquent cortex) have been found
the “steal effect” (Malik et al., 1996; Steinvorth to cause focal deficits, such as Wernicke’s apha-
et al., 2002; Wenz et al., 1998). The steal effect sia and transient right hemiparesis, identical to
refers to the assumption that shunting and symptoms caused by a focal stroke (Festa et al.,
hypertension through the AVM decreases cere- 2004). Unlike AVMs located within the paren-
bral perfusion, thus causing cerebral ischemia chyma, which are not likely to cause global neu-
and ultimately neurological deficits (Mast et al., ropsychological dysfunction, dural AV fistulas
1995). Iwama et al. (2002) demonstrated that have also been demonstrated to cause a dementia
intracranial steal and venous hypertension, syndrome, which presents like encephalopathy
and not decreased neurological activity or mass (Hurst et al., 1998; Matsuda et al., 1999; Tanaka
effect, are responsible for the hemodynamic et al., 1999). These authors reported patients
changes seen in high-flow AVMs. with angiographically confirmed dural AV fis-
Other researchers disagree with the “steal” tulas who presented with global cognitive dys-
hypothesis (Mast et al., 1995; Stabell & Nornes, function, including progressive memory decline,
1994). Stabell and Nornes (1994) reported that slowed mentation, low initiation, and almost all
AVM patients performed the same as normal patients reported focal headaches. This demen-
controls on cognitive assessment. While some tia-like syndrome may be reversible, as indicated
significant improvement was observed, Stabell by multiple case reports on patients with dural
and Nornes dispute the “steal” hypothesis as AVM dementia that reversed after treatment of
an explanation for cognitive improvement after the fistula (Hurst et al., 1998; Tanaka et al., 1999;
AVM surgery. Mast et al. (1995) were unable to Zeidman et al., 1995).
replicate the “steal effect” using their prospec-
tive database. They demonstrated, for example,
Functional Imaging
that AVM patients with chronic cerebral hypo-
tension in Wernicke’s area did not have any Functional MRI measures regional CBF and
functional cognitive impairment. In addition, has been used with AVM patients to study brain
they used positron emission tomography (PET) reorganization and help with presurgical plan-
to study 14 AVM patients and demonstrated ning (Cannestra et al., 2004). Cannestra et al.
that while these patients did have hypoperfu- found that functional magnetic resonance imag-
sion in surrounding tissue, they did not have ing (fMRI) testing was sensitive when identify-
any parenchymal volume loss and metabolism ing eloquent language regions in patients with
was normal (Mast et al., 1995). Developmental left perisylvian AVMs. They were able to catego-
learning disorders have been found in 66% of rize the patients into three surgical risk groups
adults with AVMs (Lazar et al., 1999). In this from fMRI alone and demonstrated positive
study, AVM patients reported four times the surgical outcomes from their classification sys-
rate of learning disability than the normal pop- tem. They reported that 75% of patients avoided
ulation (17%). Lazar et al. reported that perhaps awake, invasive brain mapping by using fMRI.
these disorders of higher intellectual function- In 25% of patients, however, they were not able
ing (e.g., learning) may serve as a marker for to successfully determine operative risk, and
subtle developmental cerebral dysfunction in electrocortical stimulation mapping (ESM) was
AVM patients. required. In addition, for some patients, ESM
confirmed nidus eloquence, which was not ini-
tially detected on fMRI.
Related Vascular Anomalies
The utility of fMRI in studying AVM has
Dural arteriovenous fistulas consist of shunts received mixed reviews. Hemodynamic abnor-
that are located within the dural layer, making malities in AVMs make measuring regional
up approximately 10%–15% of cerebrovascular CBF on fMRI problematic (Alkadhi et al., 2000;
322 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Cannestra et al., 2004; Lehericy et al., 2002). injects a short-acting anesthetic (usually amo-
Therefore, fMRI should be followed up with barbital or a combination of amobarbital and
superselective Wada testing or electrocortical lidocaine) that lasts for approximately three to
mapping stimulation for AVMs with significant four minutes. During the testing period, a neu-
blood flow disruptions or when eloquent cortex ropsychologist (who has previously collected
is thought to be bordering the AVM (Cannestra baseline neurocognitive data for this particular
et al., 2004; Lehericy et al., 2002). patient) performs tests of cognitive functioning
typically associated with the brain region sup-
plied by the feeding vessel. The neuroradiolo-
Treatment and Prognosis
gist will use the results of Wada testing to help
Mahalick et al. (1993) reported that surgical determine the course of treatment. If a deficit
excision of the AVM resulted in significant is apparent during Wada testing, the radiologist
improvements in tasks involving short- and may choose to embolize closer to the AVM nidus
long-term verbal memory, long-term visual- to diminish neurocognitive damage, or perhaps
spatial memory, verbal learning, verbal and decide not to embolize at all. A negative Wada
nonverbal intelligence. Overall, they found 60% test result allows the neuroradiologist to embo-
of AVM patients performed in the normal range lize the feeding vessel with diminished risk for
on neuropsychological examination postsur- significant neurological damage. Evaluation for
gery (Mahalick et al., 1993). cognitive function during superselective Wada
is typically adapted from well-known neuropsy-
chological tests (i.e., Boston Diagnostic Aphasia
Superselective Wada Testing and
Tests, Wechsler Memory Scale) with well-
Electrocortical Stimulation Mapping
established norms (Fitzsimmons et al., 2003;
Superselective Wada testing and electrocorti- Goodglass & Kaplan, 1983; Wechsler, 1987).
cal stimulation mapping are in vivo procedures Another in vivo procedure that can be used
used to help interventional neuroradiologists to determine eloquence of surrounding cortex
and neurosurgeons to identify eloquent cortex is electrocortical stimulation mapping (EMS).
that may be surrounding the AVM, and helps to Electrocortical stimulation mapping is per-
predict neurological and cognitive changes that formed intraoperatively during an awake cra-
would occur as an adverse effect of treatment. niotomy. While the patient performs different
Currently, the Spetzler-Martin grading system tasks, the brain is stimulated and essential ver-
aids surgeons by establishing a risk model by sus nonessential cortical sites can be mapped
assessing the size and location of the AVM. The out (Cannestra et al., 2004).
treating physician traditionally assumed elo- Cannestra et al. (2004) compared fMRI,
quence from the location of the AVM (i.e., if the superselective Wada testing, and EMS for effi-
AVM is located in Broca’s area, the surgeon will cacy as presurgical tools to identify eloquent
assume this area is crucial for expressive lan- cortex surrounding the AVM. They reported
guage). However, because brain reorganization that superselective Wada testing was not sen-
has now been associated with AVM (Lazar et al., sitive in identifying eloquent cortex and that
1997, 2000), the grading system without empir- fMRI in combination with EMS was required to
ical data from in vivo testing of eloquence is felt adequately predict surgical risk.
no longer capable of establishing true treatment
risk (Lazar, 2001).
Cerebral Aneurysm
Based on techniques used in epilepsy, superse-
lective Wada testing is a clinical procedure that
Pathophysiology and Epidemiology
is used to help the interventional neuroradiol-
ogist determine prior to embolization whether Intracranial aneurysms are bulges in the arte-
a feeding artery to an AVM also supplies blood rial wall, with most in the brain being thin-
necessary for eloquent function in nearby brain walled sacs arising out of the Circle of Willis
areas. With the microcatheter in place for deliv- or its major branches (Vates et al., 2004). Those
ery of embolic material, the neuroradiologist that arise from defects in the media of the blood
Cerebrovascular Disease 323

vessel wall are divided into those which are sac- approximately 5%–10% of all strokes (Adams &
cular (berry shaped and the most common) or Davis, 2004), with most unruptured aneurysms
fusiform in shape; other classifications include detected incidentally and a small proportion
those that come about from any infectious found after seizures or III-nerve palsy. It has been
(mycotic), traumatic, or neoplastic etiology estimated, largely on the basis of autopsy find-
(Mohr & Gautier, 1995). Because of their weak- ings, that the prevalence of intracranial aneurysm
ened state, the walls of these vascular anomalies is about 2% of the general population (Hassler,
are vulnerable to rupture, producing spontane- 1961). In the United States, the most common
ous subarachnoid hemorrhage (SAH), which is age period for aneurysmal rupture is between 40
the predominant reason that aneurysms come and 60 years old (Kassell & Torner, 1982), with
to clinical attention. occurrence in children and adolescents consid-
Cerebral aneurysms are the fourth leading ered rare. Although SAH can present with dif-
cause of a cerebrovascular event, accounting for ferent symptoms, it is the presence of the “worst

Anterior Cerebral Artery

Anterior Communicating Artery


Aneurysm

Posterior Communicating Artery

Posterior Cerebral Artery

Superior Cerebellar Artery

Basilar Artery

Vertebral
Arteries

Figure 14–3. Arteries at the base of the brain. The anterior communicating artery and an ACoA aneurysm
are indicated by black arrows. The arteries composing the circle of Willis are also seen (see text). (Adapted
courtesy of Hirano, 1988, p. 29, Fig. 67.)
324 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

headache of my life” that frequently occurs, the posterior circulation, basilar tip aneurysms
along with nausea and vomiting. Among the are the most common.
most common clinical tools for rating syndrome
severity are the Hunt and Hess Scale (Hunt &
Neuropsychological Findings
Hess, 1968) and Fisher Grade (Fisher et al., 1980).
SAH is associated with a significant mortality Improvements in treatment of ruptured aneu-
rate so that 12% have sudden death, 40% die in rysms have improved survival so that qual-
the first week after hospitalization (Huang & ity-of-life concerns have gained increasing
van Gelder, 2002), and 50% in the first 6 months relevance in the assessment of patient outcomes.
(Schievink, 1997). Risk factors associated with It is now well accepted that despite improve-
SAH include the size of the aneurysm, female ments in physical status, patients who survive
gender and pregnancy, smoking, and excessive SAH can suffer from significant neurobehav-
alcohol consumption (Weir, 2002). Although the ioral deficits, even with excellent outcomes as
role of sustained hypertension remains a mat- defined by the Glasgow Outcome Scale (Haug
ter of debate as a risk factor, transient increases et al., 2007). Residual deficits have been reported
in blood pressure produced by sudden exertion in patients with “good recovery” even up to 7
is considered to be the major immediate cause years after initial hospitalization (Hellawell &
of rupture (Mohr, 1984). More important, how- Pentland, 2001). Studies have largely confirmed
ever, is that first-degree relatives are seven times that aneurysmal clipping of unruptured aneu-
more likely to have a SAH than those in the gen- rysm produces few, if any, neuropsychological
eral population (Raaymakers, 1999). Because the deficits and that it is the presence of hemorrhage
presence of blood in the subarachnoid space is that appears responsible for the neurocognitive
the defining feature of SAH, a lumbar puncture sequelae in most patients (Hillis et al., 2000;
to visualize cerebrospinal was for many years the Tuffiash et al., 2003). What is not yet clear, how-
means for making a diagnosis. With the develop- ever, is the nature, extent, and time course of
ment of imaging technology, a noncontrast CT is the neuropsychological and emotional changes
currently the first step for the detection of SAH. that arise after hemorrhage and intervention.
The identification of the underlying aneurysm is There have been a wide range of impairments
still most reliability made via cerebral angiogra- associated with aneurysmal SAH. The spec-
phy, but now CT angiograms appear capable of trum of disorders measured after hemorrhage
high sensitivity in the detection of these lesions has included verbal memory deficits (DeLuca,
(Papke et al., 2007). 1993; Irle et al., 1992), visual disorders (Ogden
The deleterious effects of SAH can be made et al., 1993), and defects in information process-
worse with a number of possible complications. ing (Bellebaum et al., 2004). Frontal-lobe dysex-
Vasospasm, for example, refers to the narrow- ecutive syndromes are also commonly reported
ing of the intradural subarachnoid arteries that (Bellebaum et al., 2004). But, as has been pointed
occurs between 4 and 10 days after SAH, and out by Haug et al. (Haug et al., 2007), follow-up
can result in cerebral infarction, with ensuing of patients up to 1 year after SAH has demon-
behavioral and cognitive changes typical of strated that various cognitive functions have
those seen in ischemic stroke. In addition, there different courses of recovery. The time point of
can be hydrocephalus, seizures, recurrent hem- assessment after SAH therefore appears to be an
orrhage, electrolyte imbalance, and cardiopul- important factor in the constellation of residual
monary dysfunction (Stern et al., 2006). deficits, especially in patients with poor SAH
The International Cooperative Study on the grades (Mocco et al., 2006).
Timing of Aneurysm Surgery was instrumental One of the complicating factors in determin-
in identifying the predominant sites of intra- ing the cognitive consequences of SAH is that
cranial aneurysms (Kassell et al., 1990). Slightly the majority of patients have also received either
more than one-third of the cases had aneurysms surgical or endovascular treatment. Early stud-
located on the anterior communicating artery, ies in which patients underwent either inter-
followed by the internal carotid artery (30%) vention and were assessed at one time point
and the middle cerebral artery (19%). Within showed marginally fewer effects after coiling
Cerebrovascular Disease 325

than postclipping (e.g., Hadjivassiliou et al., proposed almost 20 years ago that local bleed-
2001). Bellebaum and his colleagues compared ing in the subarachnoid causes a more diff use,
two groups of 16 patients, assigned either to global toxic effect on the brain (Laiacona et al.,
clipping or coiling treatment (Bellebaum et al., 1989). This notion has received increasing sup-
2004). Both patient groups showed deficits in port from clinical outcome studies. Kreiter et al.
verbal and visual memory, with the clipping prospectively evaluated 113 of 248 consecutively
group demonstrating slightly greater impair- admitted nontraumatic SAH patients alive at 3
ment, especially in frontal executive function. months (Kreiter et al., 2002). Predictors of cog-
But, as pointed out by Haug et al. above, serial nitive dysfunction in two or more domains in
measurement of cognitive function can yield a multivariate model showed that global cere-
other outcomes. Individuals undergoing coil- bral edema had the highest predictive value
ing or clipping were assessed within 2 weeks of associated with residual cognitive impairment.
treatment and again at 6 months by Frazer et al. It is therefore not surprising that the severity
(Frazer et al., 2007). They found acute changes of the hemorrhage in other studies as seen on
in both groups in memory, executive function, imaging is correlated with more diff use neuro-
and information-processing speed. At fol- psychological functions, even when the precise
low-up, deficits in both groups were less severe, origin of the hemorrhage is unknown (Hutter
with residual dysfunction in memory, frontal et al., 1994). Another potential global factor,
executive dysfunction, naming skills and infor- the presence the apolipoprotein ε4 allele, which
mation-processing speed. Interestingly, there has been found important in other neurological
was no difference in functioning between the conditions, does not appear to be a global risk
treatment groups at both time points. Koivisto factor for late cognitive impairment, even 10
et al. found no differences in neuropsychologi- years after hemorrhage (Louko et al., 2006).
cal function across three measurement points,
and patients undergoing either coiling or clip-
Final Comments
ping demonstrated improvement over time
(Koivisto et al., 2000). In an earlier edition of this book, we argued
One of the presumed determining factors has that neuropsychological methods should aim
been the location of the bleed. As the most com- to bridge the anatomical, metabolic, and phys-
mon site of SAH, rupture of the anterior com- iological effects of CVD to the capacity for
municating artery has been proposed by some self-care and independent living (Brown et al.,
investigators to be associated with the triad of an 1996a). How well has recent neuropsychological
amnestic disorder, confabulation and alteration research done in relating physiology to func-
of personality, collectively labeled the “ACoA tion? The considerable literature correlating
syndrome” (Damasio et al., 1985; DeLuca & neuropsychological measures to daily function-
Diamond, 1995). Comparisons of neurocogni- ing has led to standards regarding the neuropsy-
tive outcomes of patients with ruptured aneu- chological prediction of quality of life among
rysms in this and other regions, however, have patients with CVD (e.g., Donovan et al., 2008).
not yielded consistent behavioral differences. Researchers have done much less work relating
Results of executive function tests, for exam- neuropsychological measures to physiologi-
ple, have not uniformly discriminated between cal functioning. An exception to this negative
patients with anterior and posterior SAH sites general trend has been in the presurgical eval-
(Papagno et al., 2003). uation of patients with cerebrovascular anoma-
The inconsistency of syndromal findings lies where fMRI and electrocortical stimulation
in patients with similar regions of SAH, simi- have been used to map areas of critical neuropsy-
lar behavioral manifestations in the context of chological function. Hopefully, the increasing
hemorrhage in different brain areas, and differ- availability of fMRI and its success in guiding
ing long-term outcomes in patients with com- surgical decisions among patients with vascu-
parable initial cognitive profi les does suggest lar anomalies will stimulate other investigators
the presence of other important factors that to more closely integrate neuropsychological
affect neurocognition in this setting. It was changes following a cerebrovascular event with
326 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

measurements of brain metabolism and blood is the extent to which their brevity has affected
flow. Recent work on the use of fMRI to predict their sensitivity to subtle vascular impairment
outcome in language rehabilitation following and their reliability in detecting neuropsycho-
stroke provides further evidence of the utility logical profiles at the core of focal syndromes.
that accrues from the integration of neuropsy- The expert opinions offered by consensus pan-
chological and functional brain-imaging meth- els need to be validated by classical test selection
ods (Crosson et al., 2007). methods.
The past decade has seen remarkable changes The flux of recent ideas about vascular cog-
in stroke nosology as it relates to neuropsycho- nitive disorders has highlighted the critical
logical function. Advances in cerebrovascular importance of neuropsychological assessment
nosology have been stimulated by evidence of to stroke practice and research. This chapter has
subtle neuropsychological deficits that fall short aimed to provide neuropsychologists new to the
of dementia syndromes, on the one hand, and cerebrovascular field the background needed to
progressive, usually subcortical VaDs that do continue the development of critical neuropsy-
not present as multiple strokes, on the other. The chological methodology.
current nosology, which often tries to connect
subtle forms of vascular change with dementia
References
syndromes, is likely to remain in flux as dimen-
sional models of VCI clash with syndromal Adams, H. P., & Davis, P. H. (2004). Aneurysmal sub-
models of stroke. Nonetheless, recent changes arachnoid hemorrhage. In J. P. Mohr, D. W. Choi,
to nosology have enlarged the field’s view J. C. Grotta, B. Weir and P. A. Wolf (Eds.), Stroke:
about how CVD can affect neuropsychological Pathophysiology, diagnosis, and management (4th
ed., pp. 377–396). New York: Churchill Livingston.
functioning. One implication of the changing
Albers, G. W., Caplan, L. R., Easton, J. D., Fayad, P.
perspective on CVD is that the process of diag-
B., Mohr, J. P., Saver, J. L., et al. (2002). Transient
nosing degenerative dementias might have to ischemic attack—proposal for a new definition.
change as well. For example, as more forms of New England Journal of Medicine, 347, 1713–1716.
VCI that do not involve stroke symptoms are Alkadhi, H., Kollias, S. S., Crelier, G. R., Golay, X.,
described, the vascular exclusionary criteria for Hepp-Reymond, M. C., & Valavanis, A. (2000).
AD is likely to become more difficult to meet. Plasticity of the human motor cortex in patients
What is needed is not a definition of demen- with arteriovenous malformations: A functional
tia for vascular disease that stands parallel to MR imaging study. AJNR American Journal of
the definition of degenerative dementia, but Neuroradiology, 21, 1423–1433.
a rethinking of the concept of dementia from Almkvist, O., Wahlund, L.O., Andersson-Lundman,
within a broad neuropathological framework. G., Basun, H., & Backman, L. (1992). White-
Given the flux of nosological terms currently matter hyperintensity and neuropsychological
functions in dementia and healthy aging. Archives
found in the literature, how should neurop-
of Neurology, 49, 626–632.
sychologists assess patients for the cognitive The Arteriovenous Malformation Study Group.
effects of CVD? The literature on CVD over the (1999). Arteriovenous malformations of the brain
past several decades indicates that the assess- in adults. The New England Journal of Medicine,
ment of these disorders is best performed by 340, 1812.
combining dimensional and focal-syndrome Astrup, J., Siejo, B. K., & Symon, L. (1981). Th resholds
assessment approaches. Dimensional assess- in cerebral ischema—the ischema penumbra.
ment of CVD should measure general adaptive Stroke, 12, 723–725.
functions that have an executive component. Awad, I. A., Spetzler, R. F., Hodak, J. A., Awad, C. A.,
Neuropsychological measurements that assess & Carey, R. (1986). Incidental subcortical lesions
these adaptive functions have been described identified on magnetic resonance imaging in the
elderly: Correlation with age and cerebrovascular
throughout this review. Standards for assessing
risk factors. Stroke, 17, 1084–1098.
focal neuropsychological syndromes in CVD Ballard, C., Rowan, E., Stephens, S., Kalaria, R.,
have been recently promulgated (Hachinski et & Kenny, R. A. (2003). Prospective follow-up
al., 2006). A critical question about the relatively study between 3 and 15 months after stroke:
brief batteries described in the recent standards Improvements and decline in cognitive function
Cerebrovascular Disease 327

among dementia-free stroke survivors > 75 years Bowler, J. V., & Hachinski, V. C. (2003). Vascular
of age. Stroke, 34, 2440–2444. cognitive impairment—a new concept. In J. V.
Baker, R. P., McCarter, R. J., & Porter, D. G. (2004). Bowler and V. C. Hachinski (Eds.), Vascular cogni-
Improvement in cognitive function after right tive impairment: Preventable dementia (pp. 5–14).
temporal arteriovenous malformation excision. New York: Oxford University Press.
British Journal of Neurosurgery, 18, 541–544. Breteler, M. M., van Amerongen, N. M., van Swieten,
Bakker, F. C., Klijn, C. J., Jennekens-Schinkel, A., van J. C., Claus, J. J., Grobbee, D. E., van Gijn, J., et
der Tweel, I., Tulleken, C. A., & Kappelle, L. J. (2003). al. (1994a). Cognitive correlates of ventricular
Cognitive impairment in patients with carotid enlargement and cerebral white matter lesions
artery occlusion and ipsilateral transient ischemic on magnetic resonance imaging. The Rotterdam
attacks. Journal of Neurology, 250, 1340–1347. Study. Stroke, 25, 1109–1115.
Bambakidis, N. C., Sunshine, J. L., Faulhaber, P. F., Breteler, M. M., van Swieten, J. C., Bots, M. L.,
Tarr, R. W., Selman, W. R., & Ratcheson, R. A. Grobbee, D. E., Claus, J. J., van den Hout, J. H.,
(2001). Functional evaluation of arteriovenous et al. (1994b). Cerebral white matter lesions, vas-
malformations. Neurosurgical Focus, 11(5), e2. cular risk factors, and cognitive function in a
Barker-Collo, S., & Feigin, V. (2006). The impact of population-based study: The Rotterdam Study.
neuropsychological deficits on functional stroke Neurology, 44, 1246–1252.
outcomes. Neuropsychology Review, 16, 53–64. Brown, G., G., Baird, A. D., Shatz, M. W., & Bornstein,
Bellebaum, C., Schafers, L., Schoch, B., Wanke, I., R. A. (1996a). The effects of cerebral vascular dis-
Stolke, D., Forsting, M., et al. (2004). Clipping ease on neuropsychological functioning. In I.
versus coiling: Neuropsychological follow up Grant and K. M. Adams (Eds.), Neuropsychological
after aneurysmal subarachnoid haemorrhage assessment of neuropsychiatric disorders (2nd ed.,
(SAH). Journal of Clinical and Experimental pp. 342–378). New York: Oxford University Press.
Neuropsychology, 26, 1081–1092. Brown, M. (2000). White matter changes: Possible
Bennett, D. (2001). Public health importance of cerebral blood flow abnormalities. In L. Pantoni, D.
vascular dementia and Alzheimer’s disease with Inzitari, and A. Wallin (Eds.), The matter of white
cerebrovascular disease. International Journal of matter: Clinical and pathological aspects of white
Clinical Practice, 120, 41–48. matter disease related to cognitive decline and vascu-
Benton, A. L. (1991). Cerebral vascular disease in lar dementia (pp. 299–312). Utrecht, the Netherlands:
the history of clinical neuropsychology. In R. A. Academic Pharmaceutical Publications.
Bornstein and G. G. Brown (Eds.), Neurobehavioral Brown, R. D., Jr., Wiebers, D. O., & Forbes, G. S.
aspects of cerebrovascular disease (pp. 3–13). New (1990). Unruptured intracranial aneurysms and
York: Oxford University Press. arteriovenous malformations: Frequency of intra-
Bigler, E. D., Lowry, C. M., Kerr, B., Tate, D. F., Hessel, cranial hemorrhage and relationship of lesions.
C. D., Earl, H. D., et al. (2003). Role of white matter Journal of Neurosurgery, 73, 859–863.
lesions, cerebral atrophy, and APOE on cognition Brown, R. D., Jr., Wiebers, D. O., Torner, J. C., &
in older persons with and without dementia; the O’Fallon, W. M. (1996b). Frequency of intracranial
cache county, Utah, Study of Memory and Aging. hemorrhage as a presenting symptom and subtype
Neuropsychology, 17, 339–352. analysis: A population-based study of intracra-
Bowler, J. V. (1993). Cerebral infarction and Tc nial vascular malformations in Olmsted Country,
HMPAO SPECT. London: University of London. Minnesota. Journal of Neurosurgery, 85, 29–32.
Bowler, J. V., Steenhuis, R., & Hachinski, V. (1999). Brun, A. (2003). Vascular burden of the white matter.
Conceptual background to vascular cognitive International Psychogeriatrics, 15, 53–58.
impairment. Alzheimer’s Disease and Associated Brust, J. C. (1988). Vascular dementia is overdiag-
Disorders, 13, S30–S37. nosed. Archives of Neurology, 45, 799–801.
Bowler, J. V. (2000). Criteria for vascular dementia: Buklina, S. B. (2001). Memory impairment and deep
Replacing dogma with data. Archives of Neurology, brain structures. Neuroscience and Behavioral
57, 170–171. Physiology, 31, 171–177.
Bowler, J. V. (2002). The concept of vascular cognitive Buklina, S. B. (2002). The unilateral spatial neglect
impairment. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, phenomenon in patients with arteriovenous mal-
203–204, 11–15. formations of deep brain structures. Neuroscience
Bowler, J. V., & Hachinski, V. (2002). The con- and Behavioral Physiology, 32, 555–560.
cept of vascular cognitive impairment. In T. Campbell, J. J., & Coffey, C. E. (2001). Neuropsychiatric
Erkinjuntti and S. Gauthier (Eds.), Vascular cogni- significance of subcortical hyperintensity. Journal
tive impairment (pp. 9–26). London, UK: Informa of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 13,
Healthcare. 261–288.
328 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Cannestra, A. F., Pouratian, N., Forage, J., Vascular cognitive impairment (pp. 433–457).
Bookheimer, S. Y., Martin, N. A., & Toga, A. W. London: Martin Dunitz.
(2004). Functional magnetic resonance imag- De Leeuw, F. E., de Groot, J. C., Bots, M. L.,
ing and optical imaging for dominant-hemi- Witteman, J. C., Oudkerk, M., Hofman, A., et al.
sphere perisylvian arteriovenous malformations. (2000). Carotid atherosclerosis and cerebral white
Neurosurgery, 55, 804–812; discussion 812–804. matter lesions in a population based magnetic res-
Cherubini, A., & Senin, U. (2003). Elderly stroke onance imaging study. Journal of Neurology, 247,
patient at risk for dementia: In search of a profile. 291–296.
Stroke, 34, 2445. De Leeuw, F. E., de Groot, J. C., Oudkerk, M.,
Choi, J. H., & Mohr, J. P. (2005). Brain arteriove- Witteman, J. C., Hofman, A., van Gijn, J., et al.
nous malformations in adults. Lancet Neurology, (2002). Hypertension and cerebral white matter
4, 299–308. lesions in a prospective cohort study. Brain, 125,
Chui, H. C., Mack, W., Jackson, J. E., Mungas, D., 765–772.
Reed, B. R., Tinklenberg, J., et al. (2000). Clinical DeLuca, J. (1993). Predicting neurobehavioral pat-
criteria for the diagnosis of vascular dementia: A terns following anterior communicating artery
multicenter study of comparability and interrater aneurysm. Cortex, 29, 639–647.
reliability. Archives of Neurology, 57, 191–196. DeLuca, J., & Diamond, B. J. (1995). Aneurysm of
Chui, H. (2001). Vascular dementia: A new beginning. the anterior communicating artery: A review
Shift ing focus from clinical phenotype to ischemic of neuroanatomical and neuropsychological
brain injury. Clinical Neurology, 18, 951–977. sequelae. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Chui, H., Mack, W., Varpetian, A., & Mungas, D. Neuropsychology, 17, 100–121.
(2003). Evidence-based prognosis of vascular Devasenapathy, A., & Hachinski, V. C. (2000).
dementia: Survival and rate of cognitive decline. Vascular cognitive impairment. Current Treatment
In J. V. Bowler and V. Hachinski (Eds.), Vascular Options in Neurology, 2, 61–72.
cognitive impairment; Preventable Dementia (pp. Desmond, D. W., Moroney, J. T., Sano, M., & Stern, Y.
33–39). Oxford: Oxford University Press. (1996). Recovery of cognitive function after stroke.
Chusid, J. G. (1985). Correlative neuroanatomy Stroke, 27, 1798–1803.
and functional neurology. Los Altos, CA: Lange Diaz, F., Mersky, H., Hachinski, V. C., Lee, D. H.,
Medical Publications. Boniferro, M., Wong, C. J., et al. (1991). Improved
Clarke, E. (1963). Apoplexy in early Hippocratic writ- recognition of leukoaraiosis and cognitive impair-
ings. Bulletin of the History of Medicine, 37, 301–314. ment in Alzheimer’s disease. Archives of Neurology,
Cosentino, S. A., Jefferson, A. L., Carey, M, Price, C. 48, 1022–1025.
C., Davis-Garrett, K., Swenson, R., et al. (2004). Diehl, R. R., Henkes, H., Nahser, H. C., Kuhne, D., &
The clinical diagnosis of vascular dementia: A Berlit, P. (1994). Blood flow velocity and vasomo-
comparison among four classification systems tor reactivity in patients with arteriovenous mal-
and a proposal for a new paradigm. Clinical formations. A transcranial Doppler study. Stroke,
Neuropsychologist, 18, 6–21. 25, 1574–1580.
Crosson, B., McGregor, K., Gopinath, K. S., Conway, Donovan, N. J. Kendall, D. L., Heaton, S. C., Kwon,
T. W., Benjamin, M., Chang, Y. L., et al. (2007). S., Velozo, C. A., & Duncan, P. W. (2008). Concep-
Functional MRI of language in aphasia: A review tualize functional cognition in stroke. Neuro-
of the literature and the methodological chal- rehabilitation and Neural Repair, 22, 122–135.
lenges. Neuropsychology Review, 17, 157–177. Doody, R. S., Massman, P. J., Mawad, N., & Nance, M.
Cummings, J. L. (1994). Vascular subcortical demen- (1998). Cognitive consequences of subcortical mag-
tias: Clinical aspects. Dementia, 5, 177–180. netic resonance imaging changes in Alzheimer’s
Damasio, A. R., Graff-Radford, N. R., Eslinger, P. J., disease: Comparison to small vessel ischemic vas-
Damasio, H., & Kassell, N. (1985). Amnesia follow- cular dementia. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology,
ing basal forebrain lesions. Archives of Neurology, and Behavioral Neurology, 11, 191–199.
42, 263–271. Drayer, B. P. (1988). Imaging of the aging brain. Part
DeCarli, C., Miller, B. L., Swan, G. E., Reed, T., Wolf, I. Normal findings. Radiology, 166, 785–796.
P. A., & Carmelli, D. (2001). Cerebrovascular and Emery, V. B., Gillie, E., & Ramdev, P. T. (1994).
brain morphologic correlates of mild cognitive Vascular dementia redefined. In T. E. Oxman
impairment in the National Heart, Lung, and and V. B. Emery (Eds.), The Johns Hopkins
Blood Institute Twin Study. Archives of Neurology, series in psychiatry and neuroscience. Dementia:
58, 643–647. Presentations, differential diagnosis, and nosol-
DeCarli, C., & Scheltens, P. (2002). Structural brain ogy (pp. 162–194). Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins
imaging. In T. Erkinjuntti and S. Gauthier (Eds.), University Press.
Cerebrovascular Disease 329

Erkinjuntti, T. (1987). Differential diagnosis between longitudinal investigation into cognitive outcome.
Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia: Neurosurgery, 60, 434–441; discussion 441–432.
Evaluation of some common clinical methods. Fujishima, M., Tanaka, K., Takeya, Y., & Omae, T.
Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 76, 433–442. (1974). Bilateral reduction of hemispheric blood
Erkinjuntti, T. (2000). Vascular dementia: An over- flow in patients with unilateral cerebral infarc-
view. In J. O’Brien and L. Bruns (Eds.), Dementia tion. Stroke, 5, 648–653.
(2nd ed., pp. 623–634). London, UK: Arnold Furlan, A. J., Whisnant, J. P., & Elveback, L. R. (1979).
Press. The decreasing incidence of primary intracere-
Erkinjuntti, T., & Hachinski, V. (1993). Rethinking bral hemorrhage: A population study. Annals of
vascular dementia. Cerebrovascular Disorders, 3, Neurology, 5, 367–373.
3–23. Garrett, K. D., Browndyke, J. N., Whelihan, W.,
Erkinjuntti, T., Inzitari, D., Pantoni, L., Wallin, A., Paul, R. H., DiCarlo, M., Moser, D. J., et al. (2004).
Scheltens, P., Rockwood, K., et al. (2000). Research The neuropsychological profi le of vascular cog-
criteria for subcortical vascular dementia in clini- nitive impairment—no dementia: Comparisons
cal trials. Journal of Neurological Transmission, 59, to patients at risk for cerebrovascular disease
23–30. and vascular dementia. Archives of Clinical
Erkinjuntti, T., & Rockwood, K. (2001). Vascular Neuropsychology, 19, 745–757.
cognitive impairment. Psychogeriatrics, 1, 27–38. Gilroy, J., & Meyers, J. S. (1969). Medical neurology.
Feigin, V. L. Lawes, C. M., Bennett, D. A., & London: The MacMillan Company.
Anderson, C. S. (2003). Stroke epidemiology: A Ginsberg, M. D. (1997). Animal models of global
review of population-based studies of incidence, and focal cerebral ischema. In K. M. A. Welch, L.
prevalence, and case-fatality in the late 20th cen- R. Caplan, D. J. Reis, B. K. Siejs, & B. Weir (Eds.),
tury. Lancet Neurology, 2, 43–53. Premier on cerebrovascular disease (pp. 165–181).
Festa, J. R., Lazar, R. M., Marshall, R. S., Pile- San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Spellman, J., Chong, J. Y., & Duong, H. (2004). Giovannetti-Carew, T. G., Lamar, M., Cloud, B. S.,
Dural arteriovenous fistula presents like an ische- Grossman, M., & Libon, D. J. (1997). Impairment
mic stroke. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, in category fluency in ischemic vascular dementia.
17(1), 50–53. Neuropsychology, 11, 400–412.
Fieschi, C., & Rosiers, M. D. (1976). Cerebral blood Gold, G., Bouras, C., Canuto, A., Bergallo, M.
flow measurements in stroke. In: R.W. Ross Russell F., Herrmann, F. R., Hof, P. R., et al. (2002).
(Ed.), Cerebral artery disease (pp.85–106). New Clinicopathological validation study of four sets of
York: Churchill Livingstone. clinical criteria for vascular dementia. American
Fisher, C. M., Kistler, J. P., & Davis, J. M. (1980). Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 82–87.
Relation of cerebral vasospasm to subarachnoid Goldstein, F. C., Levin, H. S., Goldman, W. P., Clark,
hemorrhage visualized by computerized tomo- A. N., & Altonen, T. K. (2001). Cognitive and neu-
graphic scanning. Neurosurgery, 6, 1–9. robehavioral functioning after mild versus moder-
Fisher, M. (2006). The ischemic penumbra: A new ate traumatic brain injury in older adults. Journal
opportunity for neuroprotection. Cerebrovascular of the International Neuropsychology Society, 7,
Diseases, 21(Suppl 2), 64–70. 373–383.
Fitzsimmons, B. F., Marshall, R. S., Pile-Spellman, Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (1983). Boston Diagnostic
J., & Lazar, R. M. (2003). Neurobehavioral differ- Aphasia Examination (BDAE). Philadelphia: Lea
ences in superselective Wada testing with amo- and Febiger.
barbital versus lidocaine. American Journal of Graf, C. J., Perret, G. E., & Torner, J. C. (1983).
Neuroradiology, 24, 1456–1460. Bleeding from cerebral arteriovenous malforma-
Fogarty-Mack, P., Pile-Spellman, J., Hacein-Bey, tions as part of their natural history. Journal of
L., Osipov, A., DeMeritt, J., Jackson, E. C., et al. Neurosurgery, 58, 331–337.
(1996). The effect of arteriovenous malformations Gross, C. R., Kase, C. S., Mohr, J. P., Cunningham, S.
on the distribution of intracerebral arterial pres- C., & Baker, W. E. (1984). Stroke in south Alabama:
sures. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 17, Incidence and diagnostic features—a population
1443–1449. based study. Stroke, 15, 249–255.
Fratiglioni, L., De Ronchi, D., & Agüero-Torres, H. Gunning-Dixon, F. M., & Raz, N. (2000). The cog-
(1999). Worldwide prevalence and incidence of nitive correlates of white matter abnormali-
dementia. Drugs and Aging, 15, 365–375. ties in normal aging: A quantitative review.
Frazer, D., Ahuja, A., Watkins, L., & Cipolotti, L. Neuropsychology, 14, 224–232.
(2007). Coiling versus clipping for the treatment Hachinski, V., Potter, P., & Merskey, H. (1986). Leuko-
of aneurysmal subarachnoid hemorrhage: A araiosis: An ancient term for a new problem.
330 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Canadian Journal of Neurological Science, 13(4 Demographic, morphological, and clinical char-
Suppl), 533–534. acteristics of 1289 patients with brain arteriove-
Hachinski, V. C. (1991). Multi-infarct dementia: nous malformation. Stroke, 31, 1307–1310.
A reappraisal. Alzheimer’s Disease Associative Hom, J. (1991). Contributions of the Halstead-Reitan
Disorders, 5, 64–68. Battery in the neuropsychological investigation of
Hachinski, V. C. (1992). Preventable senility: A call stroke. In R. A. Bornstein and G. G. Brown (Eds.),
for action against the vascular dementias. The Neurobehavioral aspects of cerebrovascular disease
Lancet, 340, 645–647. (pp. 165–181). New York: Oxford University Press.
Hachinski, V. C. (1994). Vascular dementia: A radi- Hom, J., & Reitan, R. M. (1990). Generalized cogni-
cal redefinition. Dementia, 5, 130–132. tive function after stroke. Journal of Clinical and
Hachinski, V., Iadecola, C., Petersen, R. C., Breteler, Experimental Neuropsychology, 12, 644–654.
M. M., Nyenhuis, D. L., Black, S. E., et al. (2006). House, A., Dennis, M., Warlow, C., Hawton, K., &
National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Molyneux, A. (1990). The relationship between
Stroke-Canadian Stroke Network vascular cogni- intellectual impairment and mood disorder in the
tive impairment harmonization standards. Stroke, first year after stroke. Psychological Medicine, 20,
37, 2220–2241. Erratum in: (2007). Stroke, 38, 1118. 805–814.
Hadjivassiliou, M., Tooth, C. L., Romanowski, C. A., Huang, J., & van Gelder, J. M. (2002). The probabil-
Byrne, J., Battersby, R. D., Oxbury, S., et al. (2001). ity of sudden death from rupture of intracranial
Aneurysmal SAH: Cognitive outcome and struc- aneurysms: A meta-analysis. Neurosurgery, 51,
tural damage after clipping or coiling. Neurology, 1101–1105; discussion 1105–1107.
56, 1672–1677. Hunt, A. L., Orrison, W. W., Yeo, R. A., Haaland, K.
Hashimoto, H., Iida, J., Kawaguchi, S., & Sakaki, T. Y., Rhyne, R. L., Garry, P. J., et al. (1989). Clinical
(2004). Clinical features and management of brain significance of MRI white matter lesions in the
arteriovenous malformations in elderly patients. elderly. Neurology, 39, 1470–1474.
Acta Neurochirurgica, 146, 1091–1098; discussion Hunt, W. E., & Hess, R. M. (1968). Surgical risk as
1098. related to time of intervention in the repair of
Hassler, O. (1961). Morphological studies on the large intracranial aneurysms. Journal of Neurosurgery,
cerebral arteries, with reference to the aetiology 28, 14–20.
of subarachnoid haemorrhage. Acta psychiatrica Hurst, R. W., Bagley, L. J., Galetta, S., Glosser, G.,
Scandinavica. Supplementum, 154, 1–145. Lieberman, A. P., Trojanowski, J., et al. (1998).
Haug, T., Sorteberg, A., Sorteberg, W., Lindegaard, K. Dementia resulting from dural arteriovenous fis-
F., Lundar, T., & Finset, A. (2007). Cognitive out- tulas: The pathologic findings of venous hyper-
come after aneurysmal subarachnoid hemorrhage: tensive encephalopathy. American Journal of
Time course of recovery and relationship to clin- Neuroradiology, 19, 1267–1273.
ical, radiological, and management parameters. Hutter, B. O., Gilsbach, J. M., & Kreitschmann, I.
Neurosurgery, 60, 649–656; discussion 656–647. (1994). Is there a difference in cognitive deficits
Hellawell, D. J., & Pentland, B. (2001). Relatives’ after aneurysmal subarachnoid haemorrhage and
reports of long term problems following trau- subarachnoid haemorrhage of unknown origin?
matic brain injury or subarachnoid haemorrhage. Acta Neurochirurgica, 127, 129–135.
Disability and Rehabilitation, 23, 300–305. Inzitari, D., Lamassa, M., & Pantoni, L. (2003).
Hildebrandt, H., Giesselmann, H., & Sachsenheimer, Treatment of vascular dementias. In J. V. Bowler
W. (1999). Visual search and visual target detec- and V. Hachinski (Eds.), Vascular cognitive
tion in patients with infarctions of the left or right impairment: Preventable dementia (pp. 152–176).
posterior or the right middle brain artery. Journal New York: Oxford University Press.
of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21, Irle, E., Wowra, B., Kunert, H. J., Hampl, J., & Kunze,
94–107. S. (1992). Memory disturbances following ante-
Hillis, A. E., Anderson, N., Sampath, P., & Rigamonti, rior communicating artery rupture. Annals of
D. (2000). Cognitive impairments after surgi- Neurology, 31, 473–480.
cal repair of ruptured and unruptured aneu- Ito, M., Sonokawa, T., Mishina, H., & Sato, K. (1995).
rysms. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Reversible dural arteriovenous malformation-
Psychiatry, 69, 608–615. induced venous ischemia as a cause of dementia:
Hershey, L. A., Modic, M. T., Greenough, P. G., & Treatment by surgical occlusion of draining dural
Jaffe, D. F. (1987). Magnetic resonance imaging in sinus: case report. Neurosurgery, 37, 1187–1191;
vascular dementia. Neurology, 37, 29–36. discussion 1191–1182.
Hofmeister, C., Stapf, C., Hartmann, A., Sciacca, R. Iwama, T., Hayashida, K., Takahashi, J. C., Nagata, I.,
R., Mansmann, U., TerBrugge, K., et al. (2000). & Hashimoto, N. (2002). Cerebral hemodynamics
Cerebrovascular Disease 331

and metabolism in patients with cerebral arterio- vascular dementia and Alzheimer’s disease.
venous malformations: An evaluation using pos- Neuropsychology, 11, 514–522.
itron emission tomography scanning. Journal of Laiacona, M., De Santis, A., Barbarotto, R.,
Neurosurgery, 97, 1314–1321. Basso, A., Spagnoli, D., & Capitani, E. (1989).
Jin, Y. P., Di Legge, S., Ostbye, T., Feightner, J. W., Neuropsychological follow-up of patients oper-
& Hachinski, V. (2006). The reciprocal risks of ated for aneurysms of anterior communicating
stroke and cognitive impairment in an elderly artery. Cortex, 25, 261–273.
population. Alzheimer’s and Dementia, 2, Lazar, R. M. (2001). Neuropsychological function and
171–178. brain arteriovenous malformations: Redefining
Johnston, D. C. C., & Hill, M. D. (2004). The patient eloquence as a risk for treatment. Neurosurgical
with transient cerebral ischemia: A golden oppor- Focus, 11, e4.
tunity for stroke prevention. Canadian Medical Lazar, R. M., Connaire, K., Marshall, R. S., Pile-
Association Journal, 170, 1134–1137. Spellman, J., Hacein-Bey, L., Solomon, R. A., et
Kales, H. C., Maixner, D. F., & Mellow, A. M. (2005). al. (1999). Developmental deficits in adult patients
Cerebrovascular disease and late-life depression. with arteriovenous malformations. Archives of
American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 13, Neurology, 56, 103–106.
88–98. Lazar, R. M., Fitzsimmons, B. F., Marshall, R. S.,
Kassell, N. F., & Torner, J. C. (1982). Epidemiology Mohr, J. P., & Berman, M. F. (2003). Midazolam
of intracranial aneurysms. International challenge reinduces neurological deficits after
Anesthesiology Clinics, 20, 13–17. transient ischemic attack. Stroke, 34, 794–796.
Kassell, N. F., Torner, J. C., Haley, E. C., Jr., Jane, J. Lazar, R. M., Marshall, R. S., Pile-Spellman, J.,
A., Adams, H. P., & Kongable, G. L. (1990). The Duong, H. C., Mohr, J. P., Young, W. L., et al.
International Cooperative Study on the Timing of (2000). Interhemispheric transfer of language in
Aneurysm Surgery. Part 1: Overall management patients with left frontal cerebral arteriovenous
results. Journal of Neurosurgery, 73, 18–36. malformation. Neuropsychologia, 38, 1325–1332.
Kilander, L., Andren, B., Nyman, H., Lind, L., Lazar, R. M., Marshall, R. S., Pile-Spellman, J.,
Boberg, M., & Lithell, H. (1998). Atrial fibrillation Hacein-Bey, L., Young, W. L., Mohr, J. P., et al.
is an independent determinant of low cognitive (1997). Anterior translocation of language in
function: A cross-sectional study in elderly men. patients with left cerebral arteriovenous malfor-
Stroke, 19, 1816–1820. mation. Neurology, 49, 802–808.
Klimo, P., Jr., Rao, G., & Brockmeyer, D. (2007). Lehericy, S., Biondi, A., Sourour, N., Vlaicu,
Pediatric arteriovenous malformations: A 15-year M., du Montcel, S. T., Cohen, L., et al. (2002).
experience with an emphasis on residual and Arteriovenous brain malformations: Is func-
recurrent lesions. Child’s Nervous System, 23, tional MR imaging reliable for studying language
31–37. reorganization in patients? Initial observations.
Koivisto, T., Vanninen, R., Hurskainen, H., Saari, T., Radiology, 223, 672–682.
Hernesniemi, J., & Vapalahti, M. (2000). Outcomes Libon, D. J., Bogdanoff, B., Bonavita, J., Skalina,
of early endovascular versus surgical treatment of S., Cloud, B. S., Resh, R., et al. (1997). Dementia
ruptured cerebral aneurysms. A prospective ran- associated with periventricular deep white mat-
domized study. Stroke, 31, 2369–2377. ter alterations: A subtype of subcortical dementia.
Kreiter, K. T., Copeland, D., Bernardini, G. L., Bates, Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 12, 239–250.
J. E., Peery, S., Claassen, J., et al. (2002). Predictors Libon, D. J., Bogdanoff, B., Leopold, N., Hurka,
of cognitive dysfunction after subarachnoid hem- R., Bonavita, J., Skalina, S., et al., (2001).
orrhage. Stroke, 33, 200–208. Neuropsychological profi les associated with sub-
Kumar, A., Bilker, W., Jin, Z., & Udupa, J. (2000). cortical white matter alterations and Parkinson’s
Atrophy and high intensity lesions: Complementary disease: Implications for the diagnosis of demen-
neurobiological mechanisms in late-life major tia. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 16,
depression. Neuropsychopharmacology, 22, 19–32.
264–274. Libon, D. J., Price, C. C., Giovannetti, T., Swenson, R.,
Kurl, S., Saari, T., Vanninen, R., & Hernesniemi, J. Magouirk, Bettcher, B., & Heilman, K. M. (2008).
(1996). Dural arteriovenous fistulas of superior Linking MRI hyperintensities with patterns of
sagittal sinus: Case report and review of literature. neuropsychological impairment: Evidence for a
Surgical Neurology, 45, 250–255. threshold effect. Stroke, 39, 806–813.
Lafosse, J. M., Reed, B. R., Mungas, D., Sterling, S. Longstreth, W. T., Arnold, A. M., & Manolio, T. A.
B., Wahbeh, H., & Jagust, W. J. (1997). Fluency (2000). Clinical correlates of ventricular and sul-
and memory differences between ischemic cal size on cranial magnetic resonance imaging
332 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

of 3301 elderly people. Neuroepidemiology, 19, function and frontal lobe atrophy in normal
30–42. elderly adults: Implications for dementia not as
Loring, D. W. (1999). INS Dictionary of Neuro- aging-related disorders and the reserve hypoth-
psychology. New York: Oxford University Press. esis. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 55,
Louko, A. M., Vilkki, J., & Niskakangas, T. (2006). 565–572.
ApoE genotype and cognition after subarach- Meyer, J. S., Goto, F., & Tazaki, Y. (1962). Metabolism
noid haemorrhage: A longitudinal study. Acta Following experimental cerebral embolism.
Neurology Scandavica, 114, 315–319. Journal of Neuropathology and Experimental
MacNeill, S. E., & Lichtenberg, P. A. (1997). Home Neurology, 21, 4–24.
alone: The role of cognition in return to inde- Meyer, A., & Hierons, R. (1962). Observations on the
pendent living. Archives of Physical Medicine and history of the “circle of Willis.” Medical History,
Rehabilitation, 78, 755–758. 6, 119–130.
Mahalick, D. M., Ruff, R. M., Heary, R. F., & U, H. Meyer, J. S., Xu, G., Thornby, J., Chowdhury, M. G.,
S. (1993). Preoperative versus postoperative neu- & Quach, M. (2002). Is mild cognitive impairment
ropsychological sequelae of arteriovenous malfor- prodromal for vascular dementia like Alzheimer’s
mations. Neurosurgery, 33, 563–570; discussion Disease? Stroke, 33, 1981–1985.
570–561. Mihalka, L., Smolanka, V., Bulecza, B., Mulesa, S., &
Malik, G. M., Seyfried, D. M., & Morgan, J. K. (1996). Bereczki, D. (2001). A population study of stroke
Temporal lobe arteriovenous malformations: in West Ukraine: Incidence, stroke services, and
Surgical management and outcome. Surgical 30-day case fatality. Stroke, 32, 2227–2231.
Neurology, 46, 106–114; discussion 114–105. Mirsen, T. R., Lee, D. H., Wong, C. J., Diaz, J. F., Fox,
Marshall, G. A., Jonker, B. P., Morgan, M. K., & A. J., Hachinski, V. C., et al. (1991). Clinical cor-
Taylor, A. J. (2003). Prospective study of neuropsy- relates of white-matter changes on magnetic res-
chological and psychosocial outcome following onance imaging scans of the brain. Archives of
surgical excision of intracerebral arteriovenous Neurology, 48, 1015–1021.
malformations. Journal of Clinical Neuroscience, Miyao, S., Takano, A., Teramoto, J., & Takahashi,
10, 42–47. A. (1992). Leukoaraiosis in relation to prognosis
Mast, H., Mohr, J. P., Osipov, A., Pile-Spellman, J., for patients with lacunar infarction. Stroke, 23,
Marshall, R. S., Lazar, R. M., et al. (1995). “Steal” 1434–1438.
is an unestablished mechanism for the clinical Mocco, J., Ransom, E. R., Komotar, R. J., Sergot,
presentation of cerebral arteriovenous malforma- P. B., Ostapkovich, N., Schmidt, J. M., et al.
tions. Stroke, 26, 1215–1220. (2006). Long-term domain-specific improve-
Masur, D. M., Sliwinski, M., Lipton, R. B., Blau, A. D., ment following poor grade aneurysmal sub-
& Crystal, H. A. (1994). Neuropsychological pre- arachnoid hemorrhage. Journal of Neurology, 253,
diction of dementia and the absence of dementia in 1278–1284.
healthy elderly persons. Neurology, 44, 1427–1432. Mohr, J. H., A., Mast, H., Pile-Spellman, J.,
Matsuda, S., Waragai, M., Shinotoh, H., Takahashi, Schumacher, H. C., & Stapf, C. (2004).
N., Takagi, K., & Hattori, T. (1999). Intracranial Anteriovenous Malformations and Other Vascular
dural arteriovenous fistula (DAVF) presenting Anomalies. In J. P. Mohr, D. W. Choi, J. C. Grotta,
progressive dementia and parkinsonism. Journal B. Weir, and P. A. Wolf. (Eds.), Stroke pathophys-
of the Neurological Sciences, 165, 43–47. iology, diagnosis and management (pp. 397–421).
McCormick, W. (1978). Classification, pathology, Philadelphia: Churchill Livingstone.
and natural history of the central nervous system. Mohr, J. P. (1984). Intracranial aneuryms. In J. P.
Neurological Neurosurgery, 14, 2–7. Mohr (Ed.), Manual of clinical problems in neu-
McCormick, W. F., & Rosenfield, D. B. (1973). Massive rology (1st ed., pp. 188–191). Boston: Little, Brown
brain hemorrhage: A review of 144 cases and an and Company.
examination of their causes. Stroke, 4, 946–954. Mohr, J. P., & Gautier, J. C. (1995). Subarachnoid
McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., Katzman, hemorrhage: Aneuryms and vascular malforma-
R., Price, D., & Stadlan, E. M. (1984). Clinical tions. In J. P. Mohr and J. C. Gautier (Eds.), Guide
diagnosis of Alzheimer’s Disease report of the to clinical neurology (pp. 605–618). New York:
NINCDS-ADRDA work group under the auspices Churchill Livingston.
of Department of Health and Human Services Munoz, D. G., Hastak, S. M., Harper, B., Lee, D.,
Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology, 34, & Hachinski, V. (1993). Pathologic correlates of
939–944. increased signals of the centrum ovale on mag-
Meguro, K., Shimada, M., Yamagushi, S., Ishizaki, netic resonance imaging. Archives of Neurology,
J., Ishii, H., Shimada, Y., et al. (2001). Cognitive 50, 492–497.
Cerebrovascular Disease 333

Murphy, J. (1954). Vascular tumors: Arteriovenous Paul, S. L., Srikanth, V. K., & Thrift, A. G. (2007). The
malformations of the brain. In J. P. Murphy (Ed.), large and growing burden of stroke. Current Drug
Cerebrovascular disease (pp. 242–262). Chicago: Targets, 8, 786–793.
Yearbook Medical Publishers. Perret, G., & Nishioka, H. (1966). Report on the coop-
Naarding, P., de Koning, I., dan Kooten, F., Dippel, erative study of intracranial aneurysms and sub-
D., Janzing, J., van der Mast, R., et al. (2003). arachnoid hemorrhage. Section VI. Arteriovenous
Depression in vascular dementia. International malformations. An analysis of 545 cases of cran-
Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 18, 325–330. io-cerebral arteriovenous malformations and fis-
Newton, T. H., & Cronqvist, S. (1969). Involvement tulae reported to the cooperative study. Journal of
of dural arteries in intracranial arteriovenous Neurosurgery, 25, 467–490.
malformations. Radiology, 93, 1071–1078. Pessin, M. S., Duncan, G. W., Mohr, J. P., & Poskanzer,
Nilsson, B., Cronqvist, S., & Ingvar, D. H. (1979). D. C. (1977). Clinical and angiographic features of
Regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) studies in carotid transient ischemic attacks. New England
patients to be considered for extracranial-intracra- Journal of Medicine, 296, 358–362.
nial bypass operations. In J. S. Meyer, H. Lechner, Phillips, N. A., & Mate-Kole, C. C. (1997). Cognitive
and M. Reivich (Eds.), Cerebral vascular disease 2 deficits in peripheral vascular disease: A compar-
(pp. 295–300). Amsterdam: Excerpta Medica. ison of mild stroke patients and normal controls.
Nolan, K. A., Lino, M., Seligmann, A. W., & Blass, Stroke, 28, 777–784.
J. P. (1998). Absence of vascular dementia in an Pick, T. P., & Howden, R. (Eds.). (1977). Grey’s anat-
autopsy series form a dementia clinic. Journal of omy (15th ed.). New York: Bounty Books.
the American Geriatrics Society, 46, 597–604. Pohjasvaara, T., Mantyla, R., Ylikoski, R., Kaste,
O’Brien, P., Neyastani, A., Buckley, A. R., Chang, S. M., & Erkinjuntti, T. (2000). Comparison of
D., & Legiehn, G. M. (2006). Uterine arteriove- different clinical criteria (DSM-III, ADDTC,
nous malformations: From diagnosis to treatment. ICD-10, NINDS-AIREN, DSM-IV) for the diag-
Journal of Ultrasound in Medicine, 25, 1387–1392. nosis of vascular dementia. National Institute of
Ogden, J. A., Mee, E. W., & Henning, M. (1993). A Neurological Disorders and Stroke-Association
prospective study of impairment of cognition and Internationale pour la Recherche et l’Enseignement
memory and recovery after subarachnoid hem- en Neurosciences. Stroke, 31, 2952–2957.
orrhage. Neurosurgery, 33, 572–586; discussion Raaymakers, T. W. (1999). Aneurysms in rela-
586–577. tives of patients with subarachnoid hemorrhage:
Ondra, S. L., Troupp, H., George, E. D., & Schwab, K. Frequency and risk factors. MARS Study Group.
(1990). The natural history of symptomatic arte- Magnetic Resonance Angiography in Relatives
riovenous malformations of the brain: A 24-year of patients with Subarachnoid hemorrhage.
follow-up assessment. Journal of Neurosurgery, 73, Neurology, 53, 982–988.
387–391. Rao, S. M., Mittenberg, W., Bernardin, L., Haughton,
Osborn, A. G. (1980). An introduction to cerebral V., & Leo, G. J. (1989). Neuropsychological test
angiography (pp. 78–85). New York: Harper and findings in subjects with leukoaraiosis. Archives of
Row. Neurology, 46, 40–44.
Ozdemir, F., Birtane, M., Tabatabaei, R., Ekuklu, G., & Raz, N., Rodrigue, K. M., & Acker, J. D. (2003).
Kokino, S. (2001). Cognitive evaluation and func- Hypertension and the brain: Vulnerability of
tional outcome after stroke. American Journal of the prefrontal regions and executive functions.
Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 80, 410–415. Behavioral Neuroscience, 117, 1169–1180.
Pantoni, L., & Garcia, J. H. (1995). The significance Redekop, G., TerBrugge, K., Montanera, W., &
of cerebral white matter abnormalities 100 years Willinsky, R. (1998). Arterial aneurysms associ-
after Binswanger’s report: A review. Stroke, 26, ated with cerebral arteriovenous malformations:
1293–1301. Classification, incidence, and risk of hemorrhage.
Papagno, C., Rizzo, S., Ligori, L., Lima, J., & Journal of Neurosurgery, 89, 539–546.
Riggio, A. (2003). Memory and executive func- Reitan, R. M., & Davison, L. A. (1974). Clinical neuro-
tions in aneurysms of the anterior communicat- psychology: Current status and applications. New
ing artery. Journal of Clinical and Experimental York: John Wiley and Sons.
Neuropsychology, 25, 24–35. Robertson, I. H., Ridgeway, V., Greenfield, E., & Parr,
Papke, K., Kuhl, C. K., Fruth, M., Haupt, C., Schlunz- A. (1997). Motor recovery after stroke depends
Hendann, M., Sauner, D., et al. (2007). Intracranial on intact sustained attention: A 2-year follow-up
aneurysms: Role of multidetector CT angiography study. Neuropsychology, 11, 290–295.
in diagnosis and endovascular therapy planning. Rockwood, K., Bowler, J., Erkinjuntti, T., Hachinski,
Radiology, 244, 532–540. V., & Wallin, A. (1999). Subtypes of vascular
334 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

dementia. Alzheimer’s Disease and Associated Smith, C. D., Snowdon, D. A., Wang, H., &
Disorders, 13(Suppl. 3), S59–S65. Markesbery, W. R. (2000). White matter volumes
Rockwood, K., Davis, H., MacKnight, C., Vandorpe, and periventricular white matter hyperintensities
R., Gauthier, S., Guzman, A., et al. (2003). The in aging and dementia. Neurology, 54, 838–842.
Consortium to Investigate Vascular Impairment of Snaphaan, L., & de Leeuw, F. E. (2007). Poststroke
Cognition: Methods and first findings. Canadian memory function in nondemented patients: A sys-
Journal of Neurological Science, 30, 237–243. tematic review on frequency and neuroimaging
Rockwood, K., Parhad, I., & Hachinski, V. (1994). correlates. Stroke, 38, 198–203.
Diagnosis of vascular dementia: Consortium of Sokoloff, L. (1997). Anatomy of cerebral circulation.
Canadian Centres for Clinical Cognitive Research In K. M. A. Welch, L. R. Caplan, D. J. Reis, B. K.
Consensus Statement. Canadian Journal of Siesjo, and B. Weir (Eds.), Primer on cerebrovascu-
Neurological Sciences, 21, 358–364. lar diseases (pp. 3–5). New York: Academic Press.
Rockwood, K., Wentzel, C., Hachinski, V., Hogan, Stabell, K. E., & Nornes, H. (1994). Prospective neu-
D. B., MacKnight, C., & McDowell, I. (2000). ropsychological investigation of patients with
Prevalence and outcomes of vascular cognitive supratentorial arteriovenous malformations. Acta
impairment. Neurology, 54, 447–451. Neurochirurgica, 131, 32–44.
Roman, G. C. (1999). A historical review of the con- Stapf, C., Mast, H., Sciacca, R. R., Choi, J. H., Khaw, A.
cept of vascular dementia: Lessons from the past V., Connolly, E. S., et al. (2006). Predictors of hem-
for the future. Alzheimer Disease and Associated orrhage in patients with untreated brain arterio-
Disorder, 13(Suppl 3):S4–S8. venous malformation. Neurology, 66, 1350–1355.
Roman, G. C. (2002). Vascular dementia may be the Stapf, C., Mohr, J. P., Pile-Spellman, J., Solomon,
most common form of dementia in the elderly. R. A., Sacco, R. L., & Connolly, E. S., Jr. (2001).
Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 203–204, 7–10. Epidemiology and natural history of arteriove-
Sachdev, P. S., & Looi, J. C. L. (2003). nous malformations. Neurosurgical Focus, 11, e1.
Neuropsychological differentiation of Alzheimer’s Steinvorth, S., Wenz, F., Wildermuth, S., Essig, M.,
disease and vascular dementia. In J. V. Bowler and Fuss, M., Lohr, F., et al. (2002). Cognitive function
V. Hachinski (Eds.), Vascular cognitive impair- in patients with cerebral arteriovenous malforma-
ment: Preventable dementia (pp. 152–176). New tions after radiosurgery: Prospective long-term
York: Oxford University Press. follow-up. International Journal of Radiation
Salter, K., Jutai, J., Foley, N., Hellings, C., & Teasell, Oncology, Biology, Physics, 54, 1430–1437.
R. (2006). Identification of aphasia post stroke: A Stephens, S., Kenny, R. A., Rowan, E., Allan,
review of screening assessment tools. Brain Injury, L., Kalaria, R. N., Bradbury, M., et al. (2004).
20, 559–568. Neuropsychological characteristics of mild
Sasaki, T., & Kassell, N. F. (1990). Cerebrovascular sys- vascular cognitive impairment and dementia
tem. In G. Paxinos (Ed.), The human nervous system after stroke. International Journal of Geriatric
(pp. 1135–1149). New York: Academic Press. Psychiatry, 19, 1053–1057.
Schievink, W. I. (1997). Intracranial aneurysms. New Stern, M., Chang, D., Odell, M., & Sperber, K. (2006).
England Journal of Medicine, 336, 28–40. Rehabilitation implications of non-traumatic sub-
Schmidt, R., Fazekas, F., Kapeller, P., Schmidt, H., & arachnoid haemorrhage. Brain Injury, 20, 679–685.
Hartung, H. P. (1999). MRI white matter hyper- Sundet, K., Finset, A., & Reinvang, I. (1988).
intensities: Th ree-year follow-up of the Austrian Neuropsychological predictors in stroke reha-
Stroke Prevention Study. Neurology, 53, 132–139. bilitation. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Shafer, P. W., Roccatagliata, L., & Gonzá lez, R. G. Neuropsychology, 10, 363–379.
(2006). Stroke and cerebral ischemia. In R. R. Swan, G. E., DeCarli, C., Miller, B. L., Reed, T., Wolf,
Edelman, J. R. Hesselink, M. B. Zlatkin, and J. V. P. A., & Jack, L. M. (1998). Association of midlife
Cures III (Eds.), Clinical magnetic resonance imag- blood pressure to late-life cognitive decline and
ing (3rd ed., Vol. 2, pp. 1454–1498). Philadelphia: brain morphology. Neurology, 51, 986–993.
Saunders Elsevier. Tanaka, K., Morooka, Y., Nakagawa, Y., & Shimizu,
Siesjo, B., & Smith, M. L. (1997). Mechanisms of S. (1999). Dural arteriovenous malformation
acidosis-related damage. In K. M. A. Welch, L. R. manifesting as dementia due to ischemia in bilat-
Caplan, D. J. Reis, B. K. Siesjo, and B. Weir (Eds.), eral thalami. A case report. Surgical Neurology, 51,
Primer on cerebrovascular diseases (pp. 223–226). 489–493; discussion 493–484.
New York: Academic Press. Tatemichi, T. K., Desmond, D. W., Stern, Y., Paik, M.,
Singer, R., Ogilvy, C., & Rordorf, G. (2006). Brain Sano, M., & Bagiella, E. (1994). Cognitive impair-
Anteriovenous Malformations [Electronic ment after stroke: Frequency, patterns, and relation-
Version]. http://www.uptodate.com/patients/con- ship to functional abilities. Journal of Neurology,
tent/topic.do?topicKey=~bxbI9AkWqzJgaDZ Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 57, 202–207.
Cerebrovascular Disease 335

Tham, W., Auchus, A. P., Thong, M., Goh, M-L., J. M. Barnett, J. P. Mohr, B. M., Stein, and F. M.
Chang, H-M., Wong, M-C., et al. (2002). Yatsu (Eds.), Stroke: Pathophysiology, diagnosis,
Progression of cognitive impairment after stroke: and management. Vol. 1. (pp. 75–90). New York:
One year results from a longitudinal study Churchill-Livingstone.
of Singaporean stroke patients. Journal of the Welch, K. M. A., Caplan, L. R., Reis, D. J., Siesjo, B.
Neurological Sciences, 203–204, 49–52. K., & B. Weir, B. (Eds.). (1997). Primer on cerebro-
Toole, J. F. (1990). Cerebrovascular disorders (4th vascular diseases. New York: Academic Press.
ed.). New York: Raven Press. Welch, K. M. A., & Levine, S. R. (1991). Focal brain
Tuffiash, E., Tamargo, R. J., & Hillis, A. E. (2003). ischemia and stroke: Pathophysiology and acid-
Craniotomy for treatment of unruptured aneu- base status. In R. A. Bornstein and G. G. Brown
rysms is not associated with long-term cognitive (Eds.), Neurobehavioral aspects of cerebrovascular
dysfunction. Stroke, 34, 2195–2199. disease (pp. 3–13). New York: Oxford University
Tupler, L. A., Coffey, C. E., Logue, P. E., Djang, W. T., Press.
& Fagan, S. M. (1992). Neuropsychological impor- Wentzel., C., Rockwood, K., MacKnight, C.,
tance of subcortical white matter hyperintensity. Hachinski, V. C., Hogan, D. B., & Feldman, H.
Archives of Neurology, 49, 1248–1252. (2001). Progression of impairment in patients with
Turjman, F., Massoud, T. F., Sayre, J. W., Vinuela, F., vascular cognitive impairment without dementia.
Guglielmi, G., & Duckwiler, G. (1995). Epilepsy Neurology, 57, 714–716.
associated with cerebral arteriovenous malfor- Wenz, F., Steinvorth, S., Wildermuth, S., Lohr, F.,
mations: A multivariate analysis of angioarchi- Fuss, M., Debus, J., et al. (1998). Assessment of
tectural characteristics. American Journal of neuropsychological changes in patients with arte-
Neuroradiology, 16, 345–350. riovenous malformation (AVM) after radiosur-
U.S. Census Bureau. (2005). Statistical Abstract of gery. International Journal of Radiation Oncology,
the United States 2006: The National Data Book. Biology, Physics, 42, 995–999.
Washington, DC: United States Government Wetterling, T., Kanitz, R. D., & Borgis, K. J. (1996).
Printing Office. Comparison of different diagnostic criteria for
Vates, G. E., Zabramski, J. M., Spetzler, R. F., & vascular dementia (ADDTC, DSM-IV, ICD-10,
Lawton, M. T. (2004). Intracranial Aneuryms. In NINDS-AIREN). Stroke, 27, 30–36.
J. P. Mohr, D. W. Choi, J. C. Grotta, B. Weir, and Wolf, H., Ecke, G. M., & Bettin, S., (2000). Do white
P. A. Wolf (Eds.), Stroke: Pathophysiology, diagno- matter changes contribute to the subsequent devel-
sis, and management (4th ed., pp. 1279–1335). New opment of dementia in patients with mild cognitive
York: Churchill Livingston. impairment? A longitudinal study. International
Victor, M., & Ropper, A. H. (2001). Adams and Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 15, 803–812.
Victor’s principles of neurology (7th ed.). New Wiebe-Velazquez, S., & Hachinski, V. (1991). Overview
York: McGraw-Hill. of clinical issues in stroke. In R. A. Bornstein and
Wagner, M. T., & Cushman, L. A. (1994). Neuroanatomic G. G. Brown (Eds.), Neurobehavioral aspects of
and neuropsychological predictors of unaware- cerebrovascular disease (pp. 111–130). New York:
ness of cognitive deficit in the vascular population. Oxford University Press.
Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 9, 57–69. Willis, T. (1664). Cerebri Anatome. London: J.
Waldstein, S. R., Snow, J., Muldoon, M. F., & Katzel, Flesher.
L. I. (2001). Neuropsychological consequences of Wu, C. M., McLaughlin, K., Lorenzetti, D. L., Hill, M.
cardiovascular disease. In R. E. Tarter, M. Butters, D., Manns, B. J., & Ghali, W. A. (2007). Early risk
and S. R. Beers (Eds.), Medical neuropsychology of stroke after transient ischemic attack. Archives
(2nd ed., pp. 51–84). New York: Kluwer Academic/ of Internal Medicine, 167, 2417–2422.
Plenum Publishers. Zeidman, S. M., Monsein, L. H., Arosarena, O.,
Wechsler, D. (1987). Weschler Memory Scale: Revised Aletich, V., Biafore, J. A., Dawson, R. C., et al. (1995).
manual. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation/ Reversibility of white matter changes and demen-
Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. tia after treatment of dural fistulas. American
Weir, B. (2002). Unruptured intracranial aneurysms: Journal of Neuroradiology, 16, 1080–1083.
A review. Journal of Neurosurgery, 96, 3–42. Zemke, D., Smith, J. L., Reeves, M. J., & Majid, A.
Welch, K. M. A., & Barkley, G. L. (1986). Biochemistry (2004). Ischemia and ischemic tolerance in the
and pharmacology of cerebral ischemia. In H. brain: An overview. NeuroToxicology, 25, 895–904.
15

Neuropsychological Effects of Hypoxia


in Medical Disorders
Amanda Schurle Bruce, Mark S. Aloia, and Sonia Ancoli-Israel

The human body relies on an adequate supply local terms. It is generally due to a more specific
of oxygen for healthy functioning. The brain is restriction in blood supply (and therefore oxy-
just one organ sensitive to oxygen deprivation, gen supply) to a specific organ. Hypoxemia, a
especially when it occurs over extended periods term commonly confused with hypoxia, refers
of time. There are several medical conditions to deficiency in the concentration of oxygen in
commonly associated with limited oxygen sup- arterial blood. Anoxia refers to the complete
ply to the brain, including chronic obstructive deprivation of oxygen supply. Finally, hyper-
pulmonary disease (COPD), obstructive sleep capnia is an associated state marked by an
apnea (OSA), and acute respiratory distress syn- abnormally high level of carbon dioxide (CO2)
drome (ARDS). The literature demonstrating in the blood. Hypercapnia can be the result
the detrimental effects of oxygen deprivation on of a variety of causes such as hypoventilation,
neuropsychological functioning has developed lung disease, or diminished consciousness.
rapidly over the past few decades. This chapter Hypercapnia is less often the focus of cognitive
focuses on the neuropsychological effects of dis- studies, but it is associated with hypoxia and
orders with limited supply of oxygen, address- may be a more sensitive variable in some clin-
ing questions designed to both summarize the ical populations. This chapter focuses on hyp-
existing literature and theorizing about the oxia and hypoxemia, using the definition that
potential mechanisms behind the findings and best fits the study in question, but each term is
their implications for future research. employed to refer to a limitation in oxygen sup-
ply to the brain.
Definitions
Medical Syndromes Associated
Hypoxia literally means “deficient in oxygen.” It
with Hypoxia
can refer to abnormally low oxygen availability
to body as a whole (generalized) or to a specific There are many medical conditions that can be
region of the body (tissue hypoxia). An example associated with limited oxygen supply, but three
of generalized hypoxia is that which is seen in particular are associated with hypoxia (or
at high altitudes, where reduced atmospheric hypoxemia): COPD, OSA, and ARDS.
pressure lessens the availability of oxygen in the
environment. Generalized hypoxia can occur
Chronic Obstructive Pulmonary
in otherwise healthy individuals and can result
Disease
in altitude sickness, high-altitude pulmonary
edema or high-altitude cerebral edema. Tissue COPD primarily encompasses emphysema
hypoxia, on the other hand, is described in more and chronic bronchitis. The World Health

336
Neuropsychological Effects of Hypoxia in Medical Disorders 337

Organization rated COPD tied with HIV/AIDS skills. No group differences emerged on a factor
as the fourth most common single cause of measuring verbal intelligence. Multiple regres-
death. Smoking is estimated to be responsible sion analyses demonstrated that hypoxemia had
for 80%–90% of COPD cases in the United States a modest relationship to neuropsychological
(Rennard, 2004; Strassels, 1999). Nonsmokers, functioning, and that medical and pulmonary
however, can also develop the disorder, even if function variables did not significantly con-
they are not exposed to second-hand smoke. tribute to the prediction of neuropsychological
The National Heart Lung Blood Institute esti- impairment. Within the last decade, multiple
mates that 12.5 million adults over the age of studies have examined the profile of neuropsy-
25 were diagnosed with COPD in the year 2001. chological impairments associated with COPD
The total estimate cost of COPD in 2002 was and potential relationships between cognitive
$32.1 billion dollars. functioning in COPD and other medical or pul-
monary variables.
Neuropsychological Functioning in COPD. Several studies have sought to characterize
Chronic hypoxemia in COPD has a known the profile of neuropsychological impairments
and well-studied negative effect on cognition. in COPD and compare the pattern to patterns
Systematic investigations of neuropsychological seen in other disorders, Alzheimer’s disease
functioning in COPD began in the 1970s, and (AD) being the most common (Antonelli Incalzi
initially demonstrated deficits on measures of et al., 1993). Antonelli Incalzi and colleagues
perceptual-motor functioning and simple motor performed a discriminant function analysis
functioning in hypoxic participants with COPD of the cognitive profiles of participants with
(Krop et al., 1973). These findings were extended COPD, AD, multi-infract dementia (MID), and
by two large multicenter trials conducted in the no known cognitive disorders (normal elderly).
1980s, the Nocturnal Oxygen Therapy Trial Of the COPD participants, 48.5% had a specific
(NOTT) (Grant et al., 1982) and the Intermittent cognitive profile, with impairments in verbal
Positive Pressure Breathing Trial (IPPB) functions and verbal memory, a diffuse decline
(Prigatano et al., 1983), which demonstrated in other cognitive functions, and normal visual
impairments in perceptual-motor, simple motor, attention. Relatively equal proportions of the
abstracting, executive functioning, and verbal remaining COPD participants were classified as
and nonverbal learning and memory abilities in belonging to each of the other groups. Cognitive
COPD patients. The data from these studies were impairments in the COPD patients were not as
combined in order to more thoroughly examine severe as documented in previous samples, (e.g.,
the relationship between hypoxemia severity Grant et al., 1982), but all participants with COPD
and neuropsychological functioning in a total included in this study were on oxygen therapy,
of 302 COPD patients with varying hypoxemia which may have ameliorated or slowed the pro-
severity (Grant et al., 1987). Cognitive func- gression of some cognitive deficits. Increasing
tioning was impaired in the COPD patients as a age and duration of chronic respiratory failure
whole, with 42% of the combined sample dem- were correlated with cognitive impairment. In a
onstrating neuropsychological impairments. subsequent study, Antonelli Incalzi et al. (1997)
The proportion of cognitively impaired patients again utilized discriminant function analysis
increased with worsening hypoxemia, with 27% to examine memory performance of hypoxic
of the mildly hypoxemic and 61% of the severely COPD patients, AD patients, older healthy sub-
hypoxemic patients demonstrating impair- jects, and controls (Antonelli Incalzi et al., 1997).
ments. A factor analysis was performed on the Only 19% of the participants with COPD were
27 test measures used in these studies, resulting classified as having normal memory, while 38%
in a four-factor solution. Multivariate analysis exhibited a unique memory pattern, 17% were
of variance on the factors revealed that perfor- classified as AD, and 26% were classified as older
mance on three of the four factors declined with normal controls. The participants with COPD
worsening hypoxemia. The affected factors were exhibited memory deficits suggesting impair-
perceptual learning and problem solving, alert- ments in both the encoding and retrieval of ver-
ness and psychomotor speed, and simple motor bal information.
338 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Findings from studies examining the neuro- In general, findings have been more robust in
psychological profiles of nonhypoxic or mildly patients with more advanced disease or greater
hypoxic COPD patients have been less conclu- hypoxemia, and potential associations have
sive. Kozora et al. (1999) compared the perfor- been observed between cognitive functioning
mance of mildly hypoxemic participants with and measures of blood oxygenation, carbon
COPD on oxygen therapy with AD patients nor- dioxide, and fitness. An uncontrolled study
mal and elderly (Kozora et al., 1999). The AD of 18 COPD participants found that complex
group performed significantly more poorly than attention, information-processing speed, and
both the COPD and control groups on most neu- memory were correlated with measures of car-
ropsychological measures, and both the partici- bon dioxide and oxygen partial pressure (Stuss
pants with AD and those with COPD performed et al., 1997). When participants from this study
more poorly than normal controls on verbal flu- were divided into mildly hypoxic and severely
ency to letter cues. However, the performance of hypoxic groups, the severely hypoxic group
the COPD group on this measure was not in the demonstrated poorer memory and attention
clinically impaired range. Results indicated that functioning, and had more evidence of abnor-
mildly hypoxemic participants with COPD who malities on brain CT and EEG. Poorer baseline
were treated with oxygen therapy and who had lung functioning (% predicted forced vital capac-
no neurologic histories may not exhibit cognitive ity (FVC) and forced expiratory volume in one
deficits. This finding contrasted somewhat with second (FEV1)) and more depressive symptoms
findings of neuropsychological impairments doc- were predictive of decline over a 2-year period
umented in samples of untreated mildly hypoxic on the Mini-Mental Status Exam (MMSE) in a
participants with COPD (e.g., Prigatano et al., sample of 40 COPD participants, while depres-
1983) or more severely hypoxic participants with sive symptoms and performance of activities of
COPD (Antonelli Incalzi et al., 1993). Antonelli daily living remained stable (Antonelli Incalzi
Incalzi et al. (2003) examined cerebral perfusion et al., 1998). Findings suggested that more
in nonhypoxic participants with COPD, hypoxic severe lung disease and onset of depression
participants with COPD, AD participants, and are risk factors for cognitive decline in COPD.
healthy controls. Nonhypoxic COPD participants Significant relationships between aerobic fit-
had normal cerebral perfusion, while hypoxic ness and pulmonary functioning and measures
COPD participants demonstrated an intermedi- of fluid intelligence, speed of processing, and
ate level of perfusion, between that of the non- working memory were found in a sample of 98
hypoxic COPD and AD participants (Antonelli COPD participants, although findings related
Incalzi et al., 2003). Hypoxic COPD and AD par- to pulmonary function were variable (Etnier
ticipants had reduced perfusion in anterior areas, et al., 1999). Aerobic fitness was felt to be a pro-
while AD participants also had reduced perfu- tective factor, serving to minimize or slow the
sion in association areas. Both the nonhypoxic decline of cognitive functioning. Again, find-
and hypoxic COPD participants performed bet- ings from studies investigating the relationship
ter than those with AD on neuropsychological between pulmonary and medical variables and
testing, and both groups performed below nor- neuropsychological functioning in samples of
mative standards on measures of verbal memory, nonhypoxemic COPD participants have been
attention, and deductive thinking. The authors mixed. Liesker et al. (2004) found poorer per-
hypothesized that differences between the non- formance on measures of information-process-
hypoxic and hypoxic COPD participants did not ing speed and no differences in performance on
emerge because the hypoxic participants did not measures of memory or executive functioning
exhibit severe hypercapnia, which may have a in a group of 30 nonhypoxic COPD participants
greater link to cognitive dysfunction. compared to age- and education-matched nor-
In addition to characterizing the neuro- mal controls (Liesker et al., 2004).
psychological profi le associated with COPD,
many studies have examined potential relation- Posttreatment Neuropsychological Function-
ships between neuropsychological function- ing in COPD. Several investigators have exam-
ing and pulmonary or other medical variables. ined the association between treatments
Neuropsychological Effects of Hypoxia in Medical Disorders 339

designed to improve brain oxygenation and been previously observed in older individuals,
changes in neuropsychological functioning in and potentially related to reduced postexer-
COPD. Studies have focused primarily on three cise sympathetic hyperarousal and improved
types of intervention: the provision of supple- neurotransmitter regulation associated with
mental oxygen, exercise training and rehabilita- greater oxygen carrying capacity of the blood
tion, and lung-volume reduction surgery. (Dustman et al., 1984). A number of studies
It was initially hypothesized that supplemental have subsequently examined the effects of exer-
oxygen would improve neuropsychological func- cise and rehabilitation on cognitive functioning
tioning in participants with COPD by alleviating in COPD patients. Emery et al. (1998) examined
their chronic hypoxia. Early findings suggested psychological and cognitive outcomes in par-
improvements following short-term supplemen- ticipants with COPD randomized to a 10-week
tal oxygen therapy (Krop et al., 1973). The NOTT exercise plus education and stress management
sought to examine the effects of long-term sup- condition as compared to those randomized
plemental oxygen therapy on cognition, as a to education and stress management only and
follow-up of these early findings (Heaton et al., a wait-list control. While no improvement in
1983). A total of 150 patients were enrolled, 78 of pulmonary functioning was observed, the exer-
whom received continuous oxygen therapy and cise group demonstrated improved physical
72 of whom received nocturnal oxygen therapy. endurance and reduced symptoms of anxiety
Following six months of treatment, both groups and depression. Interestingly, depressive symp-
demonstrated slight improvements compared to toms also declined in the wait-list control group
normal controls on three individual neuropsy- (Emery et al., 1998). Participants in the exercise
chological measures (sequencing ability, sim- group demonstrated improved verbal fluency/
ple motor speed, and motor strength). When verbal processing, suggesting some possible
a subsample of 37 participants was examined improvement in frontal lobe executive functions.
following 12 months of treatment, the continu- No group differences emerged on measures of
ous oxygen therapy group exhibited improved attention, motor speed, and mental efficiency.
performance relative to the nocturnal therapy The authors hypothesized that changes in the
group on three of five neuropsychological sum- release and re-uptake of neurotransmitters and
mary measures (i.e., WAIS Performance IQ, in sympathetic nervous system activity asso-
HRB Average Impairment Rating, and Brain ciated with the exercise intervention may have
Age Quotient). No significant improvement, led to the observed improvements in mood and
however, was noted on measures of emotional cognition, but no direct evidence of this was
functioning or quality of life. In a smaller sam- given. Emery et al. (2001) also demonstrated a
ple of 10 hypoxemic COPD participants followed similar improvement in cognitive functioning
through three months of treatment, COPD was in participants with COPD immediately follow-
associated with poorer baseline attention, infor- ing exercise. Both COPD patients and healthy
mation-processingspeed, and memory relative controls completed a brief neuropsychological
to age-matched normal controls (Hjalmarsen et test battery immediately before and immedi-
al., 1999). Although neuropsychological perfor- ately after both an exercise condition (bicycle
mance among the COPD participants subjec- stress test) and a video-viewing control con-
tively improved following treatment, changes dition. Neither group improved following the
were not statistically significant. Findings may control condition, and the COPD group dem-
have been due to practice effects or insufficient onstrated improved verbal fluency/verbal pro-
power. There were also no significant changes cessing postexercise, superior to that seen in the
noted on measures of cerebral blood flow. normal controls (Emery et al., 2001). Improved
With the increasing acceptance of exercise neurotransmitter functioning postexercise
and rehabilitation therapy in the treatment was hypothesized to contribute to the changes
of COPD, investigators began to examine the observed in the COPD participants, but this
effect of these treatments on psychological and study remains to be replicated.
neuropsychological functioning. Improved Long-term changes in neuropsychological
cognitive functioning following exercise had functioning following exercise interventions
340 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

have also been studied (Emery et al., 2003; post-randomization to either lung-volume
Etnier & Berry, 2001). Etnier and Berry (2001) reduction surgery (n = 19) or continuing med-
found an apparent association between impro- ical therapy (n = 20). At follow-up, participants
ved performance on a measure of fluid intelli- undergoing lung-volume reduction surgery
gence and aerobic fitness following a 3-month improved on measures of delayed verbal recall
exercise intervention in participants with and sequential psychomotor skills, and had a
COPD. Participants were subsequently random- trend toward improved verbal naming (Kozora
ized into either a 15-month structured exercise et al., 2005). Additionally, lung- volume reduc-
program or a control condition in which they tion surgery was associated with reduced
were simply encouraged to continue exercising. depressive symptoms and improved quality of
At follow-up, there were no group differences in life. Participants in the medical therapy con-
cognitive performance, although findings sug- dition improved only on one measure of accu-
gested that individual subjects demonstrating racy of visual attention at follow-up, and also
the greatest improvement in aerobic fitness also exhibited an increase in depressive symptoms.
showed the greatest improvement on a mea- The potential mechanisms contributing to this
sure of fluid intelligence. Their findings suggest change were difficult to identify, as improve-
that relatively short exercise interventions can ments in the surgery group relative to the med-
lead to improved cognitive functioning. Emery ical therapy group could not be accounted for
et al. (2003) examined the relationship between by changes in physical endurance, pulmonary
adherence and the long-term effects of exercise function, psychological symptoms, or medica-
treatment in a sample of 28 COPD participants tion changes, although improved quality of life
who had completed a 10-week exercise inter- may have influenced improved neuropsycho-
vention. Participants were re-assessed 1 year logical functioning.
after treatment and determined to be exercise
adherent or nonadherent. While no subsequent
Obstructive Sleep Apnea
improvements were noted, adherent partici-
pants maintained gains in physical endurance OSA is a sleep disorder that affects at least 4%
and cognitive functioning made following the of middle-aged men and 2% of middle-aged
10-week intervention. In contrast, nonadher- women (Young et al., 1993) and 70% of older
ent participants exhibited decline at follow-up men and 56% of older women (Ancoli-Israel
on measures of physical endurance, cognitive et al., 1991). It is a well-recognized clinical dis-
functioning (psychomotor speed and sequenc- order characterized by repeated obstructions of
ing), and psychological symptoms (increased the upper airway during sleep. OSA results in
depression and anxiety). While continued sleep fragmentation that disrupts the normal
adherence with exercise interventions did not sleep architecture and periodic oxygen desatu-
result in continuing improvement in neuropsy- rations that can drop to dangerously low levels.
chological functioning, it may have been a pro- Sleep fragmentation and hypoxemia are gener-
tective factor, maintaining previous gains and ally inextricably tied in OSA, making it difficult
preventing further decline. to make explicit statements regarding the inde-
The National Emphysema Treatment Trial pendent effects on any single cognitive factor.
(NETT) demonstrated that non-high-risk Primarily correlative techniques have been used
COPD patients undergoing lung-volume reduc- to try to tease the effects apart.
tion surgery exhibited significant improve-
ment on measures of both physical functioning Neuropsychological Functioning and OSA.
and quality of life (“The National Emphysema OSA can cause significant daytime behavioral
Treatment Trial [NETT]: How strong is the and adaptive deficits. Functional impairments
evidence?,” 2003). Kozora et al. (2005) exam- like sleepiness, impaired driving, increased risk
ined neuropsychological functioning in a of accidents, and decreased quality of life are
sample of 39 participants with emphysema frequent consequences of sleep apnea (Engleman
and 39 matched controls at baseline, follow- & Douglas, 2004; George & Smiley, 1999).
ing 6–10 weeks of rehabilitation, and 6 months Behavioral effects of OSA are often referred to
Neuropsychological Effects of Hypoxia in Medical Disorders 341

as “neurobehavioral” consequences because executive functioning) and that domains are


they are presumed to be directly related to not mutually exclusive in their functions. For
brain function (Beebe, 2005). Neurobehavioral OSA patients, the domains of cognitive func-
functioning is a broad term that includes sev- tioning may be differentially affected. Vigilance,
eral specific cognitive functions. Numerous including sustained attention, controlled atten-
studies have examined these specific cognitive tion, efficiency of information processing, and
functions and some have attempted to identify response time, is the most commonly assessed
a “pattern” of cognitive dysfunction in OSA. cognitive construct in OSA and has been found
Such patterns, when they exist, will be summa- to be the most consistently affected cognitive
rized below. Following that summary, theoret- domain in apnea patients.
ical models describing potential mechanisms Executive functioning, which includes pro-
involved in this relationship are discussed. cesses involved in planning, initiation, and the
Neurocognitive testing is common in studies execution of goal-oriented behavior and men-
involving OSA. The cognitive sequelae of the dis- tal flexibility, is another affected domain. Some
order have been repeatedly discussed, but are not argue that it is the most prominent area of cog-
always consistent across studies (e.g., Aloia et al., nitive impairment in untreated sleep-disordered
2004; Engleman et al., 2000; Sateia, 2003). Some breathing and that the dysfunction extends to
inconsistencies may be associated with the het- children with sleep apnea as well as adults (Beebe
erogeneity of the samples, while others may be the & Gozal, 2002). The broad construct of execu-
result of the different tests utilized in the studies. tive functioning makes it difficult to accurately
Too few studies utilize the same cognitive tests to describe the deficits and to construct a model
draw any definitive conclusions as to the degree explaining causes of the impairment. Examples
or pattern of cognitive deficits in OSA. of executive functioning include working
Cognition in OSA has been examined as both memory, set shifting, perseveration, planning,
a unitary function and one divided into sev- abstract reasoning, and verbal fluency. Even
eral specific domains (e.g., memory, attention, more, executive functions are in part supported
executive functioning, etc.). The utility of each by adequate attention. Therefore, complex atten-
type of examination depends upon the ques- tional problems could represent the root cause
tion being asked and the degree to which each of executive dysfunction. Despite its being a
approach would adequately address a given broad construct, OSA patients clearly perform
hypothesis. Studies of global impairment may consistently more poorly on tests of executive
be better suited for addressing the overall effects functioning than matched controls (Bedard
of a particular variable on cognition. Impaired et al., 1991, 1993; Feuerstein et al., 1997; Naegele
cognition among OSA patients is not, how- et al., 1995; Salorio et al., 2002; Verstraeten et al.,
ever, global. In fact, apnea patients may exhibit 1997). Several investigators have documented
relatively few deficits in the global cognitive executive dysfunction in OSA. Initially, these
domain when compared to normal controls (for findings allude to frontal lobe deficits associated
review see Aloia et al., 2004). Studies that limit with the disorder (Beebe & Gozal, 2002). Such a
themselves to global functioning may not have theory is supported by animal studies and neu-
a true appreciation for the various components roimaging, but foundation functions like atten-
of cognition that contribute to this global score, tion might also contribute to what is seen to be
and specific cognitive deficits can be masked. prominent executive dysfunction. Moreover, the
Domain-specific hypotheses can remedy this cause of executive dysfunction is often complex.
problem. Domains can be divided in several Learning and memory are also impaired in
ways, but common domain names include patients with OSA. Learning and memory con-
executive functioning, memory, attention, vig- stitute a broad, complex domain that includes
ilance, visuospatial ability, constructional abil- verbal memory, visual memory, short-term
ity, psychomotor functioning, and language. memory, and long-term memory. Memory per-
One should remember, however, that each of formance deficits can be attributed to several
these domains may also have subdomains that areas: initial learning, free recall, or forgetful-
further break apart their complex nature (e.g., ness, each of which has different implications
342 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

(Aloia et al., 2004). OSA patients perform of attention and information processing, even
more poorly on tests of memory and learning after controlling for several extraneous vari-
than matched controls (e.g., Aloia et al., 2004; ables (Ohayon & Vecchierini, 2002). These
Feuerstien et al., 1997; Naegele et al., 1995). A findings were significantly associated with cog-
recent study, which examined the specific type nition only when daytime sleepiness was also
of memory impairment in OSA by comparing reported. A longitudinal study employed more
performance on tests of list learning, procedural stringent criteria for diagnosing OSA. Ancoli-
memory, and working memory (a combination Israel and colleagues examined the sleep and
function including executive functions as well as global cognitive functioning of 46 community-
memory), found the most compelling evidence dwelling older adults over the course of 4 years
for cognitive dysfunction in OSA exists in work- (Cohen-Zion et al., 2001), finding that increases
ing memory. At first glance this finding suggests in apnea severity and daytime sleepiness were
that executive dysfunction could tip the scales associated with respective decreases in global
in favor of working memory being the most cognitive functioning over time. Moreover, the
commonly affected memory impairment in findings seemed to be driven by daytime sleep-
OSA. However, another recent study attempted iness when regression models were employed.
to parse out the various cognitive functions An intriguing study by Antonelli Incalzi and
underlying working memory to determine in fact colleagues compared older individuals with
whether or not working memory deficits were sleep apnea to patients with either AD or
primarily the result of learning impairments, MID on a battery of neuropsychological tests
free recall impairments, motor dyscoordination, (Antonelli Incalzi et al., 2004). Th is study sug-
or executive dysfunction. This study concluded gested that the cognitive profi le of apnea is most
that the impairments were most commonly seen like that seen in MID. They relate this finding
on complete tests of working memory than on to the probable involvement of subcortical
any specific cognitive subfunction. This suggests brain regions in apnea, a relationship that has
that this construct may be quite sensitive to the also been posited by other investigators (Aloia
consequences of OSA. et al., 2003, 2004).
Psychomotor performance is a domain that
has been assessed less frequently. Most studies, Posttreatment Neuropsychological Function-
however, show OSA patients to be impaired in ing in OSA. The most common and effective
psychomotor performance relative to controls treatment for OSA is positive airway pressure
(see Aloia et al., 2004 for review). Specifically, (PAP). When properly used, PAP has been
OSA patients perform relatively poorer on tests shown to dramatically reduce morbidity and
of fine motor coordination (Bédard et al., 1991, mortality (Campos-Rodriguez et al., 2005; He
1993; Greenberg et al., 1987; Verstraeten et al., et al., 1988; Keenan et al., 1994). Due in part
1997). Not all studies have reported impair- to these encouraging findings, the effect that
ment on tests of motor speed (Knight et al., PAP treatment has on cognition has been an
1987; Verstraeten et al., 1997). Overall, there has area of interest for many investigators. Long-
been relatively little discussion of this domain term adherence to PAP treatment, however,
as a primary source of impairment. The mecha- is less than optimal, with approximately 25%
nism for psychomotor dysfunction is not clear. of patients discontinuing use within a year
One explanation for psychomotor difficulties is (McArdle et al., 1999). Commonly cited reasons
excessive sleepiness, but this does not account for poor adherence include physical discomfort
for the discrepancy between tests of fine motor as well as psychosocial factors (Aloia et al., 2001,
skills and motor speed. 2005a; Hoffstein et al., 1992; Kribbs et al., 1993;
Few studies have been conducted examin- Waldhorn et al., 1990).
ing cognitive dysfunction associated with OSA Aloia and colleagues published a critical
in older adults. A large-scale study in France review of the literature on the neuropsycho-
reported that participant reports of snoring logical sequelae of OSA. They concluded that
and/or breathing cessation during sleep were the majority of studies examining the connec-
associated with greater impairment on tests tion between PAP and OSA have indeed cited a
Neuropsychological Effects of Hypoxia in Medical Disorders 343

positive relationship between treatment adher- discussed below propose certain mechanisms
ence and improved performance on various that may be involved in the relationship between
cognitive tests. Response to treatment, however, OSA and cognition. Beebe and Gozal hypothe-
may be a factor of the particular test being mea- sized that OSA has a predilection for affecting
sured. Just as some tests are more sensitive to the frontal lobes of the brain compared to other
dysfunction, some are likely to be more sensi- brain regions. Two primary mechanisms (i.e.,
tive to the effects of treatment. sleep fragmentation and hypoxemia) were out-
Ancoli-Israel and colleagues examined the lined as the causes of frontal lobe dysfunction
effect of continuous PAP (CPAP) treatment on (Beebe & Gozal, 2002). The model suggested that
cognitive function in patients with mild-to- OSA has a predilection for affecting the fron-
moderate AD and OSA. Results suggested that tal lobes of the brain compared to other brain
3 weeks of CPAP treatment, with an average regions. Hypoxemia is thought to result in cellu-
of 5 hours of use a night, resulted in improve- lar changes to the prefrontal cortex that directly
ments in episodic verbal learning and memory affects function, while sleep fragmentation is
and some aspects of executive functioning such posited to preferentially affect the frontal lobes
as cognitive flexibility, and mental processing of the brain by disrupting the normal restor-
speed (Ancoli-Israel et al., 2006). ative process of sleep. Together, hypoxemia and
Two recent studies concluded that the number sleep fragmentation adversely affect the exec-
of hours of CPAP adherence needs to be evalu- utive functioning of the frontal lobes. Sleep
ated when examining other outcome measures. deprivation studies provide evidence for this
Zimmerman et al. (2006) split a group of mem- model by showing a strong relationship to exec-
ory-impaired OSA patients into three adherence utive functions. The executive model has several
groups based on average PAP use at three months. strengths. First, it was one of the first models to
The reference group for the study comprised poor thoughtfully take a neurofunctional approach
users—those using 1 or fewer hours per night on to explain the cognitive dysfunction seen in
average. Moderate users (2–5 hours’ use a night) OSA. The model also employed both basic and
were 3 times as likely to develop normal memory clinical studies as evidence. There were, how-
over 3 months with PAP compared to poor users. ever, some weaknesses to the model. Data from
This was not a significant effect. Optimal users carbon monoxide poisoning studies and sleep
(6 or more hours a night), however, were 8 times deprivation studies were extrapolated to the
as likely to normalize their memory compared to conditions of hypoxemia and sleep fragmenta-
poor users at 3 months. This finding was not due tion in general. These analogies may or may not
to baseline differences in memory or any other be appropriate. In addition, the effects of sleep
intervening variables, suggesting that it takes fragmentation and hypoxemia on brain regions
as many as 6 hours of use per night to normal- other than frontal lobes were not incorporated
ize memory in OSA patients who demonstrated into this early model. Finally, as mentioned
memory impairments at baseline. above, executive dysfunction is complex and
In a second study of adherence, Weaver et al. multifactorial, something acknowledged by the
(2007) demonstrated that subjective sleepiness authors. Regardless of this criticism, the authors
can change with as little as 4 hours of CPAP use undertook a very complex task: to develop a
a night, while objective sleepiness (as measured comprehensive, neurofunctional model of OSA.
by the Multiple Sleep Latency Test) might take Another proposed model is the attentional
6 hours of use, and changes to functional out- model. Certainly attentional problems have
comes associated with sleepiness might require been implicated in OSA. Verstraeten and
over 7 hours of use per night. These two studies Cluydts (2004) have recently published two
demonstrate that adherence as well as test sensi- papers making the case that higher-order cog-
tivity and specificity must be incorporated into nitive dysfunction in OSA can be explained by
efficacy trials. the impairment of basic attentional processes
and slowed mental processing. The first paper
Potential Mechanisms for Neurobehavioral proposed a theoretical model of neurocogni-
Dysfunction in OSA. The theoretical models tive functioning marked by the hierarchical
344 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

ordering of cognitive processes that can lead supported consequence of OSA it was reason-
to the appearance of higher-order cognitive able that vascular compromise might also exist
dysfunction. This theoretical paper is quite in the brain. It was determined, on the basis of
interesting as it is the first to recognize that hypoxia literature (Caine & Watson, 2000), that
higher-order cognitive processes are complex hypoxemia would preferentially affect regions of
enough to often rely on more basic attentional the brain that were metabolically active during
and lower-level cognitive processes. The authors the event and fed by small vessels. Damage to
made the case that executive dysfunction per the small vessels might in fact precede large ves-
se should be interpreted cautiously in the case sel stroke and may result in a predictable pattern
of sleep apnea, given the potentially profound of cognitive dysfunction associated with small
effects of sleep disruption on arousal, basic pro- vessel brain disease. The pattern would involve
cessing speed, and attentional ability. The con- deficits in motor speed and coordination, exec-
clusion of this paper is that investigators should utive dysfunction, memory impairment, and
consider developing studies that allow them to some problems with attention and mental pro-
systematically control for lower-level functions cessing speed. After a review of the literature,
in the assessment of high-order cognitive abil- Aloia and colleagues argued that this pattern
ity. The second study attempted to demonstrate of cognitive dysfunction was indeed present in
this theory by fractionating these functions to OSA and may represent microvascular disease.
determine the degree to which the reliance of Several supporting studies for this model were
higher-order functions on attention can lead presented, highlighting the involvement of the
to the misinterpretation when considering the white matter in OSA, an area fed primarily by
functional deficits in OSA. Deficits in OSA small vessels and susceptible to ischemic dis-
patients were seen in processing speed, atten- ease. Functional and structural studies were
tional capacity, and short-term memory span, presented, though few had been completed at
with no differences seen in executive functions the time of the original publication. In closing
per se. The investigators provided these data the paper, it was demonstrated in a small sam-
as evidence for this hierarchical model of dys- ple that evidence of microvascular disease could
function in OSA, making the case that execu- be seen on brain MRI in OSA. Since the publi-
tive dysfunction may be misinterpreted without cation of this review, several studies have been
knowledge of lower-order skills. This series of published to support and refute this model. One
studies is quite compelling and encourages supportive study identified a subgroup of OSA
investigators to consider cognitive functions in patients with cognitive dysfunction that likened
a hierarchical manner (Verstraeten et al., 2004). a pattern seen in MID. However, other studies
Indeed, identifying the basic functional deficits have failed to find an association between white
that underlie these more complex deficits can matter ischemic disease and OSA severity using
lead to a better understanding of the neurofunc- large-scale epidemiological data in older adults.
tional mechanisms impaired in OSA. The one One primary limitation of the model was that
lacking component of this work is the provi- it did not attend strongly to the differential
sion of data to support any specific mechanisms effects of sleep fragmentation and hypoxemia.
related to sleep fragmentation or hypoxemia. The model is promising in that it is parsimoni-
Future research will undoubtedly address this ous and incorporates a known mechanism of
gap in the model and may augment the execu- dysfunction in OSA, vascular compromise, into
tive model described above. the cognitive realm. Further research, however,
The microvascular theory as a model for cog- is needed to defend, refute, or expand the model
nitive dysfunction in OSA was first put forth and to relate its effects to complaints of fatigue
by Aloia and colleagues in 2004, owing in large and sleepiness.
part to the work of Somers and colleagues The most recent model, posited by Beebe
(Lanfranchi & Somers, 2001). Aloia and col- (Beebe, 2005), is the most comprehensive to date
leagues culled mechanisms of dysfunction from and pulls upon the strengths of previous models
the cardiovascular literature and determined to develop a heuristic model of the mechanisms
that since cardiovascular dysfunction was a well- underlying cognitive dysfunction in OSA.
Neuropsychological Effects of Hypoxia in Medical Disorders 345

He hypothesized that the effects of sleep frag- than previous models. It is not, however, overly
mentation and hypoxemia are not likely to be inclusive and specifies brain regions likely to be
effectively isolated from one another. He stated involved without implying that all regions are
that their interaction may in fact be synergis- equally vulnerable. Perhaps most importantly,
tic. Moreover, he presented the likelihood that the model highlights the likely effect of moder-
these mechanisms interact with certain vulner- ating factors for cognitive impairment in OSA,
able brain regions, highlighting specifically the something that has only recently been addressed
hippocampus, the prefrontal cortex, subcortical in the literature.
gray matter, and white matter. The inclusion of
the subcortical gray and white matter reflects an
Acute Respiratory Distress Syndrome
appreciation for the potential involvement of the
small vessels of the brain. Beebe also attended ARDS results from injury to the microvascula-
to the possibility that findings in studies of the ture of the lungs, which can lead to leakage of
potential mechanisms of cognitive dysfunction fluid into the alveoli resulting in hypoxemia,
are dependent in part on task demands and dyspnea, and in some cases death. ARDS is
the environment under which testing is con- generally seen in hospitalized patients with
ducted. This addition shows an appreciation severe illnesses, including sepsis, pneumo-
for the complexity of executive dysfunction as nia, severe blood loss, chest and head injuries,
multifactorial and broadens the executive and aspiration of stomach contents, and breathing
attentional models by including several other injurious fumes. The syndrome affects roughly
cognitive tasks that may be impaired in OSA 13–18 people per 100,000, with prevalence
simply due to the demands that they present rates between 15% and 18% among ventilated
for the implicated brain regions. Finally, Beebe patients. ARDS can result in death, with mor-
went beyond the other models by incorporating tality rates between 35% and 70%. Few studies
two additional areas to consider: (1) risk and have examined the cognitive effects of ARDS.
resilience factors, and (2) direct effects on cog- Hopkins and her colleagues have conducted the
nition outside of those involved in OSA. When majority of these studies in an attempt to pro-
discussing risk and resilience, Beebe acknowl- vide evidence for cognitive dysfunction, iden-
edged recent work by Alchanatis and colleagues tify the pattern of dysfunction, and address the
showing that there may be moderators of dys- consequences of such dysfunction in longitudi-
function in OSA; for example, intelligence has nal designs (Hopkins et al., 2004, 2005, 2006;
been proposed as one moderator for vigilance Hopkins & Herridge, 2006). In general, there are
problems in OSA (Alchanatis et al., 2005). This consistent findings of cognitive dysfunction in
study identified cognitive reserve (high premor- ARDS. Global cognitive function is impaired in
bid cognitive ability that results in resistance to the majority of patients immediately following
cognitive decline with insult) as a resilience fac- discharge. There is some recovery of function
tor, but the heuristic model also includes age, over the first year after discharge, but Hopkins
sex, sociodemographic factors, and duration et al. (2005) have demonstrated that cognitive
of illness. Others have also proposed the inclu- dysfunction can persist as long as 2 years after
sion of moderators of dysfunction noting that discharge. One additional study documented
several patients with severe OSA do not suf- cognitive dysfunction as long as 6 years postdis-
fer dysfunction at all, while others with mild charge, raising concerns over long-lasting and
OSA show significant impairment. Finally, the permanent damage to the brain (Rothenhausler
model incorporates genetic endowment, prior et al., 2001). Indeed, neuroimaging studies have
experience with testing, and sociodemographic demonstrated early evidence of cortical atrophy
factors as possible extraneous variables when (Hopkins et al., 2006). Objective and subjec-
considering the mechanisms of cognitive dys- tive data suggest that the majority of the cog-
function in OSA. The model needs to be tested, nitive deficits in ARDS fall into the realm of
but there are more strengths to this model memory, with some impairment occurring in
than there are weaknesses. The model is test- executive functioning, with psychomotor speed
able with large datasets and is more inclusive and impulsivity contributing to these deficits.
346 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

This is surprisingly consistent with the OSA of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10,
data presented above. Even more compelling 772–785.
is that the ARDS cognitive data do not corre- Aloia, M. S., Arnedt, J. T., Stepnowsky Jr., C. J.,
late with illness severity, age, or smoking his- Hecht, J., & Borrelli, B. (2005a). Predicting treat-
ment adherence in obstructive sleep apnea using
tory. These data mirror much of what has been
principles of behavior change. Journal of Clinical
demonstrated in OSA and call into question the
Sleep Medicine, 1(4), 346–353.
presence of moderators that may make certain Aloia, M. S., DiDio, P., Ilniczky, N., Perlis, M. L.,
individuals more susceptible to the effects of Greenblatt, D. W., & Giles, D. E. (2001). Improving
hypoxemia compared to others. compliance with nasal CPAP and vigilance in
older adults with OSAHS. Sleep and Breathing,
5(1), 13–21.
Conclusions and Comments Aloia, M. S., Ilniczky, N., Di Dio, P., Perlis, M.
This chapter covered the effects that hypoxia L., Greenblatt, D. W., & Giles, D. E. (2003).
has on neuropsychological functioning in sev- Neuropsychological changes and treatment com-
eral different medical conditions. In closing, pliance in older adults with sleep apnea. Journal of
Psychosomatic Research, 54(1), 71–76.
it appears clear that hypoxemia and hypoxia
Aloia, M. S., Stanchina, M. L., Arnedt, J. T., Malhotra,
have unmistakable detrimental effects on cog-
A., & Millman, R. P. (2005b). Treatment adherence
nitive functioning. It is also obvious, however, and outcomes in flexible versus continuous positive
that these findings are not necessarily pervasive airway pressure therapy. Chest, 127(6), 2085–2093.
across all patients. The focus of positive stud- Ancoli-Israel, S., Kripke, D. F., Klauber, M. R.,
ies to date has been on the mediators of cog- Mason, W. J., Fell, R., & Kaplan, O. (1991). Sleep
nitive dysfunction in disease states, including disordered breathing in community dwelling
hypoxia. Future studies should also attempt to elderly. Sleep, 14(6), 486–495.
tackle the question of moderators of cognitive Ancoli-Israel, S., Palmer, B. W., Marler, M., Corey-
dysfunction in persons with hypoxic condi- Bloom, J., Loredo, J. S., & Liu, L. (2006). Effect of
tions. For example, studies have demonstrated CPAP on cognition in Alzheimer’s patients with
apnea. Sleep, 29, A106.
that high cognitive reserve spares some individ-
Antonelli Incalzi, R., Chiappini, F., Fuso, L., Torrice,
uals with OSA from developing cognitive prob-
M. P., Gemma, A., & Pistilli, R. (1998). Predicting
lems (Alchanatis et al., 2005). Investigators have cognitive decline in patients with hypoxaemic
also considered the possibility of substances COPD. Respiratory Medicine, 92, 527–533.
that are thought to be protective from inflam- Antonelli Incalzi, R., Gemma, A., Marra, C.,
mation (e.g., antioxidants) as potential mod- Muzzolon, R., Capparella, O., & Carbonin, P. U.
erators of dysfunction (Baldwin et al., 2005). (1993). Chronic obstructive pulmonary disease:
Genetic factors are also only now being consid- An original model of cognitive decline. American
ered. Probably, our understanding of the role of Review of Respiratory Diseases, 148, 418–424.
hypoxia in cognitive dysfunction will only be Antonelli Incalzi, R., Carhonin, P., Gemma, A.,
made clear with these mediator and modera- Capparella, O., Marra, C., & Fuso, L. (1997). Verbal
memory impairment in COPD: Its mechanisms
tor studies. The future of this line of research is
and clinical relevance. Chest, 112, 1506–1513.
appealing, and the need for additional studies
Antonelli Incalzi, R., Marra, C., Giordano, A.,
remains strong. Calcagni, M. L., Cappa, A., Basso, S., et al. (2003).
Cognitive impairment in chronic obstructive pul-
References monary disease: A neuropsychological and SPECT
study. Journal of Neurology, 250, 325–332.
Alchanatis, M., Zias, N., Deligiorgis, N., Amfi lochiou, Antonelli Incalzi, R., Marra, C., Salvigni, B. L.,
A., Dionellis, G., & Orphanidou, D. (2005). Sleep Petrone, A., Gemma, A., Selvaggio, D., et al. (2004).
apnea-related cognitive deficits and intelligence: Does cognitive dysfunction conform to a distinc-
An implication of cognitive reserve theory. Journal tive pattern in obstructive sleep apnea? Journal of
of Sleep Research, 14, 69–75. Sleep Research, 13, 79–86.
Aloia, M. S., Arnedt, J. T., Davis, J. D., Riggs, R. L., Baldwin, C. M., Bootzin, R. R., Schwenke, D. C., &
& Byrd, D. (2004). Neuropsychological conse- Quan, S. F. (2005). Antioxidant nutrient intake
quences of sleep apnea: A critical review. Journal and supplements as potential moderators of cog-
nitive decline and cardiovascular disease in
Neuropsychological Effects of Hypoxia in Medical Disorders 347

obstructive sleep apnea. Sleep Medicine Reviews, cognition in patients with chronic obstructive pul-
9(6), 459–476. monary disease. American Journal of Respiratory
Bédard, M. A., Montplaisir, J., Malo, J., Richer, F., & and Critical Care Medicine, 164(9), 1624–1627.
Rouleau, I. (1993). Persistent neuropsychological Engleman, H., Kingshott, R. N., Martin, S. E., &
deficits and vigilance impairment in sleep apnea Douglas, N. J. (2000). Cognitive function in
syndrome after treatment with continuous positive the sleep apnea/hypopnea syndrome. Sleep, 23,
airways pressure (CPAP). Journal of Clinical and S102–S108.
Experimental Neuropsychology, 15(2), 330–341. Engleman, H. M., & Douglas, N. J. (2004). Sleepiness,
Bédard, M. A., Montplaisir, J., Richer, F., Rouleau, cognitive function, and quality of life in obstruc-
I., & Malo, J. (1991). Obstructive sleep apnea tive sleep apnoea/hypopnoea syndrome. Thorax,
syndrome: Pathogenesis of neuropsychologi- 59, 618–622.
cal deficits. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Etnier, J., & Berry, M. (2001). Fluid intelligence in an
Neuropsychology, 13(6), 950–964. older COPD sample after short- or long-term exer-
Beebe, D. (2005). Neurobehavioral effects of obstruc- cise. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise,
tive sleep apnea: An overview and heuristic model. 33, 1620–1628.
Current Opinion in Pulmonary Medicine, 11, Etnier, J., Johnston, R., Dagenbach, D., Pollard, J.,
494–500. Rejeski, W. J., & Berry, M. (1999). The relation-
Beebe, D., & Gozal, D. (2002). Obstructive sleep ships among pulmonary function, aerobig fitness,
apnea and the prefrontal cortex: Towards a com- and cognitive functioning in older COPD patients.
prehensive model linking noctural upper airway Chest, 116, 953–960.
obstruction to daytime cognitive and behavioral Feuerstein, C., Naegele, B., Pepin, J. L., & Levy, P.
deficits. Journal of Sleep Research, 11, 1–16. (1997). Frontal lobe-related cognitive functions
Caine, D., & Watson, J. D. G. (2000). Neuro- in patients with Sleep Apnea Syndrome before
psychological and neuropathological sequelae and after treatment. Acta Neurologica Belgica, 97,
of cerebral anoxia: A critical review. Journal of 96–107.
the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, George, C. F., & Smiley, A. (1999). Sleep apnea and
86–99. automobile crashes. Sleep, 22(6), 790–795.
Campos-Rodriguez, F., Pena-Grinan, N., Reyes- Grant, I., Heaton, R., McSweeney, A., Adams, K.,
Nunez, N., De la Cruz-Moron, I., Perez-Ronchel, & Timms, R. (1982). Neuropsychologic find-
J., De la Vega-Gallardo, F., et al. (2005). Mortality ings in hypoxemic chronic obstructive pulmo-
in obstructive sleep apnea-hypopnea patients nary disease. Archives of Internal Medicine, 142,
treated with positive airway pressure. Chest, 1470–1476.
128(2), 624–633. Grant, I., Prigatano, G., Heaton, R., McSweeny,
Cohen-Zion, M., Stepnowsky Jr., C. J., Marler, M. A., Wright, E., & Adams, K. (1987). Progressive
R., Shochat, T., Kripke, D., & Ancoli Israel, S. neuropsychologic impairment and hypoxemia:
(2001). Changes in cognitive function associated Relationship in chronic obstructive pulmo-
with sleep disordered breathing in older people. nary disease. Archives of General Psychiatry, 44,
Journal of the American Geriatric Society, 49(12), 999–1006.
1622–1627. Greenberg, G. D., Watson, R. K., & Deptula, D.
Dustman, R. E., Rughling, R. O., Russell, E. M., (1987). Neuropsychological dysfunction in sleep
Shearer, D. E., Bonekat, H. W., Shigeoka, J. W., et apnea. Sleep, 10(3), 254–262.
al. (1984). Aerobic exercise training and improved He, J., Kryger, M. H., Zorick, F., Conway, W., & Roth,
neuropsychological function of older individuals. T. (1988). Mortality and apnea index in obstruc-
Neurobiology of Aging, 5(1), 35–42. tive sleep apnea: Experience in 385 male patients.
Emery, C., Schein, R., Hauck, E., & MacIntyre, N. Chest, 94, 9–14.
(1998). Psychological and cognitive outcomes of a Heaton, R., Grant, I., McSweeney, A., Adams, K.,
randomized trial of exercise among patients with Petty, T., & the NOTT Study Group. (1983).
chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. Health Psychologic effects of continuous and nocturnal
Psychology, 17, 232–240. oxygen therapy in hypoxemic chronic obstructive
Emery, C., Shermer, R., Hauck, E., Hsiao, E., & pulmonary disease. Archives of Internal Medicine,
MacIntyre, N. (2003). Cognitive and psychological 143, 1941–1947.
outcomes of exercise in a 1-year follow-up study Hjalmarsen, A., Waterloo, K., Dahl, A., Jorde, R., &
of patients with chronic obstructive pulmonary Viitanen, M. (1999). Effect of long-term oxygen
disease. Health Psychology, 22, 598–604. therapy on cognitive and neurological dysfunc-
Emery, C. F., Honn, V. J., Frid, D. J., Lebowitz, K. R., tion in chronic obstructive pulmonary disease.
& Diaz, P. T. (2001). Acute effects of exercise on European Neurology, 42, 27–35.
348 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Hoffstein, V., Viner, S., & Mateika, S. (1992). Treatment Lanfranchi, P., & Somers, V. K. (2001). Obstructive
of obstructive sleep apnea with nasal continu- sleep apnea and vascular disease. Respiratory
ous positive airway pressure. Patient compliance, Research, 2, 315–319.
perception of benefits, and side effects. American Liesker, J., Postma, D., Beukema, R., ten Hacken N.,
Review of Respiratory Diseases, 145, 841–845. van der Molen, T., Riemersma, R., et al. (2004).
Hopkins, R. O., & Herridge, M. S. (2006). Quality of Cognitive performance in patients with COPD.
life, emotional abnormalities, and cognitive dys- Respiratory Medicine, 98, 351–356.
function in survivors of acute lung injury/acute McArdle, N., Devereux, G., Heidarnejad, H.,
respiratory distress syndrome. Clinical Chest Engleman, H. M., Mackay, T. W., & Douglas, N. J.
Medicine, 27(4), 679–689. (1999). Long-term Use of CPAP Therapy for Sleep
Hopkins, R. O., Gale, S. D., & Weaver, L. K. (2006). Apnea/Hypopnea Syndrome. American Journal
Brain atrophy and cognitive impairment in sur- of Respiratory and Critical Care Medicine, 159(4),
vivors of Acute Respiratory Distress Syndrome. 1108–1114.
Brain Injury, 20(3), 263–271. Naegele, B., Thouvard, V., Pepin, J. L., Levy, P.,
Hopkins, R. O., Weaver, L. K., Collingridge, D., Bonnet, C., Perret, J. E., et al. (1995). Deficits of
Parkinson, R. B., Chan, K. J., & Orme, J. F., Jr. cognitive executive functions in patients with
(2005). Two-year cognitive, emotional, and qual- sleep apnea syndrome. Sleep, 18(1), 43–52.
ity of life outcomes in acute respiratory distress The National Emphysema Treatment Trial: How
syndrome. American Journal of Respiratory and strong is the evidence? (2003). New England
Critical Care Medicine, 171(4), 340–347. Journal of Medicine, 348(21), 2059–2073.
Hopkins, R. O., Weaver, L. K., Chan, K. J., & Orme, Ohayon, M. M., & Vecchierini, M. F. (2002). Daytime
J. F., Jr. (2004). Quality of life, emotional, and sleepiness and cognitive impairment in the elderly
cognitive function following acute respiratory population. Archives of Internal Medicine, 162,
distress syndrome. Journal of the International 201–208.
Neuropsychological Society, 10(7), 1005–1017. Prigatano, G., Parsons, O., Wright, E., Levin, D. C.,
Keenan, S. P., Burt, H., Ryan, F., & Fleetham, J. A. & Hawryluk, G. (1983). Neuropsychological test
(1994). Long-term survival of patients with obstruc- performance in mildly hypoxemic patients with
tive sleep apnea treated by uvulopalatopharyngo- chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. Journal of
plasty or nasal CPAP. Chest, 105, 155–159. Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, 108–116.
Knight, H., Millman, R. P., Gur, R. C., Saykin, A. J., Rennard, S. I. (2004). Treatment of stable chronic
Doherty, J. U., & Pack, A. I. (1987). Clinical sig- obstructive pulmonary disease. Lancet, 364(9436),
nificance of sleep apnea in the elderly. American 791–802.
Review of Respiratory Disease, 136(4), 845–850. Rothenhausler, H. B., Ehrentraut, S., Stoll, C.,
Kozora, E., Emery, C., Ellison, M., Wamboldt, F., Schelling, G., & Kapfhammer, H. P. (2001). The
Diaz, P., & Make, B. (2005). Improved neurobe- relationship between cognitive performance and
havioral functioning in emphysema patients fol- employment and health status in long-term sur-
lowing lung volume reduction surgery compared vivors of the acute respiratory distress syndrome:
with medical therapy. Chest, 128, 2653–2663. Results of an exploratory study. General Hospital
Kozora, E., Filley, C., Julian, L., & Cullum, C. Psychiatry, 23(2), 90–96.
(1999). Cognitive functioning in patients with Salorio, C. F., White, D. A., Piccirillo, J., & Uhles, M.
chronic obstructive pulmonary disease and mild L. (2002). Learning, memory, and executive con-
hypoxemia compared with patients with mild trol in individuals with obstructive sleep apnea
Alzheimer disease and normal controls. Journal of syndrome. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neuropsychology, 24(1), 93–100.
Neurology, 12, 178–183. Sateia, M. J. (2003). Neuropsychological impair-
Kribbs, N. B., Pack, A. I., Kline, L. R., Smith, P. L., ment and quality of life in obstructive sleep apnea.
Schwartz, A. R., Schubert, N. M., et al. (1993). Clinical Chest Medicine, 24(2), 249–259.
Objective measurement of patterns of nasal CPAP Strassels, S. A. (1999). Economic consequences of
use by patients with obstructive sleep apnea. chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. Current
American Review of Respiratory Diseases, 147, Opinion in Pulmonary Medicine, 5(2), 100–104.
887–895. Stuss, D., Peterkin, I., Guzman, D., Guzman, C., &
Krop, H., Block, A., & Cohen, E. (1973). Troyer, A. (1997). Chronic obstructive pulmonary
Neuropsychologic effects of continuous oxygen disease: Effects of hypoxia on neurological and
therapy in chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. neuropsychological measures. Journal of Clinical
Chest, 64, 317–322. and Experimental Neuropsychology, 19, 515–524.
Neuropsychological Effects of Hypoxia in Medical Disorders 349

Verstraeten, E., & Cluydts, R. (2004). Executive con- Weaver, T. E., Maislin, G., Dinges, D. F., Bloxham,
trol of attention in sleep apnea patients: Theoretical T., George, C. F., Greenberg, H., et al. (2007).
concepts and methodological considerations. Sleep Relationship between hours of CPAP use and
Medicine Reviews, 8, 257–267. achieving normal levels of sleepiness and daily
Verstraeten, E., Cluydts, R., Pevernagie, D., & functioning. Sleep, 30(6), 711–719.
Hoffmann, G. (2004). Executive function in sleep Young, T., Palta, M., Dempsey, J., Skatrud, J.,
apnea: Controlling for attentional capacity in Weber, S., & Badr, S. (1993). The occurrence of
assessing executive attention. Sleep, 27(4), 685–693. sleep-disordered breathing among middle-aged
Verstraeten, E., Cluydts, R., Verbraecken, J., & De adults. New England Journal of Medicine, 328(17),
Roeck, J. (1997). Psychomotor and cognitive per- 1230–1235.
formance in nonapneic snorers: Preliminary Zimmerman, M. E., Arnedt, J. T., Stanchina, M.,
fi ndings. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 84, Millman, R. P., & Aloia, M. S. (2006). Normalization
1211–1222. of memory performance and positive airway
Waldhorn, R. E., Herrick, T. W., Nguyen, M. C., pressure adherence in memory-impaired patients
O’Donnell, A. E., Sodero, J., & Potolicchio, S. J. with obstructive sleep apnea. Chest, 130(6),
(1990). Long-term compliance with nasal contin- 1772–1778.
uous positive airway pressure therapy of obstruc-
tive sleep apnea. Chest, 97, 33–38.
16

Diabetes and the Brain: Cognitive


Performance in Type 1 and Type 2 Diabetes
Augustina M. A. Brands and Roy P. C. Kessels

Neuropsychology traditionally examines cog- cause is an absolute deficiency of insulin secre-


nitive impairments due to neurological diseases tion. Individuals at increased risk for develop-
or psychiatric disorders. However, within the ing this type of diabetes can often be identified
field of internal medicine, neuropsychology has by serological evidence of an autoimmune path-
gained attention only recently. The aim of the ologic process occurring in the pancreatic
present chapter is to review the literature on islets and by genetic markers, and this form of
the neuropsychology of diabetes mellitus, one diabetes is often diagnosed during childhood
of the most common systemic diseases that is or early adulthood. In the other, much more
becoming increasingly more prevalent, not only prevalent category, type 2 diabetes, the cause
in older people but also in younger adults. First, is a combination of resistance to insulin action
we briefly introduce the diagnostic criteria for and an inadequate compensatory insulin secre-
diabetes type 1 and 2, their prevalence and treat- tory response. In the latter category, a degree of
ment options, and possible complications of this hyperglycemia sufficient to cause pathologic and
disease. Subsequently, the effects of diabetes on functional changes in various target tissues, but
the brain will be discussed, highlighting neu- without clinical symptoms may be present for a
roimaging findings, as well as neuropsycholog- long period of time before diabetes is detected.
ical correlates of diabetes type 1 and 2 and the During this asymptomatic period, it is possi-
neuropsychiatric consequences. Furthermore, ble to demonstrate an abnormality in carbohy-
we discuss possible underlying mechanisms of drate metabolism by measurement of plasma
brain dysfunction and cognitive impairment in glucose in the fasting state or after a challenge
diabetes. Finally, we will focus on the clinical with an oral glucose load (American Diabetes
implications of these findings and discuss rele- Association, 2005). Type 2 diabetes is generally
vant issues that must be addressed in neuropsy- more prevalent in older adults.
chological examination of these patients. Assigning a type of diabetes to an individual
often depends on the circumstances present at
the time of diagnosis, and many diabetic indi-
Diabetes: Some General Facts viduals do not easily fit into a single class. For
example, a woman with gestational diabetes
Classification and Prevalence
mellitus (GDM), which may be present during
Diabetes mellitus is a common metabolic disease pregnancy, may continue to be hyperglycemic
and is characterized by high blood glucose lev- after delivery and may be determined to have,
els (hyperglycemia). The vast majority of cases in fact, type 2 diabetes. Alternatively, a person
of diabetes fall into two broad etiopathogenetic who acquires diabetes because of large doses of
categories. In one category, type 1 diabetes, the exogenous steroids may become normoglycemic

350
Diabetes and the Brain: Cognitive Performance in Type 1 and Type 2 Diabetes 351

once the glucocorticoids are discontinued, but This group is defined as having impaired FPG.
then may develop diabetes many years later The categories of FPG values are as follows:
after recurrent episodes of pancreatitis. The
global prevalence of diabetes in adults was • FPG < 100 mg/dl (5.6 mmol/l) = normal
estimated to be 2.8% in 2000 and is estimated fasting glucose;
to rise to 4.4% by the year 2030 (Wild et al., • FPG 100–125 mg/dl (5.6–6.9 mmol/l) = IFG
2004). Worldwide, the number of patients is (impaired fasting glucose);
expected to increase from 171 million in 2000 • FPG ≥ 126 mg/dl (7.0 mmol/l) = provisional
to 366 million in 2030, based solely on demo- diagnosis of diabetes.
graphic changes (Wild et al., 2004). About 90%
of these diabetic patients will suffer from type The corresponding categories when the OGTT
2 diabetes. is used are the following:

• 2-hour postload glucose < 140 mg/dl (7.8


Diagnosis of Diabetes mmol/l) = normal glucose tolerance;
• 2-hour postload glucose 140–199 mg/dl
In diagnosing diabetes, physicians primarily
(7.8–11.1 mmol/l) = IGT (impaired glucose
depend upon the results of specific glucose tests
tolerance);
(American Diabetes Association, 2005). However,
• 2-hour postload glucose ≥ 200 mg/dl (11.1
test results are just part of the information that
mmol/l) = provisional diagnosis of diabetes.
supports the diagnosis of diabetes. Some people
who are significantly ill will have transient prob- Patients with IFG or IGT are referred to as hav-
lems with elevated blood sugars that return to ing “prediabetes” indicating the relatively high
normal after the illness has resolved. In addition, risk for eventual development of diabetes in
medication may alter blood glucose levels, most these patients. In the absence of pregnancy, IFG
commonly steroids and certain diuretics. The two and IGT are not clinical entities in their own
main tests used to measure the presence of blood right, but rather risk factors for future diabetes,
sugar problems are (1) the direct measurement of as well as for cardiovascular disease. IFG and
fasting plasma glucose (FPG) levels in the blood IGT are associated with the so-called metabolic
during an overnight fast, and (2) measurement syndrome, which includes obesity (especially
of the body’s ability to appropriately handle the abdominal or visceral obesity), dyslipidemia, and
excess sugar presented after consuming a high hypertension. Medical nutrition therapy aimed
glucose drink. An oral glucose tolerance test at producing 5%–10% loss of body weight, exer-
(OGTT) requires that a person being tested per- cise, and certain pharmacological agents prevent
forms the test in a fasting state. An initial blood or delay the development of diabetes in people
glucose level is drawn and then the person is with IGT; the potential impact of such interven-
given a “glucola” bottle containing a high amount tions to reduce cardiovascular risk, however, has
of sugar (75 g of glucose). The person then has not been examined to date (American Diabetes
his or her blood tested again 30 minutes, 1 hour, Association, 2005).
2 hours, and 3 hours after drinking the high glu-
cose drink. In a person without diabetes, the
Complications and Treatment Goals
glucose levels in the blood rise after intake, but
rapidly fall back to normal, because insulin is It is well known that both type 1 and type 2
produced in response to the glucose that lowers diabetes can result in several complications in
the blood glucose. In a diabetic person, glucose the long term. These include retinopathy with
levels rise stronger than normal after intake and potential loss of vision, nephropathy leading
normalize much slower or not at all. In this case, to renal failure, peripheral neuropathy with
insulin is either not produced, or is produced risk of foot ulcers, amputations, and Charcot
but the cells of the body do not respond to it. In joints, and autonomic neuropathy causing gas-
an intermediate group of people, glucose levels, trointestinal, genitourinary, and cardiovascular
although not meeting criteria for diabetes, are symptoms, as well as sexual dysfunction. The
nevertheless too high to be considered normal. characteristic clinical signs and symptoms, as
352 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

well as the techniques to diagnose these compli- the brain. Symptoms of hypoglycemia include
cations, are well established (American Diabetes hunger, nervousness and shakiness, perspira-
Association, 2002). Patients with diabetes also tion, dizziness or light-headedness, sleepiness,
have an increased incidence of atherosclerotic confusion, difficulty speaking, feeling anxious
cardiovascular, peripheral arterial, and cerebro- or weak, headache, lack of energy, and an inabil-
vascular disease. Hypertension and abnormali- ity to concentrate. Prolonged severe hypoglyce-
ties of lipoprotein metabolism are often found mia may result in a hypoglycemic coma.
in people with diabetes. The development of Symptoms of hyperglycemia are the same
these complications is dependent on the dura- as those of untreated diabetes: thirst, frequent
tion of diabetes and the level of metabolic con- urination, vomiting, drowsiness, abdominal
trol and are associated with increased mortality. pain and pain in the legs, fatigue, impairment
Since both randomized trials and large cohort of cognitive function, depressive mood, and
studies have shown that good control of blood anxiety. Prolonged severe hyperglycemia may
glucose levels is associated with reduced risk of result in ketoacidosis. Acute hypo- and hyper-
these complications (DCCT Research Group, glycemia have disruptive effects on the central
1996; Gaede et al., 2003; Reichard & Rosenqvist, nervous system (CNS) and, as a result, on cog-
1989), current treatment is aimed at obtaining nitive functioning (for a review see Weinger &
and maintaining normal glucose levels. Jacobson, 1998).
Relatively less is known about the slowly
Acute Effects developing end-organ damage to the CNS that
may present itself as electrophysiological and
In contrast to type 1 diabetes, type 2 diabetes is structural changes and impairment of cognitive
a disease of slow onset. Initially, compensatory functioning. These cerebral complications of
increases in insulin secretion (hyperinsuline- both type 1 and type 2 diabetes are referred to
mia) maintain normal glucose concentrations as “diabetic encephalopathy,” a concept intro-
by counteracting the reduced sensitivity of tis- duced several decades ago (Reske-Nielsen et al.,
sues to insulin. The aim of treatment is to 1965). In the next paragraphs, we will highlight
maintain normal glucose levels in order to pre- some of the recent findings related to diabetic
vent the occurrence of aforementioned compli- encephalopathy.
cations. In patients with type 2 diabetes, this
treatment initially consists of dietary restric-
tions and exercise. Oral hypoglycemic drugs or Long-Term Effects on the Brain
insulin injections are prescribed in later stages. Cognitive Performance in Adults
Patients with type 1 diabetes are treated with Type 1 Diabetes
with exogenous insulin. Unfortunately, even
with repeated injections or subcutaneously As early as 1922, it was recognized that patients
implanted insulin pumps, these treatments with diabetes may suffer from cognitive perfor-
cannot fully compensate for the tightly regu- mance deficits (Miles & Root, 1922). Since then,
lated insulin secretions of a normally function- middle-aged individuals with type 1 diabetes
ing pancreas. Therefore, individuals with type have been reported to show deficits on a wide
1 diabetes may experience fluctuations in blood range of neuropsychological tests compared
glucose levels throughout the day, ranging from to age-matched controls. Some studies report
low blood glucose levels (hypoglycemia) to high impairments on tests relying on problem-solving
blood glucose levels (hyperglycemia). These skills (Deary et al., 1993), whereas other stud-
fluctuations depend upon the timing, type, ies report deficiencies in psychomotor efficiency
dose of insulin administration, the quantity (Ryan et al., 1992) or memory and learning
and nutritional content of food ingested, and (Ryan & Williams, 1993; Sachon et al., 1992) or
the amount of physical activity. Since normal find no difference at all (Wredling et al., 1990).
cerebral functioning is dependent upon suffi- Although the results of these studies are rela-
cient levels of continuous circulating glucose, tively heterogeneous with respect to the severity
these fluctuations can affect functioning of and nature of the affected cognitive domains,
Diabetes and the Brain: Cognitive Performance in Type 1 and Type 2 Diabetes 353

a recent meta-analysis clearly shows that cog- (d = –0.4). No differences in performance


nitive function is mildly impaired in patients could be demonstrated between patients with
with type 1 diabetes relative to controls, mainly and without recurrent severe hypoglycemic
reflecting as slowing of mental speed and a episodes.
diminished mental flexibility (Brands et al.,
2005, see Figure 16-1). Meta analyses on case-
Cognitive Performance in Older People
control studies report findings in terms of effect
with Type 1 Diabetes
sizes (Cohen’s d) that is, the standardized dif-
ference between the experimental and the com- Thus far, all studies addressing cognition in type
pared group (Cohen, 1988). This effect size 1 diabetes examined cognition in children or
provides information about how large a differ- young adults. However, it could be hypothesized
ence is evident across all studies. Compared to that the effects of type 1 diabetes on cognition
nondiabetic controls, the type 1 diabetic group might be more pronounced in older individuals.
demonstrated a significant overall lowered This hypothesis was recently tested in a study
performance (Cohen’s d = –0.3) as well as an in which cognitive performance was assessed in
impairment on the cognitive domains of over- 40 patients with type 1 diabetes with a mean age
all intelligence (d = –0.65), implicit memory of 60 years and 40 matched controls. Cognitive
(d = –0.75), speed of information processing performance was related to cerebral magnetic
(d = –0.26), psychomotor efficiency (d = –0.56), resonance imaging (MRI) findings and mea-
motor speed (d = –0.7), visual (d = –0.41) sures of psychological well-being (Brands et al.,
and sustained attention (d = –0.31), cognitive 2006). Cognition was studied by extensive neu-
flexibility (d = –0.54) and visual perception ropsychological assessment of 11 tests tapping

Overall cognition p < 0.001


Intelligence
Crystalized intelligence p < 0.01
Fluid intelligence p < 0.01
Learning and memory
Working memory
Cognitive domains (number of patients included)

Visual learning and immediate memory


Verbal learning and immediate memory
Visual delayed memory
Verbal delayed memory
Psychomotor activity and speed of information processing
Psychomotor efficiency p < 0.05
Speed of information processing p < 0.05
Motor speed
Attention
Visual attention p < 0.001
Sustained attention p < 0.01
Divided attention
Selective attention
Cognitive flexibility p < 0.001
Visual perception p < 0.001
Language
–1.6 –1.4 –1.2 –1.0 –0.8 –0.6 –0.4 –0.2 0 0.2 0.4 0.6
Standardized effect size (Cohen’s d) and 95% Confidence Interval

Figure 16–1. Effect sizes (Cohen’s d) derived from a meta-analysis on cognitive functioning in type 1
diabetes. Adapted from Brands et al. (2005).
354 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

the major cognitive domains in both a verbal 1 are within the normal range (Brands et al.,
and a nonverbal way. Psychological well-being 2006) in that similar rates of (silent) infarcts
was assessed by two questionnaires. Both corti- and white matter lesion (WML) severity have
cal and subcortical atrophy and periventricular been reported in random samples from the gen-
and deep white matter abnormalities were rated eral population of the same age group (de Leeuw
on MRI scans, using standardized rating scales. et al., 2001; Vermeer et al., 2003; Ylikoski et al.,
The diabetic group performed significantly 1995). Other studies of brain MRI in type 1
worse than controls only on speed of informa- diabetes involved younger patients (average
tion processing (Cohen’s d < 0.4). Neither sig- ages 25–40 years) with an earlier disease onset
nificant intergroup differences were found on (average ages at onset 10–18 years) (Dejgaard
any of the MRI rating scales nor could cogni- et al., 1991; Ferguson et al., 2003; Lobnig et al.,
tive performance be related to MRI findings. 2006; Lunetta et al., 1994; Musen et al., 2006;
Type 1 diabetic patients reported significantly Perros et al., 1997; Wessels et al., 2006b; Yousem
more depressive and cognitive complaints, but et al., 1991). Some studies compared measures of
the depressive symptoms did not correlate with cerebral atrophy (Ferguson et al., 2003; Lobnig
cognitive performance. et al., 2006; Lunetta et al., 1994; Musen et al.,
The pattern as well as the severity of cognitive 2006; Perros et al., 1997; Wessels et al., 2006b;
dysfunction is comparable with the results of Yousem et al., 1991) or WML severity (Dejgaard
the meta-analysis, which included only studies et al., 1991; Lobnig et al., 2006; Yousem et al.,
using adults under the age of 50. These results 1991) in type 1 diabetic patients with control
suggest that there is only limited progression subjects. One study reported a 3% decrease
of cognitive deterioration over time in patients in total cerebral volume (Lobnig et al., 2006).
with type 1 diabetes. This is in line with the very The most detailed studies on cerebral atrophy
limited progression of cognitive deterioration so far reported modest regional reductions in
reported in very few longitudinal studies on cortical gray matter density, using voxel based
cognitive functioning in type 1 diabetic patients morphometry (Musen et al., 2006; Wessels
that are available (DCCT Research Group, et al., 2006b).
1996; Diabetes Control and Complications A study on WML in type 1 diabetic patients
Trial/Epidemiology of Diabetes Interventions with advanced microvascular complications
and Complications Study Research Group et al., did not observe WML in any of these patients
2007; Reichard et al., 1996; Ryan, 2003). Future (Yousem et al., 1991). Others reported WML in
studies should examine the course of cogni- a majority of type 1 diabetic patients, but not
tive impairment in older people with diabetes in controls (Dejgaard et al., 1991). Neither a
type 1 in more detail in order to draw firm history of severe hypoglycemia, nor the pres-
conclusions. ence of retinopathy was associated with cere-
bral atrophy or WML, although retinopathy
was associated with an increased occurrence
MRI Findings in Type 1 Diabetes
of so-called enlarged perivascular spaces
The relation between cognitive impairments (Ferguson et al., 2003, 2005). Earlier onset
and structural changes in the brain has not been of diabetes (<7 years) was associated with a
investigated systematically. Thus far, only a few higher ventricular volume, but not with corti-
MRI studies of the brain in patients with type cal atrophy or WML severity (Ferguson et al.,
1 diabetes have been published (e.g., Dejgaard 2005, but see Wessels et al., 2006b). The com-
et al., 1991; Ferguson et al., 2003, 2005; Lobnig bined results of these papers indicate that MRI
et al., 2006; Lunetta et al., 1994; Musen et al., changes in the brain of patients with type 1
2006; Perros et al., 1997; Wessels et al., 2006b; diabetes are relatively subtle, and may be more
Yousem et al., 1991). Radiological abnormali- pronounced in patients with an early diabetes
ties involving the subcortical white matter and onset. Probably, more sensitive neuroimaging
both cortical and subcortical atrophy have been paradigms, such as functional magnetic reso-
reported, but it has been suggested that the nance imaging (fMRI) or single photon emis-
abnormalities based on MRI in diabetes type sion computed tomography (SPECT) could be
Diabetes and the Brain: Cognitive Performance in Type 1 and Type 2 Diabetes 355

more informative with respect to more subtle domains, or that the observed cognitive changes
changes in brain functioning. For example, a follow a specific pattern in which impairments
different pattern of brain activation in a group in one area, such as speed of information pro-
of patients with type 1 diabetes was found dur- cessing, explain the performance decline in
ing a cognitively demanding working-memory other cognitive domains. Studies which exam-
task (Wessels et al., 2006a); patients with dia- ined relations between different disease variables
betic retinopathy showed significantly less and cognitive functioning showed that patients
deactivation in the anterior cingulate and the with worse glycemic control were more likely to
right orbital frontal gyrus than those without show cognitive deficits (Strachan et al., 1997). A
retinopathy, which may reflect a compensatory number of other factors, such as depression, and
mechanism, although cognitive functioning cardiovascular or cerebrovascular disease, are
did not differ between both the groups (Wessels also thought to increase cognitive deficits (Awad
et al., 2006a). et al., 2004). Moreover, although most studies
did not use age as an independent predictor, the
largest effect of type 2 diabetes on cognitive func-
Cognition in Type 2 Diabetes
tion was observed in studies in which patients
Numerous studies have been conducted to were older (Ryan & Geckle, 2000). As they age,
evaluate the neuropsychological functioning people with type 2 diabetes develop other related
of individuals diagnosed with type 2 diabetes. pathologies such as hypertension, atherosclero-
These studies differ with respect to demographic sis, macro- and microvascular disease that pro-
characteristics of the research participants, duce further cognitive deficits, which become
such as age or gender distribution, or diabetic most apparent in later life.
characteristics, such as diabetes duration, Two recent studies assessed cognitive perfor-
treatment regime, and comorbidity or compli- mance in elderly patients with type 2 diabetes in
cations (Awad et al., 2004; Stewart & Liolitsa, relation to MRI findings (Manschot et al., 2006;
1999). Studies also varied in neuropsychological van Harten et al., 2007). Both studies revealed
domains covered and in methodological designs that type 2 diabetes was associated with both
(cross-sectional or longitudinal, clinic or popu- cognitive dysfunction and MRI abnormalities.
lation based). The most common finding is that Patients with type 2 diabetes particularly per-
type 2 diabetes is associated with mild to mod- formed worse on the domains of attention and
erate impairments of cognitive functioning with executive functioning, information processing
lowered performance on speed of information speed, and memory.
processing, episodic memory and, although less Some data indicate that DM may place elderly
consistently, on mental flexibility (Awad et al., at risk for dementia, both Alzheimer’s disease
2004; Stewart & Liolitsa, 1999). Effect sizes are and vascular dementia. A report by Biessels
small to moderate (0.4–0.8). Cognitive domains et al. (2006) reported that the incidence of
that are less likely to show significant differ- dementia was higher in individuals with diabe-
ences between type 2 diabetic patients and con- tes than in those without diabetes in seven of
trols include visuospatial processing, auditory ten studies reviewed. This high risk included
or visual attention, long-term semantic knowl- both Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia
edge and language abilities (Awad et al., 2004). (8 of 13 studies and 6 of 9 studies respectively).
Apart from differences in design, inconsis- Detailed data on modulating and mediating
tencies across studies may also be related to the effects of glycemic control, microvascular com-
neuropsychological sensitivity of test measures plications, and comorbidity (e.g., hypertension
used. Also, in several studies a selection was and stroke) were generally absent.
made with respect to the cognitive domains that
were assessed and a rationale for that selection
MRI in Type 2 Diabetes
was not always provided (Awad et al., 2004). The
question arises whether inconsistent findings Only few studies have specifically addressed
are due to the fact that type 2 diabetes causes a brain MRI abnormalities in patients with type
global rather nonspecific decline in all cognitive 2 diabetes. These studies indicate that modest
356 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

cortical and subcortical atrophy and symptom- attention and cognitive flexibility to learning
atic or asymptomatic infarcts are more common and retrieval of recently learned information. In
in type 2 diabetic patients than in controls (Araki contrast, patients did not show lowered perfor-
et al., 1994; den Heijer et al., 2003; Longstreth mance on tasks measuring general intelligence
et al., 1998; Schmidt et al., 2004; Vermeer et al., or visuoconstructive skills. A recent review of
2003). Generally, cognitive impairments in type results in type 2 diabetes (Awad et al., 2004)
2 diabetes are associated with MRI abnormali- compared to the results of the meta-analyses
ties, more specifically with WMLs, infarcts, and in type 1 diabetes (Brands et al., 2005) suggests
atrophy (Garde et al., 2000; Gunning-Dixon & that the profile of cognitive dysfunction in type 1
Raz, 2003; Manschot et al., 2006; van Harten diabetes is not exactly similar to that seen in
et al., 2007). The CT and MRI studies reviewed type 2 diabetes. Especially learning and mem-
in a recent review by van Harten and colleagues ory seem to be relatively spared in type 1 diabe-
(2006) show a relation between diabetes and tes compared to type 2 diabetes.
cerebral atrophy and lacunar infarcts but no These findings suggest that patients with dia-
consistent relation with WMLs. betes have difficulty with cognitively demand-
The relation between cerebral atrophy and ing, effortful tasks (Kahneman, 1973). Central
hypertension in type 2 diabetic patients is less to the mental-effort hypothesis is a limited
clear, one study reporting no effects of adjust- attentional capacity. Similar ideas have been
ment for hypertension (den Heijer et al., 2003), formulated to describe the pattern of cognitive
whereas other studies indicated that hyper- deficits seen in normal aging processes (Park
tension appeared to be a major determinant et al., 2002; Salthouse, 2004). Germane to this
of cerebral atrophy in type 2 diabetic patients issue is also a large body of literature on cog-
(Schmidt et al., 2004; van Harten et al., 2007). nitive functioning in aging persons, indicating
Results of previous studies on the association that older persons display deficits in cognitive
between type 2 diabetes and WMLs are incon- domains other than memory and processing
sistent. The majority of these studies involved speed, namely attention and executive func-
selected subgroups of patients with, for example, tion (for overview see Braver & Barch, 2002;
clinically manifesting cardiovascular disease Tisserand & Jolles, 2003). That is to say, the cog-
or stroke (Manolio et al., 1994; Schmidt et al., nitive problems we see in diabetes mimic the
1992) and used relatively insensitive measures patterns of cognitive aging and could be viewed
to rate WMLs. Some of these studies in patients as “accelerated aging.”
with vascular disease reported relatively more
severe WML in patients with type 2 diabetes
Psychiatric Comorbidity in Diabetes
(Schmidt et al., 1992), whereas others did not
Mellitus
find statistically significant relations between
type 2 diabetes and WMLs (Manolio et al., The prevalence of psychiatric disorders, in par-
1994). The study on WMLs in older people with ticular depression and anxiety disorders which
diabetes that involved the largest cohort and the are known to have a negative effect on cogni-
most detailed rating method thus far reported tion, is increased in type 1 diabetes. In a recent
no reliable effect of diabetes on periventricu- meta-analysis, odds ratios and prevalence of
lar white matter lesion (PWML; Schmidt et al., depression were estimated for both type 1
2004). and 2 diabetes, from 42 studies having a com-
bined sample size of 21,351 subjects (Anderson
et al., 2001). The main conclusion is that diabe-
A General Pattern of Cognitive
tes doubles the odds ratio. A difference in the
Dysfunction in Diabetes?
prevalence of depression in type 1 compared
Compared to nondiabetic controls, both to type 2 diabetes could not be established.
patients with type 1 and type 2 diabetes exhibit This increased prevalence of depression might
a lowered performance on a range of cognitive result from an inability to cope with the stress
tasks, varying from speed of information pro- associated with diabetes and its complicated
cessing or psychomotor efficiency, aspects of treatment that requires strict compliance, but
Diabetes and the Brain: Cognitive Performance in Type 1 and Type 2 Diabetes 357

neurophysiological alterations in serotonin and Generally, the percentage of type 2 diabetic


dopaminergic activity could also be involved patients reporting serious depressive symp-
(Broderick & Jacoby, 1988; Lackovic et al., toms or high levels of psychological distress is
1990). Disturbances in glucocorticoid metabo- relatively low, compared to what is reported in
lism may play an additional role, since several general in the literature (Anderson et al., 2001).
authors have mentioned a relation between Moreover, levels of psychological distress in type
type 1 diabetes and a dysregulation of the 2 diabetes were not related to levels of cognitive
hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis activity performance or MRI findings (Brands et al.,
(Prestele et al., 2003; Roy et al., 1991). 2007a; Gregg et al., 2000; Lowe et al., 1994). The
In all, the relation between cognition and concept of “vascular depression” is therefore
depression in diabetes is complex. On the one not useful in type 2 diabetic patients.
hand the co-occurrence of depression could
influence cognitive performance in patients
Risk Factors and Possible
with diabetes negatively, since depressive symp-
Pathophysiological Mechanisms
toms have been related to cognitive dysfunction
in some studies (Elderkin-Thompson et al., Taken together, the results of the studies out-
2003; Lockwood et al., 2002). On the other lined in this chapter indicate that cognitive defi-
hand, depression and cognitive dysfunction cits in diabetic patients are not merely caused by
could each be a different expression of the acute metabolic derangements or psychological
same underlying encephalopathy. Although it factors, but point to end-organ damage in the
is well known that the burden of a chronic ill- CNS (Gispen & Biessels, 2000). Although some
ness in general may result in elevated levels of uncertainty remains about the exact pathogene-
psychological distress, biomedical factors may sis, several mechanisms through which diabetic
also play a role, since it has been reported that encephalopathy may develop have now been
depressive symptoms are related to white matter identified more clearly.
abnormalities (Jorm et al., 2005) and severity of
diabetic complications (Leedom et al., 1991).
No Role for Hypoglycemia in the
MRI studies of patients with major depression
Pathogenesis
have found a higher prevalence of WMLs, par-
ticularly in participants with late-depression Until recently, most research on the patho-
onset (Videbech, 1997). It has been suggested physiological basis of diabetic encephalopathy
that late-onset depression could be regarded as in type 1 diabetes was aimed at the hypothesis
“vascular depression,” a late-onset subtype of that hypoglycemic events are the primary cause
depression that involves increased cardiovascu- of neurocognitive dysfunction. Several studies
lar risk factors and hyperintensities of deep white reported deleterious effects of repeated episodes
matter or subcortical gray matter (Alexopoulos of severe hypoglycemia on cognition (Bale,
et al., 1997). Others refer to the co-occurrence 1973; Sachon et al., 1992; Wredling et al., 1990).
of cognitive impairments, depressed mood and One study even reported severe deterioration
vascular dysfunction as “vascular dementia” in cognitive function and personality changes
(Baldwin et al., 2006) or “pseudodementia,” in five patients with diabetes (Gold et al., 1994).
that is, geriatric depression with reversible cog- However, in a large prospective study a total
nitive deficits (Alexopoulos et al., 1997). The of 1144 patients with type 1 diabetes enrolled
question of how psychological well-being is in the Diabetes Control and Complications
related to MRI abnormalities in type 2 diabe- Trial (DCCT) and its follow-up Epidemiology
tes has not been examined yet. Type 2 diabetic of Diabetes Interventions and Complications
patients report more subjective cognitive prob- (EDIC) study were examined on entry to the
lems and show higher levels of psychological DCCT (at mean age 27 years) and a mean of 18
distress than controls (Anderson et al., 2001), years later with the same comprehensive battery
but many somatic complaints reported by these of cognitive tests. Neither the frequency of severe
patients appear to be the result of diabetes itself hypoglycemia nor the previous treatment-group
and not from depression (Brands et al., 2007a). assignment was associated with decline in any
358 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

cognitive domain (DCCT Research Group, Interventions and Complications Study


1996; Diabetes Control and Complications Research Group et al., 2007). In line with this,
Trial/Epidemiology of Diabetes Interventions a recent study suggested a link between reduced
and Complications Study Research Group et al., cortical gray matter and increased severity of
2007). The combined results of all these stud- retinopathy in diabetic patients (Musen et al.,
ies provide no evidence for a linear relationship 2006). Importantly, microvascular abnormali-
between recurrent episodes of hypoglycemia ties (i.e., microaneurysms) are associated with
and permanent brain dysfunction. The reason similar patterns of cognitive deficits (namely,
for this may be that, despite the acute energy psychomotor slowing) in middle-aged adults
failure in the brain associated with hypogly- without diabetes (Wong et al., 2002). In line
cemia, there might be a period in which the with this, experimental models of diabetes have
CNS is resistant to hypoglycemia-induced demonstrated “neurotoxic” effects due to hyper-
damage (Chabriat et al., 1994). Th is “brain- glycemia (Gispen & Biessels, 2000). These toxic
damage-free-period” contrasts with immedi- effects directly affect brain tissue and can lead
ate brain damage caused by other acute effects to microvascular changes in the brain (Biessels
in the brain, such as hypoxia or ischemia et al., 2002a, 2002b). Cerebral microvascular
(cf. Chabriat et al., 1994). pathology in diabetes may result in a decrease
This, however, does not imply that low blood of regional cerebral blood flow and an alteration
glucose levels are always unrelated to neuro- in cerebral metabolism, which could partly
psychological function. Apart from the acute explain the occurrence of cognitive impairments
risk of a hypoglycemic coma, hypoglycemia (cf. Price et al., 2002).
contributes to feelings of psychological distress
and should therefore be avoided. Furthermore,
The Role of Atherosclerotic
it could be hypothesized that specific subgroups
Risk Factors
of diabetic patients (e.g., patients with micro-
vascular complications or young children) are Previous studies in type 1 and type 2 diabetes
more sensitive to the adverse effects of hypogly- do not invariably find an association between
cemia on the brain (Frier & Hilsted, 1985; Ryan, chronic hyperglycemia, as assessed by HbA1c
2006b). levels, and the severity of cognitive impairments
(e.g., see Brands et al., 2004, 2005; Stewart &
Liolitsa, 1999; Strachan et al., 1997). Moreover,
The Role of Hyperglycemia in the
subtle cognitive dysfunction may already
Pathogenesis of Cognitive Impairments
develop in “prediabetic stages,” such as impaired
Evidence for the involvement of microvascular glucose tolerance, or in newly diagnosed type 2
abnormalities in the pathogenesis of diabetic diabetic patients who have not yet been exposed
encephalopathy comes from several studies. For to long-term hyperglycemia (Kalmijn et al.,
example, in a study in which adults with type 1 1995; Vanhanen et al., 1998). One important
diabetes were followed up over a 7-year period, recent finding is that a mean duration of 30
it appeared that only patients with significant years in type 1 diabetes appears to have a sim-
proliferative retinopathy showed a decline in ilar effect on the brain as a mean duration of 7
measures of psychomotor efficiency (Ryan et al., years in type 2 diabetes (Brands et al., 2007b).
2003). Also, a higher degree of structural brain Thus, hyperglycemia is unlikely to be the only
damage has been reported in diabetic patients factor in the development of cognitive impair-
with clinically significant retinopathy (Ferguson ments in diabetes. The results of recent studies
et al., 2003; Wessels et al., 2006b). Furthermore, (e.g., Brands et al., 2007b; Helkala et al., 1995;
a recent study showed that higher glycated Jagusch et al., 1992; Manschot et al., 2006; van
hemoglobin values were associated with moder- den Berg et al., 2006; van Harten et al., 2007)
ate declines in motor speed (P = .001) and psy- suggest that atherosclerotic risk factors, such as
chomotor efficiency (P < .001), but not in any hypercholestrolemia, dyslipidemia, or a history
other cognitive domain (Diabetes Control and of macrovascular disease, could be also impor-
Complications Trial/Epidemiology of Diabetes tant factors.
Diabetes and the Brain: Cognitive Performance in Type 1 and Type 2 Diabetes 359

Although some studies failed to detect a type 1 or type 2 diabetes indicate that different
marked effect of hypertension on cognitive degrees of hyperglycemia, hyperinsulinemia,
functioning (e.g., Manschot et al., 2006), other and insulin resistance are associated with
studies report otherwise. Blood pressure eleva- clear-cut differential effects on insulin action
tions, particularly elevations in systolic blood in the brain (cf. Banks et al., 1997; Baskin et al.,
pressure, have been associated with cognitive 1985; Figlewicz et al., 1985; Marks & Eastman,
deficits (Ryan et al., 2003; van Harten et al., 1989). Differences in insulin action in the brain
2007), cortical atrophy (Schmidt et al., 2004), between patients with type 1 and type 2 diabe-
and WMLs (van Dijk et al., 2004). Hypertension tes may therefore explain part of the distinc-
may even have a synergistical effect with diabe- tive cognitive profi les of these two conditions.
tes since individuals with diabetes and elevated Acquisition of information over time (i.e.,
blood pressure tend to have the worst neurocog- learning) and consolidation of information for
nitive outcomes (Hassing et al., 2004; Schmidt long-term storage, for example, seem to be rela-
et al., 2004). tively spared in type 1 diabetes compared with
type 2 diabetes. These two cognitive domains
are critically dependent on the hippocampus
Another Factor to be Considered:
(Squire & Alvarez, 1995). Th is structure has a
Insulin
relatively high density of insulin receptors and
Insulin receptors are widely distributed in the may therefore be extra vulnerable for defects
brain. Classically, the CNS was thought to be an in insulin action. The observation that type 2
insulin-insensitive tissue, but in the late 1970s, diabetic patients have more difficulties with
it was demonstrated that insulin receptors were learning and retrieval of information than type
present throughout the CNS (Havrankova et al., 1 diabetic patients suggests that these cogni-
1979) with particular abundance in defi ned tive deficits are not merely another illustration
areas, such as the hypothalamus and the hip- of problems with tasks that require substantial
pocampus (Zhao et al., 2004). The cortically mental effort. It could also be interpreted as the
distributed insulin receptor has been shown to result of an additional specific pathway (e.g.,
be involved in cognitive functions. Emerging failing insulin signaling in the brain) leading
evidence has suggested that insulin signaling to cognitive impairment, especially in type 2
plays a role in synaptic plasticity by modulating diabetes.
activities of excitatory and inhibitory recep-
tors such as glutamate and GABA receptors,
Interaction of Diabetic Encephalopathy
and by triggering signal transduction cascades
with Aging?
leading to alteration of gene expression that is
required for long-term memory consolidation. Cerebral hypoperfusion and other vascu-
Furthermore, deterioration of insulin receptor lar changes become more prominent over the
signaling appears to be associated with aging- course of normal aging. Also, diabetes-related
related brain degeneration such as Alzheimer’s cognitive impairment may be primarily or at
disease and cognitive impairment in older per- least partly vascular in origin. Thus, one might
sons suffering from type 2 diabetes mellitus expect to find evidence of clinically significant
(Zhao et al., 2004). Insulin signaling seems to impairment in patients who have had diabetes
be especially disturbed in type 2 diabetes, as for a prolonged period of time, or interaction
type 1 diabetes is only associated with a fairly effects between diabetes and normal aging.
limited degree of insulin resistance (DeFronzo Indeed, several processes that have been impli-
et al., 2003). To exert its effects on the brain, cated in brain aging, including oxidative stress,
insulin has to be transported across the blood– accumulation of so-called advanced glycosila-
brain barrier, bind to cerebral insulin receptors tion end-products, microvascular dysfunction,
and convey its signal through an intracellu- and alterations in cerebral glucose and insulin
lar signaling cascade. Each of these processes metabolism that may be accelerated by diabetes
may be affected by diabetes. However, variable (Biessels et al., 2002b). Related to this, studies
results in animal studies using models of either suggest that cognitive deficits appear to be more
360 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

pronounced in those individuals with type 2 majority of diabetic patients, the question arises
diabetes who are older than 60–65 years of age as to what protects the brain from further vas-
(Ryan & Geckle, 2000). In a similar vein, age cular (and nonvascular) damage (Ryan, 2006a).
was found to be related to underperformance in The concept of “cognitive reserve” could be of
three out of five cognitive domains in patients interest here (Satz, 1993). The notion of reserve
with type 2 diabetes, and the interaction term against brain damage stems from the repeated
of age and group was significant for the domain observation in clinical research that there is
of memory (Manschot et al., 2006). This points no direct relationship between the degree of
to an interaction between diabetes and aging. It brain pathology or brain damage and the clin-
has been hypothesized that cognitive deficits in ical manifestation of that damage (Satz, 1993).
older patients with type 1 diabetes may also be It could be hypothesized that the brain has
more pronounced than those in younger type specific ways to preserve homeostasis that has
1 diabetic patients, but empirical findings do been demonstrated in other areas of research
not support this hypothesis (Brands et al., 2006; (Fehm et al., 2006). It has been argued that the
Diabetes Control and Complications Trial/ brain actively attempts to cope with or com-
Epidemiology of Diabetes Interventions and pensate for pathology, which could be based on
Complications Study Research Group et al., more efficient utilization of brain networks or
2007). on enhanced ability to recruit alternate brain
networks as needed (Stern, 2002).
It could also be speculated that diabetes acts
The Paradox: Diabetic Encephalopathy
as a so-called challenge factor (Satz, 1993) to the
versus Evidence of Cognitive Resilience
amount of brain reserve and, as such, makes an
Although the combined results of all studies individual more vulnerable to symptom onset
provide compelling evidence for the adverse or functional impairments if other insults occur.
effects of both type 1 and type 2 diabetes on For example, the observation that patients with
the brain, several observations are incongru- type 2 diabetes have a twofold increased risk for
ent with the concept of slowly progressing end- the development of dementia could be viewed as
organ damage. First, the results from the study the aggregate effect of two distinct processes.
on older patients with type 1 diabetes (Brands
et al., 2006) suggest that although patients suffer
Implications for Clinical Practice
from significant peripheral complications, there
is only a limited effect on the brain. Second, The concept of diabetic encephalopathy is
older patients with type 1 diabetes do not show probably not a useful one to describe the neu-
more pronounced cognitive deficits compared rocognitive complications of diabetes, given the
to younger patients (van den Berg et al., 2006). relative subtlety of impairments in most cases.
In this context, it is important to note that Ryan Nevertheless, the large variation in cognitive
(2006a) recently pointed out that the majority performance indicates that individual patients
of cross-sectional studies on type 1 diabetic may present with cognitive impairments that
patients, including patients with childhood are clinically relevant and hamper everyday
onset, typically have found quite modest cog- functioning. However, reliable and objective
nitive deficits that hardly meet the criteria for criteria to diagnose “diabetic encephalopathy”
“clinical relevance.” These observations suggest or diabetes-related cognitive deficits are lack-
that diabetic patients have a remarkable level ing, making these symptoms difficult to detect
of what might be best conceptualized as neu- in the individual patient. The findings described
rocognitive resilience (Ryan, 2006a). This line in this chapter suggest that especially patients
of reasoning counters the concept of “diabetic with type 2 diabetes are at risk for diabetes-
encephalopathy,” which predicts a gradual, but related cognitive decline, but other potentially
apparently relentless, decline over time as dura- confounding factors, such as poor glycemic
tion of hypoperfusion increases (de Jong, 1950; control or illness-related depression, should
Reske-Nielsen et al., 1965). If there is no inex- be always taken into account. However, neuro-
orable neurocognitive deterioration in the vast psychological assessment may help to unravel
Diabetes and the Brain: Cognitive Performance in Type 1 and Type 2 Diabetes 361

the underlying deficits that may or may not be American Diabetes Association. (2005). Diagnosis
related to the diabetic complications or brain and classification of diabetes mellitus. Diabetes
alterations. In general, sensitive neuropsycho- Care, 28, S37–S42.
logical tests relying on mental effort should be Anderson, R. J., Freedland, K. E., Clouse, R. E., &
Lustman, P. J. (2001). The prevalence of comorbid
used to detect the subtle cognitive changes that
depression in adults with diabetes: A meta-analysis.
can be expected in diabetes. Although precise
Diabetes Care, 24, 1069–1078.
diagnostic criteria still need to be developed, Araki, Y., Nomura, M., Tanaka, H., Yamamoto, H.,
we especially recommend the use of sensitive Yamamoto, T., & Tsukaguchi, I. (1994). MRI of
tests focusing on information processing speed, the brain in diabetes mellitus. Neuroradiology, 36,
mental flexibility, and learning and retrieval of 101–103.
information. Awad, N., Gagnon, M., & Messier, C. (2004). The rela-
tionship between impaired glucose tolerance, type 2
diabetes, and cognitive function. Journal of Clinical
Summary and Experimental Neuropsychology, 26, 1044–1080.
Both type 1 and type 2 diabetes mellitus are Baldwin, R. C., Gallagley, A., Gourlay, M., Jackson,
A., & Burns, A. (2006). Prognosis of late life
associated with altered brain function. This
depression: A three-year cohort study of outcome
chapter focused on the clinical and neuropsy-
and potential predictors. International Journal of
chological characteristics of altered CNS func- Geriatric Psychiatry, 21, 57–63.
tioning in both type 1 and in type 2 diabetes, as Bale, R. N. (1973). Brain damage in diabetes mellitus.
well as on the possible underlying mechanisms. British Journal of Psychiatry, 122, 337–341.
Type 1 diabetes is associated with modest cog- Banks, W. A., Jaspan, J. B., & Kastin, A. J. (1997).
nitive impairments in young to middle-aged Effect of diabetes mellitus on the permeability of
adult patients, which is related to long-term the blood-brain barrier to insulin. Peptides, 18,
glycemic control and its subsequent microvas- 1577–1584.
cular complications, but not to the occurrence Baskin, D. G., Stein, L. J., Ikeda, H., Woods, S. C.,
of severe hypoglycemic episodes. Similar deficits Figlewicz, D. P., Porte, D. J., et al. (1985). Genetically
obese Zucker rats have abnormally low brain insu-
were found in older people with type 1 diabetes,
lin content. Life Science, 36, 627–633.
but their magnitude appears to be comparable
Biessels, G. J., Staekenborg, S., Brunner, E., Brayne,
to the findings in younger adults. Also, in this C., & Scheltens, P. (2006). Risk of dementia in
particular study, these cognitive deficits were diabetes mellitus: A systematic review. Lancet
not accompanied by brain changes. However, Neurology, 5, 64–74.
a clear relationship between cognitive deficits Biessels, G. J., ter Laak, M. P., Hamers, F. P., &
and brain abnormalities does exist in older Gispen W. H. (2002a). Neuronal Ca(2+) dysregu-
patients with type 2 diabetes. In all, diabetes lation in diabetes mellitus. European Journal of
patients show diabetes-related cognitive decline. Pharmacology, 447, 201–209.
Although this is typically subtle in nature and Biessels, G. J., van der Heide, L. P., Kamal, A., Bleys,
does not match the originally proposed concept R. L., & Gispen, W. H. (2002b). Ageing and dia-
betes: Implications for brain function. European
of diabetic encephalopathy, these may be clin-
Journal of Pharmacology, 441, 1–14.
ically relevant in individual patients and can
Brands, A. M. A., Biessels, G. J., de Haan, E. H. F.,
be reliably assessed with sensitive, cognitively Kappelle, L. J., & Kessels, R. P. C. (2005). The Effects
demanding neuropsychological tests. of Type 1 Diabetes on Cognitive Performance:
A meta-analysis. Diabetes Care, 28, 726–735.
Brands, A. M. A., Biessels, G. J., Kappelle, L. J., de
References
Haan E. H. F., de Valk, H. W., Algra, A., et al.
Alexopoulos, G. S., Meyers, B. S., Young, R. C., (2007b). Cognitive functioning and brain MRI
Campbell, S., Silbersweig, D., & Charlson, M. in patients with type 1 and type 2 diabetes melli-
(1997). “Vascular depression” hypothesis. Archives tus: A comparative study. Dementia and Geriatric
of General Psychiatry, 54, 915–922. Cognitive Disorders, 23, 343–350.
American Diabetes Association. (2002). Standards Brands, A. M. A., Kessels, R. P. C., de Haan, E.
of medical care for patients with diabetes mellitus. H. F., Kappelle L. J., & Biessels, G. J. (2004).
Diabetes Care, 25, 213–229. Cerebral dysfunction in type 1 diabetes: Effects
362 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

of insulin, vascular risk factors and blood- Dejgaard, A., Gade, A., Larsson, H., Balle, V., Parving,
glucose levels. European Journal of Pharmacology, A., & Parving H. H. (1991). Evidence for diabetic
490, 159–168. encephalopathy. Diabetic Medicine, 8, 162–167.
Brands, A. M. A., Kessels, R. P. C., Hoogma, R. P. L. M., de Jong, R. N. (1950). The nervous system complica-
Henselmans, J. M. L., van der Beek Boter, J. W., tions in diabetes mellitus with special reference to
Kappelle, L. J., et al. (2006). Cognitive performance, cerebrovascular changes. Journal of Nervous and
psychological well-being and brain MRI in older Mental Disease, 111, 181–206.
patients with type 1 diabetes. Diabetes, 28, 726–735. de Leeuw, F. E., de Groot, J., Achten, E., Oudkerk,
Brands, A. M. A., van den Berg, E., Manschot, S. M., M., Ramos, L. M., Heijboer, R., et al. (2001).
Biessels, G. J., Kappelle, L. J., de Haan, E. H. F., Prevalence of cerebral white-matter lesions in
et al. (2007a). A detailed profi le of cognitive dys- elderly people: A population based magnetic
function and its relation to psychological distress resonance imaging study. The Rotterdam Scan
in patients with type 2 diabetes mellitus. Journal Study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and
of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13, Psychiatry, 70, 9–14.
288–297. den Heijer, T., Vermeer, S. E., van Dijk, E. J., Prins,
Braver, T. S., & Barch, D. M. (2002). A theory of N. D., Koudstaal, P. J., Hofman, A., et al. (2003).
cognitive control, aging cognition, and neu- Type 2 diabetes and atrophy of medial temporal
romodulation. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral lobe structures on brain MRI. Diabetologia, 46,
Reviews, 26, 809–817. 1604–1610.
Broderick, P. A., & Jacoby, J. H. (1988). Serotonergic Elderkin-Thompson, V., Kumar, A., Bilker, W. B.,
function in diabetic rats: Psychotherapeutic impli- Dunkin, J. J., Mintz, J., Moberg, P. J., et al. (2003).
cations. Biological Psychiatry, 24, 234–239. Neuropsychological deficits among patients with
Butters, M., Beers, S. R., Tarter, R. E., Edwards, K. L., & late-onset minor and major depression. Archives
Van Thiel, D. H. (Eds.) (2001). Medical neuropsy- of Clinical Neuropsychology, 18, 529–549.
chology: The impact of disease on behavior (2nd Fehm, H. L., Kern, W., & Peters, A. (2006). The selfish
ed.). New York: Kluwer Academic. brain: competition for energy resources. Progress
Chabriat, H., Sachon, C., Levasseur, M., Grimaldi, in Brain Research, 153, 129–140.
A., Pappata, S., Rougemont, D., et al. (1994). Ferguson, S. C., Blane, A., Perros, P., McCrimmon,
Brain metabolism after recurrent insulin induced R. J., Best, J. J., Wardlaw, J., et al. (2003). Cognitive
hypoglycemic episodes: A PET study. Journal ability and brain structure in type 1 diabetes:
of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 57, Relation to microangiopathy and preceding severe
1360–1365. hypoglycemia. Diabetes, 52, 149–156.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for Ferguson, S. C., Blane, A., Wardlaw, J., Frier, B. M.,
the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.).Hillsdale, NJ: Perros, P., McCrimmon, R. J., et al. (2005).
Erlbaum. Influence of an early-onset age of type 1 diabe-
DCCT Research Group. (1996). Effects of inten- tes on cerebral structure and cognitive function.
sive diabetes therapy on neuropsychological Diabetes Care, 28, 1431–1437.
function in adults in the Diabetes Control and Figlewicz, D. P., Dorsa, D. M., Stein, L. J., Baskin, D. G.,
Complications Trial. Annals of Internal Medicine, Paquette, T., Greenwood, M. R., et al. (1985). Brain
124, 379–388. and liver insulin binding is decreased in Zucker
Diabetes Control and Complications Trial/ rats carrying the ‘fa’ gene. Endocrinology, 117,
Epidemiology of Diabetes Interventions and 1537–1543.
Complications Study Research Group; Jacobson, Frier, B. M., & Hilsted, J. (1985). Does hypoglycemia
A. M., Musen, G., Ryan, C. M., Silvers, N., Cleary, aggravate the complications of diabetes? Lancet, 2,
P., et al. (2007). Long-term effect of diabetes and 1175–1177.
its treatment on cognitive function. New England Gaede, P., Vedel, P., Larsen, N., Jensen, G. V., Parving,
Journal of Medicine, 356, 1842–1852. H. H., & Pedersen, O. (2003). Multifactorial inter-
Deary, I. J., Crawford, J. R., Hepburn, D. A., Langan, vention and cardiovascular disease in patients
S. J., Blackmore, L. M., & Frier, B. M. (1993). with type 2 diabetes. New England Journal of
Severe hypoglycemia and intelligence in adult Medicine, 348, 383–393.
patients with insulin-treated diabetes. Diabetes, Garde, E., Mortensoen, E. L., Krabbe, K., Rostrup,
42, 341–344. E., & Larsson, H. B. (2000). Relation between age-
DeFronzo, R. A., Hendler, R., & Simonson, D. (2003). related decline in intelligence and cerebral white-
Insulin resistance is a prominent feature of insu- matter hyperintensities in healthy octogenarians:
lin-dependent diabetes. Diabetes, 31, 795–801. A longitudinal study. Lancet, 356, 628–634.
Diabetes and the Brain: Cognitive Performance in Type 1 and Type 2 Diabetes 363

Gispen, W. H., & Biessels, G. J. (2000). Cognition and Leedom, L., Meehan, W. P., Procci, W., & Zeidler,
synaptic plasticity in diabetes mellitus. Trends in A. (1991). Symptoms of depression in patients
Neuroscience, 23, 542–549. with type II diabetes mellitus. Psychosomatics, 32,
Gregg, E. W., Yaffe, K., Cauley, J. A., Rolka, D. B., 280–286.
Blackwell, T. L., & Narayan, K. M. (2000). Is Lobnig, B. M., Krömeke, O., Optenhostert-Porst, C.,
diabetes associated with cognitive impairment & Wolf, O. T. (2006). Hippocampal volume and
and cognitive decline among older women? cognitive performance in longstanding Type 1
Study of Osteoperotic Fractures Research Group. diabetic patients without macrovascular compli-
Archives of Internal Medicine, 160, 174–180. cations. Diabetic Medicine, 23, 32–39.
Gold, A. E., Deary, I. J., Jones, R. W., O’Hare, J. P., Lockwood, K. A., Alexopoulos, G. S., & van Gorp,
Reckless, J. P., & Frier, B. M. (1994). Severe deterio- W. G. (2002). Executive dysfunction in geriatric
ration in cognitive function and personality in five depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159,
patients with long-standing diabetes: A complica- 1119–1126.
tion of diabetes or a consequence of treatment? Longstreth, W. T., Jr., Bernick, C., Manolio, T. A.,
Diabetic Medicine, 11, 499–505. Bryan, N., Jungreis, C. A., & Price, T. R. (1998).
Gunning-Dixon, F. M., & Raz, N. (2003). Lacunar infarcts defined by magnetic resonance
Neuroanatomical correlates of selected execu- imaging of 3660 elderly people: The Cadiovascular
tive functions in middle-aged and older adults: Health Study. Archives of Neurology, 55,
A prospective MRI study. Neuropsychologia, 41, 1217–1225.
1929–1941. Lowe, L. P., Tranel, D., Wallace, R. B., & Welty, T. K.
Hassing, L. B., Hofer, S. M., Nilsson, L. G., Berg, (1994). Type II diabetes and cognitive function.
S., Pedersen, N. L., McClearn, G., et al. (2004). A population-based study of Native Americans.
Comorbid type 2 diabetes mellitus and hyperten- Diabetes Care, 17, 891–896.
sion exacerbates cognitive decline: Evidence from Lunetta, M., Damanti, A. R., Fabbri, G., Lombardo,
a longitudinal study. Age and Ageing, 33, 355–361. M., Di Mauro, M., & Mughini, L. (1994). Evidence
Havrankova, J., Roth, J., & Brownstein, M. J. (1979). by magnetic resonance imaging of cerebral altera-
Concentrations of insulin and insulin receptors tions of atrophy type in young insulin-dependent
in the brain are independent of peripheral insulin diabetic patients. Journal of Endocrinological
levels. Studies of obese and streptozotocin-treated Investigation, 17, 241–245.
rodents. Journal of Clinical Investigations, 64, Manolio, T. A., Kronmal, R. A., Burke, G. L., Poirier,
636–642. V., O’Leary, D. H., Gardin, J. M., et al. (1994).
Helkala, E. L., Niskanen, L., Viinamaki, H., Partanen, Magnetic resonance abnormalities and cardiovas-
J., & Uusitupa, M. (1995). Short-term and long- cular disease in older adults. The Cardiovascular
term memory in elderly patients with NIDDM. Health Study. Stroke, 25, 318–327.
Diabetes Care, 18, 681–685. Manschot, S. M., Brands, A. M. A., van der Grond,
Jagusch, W., Cramon, D. Y. V., Renner, R., & J., Kessels, R. P. C., Algra, A., Kappelle, L. J., et al.
Hepp, K. D. (1992). Cognitive function and (2006). Brain MRI Correlates of impaired cog-
metabolic state in elderly diabetic patients. nition in patients with type 2 diabetes mellitus.
Diabetes, Nutrition and Metabolism, 5, 265–274. Diabetes, 55, 1106–1113.
Jorm, A. F., Anstey, K. J., Christensen, H., de Plater, Marks, J. L., & Eastman, C. J. (1989). Effect of starva-
G., Kumar, R., Wen, W., et al. (2005). MRI hyper- tion on insulin receptors in rat brain. Neuroscience,
intensities and depressive symptoms in a com- 30, 551–556.
munity sample of individuals 60–64 years old. Miles, W. R., & Root, H. F. (1922). Psychologic tests
American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 699–704. applied in diabetic patients. Archives of Internal
Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. New Medicine, 30, 767–777.
Jersey, USA: Prentice-Hall Inc., Englewood Cliffs. Musen, G., Lyoo, I. K., Sparks, C. R., Weinger, K.,
Kalmijn, S., Feskens, E. J., Launer, L. J., Stijnen, T., & Hwang, J., Ryan, C. M., et al. (2006). Effects of type
Kromhout, D. (1995). Glucose intolerance, hyper- 1 diabetes on gray matter density as measured by
insulinaemia and cognitive function in a gen- voxel-based morphometry. Diabetes, 55, 326–323.
eral population of elderly men. Diabetologia, 38, Park, D. C., Lautenschlager, G., Hedden, T., Davidson,
1096–1102. N. S., Smith, A. D., & Smith, P. K. (2002). Models
Lackovic, Z., Salkovic, M., Kuci, Z., & Relja, M. of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult
(1990). Effect of long-lasting diabetes mellitus life span. Psychology and Aging, 17, 299–320.
on rat and human brain monoamines. Journal of Perros, P., Best, J. J. K., Deary, I. J., Frier, B. M., &
neurochemistry, 54, 143–147. Sellar, R. J. (1997). Brain abnormalities demon-
strated by magnetic resonance imaging in adult
364 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

IDDM patients with and without a history of Sachon, C., Grimaldi, A., Digy, J. P., Pillon, B.,
recurrent severe hypoglycemia. Diabetes Care, 20, Dubois, B., & Thervet, F. (1992). Cognitive func-
1013–1018. tion, insulin-dependent diabetes and hypoglyce-
Prestele, S., Aldenhoff, J., & Reiff, J. (2003). The HPA- mia. Journal of Internal Medicine, 231, 471–475.
axis as a possible link between depression, diabetes Salthouse, T. A. (2004). What and when of cognitive
mellitus and cognitive dysfunction. Fortschritte aging. Current Directions in Psychological Science,
der Neurologie-Psychiatrie, 71, 24–36. 13, 140–144.
Price, J. C., Kelley, D. E., Ryan, C. M., Meltzer, C. Satz, P. (1993). Brain reserve capacity on symptom
C., Drevets, W. C., Mathis, C. A., et al. (2002). onset after brain injury: A formulation and review
Evidence of increased serotonin-1A receptor bind- of evidence for threshold theory. Neuropsychology,
ing in type 2 diabetes: A positron emission tomog- 7, 273–295.
raphy study. Brain Research, 927, 97–103. Schmidt, R., Fazekas, F., Kleinert, G., Offenbacher,
Reichard, P., Pihl, M., Rosenqvist, U., & Sule, J. H., Gindl, K., Payer, F., et al. (1992). Magnetic
(1996). Complications in IDDM are caused by ele- Resonance Imaging signal hyperintensities in the
vated blood glucose level: The Stockholm Diabetes deep and subcortical white-matter. A comparative
Intervention Study (SDIS) at 10-year follow up. study between stroke patients and normal volun-
Diabetologia, 39, 1483–1488. teers. Archives of Neurology, 49, 825–827.
Reichard P., & Rosenqvist, U. (1989). Nephropathy is Schmidt, R., Launer, L. J., Nilsson, L. G., Pajak, A.,
delayed by intensified insulin treatment in patients Sans, S., Berger, K., et al. (2004). Magnetic res-
with insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus and reti- onance imaging of the brain in diabetes: The
nopathy. Journal of Internal Medicine, 226, 81–87. Cardiovascular Determinants of Dementia
Reske-Nielsen, E., Lundbaek, K., & Rafaelsen, O. J. (CASCADE) Study. Diabetes, 53, 687–692.
(1965). Pathological changes in the central and Squire, L. R., & Alvarez, P. (1995). Retrograde amne-
peripheral nervous system of young long-term sia and memory consolidation: A neurobiological
diabetics. Diabetologia, 1, 233–241. perspective. Current Opinions in Neurobiology, 5,
Roy, M., Collier, B., & Roy, A. (1991). Dysregulation of 169–177.
the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis and dura- Stern, Y. (2002). What is cognitive reserve? Theory
tion of diabetes. Journal of Diabetic Complications, and research application of the reserve concept.
5, 218–220. Journal of the International Neuropsychological
Ryan, C. M. (2005). Diabetes, ageing and cognitive Society, 8, 448–460.
decline. Neurobiology of Aging, 26S, S21–S25. Stewart, R., & Liolitsa, D. (1999). Type 2 diabetes
Ryan, C. M. (2006a). Diabetes and brain damage: mellitus, cognitive impairment and dementia.
More (or less) than meets the eye? Diabetologia, Diabetic Medicine, 16, 93–112.
49, 2229–2233. Strachan, M. W., Deary, I. J., Ewing, F. M., & Frier,
Ryan, C. M. (2006b). Why is cognitive dysfunction B. M. (1997). Is type II diabetes associated with an
associated with the development of diabetes early increased risk of cognitive dysfunction? A criti-
in life? The diathesis hypothesis. Pediatrics, 7, cal review of published studies. Diabetes Care, 20,
289–297. 438–445.
Ryan, C. M., & Geckle, M. O. (2000). Why is learning Tisserand, D. J., & Jolles, J. (2003). On the involve-
and memory dysfunction in Type 2 diabetes lim- ment of prefrontal networks in cognitive ageing.
ited to older adults? Diabetes Metabolism Research Cortex, 39, 1107–1128.
and Reviews, 16, 308–315. van den Berg, E., De Craen, A. J. M., Biessels, G. J.,
Ryan, C. M., Geckle, M. O., & Orchard, T. J. (2003). Gusselkloo, J., & Westendorp R. G. J. (2006). The
Cognitive efficiency declines over time in adults impact of diabetes mellitus on cognitive decline
with type 1 diabetes: Effects of micro- and macro- in the oldest of the old: A prospective population-
vascular complications. Diabetologia, 46, 940–948. based study. Diabetologia, 49, 2015–2023.
Ryan, C. M., & Williams, T. M. (1993). Effects van Dijk, E. J., Breteler, M. M., Schmidt, R., Berger,
of insulin-dependent diabetes on learning and K., Nilsson, L. G., Oudkerk, M., et al. (2004). The
memory efficiency in adults. Journal of Clinical association between blood pressure, hypertension,
and Experimental Neuropsychology, 15, 685–700. and cerebral white-matter lesions: Cardiovascular
Ryan, C. M., Williams, T. M., Orchard, T. J., & determinants of dementia study. Hypertension, 44,
Finegold, D. N. (1992). Psychomotor slowing is 625–630.
associated with distal symmetrical polyneuropa- Vanhanen, M., Koivisto, K., Kuusisto, J., Mykkanen,
thy in adults with diabetes mellitus. Diabetes, 41, L., Helkala, E. L., Hanninen, T., et al. (1988).
107–113. Cognitive function in an elderly population with
Diabetes and the Brain: Cognitive Performance in Type 1 and Type 2 Diabetes 365

persistent impaired glucose tolerance. Diabetes grey matter density on brain MRI in patients
Care, 21, 398–402. with diabetic retinopathy 3724. Diabetologia, 49,
van Harten, B., de Leeuw, F. E., Weinstein, H. C., 2474–2480.
Scheltens, P., & Biessels, G. J. (2006). Brain imag- Wild, S., Roglic, G., Green, A., Sicree, R., & King, H.
ing in patients with diabetes: A systematic review. (2004). Global prevalence of diabetes: Estimates for
Diabetes Care, 29, 2539–2548. the year 2000 and projections for 2030. Diabetes
van Harten, B., Oosterman, J., Muslimovic, D., Care, 27, 1047–1053.
van Loon, B. J., Scheltens, P., & Weinstein, H. C. Wong, T. Y., Klein, R., Sharrett, A. R., Nieto, F.
(2007). Cognitive impairment and MRI correlates J., Boland, L. L., Couper, D. J., et al. (2002).
in the elderly patients with type 2 diabetes melli- Retinal microvascular abnormalities and cog-
tus. Age and Ageing, 36, 164–170. nitive impairment in middle-aged persons: The
Vermeer, S. E., den Heijer, T., Koudstaal, P. J., Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Study.
Oudkerk, M., Hofman, A., & Breteler, M. M. B. Stroke, 33, 1487–1492.
(2003). Incidence and risk factors of silent brain Wredling, R., Levander, S., Adamson, U., & Lins, P. E.
infarcts in the population-based Rotterdam Scan (1990). Permanent neuropsychological impair-
Study. Stroke, 34, 392–396. ment after recurrent episodes of severe hypoglyce-
Videbech, P. (1997). MRI findings in patients with mia in man. Diabetologia, 33, 152–157.
affective disorder: A meta-analysis. Acta Acta Ylikoski, A., Erkinjuntti, T., Raininko, R., Sarna, S.,
Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 96, 157–168. Sulkava, R., & Tilvis, R. (1995). White-matter
Weinger, K., & Jacobson, A. M. (1998). Cognitive hyperintensities on MRI in the neurologically
impairment in patients with type 1 (insulin- nondiseased elderly. Analysis of cohorts of consec-
dependent) Diabetes Mellitus. CNS Drugs, 9, utive subjects aged 55 to 85 years living at home.
233–252. Stroke, 26, 1171–1177.
Wessels, A. M., Rombouts, S. A., Simsek, S., Kuijer, Yousem, D. M., Tasman, W. S., & Grossman, R. I.
J. P., Kostense, P. J., Barkhof, F., et al. (2006a). (1991). Proliferative retinopathy: Absence of
Microvascular disease in type 1 diabetes alters white-matter lesions at MR imaging. Radiology,
brain activation: a functional magnetic resonance 179, 229–230.
imaging study. Diabetes, 55, 334–340. Zhao, W. O., Chen, H., Quon, M. J., & Alkon, D. L.
Wessels, A. M., Simsek, S., Remeijnse, P. L., Veltman, (2004). Insulin and the insulin receptor in experi-
D. J., Biessels, G. J., Barkhof, F., et al. (2006b). mental models of learning and memory. European
Voxel-based morphometry demonstrates reduced Journal of Pharmacology, 490, 71–81.
17

Neuropsychological Aspects of HIV Infection


Steven Paul Woods, Catherine L. Carey, Jennifer E. Iudicello, Scott L.
Letendre, Christine Fennema-Notestine, and Igor Grant

Neurobiology of HIV: An opportunistic infections to which such cohorts


Overview were highly vulnerable (e.g., Jarvik et al.,
1988). However, it became quickly appar-
The human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) ent that a subset of HIV-infected individuals
is a lentivirus that can severely compromise with no apparent CNS opportunistic infec-
immune function by damaging cluster of differ- tions exhibited nonspecific evidence of neu-
entiation 4+ (CD4) lymphocytes (i.e., T-helper rodegeneration on MRI, such as white matter
cells), thereby increasing the risk of oppor- hyperintensities and enlarged ventricles and
tunistic infections and cancers. In addition subarachnoid spaces (e.g., Dal Pan et al., 1992).
to its striking effects on the immune system, Carefully controlled magnetic resonance (MR)
HIV can be highly neurovirulent. That is, the morphometric studies have since helped to
virus is capable of penetrating the blood–brain defi ne the regional distribution of these HIV-
barrier (BBB) early in the course of infection related abnormalities (e.g., Aylward et al.,
(Davis et al., 1992), most likely as a conse- 1993), revealing that the cerebral white matter
quence of the trafficking of infected circulat- and fronto-striato-thalamo-cortical circuits
ing white blood cells (e.g., monocytes) across are particularly vulnerable (see Figure 17–1;
the BBB (Haase, 1986). Once in the brain, HIV see also the color figure in the color insert sec-
does not infect neurons. Instead, HIV primar- tion). Indeed, HIV-infected individuals may
ily replicates in perivascular macrophages and evidence diff use white matter abnormalities,
microglia, which can subsequently fuse to form including nonspecific hyperintensities, which
multinucleated giant cells, a hallmark of HIV correspond to the postmortem markers of syn-
encephalitis. Moreover, HIV infection indi- aptodendritic injury severity (Archibald et al.,
rectly alters neural functioning by triggering 2004). More recent diff usion tensor imaging
a cascade of neurotoxic molecular events, such studies also reveal increased mean diff usivity
as the upregulation of chemokines (Gonzalez- and reduced fractional anisotropy (e.g., Pomara
Scarano & Martin-Garcia, 2005). The resultant et al., 2001), with the latter being particularly
HIV-associated neuronal and glial pathologies prominent in individuals with HIV-associated
are evident in as many as 50% of HIV-infected neurocognitive disorders (see Figure 17–2; see
persons (Budka, 2005), often taking the form also the color figure in the color insert sec-
of neuronal apoptosis and/or synaptodendritic tion). Although HIV-associated pathologies
injury (Ellis et al., 2007). are evident throughout the CNS, including the
Early magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) temporolimbic system, a large body of research
studies of individuals with HIV infection has focused specifically on the effects of HIV
focused on the central nervous system (CNS) in the frontal cortex and striatum (i.e., caudate

366
Neuropsychological Aspects of HIV Infection 367

Figure 17–1. Structural morphometry in an individual infected with HIV. These two coronal sections high-
light regions of abnormality in the white matter (shown in yellow), which may be related to markers of HIV
disease and neurobehavioral performance. Dark blue = cortex, light blue = subcortical gray, purple = sulcal/
subarachnoid CSF, red = ventricular fluid, yellow = abnormal white matter, dark gray = normal appearing
white matter, light gray = cerebellum, maroon = infratentorial CSF.

nucleus and putamen). For example, Wiley et neuropathogenesis of HIV (e.g., see Langford
al. (1998) reported that the HIV RNA viral load et al., 2005).
in the caudate nucleus was significantly higher
as compared to other brain regions in persons
Neuropsychological Profile of HIV
with HIV encephalitis. HIV infection is asso-
ciated with structural abnormalities in the Considering its neurovirulence, it is not surpris-
frontal cortices, including lower volumes (e.g., ing that cognitive deficits are a common feature
Jernigan et al., 2005) and neocortical thinning of HIV infection (Grant et al., 1987). Although
(e.g., Thompson et al., 2005). Moreover, MR the use of different diagnostic nomenclatures
spectroscopic markers of neuroinflammation complicates interpretation of the published
(e.g., elevated myoinositol and choline) are prevalence data, it is generally held that HIV-
evident in the striatum and frontal gray and associated neurocognitive disorders are evident
white matter in the early stages of HIV infec- in 30%–50% of persons living with HIV (see
tion, with more prominent neuronal damage Figure 17–3).
(i.e., decreased N-acetyl asparate) emerging Among these neuropsychologically impaired
in the same regions as the disease advances individuals, approximately one-half to two-
(Chang et al., 2005). Longitudinal studies show thirds are asymptomatic (e.g., Antinori et al.,
that declines in CD4 lymphocytes are associ- 2007; Grant et al., 2005), meaning that their cog-
ated with atrophy in the caudate nucleus (e.g., nitive deficits do not interfere with the indepen-
Stout et al., 1998). Finally, hypoperfusion and dent performance of instrumental activities of
increased blood oxygenation level dependent daily living (IADLs). According to the recently
(BOLD) signal are frequently observed bilater- revised nomenclature for HIV-associated neu-
ally in the caudate and prefrontal cortex (e.g., rocognitive disorders (HAND) developed
Chang et al., 2000). The apparent susceptibility by the Frascati Group (Antinori et al., 2007),
of the frontostriatal circuits to HIV-associated these individuals would be diagnosed with
neural injury may be a function of BBB per- Asymptomatic Neurocognitive Impairment
meability to HIV trafficking in these regions (ANI), provided they demonstrate impair-
(e.g., Berger & Arendt, 2000) or the relative ment (i.e., 1 SD or more below the normative
involvement of specific neuronal populations mean) in two or more cognitive domains that
(e.g., large spiny neurons) or neurotransmitter is not better explained by developmental and/or
systems (e.g., glutamate and dopamine) in the comorbid factors. The remaining proportion of
368 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Figure 17–2. Diff usion tensor images from Gongvatana et al. (2008) showing that individuals with HIV-
associated neurocognitive disorders (HAND) have lower fractional anisotropy in the anterior callosal region
(shown in blue) relative to HIV-infected comparison participants without HAND. Images are presented in
axial sections moving from inferior (upper left) to superior (lower right) slices.

10% declines. Table 17–1 displays the revised criteria


for HAND.
15% The epidemiology of MND is not well under-
NP normal
ANI stood, but the limited data available suggest that
50% the diagnosis is evident in about 5%–20% of
MND
25% HAD HIV-infected persons (Grant et al., 2005). The
incidence of HAD has declined as much as 50%
in the era of combination antiretroviral thera-
Figure 17–3. Pie chart displaying the estimated pies (cART), with recent estimates of 10.5/1000
prevalence of HIV-associated neurocognitive disor- persons per year (e.g., Sacktor et al., 2001).
ders, including neuropsychologically (NP) normal, Prevalence rates for HAD fall between 5% and
asymptomatic (i.e., subsyndromic) neuropsychologi- 15% of HIV-infected persons (e.g., Tozzi et al.,
cal impairment (ANI), Mild Neurocognitive Disorder 2005), with higher estimates among non-im-
(MND), and HIV-associated Dementia (HAD). munosuppressed persons in recent years (Grant
et al., 2005).
The course of HIV-associated neurocogni-
tive disorders is highly variable across individ-
individuals with HIV-associated neurocognitive uals (Antinori et al., 2007), particularly when
disorders evidence IADL declines, which may contrasted against traditional neurodegenera-
be subclassified as either Mild Neurocognitive tive diseases (e.g., Alzheimer’s disease). Many
Disorder (MND, which was formerly referred to persons with mild impairment do not progress
as Minor Cognitive-Motor Disorder [MCMD]) to MCD or HAD; likewise, partial—and even
or HIV-Associated Dementia (HAD). Diagnoses full—remission of HIV-associated neurocog-
of MND and HAD require the presence of HIV- nitive disorders in conjunction with effective
associated impairment in two or more cognitive cART is not uncommon (McArthur, 2004).
domains that results in a disruption of IADLs
(e.g., financial management); however, MND
Motor and Psychomotor Abilities
carries more mild cognitive and functional
deficits than HAD, which is typically accom- Gross motor abnormalities (e.g., parkinson-
panied by greater cognitive and functional ism, chorea, myoclonus, and dystonia) occur
Neuropsychological Aspects of HIV Infection 369

Table 17–1. Revised Research Criteria for HIV-Associated Neurocognitive Disorders (HAND; Antinori
et al., 2007)

HIV-associated Asymptomatic Neurocognitive Impairment (ANI)a


1. Acquired impairment in cognitive functioning, involving at least two ability domains, documented
by performance of at least 1 SD below the mean for demographically adjusted norms on standardized
neuropsychological tests.b
2. The cognitive impairment does not interfere with everyday functioning (e.g., work, home life, and social
activities).
3. The cognitive impairment does not meet criteria for delirium (e.g., clouding of consciousness is not a
prominent feature) or HAD.
4. There is no evidence of another preexisting cause for the ANI.c
HIV-associated Mild Neurocognitive Disorder (MND)a
1. Acquired impairment in cognitive functioning, involving at least two ability domains, documented by
performance of at least 1 SD below the mean for demographically adjusted norms on standardized neu-
ropsychological tests.b
2. The cognitive impairment contributes to at least mild interference in everyday functioning as evidenced
by at least one of the following:
a) Self-report of reduced mental acuity, inefficiency in work, homemaking, or social functioning.
b) Observation by knowledgeable others that the individual has undergone at least mild decline in
mental acuity with resultant inefficiency in work, homemaking, or social functioning.
3. The cognitive impairment does not meet criteria for delirium or HAD.
4. There is no evidence of another preexisting cause for the MND.c

HIV-associated Dementia (HAD) a


1. Marked acquired impairment in cognitive functioning, involving at least two ability domains (typically
the impairment is in multiple domains), documented by performance of at least 2 SD below the mean
for demographically adjusted norms on standardized neuropsychological tests.b (If neuropsychological
testing is not available, standard neurological evaluation and simple bedside testing may be used, but
this should be done according to the algorithm provided by Antinori, et al., 2007).
2. The cognitive impairment contributes to marked interference with everyday functioning (e.g., work,
home life, and social activities).
3. The pattern of cognitive impairment does not meet criteria for delirium; or, if delirium is present,
criteria for HAD were met on a prior examination when delirium was not present.
4. There is no evidence of another, preexisting cause for HAD.c
a
A diagnosis of HAND “in remission” may be applicable if the individual has a prior diagnosis of HAND, but does not
currently meet criteria.
b
The neuropsychological assessment must survey the ability areas of language, episodic memory, attention/working memory,
executive functions, information-processing speed, sensory-perception, and motor skills.
c
See Table 17–1A.

relatively infrequently, but can manifest in information-processing demands, particularly


individuals with advanced HIV disease, likely in more severe stages of illness (Reger et al.,
arising from nigrostriatal dopaminergic dysreg- 2002) and when controlled attentional processes
ulation (e.g., Tse et al., 2004). Bradykinesia (i.e., are required (e.g., choice versus simple reaction
slowed movement) is one of the earliest identi- time; see Martin et al., 1999). It has been pos-
fied and most striking features of neuroAIDS; ited that psychomotor slowing may mediate the
for example, tests of gait velocity (e.g., timed expression of HIV-associated neuropsycholog-
gait; Robertson et al., 2006) and fine motor ical deficits (Hardy & Hinkin, 2002), even for
speed and dexterity (e.g., Grooved Pegboard; ability areas that are not expressly speeded (e.g.,
Carey et al., 2004) are sensitive indicators of Becker & Salthouse, 1999). Such data highlight
HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders. the importance of accounting for motor and
Bradyphrenia, or the slowing of mental faculties, psychomotor slowing when interpreting perfor-
may be observed on tasks with a wide variety of mance on higher-cognitive functions.
370 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Attention, Working Memory, and early stages of HIV disease, increasing in prev-
Executive Functions alence and magnitude in persons with AIDS
(e.g., Heaton et al., 1995). Neuropsychological
Attention. Attentional deficits are well docu- studies of HIV infection have typically revealed
mented in HIV (Hardy & Hinkin, 2002) and these executive deficits using standard clinical
generally increase in magnitude with advancing measures of abstraction and problem solving
disease severity (Grant et al., 2005). Regarding (e.g., Category Test errors), cognitive flexibility
specific aspects of attention, several studies have and set-shifting (e.g., perseverative errors on the
demonstrated impairment in selective atten- Wisconsin Card Sort Test), and response inhi-
tion, including deficits on complex visual search bition (e.g., interference effects on the Stroop
and discrimination tasks (Hardy & Hinkin, Color-Word Test). In addition, deficits in both
2002), covert orienting (Maruff et al., 1995), and cognitive (e.g., Bartok et al., 1997) and social
global versus local visual processing (Martin (e.g., Benedict et al., 2000) planning may be evi-
et al., 1995) in HIV. Similarly, deficits in divided dent in HIV. More recently, research on HIV-
attention are observed in individuals with HIV associated executive dysfunction has focused
infection (e.g., Hinkin et al., 2000; cf. Law et al., on decision-making processes using the Iowa
1994), and are robust predictors of motor vehi- Gambling Task, which was adapted from
cle accidents (Marcotte et al., 2006). Although research on persons with ventromedial prefron-
several studies have reported sustained atten- tal lesions (e.g., Bechara et al., 1997). These early
tion deficits in HIV (e.g., Fein et al., 1995), it is studies indicate that HIV-infected individuals
unclear whether these deficits are mediated by are prone to selecting larger immediate rewards
psychomotor slowing. that are associated with more severe long-term
consequences as compared to smaller imme-
Working Memory. Given the predominant diate rewards with lesser negative outcomes
frontostriatal dysfunction in HIV, there has (Hardy et al., 2006; Martin et al., 2004b). Martin
been considerable interest in working mem- and colleagues (2004b) suggested that such
ory, which involves the mental representation risky response styles among persons with HIV
and manipulation of information for tempo- might reflect increased vulnerability to cogni-
rary information processing and storage (e.g., tive impulsivity, which has important implica-
Baddeley & Hitch, 1994). Studies of working tions for the identification and remediation of
memory in HIV reveal impairment across mul- risky behavioral choices in everyday life (e.g.,
tiple modalities (i.e., auditory, spatial, visual, and unprotected sex).
verbal), irrespective of the type of information
being processed (Bartok et al., 1997; Farinpour
et al., 2000; Martin et al., 2001). Converging Learning and Memory
evidence further suggests that HIV-associated
working memory deficits are not limited to any Declarative—Episodic Memory. Deficits in both
one component process but rather are associated verbal and visual episodic memory are present
with several aspects of working memory, such in approximately 50% of individuals infected
as the mental operations involved in short-term with HIV (e.g., Heaton et al., 1995). Driven
memory storage, information maintenance largely by frontostriatal circuit dysfunction
across time delays, and/or immediate “online” (Wiseman et al., 1999), HIV-associated episodic
memory manipulations (Martin et al., 2001). (i.e., retrospective recall) memory impairment
is typically characterized by deficient executive
Executive Functions. Definitions of executive control of encoding and retrieval (e.g., Murji et
functioning vary widely in the literature, but al., 2003). Specifically, HIV-infected individuals
generally refer to a group of higher-order cogni- demonstrate diminished free recall, increased
tive abilities involved in complex goal-directed repetition errors, and limited use of organi-
behavior (e.g., planning and self-monitoring) zational encoding strategies (e.g., Delis et al.,
and are commonly linked to frontal systems. 1995). For example, HIV-associated semantic
Executive dysfunction is apparent even in the clustering deficits during list learning and recall
Neuropsychological Aspects of HIV Infection 371

are associated with executive dysfunction (e.g., Declarative—Semantic Memory. Few studies
Gongvatana et al., 2007) and are evident in rela- to date have specifically examined the effects
tively immunocompetent persons (e.g., Woods et of HIV on semantic memory, which refers to
al., 2005b), with impairment increasing in a step- memory for generalized or factual knowledge
wise manner in individuals with asymptomatic that is not linked to a specific learning expe-
neurocognitive impairment, MCMD, and HAD rience. Sadek et al. (2004) investigated the
(Gongvatana et al., 2007). Nevertheless, episodic temporal pattern of HAD-associated retro-
memory deficits generally improve (although grade semantic memory, finding that memory
not necessarily to the normal range of function- for famous faces and public events was mildly
ing) when retrieval demands are minimized, impaired in HAD relative to seronegative
particularly on recognition trials (e.g., Delis et comparison subjects, with relatively compara-
al., 1995). Genuine consolidation impairment is ble deficits in recall across time periods. Th is
rarely observed in HIV, except among persons pattern was similar to that of persons with
with HAD who may evidence rapid forgetting, Huntington’s disease, but contrasted with that
perhaps secondary to shallow encoding as of individuals with Alzheimer’s disease, who
indicated by an elevated recency effect (Scott demonstrated a temporal gradient of memory
et al., 2006). loss (i.e., poorer memory for recent versus
A recent series of studies has begun to elu- remote events). The mild severity and temporal
cidate the nature and extent of prospective pattern of semantic memory loss in HAD was
memory (ProM) impairment in HIV. ProM is interpreted to reflect retrieval deficits arising
a form of episodic memory involving the abil- from frontostriatal dysfunction, rather than a
ity to successfully encode, retrieve, and execute degradation of semantic memory stores, which
future intentions, or “remember to remem- would be more consistent with posterior corti-
ber,” and as such, has important implications cal involvement.
for daily functioning (e.g., medication man-
agement). Self-reported ProM complaints are Nondeclarative Memory. Nondeclarative mem-
elevated in HIV, particularly on daily tasks ory refers to those aspects of memory that are
that require self-initiated cue detection and implicit, or not directly altered within con-
retrieval (Woods et al., 2007a). Objective ProM sciousness. Consistent with research on pro-
performance is also impaired in HIV infec- cedural memory deficits in populations with
tion and is characterized by mild to moderate frontal systems dysfunction (e.g., Parkinson’s
deficits in both time- (Martin, et al., 2007) and disease; Harrington et al., 1990), Martin et al.
event-based (Carey et al., 2006b) tasks. HIV- (1993) reported that persons with MCMD
associated ProM deficits reflect impaired strate- demonstrate impaired motor skills learning.
gic encoding and retrieval of future intentions, Kalechstein and colleagues (1998) went on to
as evidenced by increased rates of task substi- suggest that, in contrast to episodic memory
tution and no response errors in the setting of deficits, HIV-associated procedural memory
normal recognition of cue-intention pairings impairment may be associated with increased
(Carey et al., 2006b). Interestingly, biomarkers affective/cognitive symptoms of depression.
of HIV disease severity such as macrophage Priming (i.e., the implicit effect of prior stim-
activation (e.g., monocyte chemoattractant pro- ulus exposure on subsequent processing) is
tein-1 [MCP-1]) and neuronal injury (e.g., tau) another important aspect of nondeclarative
are associated with ProM, but not retrospec- memory. Individuals with HIV-associated
tive memory, suggesting that the neuropatho- neurocognitive impairment demonstrate defi-
genesis of HIV-associated ProM impairment cits on semantic, but not perceptual priming
may be dissociable (Woods et al., 2006b). Of tasks (e.g., Nielsen-Bohlman et al., 1997), which
greater clinical relevance, HIV-associated ProM is consistent with conceptual models suggesting
impairment demonstrates incremental ecolog- that semantic priming is primarily mediated
ical validity as a predictor of IADL declines by frontal systems, whereas perceptual
(Woods et al., 2008a) and antiretroviral nonad- priming is more closely linked to the posterior
herence (Woods et al., 2008b; in press). neocortex.
372 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Language Abilities 25

In the absence of severe HAD or focal CNS


opportunistic infections, gross aphasia is rare

Total words
20
in HIV. Although no large-scale, comprehen-
sive assessments of basic speech and language
functioning have been published on adults with 15
HIV, it is widely held that receptive language Animals
abilities are relatively unaffected. In contrast, Actions
specific aspects of expressive language may be 10
HIV– HIV+
mildly to moderately impaired. For example, a
small case-controlled study (Lopez et al., 1994) Figure 17–4. Line graph showing the significant
suggested that HIV might cause a motor speech interaction between HIV serostatus and verbal flu-
disorder characterized by ataxic dysarthria (e.g., ency cue (adapted from Woods et al., 2005).
irregular articulatory rhythm). General expres-
sive language deficits are mild in asymptomatic
HIV infection, but may increase to medium adults (e.g., Woods et al., 2005c). In fact, an
effect sizes in persons with AIDS (Reger et al., interaction between HIV serostatus and ver-
2002). bal fluency cue exists (Woods et al., 2005a),
Verbal fluency is perhaps the most widely whereby HIV-infected individuals generate dis-
studied aspect of expressive language in HIV proportionately fewer verbs than nouns (i.e.,
infection, with a recent meta-analysis revealing animals) relative to demographically compa-
comparable, but modest deficits in both rable seronegative volunteers (see Figure 17–4),
letter and category fluency (Iudicello et al., which may be selectively driven by astrocytosis
2007). HIV-associated verbal fluency deficits (i.e., elevated S-100β; Iudicello et al., 2008a).
appear to be characterized by impairment Importantly, action fluency is sensitive to HIV-
in strategic search and retrieval from lexico- associated neurocognitive disorders (Woods et
semantic memory stores, perhaps reflecting al., 2005a) and may provide incremental eco-
a frontostriatal circuit neuropathogenesis. logical validity in predicting IADL declines as
In support of this hypothesis, deficiencies in compared to traditional letter- and noun-based
switching (i.e., the number of times an indi- fluency tasks (Woods et al., 2006a).
vidual disengages from one lexico-semantic
cluster and switches to another) but not cluster-
ing (i.e., the average number of consecutively Spatial Cognition
generated words within a category) are evident Spatial cognition refers to the diverse set of
in the verbal fluency protocols of persons with cognitive abilities required to perceive, pro-
advanced HIV infection (Millikin et al., 2004; cess, and mentally manipulate visual stimuli,
Woods et al., 2004). Moreover, HIV-associated often in the service of motor functions (e.g.,
category switching deficits are exacerbated in visuoperception). It is widely held that spatial
alternating fluency paradigms (Iudicello et al., cognition is not affected in HIV disease (e.g.,
2008b). (Cysique et al., 2006). However, the poste-
Recent data suggest that action (verb) fluency rior parietal cortex and parieto-striato-frontal
(Piatt et al., 1999b) may be singly dissociable circuits play an important role in spatial cog-
from noun fluency (e.g., animal fluency) in HIV nition (e.g., Lawrence et al., 2000), thereby rais-
(Woods et al., 2005a). Informed by the neural ing questions as to whether prior HIV studies
dissociation between noun and verb networks were too cursory in their measurement of this
(Damasio & Tranel, 1993), action fluency (i.e., construct. To this end, Martin (1994) reported
rapid oral generation of “things . . . that people impairment in egocentric spatial cognition in
do”) demonstrates construct validity as a mea- HIV, which involved the analysis of objects in
sure of frontal systems functions in Parkinson’s relation to a frame of reference that is based
disease (e.g., Piatt et al., 1999a) and healthy on an observer’s point of view, rather than the
Neuropsychological Aspects of HIV Infection 373

participant’s (i.e., allocentric). More recently, power = 34%), indicating that other validated
Olesen and colleagues (2007) observed deficits screening approaches (e.g., Grooved Pegboard)
in mental rotation (i.e., the mental manipula- and/or referral for comprehensive evaluation are
tion of visual images in space) in a small sample needed for such cases (e.g., Carey et al., 2004).
of HIV-infected men, which were particularly
pronounced at greater degrees of rotation. The Intellectual Functioning. It is widely known
cognitive (e.g., spatial working memory and/or that certain aspects of fluid intelligence (e.g.,
visuoperceptual impairment) and neurobiolog- information-processing speed) are sometimes
ical (e.g., frontostriatal and/or posterior parietal impaired in adults with HIV infection. In addi-
dysfunction) underpinnings of HIV-associated tion, studies have also indicated that crystallized
impairment in mental rotation await further intellectual abilities (e.g., general fund of knowl-
examination. edge) might also be negatively affected (e.g.,
Egan et al., 1990). Questions have arisen as to
General Cognitive Functioning whether such differences reflect low premorbid
IQ, rather than the consequences of HIV infec-
Mental Status. Delirium (i.e., acute, fluctuat- tion itself. Indeed, premorbid IQ estimates are
ing alterations in the sensorium) may be evident oftentimes lower in HIV-infected adults relative
in as many as one-half of hospitalized patients to demographically comparable seronegatives
with HIV infection (e.g., Breitbart et al., 1996). (e.g., Basso & Bornstein, 2000). Lower premor-
A variety of factors may cause delirium in HIV, bid IQ, perhaps by way of diminished cognitive
such as opportunistic infections (e.g. menin- reserve, is likely an important source of vari-
gitis), medication side effects (e.g., benzodi- ability in HIV-associated neuropsychological
azepines), and metabolic disturbances (e.g., impairment (e.g., van Gorp et al., 1993). HIV-
electrolyte imbalance) (Atkinson et al., 2005). infected individuals with minimal cognitive
Although research is limited, delirium in HIV reserve show increased prevalence (Stern et al.,
is generally associated with poorer disease out- 1996) and incidence (Basso & Bornstein, 2000)
comes, including higher mortality rates (e.g., of cognitive deficits. Whether cognitive reserve
Uldall et al., 2000). explains the subset of cases in which neuropsy-
chological functioning is normal, but extensive
HIV-associated neuropathologies are present
Cognitive Screening. The HIV Dementia Scale upon postmortem examination, remains to be
(HDS) was designed to screen for global cogni- determined by future research.
tive deficits in HIV, specifically HAD (Power
et al., 1995). As compared to other mental status
Sensory-Perception
tests (e.g., the Mini-Mental State Examination),
the HDS is more sensitive to HAD, perhaps Basic sensory-perceptual functions (e.g., tac-
given its relatively stronger demands on infor- tile sensation) can be compromised by HIV and
mation-processing speed (e.g., timed written threaten interpretation of deficits in higher-
alphabet and cube copy). Nevertheless, the HDS level cognitive abilities. Approximately 28% of
has been criticized for its poor negative pre- individuals with HIV-associated neuropsycho-
dictive power (Carey et al., 2004). In response, logical impairment show sensory-perceptual
Morgan et al. (2008) developed age- and edu- deficits as measured by the Reitan-KlØve Sensory-
cation-adjusted normative standards for the Perceptual Exam, specifically in bilateral tactile
HDS. When applied to a validation cohort of form recognition (Heaton et al., 1995). Distal
135 HIV-infected participants, the normed sensory polyneuropathy (DSPN), involving the
HDS significantly improved the sensitivity distal degeneration of long axons, affects up to
and overall classification of the HDS for iden- 50% of patients with AIDS (Simpson et al., 2006).
tifying HIV-associated neurocognitive disor- Symptoms of DSPN include painful dysesthe-
ders. Nevertheless, false negative errors were sias, paresthesias, and numbness (most often in
still problematic for individuals with very mild the lower extremities), all of which may com-
neurocognitive impairment (positive predictive promise IADLs and quality of life. Risk factors
374 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

for DSPN include older age, lower CD4 counts, compensation-seeking individuals with HIV-
neuromedical comorbidity (e.g., hepatitis C associated neurocognitive disorders fail the
infection), and neurotoxic antiretroviral drugs Hiscock Digit Memory Test (HDMT; a symptom
(e.g., d4T; Keswani et al., 2005). With regard to validity test, test designed to detect suboptimal
ocular functions, infectious (e.g., cytomegalovi- effort, etc.; Hiscock & Hiscock, 1989). Although
rus) and noninfectious (e.g., cotton wool spots these preliminary studies provide important
[CWS]) retinopathies may manifest in HIV- information regarding specificity, prospective
infected individuals in the advanced stages data regarding the sensitivity and positive pre-
of the disease. The destruction of retinal gan- dictive power of various symptom validity tests
glion cells by multiple microinfarcts can mani- in well-characterized compensation-seeking
fest clinically as CWS, the presence of which is HIV samples would also be useful.
associated with reduced visual acuity, as well as
neuropsychological impairment (e.g., Geier et
Everyday Consequences of
al., 1993; cf. Freeman et al., 2004). Otologic and
Neuropsychological Impairment
audiologic abnormalities also frequently occur
in HIV
in HIV. Early studies indicated that up to 33%
of HIV-infected patients have ear disease and A variety of factors associated with HIV infec-
many present with otitis media (Chandrasekhar tion can contribute to reduced independence
et al., 2000). In addition, high-frequency senso- in performing IADLs, including constitutional
rineural hearing loss is commonly observed and symptoms, affective distress, and substance-
tends to be more severe in patients with more related disorders. For example, HIV-infected
advanced HIV disease. Finally, deficits in odor individuals with substance dependence are at
detection (Razani et al., 1996) and identification risk for poor antiretroviral (ARV) medication
(Westervelt et al., 1997) have also been reported adherence (e.g., Arnsten et al., 2002), which in
in HIV infection. turn contributes to poorer health outcomes (e.g.,
medication resistant viral strains). In this sec-
tion, we focus on the impact of HIV-associated
Effort and Symptom Validity
neurocognitive impairment on health-related
Approximately 20% of persons with HIV infec- quality of life (HRQOL), risk behaviors, and
tion in the United States receive Social Security IADLs (i.e., employment, automobile driving,
disability benefits (Centers for Disease Control and medication adherence), and mortality rates.
and Prevention, 2001), including some spe-
cifically for HIV-associated neurocognitive
Health-Related Quality of Life
disorders. As such, the possibility that a small
subset of HIV-infected claimants might feign HRQOL is a multifaceted construct delineating
or exaggerate cognitive deficits in an effort to an individual’s ability to function and success-
secure monetary compensation and/or ser- fully perform daily activities (e.g., employment),
vice benefits must be considered (Sweet et al., and their mental, physical and emotional well-
2002). Suboptimal effort, perhaps secondary to being. Research has consistently found poorer
constitutional symptoms (e.g., fatigue), might physical, emotional, and mental HRQOL in
also confound the interpretation of neuropsy- HIV infection, particularly among individu-
chological data in HIV. Using an “embedded” als with AIDS (e.g., Ruiz Perez et al., 2005) and
effort index from within the Wechsler Memory comorbid affective disorders (e.g., Trepanier
Scale-Revised (WMS-R; Mittenberg et al., et al., 2005). The literature also supports a cor-
1993; Psychological Corporation, 1987), Slick respondence between reduced HRQOL and
and colleagues (2001) observed false positive several aspects of HIV-associated neuropsycho-
rates ranging from 7%-18% in a small sample logical impairment, including deficits in fine
of persons with HIV infection, with the great- motor coordination, episodic memory, exec-
est number of false positives among individuals utive functions, and information-processing
with higher general memory abilities. Woods speed (Tozzi et al., 2003). Cognitive impairment
et al. (2003) reported that as few as 2% of non- in these ability areas also increases the risk of
Neuropsychological Aspects of HIV Infection 375

dependence in IADLs, which ostensibly reduces set-shifting and response inhibition) are per-
HRQOL. In addition, it has been suggested that haps at greatest risk of reducing or discontinu-
individuals with HIV-associated neurocogni- ing work (van Gorp et al., 1999). The advent of
tive disorders may utilize ineffective coping cART has extended survival rates and HRQOL,
strategies (e.g., confrontive coping; Manly et al., but has also introduced the possible challenges
1997), thereby reducing HRQOL by limiting of returning to work, often after significant
their ability to effectively manage stressful life periods of unemployment and/or disability. van
events (e.g., Pukay-Martin et al., 2003). Gorp et al. (2007) reported that verbal learning
and memory abilities were the strongest predic-
tors of returning to work, such that individu-
Risk Behaviors als with the highest memory scores had greater
HIV infection is associated with a number of than a 70% chance of being employed at a 2-year
risky behaviors (e.g., injection drug use and follow-up. These findings may have implications
unprotected sex). As such, understanding the for cognitive and vocational rehabilitation, as a
underlying neuropsychological deficits that number of cognitive interventions have been
may predispose individuals to developing and designed to improve learning and memory (e.g.,
engaging in risky behaviors may have impor- spaced retrieval) in various clinical populations
tant implications for prevention and treatment (e.g., dementia).
of both “at risk” and already HIV-infected indi-
viduals. Although HIV-infected individuals Automobile Driving. Driving an automobile
evidence risky decision-making styles (Hardy requires a complex combination of multiple cog-
et al., 2006), the limited research to date in nitive domains including perception, sustained
HIV-infected substance users has not identified and selective attention, visual and psychomotor
a strong relationship between self-reported risk processing speed, motor sequencing, judgment,
behavior and measures of executive functions and planning, many of which may be adversely
(e.g., Gonzalez et al., 2005) or working memory affected in HIV. In fact, individuals with HIV-
(Martin et al., 2004b). Nevertheless, Martin et al. associated neurocognitive disorders perform
(2007) recently reported an association between significantly worse on computerized driving
risky behaviors in HIV-infected substance- simulators and are more likely to fail on-road
dependent individuals and impaired time- driving evaluations (Marcotte et al., 2004) than
based ProM, which may moderate the relation- their unimpaired HIV counterparts. Deficits in
ship between one’s intention to implement HIV executive functions, complex attention, speed
prevention strategies and actual participation of information processing, fine motor dexter-
in risky HIV transmission behaviors. ity, and sensory-perceptual functions are most
closely associated with poor driving perfor-
mance (Marcotte et al., 1999, 2004). Although
Instrumental Activities of Daily Living these findings highlight the potential impact of
(IADL) HIV-associated neurocognitive deficits on driv-
ing ability, one must also weigh the relative risk
Employment. Retaining gainful employment of on-road accidents to the potential impact of
is a critical issue for individuals living with HIV lost driving privileges on HRQOL. As such, evi-
infection, especially vis-à-vis the debilitating dence of neuropsychological impairment may
physical (e.g., fatigue), affective (e.g., depression), warrant referral for an on-road evaluation, but
and cognitive complications of the disease. HIV- should not be the sole arbiter of a recommenda-
related neuropsychological impairment is asso- tion to suspend driving privileges.
ciated with poor performance on standardized
work samples, higher rates of unemployment, Medication Adherence. Optimal adher-
and incident work disability over time, even ence to cART regimens is associated with dra-
after controlling for HIV disease severity (e.g., matic reductions in morbidity and mortality,
Heaton et al., 2004). Individuals with deficits in whereas nonadherence has potentially life-
learning efficiency and executive functions (i.e., threatening consequences (e.g., development
376 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

of drug-resistance). The literature consistently Mortality


reveals that HIV-associated cognitive deficits
HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders are
are associated with deficient medication adher-
potentially a harbinger of mortality (Ellis et al.,
ence. In fact, cognitive impairment confers a
1997a). Global cognitive impairment is signifi-
twofold greater risk of cART nonadherence,
cantly associated with lower survival rates, even
which is driven primarily by episodic memory
prior to progression to AIDS (Ellis et al., 1997a;
deficits and executive dysfunction (e.g., Hinkin
Mayeux et al., 1993). Studies from both the pre-
et al., 2002). Furthermore, individuals with
and post-cART eras show higher rates of mor-
HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders who
tality among HIV-infected persons with a range
are prescribed more complex cART regimens
of cognitive impairment severity (e.g., Mayeux
may be at greater risk for nonadherence (Gallant
et al., 1993; Sevigny et al., 2007). There is a partic-
& Block, 1998; Hinkin et al., 2002). A recent
ularly strong association between HAD and time
study demonstrated the importance of HIV-
to death, independent of disease progression (e.g.,
associated ProM impairment to medication
CD4 count), treatment effects (e.g., ARV use) and
adherence in 79 volunteers who were followed
demographic factors (Sevigny et al., 2007). Future
for approximately 1 month using medication
research should investigate possible mediators
event monitoring caps (Woods et al., in press).
of the relationship between neuropsychological
As shown in Figure 17–5, nonadherent individu-
impairment and death (e.g., medication adher-
als (n = 31) demonstrated significantly poorer
ence) in an effort to develop effective interven-
prospective memory functioning as compared
tions and further improve survival rates.
to adherent persons (n = 48), particularly on
an index of time-based ProM (i.e., elevated loss
of time errors). Importantly, time-based ProM Biomarkers of HIV-Associated
impairment remained significant, independent Neurocognitive Disorders
predictor of non-adherence, even after consid-
ering demographics, disease severity, affective CDC Staging
distress, and general cognitive impairment,
suggesting that cART adherence may be partic- The Center for Disease Control and Prevention,
ularly dependent on the self-initiated cognitive (CDC, 1993) defined a set of staging guide-
processes involved in retrieving future inten- lines based on three ranges of CD4 lymphocyte
tions (e.g., time monitoring; see also Woods counts (i.e., ≥ 500, 499–200, and < 200 cells/
et al., 2008b). µL) and three severities of symptomatology (i.e.,
A, B, and C), which comprise nine exclusive
categories. The prevalence and severity of neu-
8
* Adherent rocognitive impairment generally increases in
7 Non-adherent a stepwise fashion as the disease advances (e.g.,
6
Reger et al., 2002). Despite early controversies
Raw score

5
over whether cognitive impairment existed in
4
the asymptomatic phase (e.g., CDC A; Janssen
3
et al., 1989), subsequent research shows that
2
approximately 30% of persons in this stage evi-
1
dence overall mild cognitive impairment across
0
Event-based Time-based numerous domains of functioning (Heaton
MIST subscale et al., 1995). An important future direction of
this research is examination of the CNS effects
Figure 17–5. Bar chart showing the time- and event- of acute (i.e., the period between transmission
based prospective memory scores from the Memory and seroconversion) and early (i.e., < 1 year
for Intentions Screening Test (MIST) in antiretroviral posttransmission) HIV infection, a period in
adherent (n = 48) and nonadherent (n = 31) partici- which rapid viral replication and neuroinflam-
pants (adapted from Woods et al., in press). matory responses are present in animal and cel-
* p < .05, Cohen’s d = –0.6. lular models (e.g., Greco et al., 2004).
Neuropsychological Aspects of HIV Infection 377

CNS Opportunistic Infections (Watters et al., 2004), thereby supporting the


possible utility of this measure as a marker of
Although less common in the cART era, several nervous system disease.
of the AIDS-defining opportunistic infections
(OIs) have implications for CNS function-
ing. Typically occurring when an individual is HIV RNA
immunosuppressed (i.e., CD4 count < 200 cells/ HIV RNA, an indicator of HIV replication or
µL), CNS OIs include Toxoplasma encephalitis, “viral load” can be quantified in blood plasma
JC virus encephalitis (i.e., progressive multi- or cerebrospinal fluid (CSF). Although plasma
focal leukoencephalopathy), Cytomegalovirus viral load is integral to monitoring general
encephalitis, and primary CNS lymphoma. The immune health and treatment efficacy, it is
most common CNS OI is Cryptococcal men- not reliably predictive of HIV-associated neu-
ingitis, which has been reported to occur in up ropsychological impairment (e.g., Reger et al.,
to 10% of persons with AIDS, particularly in 2005). Evidence of viral compartmentalization
untreated individuals or in international set- in the CNS (e.g., Ellis et al., 1997b) inspired the
tings (Chayakulkeeree & Perfect, 2006; Mirza hypothesis that CSF viral load may provide a
et al., 2003). clearer window onto the CNS burden of HIV
infection. In further support of this notion,
CD4 Lymphocyte Count Ellis et al. (2002) found that detectable CSF
viral load was a stronger predictor of incident
A primary target of HIV, CD4 lymphocytes cognitive impairment than plasma HIV RNA.
are a type of white blood cell (i.e., leukocytes) Despite these encouraging fi ndings from the
that are vital to immune function by assisting pre-cART era, more recent fi ndings have
in the destruction of bacteria, viruses, fungi, identified weakened relationships between
and parasites. The normal CD4 count range CSF viral loads and cognitive outcomes (e.g.,
varies by laboratory, but is generally between Sevigny et al., 2004). CSF viral load may still
500 to 1,500 cells/μL. The specific neuropath- provide insights into pathologic events in the
ological processes that underlie the relation- CNS, but it is not currently considered an
ship between the number of circulating CD4 optimal surrogate marker (McArthur et al.,
lymphocytes and the risk for HIV-associated 2004).
neurological complications are still not well
understood. For example, CD4 lymphocyte
counts among individuals recently diagnosed Host Biomarkers
with HAD appears to be higher than those Biomarkers that reflect host processes that
reported in the pre-cART era (Brew, 2004), may contribute to HIV neuropathogen-
suggesting that current CD4 lymphocyte esis have also been explored (see Gonzalez-
count may now be less useful as a clinical bio- Scarano & Martin-Garcia, 2005), including
marker of neuroAIDS. In light of this, several chemokines (e.g., MCP-1), neuroprotective
research groups have investigated the predic- factors (e.g., FGF-1), and markers of mono-
tive value of self-reported CD4 nadir (i.e., the cyte activation (e.g., beta-2 microglobulin),
lowest ever CD4 count) in neuroAIDS. For excitotoxicity (e.g., quinolinic acid), astro-
example, Valcour et al. (2006) demonstrated cytosis (e.g., S-100β), neuroinflammation
that nadir CD4 count significantly predicted (e.g., tumor necrosis factor-alpha), oxidative
HAD and MCMD, even after modeling the stress (e.g., ceramide), and neuronal damage
effects of demographics, current CD4 count, (e.g., neurofi lament-light). One of the most
and HIV disease duration. Specifically, a dif- widely studied biomarkers of HIV neuro-
ference of 100 cells/μL was associated with a pathogenesis is MCP-1 (i.e., CC chemokine
40% increased risk of HAD. These fi ndings ligand 2), which is produced by astrocytes,
replicate others that have identified lower nadir is a marker of mononuclear phagocyte (e.g.,
CD4 counts as a risk factor for neurocognitive macrophages) trafficking, and is associated
impairment (Tozzi et al., 2005) and neuropathy with higher CSF viral loads (e.g., Monteiro
378 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

de Almeida et al., 2005), HAD (e.g., Kelder Comorbidities of HIV-Associated


et al., 1998), and HIV encephalitis (Cinque Neurocognitive Disorders
et al., 1998). Although concerns regarding the
specificity of biomarkers to HIV disease persist, Affective Disorders
their value in delineating the neuropathogenic
mechanisms of HIV-associated neurocognitive Depression. Major Depressive Disorder (MDD)
disorders is nonetheless compelling. is perhaps the most common comorbid psychi-
atric disorder found in HIV-infected individu-
Genetic Factors als, with estimated prevalence rates as high as
50% (Ciesla & Roberts, 2001). The etiology of
The past 10 years have witnessed an increased depressive symptoms may be either primary
effort to understand the viral and host genetic (e.g., premorbid) or secondary (e.g., associated
factors involved in HIV-associated neu- with the psychological impact of HIV infection
rocognitive disorders. Extending the HIV or due to organic causes related to infection
biomarker research described above, several of the CNS) to HIV infection (Treisman et al.,
investigators have begun to examine the role 1998). Depression in HIV infection is of partic-
of genetic variants of various chemokines and ular clinical importance as a predictor of poorer
chemokine receptors in neuroAIDS. For exam- treatment outcomes, reduced medication adher-
ple, Gonzalez et al. (2002) demonstrated that ence, and everyday functioning (Heaton et al.,
variability in the promoter region of the gene 2004) as well as increased mortality (Mayeux
encoding MCP-1 was associated with a 4.5-fold et al., 1993). In seronegative groups, MDD is
increased risk of HAD. In addition, Singh et associated with neuropsychological impair-
al. (2004) reported that variability in the gene ment, including deficits in attention, learning
encoding CCR2, the receptor for MCP-1, was and memory, psychomotor speed and executive
associated with a twofold increased risk of inci- functions. In HIV, MDD and neuropsycholog-
dent neuropsychological impairment, perhaps ical impairment are independently associated
reflecting increased susceptibility to neuro- with HRQOL (Trepanier et al., 2005), IADLs
toxic inflammatory processes. Several studies (Heaton et al., 1996), and mortality (Ellis et al.,
have also examined the role of apolipoprotein 1997a). Depressed mood is also often associated
E ε4 allele (APOE-ε4) in the expression of neu- with increased cognitive complaints in HIV,
roAIDS. Corder et al. (1998) were the first to even though neuropsychological performance
describe an association between APOE-ε4 and may be within normal limits(Rourke et al.,
increased frequency of neurological symptoms 1999). However, research on the potential addi-
in HIV, including HAD. More recently, Valcour tive effects of MDD on HIV-associated neu-
et al. (2004b) demonstrated that APOE-ε4 was ropsychological impairment has been largely
associated with a threefold increased risk for negative. Despite their similar pathological
HAD in older adults with HIV. Postmortem features (e.g., frontostriatal circuits) and neu-
studies suggest that, among persons with ropsychological profiles, the majority of stud-
HAD, APOE-ε4 is associated with increased ies have failed to identify significant additive
susceptibility of neurons to lipid metabolism effects of MDD and HIV on cognition (e.g.,
pathologies (e.g., increased concentrations of Grant et al., 1993), suggesting possibly disso-
sphingolipids in the prefrontal cortex) (Cutler ciable neuropathogenic mechanisms (Cysique
et al., 2004). Considering the neuropathogen- et al., 2007).
esis of HIV-associated neurocognitive dis-
orders and prior research on the genetics of Anxiety Disorders. HIV-infected individuals
cognition (Goldberg & Weinberger, 2004), are also at risk of developing anxiety disor-
numerous other candidate genes might be con- ders, with comorbid prevalence estimates as
sidered, including brain derived neurotrophic high as 38% (Elliott, 1998). Typically, symp-
factor (BDNF), catechol-O-methyltransferase toms of anxiety found in HIV infection result
(COMT), monoamine oxidase A (MAOA), and from a premorbid condition, or from a preoc-
serotonin 2A receptor (5-HT2A). cupation with HIV and difficulties in coping
Neuropsychological Aspects of HIV Infection 379

with the illness, rather than from an organic HIV, along with the neuropsychiatric, cogni-
basis resulting directly from infection (Grant & tive, and functional (e.g., medication adherence
Atkinson, 1999). Comorbid anxiety disorders and HIV transmission risk) consequences of
in HIV infection are predictive of medication comorbidity is needed.
nonadherence and poorer HRQOL (Tucker
et al., 2003). This is a potentially important
area of research because neuropsychological Substance-Related Disorders
impairments are evident in a subset of anxi-
ety disorders, including Obsessive-Compulsive Alcohol. Alcohol-related disorders (i.e., abuse
Disorder, Post-traumatic Stress Disorder, and and/or dependence) often accompany HIV
Generalized Anxiety Disorder (e.g., Airaksinen infection, with the rate of heavy drinking
et al., 2005). Mapou et al. (1993) found that almost twice that of the general population
higher self-reported anxiety symptoms in HIV (Galvan et al., 2002). In addition, alcohol abus-
were associated with subjective cognitive com- ers have a 5%–10% higher risk of acquiring HIV
plaints, but not performance-based neuropsy- (Meyerhoff, 2001) as consumption has been
chological impairment. Similarly, diagnoses of linked to risk behaviors (e.g., sexual disinhibi-
Generalized Anxiety Disorder were not asso- tion) associated with HIV transmission (Stein et
ciated with neuropsychological impairment al., 2005) and reduced medication compliance
in a large HIV sample (Heaton et al., 1995). (Braithwaite et al., 2005). Furthermore, alco-
Considering the prevalence of anxiety disorders hol has been shown to influence the progres-
in HIV infection and their independent effects sion of HIV infection by altering the immune
on the CNS, HRQOL, and IADLs, research on response (Wang et al., 2002), diminishing the
the neurocognitive consequences of comorbid effectiveness of cART (Miguez et al., 2003), and
anxiety disorders and HIV infection is clearly potentiating the neurotoxicity of HIV-specific
indicated. protein-induced gp120 apoptosis (Chen et al.,
2005). Alcohol use can also exacerbate HIV-
Bipolar Disorder. HIV-infected individuals may associated white matter damage (e.g., fractional
also display either primary or secondary symp- anisotropy in the corpus callosum; Pfefferbaum
toms of mania. Although Bipolar Disorder and et al., 2007) and interacts with disease severity
HIV affect similar neural systems (i.e., fronto- with respect to volume reduction in the corpus
striatal and limbic networks), little is known callosum and enlargement of the supratentorial
about their potential additive effects on the CNS ventricles (Pfefferbaum et al., 2006). Metabolic
or how frequently these conditions co-occur. abnormalities (i.e., reduced N-acetyl aspar-
Importantly, Bipolar Disorder and HIV and tate and creatine) are also evident in HIV-
are each associated with increased risk-taking infected individuals with alcohol dependence
behavior (e.g., unprotected sex), neuropsycho- (Pfefferbaum et al., 2005).
logical deficits (e.g., executive dysfunction), and Previously, neuropsychological studies of
medical noncompliance, suggesting that inves- alcohol use in the context of HIV infection
tigation into their combined effects is highly were confounded by the inclusion of polysub-
relevant. To this end, Moore and colleagues stance dependent individuals or limited cog-
(2006) recently reported evidence of additive nitive assessments, thereby limiting reliable
effects of HIV and Bipolar Disorder in a sample conclusions about the potential interaction.
of polysubstance users, such that global neuro- Three recent studies, however, have addressed
psychological impairment was more prevalent this limitation by excluding comorbid sub-
in individuals with comorbid Bipolar Disorder stance users in their sample. Each demonstrated
and HIV infection (70%) as compared to demo- evidence for an additive effect of alcohol and
graphically comparable subjects with only one HIV, specifically on measures of selective atten-
risk factor (i.e., HIV or Bipolar Disorder alone; tion (Schulte et al., 2005), verbal reasoning and
61%) or neither condition (24%). Prospective auditory processing (Green et al., 2004), and
research regarding the prevalence and neuro- psychomotor speed (Durvasula et al., 2006).
biological mechanisms of Bipolar Disorder in Whether such elevated rates of impairment in
380 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

these cohorts are associated with increased risk 2004). Other proposed mechanisms include
of IADL dependence, HRQOL, and/or mortal- increased neuroinvasion and neurovascular
ity remains to be determined. complications secondary to cocaine’s direct
effects on microvascular endothelial cells in the
Marijuana. Marijuana use is widespread BBB (e.g., Zhang et al., 1998), amplification of
among HIV-infected individuals (Whitfield HIV replication (e.g., Peterson et al., 1991), and
et al., 1997), both in terms of recreational and augmented expression of inflammatory cytok-
medicinal (e.g., analgesia, increasing appetite, ines, leukocyte transmigration, and adhesion
and nausea control) consumption. Importantly, molecules (e.g., Gan et al., 1998).
marijuana suppresses immune function and may Human cognitive studies have been primar-
have an additive effect in the context of already ily limited to polysubstance abusers who use
immunocompromised systems (e.g., Ongradi cocaine, making it difficult to isolate cocaine-
et al., 1998). A meta-analysis of the residual specific deficits. In a sample of African American
effects of long-term cannabis use on neurocog- men with comorbid HIV infection and cocaine
nitive functioning found only a small effect in abuse, Durvasula and colleagues (2000)
the ability to learn and remember new infor- reported that, with the exception of an associa-
mation (Grant et al., 2003), but to our knowl- tion between moderate to heavy recent cocaine
edge, only one study has examined the effects use and slower psychomotor speed, cocaine
of marijuana use on cognition in HIV-infected use did not contribute to HIV-associated neu-
individuals. Cristiani et al. (2004) stratified ropsychological deficits. Another study found
groups according to self-reported marijuana use that cocaine-dependent individuals with HIV
over the past 12 months with the “no/minimal reported significantly more cognitive impair-
use” group reporting using less than once per ment than seronegative cocaine users (Avants
month as compared to the “frequent use” group et al., 1997); however, performance-based cog-
who reported use at least once a week, as well nitive abilities were not investigated. Finally, a
as HIV disease stage. A significant interaction study of active stimulant users (cocaine and/
was observed, suggesting that frequent mari- or amphetamines) with HIV infection found
juana use was associated with greater memory impairment in sustained attention (Levine et
impairment among symptomatic participants. al., 2006). Prospective research, perhaps using
Further study is needed regarding the cogni- a parametric study design is needed to more
tive and neurobiological sequelae of marijuana clearly articulate the possible combined effects
use in HIV-infected individuals, particularly of cocaine use in HIV infection.
considering evidence that marijuana may be
neuroprotective in HIV (Croxford, 2003), and Methamphetamine. Methamphetamine dep-
is a potentially effective treatment for chronic endence is frequent among HIV-infected
neuropathic pain from HIV-associated sensory individuals and in persons at high-risk for
neuropathy (Abrams et al., 2007). HIV infection (Woody et al., 1999). Metham-
phetamine dependence and HIV infection
Cocaine. Animal model studies suggest that are both independently associated with cog-
chronic cocaine abuse may hasten the progres- nitive dysfunction (e.g., Reger et al., 2002;
sion of the HIV disease process. For instance, Scott et al., 2007) and converge in their neu-
cocaine-injected mice also inoculated with HIV- ropathophysiological effects on frontostriatal
infected macrophages demonstrated increased circuits (e.g., Nath et al., 2002). Interestingly, it
astrogliosis and microgliosis, more impaired appears that structural brain alterations associ-
cognitive performance on a spatial learning ated with methamphetamine dependence and
task, and recovered more slowly from fatigue in HIV are distinct, yet overlapping with oppos-
comparison to uninfected mice (Griffin et al., ing effects on brain volume. Specifically, HIV
2007). The possible additive effects of cocaine is associated with volume atrophy (e.g., fron-
and HIV may be secondary to their combined tal cortex and caudate nucleus), whereas
neurotoxic effects, particularly with respect to methamphetamine is associated with cortical
dopaminergic dysfunction (e.g., Wang et al., (e.g., parietal) and basal ganglia volume increases
Neuropsychological Aspects of HIV Infection 381

(Jernigan et al., 2005). Nevertheless, additive liver disease. Research increasingly supports
effects are evident using MR spectroscopy, spe- the neurovirulence of chronic HCV infection:
cifically reductions in N-acetyl aspartate (NAA) HCV RNA is detectable in brain parenchyma
in the basal ganglia, frontal white matter, and (e.g., Radkowski et al., 2002), and elevated met-
frontal gray matter, diminished creatine in the abolic markers of neuronal injury and neuroin-
basal ganglia, and myoinositol reductions in flammation are evident in the basal ganglia and
the frontal white matter (Chang et al., 2005). frontal cerebral white matter of HCV-infected
Not surprisingly, comorbid HIV and metham- individuals (e.g., Forton et al., 2005). Cognitive
phetamine dependence are associated with a deficits, most notably slowed information-
compounding of adverse neurocognitive effects processing speed and complex attentional
(Rippeth et al., 2004), particularly in a subset of impairments, have also been associated with
immunosuppressed individuals (Carey et al., HCV (e.g., Forton et al., 2005), even after con-
2006a). Episodic memory is perhaps most sus- trolling for potentially confounding comor-
ceptible to the additive effects of HIV and meth- bidities (e.g., substance abuse; Cherner et al.,
amphetamine, the cellular basis for which may 2005). HCV-associated deficits in motor coor-
be a severe loss of calbindin and parvalbumin dination and information-processing speed
interneurons in the frontal cortex (Chana et al., are correlated with declines in the independent
2006). performance of basic and instrumental ADLs,
respectively (Vigil, et al., 2008).
Opioids. Injection opioid (i.e., heroin) users are Since HIV and HCV each adversely affect
at significantly increased risk for acquiring HIV cognition, several groups have posited that
infection. Opioids readily cross the BBB and are co-infection may confer additive risks of neu-
associated with feelings of euphoria, but the rocognitive impairment, perhaps by way of
research on possible neuropsychological deficits inflammatory mechanisms (e.g., Letendre et al.,
in seronegative persons dependent on heroin 2005). Individuals with HIV–HCV co-infection
are generally inconclusive (e.g., Pau et al., 2002). may be particularly vulnerable to neuropsychi-
Although some studies show an independent atric disturbances (e.g., affective distress; von
effect of heroin use on cognitive impairment Giesen et al., 2004) and psychomotor slowing
in HIV-infected individuals (e.g., Margolin et al., (e.g., Clifford et al., 2005). For example, Martin
2002), there is no clear evidence that heroin et al. (2004a) reported that HIV and HCV were
necessarily exacerbates HIV-associated neuro- additively associated with slower reaction times
psychological impairment (Selnes et al., 1997). to both congruent and incongruent Stroop tri-
However, the neuroAIDS research on heroin als. Nevertheless, the limited data published to
users is difficult to interpret given multiple con- date on the possible additive neuropsychologi-
founding factors, including polysubstance abuse cal effects of co-infection are mixed (e.g., Ryan
(especially cocaine), psychiatric comorbidity, et al., 2004). Multidisciplinary studies designed
premorbid CNS vulnerabilities, demographic to clarify the prevalence, mechanisms, and
effects, and various (often unmeasured) health- functional impact of co-infection on the CNS
related factors, such as nutritional deficiencies are warranted and should give consideration to
and co-infection with the hepatitis C virus. potential confounds, such as alcohol and drug
use, socioeconomic status, developmental dis-
orders (e.g., learning disabilities), liver disease
Hepatitis C Co-Infection
severity, and antiviral medications.
Between 15% and 50% of persons living with
HIV are co-infected with the Hepatitis C
Normal Aging
Virus (HCV), with the highest rates of co-
infection among HIV-seropositive injection With the advent of more effective cART, the
drug users (e.g., Mohsen et al., 2005). HCV mortality rates in HIV have decreased dra-
is a single-stranded RNA Flavivirus that has matically, resulting in an increased preva-
infected approximately 3 million adults in the lence of older adults living with HIV infection
United States and is associated with chronic (CDC, 2005). Eighteen percent of the incident
382 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

AIDS cases and 32% of AIDS-related deaths Treatment of HIV-Associated


in the United States are among HIV-infected Neurocognitive Disorders
individuals aged 50 years and older (CDC,
2005). Although controversy exists regarding Antiretroviral Therapies
the impact of normal aging on HIV disease
(Casau, 2005), older adults with HIV may be Although cART has reduced the incidence of
at greater risk for premorbid immune down- HAD (Sacktor et al., 2006), HIV-associated neu-
regulation (e.g., Fagnoni et al., 2000), more ropsychological impairment is still highly prev-
rapid HIV disease progression (e.g., Goetz et alent and continues to adversely affect HRQOL
al., 2001) and morbidity (Perez & Moore, 2003). and IADLs (Tozzi et al., 2004). Research gen-
Numerous CNS risk factors also accompany erally supports the positive impact of cART
normal aging, including cardiovascular disease initiation on HIV-associated neuropsycholog-
(e.g., hypertension), cerebrovascular comor- ical deficits (e.g., psychomotor slowing) (e.g.,
bidity, metabolic dysregulation (e.g., insulin Ferrando et al., 1998); however, the literature
resistance), and non-HIV-associated demen- is not entirely consistent in this regard (e.g.,
tias (e.g., Alzheimer’s disease) (Valcour et Cysique et al., 2004). In fact, a very recent study
al., 2004a). Indeed, older age may confer an identified that neuropsychological performance
increased risk of HIV-associated neurocogni- may improve when cART is discontinued,
tive disorders. Valcour et al. (2004a) reported although the mechanisms for this observation
that older adults (aged ≥ 50 years) were three are not yet understood (Robertson et al., 2007).
times more likely to be diagnosed with HAD One complexity of observational research on
than their younger counterparts, even after the effects of cART is that many patients are
controlling for demographics, HIV disease not prescribed ARVs until their CD4 counts fall
severity, psychiatric comorbidities, and cART. below 200 cells/µL (e.g., Powderly, 2002), possi-
Among nondemented older HIV-infected bly increasing the risk of cognitive impairment
adults, individuals with detectable CSF viral associated with immunocompromise (Munoz-
loads evidence a twofold risk of neurocognitive Moreno et al., 2007).
impairment (Cherner et al., 2004). Whether the The discrepant findings regarding the effec-
profi le of HIV-associated neuropsychological tiveness of cART in improving HIV-associated
deficits is different in older adults remains to cognitive deficits could be due to the differences
be determined, but the increased prevalence of in the distribution of ARVs into the CNS. The
argyrophilic amyloid plaques (Esiri et al., 1998) effectiveness of ARVs in the CNS can be estimated
and possible expression of APOE-ε4 genotype by their molecular and chemical properties,
(Valcour et al., 2004b) suggest that cognitive their concentrations in CSF, or their effective-
sequelae of medial temporal pathology may be ness in reducing viral loads in CSF or treating
more prominent in this cohort (Brew, 2004). neurocognitive impairment (see Letendre et al.,
Finally, the contribution of neurocognitive def- 2008). Cross-sectional research indicates that
icits to everyday functioning (e.g., automobile ARVs with better penetration characteristics
driving and HRQOL) in HIV-infected older are associated with better episodic memory
adults deserves exploration. Although older age performance among HIV-infected persons with
is generally associated with better cART com- generalized neuropsychological impairment
pliance (Wutoh et al., 2001), older adults with (Cysique et al., 2004). In a single-group longitu-
HIV-associated cognitive impairment (e.g., dinal study, Letendre et al. (2004) reported that
executive dysfunction) may be at increased risk the regimens that included ARVs with better
for nonadherence (Hinkin et al., 2004). CNS penetration characteristics were associated
The revised criteria for HAND begin to with greater reductions in CSF viral load, which
address the issue of diagnosis of HAND in the in turn was related to improvement in global
context of potential confounds. Table 17–1A HIV-associated neuropsychological impair-
provides an approach to classifying such con- ment. Randomized clinical trials are needed to
founds which was developed by the Frascati more thoroughly investigate the potential rela-
Group (see also Antinori et al., 2007). tionship between optimized CNS penetration of
Table 17–1A. Published Guidelines for Classifying Confounds to HIV-Associated Neurocognitive Disorders
(Antinori et al., 2007)
Comorbid Condition Recommendation
Depression
Secondary Depressed mood and/or major depressive disorder but without
psychotic features, and no clinical indication of inadequate
effort/motivation on cognitive testing (NP or MSE). Normal per-
formance on ≥1 effort-demanding cognitive test (e.g., PASAT).
Contributing Major depressive disorder with psychotic features or some clinical
evidence of fluctuating or suboptimal effort on cognitive testing.
Nevertheless, impairment is present on non-speeded tests or on
tests on which patient appeared to put forth good effort. Patient
responds well to task demands with some examiner encouragement.
Confounding Major depressive disorder with psychotic features and/or persist-
ing clinical evidence of suboptimal effort in the cognitive testing
process. Patient does not respond well to examiner prompting or
encouragement.
Traumatic brain injury (TBI)
Secondary Mild TBI with no apparent functional decline (return to indepen-
dent living; resumed successful employment and/or schoolwork, as
appropriate to prior level of functioning). Subsequent cognitive and
functional decline within the context of HIV.
Contributing Mild TBI with some evidence of persisting mild functional decline or
Severe TBI with no more than mild functional sequelae. Subsequent
additional decline within the context of HIV.
Confounding Mild or Severe TBI without return to work/school (as appropriate to
prior level of functioning), and with increased dependence on oth-
ers for IADLs. No clear subsequent cognitive or functional decline
within the context of HIV.
Developmental disability
Secondary Isolated mild academic problem, but with passing performance in
regular academic classes (not “special education”), and no repeated
grades, followed by vocational and/or IADL independence.
Subsequent cognitive and functional decline within the context
of HIV.
Contributing Mild to moderate cognitive/educational disability (lifelong): Some
regular academic classes with passing performance, and no more
than 1 repeated grade, followed by IADL independence. Subsequent
cognitive and functional decline within the context of HIV.
Confounding Mental Retardation and/or severe educational disability, followed by
some dependence in IADLs and no employment with significant
cognitive demands. Cognitive or functional decline with HIV is
difficult or impossible to establish.
History of alcohol or other substance use disorder
Secondary Past history of substance use disorder, but patient does not meet
DSM-IV criteria for abuse or dependence during last 6 months.
No clinical evidence of intoxication or withdrawal at the time of
evaluation. No prior overdoses with significant sequelae. Despite
substance use disorder, patient has maintained adequate employ-
ment and/or independence of IADLs in the past (during or after
the period of abuse or dependence). Evidence of cognitive and
functional decline within the context of HIV, which began during
or extended into the recent period when the patient did not meet
criteria for abuse or dependence.
(continued)

383
Table 17–1A. Continued
Comorbid Condition Recommendation
Contributing Past history of substance use disorder but patient does not meet
DSM-IV criteria for abuse or dependence within the last 30 days.
Cognitive and functional decline within the context of HIV
appears worse than in the past (even with ongoing substance use
disorder in the past) and extends to the current time. No clinical
evidence of intoxication or withdrawal at the time of the current
evaluation. OR
Ongoing substance use disorder, which would be considered a
“confounding condition” in a baseline or cross-sectional assessment
(see next row), but there is strong, longitudinal evidence of an addi-
tional HIV effect: Without any change in the pattern of substance
use, a stable or progressive worsening in both neurocognitive per-
formance and everyday functioning is documented over at least 3
longitudinal assessments (baseline and at least 2 follow-ups). This
worsening occurs within the context of HIV infection, and cannot
be explained by a drug overdose, by evidence of intoxication or
withdrawal at the times of the evaluations, or by any other
neuromedical or psychiatric confound.
Confounding Ongoing substance use disorder with significant impact on everyday
functioning. Difficult or impossible to determine whether cognitive
or functional decline is due to substance use and/or HIV. OR
Patient gave clinical evidence of intoxication or withdrawal during the
time of the current evaluation.
HIV-related opportunistic CNS disease
Secondary History of space-occupying brain lesions with contrast enhance-
ment and edema, such as tumor or abscess; space-occupying
lesions without enhancement and edema, such as progressive
multifocal leukoencephalopathy (PML); symptomatic meningi-
tis, such as due to Cryptococcus and syphilis. Patient returned to
normal functioning but then experienced subsequent cognitive and
functional decline within the context of HIV.
Contributing History of space-occupying brain lesions with contrast enhancement
and edema, such as tumor or abscess; space-occupying lesions
without enhancement and edema, such as PML; symptomatic
meningitis, such as due to Cryptococcus and syphilis. Patient
did not return to normal cognition but experienced subsequent
additional cognitive and functional decline within the context
of HIV.
Confounding Acutely symptomatic space-occupying brain lesions with
contrast enhancement and edema, such as tumor or abscess;
space-occupying lesions without enhancement and edema, such as
PML; symptomatic meningitis, such as due to Cryptococcus and
syphilis.
Non-HIV-related Neurologic Condition
Secondary Prior stroke or cardiac bypass surgery with return to normal
cognition after the event, no intervening event (another stroke
or bypass surgery) but with subsequent cognitive and functional
decline within the context of HIV.
Contributing Prior stroke or cardiac bypass surgery without return to normal
cognition after the event, no intervening event (another stroke or
bypass surgery) but subsequent cognitive and functional decline
within the context of HIV.
(continued)

384
Neuropsychological Aspects of HIV Infection 385

Table 17–1A. Continued


Comorbid Condition Recommendation
Confounding As in HIV-related opportunistic CNS disease, but not a direct conse-
quence of HIV. Other considerations include acutely symptomatic
primary or metastatic brain tumor, brain abscess, symptomatic
meningitis, uncontrolled epilepsy, progressive multiple scle-
rosis, stroke, and dementia due to causes other than HIV (e.g.,
Alzheimer’s disease for older persons).
Systemic disease
Secondary Chronic, stable systemic condition such as hypertension or asthma,
with no apparent cognitive sequelae or significant loss of function
but with subsequent cognitive and functional decline within the
context of HIV.
Contributing Chronic, stable systemic illness (e.g., chronic obstructive pulmonary
disease) associated with mild cognitive and/or functional impair-
ment, but with subsequent additional cognitive and functional
decline within the context of HIV.
Confounding Constitutional illness (e.g., persistent unexplained fever, diarrhea,
significant weight loss, disabling weakness). These could be due to
factors related or unrelated to HIV or to medication side effects.
Symptomatic new AIDS-related opportunistic conditions. Newly
diagnosed nutritional abnormality including B12 or thiamine
deficiency or newly diagnosed malnutrition. In these cases, direct
effects of HIV on cognitive or functional decline are difficult or
impossible to establish.
Co-infection with Hepatitis-C Virus (HCV)
Secondary Evidence of prior HCV infection with successful clearance (i.e., sero-
positive, but HCV RNA undetectable in plasma), either spontane-
ously or with treatment, and absence of decompensated liver disease
(defined by total serum bilirubin ≥ 1.5 mg/dL; INR ≥ 1.5; albumin
≤ 3.4 g/dL; platelet count < 75,000 K/MM3; or evidence of hepatic
encephalopathy or ascites). Evidence of subsequent cognitive and
functional decline within the context of HIV.
Contributing Evidence of current HCV infection (i.e., HCV RNA present in plasma)
but without decompensated liver disease (defined by total serum
bilirubin ≥ 1.5 mg/dL; INR ≥ 1.5; albumin ≤ 3.4 g/dL; platelet
count < 75,000 K/MM3; or evidence of hepatic encephalopathy or
ascites). In this situation, HIV and HCV may be associated with
independent risks for cognitive impairment and functional decline.
Confounding Evidence of current HCV infection (i.e., HCV RNA present in plasma)
with severe or decompensated liver disease (defined by total serum
bilirubin ≥ 1.5 mg/dL; INR ≥ 1.5; albumin ≤ 3.4 g/dL; platelet
count < 75,000 K/MM3; or evidence of hepatic encephalopathy or
ascites). In this situation, any cognitive impairments and functional
decline cannot be confidently attributed to HIV because of higher
risk of hepatic encephalopathy.

cART regimens and the amelioration of HIV- for HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders,
associated neurocognitive disorders. including psychostimulants, N-methyl-D-
aspartic acid (NMDA) antagonists (e.g.,
memantine), antioxidants (e.g., CPI-1189), anti-
Non-antiretroviral Treatments inflammatory agents (e.g., lexipafant), mood sta-
A variety of adjunctive, non-antiretroviral drugs bilizers, and antidepressants. Methylphenidate,
have been evaluated as possible treatments for example, has demonstrated modest effects
386 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

in improving psychomotor slowing in HIV directly restore the underlying impaired func-
(e.g., Hinkin et al., 2001). Extending in vitro tion (“restitution training”). Although system-
research on the possible neuroprotective effects atic reviews generally support the effectiveness of
of lithium (viz., inhibition of gp120-mediated cognitive remediation in brain injury (Cicerone
neurotoxicity; Everall et al., 2002), Letendre et al., 2005), there has been virtually no research
et al. (2006) reported that low doses of oral lith- investigating its effectiveness in persons with
ium improved neuropsychological functioning HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders. One
in a small, single-arm, open-label study of eight small pilot study examined an intervention
individuals with HIV-associated neurocognitive based on Spaced Retrieval (SR), a memory tech-
disorders. Another class of psychotropic agents, nique that teaches individuals to recall informa-
serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SRIs; e.g., citalo- tion over progressively longer intervals of time,
pram), were associated with a reduced HIV rep- in combination with external memory aids in
lication in the CSF and better neurocognitive ten older adults with HIV. Among those who
performance in a large, observational cohort identified at least one goal (e.g., remembering
study (Letendre et al., 2007). Importantly, the appointments), 90% initially mastered the cor-
cognitive benefits of these psychotropic drugs rect response and procedure, 60% retained the
do not appear to be a function of improvements correct response and procedure over a 2-month
in depressive symptomatology, which (as noted interval (demonstrating retention of mastery),
above) is not reliably related to neuropsycholog- and 100% self-reported that the intervention
ical impairment in HIV (Goggin et al., 1997). had helped them achieve their goal (Neundorfer
Nevertheless, the veracity of these preliminary et al., 2004). In another recent study, Andrade
findings regarding the effectiveness of adjunc- and colleagues (2005) employed a randomized
tive therapies for HIV-associated neurocog- controlled trial to investigate the effect of an
nitive disorders remains to be systematically electronic memory prompting device on adher-
evaluated in larger clinical trials. For example, ence to cART and found that the device, which
Selegiline (i.e., L-deprenyl) is a monoamine provided electronic verbal reminders at dosing
oxidase inhibitor with neuroprotective poten- times, improved medication adherence in HIV-
tial (e.g., Turchan-Cholewo et al., 2006) that infected individuals with memory impairment.
ameliorated deficits in delayed list recall and
motor speed in a small pilot study (Sacktor
HIV-Associated Neurocognitive
et al., 2000), but was generally ineffective in
Disorders in the Developing
a phase II clinical trial (Schifitto et al., 2006).
World
More recent novel agents of interest for treat-
ing HIV-associated cognitive disorders include The HIV pandemic is not restricted to the devel-
Substance P, a neuropeptide with an affinity oped world as approximately 40 million people
for the neurokinin-1 receptor (Ho & Douglas, are living with HIV infection worldwide, with
2004), and Minocycline, an antibacterial with the low- and middle-income countries in Africa
potent anti-inflammatory and neuroprotec- and Asia being particularly affected (UNAIDS,
tive properties (Zink et al., 2005), which is the 2006). Accompanying the much-needed
focus of a new, randomized, placebo-controlled humanitarian efforts to increase access to HIV
clinical trial. prevention strategies and antiretroviral thera-
pies in these resource-limited areas, there has
been considerable scientific interest in interna-
Cognitive Rehabilitation
tional neuroAIDS. Basic science observations
Cognitive remediation is a form of rehabili- regarding the differential neurovirulence of
tation therapy used to treat individuals with genetic variations in viral clades have stimu-
neurological deficits, typically those recover- lated interest in the prevalence, mechanisms,
ing from brain injury. Cognitive rehabilitation and functional impact of HIV-associated CNS
techniques have generally emphasized strate- dysfunction in these settings. Although find-
gies to compensate for residual cognitive deficits ings so far are inconsistent, some research sug-
(“strategy training”), rather than attempting to gests that B clade virus (more prevalent in the
Neuropsychological Aspects of HIV Infection 387

United States and Europe) may be more neuro- Archibald, S. L., Masliah, E., Fennema-Notestine,
virulent than non-B clade variants (e.g., C clade C., Marcotte, T. D., Ellis, R. J., McCutchan, J. A.,
virus in Africa, India, and Brazil) perhaps as et al. (2004). Correlation of in vivo neuroimaging
a function of differing chemotactic properties abnormalities with postmortem human immuno-
deficiency virus encephalitis and dendritic loss.
(Ranga et al., 2004) or glutamate production in
Archives of Neurology, 61, 369–376.
infected macrophages (Zheng et al., 2007). Of
Arnsten, J. H., Demas, P. A., Grant, R. W., Gourevitch,
course, investigation of the cognitive sequelae M. N., Farzadegan, H., Howard, A. A., et al. (2002).
of HIV infection in resource-limited countries Impact of active drug use on antiretroviral therapy
is a complex undertaking for which a sophisti- adherence and viral suppression in HIV-infected
cated and collaborative cross-cultural approach drug users. Journal of General Internal Medicine,
is necessary. The HIV Dementia Scale, for 17, 377–381.
example, was recently adapted by Sacktor and Atkinson, J. H., Person, C., Young, C., Deitch, D., &
colleagues (2005) for international use in per- Treisman, G. (2005). Psychiatric disorders. In H.
sons not fluent in English by replacing its lan- E. Gendelman, I. Grant, I. Everall, S. A. Lipton,
guage-based subtests with finger tapping and and S. Swindells (Eds.), The Neurology of AIDS
(2nd edition, pp. 553–564). New York: Oxford
alternating hand position tests. Preliminary
University Press.
data suggest that the International HDS may
Avants, S. K., Margolin, A., McMahon, T. J., & Kosten,
be useful for basic screening and monitoring of T. R. (1997). Association between self-report of
HAD in resource-limited settings (e.g., Sacktor cognitive impairment, HIV status, and cocaine
et al., 2006). However, in addition to the obvi- use in a sample of cocaine-dependent methadone-
ous linguistic complexities of test translation maintained patients. Addictive Behaviors, 22,
and adaptation, the myriad socioeconomic and 599–611.
cultural factors that ostensibly influence the Aylward, E.H., Henderer, J.D., McArthur, J.C.,
validity of cognitive testing in these settings Brettschneider, P.D., Harris, G.J., Barta, P.E., et
must also be considered in future international al. (1993). Reduced basal ganglia volume in HIV-
neuroAIDS research efforts (e.g., the relevance 1-associated dementia: Results from quantitative
neuroimaging. Neurology, 43(10), 2099–2104.
of some cognitive constructs, such as speeded
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1994). Developments in
motor and information processing, may vary
the concept of working memory. Neuropsychology,
widely across cultures). 8, 485–493.
Bartok, J.A., Martin, E. M., Pitrak, D. L., Novak, R. M.,
References Pursell, K. J., Mullane, K. M., et al. (1997). Working
memory deficits in HIV-seropositive drug users.
Abrams, D. I., Jay, C. A., Shade, S. B., Vizoso, H., Journal of the International Neuropsychological
Reda, H., Press, S., et al. (2007). Cannabis in pain- Society, 3, 451–456.
ful HIV-associated sensory neuropathy: A ran- Basso, M. R., & Bornstein, R. A. (2000). Estimated
domized placebo-controlled trial. Neurology, 68, premorbid intelligence mediates neurobehavioral
515–521. change in individuals infected with HIV across
Airaksinen, E., Larsson, M., & Forsell, Y. (2005). 12 months. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Neuropsychological functions in anxiety disor- Neuropsychology, 22, 208–218.
ders in population-based samples: Evidence of epi- Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio,
sodic memory dysfunction. Journal of Psychiatric A. R. (1997). Deciding advantageously before
Research, 39, 207–214. knowing the advantageous strategy. Science, 275,
Andrade, A. S., McGruder, H. F., Wu, A. W., 1293–1295.
Celano, S. A., Skolasky, R. L., Jr., Selnes, O. A., Becker, J. T., & Salthouse, T. A. (1999).
et al. (2005). A programmable prompting device Neuropsychologicaltestperformanceintheacquired
improves adherence to highly active antiretrovi- immunodeficiency syndrome: Independent effects
ral therapy in HIV-infected subjects with mem- of diagnostic group on functioning. Journal of the
ory impairment. Clinical Infectious Diseases, 41, International Neuropsychological Society, 5, 41–47.
875–882. Benedict, R. H. B., Mezhir, J. J., Walsh, K., & Hewitt,
Antinori, A., Arendt, G., Becker, J. T., Brew, B. J., R. G. (2000). Impact of human immunodeficiency
Byrd, D. A., Cherner, M., et al. (2007). Updated virus type-1-associated cognitive dysfunction on
research nosology for HIV-associated neurocog- activities of daily living and quality of life. Archives
nitive disorders. Neurology, 69, 1789–1799. of Clinical Neuropsychology, 15, 535–544.
388 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Berger, J. R., & Arendt, G. (2000). HIV dementia: The and degeneration of interneurons in HIV+ meth-
role of the basal ganglia and dopaminergic systems. amphetamine users. Neurology, 67, 1486–1489.
Journal of Psychopharmacology, 14, 214–221. Chandrasekhar, S. S., Connelly, P. E., Brahmbhatt, S.
Braithwaite, R. S., McGinnis, K. A., Conigliaro, J., S., Shah, C. S., Kloser, P. C., & Baredes, S. (2000).
Maisto, S. A., Crystal, S., Day, N., et al. (2005). A Otologic and audiologic evaluation of human
temporal and dose-response association between immunodeficiency virus-infected patients.
alcohol consumption and medication adherence American Journal of Otolaryngology, 21, 1–9.
among veterans in care. Alcoholism: Clinical and Chang, L., Ernst, T., Leonido-Yee, M., & Speck, O.
Experimental Research, 29, 1190–1197. (2000). Perfusion MRI detects rCBF abnormalities
Breitbart, W., Marotta, R., Platt, M. M., Weisman, in early stages of HIV-cognitive motor complex.
H., Derevenco, M., Grau, C., et al. (1996). A dou- Neurology, 54, 389–396.
ble-blind trial of haloperidol, chlorpromazine, Chang, L., Ernst, T., Speck, O., & Grob, C. S.
and lorazepam in the treatment of delirium in (2005). Additive effects of HIV and chronic
hospitalized AIDS patients. American Journal of methamphetamine use on brain metabolite
Psychiatry, 153, 231–237. abnormalities. American Journal of Psychiatry,
Brew, B. J. (2004). Evidence for a change in AIDS 162, 361–369.
dementia complex in the era of highly active anti- Chayakulkeeree, M., & Perfect, J. R. (2006).
retroviral therapy and the possibility of new forms Cryptococcosis. Infectious Disease Clinics of North
of AIDS dementia complex. AIDS, 18(Suppl 1), America, 20, 507–544, v–vi.
S75–S78. Chen, W., Tang, Z., Fortina, P., Patel, P., Addya, S.,
Budka, H. (2005). The neuropathology of HIV- Surrey, S., et al. (2005). Ethanol potentiates HIV-1
associated brain disease. In H. E. Gendelman, I. gp120-induced apoptosis in human neurons via
Grant, I. Everall, S. A. Lipton, and S. Swindells both the death receptor and NMDA receptor path-
(Eds.), The neurology of AIDS (2nd ed., pp. 375– ways. Virology, 334, 59–73.
391). New York: Oxford University Press. Cherner, M., Ellis, R. J., Lazzaretto, D., Young, C.,
Carey, C. L., Woods, S. P., Rippeth, J. D., Gonzalez, Mindt, M. R., Atkinson, J. H., et al. (2004). Effects
R., Heaton, R. K., & Grant, I. (2006a). Additive del- of HIV-1 infection and aging on neurobehavioral
eterious effects of methamphetamine dependence functioning: Preliminary findings. AIDS, 18(Suppl
and immunosuppression on neuropsychological 1), S27–S34.
functioning in HIV infection. AIDS and Behavior, Cherner, M., Letendre, S., Heaton, R. K., Durelle, J.,
10, 185–190. Marquie-Beck, J., Gragg, B., et al. (2005). Hepatitis
Carey, C. L., Woods, S. P., Rippeth, J. D., Gonzalez, R., C augments cognitive deficits associated with HIV
Moore, D. J., Marcotte, T. D., et al. (2004). Initial infection and methamphetamine. Neurology, 64,
validation of a screening battery for the detec- 1343–1347.
tion of HIV-associated cognitive impairment. The Cicerone, K. D., Dahlberg, C., Malec, J. F.,
Clinical Neuropsychologist, 18, 234–248. Langenbahn, D. M., Felicetti, T., Kneipp, S., et
Carey, C. L., Woods, S. P., Rippeth, J. D., Heaton, al. (2005). Evidence-based cognitive rehabilita-
R. K., & Grant, I. (2006b). Prospective mem- tion: Updated review of the literature from 1998
ory in HIV-1 infection. Journal of Clinical and through 2002. Archives of Physical Medicine and
Experimental Neuropsychology, 28, 536–548. Rehabilitation, 86, 1681–1692.
Casau, N. C. (2005). Perspective on HIV infection Ciesla, J. A., and Roberts, J. E. (2001). Meta-analysis
and aging: Emerging research on the horizon. of the relationship between HIV infection and
Clinical Infectious Diseases, 41, 855–863. risk for depressive disorders. American Journal of
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (1993). Psychiatry, 158, 725–730.
Classification system for HIV infection and Cinque, P., Vago, L., Mengozzi, M., Torri, V., Ceresa,
expanded surveillance case definition for AIDS D., Vicenzi, E., et al. (1998). Elevated cerebrospi-
among adolescents and adults. Journal of the nal fluid levels of monocyte chemotactic protein-1
American Medical Association, 269,729–730. correlate with HIV-1 encephalitis and local viral
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2001). replication. AIDS, 12, 1327–1332.
HIV/AIDS surveillance report. Atlanta, GA: Author. Clifford, D. B., Evans, S. R., Yang, Y., & Gulick, R. M.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2005). (2005). The neuropsychological and neurological
HIV/AIDS surveillance report, 2004. Atlanta, GA: impact of hepatitis C virus co-infection in HIV-
Author. infected subjects. AIDS, 19(Suppl 3), S64–S71.
Chana, G., Everall, I. P., Crews, L., Langford, D., Corder, E. H., Robertson, K., Lannfelt, L.,
Adame, A., Grant, I., et al. (2006). Cognitive deficits Bogdanovic, N., Eggertsen, G., Wilkins, J., et al.
Neuropsychological Aspects of HIV Infection 389

(1998). HIV-infected subjects with the E4 allele for (2000). HIV-1, cocaine, and neuropsychological
APOE have excess dementia and peripheral neu- performance in African American men. Journal
ropathy. Nature Medicine, 4, 1182–1184. of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6,
Cristiani, S. A., Pukay-Martin, N. D., & Bornstein, 322–335.
R. A. (2004). Marijuana use and cognitive func- Egan, V. G., Crawford, J. R., Brettle, R. P., & Goodwin,
tion in HIV-infected people. The Journal of G. M. (1990). The Edinburgh cohort of HIV-
Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 16, positive drug users: Current intellectual function
330–335. is impaired, but not due to early AIDS dementia
Croxford, J. L. (2003). Therapeutic potential of complex. AIDS, 4, 651–656.
cannabinoids in CNS disease. CNS Drugs, 17, Elliott, A. (1998). Anxiety and HIV infection. STEP
179–202. Perspective, 98, 11–14.
Cutler, R. G., Haughey, N. J., Tammara, A., Ellis, R., Langford, D., & Masliah, E. (2007). HIV
McArthur, J. C., Nath, A., Reid, R., et al. (2004). and antiretroviral therapy in the brain: Neuronal
Dysregulation of sphingolipid and sterol metabo- injury and repair. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience,
lism by APOE4 in HIV dementia. Neurology, 63, 8, 33–44.
626–630. Ellis, R. J., Deutsch, R., Heaton, R. K., Marcotte, T.
Cysique, L. A., Deutsch, R., Atkinson, J. H., Young, D., McCutchan, J. A., Nelson, J. A., et al. (1997a).
C., Marcotte, T. D., Dawson, L., et al. (2007). Neurocognitive impairment is an independent
Incident major depression does not affect neuro- risk factor for death in HIV infection. Archives of
psychological functioning in HIV-infected men. Neurology, 54, 416–424.
Journal of the International Neuropsychological Ellis, R. J., Hsia, K., Spector, S. A., Nelson, J. A., Heaton,
Society, 13, 1–11. R. K., Wallace, M. R., et al. (1997b). Cerebrospinal
Cysique, L. A., Maruff, P., & Brew, B. J. (2004). fluid human immunodeficiency virus type 1 RNA
Antiretroviral therapy in HIV infection: Are neu- levels are elevated in neurocognitively impaired
rologically active drugs important? Archives of individuals with acquired immunodeficiency syn-
Neurology, 61, 1699–1704. drome. Annals of Neurology, 42, 679–688.
Cysique, L. A., Maruff, P., & Brew, B. J. (2006). Ellis, R. J., Moore, D. J., Childers, M. E., Letendre,
The neuropsychological profi le of symptom- S., McCutchan, J. A., Wolfson, T., et al. (2002).
atic AIDS and ADC patients in the pre-HAART Progression to neuropsychological impairment
era: A meta-analysis. Journal of the International in human immunodeficiency virus infection pre-
Neuropsychological Society, 12, 368–382. dicted by elevated cerebrospinal fluid levels of
Damasio, A. R., & Tranel, D. (1993). Nouns and verbs human immunodeficiency virus RNA. Archives of
are retrieved with differently distributed neural Neurology, 59, 923–928.
systems. Proceedings of the National Academy Esiri, M. M., Biddolph, S. C., & Morris, C. S. (1998).
of Sciences of the United States of America, 90, Prevalence of Alzheimer plaques in AIDS. Journal
4957–4960. of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 65,
Davis, S. J., Schockmel, G. A., Somoza, C., Buck, 29–33.
D. W., Healey, D. G., Rieber, E. P., et al. (1992). Everall, I. P., Bell, C., Mallory, M., Langford, D.,
Antibody and HIV-1 gp120 recognition of CD4 Adame, A., Rockestein, E., et al. (2002). Lithium
undermines the concept of mimicry between anti- ameliorates HIV-gp120-mediated neurotoxic-
bodies and receptors. Nature, 358, 76–79. ity. Molecular and Cellular Neurosciences, 21,
Delis, D. C., Peavy, G., Heaton, R., Butters, N., 493–501.
Salmon, D. P., Taylor, M., et al. (1995). Do patients Fagnoni, F. F., Vescovini, R., Passeri, G., Bologna,
with HIV-associated minor cognitive/motor dis- G., Pedrazzoni, M., Lavagetto, G., et al. (2000).
order exhibit a “subcortical” memory profile? Shortage of circulating naive CD8(+) T cells pro-
Evidence using the California Verbal Learning vides new insights on immunodeficiency in aging.
Test. Assessment, 2, 151–165. Blood, 95, 2860–2868.
Durvasula, R. S., Myers, H. F., Mason, K., & Hinkin, Farinpour, R., Martin, E. M., Seidenberg, M., Pitrak,
C. (2006). Relationship between alcohol use/ D. L., Pursell, K. J., Mullane, K. M., et al. (2000).
abuse, HIV infection and neuropsychological per- Verbal working memory in HIV-seropositive
formance in African American men. Journal of drug users. Journal of the International
Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28, Neuropsychological Society, 6, 548–555.
383–404. Fein, G., Biggins, C. A., & MacKay, S. (1995). Delayed
Durvasula, R. S., Myers, H. F., Satz, P., Miller, E. latency of the event-related brain potential P3A
N., Morgenstern, H., Richardson, M. A., et al. component in HIV disease. Progressive effects
390 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

with increasing cognitive impairment. Archives of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3,


Neurology, 52, 1109–1118. 457–464.
Ferrando, S., van Gorp, W., McElhiney, M., Goggin, Goldberg, T. E., & Weinberger, D. R. (2004). Genes
K., Sewell, M., & Rabkin, J. (1998). Highly active and the parsing of cognitive processes. Trends in
antiretroviral treatment in HIV infection: Benefits Cognitive Sciences, 8, 325–335.
for neuropsychological function. AIDS, 12, Gongvatana, A., Schweinsburg, B. C., Taylor, M. J.,
F65–F70. Theilmann, R. J., Letendre, S. L., Alhassoon,
Forton, D. M., Allsop, J. M., Cox, I. J., Hamilton, G., O., et al. (2008). HIV-associated white mat-
Wesnes, K., Thomas, H. C., et al. (2005). A review ter tract injury and neurocognitive impair-
of cognitive impairment and cerebral metabolite ment in the HAART era. [abstract]. Journal of
abnormalities in patients with hepatitis C infec- the International Neuropsychological Society, 14
tion. AIDS, 19(Suppl 3), S53–S63. (Suppl. 1), 145–146.
Freeman, W. R., McCutchan, J. A., Arevalo, J. F., Gongvatana, A., Woods, S. P., Taylor, M. J., Vigil, O.,
Wolfson, T., Marcotte, T. D., Heaton, R. K., et al. Grant, I., & The HNRC Group. (2007). Semantic
(2004). The relationship between AIDS retinal clustering inefficiency in HIV-associated demen-
cotton wool spots and neuropsychological impair- tia. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical
ment in HIV-positive individuals in the pre- Neurosciences, 19, 36–42.
highly active antiretroviral therapy era. Ocular Gonzalez, E., Rovin, B. H., Sen, L., Cooke, G., Dhanda,
Immunology and Inflammation, 12, 25–33. R., Mummidi, S., et al. (2002). HIV-1 infection and
Gallant, J. E., & Block, D. S. (1998). Adherence to AIDS dementia are influenced by a mutant MCP-1
antiretroviral regimens in HIV-infected patients: allele linked to increased monocyte infi ltration
Results of a survey among physicians and patients. of tissues and MCP-1 levels. Proceedings of the
Journal of International Association of Physicians National Academy of Sciences of the United States
in AIDS Care, 4, 32–35. of America, 99, 13795–13800.
Dal Pan, G.J., McArthur, J.H., Aylward, E., Selnes, Gonzalez, R., Vassileva, J., Bechara, A., Grbesic, S.,
O.A., Nance-Sproson, T.E., Kumar, A.J., Mellits, Sworowski, L., Novak, R. M., et al. (2005). The
E.D., & McArthur, J.C. (1992). Patterns of cere- influence of executive functions, sensation seek-
bral atrophy in HIV-1-infected individuals: results ing, and HIV serostatus on the risky sexual prac-
of a quantitative MRI analysis. Neurology, 42, tices of substance-dependent individuals. Journal
2125–2130. of the International Neuropsychological Society, 11,
Galvan, F. H., Bing, E. G., Fleishman, J. A., London, 121–131.
A. S., Caetano, R., Burnam, M. A., et al. (2002). Gonzalez-Scarano, F., & Martin-Garcia, J. (2005).
The prevalence of alcohol consumption and heavy The neuropathogenesis of AIDS. Nature Reviews.
drinking among people with HIV in the United Immunology, 5, 69–81.
States: Results from the HIV cost and services uti- Grant, I., & Atkinson, J. H. (1999). Neuropsychiatric
lization study. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 63, aspects of HIV infection and AIDS. In B. J. Sadock
179–186. and V. A. Sadock (Eds.), Kaplan and Sadock’s com-
Gan, X., Zhang, L., Newton, T., Chang, S. L., Ling, prehensive textbook of psychiatry VII (pp. 308–
W., Kermani, V., et al. (1998). Cocaine infusion 335). Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins.
increases interferon-gamma and decreases inter- Grant, I., Atkinson, J. H., Hesselink, J. R., Kennedy,
leukin-10 in cocaine-dependent subjects. Clinical C. J., Richman, D. D., Spector, S. A., et al. (1987).
Immunology and Immunopathology, 89, 181–190. Evidence for early central nervous system involve-
Geier, S. A., Perro, C., Klauss, V., Naber, D., ment in the acquired immunodeficiency syndrome
Kronawitter, U., Bogner, J. R., et al. (1993). HIV- (AIDS) and other human immunodeficiency virus
related ocular microangiopathic syndrome and (HIV) infections. Studies with neuropsychologic
cognitive functioning. Journal of Acquired Immune testing and magnetic resonance imaging. Annals
Deficiency Syndromes, 6, 252–258. of Internal Medicine, 107, 828–836.
Goetz, M. B., Boscardin, W. J., Wiley, D., & Grant, I., Gonzalez, R., Carey, C. L., Natarajan,
Alkasspooles, S. (2001). Decreased recovery of L., & Wolfson, T. (2003). Non-acute (resid-
CD4 lymphocytes in older HIV-infected patients ual) neurocognitive effects of cannabis use: A
beginning highly active antiretroviral therapy. meta-analytic study. Journal of the International
AIDS, 15, 1576–1579. Neuropsychological Society, 9, 679–689.
Goggin, K. J., Zisook, S., Heaton, R. K., Atkinson, Grant, I., Olshen, R. A., Atkinson, J. H., Heaton, R.,
J. H., Marshall, S., McCutchan, J. A., et al. (1997). Nelson, J., McCutchan, J. A. et al. (1993). Depressed
Neuropsychological performance of HIV-1 mood does not explain neuropsychological deficits
infected men with major depression. Journal of in HIV-infected persons. Neuropsychology, 7, 53–61.
Neuropsychological Aspects of HIV Infection 391

Grant, I., Sacktor, N., & McArthur, J. C. (2005). HIV of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 22,
neurocognitive disorders. In H. E. Gendelman, 16–24.
I. Grant, I. Everall, S. A. Lipton, and S. Swindells Hinkin, C. H., Castellon, S. A., Hardy, D. J.,
(Eds.), The neurology of AIDS (2nd ed. pp. 359– Farinpour, R., Newton, T., & Singer, E. (2001).
373). New York: Oxford University Press. Methylphenidate improves HIV-1-associated cog-
Greco, J. B., Westmoreland, S. V., Ratai, E. M., Lentz, nitive slowing. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and
M. R., Sakaie, K., He, J., et al. (2004). In vivo 1H Clinical Neuroscience, 13, 248–254.
MRS of brain injury and repair during acute SIV Hinkin, C. H., Hardy, D. J., Mason, K. I., Castellon,
infection in the macaque model of neuroAIDS. S. A., Durvasula, R. S., Lam, M. N., et al. (2004).
Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 51, 1108–1114. Medication adherence in HIV-infected adults:
Green, J. E., Saveanu, R. V., & Bornstein, R. A. (2004). Effect of patient age, cognitive status, and sub-
The effect of previous alcohol abuse on cognitive stance abuse. AIDS, 18(Suppl 1), S19–S25.
function in HIV infection. American Journal of Hiscock, M., & Hiscock, C. K. (1989). Refining the
Psychiatry, 161, 249–254. forced-choice method for the detection of malin-
Griffin, W. C., 3rd, Middaugh, L. D., & Tyor, W. R. gering. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
(2007). Chronic cocaine exposure in the SCID Neuropsychology, 11, 967–974.
mouse model of HIV encephalitis. Brain Research, Ho, W. Z., & Douglas, S. D. (2004). Substance P and
1134, 214–219. neurokinin-1 receptor modulation of HIV. Journal
Haase, A. T. (1986). Pathogenesis of lentivirus infec- of Neuroimmunology, 157, 48–55.
tions. Nature, 322, 130–136. Iudicello, J. E., Letendre, S. L, Carey, C. L., Dawson,
Hardy, D. J., & Hinkin, C. H. (2002). Reaction time M. S., Durelle, J., Grant, I., et al. (2008a). HIV-
performance in adults with HIV/AIDS. Journal of associated deficits in action (verb) fluency reflect
Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24, astrocytosis [abstract]. Journal of the International
912–929. Neuropsychological Society, 14(Suppl. 1), 146.
Hardy, D. J., Hinkin, C. H., Levine, A. J., Castellon, Iudicello, J. E., Woods, S. P., Parsons, T. D., Moran, L.
S. A., & Lam, M. N. (2006). Risky decision mak- M., Carey, C. L., & Grant, I. (2007). Verbal fluency
ing assessed with the gambling task in adults with in HIV infection: A meta-analytic review. Journal
HIV. Neuropsychology, 20, 355–360. of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13,
Harrington, D. L., Haaland, K. Y., Yeo, R. A., 183–189.
& Marder, E. (1990). Procedural memory in Iudicello, J. E., Woods, S. P., Weber, E., Dawson, M.
Parkinson’s disease: Impaired motor but not S., Scott, J. C., Carey, C. L., et al. (2008b). Cognitive
visuoperceptual learning. Journal of Clinical and mechanisms of switching in HIV-associated cat-
Experimental Neuropsychology, 12, 323–339. egory fluency deficits. Journal of Clinical and
Heaton, R., Marcotte, T. D., White, D. A., Ross, D., Experimental Neuropsychology, 30(7), 797–804.
Meredith, K., Taylor, M. J., et al. (1996). Nature Janssen, R. S., Saykin, A. J., Cannon, L., Campbell,
and vocational significance of neuropsychological J., Pinsky, P. F., Hessol, N. A., et al. (1989).
impairment associated with HIV infection. The Neurological and neuropsychological manifesta-
Clinical Neuropsychologist, 10, 1–14. tions of HIV-1 infection: Association with AIDS-
Heaton, R. K., Grant, I., Butters, N., White, D. A., related complex but not asymptomatic HIV-1
Kirson, D., Atkinson, J. H., et al. (1995). The HNRC infection. Annals of Neurology, 26, 592–600.
500—Neuropsychology of HIV infection at dif- Jarvik, J. G., Hesselink, J. R., Kennedy, C., Teschke,
ferent disease stages. Journal of the International R., Wiley, C., Spector, S.,et al. (1988). Acquired
Neuropsychological Society, 1, 231–251. immunodeficiency syndrome. Magnetic resonance
Heaton, R. K., Marcotte, T. D., Mindt, M. R., Sadek, patterns of brain involvement with pathologic cor-
J., Moore, D. J., Bentley, H., et al. (2004). The relation. Archives of Neurology, 45, 731–736.
impact of HIV-associated neuropsychological Jernigan, T. L., Gamst, A. C., Archibald, S. L.,
impairment on everyday functioning. Journal of Fennema-Notestine, C., Mindt, M. R., Marcotte,
the International Neuropsychological Society, 10, T. D., et al. (2005). Effects of methamphetamine
317–331. dependence and HIV infection on cerebral mor-
Hinkin, C. H., Castellon, S. A., Durvasula, R. S., phology. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162,
Hardy, D. J., Lam, M. N., Mason, K. I., et al. (2002). 1461–1472.
Medication adherence among HIV+ adults: Effects Kalechstein, A. D., Hinkin, C. H., van Gorp, W. G.,
of cognitive dysfunction and regimen complexity. Castellon, S. A., & Satz, P. (1998). Depression pre-
Neurology, 59, 1944–1950. dicts procedural but not episodic memory in HIV-1
Hinkin, C. H., Castellon, S. A., & Hardy, D. J. (2000). infection. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Dual task performance in HIV-1 infection. Journal Neuropsychology, 20, 529–535.
392 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Kelder, W., McArthur, J. C., Nance-Sproson, T., of recent stimulant use on sustained attention
McClernon, D., & Griffin, D. E. (1998). Beta- in HIV-infected adults. Journal of Clinical and
chemokines MCP-1 and RANTES are selec- Experimental Neuropsychology, 28, 29–42.
tively increased in cerebrospinal fluid of patients Lopez, O. L., Becker, J. T., Dew, M. A., Banks, G.,
with human immunodeficiency virus-associated Dorst, S. K., & McNeil, M. (1994). Speech motor
dementia. Annals of Neurology, 44, 831–835. control disorder after HIV infection. Neurology,
Keswani, S., Luciano, C., Pardo, C., Cherry, K., 44, 2187–2189.
Hoke, A., & McArthur, J. C. (2005). The spec- Manly, J. J., Patterson, T. L., Heaton, R., Semple, S. J.,
trum of peripheral neuropathies in AIDS. In H. E. White, D. A., Velin, R. A., et al. (1997). The rela-
Gendelman, I. Grant, I. Everall, S. A. Lipton, and tionship between neuropsychological function-
S. Swindells (Eds.), The neurology of AIDS (2nd ed., ing and coping activity among HIV-positive men.
pp. 423–443). New York: Oxford University Press. AIDS and Behavior, 1, 81–91.
Langford, D., Hurford, R., Hashimoto, M., Mapou, R. L., Law, W. A., Martin, A., Kampen,
Digicaylioglu, M., & Masliah, E. (2005). Signalling D., Salazar, A. M., & Rundell, J. R. (1993).
crosstalk in FGF2-mediated protection of endothe- Neuropsychological performance, mood, and
lial cells from HIV-gp120. BMC Neuroscience, 6, 8. complaints of cognitive and motor difficulties in
Law, W. A., Martin, A., Mapou, R. L., Roller, T. L., individuals infected with the human immunode-
Salazar, A. M., Temoshok, L. R., et al. (1994). ficiency virus. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and
Working memory in individuals with HIV Clinical Neurosciences, 5, 86–93.
infection. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Marcotte, T. D., Heaton, R. K., Wolfson, T., Taylor, M.
Neuropsychology, 16, 173–182. J., Alhassoon, O., Arfaa, K., et al. (1999). The impact
Lawrence, A. D., Watkins, L. H., Sahakian, B. J., of HIV-related neuropsychological dysfunction
Hodges, J. R., & Robbins, T. W. (2000). Visual on driving behavior. Journal of the International
object and visuospatial cognition in Huntington’s Neuropsychological Society, 5, 579–592.
disease: Implications for information processing Marcotte, T. D., Lazzaretto, D., Scott, J. C., Roberts,
in corticostriatal circuits. Brain, 123, 1349–1364. E., Woods, S. P., & Letendre, S. (2006). Visual
Letendre, S. L., Cherner, M., Ellis, R. J., Marquie- attention deficits are associated with driving
Beck, J., Gragg, B., Marcotte, T., et al. (2005). The accidents in cognitively-impaired HIV-infected
effects of hepatitis C, HIV, and methamphetamine individuals. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
dependence on neuropsychological performance: Neuropsychology, 28, 13–28.
Biological correlates of disease. AIDS, 19(Suppl 3), Marcotte, T. D., Wolfson, T., Rosenthal, T. J., Heaton,
S72–S78. R. K., Gonzalez, R., Ellis, R. J., et al. (2004). A mul-
Letendre, S. L., Marquie-Beck, J., Capparelli, E., timodal assessment of driving performance in
Best, B., Clifford, D. B., Collier, A. C., et al. (2008). HIV infection. Neurology, 63, 1417–1422.
Validation of the CNS Penetration-Effectiveness Margolin, A., Avants, S. K., Warburton, L. A., &
rank for quantifying antiretroviral penetra- Hawkins, K. A. (2002). Factors affecting cognitive
tion into the central nervous system. Archives of functioning in a sample of human immunodefi-
Neurology, 65, 65–70.. ciency virus-positive injection drug users. AIDS
Letendre, S. L., Marquie-Beck, J., Woods, S. P., Best, Patient Care and STDs, 16, 255–267.
B., Clifford, D. B., Collier, A. C., et al. (2007). Martin, A. (1994). HIV, cognition, and the basal
The role of cohort studies in drug development: ganglia. In I. Grant and A. Martin (Eds.),
Clinical evidence of antiviral activity of seroto- Neuropsychology of HIV infection (pp. 234–259).
nin reuptake inhibitors and HMG-CoA reductase New York: Oxford University Press.
inhibitors in the central nervous system. Journal Martin, E. M., Nixon, H., Pitrak, D. L., Weddington,
of Neuroimmune Pharmacology, 2, 120–127. W., Rains, N. A., Nunnally, G., et al. (2007).
Letendre, S. L., McCutchan, J. A., Childers, M. E., Characteristics of prospective memory deficits
Woods, S. P., Lazzaretto, D., Heaton, R. K., et in HIV-seropositive substance-dependent indi-
al. (2004). Enhancing antiretroviral therapy for viduals: Preliminary observations. Journal of
human immunodeficiency virus cognitive disor- Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 29,
ders. Annals of Neurology, 56, 416–423. 496–504.
Letendre, S. L., Woods, S. P., Ellis, R. J., Atkinson, J. Martin, E. M., Novak, R. M., Fendrich, M., Vassileva,
H., Masliah, E., van den Brande, G., et al. (2006). J., Gonzalez, R., Grbesic, S., et al. (2004a). Stroop
Lithium improves HIV-associated neurocognitive performance in drug users classified by HIV
impairment. AIDS, 20, 1885–1888. and hepatitis C virus serostatus. Journal of the
Levine, A. J., Hardy, D. J., Miller, E., Castellon, S. A., International Neuropsychological Society, 10,
Longshore, D., & Hinkin, C. H. (2006). The effect 298–300.
Neuropsychological Aspects of HIV Infection 393

Martin, E. M., Pitrak, D. L., Novak, R. M., Pursell, Mirza, S. A., Phelan, M., Rimland, D., Graviss, E.,
K. J., & Mullane, K. M. (1999). Reaction times are Hamill, R., Brandt, M. E., et al. (2003). The chang-
faster in HIV-seropositive patients on antiretro- ing epidemiology of cryptococcosis: An update
viral therapy: A preliminary report. Journal of from population-based active surveillance in 2
Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21, large metropolitan areas, 1992–2000. Clinical
730–735. Infectious Diseases, 36, 789–794.
Martin, E. M., Pitrak, D. L., Robertson, L. C., Mittenberg, W., Azrin, R., Millsaps, C., & Heilbronner,
Novak, R. M., Mullane, K. M., & Pursell, K. J. R. (1993). Identification of malingered head
(1995). Global-local analysis in HIV-1 infection. injury on the Wechsler Memory Scale—Revised.
Neuropsychology, 9, 102–109. Psychological Assessment, 5, 34–40.
Martin, E. M., Pitrak, D. L., Weddington, W., Mohsen, A. H., Murad, S., & Easterbrook, P. J. (2005).
Rains, N. A., Nunnally, G., Nixon, H., et al. Prevalence of hepatitis C in an ethnically diverse
(2004b). Cognitive impulsivity and HIV serosta- HIV-1-infected cohort in south London. HIV
tus in substance dependent males. Journal of the Medicine, 6, 206–215.
International Neuropsychological Society, 10, Monteiro de Almeida, S., Letendre, S., Zimmerman,
931–938. J., Lazzaretto, D., McCutchan, A., & Ellis, R.
Martin, E. M., Robertson, L. C., Sorensen, D. J., (2005). Dynamics of monocyte chemoattractant
Jagust, W. J., Mallon, K. F., & Chirurgi, V. A. protein type one (MCP-1) and HIV viral load in
(1993). Speed of memory scanning is not affected human cerebrospinal fluid and plasma. Journal of
in early HIV-1 infection. Journal of Clinical and Neuroimmunology, 169, 144–152.
Experimental Neuropsychology, 15, 311–320. Moore DJ, Woods SP, Lazzaretto DL, Depp CA,
Martin, E. M., Sullivan, T. S., Reed, R. A., Fletcher, Atkinson JH, Heaton RK, Grant I, and the HNRC
T. A., Pitrak, D. L., Weddington, W., et al. (2001). group. (2006). Cognitive impairment among indi-
Auditory working memory in HIV-1 infection. viduals with Bipolar Disorder and HIV infection.
Journal of the International Neuropsychological Journal of the International Neuropsychological
Society, 7, 20–26. Society, 12(Suppl 1), 150..
Maruff, P., Malone, V., McArthur-Jackson, C., Morgan, E. E., Woods, S. P., Childers, M., Marquie-
Mulhall, B., Benson, E., & Currie, J. (1995). Beck, J., Ellis, R. J., Grant, I., et al. (2008). Predictive
Abnormalities of visual spatial attention in HIV validity of demographically-adjusted normative
infection and the HIV-associated dementia com- standards for the HIV Dementia Scale. Journal of
plex. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 30,
Neurosciences, 7, 325–333. 83–90.
Mayeux, R., Stern, Y., Tang, M. X., Todak, G., Munoz-Moreno, J. A., Rodriguez, C., Prats, A.,
Marder, K., Sano, M., et al. (1993). Mortality risks Ferrer, M., Negredo, E., Garolera, M., et al.
in gay men with human immunodeficiency virus (2007). Recommended earlier initiation of ART
infection and cognitive impairment. Neurology, based on nadir CD4 cell count as a risk factor for
43, 176–182. HIV-related neurocognitive impairment (abstract
McArthur, J. C. (2004). HIV dementia: An evolving 383). Paper presented at the 14th Conference on
disease. Journal of Neuroimmunology, 157, 3–10. Retroviruses and Opportunistic Infections, Los
McArthur, J. C., McDermott, M. P., McClernon, Angeles, CA.
D., St Hillaire, C., Conant, K., Marder, K., et al. Murji, S., Rourke, S. B., Donders, J., Carter, S. L.,
(2004). Attenuated central nervous system infec- Shore, D., & Rourke, B. P. (2003). Theoretically
tion in advanced HIV/AIDS with combination derived CVLT subtypes in HIV-1 infection:
antiretroviral therapy. Archives of Neurology, 61, Internal and external validation. Journal of the
1687–1696. International Neuropsychological Society, 9, 1–16.
Meyerhoff, D. J. (2001). Effects of alcohol and HIV Nath, A., Hauser, K. F., Wojna, V., Booze, R. M.,
infection on the central nervous system. Alcohol Maragos, W., Prendergast, M., et al. (2002).
Research and Health, 25, 288–298. Molecular basis for interactions of HIV and drugs
Miguez, M. J., Shor-Posner, G., Morales, G., of abuse. Journal of Acquired Immune Deficiency
Rodriguez, A., & Burbano, X. (2003). HIV treat- Syndromes, 31(Suppl 2), S62–S69.
ment in drug abusers: Impact of alcohol use. Neundorfer, M. M., Camp, C. J., Lee, M. M.,
Addiction Biology, 8, 33–37. Skrajner, M. J., Malone, M. L., & Carr, J. R. (2004).
Millikin, C. P., Trepanier, L. L., & Rourke, S. B. (2004). Compensating for cognitive deficits in persons
Verbal fluency component analysis in adults with aged 50 and over with HIV/AIDS: A pilot study of
HIV/AIDS. Journal of Clinical and Experimental a cognitive intervention. Journal of HIV/AIDS and
Neuropsychology, 26, 933–942. Social Services, 3, 79–97.
394 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Nielsen-Bohlman, L., Boyle, D., Biggins, C., Powderly, W. G. (2002). Sorting through confusing
Ezekiel, F., & Fein, G. (1997). Semantic priming messages: The art of HAART. Journal of Acquired
impairment in HIV. Journal of the International Immune Deficiency Syndromes, 31(Suppl 1), S3–S9;
Neuropsychological Society, 3, 348–358. discussion S24–S25.
Olesen, P. J., Schendan, H. E., Amick, M. M., & Power, C., Selnes, O. A., Grim, J. A., & McArthur,
Cronin-Golomb, A. (2007). HIV infection affects J. C. (1995). HIV dementia scale: A rapid screen-
parietal-dependent spatial cognition: Evidence ing test. Journal of Acquired Immune Deficiency
from mental rotation and heirarchical pattern per- Syndromes and Human Retrovirology, 8, 273–278.
ception. Behavioral Neuroscience, 121, 1163–1173. Psychological Corporation. (1987). Wechsler Memory
Ongradi, J., Specter, S., Horvath, A., & Friedman, H. Scale—Revised. San Antonio, TX: Author.
(1998). Combined in vitro effect of marijuana and Pukay-Martin, N. D., Cristiani, S. A., Saveanu, R., &
retrovirus on the activity of mouse natural killer Bornstein, R. A. (2003). The relationship between
cells. Pathology Oncology Research, 4, 191–199. stressful life events and cognitive function in HIV-
Pau, C. W., Lee, T. M., & Chan, S. F. (2002). The infected men. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and
impact of heroin on frontal executive functions. Clinical Neurosciences, 15, 436–441.
Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 17, 663–670. Radkowski, M., Wilkinson, J., Nowicki, M., Adair,
Perez, J. L., & Moore, R. D. (2003). Greater effect of D., Vargas, H., Ingui, C., et al. (2002). Search for
highly active antiretroviral therapy on survival hepatitis C virus negative-strand RNA sequences
in people aged > or =50 years compared with and analysis of viral sequences in the central ner-
younger people in an urban observational cohort. vous system: Evidence of replication. Journal of
Clinical Infectious Diseases, 36, 212–218. Virology, 76, 600–608.
Peterson, P. K., Gekker, G., Chao, C. C., Schut, R., Ranga, U., Shankarappa, R., Siddappa, N. B.,
Molitor, T. W., & Balfour, H. H., Jr. (1991). Cocaine Ramakrishna, L., Nagendran, R., Mahalingam,
potentiates HIV-1 replication in human peripheral M., et al. (2004). Tat protein of human immunode-
blood mononuclear cell cocultures. Involvement ficiency virus type 1 subtype C strains is a defective
of transforming growth factor-beta. Journal of chemokine. Journal of Virology, 78, 2586–2590.
Immunology, 146, 81–84. Razani, J., Murphy, C., Davidson, T. M., Grant,
Pfefferbaum, A., Adalsteinsson, E., & Sullivan, E. V. I., & McCutchan, A. (1996). Odor sensitivity is
(2005). Cortical NAA deficits in HIV infection impaired in HIV-positive cognitively impaired
without dementia: Influence of alcoholism comor- patients. Physiology and Behavior, 59, 877–881.
bidity. Neuropsychopharmacology, 30, 1392–1399. Reger, M., Welsh, R., Razani, J., Martin, D. J., &
Pfefferbaum, A., Rosenbloom, M. J., Adalsteinsson, Boone, K. B. (2002). A meta-analysis of the neuro-
E., & Sullivan, E. V. (2007). Diff usion tensor imag- psychological sequelae of HIV infection. Journal
ing with quantitative fibre tracking in HIV infec- of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8,
tion and alcoholism comorbidity: Synergistic 410–424.
white matter damage. Brain, 130s, 48–64. Reger, M. A., Martin, D. J., Cole, S. L., & Strauss, G.
Pfefferbaum, A., Rosenbloom, M. J., Rohlfi ng, T., (2005). The relationship between plasma viral load
Adalsteinsson, E., Kemper, C. A., Deresinski, S., and neuropsychological functioning in HIV-1
et al. (2006). Contribution of alcoholism to brain infection. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology,
dysmorphology in HIV infection: Effects on the 20, 137–143.
ventricles and corpus callosum. Neuroimage, 33, Rippeth, J. D., Heaton, R. K., Carey, C. L., Marcotte,
239–251. T. D., Moore, D. J., Gonzalez, R., et al. (2004).
Piatt, A. L., Fields, J. A., Paolo, A. M., Koller, W. C., Methamphetamine dependence increases risk
& Troster, A. I. (1999a). Lexical, semantic, and of neuropsychological impairment in HIV
action verbal fluency in Parkinson’s disease with infected persons. Journal of the International
and without dementia. Journal of Clinical and Neuropsychological Society, 10, 1–14.
Experimental Neuropsychology, 21, 435–443. Robertson, K., Su, Z., Krambrink, A., Evans, S.,
Piatt, A. L., Fields, J. A., Paolo, A. M., & Troster, A. I. Havlir, D., Margolis, D., et al. (2007). This is your
(1999b). Action (verb naming) fluency as an exec- brain off drugs: Neurocognitive function before and
utive function measure: Convergent and diver- after ART discontinuation in patients with high
gent evidence of validity. Neuropsychologia, 37, CD4 nadir (ACTG A5170) (abstract 113). Paper
1499–1503. presented at the 14th Conference on Retroviruses
Pomara, N., Crandall, D. T., Choi, S. J., Johnson, G., and Opportunistic Infections, Los Angeles, CA.
& Lim, K. O. (2001). White matter abnormalities Robertson, K. R., Parsons, T. D., Sidtis, J. J., Hanlon
in HIV-1 infection: A diff usion tensor imaging Inman, T., Robertson, W. T., Hall, C. D., et al.
study. Psychiatry Research, 106, 15–24. (2006). Timed gait test: Normative data for the
Neuropsychological Aspects of HIV Infection 395

assessment of the AIDS dementia complex. Journal Scott, J. C., Woods, S. P., Patterson, K. A., Morgan, E.
of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28, E., Heaton, R. K., Grant, I., et al. (2006). Recency
1053–1064. effects in HIV-associated dementia are character-
Rourke, S. B., Halman, M. H., & Bassel, C. (1999). ized by deficient encoding. Neuropsychologia, 44,
Neurocognitive complaints in HIV-infection 1336–1343.
and their relationship to depressive symptoms Scott, J. C., Woods, S. P., Matt, G. E., Meyer, R.
and neuropsychological functioning. Journal of A., Heaton, R. K., Atkinson, J. H., et al. (2007).
Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21, Neurocognitive effects of methamphetamine: A
737–756. critical review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychology
Ruiz Perez, I., Rodriguez Bano, J., Lopez Ruz, M. A., Review, 17, 275–297.
del Arco Jimenez, A., Causse Prados, M., Pasquau Selnes, O. A., Galai, N., McArthur, J. C., Cohn, S.,
Liano, J., et al. (2005). Health-related quality of Royal, W., Esposito, D., & Vlahov, D. (1997). HIV
life of patients with HIV: Impact of sociodemo- infection and cognition in intravenous drug users:
graphic, clinical and psychosocial factors. Quality long-term follow-up. Neurology, 48, 223–230.
of Life Research, 14, 1301–1310. Sevigny, J. J., Albert, S. M., McDermott, M. P.,
Ryan, E. L., Morgello, S., Isaacs, K., Naseer, M., & McArthur, J. C., Sacktor, N., Conant, K., et al.
Gerits, P. (2004). Neuropsychiatric impact of hep- (2004). Evaluation of HIV RNA and markers of
atitis C on advanced HIV. Neurology, 62, 957–962. immune activation as predictors of HIV-associated
Sacktor, N., Lyles, R. H., Skolasky, R., Kleeberger, dementia. Neurology, 63, 2084–2090.
C., Selnes, O. A., Miller, E. N., et al. (2001). HIV- Sevigny, J. J., Albert, S. M., McDermott, M. P.,
associated neurologic disease incidence changes: Schifitto, G., McArthur, J. C., Sacktor, N., et al.
Multicenter AIDS cohort study, 1990–1998. (2007). An evaluation of neurocognitive status
Neurology, 56, 257–260. and markers of immune activation as predictors of
Sacktor, N., Nakasujja, N., Skolasky, R., Robertson, K., time to death in advanced HIV infection. Archives
Wong, M., Musisi, S., et al. (2006). Antiretroviral of Neurology, 64, 97–102.
therapy improves cognitive impairment in HIV+ Simpson, D. M., Kitch, D., Evans, S. R., McArthur,
individuals in sub-Saharan Africa. Neurology, 67, J. C., Asmuth, D. M., Cohen, B., et al. (2006).
311–314. HIV neuropathy natural history cohort study:
Sacktor, N., Schifitto, G., McDermott, M. P., Marder, Assessment measures and risk factors. Neurology,
K., McArthur, J. C., & Kieburtz, K. (2000). 66, 1679–1687.
Transdermal selegiline in HIV-associated cogni- Singh, K. K., Ellis, R. J., Marquie-Beck, J., Letendre,
tive impairment: Pilot, placebo-controlled study. S., Heaton, R. K., Grant, I., et al. (2004). CCR2
Neurology, 54, 233–235. polymorphisms affect neuropsychological
Sacktor, N. C., Wong, M., Nakasujja, N., Skolasky, R. impairment in HIV-1-infected adults. Journal of
L., Selnes, O. A., Musisi, S., et al. (2005). The inter- Neuroimmunology, 157, 185–192.
national HIV dementia scale: A new rapid screen- Slick, D. J., Hinkin, C. H., van Gorp, W. G., & Satz,
ing test for HIV dementia. AIDS, 19, 1367–1374. P. (2001). Base rate of a WMS-R malingering index
Sadek, J. R., Johnson, S. A., White, D. A., Salmon, in a sample of non-compensation-seeking men
D. P., Taylor, K. I., Delapena, J. H., et al. (2004). infected with HIV-1. Applied Neuropsychology, 8,
Retrograde amnesia in dementia: Comparison of 185–189.
HIV-associated dementia, Alzheimer’s disease, Stein, M., Herman, D. S., Trisvan, E., Pirraglia, P.,
and Huntington’s disease. Neuropsychology, 18, Engler, P., & Anderson, B. J. (2005). Alcohol use
692–699. and sexual risk behavior among human immu-
Schifitto, G., Sacktor, N., Zhang, J., Evans, S., Simpson, nodeficiency virus-positive persons. Alcoholism:
D., Millar, L., et al. (2006). A phase ii, placebo-con- Clinical and Experimental Research, 29, 837–843.
trolled, double-blind study of the selegiline trans- Stern, R. A., Silva, S. G., Chaisson, N., & Evans, D. L.
dermal system in the treatment of HIV-associated (1996). Influence of cognitive reserve on neuropsy-
cognitive impairment (abstract 364). Paper pre- chological functioning in asymptomatic human
sented at the 13th Conference on Retroviruses and immunodeficiency virus-1 infection. Archives of
Opportunistic Infections, Denver, CO. Neurology, 53, 148–153.
Schulte, T., Mueller-Oehring, E. M., Rosenbloom, Stout, J. C., Ellis, R. J., Jernigan, T. L., Archibald,
M. J., Pfefferbaum, A., & Sullivan, E. V. (2005). S. L., Abramson, I., Wolfson, T., et al. (1998).
Differential effect of HIV infection and alcoholism Progressive cerebral volume loss in human immu-
on conflict processing, attentional allocation, and nodeficiency virus infection: A longitudinal vol-
perceptual load: Evidence from a Stroop match- umetric magnetic resonance imaging study.
to-sample task. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 67–75. Archives of Neurology, 55, 161–168.
396 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Sweet, J. J., King, J. H., Malina, A. C., Bergman, M. A., UNAIDS. (2006). Report on the global AIDS epidemic
& Simmons, A. (2002). Documenting the prom- 2006. Geneva: UNAIDS.
inence of forensic neuropsychology at national Valcour, V., Shikuma, C., Shiramizu, B., Watters,
meetings and in relevant professional journals M., Poff, P., Selnes, O., et al. (2004a). Higher fre-
from 1990 to 2000. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, quency of dementia in older HIV-1 individuals:
16, 481–494. The Hawaii aging with HIV-1 cohort. Neurology,
Thompson, P.M., Dutton, R.A., Hayashi, K.M., Toga, 63, 822–827.
A.W., Lopez, O.L., Aizenstein, H.J., & Becker, J.T. Valcour, V., Shikuma, C., Shiramizu, B., Watters,
(2005). Thinning of the cerebral cortex visual- M., Poff, P., Selnes, O. A., et al. (2004b). Age, apo-
ized in HIV/AIDS reflects CD4+ T lymphocyte lipoprotein E4, and the risk of HIV dementia:
decline. Proceedings of the National Academy of The Hawaii aging with HIV cohort. Journal of
the Sciences USA, 102, 15647–15652. Neuroimmunology, 157, 197–202.
Tozzi, V., Balestra, P., Galgani, S., Murri, R., Valcour, V., Yee, P., Williams, A. E., Shiramizu, B.,
Bellagamba, R., Narciso, P., et al. (2003). Watters, M., Selnes, O., et al. (2006). Lowest ever
Neurocognitive performance and quality of life in CD4 lymphocyte count (CD4 nadir) as a predictor
patients with HIV infection. AIDS Research and of current cognitive and neurological status in
Human Retroviruses, 19, 643–652. human immunodeficiency virus type 1 infection-
Tozzi, V., Balestra, P., Lorenzini, P., Bellagamba, R., -the Hawaii aging with HIV cohort. Journal of
Galgani, S., Corpolongo, A., et al. (2005). Prevalence Neurovirology, 12, 387–391.
and risk factors for human immunodeficiency van Gorp, W. G., Baerwald, J. P., Ferrando, S. J.,
virus-associated neurocognitive impairment, McElhiney, M. C., & Rabkin, J. G. (1999). The rela-
1996 to 2002: Results from an urban observational tionship between employment and neuropsycho-
cohort. Journal of Neurovirology, 11, 265–273. logical impairment in HIV infection. Journal of the
Tozzi, V., Balestra, P., Murri, R., Galgani, S., Bellagamba, International Neuropsychological Society, 5, 534–539.
R., Narciso, P., et al. (2004). Neurocognitive impair- van Gorp, W. G., Lamb, D. G., & Schmitt, F. A.
ment influences quality of life in HIV-infected (1993). Methodologic issues in neuropsychologi-
patients receiving HAART. International Journal of cal research with HIV-spectrum disease. Archives
STD and AIDS, 15, 254–259. of Clinical Neuropsychology, 8, 17–33.
Treisman, G., Fishman, M., Schwartz, J., Hutton, van Gorp, W. G., Rabkin, J. G., Ferrando, S. J.,
H., & Lyketsos, C. (1998). Mood disorders in HIV Mintz, J., Ryan, E., Borkowski, T., et al. (2007).
infection. Depression and Anxiety, 7, 178–187. Neuropsychiatric predictors of return to work
Trepanier, L. L., Rourke, S. B., Bayoumi, A. M., in HIV/AIDS. Journal of the International
Halman, M. H., Krzyzanowski, S., & Power, C. Neuropsychological Society, 13, 80–89.
(2005). The impact of neuropsychological impair- Vigil, O., Posada, C., Woods, S. P., Atkinson, J. H.,
ment and depression on health-related quality Heaton, R. K., Perry, W., et al. (2008). Impairments
of life in HIV-infection. Journal of Clinical and in fine-motor coordination and speed of informa-
Experimental Neuropsychology, 27, 1–15. tion processing predict dependence in everyday
Tse, W., Cersosimo, M. G., Gracies, J. M., Morgello, functioning in Hepatitis C infection. Journal of
S., Olanow, C. W., & Koller, W. (2004). Movement Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 30,
disorders and AIDS: A review. Parkinsonism and 805–815.
Related Disorders, 10, 323–334. von Giesen, H. J., Heintges, T., Abbasi-Boroudjeni,
Tucker, J. S., Burnam, M. A., Sherbourne, C. D., N., Kucukkoylu, S., Koller, H., Haslinger, B. A.,
Kung, F. Y., & Gifford, A. L. (2003). Substance use et al. (2004). Psychomotor slowing in hepatitis C
and mental health correlates of nonadherence to and HIV infection. Journal of Acquired Immune
antiretroviral medications in a sample of patients Deficiency Syndromes, 35, 131–137.
with human immunodeficiency virus infection. Wang, G. J., Chang, L., Volkow, N. D., Telang, F.,
The American Journal of Medicine, 114, 573–580. Logan, J., Ernst, T., et al. (2004). Decreased brain
Turchan-Cholewo, J., Liu, Y., Gartner, S., Reid, R., dopaminergic transporters in HIV-associated
Jie, C., Peng, X., et al. (2006). Increased vulner- dementia patients. Brain, 127, 2452–2458.
ability of ApoE4 neurons to HIV proteins and Wang, X., Douglas, S. D., Metzger, D. S., Guo, C. J.,
opiates: Protection by diosgenin and L-deprenyl. Li, Y., O’Brien, C. P., et al. (2002). Alcohol potenti-
Neurobiology of Disease, 23, 109–119. ates HIV-1 infection of human blood mononuclear
Uldall, K. K., Ryan, R., Berghuis, J. P., & Harris, V. phagocytes. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental
L. (2000). Association between delirium and death Research, 26, 1880–1886.
in AIDS patients. AIDS Patient Care and STDS, 14, Watters, M. R., Poff, P. W., Shiramizu, B. T., Holck,
95–100. P. S., Fast, K. M., Shikuma, C. M., et al. (2004).
Neuropsychological Aspects of HIV Infection 397

Symptomatic distal sensory polyneuropathy in Prospective memory in HIV infection: Is “remem-


HIV after age 50. Neurology, 62, 1378–1383. bering to remember” a unique predictor of self-
Westervelt, H. J., McCaff rey, R. J., Cousins, J. P., reported medication management? Archives of
Wagle, W. A., and Haase, R. F. (1997). Longitudinal Clinical Neuropsychology, 23, 257–270.
analysis of olfactory deficits in HIV infection. Woods, S. P., Morgan, E. E., Dawson, M., Scott, J. C.,
Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 12, 557–565. & Grant, I. (2006a). Action (verb) fluency predicts
Whitfield, R. M., Bechtel, L. M., and Starich, G. dependence in instrumental activities of daily
H. (1997). The impact of ethanol and marinol/ living in persons infected with HIV-1. Journal of
marijuana usage on HIV+/AIDS patients Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28,
undergoing azidothymidine, azidothymidine/ 1030–1042.
dideoxycytidine, or dideoxyinosine therapy. Woods, S. P., Morgan, E. E., Marquie-Beck, J., Carey,
Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, C. L., Grant, I., & Letendre, S. L. (2006b). Markers
21, 122–127. of macrophage activation and axonal injury are
Wiley, C. A., Soontornniyomkij, V., Radhakrishnan, associated with prospective memory in HIV-1
L., Masliah, E., Mellors, J., Hermann, S. A., et al. disease. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 19,
(1998). Distribution of brain HIV load in AIDS. 217–221.
Brain Pathology, 8, 277–284. Woods, S. P., Scott, J. C., Dawson, M. S., Morgan, E. E.,
Wiseman, M. B., Sanchez, J. A., Buechel, C., Mintun, Carey, C. L., Heaton, R. K., et al. (2005b). Construct
M. A., Lopez, O. L., Milko, D., et al. (1999). Patterns validity of Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised
of relative cerebral blood flow in minor cognitive component process measures in an HIV-1 sam-
motor disorder in human immunodeficiency virus ple. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 20,
infection. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical 1061–1071.
Neurosciences, 11, 222–233. Woods, S. P., Scott, J. C., Sires, D. A., Grant, I., Heaton,
Woods, S. P., Carey, C. L., Moran, L. M., Dawson, M. R. K., & Troster, A. I. (2005c). Action (verb) flu-
S., Letendre, S. L., & Grant, I. (2007a). Frequency ency: Test–retest reliability, normative standards,
and predictors of self-reported prospective and construct validity. Journal of the International
memory complaints in individuals infected with Neuropsychological Society, 11, 408–415.
HIV. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22, Woody, G. E., Donnell, D., Seage, G. R., Metzger,
187–195. D., Marmor, M., Koblin, B. A., et al. (1999). Non-
Woods, S. P., Carey, C. L., Troster, A. I., & Grant, I. injection substance use correlates with risky sex
(2005a). Action (verb) generation in HIV-1 infec- among men having sex with men: Data from
tion. Neuropsychologia, 43, 1144–1151. HIVNET. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 53,
Woods, S. P., Conover, E., Rippeth, J. D., Carey, C. 197–205.
L., Gonzalez, R., Marcotte, T. D., et al. (2004). Wutoh, A. K., Brown, C. M., Kumoji, E. K., Daft ary,
Qualitative aspects of verbal fluency in HIV- M. S., Jones, T., Barnes, N. A., et al. (2001).
associated dementia: A deficit in rule-guided lex- Antiretroviral adherence and use of alterna-
ical-semantic search processes? Neuropsychologia, tive therapies among older HIV-infected adults.
42, 801–809. Journal of the National Medical Association, 93,
Woods, S. P., Conover, E., Weinborn, M., Rippeth, 243–250.
J. D., Brill, R. M., Heaton, R. K., et al. (2003). Base Zhang, L., Looney, D., Taub, D., Chang, S. L., Way,
rate of Hiscock Digit Memory Test Failure in HIV- D., Witte, M. H., et al. (1998). Cocaine opens the
associated neurocognitive disorders. The Clinical blood-brain barrier to HIV-1 invasion. Journal of
Neuropsychologist, 17, 383–389. Neurovirology, 4, 619–626.
Woods, S. P., Dawson, M. S., Weber, E., Gibson, S., Zheng, J., Lopez, A., Huang, Y., Persidsky, A.,
Grant, I., Atkinson, J. H., and The HNRC Group. He, J., Zhang, H., et al. (2007). Comparison
(in press). Timing is everything: Antiretroviral of different viral strains from clade B and C on
non-adherence is associated with impairment in macrophage-mediated infl ammatory factor and
time-based prospective memory. Journal of the glutamate production and neurotoxicity (abstract
International Neuropsychological Society. 354). Paper presented at the 14th Conference on
Woods, S. P., Iudicello, J. E., Moran, L. M., Carey, Retroviruses and Opportunistic Infections, Los
C. L., Dawson, M. S., Grant, I., et al. (2008a). Angeles, CA.
HIV-associated prospective memory impairment Zink, M. C., Uhrlaub, J., DeWitt, J., Voelker,
increases risk of dependence in everyday func- T., Bullock, B., Mankowski, J., et al. (2005).
tioning. Neuropsychology, 22, 110–117. Neuroprotective and anti-human immunodefi-
Woods, S. P., Moran, L. M., Carey, C. L., Dawson, ciency virus activity of minocycline. Journal of the
M. S., Iudicello, J. E., Gibson, S., et al. (2008b). American Medical Association, 293, 2003–2011.
18

The Neurobehavioral Correlates of Alcoholism


Sean B. Rourke and Igor Grant

In the United States about 10% of adults meet the evidence for the recovery of brain structure and
diagnostic criteria for alcohol abuse or depen- function, and identify some of the variables that
dence. It is estimated that of those who meet the may mediate this recovery. Finally, we focus on
criteria for alcohol dependence approximately clinical implications and the role of neuropsy-
50% (range 31%–85%) manifest neurocogni- chological performance in the treatment out-
tive impairments on neuropsychological test- come of alcoholics.
ing when abstinent for 3–4 weeks (Eckardt &
Martin, 1986; Grant et al., 1984; Parsons, 1986a;
Diagnosis of Alcoholism and
Rourke & Grant, 1995).
Clinical Course
The main goal of this chapter is to delin-
eate the neurobehavioral correlates of alcohol-
Diagnosis of Alcohol Dependence and
ism. We have divided the chapter into seven
Alcohol-Related Disorders
major sections. In the first, we briefly describe
the diagnosis of alcoholism and related disor- Alcohol dependence, or simply alcoholism,
ders, and the clinical course associated with the refers to a constellation of symptoms that
“typical” alcoholic. Next, we summarize the develop in the context of a maladaptive pattern
neurobehavioral findings associated with alco- of alcohol consumption and continue despite
holism, as well as the neuroimaging, electro- adverse life consequences. In the most recent
physiological, and neuropathological correlates. update (the fourth edition) of the Diagnostic
In this section, we focus primarily on chronic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
alcoholics who do not meet the criteria for either (DSM-IV) (American Psychiatric Association,
an amnestic disorder and/or a dementia associ- 1994), alcohol dependence falls under sub-
ated with alcoholism. A brief description of the stance-related disorders, and more specifically,
models that have been proposed to explain the substance dependence. The diagnosis for alco-
brain dysfunction associated with alcoholism is hol dependence is reproduced in Table 18–1.
presented in the third section. A summary of According to DSM-IV criteria, a person is
the neurobehavioral and neuroimaging findings diagnosed as having alcohol dependence if he
associated with the Wernicke–Korsakoff syn- or she has three or more of the above symptoms;
drome is presented in the next section. While in addition, the diagnosis specifies the condi-
there is no doubt that alcohol is a neurotoxic tions for physiological dependence, the presence
agent, we present a conceptual model in the fift h of which is indicated by tolerance and/or with-
section to illustrate the multifactorial etiology drawal symptoms. Further course specifiers are
of the neurobehavioral impairments that are used to reflect the amount of time that a person
detected in alcoholics. Next, we summarize the has been abstinent (i.e., early versus sustained

398
The Neurobehavioral Correlates of Alcoholism 399

remission) and whether there have been any induced persisting amnestic disorder, and refer
symptoms of dependence or abuse during this to Martin et al. [1986, 1989] for more information
time (i.e., partial versus full remission) (American regarding alcohol-induced persisting dementia.)
Psychiatric Association, 1994). Alcohol abuse is Although most alcoholics do not meet the
similar to alcohol dependence in some respects criteria for either of these severe cognitive dis-
(i.e., both refer to a maladaptive use of alcohol orders listed above, many (i.e., approximately
that leads to clinically significant impairment or 50%) nevertheless demonstrate significant cog-
distress), but the former does not involve toler- nitive deficits that can have important effects
ance, withdrawal, or a pattern of compulsive use, on their treatment compliance and everyday
as is the case with the latter diagnosis (American functioning (see Figure 18–1). Because the DSM
Psychiatric Association, 1994). system does not yet admit to concepts like mild
Severe cognitive disorders occur in approx- neurocognitive disorder, alcoholics with milder
imately 10% of persons diagnosed with alco- deficits might be assigned the vague diagnosis
hol dependence (Horvath, 1973). The DSM-IV of alcohol-related disorder not otherwise spec-
diagnostic criteria for these disorders are listed ified (American Psychiatric Association, 1994);
below in Table 18–2. (See Wernicke–Korsakoff see the next section for the neurobehavioral
syndrome, below, for a summary of the neurobe- and neuroimaging correlates of alcoholics who
havioral and neuroimaging correlates of alcohol- meet the latter criteria. Refer also to Figure 18–1

Table 18–1. Alcohol Dependence

A maladaptive pattern of alcohol use, leading to clinically significant impairment or distress by three (or
more) of the following, occurring at any time in the same 12-month period:
1. tolerance, as defined by either of the following:
(a) a need for markedly increased amounts of alcohol to achieve intoxication or desired effect
(b) markedly diminished effect with continued use of the same amount of alcohol
2. withdrawal, as manifested by either of the following:
(a) the characteristic alcohol withdrawal syndrome that occurs after cessation of (or reduction in)
alcohol use that has been heavy and prolonged, and which has two (or more) of the following that
develop within several hours to a few days: autonomic hyperactivity (e.g., sweating or pulse rate
greater than 100), increased hand tremor, insomnia, nausea or vomiting, transient visual, tactile, or
auditory hallucinations or illusions, psychomotor agitation, anxiety, or grand mal seizures.
(b) the same (or a closely related) substance is taken to relieve or avoid withdrawal symptoms
3. alcohol is often taken in larger amounts or over a longer period than was intended
4. there is a persistent desire or unsuccessful efforts to cut down or control alcohol use
5. a great deal of time is spent in activities necessary to obtain alcohol, use alcohol, or recover from its
effects
6. important social, occupational, or recreational activities are given up or reduced because of
substance use
7. the alcohol use is continued despite knowledge of having a persistent or recurrent physical or psycho-
logical problem that is likely to have been caused or exacerbated by alcohol (e.g., continued drinking
despite recognition that an ulcer was made worse by alcohol consumption).
(DSM-IV: pp. 181, 198–199)

Table 18–2. Alcohol-Induced Persisting Amnestic Disorder

A. The development of memory impairment as manifested by impairment in the ability to learn new
information or the inability to recall previously learned information.
B. The memory disturbance causes a significant impairment in social or occupational functioning and
represents a significant decline from a previous level of functioning.
C. The memory disturbance does not occur exclusively during the course of a delirium or a dementia and
persists beyond the usual duration of Alcohol Intoxication and Withdrawal.
D. There is evidence from the history, physical examination, or laboratory findings that the memory
disturbance is etiologically related to the persisting effects of alcohol use.
(DSM-IV: p. 162)
400 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Percentage of alcoholic patients with and without


associated neruopsychological (NP) impairments

10% with severe NP impairments


90% Meet DSM-IV criteria for either:
Do not meet DSM-IV criteria (i) Alcohol-Induced Persisting
for amnesia or dementia Amnestic Disorder (291.1)
(ii) Alcohol-Induced Persisting
Dementia (291.2)

Approximately 50%
Approximately 50%
No NP impairments
detectable after 2–3 Mild to moderate NP impairments after 3 weeks of
weeks of abstinence abstinence: Alcohol-Related Disorder NOS (291.9)

Approximately 70–90% Approximately 10–30%


(i) Intermediate-Duration mild (ii) Persisting Mild Neurocognitive
Neurocognitive Disorder Associated Disorder Associated with
with Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism Alcohol Abuse or Alcoholism
(improvement in NP after several (with continued NP deficits
months to years of abstinence) despite 1-year abstinence)

Figure 18–1. Prevalence of neuropsychological deficits associated with alcoholism.

for an additional nosology to better character- neurotoxic effects of alcohol (see Multifactorial
ize the neurocognitive status of alcoholics with Etiology of Neuropsychological Deficits, below).
extended periods of abstinence (Grant et al., A study by Schuckit and his colleagues (1993)
1987b). provides useful information about the relative
appearance of the various symptoms that occur
with alcohol dependence (Schuckit et al., 1993).
Clinical Course Associated with
In their study, 636 male inpatients with primary
Alcoholism
alcoholism and a mean age and education of
Understanding the natural history of alcohol- 45.0 and 12.8 years, respectively, were admin-
ism has practical usefulness for both clinicians istered a structured interview regarding the age
and researchers. For clinicians, it can help iden- at which 21 alcohol-related major life events first
tify the time course associated with the emer- occurred; the information from the patients
gence of various alcohol-related symptoms, and was also further corroborated with at least one
it can dictate when different treatment interven- resource person. Of the 21 alcohol-related life
tions should be implemented depending on the events, the alcoholics as a group experienced an
severity of alcohol-related symptoms (Schuckit average of 10.9 events. These alcohol-dependent
et al., 1993). For researchers in the field of neu- men experienced several alcohol-related prob-
ropsychology, knowledge of the clinical course lems by their late twenties (e.g., 96% drank before
of alcoholism is important because of the neu- noon, 74% had binges of drinking lasting for 12
romedical events and systemic illnesses that hours straight). By their early thirties, there was
can often arise during the course of alcoholism evidence that alcohol had begun to interfere with
(e.g., head injuries, blackouts, hypertension, functioning in multiple life areas (e.g., 91% were
diabetes, withdrawal seizures, hepatic dysfunc- experiencing a withdrawal syndrome, 82% were
tion, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease having blackouts, 49% were involved in auto
[COPD]); these can have separate and additive accidents, 76% were having morning shakes). By
negative effects on the brain, independent of the age 34, 86% reported that they had lost control
The Neurobehavioral Correlates of Alcoholism 401

of their drinking. More serious job- and social- dementia disorder). Many of the remaining 90%
related problems occurred in their late thirties of alcoholics who do not meet the criteria for
(43% had been fired, 61% were divorced or sepa- either of these disorders nevertheless demon-
rated). Finally, when alcoholic subjects reached strate clear evidence of neurocognitive dysfunc-
their late thirties and early forties, there was evi- tion after 2–3 weeks of detoxification. Rather
dence of severe long-term consequences related consistent findings have emerged regarding the
to alcohol use (i.e., between the ages of 40 and intellectual and neuropsychological ability pat-
42, 5% had withdrawal convulsions, 24% had terns of such recently detoxified alcoholics.
been hospitalized, 37% endorsed that their phy- One of the first systematic neuropsycholog-
sician had noted health problems, and 26% had ical studies compared hospitalized alcohol-
hepatitis/pancreatitis). ics with nonalcoholic brain-damaged patients
Schuckit and his colleagues further explored and control subjects on the Halstead–Reitan
whether there were any subgroups within this Neuropsychological Test Battery (Fitzhugh
sample that varied in the emergence of alcohol- et al., 1960). Alcoholics performed at levels
related symptoms. Interestingly, the order of similar to those of brain-damaged subjects on
occurrence of these 21 symptoms of alcohol the Halstead measures and on the Trail-Making
dependence was consistent across the sample Test, but more like controls on the Wechsler-
when they were divided according to subjects’ Bellevue subtests. Furthermore, the alcoholics,
age of onset of alcohol dependence (≤30 years, when compared with the controls, exhibited
>30 years), or the presence of family history worse performance on the Category Test, the
of alcoholism, a secondary diagnosis of drug Halstead Impairment Index, and the Tactual
dependence or depressive disorder, or a pri- Performance Test total time. A subsequent
mary diagnosis of antisocial personality disor- study by these investigators, which included
der (ASPD; Schuckit et al., 1993). a larger number of subjects, revealed similar
Thus, alcohol-related health consequences are findings (Fitzhugh et al., 1965). Since these ini-
extremely common in alcoholic men, especially tial studies, other investigators have confirmed
when they have reached their late thirties and that recently detoxified alcoholics can perform
early forties. It is not until this point in time (i.e., at levels similar to those of brain-damaged
after heavy drinking for 10–15 years), that most patients on the Category Test (Jones & Parsons,
alcohol-dependent men will seek treatment for 1971) and the Halstead–Reitan Battery (HRB;
their alcohol-related medical problems. If neu- Goldstein & Shelly, 1982; Miller & Orr, 1980).
ropsychological deficits are also present when A review of earlier studies by Parsons and Leber
the alcoholic patient is recently detoxified (i.e., (1981) noted that the Halstead Impairment
sober for 2–3 weeks), one needs to entertain the Index (a summary index of impaired per-
possibility that the etiology of these deficits is formance on the seven measures comprising
multifactorial. (See Multifactorial Etiology of the Halstead Battery) was either significantly
Neuropsychological Deficits, below, for a review higher (worse) in alcoholics than nonalcoholic
of the medical problems that arise during the controls, or in the impaired range according to
course of alcohol abuse, which can contribute normative data, in 18 of 20 studies reviewed.
in both direct and indirect ways to neurobehav-
ioral dysfunction.)
Intellectual Performance in Recently
Detoxified Alcoholics
Neurobehavioral and
Despite evidence of neurocognitive dysfunc-
Neuroimaging Findings
tion, recently detoxified alcoholics generally
Associated with Alcoholism
perform at levels comparable to those of non-
While damage to or dysfunction of the functional alcoholic controls on Verbal and Full Scale
and structural integrity of the human brain is IQ values, but often show lower Performance
most striking, approximately 10% of alcoholic IQ values because of inferior performances
patients have severe neurocognitive decline (e.g., on Block Design, Object Assembly, and Digit
alcohol-induced amnestic or alcohol-induced Symbol subtests from the Wechsler-Bellevue or
402 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scales (original or visuospatial deficits in their female alcoholics.
revised versions) (Grant, 1987; Kleinknecht & On balance, both male and female alcoholics,
Goldstein, 1972; Miller & Orr, 1980; Parsons & when compared with demographically matched
Farr, 1981; Tarter & Van Thiel, 1985). In the nonalcoholic controls, generally perform less
review by Parsons and Leber (1981), the mean accurately on a broad array of neuropsycholog-
Verbal and Performance IQ values of alcoholics ical tests and take more time to complete these
across 14 studies was 108.7 and 104.7, respec- tests. They also show reduced neurocognitive
tively; in eight of the studies where subtest scores efficiency on summary measures that consider
were reported, Block Design, Object Assembly, both accuracy and time together (Glenn &
and Digit Symbol subtest scores from the WAIS Parsons, 1990, 1991a), as well as on tests that
were significantly lower than those of controls sample verbal skills, learning and memory,
in 100%, 89%, and 75% of the studies evaluated, problem solving and abstracting, and percep-
respectively (Parsons & Leber, 1981). tual–motor skills (Glenn & Parsons, 1992).
Next, we turn to a more detailed description
of the neuropsychological ability patterns that
Neuropsychological Deficits in Recently
have been generally associated with alcoholism.
Detoxified Alcoholics
For a more in depth treatment of older litera-
Although not all recently detoxified alcoholics ture we refer the reader to a number of excel-
show evidence of impairment on formal neuro- lent reviews (Chelune & Parker, 1981; Eckardt
psychological testing, there is a “typical” neu- & Martin, 1986; Grant, 1987; Goldman, 1983;
rocognitive profi le that is generally observed in Kleinknecht & Goldstein, 1972; Parsons, 1987;
chronic detoxified alcoholics who are sober for Parsons & Farr, 1981; Parsons & Nixon, 1993;
2–4 weeks. That is, alcoholics generally display Reed & Grant, 1990; Ryan & Butters, 1986;
intact verbal skills and have intelligence quo- Tarter, 1980; Tarter & Van Thiel, 1985).
tients within the normal range, but often show
impairments on tests of novel problem solving
Attention and Concentration Skills
and abstract reasoning, learning and memory,
visual–spatial analysis, and complex percep- Mixed results have been obtained regarding
tual–motor integration and on tests of simple whether alcoholics have deficits in attention
motor skills. and concentration (Miller & Orr, 1980). Tests
Much of the research in the field of alcohol- of attention and concentration from the HRB
ism has involved the study of alcoholic men; (Speech Sounds Perception Test and Seashore
however, where female comparisons are avail- Rhythm Test) were found to be impaired in
able both male and female alcoholics generally 62% and 44% of the alcoholic samples reviewed
show similar patterns of neuropsychological (Parsons & Leber, 1981). Attentional tasks that
deficits (Fabian et al., 1981; Fabian & Parsons, place a higher processing demand can reveal
1983; Silberstein & Parsons, 1981); women, how- deficits (Bartsch et al., 2007; Cairney et al., 2007)
ever, often show deficits after shorter drinking and these have been related to imaging indica-
histories than men (Acker, 1985; Fabian et al., tors of neuronal integrity (Bartsch et al., 2007).
1981; Glenn & Parsons, 1990). But in one study
of Russian alcoholics, women manifested worse
Abstraction, Problem Solving, and
performance than men on tasks of visual work-
Executive Functioning
ing memory, spatial planning, problem solving,
and cognitive flexibility (Flannery et al., 2007). Alcoholics most frequently perform in the
Some investigators have reported that alco- impaired range on neuropsychological tests that
holic women may escape deficits on learning place demands on abstraction, reasoning and
and memory tests (Fabian et al., 1984; Sparadeo problem-solving skills, and cognitive flexibility.
et al., 1983) even when matched on years of For example, alcoholics were impaired on the
alcoholic drinking (Sparadeo et al., 1983). Category Test in 89% of the studies reviewed (i.e.,
However, Sullivan et al. (2002) reported both 17 of 19), relative to controls or normative data
verbal and nonverbal working memory and that indicated impairment (Parsons & Leber,
The Neurobehavioral Correlates of Alcoholism 403

1981), with degree of impairment related to the who noted reduced response inhibition on the
length of abstinence (Rourke & Grant, 1999). Iowa Gambling Task, and Hildebrand et al.
Alcoholics have the most difficulty on subtest (2004) who noted abnormalities in response to
4 from the Category Test, a subtest requiring shifting, but not working memory.
spatial discrimination (Jones & Parsons, 1972);
in addition, length of drinking history in alco-
Visual–Spatial and Complex
holics has been shown to be positively associ-
Perceptual–Motor Integration
ated with the number of errors on the Category
Test (Jones & Parsons, 1971). Deficits have Complex perceptual–motor deficits are com-
also been noted on other nonverbal reasoning mon in alcoholics. One of the more common
and problem-solving tests—for example, the tests that assess this cognitive domain is the
Raven’s Progressive Matrices (Jones & Parsons, Tactual Performance Test from the HRB, which
1972) and the Levine Hypothesis Testing has previously revealed impairments in 84% of
procedure (Schaeffer et al., 1989; Turner & the 19 studies reviewed by Parsons and Leber
Parsons, 1988)—as well as on verbal reasoning (1981). Deficits in visuoperceptual and visu-
and problem-solving measures—for example ospatial processing also occur, both on tests
the Conceptual Level Analogy Test (Turner & with a motor component (e.g., Visual Search
Parsons, 1988; Yohman & Parsons, 1987), the [Bertera & Parsons, 1978; Glosser et al., 1977])
Word Finding Test (Reitan, 1972; Turner & and those without (e.g., on the Embedded
Parsons, 1988), and the Shipley Institute of Figures Test [Brandt et al., 1983; Donovan et al.,
Living Scale Abstracting Test (Silberstein & 1976]). Other tests that have been used in dis-
Parsons, 1981; Turner & Parsons, 1988). criminating visuospatial functions of alcohol-
Tests of cognitive flexibility (e.g., Part B of ics include the Gollin Incomplete Pictures Test,
the Trail-Making Test) revealed impairments Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS) drawings, Rey–
in 80% of the studies reviewed (i.e., 12 of 15) Osterrieth Complex Figure, and the Hidden
(Parsons & Leber, 1981). Alcoholics have also Figures Test (Sullivan et al., 2000). Performance
been shown to be impaired on the Wisconsin of alcoholics may be impaired on tests of psy-
Card Sorting Test (WCST). In general, they chomotor speed (Cairney et al., 2007).
make more total errors and require more trials
to reach the criterion level (Beatty et al., 1993;
Learning and Memory
Joyce & Robbins, 1991; Tarter & Parsons, 1971),
and tend to make elevated rates of perseverative Initial evaluations of learning and memory
responses and errors (Beatty et al., 1993; Joyce & performance using the Memory Quotient
Robbins, 1991; Tarter & Parsons, 1971). It has (MQ) from the WMS revealed that alcoholics
been suggested that this pattern of performance generally exhibit intact memory functioning
on the WCST reflects a deficit in the ability to and have comparable MQ relative to their IQ
sustain and persist with problem-solving tasks values (Butters et al., 1977; Løberg, 1980a;
(Tarter & Parsons, 1971). When one examines Parsons & Prigatano, 1977; Ryan & Butters,
the effects of age and length of drinking his- 1980a). However, although alcoholics without
tory on WCST performance, younger alcoholics memory complaints were comparable to non-
tend to show less deficits (Cynn, 1992), whereas alcoholics on the MQ from the WMS, alco-
considerably more deficits are evident in older holics were found to be impaired relative to
alcoholics and in those with more than 10 years controls when memory tests were made more
of drinking (Tarter, 1973). Finally, Parsons and difficult (Brandt et al., 1983; Ryan & Butters,
colleagues have also demonstrated that alco- 1980a). Later modifications to the original WMS
holics have problem-solving deficits on ecolog- (Russell, 1975), which resulted in separate learn-
ically valid measures of abstraction (e.g., on a ing, recall, and retention measures for verbal and
Piagetian-type task [Nixon & Parsons, 1991]), visual information, revealed that some alcohol-
and on an Adaptive Skills Battery (Patterson ics do have mild deficits in learning and memory
et al., 1988). Additional evidence for executive for both verbal and figural information
dysfunction is provided by Noël et al. (2007), (Hightower & Anderson, 1986; Nixon et al.,
404 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

1987). In the study by Nixon and colleagues, 1985). More recent studies, however, have shown
using Russell’s modification to the WMS (Russell, that alcoholics have deficits also on verbal learn-
1975), alcoholics were found to recall less verbal ing tests. For example, alcoholics were found to
and figural information at immediate and delay have acquisition deficits on the Luria Memory
conditions, with both verbal and figural learn- word test, but comparable rates of retention
ing being affected to the same degree (Nixon (Sherer et al., 1992). Using the California Verbal
et al., 1987). When these investigators calculated Learning Test (CVLT), Kramer and colleagues
savings or retention scores for both groups (i.e., demonstrated that alcoholics have deficits on
taking into account the amount of information immediate and free recall, reduced perfor-
that had been acquired when evaluating recall mance on recognition testing, and produce
scores), alcoholics were not found to differ from more intrusions and false-positive errors than
controls. What this pattern of performance on expected (Kramer et al., 1989). Consistent with
the revised WMS suggests is that alcoholics have studies using the WMS and WMS-R, alcoholics
deficits with acquisition and encoding, and pos- exhibited comparable retention on the CVLT to
sibly with retrieval, but not with retention of that of controls, when adjustments were made
information once adjustments are made for the for the amount of material acquired. A similar
amount of information acquired (Nixon et al., pattern of immediate and free recall and rec-
1987). Deficits in working memory and implicit ognition performance has also been obtained
memory have also been reported (Cairney et al., using the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test
2007; Sullivan et al., 2000). (Tuck & Jackson, 1991).
Studies using the WMS-R (Ryan & Lewis, A number of investigators have questioned
1988) confirm the WMS results. That is, alco- the external validity of the learning and mem-
holics differed from controls in level of perfor- ory findings obtained in the laboratory with
mance on all five summary measures from the alcoholics, and have subsequently developed
WMS-R, but not with respect to their pattern more ecologically valid learning and memory
of performance. Although the authors of this tests. For example, Becker and his colleagues
study did not make a distinction between learn- compared both young and old alcoholics on
ing ability and savings or retention of infor- an ecologically relevant task (i.e., learning to
mation, the differences between the alcoholics associate names with faces; Becker et al., 1983).
and controls on the memory summary scores They found that alcoholics made more errors
(i.e., verbal, visual, and general memory) indi- and were not able to achieve the same crite-
cate that alcoholics have a generalized learning rion level as a demographically matched group
impairment, while the difference scores between of controls (Becker et al., 1983). In contrast,
general memory and delayed memory for each their recognition performance for the faces and
group were virtually identical, thus suggest- names, and their retention was similar to con-
ing similar savings or retention of information trols. Becker and his colleagues attributed the
across both groups. We would like to stress that learning problem in alcoholics to a deficit in
it is important to make a distinction between forming associations during encoding, rather
learning and savings or “memory,” because if than to a visuoperceptual deficit (Becker et al.,
an alcoholic has both a learning and a retention 1983). Learning impairments on this face-name
problem, a separate neuropathological process test have also been replicated with a VA sample
may be present (e.g., alcohol-induced persisting of alcoholics (Schaeffer & Parsons, 1987), as well
dementia or amnestic disorder). as with a community sample of male and female
Over the years, there has been controversy in alcoholics (Everett et al., 1988).
the literature regarding whether alcoholics per-
form differentially on verbal and visual learning
Simple Motor Skills
and memory tasks. For example, several studies
have indicated that alcoholics are impaired on Gait and balance disturbances have been
visual learning (Fabian et al., 1984; Leber et al., reported in recently detoxified alcoholics
1981), but perform normally on verbal learn- (Sullivan et al., 2000, 2002). The evidence for
ing tests (Leber et al., 1981; Yohman & Parsons, simple motor deficits in alcoholics has been
The Neurobehavioral Correlates of Alcoholism 405

inconsistent. While one study showed that alco- alcoholics of all ages, whereas increased CSF in
holics have deficits on tests of fine-motor coor- the ventricular system has predominantly been
dination, but not necessarily on a test of grip seen in older and nutritionally compromised
strength (Parsons et al., 1972), another study alcoholics. Cortical atrophy tends to be more
reported impairments in fine-motor coordina- evident bilaterally in frontal and frontal-tempo-
tion, grip strength, and motor speed (Tarter & ral-parietal areas of the brain, although reduced
Jones, 1971). Impairments in motor speed (fin- density only in the left hemisphere of alcohol-
ger tapping) have been reported in 29% of stud- ics has been reported (Golden et al., 1981). In a
ies reviewed (i.e., four of 14 studies) (Parsons & review of 14 CT studies by Cala and colleagues,
Leber, 1981). Another study showed that alco- the rate of cerebral atrophy ranged from 4% to
holics were 9.3% slower than age-matched con- 100% (Cala & Mastaglia, 1981). The extreme var-
trols on motor speed (York & Biederman, 1991). iability in the prevalence rate of atrophy reflects
Because polyneuropathy occurs in 20%–74% of to a large degree the referral source of alcohol-
alcoholics (Feuerlein, 1977; Franceschi et al., ics tested; many of the earlier studies either did
1984; Neundorfer et al., 1984; Tuck & Jackson, not screen for adverse neuromedical and neuro-
1991), it is possible that the variability in motor logical abnormalities, or had selected alcoholics
test performance in alcoholics may be the result patients for clinical suspicion of central nervous
of a combination of peripheral and/or central system damage (Bergman et al., 1980a; Carlen &
nerve dysfunction or deterioration. Wilkinson, 1980; Ron, 1983).
Given the robust nature of the neuropsy- In one of the largest studies of alcoholics using
chological deficits observed in alcoholics, we CT imaging, Bergman and colleagues (1980a)
next turn to a summary of the neuroimaging, demonstrated that a group of 148 male alco-
electrophysiological, and neuropathological holic patients had significantly more evidence of
correlates associated with alcoholism. cerebral atrophy, relative to a random sample of
men (n = 200) not diagnosed with alcoholism.
Cortical changes were evident across the entire
Neuroimaging Findings in Alcoholics
age range in the alcoholics, whereas central
Pneumoencephalographic studies provided the changes accelerated with increasing age, partic-
first evidence of ventricular dilatation in alco- ularly in those alcoholics with longer drinking
holics, reflecting cortical and cerebellar atro- histories (Bergman et al., 1980a).
phy (Brewer & Perrett, 1971; Haug, 1968). This Using a semiautomated method for estimat-
technique, in which air is exchanged for cere- ing CSF volumes obtained from CT imaging
brospinal fluid (CSF), enabled investigators to (Zatz et al., 1982), Jernigan and her colleagues
examine the contrast between cerebral tissue examined fluid volumes in various intracranial
and CSF-containing spaces. However, because zones in 46 male chronic alcoholics (exclud-
of the associated morbidity and pain to subjects, ing those with liver disease) and compared
this technique never achieved widespread use. them with 31 male normal volunteers (Jernigan
The advent of computed tomography (CT) et al., 1982). Correcting for cerebral changes
quickly replaced pneumoencephalography. A that occur with normal aging (i.e., by using
number of CT reviews have appeared in the z-scores corrected for changes due to aging),
1980s (Bergman, 1987; Carlen & Wilkinson, these investigators found that alcoholics had
1983, 1987; Ron, 1987; Wilkinson, 1982, 1987). significantly more CSF than the control group
These reviews, from groups of investigators in on all sulcal measures (i.e., sulcal score at low
various countries, provide extensive cross-vali- and high convexity sections, and at the level
dation of the neuroradiological abnormalities in of the ventricles). Group differences, however,
heterogeneous groups of alcoholics. In general, on the measure of ventricular CSF volume fell
increases of CSF in subarachnoid spaces and short of significance (p < 0.06). No significant
in the ventricular system (i.e., lateral and third correlations were observed between the num-
ventricles) were interpreted to reflect cortical ber of years of alcohol drinking and any CSF
and subcortical atrophy, respectively. Increases measurement. In a subsequent investigation,
in cortical CSF have generally been observed in Pfefferbaum and his colleagues examined the
406 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

vulnerability of various brain structures to age,


Sulcal dilation on brain MRI
level of alcohol consumption, and nutritional evident in alcoholics
status (Pfefferbaum et al., 1988). They utilized
a similar semiautomated technique to measure A B
the percentage of fluid at the ventricles and cor-
tical sluci, and found that male alcoholics as a
group had more CSF than controls for both ven-
tricular and sulcal CT measures. Interestingly,
the ventricular enlargement was evident only
in older alcoholics, whereas increases in sulcal
CSF were evident across the entire age range.
Years of alcoholic drinking were correlated with Alcoholic Healthy control
both cortical and ventricular CT measures. In
addition, the central ventricular measure, but Figure 18–2. Brain MRI showing loss of brain
not the cortical measure, was significantly volume in alcoholic (A) versus healthy control (B).
associated with measure of body weight, body Note reduced cortical thickness and increased
mass index, hematocrit, and mean corpuscular sulcal volume. (From San Diego VA Alcohol Study,
volume, suggesting that nutritional status may I. Grant, P.I.)
have been compromised in these alcoholics.
These results support the theory that cerebral Jernigan and her colleagues examined 28
structures (cortical and subcortical) are dif- chronic alcoholics and 36 age- and sex-matched
ferentially affected by alcohol. That is, alcohol nonalcoholics with MRI and brain morphomet-
appears to affect cortical structures, regardless ric analyses (Jernigan et al., 1991a). The focus of
of age, whereas subcortical structures may be their study was to (1) evaluate the relationship
more affected by nutritional status (i.e., acute between CSF volume increases and gray mat-
thiamine deficiency) and/or age. ter losses, as well as the signal hyperintensities
Most of the CT studies have been conducted occurring in the white matter, and (2) correlate
with alcoholic men, but CT studies carried out the cognitive losses observed in the alcoholics
in Sweden (Bergman, 1985, 1987), England with various MR indices. The results from their
(Jacobson, 1986a, 1986b), and Germany (Mann study indicated that the increases in CSF (in
et al., 1992) have indicated that women also cortical and ventricular areas) were associated
demonstrate similar abnormal brain morphol- with significant volume reductions in specific
ogy, although the abnormalities tend to be evi- cerebral gray matter structures (both cortical
dent after a much shorter drinking history and and subcortical). Specifically, the chronic alco-
with a lower daily intake of alcohol, than they holics demonstrated significant volume reduc-
are with alcoholic men. Given recent reports tions in subcortical structures (e.g., caudate
that drinking patterns of women are chang- and diencephalon) as well as in cortical vol-
ing, and are becoming much more like that of umes (e.g., mesial temporal cortex, dorsolateral
men, this trend could have serious medical and frontal cortex, and parieto-occipital cortex). In
neuropsychological consequences (Mercer & addition, the alcoholics had significantly more
Khavari, 1990). white matter signal hyperintensities than did
Increases in CSF in chronic alcoholics have the controls. With respect to the relationship
consistently been demonstrated on CT scan- between neuropsychological performance and
ning using linear measurements, clinical rat- MRI indices, there were substantially more sig-
ings, and volumetric analyses. While inferences nificant correlations between cognitive mea-
have been made regarding the meaning of these sures and subcortical fluid volumes (i.e., 6 of
CSF changes, studies using magnetic resonance 13) than with cortical fluid volumes (i.e., 3 of
imaging (MRI) have opened up the possibility 13 were significant); gray and white matter vol-
of much more detailed in vivo brain structural umes did not relate in any significant way to
investigation (See Figure 18–2; see also the color cognitive measures. Jernigan and her colleagues
figure in the color insert section). also noted that the reductions in cortical tissue
The Neurobehavioral Correlates of Alcoholism 407

in the superior frontal and parietal areas in microstructure (Pfefferbaum et al., 2000, 2007;
alcoholics are consistent with neuropatholog- Pfefferbaum & Sullivan, 2005).
ical reports (see results from Harper’s group
listed below).
Relation of Neuroimaging and
Research at the Palo Alto VA Medical Center
Neuropsychological Variables
and Stanford University (Pfefferbaum et al.,
1992) has extended the fi ndings reported by Attempts to find associations between neuro-
Jernigan and her group in San Diego. A sim- psychological test performance and CT brain
ilar semiautomated procedure with MRI was imaging correlates have yielded mixed results
used, which allowed subcortical regions to be (Jernigan et al., 1991a; Pfefferbaum et al., 1988;
segmented into CSF and brain tissue, and cor- Rourke, et al., 1993). That is, while Pearson corre-
tical regions into CSF, gray matter and white lations between CT measures and neuropsycho-
matter. Forty-nine alcoholics were tested 3–4 logical performance with alcoholics were highly
weeks after their last drink and compared with significant in most cases (i.e., 33 of 45 were signif-
43 healthy controls. Adjusting for the effects icant), only two of 45 remained significant once
of aging, the alcoholics had decreased paren- age was partialled out (Bergman et al., 1980b).
chymal volume and increased CSF (alcohol- Bergman and colleagues also did not find any sig-
ics had 30% more CSF than controls); these nificant correlations between alcohol consump-
included significant reductions in cortical tion or neuropsychological test performance with
and subcortical gray matter volume, as well as CT brain measures in a large random sample of
loss of white matter (Pfefferbaum et al., 1992). men (n = 200) and women (n = 200) not diag-
While some studies report generalized reduc- nosed with alcoholism (Bergman et al., 1983).
tion in brain volume, others have noted some Nevertheless, there are reports in the lit-
regional predilections, including selective fron- erature of associations between specific neu-
tal and temporal shrinkage (Cardenas et al., ropsychological tests and CSF volumes, or
2007; Pfefferbaum et al., 1997) and losses in the structures adjacent to these fluid-fi lled areas.
insula, thalamus, and cerebellum (Chanraud For example, in one study, CT scan measures
et al., 2007). The observed losses in white mat- of the third ventricle, but not the lateral ven-
ter volume (Chick et al., 1989; Gallucci et al., tricles, were associated with memory perfor-
1989) have led to increased interest in exploring mance in male alcoholics (Acker et al., 1987); in
the integrity fiber tracts. The emergence of the another study, mean CT thalamic density was
MRI based technique of diff usion tensor imag- shown to be correlated with verbal and symbol
ing (DTI) has facilitated this line of research. digit paired-associated learning test perfor-
This method quantifies the diff usion properties mance (Gebhardt et al., 1984). Bergman and
of white matter, the notion being that within colleagues reported that learning and memory
intact white matter tracts, the movement of performance was related to central changes in
water in a magnetic field will not be random, 106 alcoholics, whereas performance on the
but constrained consistent with the orienta- Halstead Impairment Index was related to cor-
tion of the bundles. Two common indices are tical changes (Bergman et al., 1980c).
computed: fractional anisotropy (FA), which MRI indices of structural brain changes have
reflects diff usion orientation and coherence, yielded more consistent correlates with neuro-
and mean diff usivity (MD) or bulk mean diff u- psychological function. Decline in block design
sion (Pfefferbaum & Sullivan, 2005). Generally has been related to general brain volume reduc-
speaking one expects FA to be high, and MD to tion in alcoholics (Schottenbauer et al., 2007),
be low in intact white matter tracts, and this is while regional changes in gray and white mater
most readily measured in brain regions where volumes predicted worse performance on tests
these are regularly oriented, for example, cor- of executive function (Chanraud et al., 2007).
pus callosum. Studies comparing recently Callosal abnormalities detected with DTI have
detoxified alcoholics to controls do indeed been linked to measures of working memory
report lower FA and higher MD in the patient and visuospatial ability (Pfefferbaum et al.,
groups, suggesting disruption of white matter 2006).
408 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Functional Neuroimaging: Cerebral some neuropsychological measures, their pattern


Blood Flow and Metabolism in of brain activation to cognitive tasks differs. For
Alcoholics example, Pferrerbaum and Sullivan (summarized
in Crews et al., 2005) note that on a visuospatial
Cerebral blood flow (CBF) is normally propor- working memory n-back task controls show
tional to metabolic brain activity (except under activation in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex;
severe pathological conditions such as signifi- alcoholics activated the ventromedial prefrontal
cant cerebrovascular disease), and is therefore cortex (Pfefferbaum et al., 1998), whereas other
a sensitive measure of neuronal function. Blood research finds that alcoholics activate broader
flow imaging techniques—for example, xenon frontal and cerebellar regions to a verbal work-
injection and inhalation, positron emission ing memory task (Desmond et al., 2003). Other
tomography (PET), and single photon emit- data indicate that alcoholics recruit more brain
ted computed tomography (SPECT)—have the regions to accomplish cognitive operations than
advantage over CT and magnetic resonance controls, suggesting the possibility that neural
brain imaging, in that they provide a window injury from alcohol requires some neural reor-
through which the brain can be examined in ganization and recruitment of circuitries that are
a more functional and dynamic state. Studies not typically involved.
of CBF in “normal” healthy volunteers have
demonstrated an inverse relationship with age
and cerebrovascular status (Gur et al., 1987; Electrophysiological Findings
Shaw et al., 1984). A gender effect has also been in Alcoholics
observed, with men showing reduced CBF as Neurophysiological aberrations (EEG and
compared with women (Mathew et al., 1986), event-related potentials—ERPs) have frequently
although this effect disappears after the sixth been observed in alcoholics (Porjesz & Begleiter,
decade of life (Shaw et al., 1984). 2003). In general, differences on electrophysio-
Most studies of chronic alcoholics without logical measures in alcoholics seem to depend on
the Wernicke–Korsakoff syndrome have shown the recency and intensity of alcohol use, family
reductions and/or abnormalities, particularly in history loading for alcoholism, which particular
frontal and parietal areas, using both cerebral component of the electrophysiological response
blood flow (Berglund et al., 1987; Caspari et al., is examined, and what tasks or stimuli are used
1993; Dally et al., 1988; Erbas et al., 1992; Hata to elicit the electrophysiological response.
et al., 1987; Ishikawa et al., 1986; Mathew & Data from quantitative EEG analysis (QEEG)
Wilson, 1991; Melgaard et al., 1990; Nicolas have reported alterations in both alpha (Finn &
et al., 1993; Rogers et al., 1983) and brain metab- Justus, 1999) and beta (Bauer, 1994; Costa &
olism techniques (Adams et al., 1993; Dupont, Bauer, 1997; Rangaswami et al., 2002, 2004;
et al., 1996; Gilman et al., 1990; Sachs et al., Winterer et al., 1998) frequencies. Increased
1987; Samson et al., 1986; Volkow et al., 1992, beta activity has been linked to family history of
1994; Wang et al., 1993; Wik et al., 1988), which alcoholism, and has been observed in some first-
tend to improve after several weeks of absti- degree relatives of alcoholics (Ehlers & Schuckit,
nence (Berglund et al., 1987; Caspari et al., 1993; 1990; Rangaswami et al., 2004). Results con-
Hata et al., 1987; Ishikawa et al., 1986; Volkow cerning slower wave frequencies (theta, delta)
et al., 1994). have been mixed. For example, Rangaswami
Functional Magnetic Resonance (fMRI), with et al. (2003) reported increased theta activ-
its capacity to detect brief changes in blood flow ity, whereas Saletu-Zyhlarz et al. (2004) and
in circumscribed brain regions in response to Coutin-Churchman et al. (2003, 2006) found
cognitive and sensory-motor tasks has opened reduced power in theta and delta bands. The
new avenues in exploring the relationship latter authors also noted that decreased power
between specific neural circuitry disturbance in the slow bands was correlated with a degree
and neurocognitive dysfunction. Such studies of brain atrophy on MRI.
have revealed, for instance that although alcohol- Alcoholics have been shown to have increased
ics sometimes perform comparably to controls of latencies and longer transmission times on
The Neurobehavioral Correlates of Alcoholism 409

auditory brain stem potentials, suggesting may be a biological marker of risk for alcohol-
possible demyelination (Begleiter et al., 1981; ism (Begleiter et al., 1984; Pfefferbaum et al.,
Porjesz & Begleiter, 2003). In one study, 41% of 1991). As such, there is growing evidence that
alcoholics had abnormal auditory brain stem the P300 is genetically determined, and it may
responses, which were associated with the age also be involved with the dopaminergic system,
of the subject, CT evidence of cerebral atro- particularly the D2 dopamine receptor gene
phy, and severity of neurological complications (Noble et al., 1994).
(Chu, 1985; Chu et al., 1982). Cadaveira et al.
(1994) observed increased latencies for peak V
Neuropathology in Alcoholism
and prolonged III–V and I–V intervals.
Alcoholics have also been shown to have A number of autopsy studies have addressed
reduced amplitudes of N100, N200, P300, and the neuropathological correlates of alcohol-
increased latency of P300 (Emmerson et al., ism (Courville, 1955; Harper, 1998; Harper
1987; Kaseda et al., 1994; Miyazato & Ogura, et al., 2003; Harper & Kril, 1990; Harper &
1993). In a review by Oscar-Berman, reduced Matsumoto, 2005; Torvik, 1987; Torvik et al.,
P300 amplitude (i.e., a late positive waveform 1982). These studies indicate that alcoholics have
elicited during the discrimination of a stimu- reduced brain weights (Harper & Blumbergs,
lus) was found to be the most consistent finding 1982; Torvik, 1987), as well as increased peric-
(more reliable than the P300 latency) in absti- erebral spaces that reflect changes in the pro-
nent alcoholics (Oscar-Berman, 1987). Reduced portion of brain volume to intracranial volume
P300 has been related to imaging measures of (Harper & Kril, 1985; Harper et al., 1990).
brain shrinkage (Begleiter et al., 1980; Kaseda Since the mid-1980s, Harper and his col-
et al., 1994; Ogura & Miyazato, 1991). There leagues have carried out a number of morpho-
may also be gender effects on ERPs. For exam- metric and histological examinations of the
ple, male alcoholics tend to show reduced N100 brains of alcoholics (Harper, 2007). The first
and N300 amplitudes whereas females often do study in this series demonstrated that chronic
not (Parsons et al., 1990b). alcoholics have significant reductions in white
There have been several attempts to corre- matter tissue in the cerebral hemispheres, as
late ERP and neuropsychological test findings. well as increased ventricular volume, but no sig-
When significant correlations are found, they nificant differences in the mean volume of cor-
tend to occur between P300 amplitude and/ tical gray matter, or in the volume of the basal
or P300 latency and tests of perceptual–motor ganglia (Harper et al., 1985). These morpho-
functioning (Parsons et al., 1990b; Patterson metric differences were also most pronounced
et al., 1989), and on tests of delayed figural in those alcoholics with a history of cirrhosis
memory (Patterson et al., 1989). The relation- or Wernicke’s encephalopathy (Harper et al.,
ship between ERP and memory may not be as 1988). More specifically, brains of patients with
reliable; another group of investigators did not chronic Wernicke–Korsaff syndromes showed
find significant correlations between ERP mea- substantial neuronal loss in thalamus, mamil-
sures and MQ score from the WMS (Romani & lary bodies, basal forebrain, dorsal and medial
Cosi, 1989). raphe, and cerebellar vermis (Harper, 1998;
Abnormal ERP findings, particularly Harper et al., 2003; Harper & Matsumoto,
reduced average P300 amplitudes, are more 2005). Three subsequent studies were carried
pronounced in alcoholics who are family his- out and focused on specific white and gray
tory positive (FH+)—that is, have alcoholic matter volumes. These reported a reduction
first-degree relatives—relative to family his- in the size of the corpus callosum (Harper &
tory negative (FH–) alcoholics (Parsons et al., Kril, 1988) and a selective loss in the number
1990b; Patterson et al., 1987; Pfefferbaum et al., of neurons in the superior frontal cortex, but
1991; Whipple et al., 1988). Because the reduced not in the motor cortex (Harper et al., 1987;
P300 in the FH+ alcoholics has been shown to Kril & Harper, 1989), or in the frontal cingulate
be independent of lifetime alcohol consump- and temporal cortices (Kril & Harper, 1989);
tion, several investigators have suggested that it the reductions in the superior frontal cortex
410 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

occurred mainly in the terminal branches of shown that alcohol may also cause a dispropor-
the arbor (Harper & Corbett, 1990) and in neu- tionate atrophy of white matter (de la Monte,
rons greater than 90 µ 2 (Harper & Kril, 1989). 1988; Harper, 1998, 2007; Harper et al., 2003;
In addition to the selective loss of superior fron- Jensen & Pakkenberg, 1993). In one study, alco-
tal neurons, a significant reduction in the mean holics had similar brain weights and subcortical
neuronal surface area was also noted in the nuclei size, when compared with a demograph-
superior frontal as well as the motor and cin- ically matched control group, but had evidence
gulate cortical areas (Kril & Harper, 1989). The of mild atrophy of the cerebral cortex, enlarge-
latter data indicate that shrinkage of the neuro- ment of the ventricular system, and moderate
nal cell body is quite widespread in a number atrophy of white matter that corresponded well
of cortical areas, as compared to actual neuro- to amount of ventricular enlargement (de la
nal loss, and may provide the basis for the clin- Monte, 1988). Support for the differential effects
ical improvements on neurobehavioral testing on cerebral white matter with heavy exposure
and on neuroimaging in alcoholics, for exam- to alcohol is also supported by animal studies
ple, through rearborization once abstinence is (Hansen et al., 1991). The mechanism for white
achieved (Harper & Kril, 1990). Th is possibility matter vulnerability is not understood, but may
seems tenable, especially given the results from involve downregulation of myelin regulating
animal studies showing dendritic alterations genes (Lewohl et al., 2001).
with chronic ethanol treatment (Durand et al.,
1989; McMullen et al., 1984), as well as recov-
Efforts to Link Pattern of
ery in dendritic morphology with abstinence
Neuropsychological Impairment
(McMullen et al., 1984) in the hippocampus
in Alcoholics to Brain Regions
(King et al., 1988), particularly in the termi-
nal segments of this structure (Pentney, 1991;
Right Hemisphere Dysfunction Model
Pentney et al., 1989).
With respect to changes at the neurorecep- In 1972, Jones and Parsons suggested that the
tor level, a 40% reduction in the density of cho- right hemisphere might be more vulnerable to
linergic muscarinic receptors was found in the long-term alcohol exposure than the left hemi-
frontal cortex of alcoholics, following a histo- sphere (Jones & Parsons, 1972). This led to the
logical examination that compared the brains right hemisphere dysfunction (RHD) model of
of 30 alcoholics to 49 age-matched controls who alcoholism. In support of the RHD model, prom-
were nondemented (Freund & Ballinger, 1988). inent difficulties have been observed with visu-
In a follow-up study, these investigators dem- ospatial integration, disproportionately worse
onstrated similar reductions in the putamen Performance IQ than Verbal IQ, and impaired
(i.e., 40% of cholinergic muscarinic receptors), motor regulation of the left hand (Parsons et al.,
but not in receptor densities of benzodiazepine 1972). Subsequent studies by Parsons and col-
receptors (Freund & Ballinger, 1989). These leagues, as well as by other investigators, have
investigators suggest that neuronal loss and/or provided further support for the RHD model,
the loss of respective receptors in the brains of as indicated by the pattern of performance on
alcoholics is not random, and that alcohol may the Tactual Performance Test—reduced perfor-
affect certain receptors in specific brain areas mance with the left hand by right-handed indi-
(Freund & Ballinger, 1989). In another study, viduals (Fabian et al., 1981; Jenkins & Parsons,
muscarinic receptors were found to be reduced 1981), reduced performance on a visual search
by 40% in temporal cortex, as well as in fron- test (Chandler & Parsons, 1977), and worse
tal and putamen areas, while benzodiazepine performance on the Rey–Osterrieth Complex
receptors decreased by 30% in the hippocam- Figure Test relative to a verbal learning test per-
pus, 30% in the frontal cortex, but not in the formance (Miglioni et al., 1979). Several neu-
putamen or the temporal cortex (Freund & ropsychological follow-up studies (Berglund
Ballinger, 1991). et al., 1987; Page & Schaub, 1977; Schau et al.,
In addition to cerebral gray matter changes 1980) also provide support for the RHD model.
with alcoholism, a number of investigators have Using experimental techniques to study brain
The Neurobehavioral Correlates of Alcoholism 411

lateralization, Kostandov et al. (1982) found that Verbal IQ values, there were no differences in
using a paradigm to study differences in left and left- or right-hand performance on the Tactual
right visual field perception and lateral evoked Performance Test, the Finger Tapping Test, or
potentials, both nonalcoholics given alcohol and on Finger Agnosia or Finger-Tip Writing tests
abstinent alcoholics evidenced a slower process- (Barron & Russell, 1992). Furthermore, in one
ing rate in the right than in the left hemisphere study, motor speed was found to be slower in
(Kostandov et al., 1982). Consistent with this the dominant as compared to the nondominant
finding, male alcoholics have also been shown hand, and significantly more static tremor was
to exhibit a pattern of RHD on a dichotic listen- found in the right than the left hand (Løberg,
ing task (Drake et al., 1990). 1980a). Grant and his colleagues found practi-
Despite the research supporting the RHD cally the same level on Performance and Verbal
model, there are, however, a number of prob- IQ’s at initial testing for both recently detox-
lems with this model. For example, tasks ified and long-term abstinent male alcohol-
generally thought to be “right hemispheric” ics (Grant et al., 1979a), and a quantitatively
are not entirely specific to that brain region. higher Performance IQ than Verbal IQ for both
Further, many of the “right hemisphere” tasks groups 1 year later (Adams et al., 1980). The lat-
are inherently harder, or more novel than “left ter, may, of course, be due to the effect of prac-
hemisphere” tasks. Thus, what we might be tice on timed performance tests. O’Leary and
measuring is selective difficulty, which alco- colleagues found Performance IQ and Verbal
holics experience on more demanding tests. In IQ to be equal in field-independent alcoholics
addition, recovery for such “right hemisphere” (O’Leary et al., 1977a). Fitzhugh and colleagues
functions might also take more time. Sex dif- found quantitatively higher Performance than
ferences may play a role; in a review of 14 stud- Verbal IQ in both of their studies of alcoholic
ies in general neuropsychology, men with left male subjects (Fitzhugh et al., 1960, 1965).
and right-sided lesions showed the expected While alcoholics frequently perform poorly
Verbal IQ-Performance IQ differences, whereas on the WAIS Block Design subtest, they do not
women did not show such a pattern (Inglis & resemble right hemisphere damaged neurolog-
Lawson, 1981). Similarly, male alcoholics were ical patients in their pattern of performance
found to exhibit the RHD pattern on dichotic (Akshoomoff et al., 1989). Using a dichotic lis-
listening, but not alcoholic women (Drake et al., tening paradigm, Ellis (1990) found that both
1990). Also, women show less impairment on hemispheres were affected by alcoholism and
visuospatial and tactile-spatial tests, which may aging. Finally, an extensive review of the RHD
suggest that less lateralization in brain organi- model by Ellis and Oscar-Berman (1989) found
zation occurs in women; if this were the case, similar patterns of functional laterality with
one may also expect less dramatic lateraliza- alcoholism.
tion differences (Silberstein & Parsons, 1979).
Depressive symptomatology, which frequently
Frontal Lobe Dysfunction Model
occurs in alcoholics undergoing detoxifica-
tion, has been shown to relate significantly to Another hypothesis that has been proposed
impaired Performance IQ in alcoholics (Løberg, to explain the neuropsychological deficits
1980b). Because many “right hemisphere” tests observed in alcoholics is the frontal lobe
are timed, depression-related lack of motivation dysfunction model (Parsons & Leber, 1981)
and psychomotor retardation might contrib- and the frontolimbic-diencephalic dysfunction
ute to poor performance, although there is no model (Tarter, 1973, 1975). Tarter suggests that
strong evidence to support this mechanism. the commonly observed deficits of alcoholics
Several additional studies are inconsis- in categorizing or abstraction (in the absence
tent with the RHD model. Prigatano (1980) of other severe impairment) is very consistent
reported that men tested before disulfiram with selective frontal lobe dysfunction. The
treatment had higher Performance than Verbal fi nding of Berglund and others that frontal
IQ values (Prigatano, 1980). Also, although regional blood flow was most clearly related to
alcoholics had lower Performance relative to Block Design performance may also imply that
412 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

of the different brain systems relevant for such range on the AIR. Two discriminant function
a task, the frontal lobes play a significant part analyses were carried out, one including both
at least in the early abstinence phase in alco- sensory-motor and cognitive tests, and the other
holics. Neuropathological data indicate selec- including only the cognitive tests. Alcoholics
tive loss of frontal cortical neurons (Harper, were often classified as similar to patients with
2007; Harper & Matsumoto, 2005), particu- diff use brain damage of nonalcoholic etiology,
larly in the superior frontal association cortex when compared with brain-damaged subjects
(Harper, 1998; Harper et al., 2003), whereas with anterior, posterior, and diff use pathology.
MR spectroscopic studies reveal metabolite When alcoholics were compared with left, right,
changes especially in frontal regions (Bartsch and diff use brain-damaged patients, using both
et al., 2007; Schweinsburg et al., 2001). These cognitive and sensory-motor measures, alco-
authors speculated that because frontal struc- holics were more similar to the diff use brain-
tures are particularly heavily innervated by damaged group. Eliminating the sensory-motor
glutamatergic neurons, repeated bouts of with- measures led to a slight indication of right hemi-
drawal might induce excitotoxic damage espe- sphere dysfunction, but this was not dramatic
cially in this region. It has also been suggested (Goldstein & Shelly, 1982).
that due to the relatively late maturation of the At this point in our understanding, data from
frontal lobes, they may be particularly vulnera- NP, imaging, and neuropathological studies
ble to heavy drinking in adolescence and young converge on the notion that while any area of
adulthood. In animal models, adolescent rats the brain can be affected by alcoholism, among
fed ethanol showed injury particularly in their uncomplicated (non-Wernicke–Korsakoff )
frontal association areas, which, according to alcoholics, gray and white matter injury appears
the authors, correspond to orbital frontal cor- to be more prominent in the frontal lobes, with
tex of the human brain (Crews et al., 2000). the possibility that injury to the reciprocal con-
Some of the neurocognitive deficits noted in nections between the brain stem and cerebel-
alcohol abusing adolescents—for example, lum might contribute to some of the cognitive
difficulties in problem solving and working motor presentation (Sullivan et al., 2003).
memory—may reflect frontal lobe involvement
(Moss et al., 1994; Tapert & Brown, 1999).
Wernicke–Korsakoff Syndrome/
Alcohol-Induced Persisting
Diffuse or Generalized Amnestic Disorder
Dysfunction Model
Neurobehavioral Findings
Two studies carried out by Goldstein and
Shelly provide support for the diff use or gen- The WKS, also referred to as Alcohol-Induced
eralized dysfunction model associated with Persisting Amnestic Disorder in DSM-IV,
alcoholism. Using the neuropsychological key has been well researched over the past several
approach, alcoholics have been shown to have decades and is described in more detail in sev-
a diff use pattern of deficits (Goldstein & Shelly, eral recent reviews (Butters & Granholm, 1987;
1980). In a subsequent evaluation by Goldstein, Butters & Salmon, 1986; Jacobson et al., 1990;
right-handed alcoholics were compared with Martin et al., 1986, 1989; Salmon & Butters,
right-handed brain-damaged patients who had 1987; Salmon et al., 1993).
well-documented left hemisphere damage, right The Wernicke part of the syndrome involves
hemisphere damage, frontal or frontal-temporal the presentation of a clinical triad of symptoms,
lesions, parietal, occipital, or parieto-occipital which include global confusion or delirium,
lesions, and diff use brain damage not associ- abnormal eye movements (e.g., ophthalmople-
ated with alcoholism (Goldstein & Shelly, 1982). gia, nystagmus), and gait ataxia. Confabulation,
Alcoholics with an average impairment rating if present, occurs predominantly in the early
greater than 1.55 were included and compared to stages of the WKS. It is now well established
the brain-damaged patients; all recently detox- that this clinical triad arises because of a severe
ified alcoholics performed in the brain-damage thiamine deficiency, and if not properly treated,
The Neurobehavioral Correlates of Alcoholism 413

death may ensue after several weeks. However, a study by Butters and colleagues that demon-
with aggressive treatment, involving large doses strated that WKS subjects have WMS-R General
of thiamine supplementation, the delirium and Memory indices of 65 and 57, respectively
clears, and the ophthalmoplegia and ataxia (Butters et al., 1988). Patients with KS also show
improve dramatically, although other features evidence of frontal lobe dysfunction. That is,
such as peripheral neuropathy and amnesia patients with frontal lobe lesions, but who are
usually persist. These residual symptoms have not amnesic, perform similarly to patients with
come to be referred to as the alcoholic Korsakoff WKS on the WCST, as well as on the initiation
syndrome. The residual memory deficits consist and perseveration subtests from the Dementia
of a severe anterograde amnesia (with increased Rating Scale (Janowsky et al., 1989). This fron-
sensitivity to interference) and a temporally tal lobe dysfunction may also contribute to
graded retrograde amnesia, which are over impairments in other neuropsychological areas.
and above the cognitive deficits (e.g., visuoper- Finally, the impairments in planning and exec-
ceptual, perceptual–motor, abstracting, and utive functioning appear to be independent of
problem-solving impairments) that frequently other impairments in memory and visuopercep-
are present secondary to a long history of severe tive ability (Joyce & Robbins, 1991).
alcohol use (Butters & Salmon, 1986).
It is important to note that while the alco-
Neuroimaging and Neuropathology
holic Korsakoff syndrome is the expected sequel
Findings
to Wernicke’s encephalopathy, a recent study
indicated that of 44 patients diagnosed with The neuropathology of WKS is also well estab-
Alcohol Amnestic Disorder (DSM-III-R), 33 of lished. Specifically, thiamine deficiency is
the cases had no obvious neurological symptoms thought to cause hemorrhagic lesions in the
of Wernicke’s disease (Blansjaar & van Dijk, brain stem, cell loss in the periaqueductal and
1992). In another study, of 70 cases of Wernicke’s periventricular gray matter, and midline dien-
encephalopathy that were identified at autopsy cephalic nuclei, particularly the dorsomedial
(i.e., in 0.8% of all 8735 autopsies and 12.5% of nucleus of the thalamus and the mammillary
all examined alcoholics), 22 were characterized bodies (Harper, 2007). In addition, cell loss in
as active, whereas 48 were inactive (chronic); in the area of the nucleus basalis, which includes
addition, one - third of the cases with inactive the medial septal nucleus, the nucleus of the
encephalopathy resembled a pure Korsakoff psy- diagonal band of Broca, and the nucleus basalis
chosis, while the remaining cases exhibited a pat- Maynert neurons in the substantia inominata,
tern of a global dementia (“alcoholic dementia”) may also occur (Arendt et al., 1983). Recent
(Torvik et al., 1982). These two studies illustrate research has provided data to suggest that there
that WKS may be more heterogeneous than may a genetic predisposition (i.e., an abnor-
previously thought (see Bowden [1990] for an mality in the enzyme transketolase) to the
excellent review of this area of controversy). development of WKS (Blass & Gibson, 1977),
One of the hallmark features of WKS is the particularly since not all malnourished alcohol-
dramatic difference between the patients’ IQ ics develop WKS.
(Wechsler, 1955, 1981), which is often relatively The majority of WKS cases have occurred in
comparable to that of non-WKS alcoholics and persons with lengthy and heavy alcohol con-
controls, and their severely impaired MQ from sumption histories, although a number of case
the WMS (Wechsler & Stone, 1945). It is not studies have emphasized that Korsakoff ’s syn-
uncommon for this difference to be 20–30 points drome (KS) can develop without any history of
(e.g., IQ = 100, MQ = 70–80) (Butters & Cermak, alcohol abuse or dependence. For example, Cole
1980). In addition, however, because WKS sub- and colleagues (1992) presented evidence that
jects have normal attention and concentration, KS can develop following a left (dominant) tha-
the revised WMS, which has indices that do not lamic infarction. Vighetto and colleagues (1991)
confound attention/concentration with mem- reported on a 37-year-old man with multiple
ory, may actually better reflect the severity of sclerosis with disseminated white matter lesions
their memory performance; this is indicated by especially in both medial temporal lobes, who
414 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

initially presented with an acute amnestic syn- who found that many Korsakoff patients show
drome consistent with KS. Parkin et al. (1991) evidence of cerebral damage, particularly
reported that the retrograde and anterograde frontal shrinkage, in addition to diencephalic
memory deficits of a woman with anorexia ner- lesions; both non-Korsakoff and Korsakoff
vosa (after intravenous feeding and intestinal alcoholics exhibit similar cortical atrophy, as
surgery) was similar to the memory disorder indicated by similar sulcal and sylvian fissure
found in WKS (Parkin et al., 1991, 1993). Other widths (Jacobson & Lishman, 1990).
investigators have also found similar findings Jernigan and colleagues have recently rep-
with other anorexia nervosa patients (Beatty licated their CT findings with Korsakoff and
et al., 1989; Becker et al., 1990). The WKS cases non-Korsakoff alcoholics using MRI (Jernigan
that develop secondary to anorexia nervosa et al., 1991b). That is, although both alcoholic
(i.e., with severe thiamine deficiency), as well as groups showed significant increases in CSF in
with thalamic infarcts, and not in the context both subarachnoid spaces and the ventricular
of serious alcohol consumption, underscore the system as compared to controls, the Korsakoff
importance of thalamic structures in the neuro- subjects also showed increased fluid volume in
pathology of the disorder. the ventricles relative to the nonamnesic alco-
holics. With respect to gray matter volumes,
post hoc analyses revealed that both alcoholic
A Comparison of Nonamnesic and
groups showed losses of gray matter in dience-
Amnesic (Wernicke-Korsakoff)
phalic structures, as well as in the parietal and
Alcoholics
superior frontal cortices; however, the Korsakoff
A few studies have addressed the neuroimag- alcoholics also experienced additional tissue loss
ing and neuropathology similarities and differ- in the anterior diencephalon (which included
ences between nonamnesic (i.e., non-Korsakoff ) septal nuclei and anterior hypothalamic gray
alcoholics and amnesics (i.e., alcoholics with matter) and in the mesial temporal and orbito-
Korsakoff syndrome). In 1988, Shimamura and frontal cortical areas. These results have been
colleagues used a quantitative method to com- supported by a neuropathological examination
pare tissue densities on CT imaging in six areas of Korsakoff patients; 38 of 43 cases experienced
(thalamus, head of the caudate, putamen, ante- atrophy in the dorsomedial nucleus of the thala-
rior white matter, posterior white matter, and mus and all evidenced mamillary body atrophy
centrum semiovale) and fluid volumes in seven (Victor et al., 1989).
areas (total ventricular space, third ventri- Although the results from Shimamura et al.
cle, interventricular region, frontal sulci, peri- (1988) and Jernigan et al. (1991b) provide evi-
sylvian region, medial cerebellum, and vertex) dence to support the notion of two distinct and
in a small group of patients with Korsakoff syn- separable neuropathological processes asso-
drome, age-matched alcoholic subjects, and age- ciated with alcoholism, there continues to be
matched healthy control subjects (Shimamura a debate whether this distinction can be made
et al., 1988). Results from their study revealed clinically (Bowden, 1990). In addition, the neu-
that the Korsakoff patients had greater bilat- ropathological results presented above, and
eral decreases in the region of the thalamus, as a recent MRI study showing similar cortical
well as increased fluid volumes in the area of and subcortical lesions in alcoholic Korsakoff
the third ventricle, as compared with the non- patients and chronic alcoholics without cog-
Korsakoff alcoholics. The lower densities nitive deficits (Blansjaar et al., 1992), further
observed in the thalamus of the Korsakoff emphasize the heterogeneity of WKS.
patients, along with greater fluid volume in the
frontal sulci, were correlated with performance
The Multifactorial Etiology of
on memory tests. Alcoholic and Korsakoff
Neuropsychological Deficits in
subjects, however, demonstrated similar
Nonamnesic Alcoholics
enlargement of frontal and peri-sylvian sulci
(Shimamura et al., 1988). Similar findings have Several investigators have discussed the
been reported by another group of investigators possible multifactorial etiology for the
The Neurobehavioral Correlates of Alcoholism 415

neuropsychological deficits observed in alcohol- investigators have utilized the correlational


ics (Adams & Grant, 1986; Grant, 1987; Tarter & approach of analyzing the relationship between
Alterman, 1984; Tarter & Edwards, 1986). Grant drinking dimensions (e.g., years of excessive
(1987) presented a model (Figure 18–3) suggest- drinking, amount consumed at drinking occa-
ing a number of variables that could directly or sion) and neuropsychological measures specifi-
indirectly contribute to the neuropsychological cally in abusive drinkers.
deficits in recently detoxified alcoholics, and In support of the continuity notion of
we use this model to guide our review of these impairment originally proposed by Ryback
associated factors. (1971), Parker and Noble (1977) investigated
cognitive functioning in social drinkers.
They found that good performance on tests of
Drinking Indices and
abstraction and adaptive abilities were nega-
Neuropsychological Performance
tively associated with the amount of alcohol
There are two main approaches to the issue of per drinking occasion in male social drinkers.
predicting neuropsychological performance These results were subsequently confi rmed in
from drinking history. One approach has been their laboratory with male college students and
to compare neuropsychological performance of social drinkers (Parker et al., 1980, 1983), and
groups with different levels of alcohol consump- replicated by another group of investigators
tion (e.g., social drinkers, problem drinkers, (Mac Vane et al., 1982). Parsons and Fabian
and alcoholics). Such studies can be said to be (1982), as well as Parker’s group (Parker, 1982),
exploring the “continuity” notion: that there is have provided valuable comments on the pre-
putative progressive neuropsychological decline vious studies of social drinkers (Jones & Jones,
with amount of drinking, detectable even at the 1980; Parker et al., 1980; Parker & Noble, 1977,
social drinking level. On the other hand, some 1980).

Neuropsychiatric Risk Factors


Pre-abuse: FAS/FAE
Systemic illnesses and general health
Head injury
Psychiatric comorbidity
Use of other drugs

Alcohol Abuse
Age Amount per occasion
Genetics Duration of abusive drinking Education
Temperament Pattern over lifetime and SES
FH+ Recent amount/duration
Length of abstinence

Brain Structure
and Function Motivation
Expectancies

Test Characteristics Neuropsychological


and Sample Performance

Figure 18–3. Variables to consider in any causal model of alcohol-associated neuropsychological deficit.
(Modified from Grant [1987], p. 320).
416 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

The findings of Parker and colleagues could (1986b), there does not appear to be any con-
be summarized as follows: the abstracting sistent evidence to suggest that social drinking
score from the Shipley Institute of Living Scale causes neuropsychological impairment.
was found to be negatively associated with Correlational studies relating length of
the amount of alcohol per drinking occasion excessive drinking to neuropsychological
in male social drinkers and college students, impairment in alcoholics have also generally
whereas lifetime consumption and current fre- been disappointing (i.e., correlations are usu-
quency of drinking was not (Parker et al., 1980, ally in the 0.20–0.30 range). One possibility for
1983; Parker & Noble, 1977, 1980). A later study the attenuated correlations may be inaccuracy
by the same group of investigators showed that of self-report of drinking (Fuller et al., 1988).
psychological distress could not account for the Nevertheless, when relationships are found,
differences (Parker et al., 1991). Another group they are most often in the expected direction:
of investigators reported similar findings with the greater the intake or length of drinking,
social drinking; increased alcohol consumption the worse the neuropsychological test perfor-
per occasion and total lifetime consumption mance (Parsons, 1989). The maximum quan-
was associated with worse neuropsychologi- tity frequency (MQF) of alcohol consumption
cal performance in college students (Hannon over the past 6 months has often been demon-
et al., 1983, 1985, 1987), although replication of strated to be the best predictor of neuropsy-
Parker’s studies have not always been consistent chological impairment (Schaeffer & Parsons,
(Parsons & Fabian, 1982). 1986), although some studies have shown that
In contrast, a number of reports and reviews longer alcohol consumption leads to more neu-
have been unable to document social drink- ropsychological deficits (Jones & Parsons, 1971;
ing effects on neuropsychological test per- Tarter, 1973). Some investigators (e.g., Eckardt
formance. In a review of the literature on the and colleagues, 1978), have argued for the use of
effects of “social drinking” on neuropsycholog- nonlinear regression models (i.e., curvilinear)
ical test performance, Hill argued that there is to explain the relationship between cognitive
no compelling evidence that moderate intake measures in alcoholics and various consump-
of alcohol results in permanent alteration in tion scores. However, Adams and his colleagues
the structural integrity of the brain (Hill & have been unable to replicate Eckardt’s results
Ryan, 1985). In another review by Bowden, he using analogous quadratic equations, and they
reiterated the same opinion and proposed that have suggested that linear models may be more
previous associations between social drinking parsimonious (Adams & Grant, 1984).
and neuropsychological test performance did Finally, Parsons and Stevens (1986) reported
not properly account for innate ability, demo- that in 57% of the studies they reviewed (i.e., 16
graphics, and variations in drinking behavior of 28), one or more significant correlations were
(Bowden, 1987); similar conclusions have also observed between duration of alcohol abuse and
been reached in Grant’s (1987) review. In a pro- neuropsychological test performance; however,
spective study of the development of alcohol in 12 of the 28 studies (or 43%), there was no
and other drug use behaviors in adolescents relationship. Furthermore, Parsons and Stevens
and young adults (Rutgers Health and Human did not find that demographics (i.e., age, educa-
Development Project), there was little direct tion, and sex) or length of abstinence (i.e., dif-
relation between cognitive performance and ference of less than 10 days or greater than 10
“social” drinking in a young sample (Bates & days of abstinence) influenced these results. In
Tracy, 1990). Although most of the research contrast to their review of the human studies,
with social drinking has been carried out with their review of animal literature did provide
men, there does not appear to any relation- convincing evidence that performance deficits
ship between social drinking and neurocogni- and neuroanatomical changes occur in detox-
tive performance in women (Carey & Maisto, ified animals that are fed alcohol whose nutri-
1987), even with mild to moderate social drink- tion is comparable to control animals. Parsons
ing (Waugh et al., 1989). Finally, according to and Stevens further reported that the animal
an extensive review of the literature by Parsons studies suggest a duration threshold effect
The Neurobehavioral Correlates of Alcoholism 417

for impairment. That is, impairments are not from the WAIS, the young alcoholics performed
detected until a certain point, but after a con- significantly worse than both the young and old
sumption threshold has been reached, there is controls on the Brain Age Quotient (BAQ), with
a negative inverse relation between amount of the latter groups performing similarly. When
alcohol and performance. They also suggest that the individual tests from the BAQ were exam-
impairments may increase with age but reach ined (not corrected for age), the overall pattern
an asymptote, after which no further increase on the individual subtests from the BAQ was
in consumption is associated with a further similar across groups, with the young alcohol-
decrease in performance (Parsons & Stevens, ics performing intermediate to the young and
1986). old controls (Hochla & Parsons, 1982); length
of alcoholism history or duration of abstinence
was not related to performance in the alcohol-
The Influence of Age on
ics on the BAQ. Although there are a number
Neuropsychological Test Performance
of other studies that have supported the prema-
There continues to be controversy about when ture aging model (Blusewicz et al., 1977a, 1977b;
neuropsychological deficits can first be detected, Hochla & Parsons, 1982; Parsons & Leber, 1981),
and which variables seem to mediate this more recent studies do not (Burger et al., 1987;
emergence. Some investigators have suggested Kramer et al., 1989; Ryan & Butters, 1984).
that neuropsychological impairments are not Grant and associates (1984) found that
detectable in alcoholics until they reach their whereas both recently abstinent alcoholic sta-
mid- to late forties (Adams et al., 1980; Eckardt tus and age were related to a decline in learning
et al., 1995; Grant et al., 1979a; Jones & Parsons, and problem solving, aging but not alcoholism
1971; Klisz & Parsons, 1977), whereas other was associated with reduction in psychomotor
investigators have reported neuropsychologi- speed and attention. While this study suggested
cal deficits regardless of age (Brandt et al., 1983; that age and alcohol effects had somewhat dif-
Eckardt et al., 1980b; Ryan & Butters, 1980b). ferent profi les, an actual age by alcohol inter-
Furthermore, age of onset of heavy alcohol use action was not demonstrated. A morphometric
may also influence neuropsychological test per- MRI study reported that there was increased
formance, with those beginning at an early age brain tissue loss with advanced age in alcohol-
showing more deficits (Portnoff, 1982). ics relative to controls (Pfefferbaum et al., 1992).
There continues to be a debate as to the The same research group provided evidence for
degree to which age may interact with alcohol age by alcoholism interaction on bulk diff usiv-
abuse to make elders more vulnerable to neu- ity, a measure of microstructural change in the
rocognitive insult. There are two variants of corpus callosum (Pfefferbaum et al., 2006).
the premature-aging model: the accelerated In sum, age and drinking patterns may inter-
aging hypothesis and the increased vulnerabil- act, such that older alcoholics who continue
ity hypothesis. In the accelerated aging version, to drink at levels comparable to those of their
both young and old alcoholics perform at infe- younger years may suffer more brain injury,
rior levels on neuropsychological tests relative particularly in those older alcoholics who have
to their age-matched controls (Noonberg et al., already begun to show neuropsychological
1985). With respect to the increased vulnerabil- deficits.
ity hypothesis, alcoholics and controls perform
similarly then later diverge, with the alcoholics Genetic and Developmental Factors in
showing increasing deficits with age relative to Alcoholism Associated Brain Dysfunction. It
the controls. Much of the support for the pre- is generally believed that genetic factors are
mature aging hypothesis has come from studies important in the development of alcoholism
showing similarities between young alcoholics (Begleiter & Porjesz, 1999; Prescott & Kendler,
and old nonalcoholic controls. For example, 1999; Schuckit, 1987, 1994). The search to deter-
although alcoholics and controls as a group per- mine whether neuropsychological deficits exist
formed similarly on the Shipley Vocabulary, and prior to the beginning of heavy alcohol use has
the Comprehension and Similarities subtests proceeded along two lines. One approach has
418 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

been to study individuals at high risk for the (Ozkaragoz & Noble, 1995; Parsons, 1989; Tarter
development of alcoholism (i.e., children of et al., 1984). The fact that adult male COA have
alcoholics [COA])—mostly sons of male alcohol- deficits in visual–spatial learning that resem-
ics), and to compare them to demographically ble the deficits found in children may suggest
matched individuals without a family history that certain deficits may be premorbid (Garland
of alcoholism on intellectual and neuropsycho- et al., 1993).
logical tests, as well as on electrophysiological While there continues to be controversy
measures of brain functioning. This approach regarding the neuropsychological deficits in
has been driven largely by the fact that alcohol- COA, one possible explanation for the mixture
ics who also have a positive family history for of findings is the failure to consider comorbid
alcoholism demonstrate more neuropsycholog- psychopathology (Gillen & Hesselbrock, 1992).
ical deficits in some studies than those without For example, Gillen and Hesselbrock have
alcoholic family members (see below). A second shown that neuropsychological deficits in the
approach has been to administer questionnaires areas of higher level motor control and verbal
that retrospectively assess childhood behavioral concept formation occur in alcoholics with
disturbances in adult alcoholics, and to deter- ASPD; however, there was no ASPD and family
mine if these symptoms and disturbances relate history interaction. In contrast, another group
to residual neuropsychological test performance of investigators demonstrated that young adult
as adults. Ultimately, both approaches have tre- COA exhibited more childhood attentional and
mendous clinical implications in that they may social problems, but not elevated rates of cogni-
help to identify individuals who are at risk for tive problems, drug use, or mental health prob-
a more severe course of alcoholism or for more lems (Alterman et al., 1989a). Alterman and
neurobehavioral impairments, as well as target colleagues have suggested that it may be impor-
them for primary prevention and/or interven- tant to consider both drinking and familial
tion (Bates & Pandina, 1992). alcoholism risk status when performing com-
Regarding the intellectual or academic defi- parisons on neuropsychological tests (Alterman
cits in COA, some studies have not revealed any et al., 1986), although they were unable to find a
deficits (Bates & Pandina, 1992; Johnson & Rolf, relationship between heavy drinking and famil-
1988; Ozkaragoz & Noble, 1995); other studies ial risk with neuropsychological performance
have shown that COA, relative to children with- in college men in one of their subsequent stud-
out alcoholic family members, have lower verbal ies (Alterman & Hall, 1989).
ability and academic achievement (Drejer et al., Hesselbrock and colleagues (1985b) have
1985; Parsons, 1989; Sher et al., 1991; Tarter reported that Attention Deficit Disorder/hyper-
et al., 1984), and more psychiatric distress and activity and conduct disorder prior to age 12
alcohol/drug problems (Sher et al., 1991). predicted onset of drinking. However, they
With regard to the neuropsychological defi- were unable to detect any neuropsychologi-
cits in COA, some studies have found deficits cal performance differences in young adults
(Ozkaragoz & Noble, 1995; Tarter et al., 1984, who were characterized as being at high or low
1989a), while others have not (Bates & Pandina, risk, and there was also no difference between
1992; Schuckit et al., 1987; Workman-Daniels & these subjects when the data were broken down
Hesselbrock, 1987). In those studies that have according to frequency of intoxication, or when
found deficits, the pattern of results do not sug- a second cohort similar in age was compared
gest generalized effects (Tarter et al., 1989a), on neuropsychological tests, based on whether
but rather specific deficits on tests measuring neither parent, one parent, or both parents were
verbal skills and language processing (Parsons, alcoholics (Hesselbrock et al., 1985b). What
1989; Tarter et al., 1984; Tarter et al., 1989b), these results may suggest is that a higher prev-
visual scanning and attention, planning abil- alence of childhood problem behaviors may be
ity (Ozkaragoz & Noble, 1995; Tarter et al., related to the development of a variety of psy-
1984, 1989a, 1989b), perceptual–motor perfor- chiatric problems, in addition to alcoholism.
mance, abstraction (Parsons, 1989; Peterson & Hesselbrock and his colleagues proposed that
Pihl, 1990; Tarter et al., 1984), and memory the evidence in support of neuropsychological
The Neurobehavioral Correlates of Alcoholism 419

differences between low- and high-risk individ- conduct disorder, and an adult history of anti-
uals is not strong and that variations in litera- social behavior (Goodwin, 1983). In sum, these
ture are likely the result of differences in study results suggest that primary or essential alcohol-
design and methodology, and in how family his- ics, and possibly those with a family history for
tory of alcoholism is characterized (Hesselbrock alcoholism, are more at risk for antecedent neu-
et al., 1991). According to Hesselbrock et al., the ropsychiatric deficits.
strongest differences and deficits in COA, as
well as in FH+ alcoholics, occur on tests of ver- Electrophysiological Findings in Children of
bal ability and abstracting/conceptual reason- Alcoholics. In parallel with the neurocognitive
ing skills. Hesselbrock proposed that a number studies of COA, investigators have attempted to
of additional factors may explain the equivocal determine whether there are electrophysiologi-
results that have been obtained (e.g., drinking cal abnormalities in COA that may predispose
of mother, parental psychiatric comorbidity, one to develop alcoholism.
conduct disorder). Comparisons of EEG activity between indi-
A number of years ago, Tarter and his viduals at low and high risk for alcoholism have
colleagues developed a 50-item questionnaire produced mixed results. For example, there
that assessed childhood “minimal brain dys- were no differences on four measures of EEG
function” (MBD) symptoms (Tarter et al., 1977). activity in a study that compared male COA to
This MBD inventory has been shown to cap- those without positive family history for alco-
ture four factors: hyperactivity–impulsivity, holism, suggesting that EEG does not effectively
attentional–socialization problems, antisocial discriminate between individuals at high and
behavior, and learning disability (Alterman & low risk for alcoholism prior to alcohol admin-
Gerstley, 1986; Alterman & McLellan, 1986). In istration (Cohen et al., 1991). However, EEGs of
a series of studies using this inventory, Tarter young male COA do show different EEG wave
and his colleagues found that primary alcoholics patterns than those without a positive family
reported more childhood MBD symptoms than history risk when given an alcohol challenge
less severe drinkers (secondary alcoholics), psy- (Ehlers & Schuckit, 1990, 1991).
chiatric patients, and controls (Tarter et al., 1977); Far more electrophysiological studies have
in addition, he found that alcoholics character- been carried out examining various wave-
ized as essential and reactive differed in their ret- forms from ERPs in individuals at low versus
rospective accounts of symptoms of hyperactivity high risk for alcoholism. Many investigators
and MBD, with the former having twice as many have reported electrophysiological abnormal-
symptoms as the latter (Tarter, 1982). This finding ities, particularly differences in P300, in ado-
has also been replicated (i.e., essential alcoholics lescent males with FH+ (Begleiter et al., 1984;
scored higher on the Beck Depression Inventory, O’Connor & Tasman, 1990; Porjesz & Begleiter,
the Neuropsychological Impairment Scale, and 1990, 1991, 1993; Whipple et al., 1988) as well as
had a tendency to report more symptoms on in boys whose fathers are characterized as Type
the MBD questionnaire; Braggio et al., 1991). In 2 alcoholics (Begleiter et al., 1987). There are,
another study, alcoholics who scored high on however, some investigators who have not been
the MBD inventory were found to report more able to find any family history effect of alcohol-
psychopathology on the Minnesota Multiphasic ism in latency or amplitude of P300, although
Personality Inventory (MMPI), differed in alco- in some of their studies latency increased
holism use patterns and consequences, had worse and amplitude decreased with an increase
emotional and psychosocial functioning, and in the reported amount of alcohol consump-
were more likely to use other drugs (Alterman tion (Polich & Bloom, 1986, 1988; Polich et al.,
et al., 1985a). The results obtained using the MBD 1988a, 1988b).
questionnaire are generally consistent with previ- Some investigators have suggested that P300
ous findings that alcoholics with a positive family may provide a phenotypic marker for alco-
history for alcoholism begin drinking at an ear- holism (Begleiter & Porjesz, 1990; Porjesz &
lier age and have increased prevalence of child- Begleiter, 1993), which may also predict later
hood history of learning problems, hyperactivity, substance (including alcohol) abuse in young
420 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

boys (Berman et al., 1993). However, Polich and history effects, a number of investigators have
colleagues (1994) have argued that the P300 may been unable to fi nd differences between FH+
not be specific to alcoholism, because it is often and FH\ alcoholics using various classification
abnormal in a number of other neuropsychiatric schemes that classified alcoholics according
patient groups. In their recent meta-analysis of to strength of family history (Alterman et al.,
the P300 findings in COA, they calculated that 1987; Reed et al., 1987).
the most reliable finding with COA is the reduc-
tion in P300 amplitude using the most difficult
visual tasks, and that the variability obtained The Influence of Pre-Abuse
in past studies can be explained by a number of Neuropsychiatric Risk Factors
moderator variables (e.g., age of the individual,
source of recruitment, stimulus material used Fetal Alcohol Syndrome and Fetal Alcohol
to elicit the ERP (task difficulty and modality), Effects. Alcohol and other drugs consumed
and the strength of family history for alcohol- during pregnancy can have a variety of neu-
ism) (Polich et al., 1994). rological effects on the developing fetus (Riley
& McGee, 2005) The more severe manifesta-
The Influence of Family History of Alcoholism tion of intrauterine ethanol exposure have been
on Neuropsychological Differences in termed fetal alcohol syndrome (FAS) and can
Alcoholics. In trying to explain the neuropsy- range from life threatening brain and other
chological deficits in alcoholics, investigators organ deformities in exceptional cases, to more
have compared alcoholics with positive and commonly recognized stigmata such as char-
negative family histories for alcoholism (FH+ acteristic facial features (e.g., short palpebral
and FH–, respectively). In one study, male fissures, flat midface, short nose, indistinct phil-
alcoholics with family histories of alcoholism trum, thin upper lip, accompanied by mental
were found to have an earlier onset of alcohol- retardation, and microcephaly, see Figure 18–4;
ism, report more symptoms of childhood con- see also the color figure in the color insert sec-
duct disorder, tended to have higher level of tion). However, it is generally acknowledged that
depressive symptoms, and performed worse on FAS is but the more severe form of a spectrum of
the Shipley Abstraction Test, relative to male conditions that ranges from very mild effects of
alcoholics without such histories (Schaeffer intrauterine alcohol exposure to the most pro-
et al., 1988). An earlier study also suggested found. Terms describing these less severe forms
that alcoholics’ deficits in abstracting/problem include fetal alcohol effects (FAE), alcohol-
solving, and possibly learning and memory, related neurodevelopment disorders (ARND),
may antedate alcohol use in FH+ individuals, and alcohol-related birth defects (ARBD). Riley
and that the lack of interaction between family and colleagues have proposed that FAS, FAE,
history and alcohol/control status suggested and other manifestations be grouped as fetal
alcoholism and that family history has addi- alcohol spectrum disorders (Riley & McGee,
tive effects on neuropsychological test perfor- 2005). Brain effects in the more severe forms
mance (Schaeffer et al., 1984). In a review of (i.e., the dysmorphic, or FAS type) can include
the family history studies carried out in the marked reduction in brain volume and thinning
Oklahoma laboratory, neuropsychological def- or agenesis of the corpus callosum (Archibald
icits were found to be more prominent in male et al., 2001). FAE cases can also manifest a variety
alcoholics with an alcoholic father and/or in of milder changes of the same types (Archibald,
those with a history of childhood behavioral et al., 2001; Ma et al., 2005; Mattson et al., 1994;
disorders, although Cloninger’s Type 1 and Mattson & Riley, 1995; Riley, et al., 1995, 2004;
Type 2 classification (i.e., consideration of the Sowell, et al., 2001; see Figure 18–5; see also the
course of alcoholism, psychiatric comorbidity, color figure in the color insert section).
and personality traits; Cloninger, 1987) may There is continued controversy regarding
have contributed to the variability across stud- the critical threshold of drinking that leads
ies (Parsons, 1989; Parsons & Nixon, 1993). to fetal effects. Recently, it has been suggested
In contrast to studies demonstrating family that the threshold may be around 30 to 40
The Neurobehavioral Correlates of Alcoholism 421

Discriminating Associated
features features

Short palpebral Low nasal bridge


fissures
Epicanthal folds
Flat midface
Short nose Minor ear
anomalies
Indistinct philtrum
Thin upper lip Micrognathia

Figure 18–4. Cartoon showing some typical facial features in child with fetal alcohol syndrome. (Modified
from Ann Streissguth, PhD, University of Washington and Edward Riley, PhD, San Diego State University.
Courtesy of Dr. Riley.)

g/day (above the level generally defi ned as with binge drinking (i.e., with more than five
moderate drinking), though the authors of drinks per occasion) (Streissguth et al., 1990).
the study state that current evidence is insuf- A follow-up of these children (n = 462) at age 14
ficient (Plant et al., 1993). Several other inves- revealed that the number of drinks per occasion
tigators argue against the use of a cutoff as the was the strongest alcohol predictor, and alcohol
critical determinant, and point to the increas- exposure prenatally was associated with atten-
ing probability of damage with increasing tion and memory deficits in a dose-dependent
amounts of alcohol. Russell et al. examined fashion (Streissguth et al., 1994). Other neuro-
growth, dysmorphology, and cognitive devel- psychological impairments are also common
opment in 6-year-old children exposed to (Conry, 1990; Mattson et al., 1992; Smith &
alcohol prenatally (Russell et al., 1991). Even Eckardt, 1991; Streissguth et al., 1989, 1994),
after excluding children born to mothers who including deficits in executive functioning and
drank over seven drinks a day, and children working memory (Kodituwakku et al., 2001;
with probable/possible FAE, Russell and col- Mattson et al., 1999).
leagues found significant effects of maternal
drinking on cognitive functions (and head The Influence of Concurrent
circumference). Neuromedical Risk Factors in
Decrements in intellectual performance, Alcoholics
particularly verbal-language skills, have
been reported with prenatal alcohol expo- The Effects of Hepatic Dysfunction on
sure (Caruso & ten Bensel, 1993; Conry, 1990; Neuropsychological Test Performance. It is
Mattson et al., 1992, 1998; Nanson & Hiscock, well established that hepatic dysfunction and
1990; Smith & Eckardt, 1991; Streissguth et al., disease can result in neuropsychological dys-
1990) in infants born to mothers with more than function (Tarter et al., 1987). For example,
one indication of problem drinking (Russell biochemical measures of hepatic dysfunction
et al., 1991), and in children raised by an alco- have been shown to correlate with neuropsy-
holic father (Ervin et al., 1984). Consumption chological dysfunction in alcoholics with cir-
of two or more drinks per day on an average rhosis (Tarter et al., 1986b), particularly on tests
was associated with a 7-point decrement in IQ of memory performance (Arria et al., 1991a).
in 482 7-year-old children (Streissguth et al., Psychomotor, visuopractic, and abstracting
1990). In addition, the effect of prenatal alcohol abilities can recover following liver transplan-
exposure was exacerbated if parental education tation; memory deficits, however, may persist
was low and with increased number of children (Arria et al., 1991b). The fact that alcoholics and
in the home; learning problems were associated nonalcoholics with cirrhosis display similar
422 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Corpus callosum abnormalities

A B C

D E

Figure 18–5. One anomaly that has been seen in FAS is agenesis, or absence, of the corpus callosum. While
not common, it occurs in FAS cases (~6%) more frequently than in the general population (0.1%) or in the
developmentally disabled population (2%–3%). In fact it has been suggested that FAS may be the most common
cause of agenesis of the corpus callosum.
The top left MRI scan (A), is a control brain. The other images are from children with FAS. In the top middle the
corpus callosum is present, but it is very thin at the posterior section of the brain (B, arrow). In the upper right
the corpus callosum is essentially missing (C, arrow). The bottom two pictures (D, E) are from a 9-year old girl
with FAS. She has agenesis of the corpus callosum and the large dark area in the back of her brain (E, arrow)
above the cerebellum is a condition known as colpocephaly. It is essentially empty space. (Courtesy Edward
Riley, PhD, San Diego State University).

neuropsychological deficits (Tarter et al., 1986a, groups according to their plasma levels of
1988) suggests that alcohol is not the only eti- gamma-glutamyl transferase (GGT, a liver
ological mechanism for brain dysfunction in enzyme where plasma level rises during acute
alcoholics. heavy drinking) at admission. There were 69
Although less than one-third of alcoholics or men with normal GGT values (>40 IU/L), 41
heavy drinkers develop serious alcohol-related men with moderately elevated GGT levels (40
liver damage (Grant et al., 1988), elevated lev- ≤ GGT ≤ 100 IU/L), and 22 with extremely
els of liver enzymes in noncirrhotic alcoholics elevated GGT levels (>100 IU/L). There were
have been shown to be significantly related to no differences in age, education, and WAIS-R
a number of neuropsychological measures, par- vocabulary scores, suggesting that the groups
ticularly those that assess visuoperceptual and were comparable on premorbid neuropsycho-
visuoconceptual abilities. logical functioning. When the three groups
A study by Irwin and colleagues illustrates were compared on a brief battery of neu-
the effects of liver enzymes on neuropsycho- ropsychological tests, which included the
logical test performance (Irwin et al., 1989). Trail-Making Test (Parts A and B), WAIS-R
Blood and serum were collected from 132 pri- Digit Symbol, and Visual Search, significant
mary alcoholic men 24–48 hours after admis- differences emerged as GGT level increased.
sion, but approximately a mean of 7 days after When multiple regressions were performed to
their last drink. Based on blood and serum examine the influence of various parameters
values, alcoholics were divided into three (i.e., GGT, liver injury tests, demographics,
The Neurobehavioral Correlates of Alcoholism 423

alcohol consumption, and depressive symp- neurotoxic effects of alcohol. Traumatic head
toms as measured by the Hamilton Depression injuries appear to be two to four times more prev-
Rating Scale) on neuropsychological test per- alent in alcoholics than in the general population
formance, it was found that over and above (Hillbom & Holm, 1986). Excessive alcohol con-
the expected influence of age and education, sumption has been shown to lead to increased
GGT contributed a significant amount of var- susceptibility to head injuries in both alcoholic
iance to Trails B and Visual Search. In addi- and control groups (Hillbom & Holm, 1986), as
tion, even when other liver function tests were well as worse head injury outcome (Ruff et al.,
included in the analyses, GGT continued to 1990). In fact, heavy alcohol use and head injury
explain Visual Search, Digit Symbol, and severity may lead to independent or interactive
Trails B performance, suggesting that level of effects on neuropsychological performance and
GGT in alcoholics is associated with neuro- outcome (Dikmen et al., 1993; Solomon & Malloy,
psychological deficits in the areas of visuoper- 1992). For example, one study showed a signif-
ceptual and visuoconceptual functions (Irwin icant interaction between head injury severity
et al., 1989). (i.e., length of post-traumatic amnesia) and alco-
In a later study by Irwin and his colleagues hol use, with increasing memory deficits occur-
(Schafer et al., 1991), liver function tests and ring with increasing alcohol use (Brooks et al.,
depression at admission were found to be related 1989). Several studies have examined whether
to neuropsychological dysfunction in alcohol- the degree of intoxication at time of head injury, a
ics, but not at discharge several weeks later. In history of alcohol abuse, or both contribute to the
a study by another group of investigators, liver severity of traumatic brain injury (TBI)-induced
enzyme levels, when collected from alcoholics brain injury and predicts recovery. Brooks et al.
at hospital admission, were shown to have con- (1989) reported that posttraumatic amnesia was
tinued effects on neuropsychological test per- worst among heaviest drinkers at injury, while
formance at least 21 days later, particularly on Wilde et al. (2004) found that both blood alco-
tests of visuoperceptual and conceptual abilities hol concentration on day of injury and history of
(Richardson et al., 1991). chronic use were associated with greater brain
Persons with alcoholism can also have liver atrophy on MRI and worse NP outcome. Other
disease by virtue of hepatitis C virus (HCV) studies report no effect of day of injury drinking
infection, which is more common among alco- on NP outcome (Alexander et al., 2004; Lange
holics who have concurrend drug abuse, espe- et al., 2007). The latter study also considered his-
cially injection drug use. There is accumulating tory of alcohol abuse and noted a significant, but
evidence that HCV can produce neurocogni- small effect on neurocognitive outcome of head
tive impairment even in the absence of severe injury. Family history for alcoholism may also be
liver disease (Forton et al., 2002, 2005, 2006; a significant risk factor, because alcoholics with a
Hilsabeck et al., 2003). HCV has been identi- positive family risk have been shown to have ele-
fied in astrocytes and macrophages in the brain vated rates of closed head injuries (60% in FH+
(Letendre et al., 2007) and neurocognitive dis- versus 35% in FH-) (Alterman & Tarter, 1985).
turbance has been linked to systemic inflam- One group of investigators have found that
matory markers associated with HCV disease a history of head injuries independently pre-
(Letendre et al., 2006). Therefore, testing for dicted neuropsychological deficits, particu-
HCV serostatus may be indicated, especially larly on a factor of verbal skills and on a factor
where apparent alcohol-related neuropsy- that reflected nonverbal learning and problem
chological deficits persist or worsen despite solving (Grant et al., 1984). In another study,
abstinence. alcoholics with head injuries performed sig-
nificantly worse than alcoholics without head
The Impact of Head Injury on injuries on the Halstead Impairment Index, the
Neuropsychological Test Performance. Head Finger Tapping Test (dominant hand) and on the
injuries are extremely common in alcoholics and Location score from the Tactual Performance
they represent a significant risk factor for neu- Test (Hillbom & Holm, 1986). There is no una-
ropsychological impairment independent of the nimity that mild head injury affects NP in
424 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

alcoholism, as several studies failed to detect that persist for extended periods of time after
an association (Alterman et al., 1985b; Ryan & detoxification that may also contribute to
Lewis, 1988). daytime impairments. For example, young
primary alcoholics have been shown to have
Nutrition. A few studies in the early 1980s sleep patterns typical of older controls (Gillin
examined how nutrition may impact neuropsy- et al., 1990b); in addition, length of abstinence,
chological test performance in alcoholics. These drinks per drinking day in past 3 months, and
studies provided evidence that folic acid level maximum number of withdrawal symptoms
was related to deficits on a number of neuropsy- ever experienced by the patient were related
chological tests in recently detoxified alcoholics to sleep abnormalities (Gillin et al., 1990b).
(Albert et al., 1982, 1983). The authors of these Furthermore, alcoholics (both primary and
studies suggested that nutrition effects may be those with a secondary depression) have sig-
important only early in the detoxification pro- nificantly longer sleep latency and less sleep
cess, whereas the more long-standing neuro- efficiency, less total sleep time, and delta sleep
psychological deficits that are detected may be than controls; primary alcoholics with sec-
related to the neurotoxic effects of alcohol. In ondary depression have shorter REM latency
a more recent study, alcoholics who had lower and less non-REM sleep than alcoholics with-
serum levels of thiamine scored lower on intel- out secondary depression (Gillin et al., 1990a).
lectual and visual–spatial tasks relative to con- Finally, there is also some evidence that
trols. There were a few significant correlations decreases in slow wave sleep in alcoholics may
between thiamine level and neuropsychological be related to increased atrophy of the cerebral
test performance; however, regression analy- cortex (Ishibashi et al., 1987).
ses revealed that duration of alcohol intake and
educational level were the major contributors to Blackouts, Seizures, and Effects of Repeated
neuropsychological test performance (Molina Withdrawal from Alcohol. There are a num-
et al., 1994). ber of consequences that result from heavy
drinking (e.g., blackouts, seizures, and
Possible Effects of Hypoxemia. It is esti- repeated withdrawals) that could indirectly
mated that between 83% and 94% of alcoholics influence brain functioning. For example, of
smoke cigarettes (Ayers et al., 1976; DiFranza 135 alcoholics seeking help for an alcohol/
& Guerrera, 1990). It is well established that drug problem, 86% reported having experi-
COPD can cause significant neuropsycho- enced alcohol-related blackouts (Campbell &
logical impairment with increasing severity Hodgins, 1993); these blackouts were found to
of hypoxemia (Grant et al., 1987a). Although be related to the severity of alcohol problems
most neuropsychological studies of alcoholics (i.e., those with blackouts started drinking
generally exclude those with obvious lung dis- at a younger age, drank larger amounts, had
ease or COPD, it is possible that mild levels of greater dependence, were more likely to have
hypoxemia in those who are in the early stages delirium tremens, shakes, and family his-
of COPD may contribute to neuropsycholog- tory of alcoholism). Approximately 14%–21%
ical deficits independent of alcohol. In addi- of hospitalized alcoholics report having had
tion, because chronic alcohol consumption can seizures, typically of the grand mal vari-
also lead to increasing night time hypoxemia ety (Feuerlein, 1977; Neundorfer et al., 1984;
and sleep apnea, particularly in older men. Tuck & Jackson, 1991). Although a small study
Additional deficits may be observed in alcohol- showed that alcoholics who presented to the
ics with such conditions since sleep apnea itself hospital experiencing withdrawal seizures (n
can be associated with NP impairment (Roth = 12) were not different from those without
et al., 1995; Vitiello et al., 1987). (n = 22) intellectual or neuropsychological
measures, African American alcoholics were
Sleep Disturbances. Notwithstanding pos- more likely than White alcoholics in this study
sible nighttime hypoxemia in alcoholics, to experience seizures (Tarter et al., 1983). In
there are a number of sleep abnormalities another study, African American alcoholics
The Neurobehavioral Correlates of Alcoholism 425

with seizure histories performed worse on alcoholics with ASPD may have a higher prev-
intellectual and neuropsychological tests com- alence of neuropsychological deficits because
pared to African American alcoholics without they begin drinking earlier and suffer more
such histories; there were no differences among alcohol-related problems, including more black-
White alcoholics (Goldstein et al., 1983). More outs, more physical injuries, more frequent sleep
recent data indicate lowered N-acetyl aspartate disturbances, and engage in more polydrug use
(NAA), a metabolite thought to reflect neuro- than those alcoholics without ASPD (Malloy
nal integrity, among detoxified alcoholics who et al., 1990). Finally, violent, recently detoxi-
reported withdrawal seizures (Schweinsburg, fied alcoholics have been shown to have more
2002). Regarding repeated alcohol withdraw- severe neuropsychological impairments (e.g.,
als uncomplicated by seizures, male and female on abstraction and mental coordination) and
alcoholics have been shown to perform worse more deviant personality profi les on the MMPI,
on memory testing when repeated withdrawals compared with nonviolent alcoholics (Løberg,
occurred in the past year (Glenn et al., 1988). 1981).
To circumvent the problem of discrete clas-
The Influence of Psychiatric Comorbidity sification associated with the diagnosis of
on Neuropsychological Test Performance in ASPD, Glenn and her colleagues carried out
Alcoholics. Personality disorders are associated a study to examine how “subclinical” levels of
with alcoholism (Løberg & Miller, 1986); one antisocial behaviors influenced neuropsycho-
study reported that 78% of alcoholics admitted logical impairment in adult alcoholics (i.e.,
to an addiction treatment unit (n = 178) had at they included subjects who did not meet crite-
least one personality disorder, with the aver- ria for ASPD, but nevertheless had a number
age being 1.8 per patient (DeJong et al., 1993). of symptoms that were characteristic of this
Personality disturbances, based on the MMPI, disorder); in this study, they also investigated
show many significant correlations with drink- how childhood behavioral disorder symptoms
ing variables, but few with neuropsychological and affective symptomatology affected neuro-
test variables; however, alcoholics with severe psychological performance (Glenn et al., 1993).
neuropsychological impairment often show Childhood behavior symptoms proved to be the
pathological personality profi les (Løberg, 1981; most consistent predictor of neuropsychological
Løberg & Miller, 1986). performance for both male and female alcohol-
ASPD and antisocial traits, for example, ics and controls, although childhood affective
as indexed by elevated Pd scale of the MMPI symptoms were also related.
(Løberg, 1981), characterize a subgroup of alco- Goldstein and Shelly used MMPI to classify
holics. Although some investigators have shown alcoholics as normal, depressed, or psychotic
that alcoholics with ASPD perform worse and then compared them on intellectual and
on neuropsychological testing (Gorenstein, neuropsychological measures (Goldstein et al.,
1982; Hesselbrock et al., 1985a; Malloy et al., 1985). There were no differences in intellec-
1989, 1990), this has not always been the case tual or neuropsychological performance in
(Hoffman et al., 1987; Sutker & Allain, 1987). alcoholics who were classified as “normal” and
One possible reason for these discrepant find- “depressed” using the MMPI; however, alcohol-
ings may be that some ASPD substance abusers ics classified as “psychotic” were more impaired
may have histories of “minimal brain dysfunc- with regard to conceptual and visual–spatial
tion,” Attention Deficit Disorder (ADD), and/ skills; 80% of the “psychotic” alcoholics were
or developmental disorders (Tarter & Edwards, classified as impaired using an AIR of 1.55,
1986) before they begin substance use. Another whereas only 50% of the other groups were
reason may be that certain deviant behaviors similarly classified; “psychotic” alcoholics also
(e.g., drinking-related arrests) may be common had lower verbal skills (Goldstein et al., 1985).
to both antisocial personality and alcoholism, There are, however, some apparently discrepant
and these may carry with them higher neu- findings and infrequent relations between neu-
romedical risks leading to specific neuropsy- ropsychological test performances and person-
chological deficits (Gorenstein, 1987). That is, ality when correlations are performed (Løberg
426 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

& Miller, 1986). For example, it may be that induced effects of alcohol and drugs, we refer
certain personality profi le types may be associ- the interested reader to the study of Anthenelli
ated with elevated levels of neuropsychological and Schuckit (1993).
impairments, which are not obvious when sim- A number of investigators have carried out
ple correlations are made between neuropsy- studies to examine how depressive and anx-
chological test measures and individual clinical iety symptoms influence neuropsychological
scales from the MMPI (Goldstein et al., 1985; test performance. For example, Sinha and col-
Løberg, 1981). leagues (1992) demonstrated that scores on the
Psychiatric symptomatology, particularly Beck Depression Inventory correlated with an
depressive and anxiety symptoms, is common overall measure of neuropsychological impair-
during heavy drinking and during detoxifica- ment in both FH+ and FH– alcoholics. Scores
tion in alcoholics. In one study, 82% of alcoholics on the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale were
(350 of 428 patients) presented to treatment with also shown to be related to Trails B and WAIS
depressive symptomatology (Angelini et al., 1990). Digit Symbol when alcoholics were on average 12
Depressive symptomatology may be explained days sober (Schafer et al., 1991). At the 3-month
both biochemically, since alcohol is a CNS follow-up, estimates of drinking following dis-
depressant, and psychologically, due to remorse charge and severity of depressive symptoms
and guilt over actions not taken, obligations not were significantly related to neuropsychological
fulfilled, and actions performed that should not test performance. However, in another study,
have been undertaken while drinking. severity of depressive symptoms (using the Beck
Brown and her colleagues at the San Diego Depression Inventory) was not associated with
VA Medical Center have documented that cognitive impairment 7–14 days after admission
significant changes in depressive (Brown & or at 6 months (Clark et al., 1984).
Schuckit, 1988; Brown et al., 1995) and anx- Alcoholism and alcohol abuse can also be
iety (Brown et al., 1991) symptoms occur in comorbid with other major mental disorders
primary alcoholics during detoxification. For such as schizophrenia, but the combined effects
example, whereas 42% of alcoholic men had of these disorders on NP are not well understood,
clinically significant levels of depression within since in many NP studies of alcoholics, schizo-
48 hours of admission into an inpatient unit phrenia is an exclusion criterion. Interestingly,
(i.e., with Hamilton Depression Rating Scores Sullivan et al. (2003) reported that there were
of ≥20), only 6% had elevated levels at discharge selective reductions in pontine volumes on MRI
4 weeks later. Th is level of depressive symptom- in dually affected patients that were not noted in
atology at discharge corresponds well with pre- schizophrenics who were not alcoholic. Finally,
vious reports that alcohol-dependent men do alcoholics who also use depressants and opiates
not have elevated rates for major depressive may have more neuropsychological impair-
disorder independent of alcohol-induced mood ments than those who consume only alcohol
syndromes (Schuckit et al., 1994). With regard (Carlin, 1986; Carlin et al., 1978; Grant et al.,
to anxiety, 98% of men (n = 171) reported at 1977, 1978, 1979b).
least one symptom of anxiety during drinking In sum, though depression and anxiety are
or withdrawal, but only 4% fulfi lled criteria for frequent correlates of heavy drinking, but
generalized anxiety disorder with protracted these do not consistently relate to NP fi ndings.
abstinence (i.e., >3 months) (Schuckit et al., ASPD has been variably associated with NP
1990). In another report by these investiga- deficits, and alcoholics with more severe NP
tors, 40% of primary alcoholic men reported deficits can exhibit substantial personality dis-
significantly elevated levels of anxiety state at turbance that may be secondary to their brain
admission, which had returned to within the damage.
normal range by the second week of treatment,
and further reductions were evident with con-
Education Influences
tinued abstinence (Brown et al., 1991). For
information on how to clinically separate affec- Certain intellectual and neuropsychologi-
tive and anxiety disorders from the substance- cal abilities are affected by the level of formal
The Neurobehavioral Correlates of Alcoholism 427

education (Heaton, 1992; Heaton et al., 1991; see controls. In one of their first studies, his labora-
also Heaton et al., this volume, Chapter 7). To tory showed that both alcoholics and controls
illustrate this relationship in alcoholics, when 14 do show a trend to improve their performance
samples of alcoholics were ranked according to on a face-name learning task when monetary
their level of education and their performance incentives were made contingent on their per-
on the Halstead Impairment Index, alcoholics formance; however, though alcoholics did show
with lower education were more often impaired deficits on this task, the authors did not find an
(Grant et al., 1984). In addition, there may be interaction between group membership and
an interaction between drinking and education, incentive (Schaeffer & Parsons, 1988). A sim-
because heavy drinkers with low education gen- ilar paradigm, used to evaluate motivational
erally show more neuropsychological deficits effects on problem-solving ability in alcoholics,
(Williams & Skinner, 1990). also showed no interaction between incentive
A number of investigators have examined and group performance (Schaeffer et al., 1989).
the relationship between premorbid function- In both of the preceding studies, depressive
ing (using in most cases the Vocabulary subtest symptoms, as measured by the Beck Depression
from the WAIS/WAIS-R) and neuropsycholog- Inventory, could not account for these differ-
ical deficits observed in alcoholics. Some have ences. Because alcoholics rate themselves as
suggested that neuropsychological deficits in being more impaired cognitively (Errico et al.,
alcoholics may be explained by premorbid dif- 1990; Shelton & Parsons, 1987), their reduced
ferences, because a number of studies have expectancies may in some way affect their per-
found that the level of neuropsychological defi- formance on neuropsychological tests. A study
cits parallels Vocabulary level (Cutting, 1988; by Sander et al. (1989) addressed this issue and
Draper & Manning, 1982; Emmerson et al., found that alcoholics do have reduced expec-
1988). Lower Vocabulary scores in alcoholics tancies about their performance, which also
may also be a surrogate indicator of past learn- correlate with their actual neuropsychological
ing problems in school. Along these lines, 40% performance; however, neuropsychological dif-
of alcohol-dependent male adults were found to ferences persist when one statistically removes
have had special education, remedial services, or the variance associated with pretest expectan-
repeated grade failure (concurrent with a famil- cies (Sander et al., 1989). Finally, both alcoholics
ial history of alcoholism and current indices of and controls respond similarly to experimental
learning disability) (Rhodes & Jasinski, 1990). manipulations that enhance personal involve-
The authors from this study also suggested that ment or reduce negative affect, further suggest-
having a history of childhood learning disorders ing that motivation differences do not explain
may be related to the development of alcohol- neuropsychological deficits in alcoholics (Nixon
ism (i.e., a compromised central nervous system et al., 1992).
may make one less resistant to familial or social
risk factors for the development of unhealthy Sample Selection influences on NP
drinking practices). Residual learning problems performance
may also explain some of the neuropsycholog-
ical impairment found among detoxified alco- A study by Parsons and his group revealed that
holics (Løberg, 1989; Rhodes & Jasinski, 1990). community alcoholics performed intermedi-
ate to VA alcoholics and controls on a number
of neuropsychological measures suggesting that
The Influence of Motivation and referral source is an important determinant of
Expectancies on Neuropsychological whether deficits will be present (Tivis et al., 1993).
Test Performance in Alcoholics Furthermore, alcoholics who declined participa-
Parsons and his group carried out a number tion in a research study on the effects of alcohol
of studies to determine whether motivation on brain functioning performed worse on the
or expectancies about one’s performance may Shipley Abstraction Test, suggesting that alco-
explain the neuropsychological differences holics who are enrolled may not be as impaired
that are often observed between alcoholics and as those that decline (Nixon et al., 1988).
428 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Neurobehavioral Recovery and et al., 1979, 1980b; Page & Linden, 1974; Page &
Neuroimaging Reversibility in Schaub, 1977; Ryan & Butters, 1980b; Unkenstein
Alcoholics & Bowden, 1991).
There are also conflicting reports on the
Neurobehavioral Recovery extent of neuropsychological recovery in alco-
holics who have maintained longer periods of
The extent to which CNS abnormalities persist abstinence (i.e., many months to several years).
in alcoholics, or recover with increasing length Some investigators have shown that long-term
of stable abstinence, is an area of continuing abstinence can be associated with normal
debate. Certainly, if one were to focus only on or improved neuropsychological functioning
the data from recently detoxified alcoholics (Adams et al., 1980; Berglund et al., 1977;
tested weeks to several months after their last Chaney et al., 1980; Fabian & Parsons, 1983; Fein
drink, one might erroneously conclude that et al., 2006; Gardner et al., 1989; Grant et al.,
alcohol causes chronic and permanent CNS 1979a, 1984, 1987b; Long & McLachlan, 1974;
damage in a high percentage of cases. However, Marchesi et al., 1992; McLachlan & Levinson,
studies that have examined alcoholics after 1974; O’Leary et al., 1977b; Reed et al., 1992;
longer periods of abstinence have been able to Schau et al., 1980; Templer et al., 1975); others,
show that neurobehavioral recovery continues however, have been unable to demonstrate much
for several months, and perhaps years, after a change with long-term abstinence (Brandt et al.,
person stops drinking. A nosology that takes 1983; Eckardt et al., 1980a; Parsons et al., 1990a;
into account this time course, as well as the dif- Ryan et al., 1980; Yohman et al., 1985).
ferential rate of neurobehavioral recovery, has With respect to resumption of drinking, a
been previously described (Grant et al., 1987b) number of longitudinal studies have exam-
(see Figure 18–1). ined the effects of interim drinking on neuro-
Since the first systematic neuropsychological psychological recovery. Those alcoholics who
investigation of alcoholics (Fitzhugh et al., 1960), maintained continuous abstinence, or dem-
a significant number of studies have focused on onstrated “improved drinking habits” over a
the neuropsychological recovery of alcohol- follow-up period, showed more neuropsycho-
ics, particularly with respect to the effects that logical recovery and less deficits at follow-up,
increasing length of abstinence, resumption of relative to alcoholics who resumed drinking at
drinking, and age at testing may have on the moderate or severe levels (Abbott & Gregson,
amount of recovery. 1981; Adams et al., 1980; Berglund et al., 1977;
Of the studies that have examined alcohol- Eckardt et al., 1980a; Fabian & Parsons, 1983;
ics after short periods of abstinence (i.e., weeks Grant et al., 1987b; Gregson & Taylor, 1977;
to several months), some have shown improve- Guthrie & Elliott, 1980; McLachlan & Levinson,
ments in neuropsychological functioning 1974; Muuronen et al., 1989; Parsons et al.,
(Ayers et al., 1978; Bean & Karasievich, 1975; 1990a; Rourke & Grant, 1999; Yohman et al.,
Cermak & Ryback, 1976; Ellenberg et al., 1980; 1985); however, there are two reports that have
Farmer, 1973; Gechter, 1987; Guthrie & Elliott, not found an effect of level of interim drinking
1980; Hester et al., 1980; Kish et al., 1980; Leber on neuropsychological performance (O’Leary
et al., 1981; McIntyre, 1987; Muuronen et al., et al., 1977b; Schau et al., 1980).
1989; Sharp et al., 1977; Smith & Layden, 1972), One of the more comprehensive studies was
particularly with “experience-dependent” stim- reported by Rourke and Grant (1999). These
ulation or training (Forsberg & Goldman, 1985, authors examined alcoholics at the conclusion of
1987; Goldman & Goldman, 1988; Goldman an inpatient treatment program (approximately
et al., 1983, 1985; Roehrich & Goldman, 1993; one month abstinent) and re-evaluated them
Stringer & Goldman, 1988); others, however, about 2 years later. Those who relapsed in the
have shown little neuropsychological recov- interim were compared to those who were con-
ery with short periods of abstinence (Brandt stantly sober in the interim (these were termed
et al., 1983; Claiborn & Greene, 1981; Clarke & intermediate duration sobers). In addition, to
Haughton, 1975; de Obaldia et al., 1981; Eckardt evaluate possible practice effects, and effects of
The Neurobehavioral Correlates of Alcoholism 429

very long-term abstinence, groups of nonalco- Irwin et al., 1989; Schafer et al., 1991; Tarter
holics and alcoholics who were abstinent for a et al., 1986b, 1988) perform worse on neuro-
minimum of 18 months at first assessment were psychological testing. The extent to which these
re-examined 2 years later. Change in NP per- symptoms, events, and conditions influence the
formance was examined with respect to initial rate and amount of neuropsychological recov-
group assignment and interim drinking history. ery when subjects are followed over time has yet
The data indicated that all groups registered to be addressed.
some NP improvement on 2-year re-evaluation, Third, psychometric evaluations were often
but that the improvement in the intermedi- conducted during the first 2 weeks of abstinence
ate group (i.e., the alcoholics who maintained before acute withdrawal effects and detoxifica-
abstinence after discharge) was significantly tion were complete (Ayers et al., 1978; Bean &
more on measures like the Category Test than Karasievich, 1975; Cermak & Ryback, 1976;
for the other groups. This study provided strong Chaney et al., 1980; Claiborn & Greene, 1981;
evidence in support of the notion that there is Clarke & Haughton, 1975; Eckardt et al., 1979,
continued recovery in brain function after the 1980b; Ellenberg et al., 1980; Farmer, 1973;
initial month to 6 weeks of abstinence, indicat- Guthrie & Elliott, 1980; Hester et al., 1980; Kish
ing that conclusions about persisting alcohol et al., 1980; Long & McLachlan, 1974; McIntyre,
effects ought to be deferred until some months 1987; Muuronen et al., 1989; O’Leary et al.,
after completion of treatment. 1977b; Page & Linden, 1974; Page & Schaub, 1977;
At least four methodological shortcomings Schau et al., 1980; Sharp et al., 1977; Smith &
likely contribute to the discrepant findings in Layden, 1972).
the literature regarding the rate and amount Fourth, failure to use a number of tests to
of neuropsychological recovery in alcoholics. adequately represent a single neuropsycholog-
First, many of the longitudinal studies, partic- ical ability area, or to sample a wide range of
ularly those in the 1970s and early 1980s, did neuropsychological abilities, has most likely
not include age- and education-matched control led to an inaccurate estimation of neuropsy-
groups to correct for demographic differences chological deficits. This is particularly salient
and the effects of practice (Ayers et al., 1978; when performance on one test is used to infer
Bean & Karasievich, 1975; Berglund et al., 1977; performance on a neuropsychological ability, or
Cermak & Ryback, 1976; Farmer, 1973; Gechter, when the task difficulty/complexity is manipu-
1987; Guthrie & Elliott, 1980; Hester et al., 1980; lated. Relative to all of the studies carried out
Kish et al., 1980; Long & McLachlan, 1974; on the neuropsychological recovery of alcohol-
McLachlan & Levinson, 1974; Muuronen et al., ics, only a selected number of cross-sectional
1989; Page & Linden, 1974; Page & Schaub, 1977; (Adams & Grant, 1986; de Obaldia et al., 1981;
Smith & Layden, 1972; Unkenstein & Bowden, Grant et al., 1984; Reed et al., 1992) and longitu-
1991). dinal studies (Adams et al., 1980; Chaney et al.,
Second, many studies have not screened and/ 1980; Claiborn & Greene, 1981; Eckardt et al.,
or controlled for adverse neuromedical con- 1979, 1980a, 1980b; Fabian & Parsons, 1983;
founds or risk factors that predate, coexist with, Grant et al., 1987b; Long & McLachlan, 1974;
or occur as a consequence of heavy alcohol McIntyre, 1987; Page & Linden, 1974; Page &
consumption (Tarter & Edwards, 1986). These Schaub, 1977; Parsons et al., 1990a; Rourke &
risk factors or confounds are likely to contrib- Grant, 1999; Schau, et al., 1980; Yohman et al.,
ute or lead to neuropsychological impairments 1985) have included a comprehensive sampling
over and above the neurotoxic effects of alcohol. of neuropsychological abilities.
Along these lines, investigators have shown that
alcoholics with MBD symptoms (de Obaldia &
Parsons, 1984; Tarter, 1982), head injuries Neuroimaging Evidence for Reversibility
(Adams & Grant, 1986; Alterman & Tarter, in Structural Brain Abnormalities
1985; Grant et al., 1984; Hillbom & Holm, 1986; If there is evidence of neurobehavioral recovery
Solomon & Malloy, 1992), or liver dysfunction/ in alcoholics, one would also expect to see evi-
pathology (Acker et al., 1982; Arria et al., 1991a; dence at the structural and functional level for
430 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

brain recovery. In fact, several investigators have Alcoholics had significantly larger ventricles at
provided evidence to suggest that brain mor- the time of their first MRI scan (scan within 2
phology can improve with stable and increasing weeks of alcohol withdrawal; mean 7.3 days);
length of abstinence. however, 19–28 days later (mean 22.6 days), the
In 1978, Carlen and his colleagues demon- difference between alcoholics and controls was
strated a measurable decrease in the degree no longer significant. MRI reports have dem-
of cerebral atrophy using CT, as well as some onstrated that decreases in CSF were associated
functional improvement, in four of eight with increases in tissue volumes, particularly
chronic alcoholics who had maintained their white matter, in alcoholics who maintained sta-
abstinence over the interim. These results ble abstinence over 3 months (Shear et al., 1994)
represented the first report of reversible cere- and 1 year (Drake et al., 1995). Pfefferbaum
bral “atrophy” in recently abstinent chronic (1995) and colleagues confirmed that abstain-
alcoholics using CT scanning. Since this ing alcoholics showed reduction in ventricu-
time, there have been a number of CT as well lar volume and tended to have increase in gray
as MRI reports that have documented reduc- matter volume after one month of abstinence;
tions in cortical and central CSF volumes in a 2–12 month follow-up, those who remained
with extended periods of abstinence or in abstinent had further reduction in third ven-
alcoholics who improve their drinking habits tricle volume, while those who relapsed had
(Artmann et al., 1981; Carlen & Wilkinson, some reduction in gray matter. Other studies
1983, 1987; Carlen et al., 1984; Drake et al., have confirmed that white matter recovers with
1995; Jacobson, 1986a; Mann et al., 1993; abstinence and that volume gains regress with
Marchesi et al., 1994; Muuronen et al., 1989; relapse (Cardenas et al., 2007; O’Neill et al., 2001;
Ron, 1983; Ron et al., 1982; Rourke et al., 1993; Pfefferbaum et al., 1998). Recovery in brain vol-
Shear et al., 1994). umes appears to be most rapid in the early part
Whereas most of these studies have been of the abstinence process (e.g., first month), but
with male alcoholics, a cross-sectional com- may continue for a year (Gazdzinski et al., 2005,
parison of CT parameters in 26 recently detox- see Figure 18–6; see also the color figure in the
ified female alcoholics (abstinent for a mean of color insert section).
33 days) and eight long-term female abstinent Studies of brain metabolites also note absti-
alcoholics (abstinent for a mean of 3.3 years) nence associated reversal of reduction in NAA
recruited from local AA chapters indicated and Cho as well as in myoinositol, which
that female AA members were more compa- may be a marker of inflammation or osmotic
rable to nonalcoholic female controls; in addi- stress (Bartsch et al., 2007; Ende et al., 2005;
tion, there was some indication that cerebral Schweinsburg et al., 2001, 2003). Benzdsus et al.
changes were evident after briefer periods (2001) noted that degree of NAA recovery cor-
of abstinence than with male AA members related to NP improvement. Perfusion studies
(Jacobson, 1986a). Thus, although female including those using single photon emission
alcoholics may experience cerebral struc- computed tomography (SPECT) have noted
tural abnormalities similar to those obtained normalization of tracer uptake in longer term
in male alcoholics that tend to appear after abstainers (Figure 18–7; see also the color figure
shorter and less intense drinking careers, they in the color insert section).
may also tend to recover more quickly than Based on a convergence of evidence from NP,
their male counterparts. MRI, MRI spectroscopy, and electrophysiologi-
MRI studies of brain volumes consistently cal data, it is clear that substantial, and perhaps,
report improvements over time. Mann et al. among younger alcoholics, complete recovery
(1989) reported significant decreases in total of structure and function can occur; however
CSF, ventricular and subarachnoid volumes in the mechanisms of this recovery are unclear.
nine chronic alcoholics after 5 weeks of alcohol While changes in hydration have been sug-
abstinence. In another study (Zipursky et al., gested to explain these effects, current insights
1989), ventricular volumes from 10 alcohol- do not support this notion. In animal models,
ics were compared to 10 age-matched controls. cessation of heavy ethanol exposure has been
The Neurobehavioral Correlates of Alcoholism 431

MR improvement with abstinence

A B

Case 1

Baseline 8 months abstinence

C D

Case 2

Baseline Relapsed after 10 months

Figure 18–6. MRI brain images of two cases. Case 1 was abstinent 1 week (A) and rescanned after 8 months
of continued abstinence (B). Note lessening prominence of suci and ventricles in B. Case 2 was abstinent 30 days
(C) and rescanned at 10 months after relapsing in the interim. Note tissue loss in D, particularly in periventricu-
lar white matter, cerebellar vermis, and surrounding IV ventricle. (Derived from images shown in Gazdzinski,
et al. [2005], Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 78, 263–273. Reprinted with permission from Elsevier.)

associated with dendritic regrowth (McMullen


Decreased cerebral blood flow in alcoholics
and partial recovery after abstinence
et al., 1984). Recovery in NAA, a marker of
neuronal integrity, suggests that a comparable
A B C
mechanism might take place in humans. Animal
models also suggest that ethanol can suppress
neural progenitor cells, an effect that reversed
when ethanol exposure ceased (Nixon & Crews,
2004). Such changes might be linked to reduced
Non-alcoholic Long-term Recently
controls abstinent detoxified availability of trophic factors such as brain
alcoholics alcoholics derived neurotrophic factor, or their receptors
(Climent et al., 2002; Miller et al., 2002). Other
Figure 18–7. Color coded images representing brain changes found in animal models include
intensity of uptake of the tracer HMPAO during a damage to myelin sheaths (Phillips et al., 1991;
cognitive activation task in a nonalcoholic control Vrbaski & Ristic, 1985) and increased lipid per-
(A), alcoholic abstinent over 18 months (B), and a oxidation (Agar et al., 2003) which may provide
recently detoxified alcoholic (C) abstinent 4 weeks. an insight into the white matter loss observed
Cooler colors indicate less perfusion, especially in humans, and the recovery in choline on MRS
in frontal areas in C versus A. Case B has values with abstinence.
intermediate between A and C, suggesting recov- Of interest, studies conducted with human
ery. (From Grant, Alhassoon, et al., San Diego VA postmortem specimens indicate that expression
Alcohol Study.) of genes implicated in control of inflammation,
432 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

cell survival, and myelin integrity is altered in changes to help maintain their abstinence and
alcoholism. One of the molecules regulating to develop more adaptable behaviors. In con-
these genes is Nuclear Factor-kappa B (NF-kB). trast, many older alcoholics who have a num-
Among other things, NF-kB mediated gene ber of medical complications and neuromedical
transcription can alter synaptic signaling and risk factors may have many neuropsychological
promote cell death. NF-kB is suppressed in acute deficits that place significant limitations on their
alcohol exposure, but is elevated in chronic expo- ability to utilize the standard treatment modal-
sure (Ökvist et al., 2007). It seems possible that ities for alcoholism. In a recent study, elderly
alcohol-associated modification of glutamate alcoholics were found to experience more
transmission as well as accompanying oxidative severe withdrawal symptoms (i.e., more cogni-
stress causes intracytoplasmic release of NF-kB tive impairment, disorientation and confusion,
from its binding protein, resulting in its trans- increased daytime sleepiness, weakness, cardiac
location into the cell’s nucleus, and activation disease and high blood pressure) than younger
of genes whose downstream effects are neural alcoholics, despite similar recent drinking his-
dysfunction and injury. If so, abstinence from tory and educational experience (Brower et al.,
alcohol may reverse these molecular changes. 1994). Overall, these results suggest that treat-
ment for alcoholism may take longer in elderly
patients, and interventions should target the
Clinical Implications and
medical comorbidity (Brower et al., 1994) as
Treatment Outcome
well as develop ways to circumvent the neuro-
Even though mild to moderate neuropsycho- psychological deficits that are often prevalent in
logical deficits that are often detected in alco- elderly alcoholics.
holics after 2–4 weeks of detoxification are not A number of investigators have explored how
severe enough to warrant a diagnosis of either neuropsychological test performance during
an Alcohol-Induced Persisting Dementia or an inpatient treatment may predict treatment suc-
Alcohol-Induced Amnestic Disorder (American cess and compliance, later relapse, and everyday
Psychiatric Association, 1994), these deficits can, functioning. Some have demonstrated that neu-
however, have significant effects on treatment ropsychological performance is associated or
compliance, aftercare success, employment, predictive of time in residence (Fals-Stewart &
and everyday functioning of recovering alco- Schafer, 1992), relapse (Gregson & Taylor, 1977),
holics (Abbott & Gregson, 1981; Alterman et al., therapists’ ratings of improvement during ther-
1989b; Donovan et al., 1984, 1985, 1986; Eckardt apy (Parsons, 1983) as well as prognosis (Leber
et al., 1980a; Glenn & Parsons, 1991b; Goldman, et al., 1985), length of abstinence, treatment
1990; Gregson & Taylor, 1977; Guthrie & Elliott, compliance and aftercare success, and later
1980; Leber et al., 1985; McCrady & Smith, employment (Donovan et al., 1984, 1985, 1986;
1986; O’Leary et al., 1979; Parsons et al., 1990a; O’Leary et al., 1979; Walker et al., 1983). In one
Trivedi & Raghavan, 1989). study, alcoholics who remained sober through
One limitation in many inpatient alcohol and 1 year were better educated and had higher
drug treatment programs is that it is difficult memory scores at baseline than those alcoholics
and time consuming to make accommodations who relapsed (George et al., 1992). However, in
for individual patients who might have quite contrast, there are a number of reports in which
different neuropsychological abilities and defi- neuropsychological test performance is not
cits. For example, some younger healthy alco- related to treatment outcome (Alterman et al.,
holic patients may be relatively normal from a 1990; Eckardt et al., 1988; Macciocchi et al.,
neurocognitive perspective, and may be more 1989; Prange, 1988).
ready to pay attention, assimilate new infor- Glenn and Parsons recently examined how
mation (e.g., relapse prevention material), and a number of variables collected at baseline (i.e.,
be able to reason, problem solve, and use their depressive symptoms, neuropsychological test
intact abstracting skills to see how a number performance, psychosocial maladjustment, pre-
of factors have contributed to the development vious treatment, and childhood ADD symptoms)
of their disease, and how they might make life predict resumption of drinking in alcoholics 14
The Neurobehavioral Correlates of Alcoholism 433

months later (Glenn & Parsons, 1991b; Parsons the frontal lobes, and reduced NAA, a metab-
et al., 1990a). They found that alcoholics who olite reflecting neuronal integrity on MR spec-
later resumed drinking performed significantly troscopy. Changes in choline and myoinositol
worse at baseline on all five factors that explained on MRS may reflect white matter injury and
27% of the variance. Overall, 75% of the sample inflammatory changes, and these are related
was correctly classified, and depressive symp- to NP performance. Similarly there may be
toms at baseline were discovered to be the most electrophysiological changes, such as reduced
predictive of relapse. When they excluded neuro- P300 evoked potential, perhaps reflecting fron-
psychological test performance from the model, tostriatal dysfunction. Structural imaging can
the classification rate was slightly reduced to reveal reduction in gray and white matter vol-
72.5% (Glenn & Parsons, 1991b). These results umes on in vivo MRI, with some studies partic-
provide further support that neuropsychological ularly noting changes in frontal and cerebellar
test performance by itself does not significantly structures. With regard to neuropathology both
predict later relapse or treatment success. We neuronal injury, and in advanced cases, neuro-
believe that further studies are needed to deter- nal loss are evident, with frontal and cerebellar
mine how neuropsychological test performance pathology being most prominent. In Wernicke–
may be more of a mediator or intervening vari- Korsakoff cases, additional volume loss can be
able in a model that also incorporates measures noted in mamillary bodies, hippocampus, and
of social support, affective symptomatology, and other diencephalic structures. Loss of dendritic
measures of everyday functioning when trying arbor may be one of the substrates for the NP
to predict treatment compliance, success, and impairment. The molecular mechanisms that
everyday functioning. underlie alcohol’s effect are not well understood,
The potential benefits of cognitive stimu- but may involve excitotoxic cascades, oxidative
lation and remediation during treatment, an stress, and other changes converging to disrupt
area that Goldman and his colleagues have the balance of neurotrophic and cell injurious
advanced (Forsberg & Goldman, 1987; Roehrich pathways. For example, the transcriptional fac-
& Goldman, 1993), deserves further study. tor NF-kappa B may be released, altering gene
For example, Roehrich and Goldman (1993) expression and initiating apoptotic cascades.
reported that alcoholics who received ecologi- Downregulation of genes controlling cytoskele-
cally relevant or neuropsychological remedia- ton and myelin integrity have been suggested as
tion during inpatient treatment showed more mechanisms. It should be noted that many alco-
cognitive improvements over the course of their holics, especially those under 50 without major
treatment, decreased affective symptomatology, comorbidities, do not manifest NP deficits at
a decline in their self-reported cognitive com- any time. The mechanisms underlying host
plaints, and were able to learn more informa- resistance or vulnerability to effects of alcohol
tion on relapse prevention. on the brain are largely unstudied. The picture
is different for the developing brain, and studies
on fetal alcohol exposure document permanent
Summary
brain injury in cases of prenatal exposure.
Alcoholism can be associated with brain injury, Important to recognize is the fact that NP
and this can be reflected in persisting neurocog- deficits and other brain changes observed in
nitive impairment, and associated changes on alcoholics who have been recently detoxified
brain imaging. Disturbances in abstraction- are not necessarily permanent. Indeed, neu-
executive abilities are the most consistently ropsychological recovery occurs through-
reported NP changes, along with reduced out the first year of abstinence, and perhaps
verbal and nonverbal learning and perceptu- longer. This brain recovery is observed on NP
al–motor deficits. Language skills tend to be testing, structural and functional imaging, as
preserved, and accelerated forgetting is uncom- well as in electrophysiological indicators. Such
mon except in cases of Wernicke–Korsakoff recovery may be less complete in older alco-
syndrome. Other functional changes include holics, and those with comorbidities such as
reduced regional brain blood flow, especially in prior mild head injuries, withdrawal seizures,
434 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

medical conditions (e.g., HCV infection; liver performance in alcoholics. Journal of Clinical
disease), and other concurrent substance abuse. and Experimental Neuropsychology, 8, 362–370.
The mechanisms underlying recovery are not Adams, K. M., Grant, I., & Reed, R. (1980).
understood; however animal models suggest Neuropsychology in alcoholic men in their late
thirties: One-year follow-up. American Journal of
that dendritic rearborization may be a factor.
Psychiatry, 137, 928–931.
Thus, we need to view effects of alcoholism on
Agar, E., Demir, S., Amanvermez, R., Bosnak, M.,
the brain as a dynamic process, whose out-
Ayyildiz, M., & Celik, C. (2003). The effects of
comes vary from no injury in the first place, ethanol consumption on the lipid peroxidation
to reversible injury in the majority of cases of and glutathione levels in the right and left brains
adult alcoholism. Whether the recovery is com- of rats. International Journal of Neuroscience, 13,
plete or partial depends on a range of host fac- 1643–1652.
tors, including developmental stage, conditions Akshoomoff, N. A., Delis, D. C., & Kiefner, M. G.
of use, and comorbidities. (1989). Block constructions of chronic alcoholic
and unilateral brain-damaged patients: A test of
the right hemisphere vulnerability hypothesis of
Acknowledgment alcoholism. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology,
Parts of the work referred to in this chapter 4, 275–281.
were supported by a Department of Veterans Albert, M., Butters, N., Rogers, S., Pressman, J., &
Affairs Merit Award, “Alcohol Abuse and Geller, A. (1982). A preliminary report: Nutritional
levels and cognitive performance in chronic alco-
Neuropsychological Impairment” (I. Grant, P.I.).
hol abusers. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 9,
The authors wish to acknowledge the contribu-
131–142.
tions of Dr. Tor Løberg to earlier versions of this Albert, M., Butters, N., Rogers, S., Pressman, J., &
chapter. The authors thank Ms. Felicia Roston for Geller, A. (1983). Nutritional links between cog-
her editorial assistance in the preparation of this nitive performance and alcohol misuse. Digest of
chapter. Alcoholism Theory and Application, 2, 44–47.
Alexander, S., Kerr, M. E., Yonas, H., & Marion, D. W.
References (2004). The effects of admission alcohol level on
cerebral blood flow and outcomes after severe
Abbott, M. W., & Gregson, R. A. M. (1981). Cognitive traumatic brain injury. Journal of Neurotrauma,
dysfunction in the prediction of relapse in alco- 21(5), 575–583.
holics. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 42, 230–243. Alterman, A. I., Bridges, K. R., & Tarter, R. E. (1986).
Acker, C. (1985). Performance of female alcohol- The influence of both drinking and familial
ics on neuropsychological testing. Alcohol and risk statuses on cognitive functioning of social
Alcoholism, 20, 379–386. drinkers. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental
Acker, C., Jacobson, R. R., & Lishman, W. A. (1987). Research, 10, 448–451.
Memory and ventricular size in alcoholics. Alterman, A. I., & Gerstley, L. J. (1986). Predictive
Psychological Medicine, 17, 343–348. validity of four factors derived from an
Acker, W., Aps, E. J., Majumdar, S. K., Shaw, G. K., & “Hyperactivity/MBD” questionnaire. Drug and
Thomson, A. D. (1982). The relationship between Alcohol Dependence, 18, 259–271.
brain and liver damage in chronic alcoholic Alterman, A. I., Gerstley, L. J., Goldstein, G., &
patients. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Tarter, R. E. (1987). Comparisons of the cognitive
Psychiatry, 45, 984–987. functioning of familial and nonfamilial alcoholics.
Adams, K. M., Gilman, S., Koeppe, R. A., Kluin, Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 48, 425–429.
K. J., Brunberg, J. A., Dede, D., et al. (1993). Alterman, A. I., Goldstein, G., Shelly, C., Bober, B.,
Neuropsychological deficits are correlated with fron- & Tarter, R. E. (1985b). The impact of mild head
tal hypometabolism in positron emission tomogra- injury on neuropsychological capacity in chronic
phy studies of older alcoholic patients. Alcoholism: alcoholics. International Journal of Neuroscience,
Clinical and Experimental Research, 17, 205–210. 28, 155–162.
Adams, K. M., & Grant, I. (1984). Failure of nonlin- Alterman, A. I., & Hall, J. G. (1989). Effects of social
ear models of drinking history variables to predict drinking and familial alcoholism risk on cognitive
neuropsychological performance in alcoholics. functioning: Null findings. Alcoholism: Clinical
American Journal of Psychiatry, 141, 663–667. and Experimental Research, 13, 799–803.
Adams, K. M., & Grant, I. (1986). Influence of Alterman, A. I., Holahan, J. M., Baughman, T. G.,
premorbid risk factors on neuropsychological & Michels, S. (1989b). Predictors of alcoholics’
The Neurobehavioral Correlates of Alcoholism 435

acquisition of treatment-related knowledge. fluid spaces in chronic alcoholics. American


Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 6, 49–53. Journal of Neuroradiology, 2, 23–27.
Alterman, A. I., Kushner, H., & Holahan, J. M. (1990). Ayers, J., Ruff, C. F., & Templer, D. I. (1976).
Cognitive functioning and treatment outcome in Alcoholism, cigarette smoking, coffee drinking
alcoholics. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, and extraversion. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 37,
178, 494–499. 983–985.
Alterman, A. I., & McLellan, A. T. (1986). A factor- Ayers, J. L., Templer, D. I., Ruff, C. F., & Barthlow, V. L.
analytic study of Tarter’s “Hyperactivity–MBD” (1978). Trail making test improvement in absti-
questionnaire. Addictive Behaviors, 11, 287–294. nent alcoholics. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 39,
Alterman, A. I., Searles, J. S., & Hall, J. G. (1989a). 1627–1629.
Failure to find differences in drinking behavior Barron, J. H., & Russell, E. W. (1992). Fluidity the-
as a function of familial risk for alcoholism: A ory and neuropsychological impairment in alco-
replication. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98, holism. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 7,
50–53. 175–188.
Alterman, A. I., & Tarter, R. E. (1985). Relationship Bartsch, A. J., Homola, G., Biller, A., Smith, S. M.,
between familial alcoholism and head injury. Weijers, H. G., Wiesbeck, G. A., et al. (2007).
Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 46, 256–258. Manifestations of early brain recovery associ-
Alterman, A. I., Tarter, R. E., Baughman, T. G., Bober, ated with abstinence from alcoholism. Brain, 130
B. A., & Fabian, S. A. (1985a). Differentiation of (Pt 1), 36–47.
alcoholics high and low in childhood hyperactiv- Bates, M. E., & Pandina, R. J. (1992). Familial alco-
ity. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 15, 111–121. holism and premorbid cognitive deficit: A failure
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic to replicate subtype differences. Journal of Studies
and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th on Alcohol, 53, 320–327.
ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Bates, M. E., & Tracy, J. I. (1990). Cognitive function-
Association. ing in young “social drinkers”: Is there impair-
Angelini, G., Bogetto, F., Borio, R., Meluzzi, A., Mucci, ment to detect? Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
P., Patria, D., et al. (1990). Clinico-nosographic 99, 242–249.
remarks on the relationship between depression Bauer, L. O. (1994). Electroencephalographic and
and alcoholism in a population of 450 hospitalized autonomic predictors of relapse in alcohol-
alcoholics. Minerva Psichiatrica, 31, 41–45. dependent patients. Alcoholism: Clinical and
Anthenelli, R. M., & Schuckit, M. A. (1993). Affective Experimental Research, 18, 760–775.
and anxiety disorders and alcohol and drug Bean, K. L., & Karasievich, G. O. (1975). Psychological
dependence: Diagnosis and treatment. Journal of test results at three stages of inpatient alcohol-
Addictive Diseases, 12, 73–87. ism treatment. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 36,
Archibald, S. L., Fennema-Notestine, C., Gamst, A., 838–852.
Riley, E. P., Mattson, S. N., & Jernigan, T. L. Beatty, W. W., Bailly, R. C., & Fisher, L. (1989).
(2001). Brain dysmorphology in individuals with Korsakoff-like amnesic syndrome in a patient
severe prenatal alcohol exposure. Developmental with anorexia and vomiting. International Journal
Medicine & Child Neurology, 43(3), 148–154. of Clinical Neuropsychology, 11, 55–65.
Arendt, T., Bigl, V., Arendt, A., & Tennstedt, A. Beatty, W. W., Katzung, V. M., Nixon, S. J., &
(1983). Loss of neurons in the nucleus basilis of Moreland, V. J. (1993). Problem-solving deficits
Meynert in Alzheimer’s disease, paralysis agitans in alcoholics: Evidence from the California Card
and Korsakoff ’s disease. Acta Neuropathologica, Sorting Test. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 54,
61, 101–108. 687–692.
Arria, A. M., Tarter, R. E., Kabene, M. A., Laird, S. B., Becker, J. T., Butters, N., Hermann, A., & D’Angelo, N.
Moss, H., & Van Thiel, D. H. (1991a). The role of (1983). Learning to associate names and faces:
cirrhosis in memory functioning of alcoholics. Impaired acquisition on an ecologically rele-
Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, vant memory task by male alcoholics. Journal of
15, 932–937. Nervous and Mental Disease, 171, 617–623.
Arria, A. M., Tarter, R. E., Starzl, T. E., & Van Th iel, D. H. Becker, J. T., Furman, J. M., Panisset, M., & Smith, C.
(1991b). Improvement in cognitive functioning of (1990). Characteristics of the memory loss of a
alcoholics following orthotopic liver transplan- patient with Wernicke-Korsakoff ’s syndrome with-
tation. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental out alcoholism. Neuropsychologia, 28, 171–179.
Research, 15, 956–962. Begleiter, H., & Porjesz, B. (1990). Neuroelectric pro-
Artmann, H., Gall, M. V., Hacker, H., & Herrlich, J. cesses in individuals at risk for alcoholism. Alcohol
(1981). Reversible enlargement of cerebral spinal and Alcoholism, 25, 251–256.
436 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Begleiter, H., & Porjesz, B. (1999). What is inherited in of the brain and neuropsychological assessment
the predisposition toward alcoholism? A proposed of alcoholic patients. Advances in Experimental
model. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Medicine and Biology, 126, 771–786.
Research, 23(7), 1125–1135. Berman, S. M., Whipple, S. C., Fitch, R. J., &
Begleiter, H., Porjesz, B., Bihari, B., & Kissin, B. Noble, E. P. (1993). P3 in young boys as a predictor
(1984). Event-related brain potentials in boys at of adolescent substance use. Alcohol, 10, 69–76.
risk for alcoholism. Science, 225, 1493–1496. Bertera, J. H., & Parsons, O. A. (1978). Impaired
Begleiter, H., Porjesz, B., & Chou, C. L. (1981). visual search in alcoholics. Alcoholism: Clinical
Auditory brainstem potentials in chronic alcohol- and Experimental Research, 2, 9–14.
ics. Science, 211, 1064–1066. Blansjaar, B. A., & van Dijk, J. G. (1992). Korsakoff
Begleiter, H., Porjesz, B., Rawlings, R., & Eckardt, M. minus Wernicke syndrome. Alcohol and
(1987). Auditory recovery function and P3 in boys Alcoholism, 27, 435–437.
at high risk for alcoholism. Alcohol, 4, 315–321. Blansjaar, B. A., Vielvoye, G. J., van Dijk, J. G., &
Begleiter, H., Porjesz, B., & Tenner, M. (1980). Rijnders, R. J. (1992). Similar brain lesions in alco-
Neuroradiological and neurophysiological evi- holics and Korsakoff patients: MRI, psychomet-
dence of brain deficits in chronic alcoholics. Acta ric and clinical findings. Clinical Neurology and
Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 62, 3–13. Neurosurgery, 94, 197–203.
Berglund,M.,Hagstadius,S.,Risberg,J.,Johanson,T.M., Blass, J. P., & Gibson, G. E. (1977). Abnormality of
Bliding, A., & Mubrin, Z. (1987). Normalization of a thiamine-requiring enzyme in patients with
regional cerebral blood flow in alcoholics during Wernicke–Korsakoff syndrome. New England
the first 7 weeks of abstinence. Acta Psychiatrica Journal of Medicine, 297, 1367–1370.
Scandinavica, 75, 202–208. Blusewicz, M. J., Dustman, R. E., Schenkenberg, T.,
Berglund, M., Leijonquist, H., & Horlen, M. (1977). & Beck, E. C. (1977a). Neuropsychological corre-
Prognostic significance and reversibility of cere- lates of chronic alcoholism and aging. Journal of
bral dysfunction in alcoholics. Journal of Studies Nervous and Mental Disease, 165, 348–355.
on Alcohol, 38, 1761–1770. Blusewicz, M. J., Schenkenberg, T., Dustman, R. E.,
Bergman, H. (1985). Cognitive deficits and mor- & Beck, E. C. (1977b). WAIS performance in
phological cerebral changes in a random sam- young normal, young alcoholic, and elderly nor-
ple of social drinkers. Recent Developments in mal groups: An evaluation of organicity and men-
Alcoholism, 3, 265–276. tal aging indices. Journal of Clinical Psychology,
Bergman, H. (1987). Brain dysfunction related to 33, 1149–1153.
alcoholism: Some results from the KARTAD Bowden, S. C. (1987). Brain impairment in social
Project. In O. A. Parsons, N. Butters, and P. E. drinkers? No cause for concern. Alcoholism:
Nathan (Eds.), Neuropsychology of alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 11,
Implications for diagnosis and treatment (pp. 407–410.
21–44). New York: Guilford Press. Bowden, S. C. (1990). Separating cognitive impair-
Bergman, H., Axelsson, G., Idestrom, C. M., Borg, S., ment in neurologically asymptomatic alcoholism
Hindmarsh, T., Makower, J., et al. (1983). Alcohol from Wernicke–Korsakoff syndrome: Is the neu-
consumption, neuropsychological status and ropsychological distinction justified? Psychological
computer-tomographic findings in a random Bulletin, 107, 355–366.
sample of men and women from the general pop- Braggio, J. T., Pishkin, V., Parsons, O. A., Fishkin,
ulation. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, S. M., & Tassey, J. R. (1991). Differences between
18, 501–505. essential and reactive alcoholics on tests of neuro-
Bergman, H., Borg, S., Hindmarsh, T., Idestrom, C. M., psychological functioning and affect. Psychological
& Mutzell, S. (1980a). Computed tomography of Reports, 69, 1131–1136.
the brain and neuropsychological assessment of Brandt, J., Butters, N., Ryan, C., & Bayog, R. (1983).
male alcoholic patients and a random sample from Cognitive loss and recovery in long-term alco-
the general male population. Acta Psychiatrica hol abusers. Archives of General Psychiatry, 40,
Scandinavica, 286(Suppl.), 77–88. 435–442.
Bergman, H., Borg, S., Hindmarsh, T., Idestrom, C. M., Brewer, C., & Perrett, L. (1971). Brain damage due
& Mutzell, S. (1980b). Computed tomography of to alcohol consumption: An airencephalographic,
the brain, clinical examination and neuropsycho- psychometric and electroencephalographic study.
logical assessment of a random sample of men British Journal of Addiction, 66, 170–182.
from the general population. Acta Psychiatrica Brooks, N., Symington, C., Beattie, A., Campsie, L.,
Scandinavica, 286(Suppl.), 47–56. Bryden, J., & McKinlay, W. (1989). Alcohol and
Bergman, H., Borg, S., Hindmarsh, T., Idestrom, C. M., other predictors of cognitive recovery after severe
& Mutzell, S. (1980c). Computed-tomography head injury. Brain Injury, 3, 235–246.
The Neurobehavioral Correlates of Alcoholism 437

Brower, K. J., Mudd, S., Blow, F. C., Young, J. P., & Cala, L. A., & Mastaglia, F. L. (1981). Computerized
Hill, E. M. (1994). Severity and treatment of alco- tomography in chronic alcoholics. Alcoholism:
hol withdrawal in elderly versus younger patients. Clinical and Experimental Research, 5, 283–294.
Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, Campbell, W. G., & Hodgins, D. C. (1993).
18, 196–201. Alcohol-related blackouts in a medical practice.
Brown, S. A., Inaba, R. K., Gillin, J. C., Schuckit, American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 19,
M. A., Stewart, M. A., & Irwin, M. R. (1995). 369–376.
Alcoholism and affective disorder: Clinical course Cardenas, V. A., Studholme, C., Gazdzinski, S.,
of depressive symptoms. American Journal of Durazzo, T. C., & Meyerhoff, D. J. (2007).
Psychiatry, 152, 45–52. Deformation-based morphometry of brain
Brown, S. A., Irwin, M., & Schuckit, M. A. (1991). changes in alcohol dependence and abstinence.
Changes in anxiety among abstinent male alco- Neuroimage, 34(3), 879–887.
holics. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 52, 55–61. Carey, K. B., & Maisto, S. A. (1987). Effect of a change
Brown, S. A., & Schuckit, M. A. (1988). Changes in in drinking pattern on the cognitive function
depression among abstinent alcoholics. Journal of of female social drinkers. Journal of Studies on
Studies on Alcohol, 49, 412–417. Alcohol, 48, 236–242.
Burger, M. C., Botwinick, J., & Storandt, M. (1987). Carlen, P. L., & Wilkinson, D. A. (1980). Alcoholic
Aging, alcoholism, and performance on the Luria- brain damage and reversible deficits. Acta
Nebraska Neuropsychological Battery. Journal of Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 286(Suppl.), 103–118.
Gerontology, 42, 69–72. Carlen, P. L., & Wilkinson, D. A. (1983). Assessment
Butters, N., & Cermak, L. S. (1980). Alcoholic of neurological dysfunction and recovery in
Korsakoff ’s syndrome: An information processing alcoholics: CT scanning and other techniques.
approach to amnesia. New York: Academic Press. Substance and Alcohol Actions/Misuse, 4, 191–197.
Butters, N., Cermak, L. S., Montgomery, K., & Carlen, P. L., & Wilkinson, D. A. (1987). Reversibility
Adinolfi, A. (1977). Some comparisons of the of alcohol-related brain damage: Clinical
memory and visuoperceptive deficits of chronic and experimental observations. Acta Medica
alcoholics and patients with Korsakoff ’s disease. Scandinavica, 717(Suppl.), 19–26.
Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, Carlen, P. L., Wilkinson, D. A., Wortzman, G., &
1, 73–80. Holgate, R. (1984). Partially reversible cerebral
Butters, N., & Granholm, E. (1987). The continuity atrophy and functional improvement in recently
hypothesis: Some conclusions and their implica- abstinent alcoholics. Le Journal Canadien des
tions for the etiology and neuropathology of alco- Sciences Neurologiques, 11, 441–446.
holic Korsakoff ’s syndrome. In O. A. Parsons, N. Carlin, A. S. (1986). Neuropsychological conse-
Butters, and P. E. Nathan (Eds.), Neuropsychology quences of drug abuse. In I. Grant and K. M.
of alcoholism: Implications for diagnosis and Adams (Eds.), Neuropsychological assessment of
treatment (pp. 176–206). New York: Guilford Press. neuropsychiatric disorders (pp. 478–497). New
Butters, N., & Salmon, D. P. (1986). Etiology and York: Oxford University Press.
neuropathology of alcoholic Korsakoff ’s syn- Carlin, A. S., Stauss, F. F., Adams, K. M., & Grant, I.
drome: New findings and speculations. In I. (1978). The prediction of neuropsychologi-
Grant (Ed.), Neuropsychiatric correlates of alco- cal impairment in polydrug abusers. Addictive
holism (pp. 61–108). Washington, DC: American Behaviors, 3, 5–12.
Psychiatric Press. Caruso, K., & ten Bensel, R. (1993). Fetal alcohol syn-
Butters, N., Salmon, D. P., Cullum, C. M., Cairns, P., drome and fetal alcohol effects: The University of
Tröster, A. I., Jacobs, D., et al. (1988). Differentiation Minnesota experience. Minnesota Medicine, 76,
of amnesic and demented patients with the 25–29.
Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised. The Clinical Caspari, D., Trabert, W., Heinz, G., Lion, N., Henkes,
Neuropsychologist, 2, 133–148. H., & Huber, G. (1993). The pattern of regional
Cadaveira, F., Corominas, M., Rodríguez Holguín, S., cerebral blood flow during alcohol withdrawal:
Sánchez-Turet, M., & Grau, C. (1994). Reversibility A single photon emission tomography study with
of brain-stem evoked potential abnormali- 99mTc-HMPAO. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica,
ties in abstinent chronic alcoholics: one year 87, 414–417.
follow-up. Electroencephalography and Clinical Cermak, L. S., & Ryback, R. S. (1976). Recovery of
Neurophysiology, 90(6), 450–455. verbal short-term memory in alcoholics. Journal
Cairney, S., Clough, A., Jaragba, M., & Maruff, P. of Studies on Alcohol, 37, 46–52.
(2007). Cognitive impairment in Aboriginal Chandler, B. C., & Parsons, O. A. (1977). Altered
people with heavy episodic patterns of alcohol use. hemispheric functioning under alcohol. Journal of
Addiction, 102(6), 909–915. Studies on Alcohol, 38, 381–391.
438 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Chaney, E. F., O’Leary, M. R., Fehrenbach, P. A., & Korsakoff syndrome due to left thalamic infarction.
Donovan, D. (1980). Cognitive deficit in middle- Journal of Neurological Sciences, 110, 62–67.
aged alcoholics. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 6, Conry, J. (1990). Neuropsychological deficits in
219–226. fetal alcohol syndrome and fetal alcohol effects.
Chanraud, S., Martelli, C., Delain, F., Kostogianni, Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research,
N., Douaud, G., Aubin, H. J., et al. (2007). Brain 14, 650–655.
morphometry and cognitive performance in Costa, L., & Bauer, L. (1997). Quantitative electroen-
detoxified alcohol-dependents with preserved psy- cephalographic differences associated with alcohol,
chosocial functioning. Neuropsychopharmacology, cocaine, heroin and dual-substance dependence.
32(2), 429–438. Drug Alcohol Dependence, 46(1–2), 87–93.
Chelune, G. J., & Parker, J. B. (1981). Neu- Courville, C. B. (1955). Effects of alcohol on the ner-
ropsychological deficits associated with chronic vous system of man. Los Angeles: San Lucas Press.
alcohol abuse. Clinical Psychology Review, 1, Coutin-Churchman, P., Añez, Y., Uzcátegui, M.,
181–195. Alvarez, L., Vergara, F., Méndez, L., et al. (2003).
Chick, J. D., Smith, M. A., Engleman, H. M., Quantitative spectral analysis of EEG in psychi-
Kean, D. M., Mander, A. J., Douglas, R. H. B., atry revisited: drawing signs out of numbers in
et al. (1989). Magnetic resonance imaging of the a clinical setting. Clinical Neurophysiology, 114,
brain in alcoholics: Cerebral atrophy, lifetime 2294–2306.
alcohol consumption, and cognitive deficits. Coutin-Churchman, P., Moreno, R., Añez, Y., &
Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, Vergara, F. (2006). Clinical correlates of quantita-
13, 512–518. tive EEG alterations in alcoholic patients. Clinical
Chu, N. S. (1985). Computed tomographic correlates Neurophysiology, 117(4), 740–751.
of auditory brainstem responses in alcoholics. Crews, F. T., Braun, C. J., Hoplight, B., Switzer, R.
Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, C. 3rd, & Knapp, D. J. (2000). Binge ethanol con-
48, 348–353. sumption causes differential brain damage in
Chu, N. S., Squires, K. C., & Starr, A. (1982). Auditory young adolescent rats compared with adult rats.
brain stem responses in chronic alcoholic Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research,
patients. Electroencephalography and Clinical 24(11), 1712–1723.
Neurophysiology, 54, 418–425. Crews, F. T., Buckley, T., Dodd, P. R., Ende, G.,
Claiborn, J. M., & Greene, R. L. (1981). Foley, N., Harper, C., et al. (2005). Alcoholic neu-
Neuropsychological changes in recovering men robiology: changes in dependence and recovery.
alcoholics. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 42, Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research,
757–765. 29(8), 1504–1513.
Clark, D. C., Pisani, V. D., Aagesen, C. A., Sellers, D., Cutting, J. C. (1988). Alcohol cognitive impairment
& Fawcett, J. (1984). Primary affective disorder, and aging: Still an uncertain relationship. British
drug abuse, and neuropsychological impairment Journal of Addiction, 83, 995–997.
in sober alcoholics. Alcoholism: Clinical and Cynn, V. E. (1992). Persistence and problem-solving
Experimental Research, 8, 399–404. skills in young male alcoholics. Journal of Studies
Clarke, J., & Haughton, H. (1975). A study of intel- on Alcohol, 53, 57–62.
lectual impairment and recovery rates in heavy Dally, S., Luft, A., Ponsin, J. C., Girre, C., Mamo, H.,
drinkers in Ireland. British Journal of Psychiatry, & Fournier, E. (1988). Abnormal pattern of cere-
126, 178–184. bral blood flow distribution in young alcohol
Climent, E., Pascual, M., Renau-Piqueras, J., & addicts. British Journal of Addiction, 83, 105–109.
Guerri, C. (2002). Ethanol exposure enhances cell de la Monte, S. M. (1988). Disproportionate atrophy
death in the developing cerebral cortex: role of of cerebral white matter in chronic alcoholics.
brain-derived neurotrophic factor and its signal- Archives of Neurology, 45, 990–992.
ing pathways. Journal of Neuroscience Research, de Obaldia, R., Leber, W. R., & Parsons, O. A. (1981).
68(2), 213–225. Assessment of neuropsychological functions
Cloninger, C. R. (1987). Neurogenetic adaptive in chronic alcoholics using a standardized ver-
mechanisms in alcoholism. Science, 236, 410–416 sion of Luria’s Neuropsychological Technique.
Cohen, H. L., Porjesz, B., & Begleiter, H. (1991). EEG International Journal of Neuroscience, 14, 85–93.
characteristics in males at risk for alcoholism. de Obaldia, R., & Parsons, O. A. (1984). Relationship
Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, of neuropsychological performance to primary
15, 858–861. alcoholism and self-reported symptoms of child-
Cole, M., Winkelman, M. D., Morris, J. C., hood minimal brain dysfunction. Journal of
Simon, J. E., & Boyd, T. A. (1992). Thalamic amnesia: Studies on Alcohol, 45, 386–392.
The Neurobehavioral Correlates of Alcoholism 439

DeJong, C. A., van den Brink, W., Harteveld, F. M., & Dupont, R. M., Rourke, S. B., Grant, I., Lehr, P. P.,
van der Wielen, E. G. (1993). Personality disorders Reed, R. J., Challakere, K., et al. (1996). Single
in alcoholics and drug addicts. Comprehensive photon emission computed tomography with
Psychiatry, 34, 87–94. iodo-amphetamine-123 and neuropsycholog-
Desmond, J. E., Chen, S. H., DeRosa, E., Pryor, M. ical studies in long-term abstinent alcoholics.
R., Pfefferbaum, A., & Sullivan, E. V. (2003). Psychiatry Research, 67, 99–111.
Increased frontocerebellar activation in alcoholics Durand, D., Saint-Cyr, J. A., Gurevich, N., &
during verbal working memory: An fMRI study. Carlen, P. L. (1989). Ethanol-induced dendritic
Neuroimage, 19(4), 1510–1520. alterations in hippocampal granule cells. Brain
DiFranza, J. R., & Guerrera, M. P. (1990). Alcoholism Research, 477, 373–377.
and smoking. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 51, Eckardt, M. J., & Martin, P. R. (1986). Clinical assess-
130–135. ment of cognition in alcoholism. Alcoholism:
Dikmen, S. S., Donovan, D. M. Løberg T., Clinical and Experimental Research, 10, 123–127.
Machamer, J. E., & Temkin, N. R. (1993). Alcohol Eckardt, M. J., Parker, E. S., Noble, E. P., Feldman,
use and its effects on neuropsychological outcome D. J., & Gottschalk, L. A. (1978). Relationship
in head injury. Neuropsychology, 7, 296–305. between neuropsychological performance and
Donovan, D. M., Kivlahan, D. R., & Walker, R. D. alcohol consumption in alcoholics. Biological
(1984). Clinical limitations of neuropsychological Psychiatry, 13, 551–565.
testing in predicting treatment outcome among Eckardt, M. J., Parker, E. S., Noble, E. P., Pautler,
alcoholics. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental C. P., & Gottschalk, L. A. (1979). Changes in neu-
Research, 8, 470–475. ropsychological performance during treatment for
Donovan, D. M., Kivlahan, D. R., & Walker, R. D. alcoholism. Biological Psychiatry, 14, 943–954.
(1986). Alcoholic subtypes based on multiple Eckardt, M. J., Parker, E. S., Pautler, C. P., Noble, E. P.,
assessment domains: Validation against treatment & Gottschalk, L. A. (1980a). Neuropsychological
outcome. Recent Developments in Alcoholism, 4, consequences of posttreatment drinking behav-
207–222. ior in male alcoholics. Psychiatry Research, 2,
Donovan, D. M., Kivlahan, D. R., Walker, R. D., & 135–147.
Umlauf, R. (1985). Derivation and validation of Eckardt, M. J., Rawlings, R. R., Graubard, B. I.,
neuropsychological clusters among men alcohol- Faden, V., Martin, P. R., & Gottschalk, L. A. (1988).
ics. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 46, 205–211. Neuropsychological performance and treatment
Donovan, D. M., Queisser, H. R., & O’Leary, M. R. outcome in male alcoholics. Alcoholism: Clinical
(1976). Group embedded figures test performance and Experimental Research, 12, 88–93.
as a predictor of cognitive impairment among Eckardt, M. J., Ryback, R. S., & Pautler, C. P. (1980b).
alcoholics. International Journal of the Addictions, Neuropsychological deficits in alcoholic men in
11, 725–739. their mid thirties. American Journal of Psychiatry,
Drake, A. I., Hannay, H. J., & Gam, J. (1990). Effects 137, 932–936.
of chronic alcoholism on hemispheric function- Eckardt, M. J., Stapleton, J. M., Rawlings, R. R.,
ing: An examination of gender differences for Davis, E. Z., & Grodin, D. M. (1995).
cognitive and dichotic listening tasks. Journal of Neuropsychological functioning in detoxi-
Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 12, fied alcoholics between 18 and 35 years of age.
781–797. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 53–59.
Drake, A. I., Jernigan, T. L., Butters, N., Shear, P. K., Ehlers, C. L., & Schuckit, M. A. (1990). EEG fast fre-
& Archibald, S. L. (1995). Volumetric changes on quency activity in the sons of alcoholics. Biological
magnetic resonance imaging in chronic alcoholics: Psychiatry, 27, 631–641.
A one year follow-up. Journal of the International Ehlers, C. L., & Schuckit, M. A. (1991). Evaluation
Neuropsychological Society, 1, 393–394 (Abst.). of EEG alpha activity in sons of alcoholics.
Draper, R. J., & Manning, A. (1982). Vocabulary def- Neuropsychopharmacology, 4, 199–205.
icit and abstraction impairment in hospitalized Ellenberg, L., Rosenbaum, G., Goldman, M. S., &
alcoholics. Psychological Medicine, 12, 341–347. Whitman, R. D. (1980). Recoverability of psy-
Drejer, K., Theilgaard, A., Teasdale, T. W., chological functioning following alcohol abuse:
Schulsinger, F., & Goodwin, D. W. (1985). A pro- Lateralization effects. Journal of Consulting and
spective study of young men at high risk for Clinical Psychology, 48, 503–510.
alcoholism: Neuropsychological assessment. Ellis, R. J. (1990). Dichotic asymmetries in aging
Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 9, and alcoholic subjects. Alcoholism: Clinical and
498–502. Experimental Research, 14, 863–871.
440 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Ellis, R. J., & Oscar-Berman, M. (1989). Alcoholism, Bender-Gestalt. Quarterly Journal of Studies on
aging, and functional cerebral asymmetries. Alcohol, 34, 786–796.
Psychological Bulletin, 106, 128–147. Fein, G., Torres, J., Price, L. J., & Di Sciafani, V.
Emmerson, R. Y., Dustman, R. E., Heil, J., & (2006). Cognitive performance in long-term absti-
Shearer, D. E. (1988). Neuropsychological perfor- nent alcoholic individuals. Alcoholism, Clinical
mance of young nondrinkers, social drinkers, and and Experimental Research, 30(9), 1538–1544.
long- and short-term sober alcoholics. Alcoholism: Feuerlein, W. (1977). Neuropsychiatric disorders of
Clinical and Experimental Research, 12, 625–629. alcoholism. Nutrition and Metabolism, 21, 163–174.
Emmerson, R. Y., Dustman, R. E., Shearer, D. E., & Finn, P. R., & Justus, A. (1999). Reduced EEG alpha
Chamberlin, H. M. (1987). EEG, visually evoked power in the male and female offspring of alco-
and event related potentials in young abstinent holics. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental
alcoholics. Alcohol, 4, 241–248. Research, 23(2), 256–262.
Ende, G., Welzel, H., Walter, S., Weber-Fahr, W., Fitzhugh, L. C., Fitzhugh, K. B., & Reitan, R. M.
Diehl, A., Hermann, D., et al. (2005). Monitoring (1960). Adaptive abilities and intellectual func-
the effects of chronic alcohol consumption and tioning in hospitalized alcoholics. Quarterly
abstinence on brain metabolism: A longitudinal Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 21, 414–423.
proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy study. Fitzhugh, L. C., Fitzhugh, K. B., & Reitan, R. M.
Biological Psychiatry, 58(12), 974–980. (1965). Adaptive abilities and intellectual func-
Erbas, B., Bekdik, C., Erbengi, G., Enunlu, T., Aytac, S., tioning of hospitalized alcoholics: Further consid-
Kumbasar, H., et al. (1992). Regional cerebral erations. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol,
blood flow changes in chronic alcoholism using 26, 402–411.
Tc-99m HMPAO SPECT: Comparison with Flannery, B., Fishbein, D., Krupitsky, E., Langevin,
CT parameters. Clinical Nuclear Medicine, 17, D., Verbitskaya, E., Bland, C., et al. (2007). Gender
123–127. differences in neurocognitive functioning among
Errico, A. L., Nixon, S. J., Parsons, O. A., & Tassey, J. alcohol-dependent Russian patients. Alcoholism,
(1990). Screening for neuropsychological impair- Clinical and Experimental Research, 31(5),
ment in alcoholics. Psychological Assessment, 2, 745–754.
45–50. Forsberg, L. K., & Goldman, M. S. (1985). Experience-
Ervin, C. S., Little, R. E., Streissguth, A. P., & dependent recovery of visuospatial functioning
Beck, D. E. (1984). Alcoholic fathering and its in older alcoholic persons. Journal of Abnormal
relation to child’s intellectual development: Psychology, 94, 519–529.
A pilot investigation. Alcoholism: Clinical and Forsberg, L. K., & Goldman, M. S. (1987). Experience-
Experimental Research, 8, 362–365. dependent recovery of cognitive deficits in alco-
Everett, M., Schaeffer, K. W., & Parsons, O. A. (1988). holics: Extended transfer of training. Journal of
Learning impairment in male and female alco- Abnormal Psychology, 96, 345–353.
holics. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 3, Forton, D. M., Allsop, J. M., Cox, I. J., Hamilton, G.,
203–211. Wesnes, K., Thomas, H. C., et al. (2005). A review
Fabian, M. S., Jenkins, R. L., & Parsons, O. A. (1981). of cognitive impairment and cerebral metabolite
Gender, alcoholism, and neuropsychological abnormalities in patients with hepatitis C infec-
functioning. Journal of Consulting and Clinical tion. AIDS, 19(Suppl 3), S53–S63.
Psychology, 49, 138–140. Forton, D. M., Taylor-Robinson, S. D., &
Fabian, M. S., & Parsons, O. A. (1983). Differential Thomas, H. C. (2006). Central nervous system
improvement of cognitive functions in recovering changes in hepatitis C virus infection. European
alcoholic women. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Journal of Gastroenterology and Hepatology, 18(4),
92, 87–95. 333–338.
Fabian, M. S., Parsons, O. A., & Sheldon, M. D. Forton, D. M., Thomas, H. C., Murphy, C. A.,
(1984). Effects of gender and alcoholism on verbal Allsop, J. M., Foster, G. R., Main, J., et al. (2002).
and visual–spatial learning. Journal of Nervous Hepatitis C and cognitive impairment in a cohort
and Mental Disease, 172, 16–20. of patients with mild liver disease. Hepatology,
Fals-Stewart, W., & Schafer, J. (1992). The rela- 35(2), 433–439.
tionship between length of stay in drug-free Franceschi, M., Truci, G., Comi, G., Lozza, L.,
therapeutic communities and neurocognitive Marchettini, P., Galardi, G., et al. (1984). Cognitive
functioning. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 48, deficits and their relationship to other neurologi-
539–543. cal complications in chronic alcoholic patients.
Farmer, R. H. (1973). Functional changes dur- Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry,
ing early weeks of abstinence, measured by the 47, 1134–1137.
The Neurobehavioral Correlates of Alcoholism 441

Freund, G., & Ballinger, W. E., Jr. (1988). Loss of Gillin, J. C., Smith, T. L., Irwin, M., Kripke, D. F.,
cholinergic muscarinic receptors in the frontal Brown, S., & Schuckit, M. (1990a). Short REM
cortex of alcohol abusers. Alcoholism: Clinical and latency in primary alcoholic patients with second-
Experimental Research, 12, 630–638. ary depression. American Journal of Psychiatry,
Freund, G., & Ballinger, W. E., Jr. (1989). 147, 106–109.
Neuroreceptor changes in the putamen of alcohol Gillin, J. C., Smith, T. L., Irwin, M., Kripke, D. F.,
abusers. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental & Schuckit, M. (1990b). EEG sleep studies in
Research, 13, 213–218. “pure” primary alcoholism during subacute with-
Freund, G., & Ballinger, W. E., Jr. (1991). Loss of syn- drawal: Relationships to normal controls, age, and
aptic receptors can precede morphologic changes other clinical variables. Biological Psychiatry, 27,
induced by alcoholism. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 477–488.
1(Suppl.), 385–391. Gilman, S., Adams, K., Koeppe, R. A., Berent, S.,
Fuller, R. K., Lee, K. K., & Gordis, E. (1988). Validity Kluin, K. J., Modell, J. G., et al. (1990). Cerebellar
of self-report in alcoholism research: Results of and frontal hypometabolism in alcoholic cerebel-
a Veterans Administration Cooperative Study. lar degeneration studied with positron emission
Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, tomography. Annals of Neurology, 28, 775–785.
12, 201–205. Glenn, S. W., Errico, A. L., Parsons, O. A., King,
Gazdzinski, S., Durazzo, T. C., & Meyerhoff, D. J. A. C., & Nixon, S. J. (1993). The role of antisocial,
(2005). Temporal dynamics and determinants of affective, and childhood behavioral characteristics
whole brain tissue volume changes during recov- in alcoholics’ neuropsychological performance.
ery from alcohol dependence. Drug and Alcohol Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research,
Dependence, 78(3), 263–273. 17, 162–169.
Gallucci, M., Amicarelli, I., Rossi, A., Stratta, P., Glenn, S. W., & Parsons, O. A. (1990). The role of time
Masciocchi, C., Zobel, B. B., et al. (1989). MR in neuropsychological performance: Investigation
imaging of white matter lesions in uncomplicated and application in an alcoholic population. The
chronic alcoholism. Journal of Computer Assisted Clinical Neuropsychologist, 4, 344–354.
Tomography, 13, 395–398. Glenn, S. W., & Parsons, O. A. (1991a). Impaired effi-
Gardner, M. K., Clark, E., Bowman, M. A., & ciency in female alcoholics’ neuropsychological
Miller, P. J. (1989). Analogical reasoning abilities performance. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
of recovering alcoholics. Alcoholism: Clinical and Neuropsychology, 13, 895–908.
Experimental Research, 13, 508–511. Glenn, S. W., & Parsons, O. A. (1991b). Prediction
Garland, M. A., Parsons, O. A., & Nixon, S. J. (1993). of resumption of drinking in posttreatment alco-
Visual–spatial learning in nonalcoholic young holics. International Journal of the Addictions, 26,
adults with and those without a family history 237–254.
of alcoholism. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 54, Glenn, S. W., & Parsons, O. A. (1992).
219–224. Neuropsychological efficiency measures in male
Gebhardt, C. A., Naeser, M. A., & Butters, N. (1984). and female alcoholics. Journal of Studies on
Computerized measures of CT scans of alcohol- Alcohol, 53, 546–552.
ics: Thalamic region related to memory. Alcohol, Glenn, S. W., Parsons, O. A., Sinha, R., & Stevens, L.
1, 133–140. (1988). The effects of repeated withdrawals from
Gechter, G. L. (1987). Changes in neuropsychologi- alcohol on the memory of male and female alco-
cal functions among detoxifying and recovering holics. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 23, 337–342.
alcoholics as measured by the Luria Nebraska Glosser, G., Butters, N., & Kaplan, E. (1977).
Neuropsychological Battery. Dissertation Abstracts Visuoperceptual processes in brain damaged
International, 48, 262–B. patients on the digit symbol substitution test.
George, D. T., Lindquist, T., Rawlings, R. R., International Journal of Neuroscience, 7, 59–66.
Eckardt, M. J., Moss, H., Mathis, C., et al. Golden, C. J., Graber, B., Blose, I., Berg, R., Coff man, J.,
(1992). Pharmacologic maintenance of absti- & Bloch, S. (1981). Difference in brain densities
nence in patients with alcoholism: No efficacy between chronic alcoholic and normal control
of 5-hydroxytryptophan or levodopa. Clinical patients. Science, 211, 508–510.
Pharmacology Therapy, 52, 553–560. Goldman, M. S. (1983). Cognitive impairment in
Gillen, R., & Hesselbrock, V. (1992). Cognitive func- chronic alcoholics: Some cause for optimism.
tioning, ASP, and family history of alcoholism American Psychologist, 38, 1045–1054.
in young men at risk for alcoholism. Alcoholism: Goldman, M. S. (1990). Experience-dependent neu-
Clinical and Experimental Research, 16, 206–214. ropsychological recovery and the treatment of
442 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

chronic alcoholism. Neuropsychology Review, 1, neuropsychological study of polydrug users.


75–101. Archives of General Psychiatry, 35, 1063–1074.
Goldman, M. S., Klisz, D. K., & Williams, D. L. Grant, I., Adams, K., & Reed, R. (1979a). Normal
(1985). Experience-dependent recovery of cogni- neuropsychological abilities of alcoholic men in
tive functioning in young alcoholics. Addictive their late thirties. American Journal of Psychiatry,
Behaviors, 10, 169–176. 136, 1263–1269.
Goldman, M. S., Williams, D. L., & Klisz, D. K. Grant, I., Adams, K. M., & Reed, R. (1984). Aging,
(1983). Recoverability of psychological func- abstinence, and medical risk factors in the pre-
tioning following alcohol abuse: Prolonged diction of neuropsychologic deficit among long-
visual–spatial dysfunction in older alcoholics. term alcoholics. Archives of General Psychiatry, 41,
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 710–718.
51, 370–378. Grant, I., Prigatano, G. P., Heaton, R. K., McSweeny,
Goldman, R. S., & Goldman, M. S. (1988). Experience- A. J., Wright, E. C., & Adams, K. M. (1987a).
dependent cognitive recovery in alcoholics: A task Progressive neuropsychologic impairment and
component strategy. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, hypoxemia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 44,
49, 142–148. 999–1006.
Goldstein, G., & Shelly, C. (1980). Neuropsychological Grant, I., Reed, R., & Adams, K. M. (1987b). Diagnosis
investigation of brain lesion localization in alco- of intermediate-duration and subacute organic
holism. Advances in Experimental Medicine and mental disorders in abstinent alcoholics. Journal
Biology, 126, 731–743. of Clinical Psychiatry, 48, 319–323.
Goldstein, G., & Shelly, C. (1982). A multivari- Grant, I., Reed, R., Adams, K., & Carlin, A. (1979b).
ate neuropsychological approach to brain lesion Neuropsychological function in young alcohol-
localization in alcoholism. Addictive Behaviors, 7, ics and polydrug abusers. Journal of Clinical
165–175. Neuropsychology, 1, 39–47.
Goldstein, G., Shelly, C., Mascia, G. V., & Tarter, R. E. Gregson, R. A., & Taylor, G. M. (1977). Prediction
(1985). Relationships between neuropsychological of relapse in men alcoholics. Journal of Studies on
and psychopathological dimensions in male alco- Alcohol, 38, 1749–1760.
holics. Addictive Behaviors, 10, 365–372. Gur, R. C., Gur, R. E., Obrist, W. D., Skolnick, B. E.,
Goldstein, G., Tarter, R. E., Shelly, C., Alterman, A. & Reivich, M. (1987). Age and regional cerebral
I., & Petrarulo, E. (1983). Withdrawal seizures in blood flow at rest and during cognitive activity.
black and white alcoholic patients: Intellectual and Archives of General Psychiatry, 44, 617–621.
neuropsychological sequelae. Drug and Alcohol Guthrie, A., & Elliott, W. A. (1980). The nature and
Dependence, 12, 349–354. reversibility of cerebral impairment in alcohol-
Goodwin, D. W. (1983). Familial alcoholism: A ism: Treatment implications. Journal of Studies on
separate entity? Substance and Alcohol Actions/ Alcohol, 41, 147–155.
Misuse, 4, 129–136. Hannon, R., Butler, C. P., Day, C. L., Khan, S. A.,
Gorenstein, E. E. (1982). Frontal lobe functions in Quitoriano, L. A., Butler, A. M., et al. (1985).
psychopaths. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 91, Alcohol use and cognitive functioning in men and
368–379. women college students. Recent Developments in
Gorenstein, E. E. (1987). Cognitive-perceptual defi- Alcoholism, 3, 241–252.
cit in an alcoholism spectrum disorder. Journal of Hannon, R., Butler, C. P., Day, C. L., Khan, S. A.,
Studies on Alcohol, 48, 310–318. Quitoriano, L. A., Butler, A. M., et al. (1987).
Grant, B. F., Dufour, M. C., & Harford, T. C. (1988). Social drinking and cognitive functioning in col-
Epidemiology of alcoholic liver disease. Seminars lege students: A replication and reversibility study.
in Liver Disease, 8, 12–25. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 48, 502–506.
Grant, I. (1987). Alcohol and the brain: Hannon, R., Day, C. L., Butler, A. M., Larson, A. J., &
Neuropsychological correlates. Journal of Casey, M. (1983). Alcohol consumption and cog-
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 310–324. nitive functioning in college students. Journal of
Grant, I., Adams, K. M., Carlin, A. S., & Studies on Alcohol, 44, 283–298.
Rennick, P. M. (1977). Neuropsychological def- Hansen, L. A., Natelson, B. H., Lemere, C., Niemann,
icit in polydrug users: A preliminary report of W., De Teresa, R., Regan, T. J., et al. (1991).
the findings of the collaborative neuropsycho- Alcohol-induced brain changes in dogs. Archives
logical study of polydrug users. Drug and Alcohol of Neurology, 48, 939–942.
Dependence, 2, 91–108. Harper, C. (1998). The neuropathology of alcohol-
Grant, I., Adams, K. M., Carlin, A. S., Rennick, P. M., specific brain damage, or does alcohol dam-
Judd, L. L., & Schooff, K. (1978). The collaborative
The Neurobehavioral Correlates of Alcoholism 443

age the brain? Journal of Neuropathology and Haug, J. O. (1968). Pneumoencephalographic evi-
Experimental Neuroogyl, 57(2), 101–110. dence of brain damage in chronic alcoholics: A pre-
Harper C. (2007). The neurotoxicity of alco- liminary report. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica,
hol. Human & Experimental Toxicology, 26(3), 203, 135–143.
251–257. Heaton, R. K. (1992). Comprehensive norms for an
Harper, C. , & Blumbergs, P. C. (1982). Brain weights expanded Halstead–-Reitan Battery: A supplement
in alcoholics. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised.
and Psychiatry, 45, 838–840. Odessa, Florida: Psychological Assessment
Harper, C., & Corbett, D. (1990). Changes in the Resources.
basal dendrites of cortical pyramidal cells from Heaton, R. K., Grant, I., & Matthews, C. G. (1991).
alcoholic patients: A quantitative Golgi study. Comprehensive norms for an expanded Halstead–
Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, Reitan Battery: Demographic corrections, research
53, 856–861. findings, and clinical applications. Odessa, Florida:
Harper, C., Dixon, G., Sheedy, D., & Garrick, T. Psychological Assessment Resources.
(2003). Neuropathological alterations in alco- Hesselbrock, M. N., Weidenman, M. A., & Reed, H.
holic brains. Studies arising from the New B. (1985a). Effect of age, sex, drinking history and
South Wales Tissue Resource Centre. Progress antisocial personality on neuropsychology of alco-
in Neuropsychopharmacology and Biological holics. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 46, 313–320.
Psychiatry, 27(6), 951–961. Hesselbrock, V., Bauer, L. O., Hesselbrock, M. N.,
Harper, C., & Kril, J. (1985). Brain atrophy in chronic & Gillen, R. (1991). Neuropsychological factors
alcoholic patients: A quantitative pathological in individuals at high risk for alcoholism. Recent
study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Developments in Alcoholism, 9, 21–40.
Psychiatry, 48, 211–217. Hesselbrock, V. M., Stabenau, J. R., & Hesselbrock,
Harper, C., & Kril, J. J. (1988). Corpus callosal thick- M. N. (1985b). Minimal brain dysfunction and
ness in alcoholics. British Journal of Addiction, 83, neuropsychological test performance in offspring
577–580. of alcoholics. Recent Developments in Alcoholism,
Harper, C. G., & Kril, J. (1989). Patterns of neuro- 3, 65–82.
nal loss in the cerebral cortex in chronic alcoholic Hester, R. K., Smith, J. W., & Jackson, T. R. (1980).
patients. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 92, Recovery of cognitive skills in alcoholics. Journal
81–89. of Studies on Alcohol, 41, 363–367.
Harper, C., & Kril, J. J. (1990). Neuropathology Hightower, M. G., & Anderson, R. P. (1986). Memory
of alcoholism. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 25, evaluation of alcoholics with Russell’s revised
207–216. Wechsler Memory Scale. Journal of Clinical
Harper, C., Kril, J., & Daly, J. (1987). Are we drink- Psychology, 42, 1000–1005.
ing our neurones away? British Medical Journal Hildebrandt, H., Brokate, B., Eling, P., & Lanz, M.
(Clinical Research Edition), 294, 534–536. (2004). Response shift ing and inhibition, but not
Harper, C., Kril, J., & Daly, J. (1988). Does a “mod- working memory, are impaired after long-term
erate” alcohol intake damage the brain? Journal heavy alcohol consumption. Neuropsychology,
of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 51, 18(2), 203–211.
909–913. Hill, S. Y., & Ryan, C. (1985). Brain damage in
Harper, C., Kril, J. J., & Holloway, R. L. (1985). Brain social drinkers? Reasons for caution. Recent
shrinkage in chronic alcoholics: A pathological Developments in Alcoholism, 3, 277–288.
study. British Medical Journal, 290, 501–504. Hillbom, M., & Holm, L. (1986). Contribution of trau-
Harper, C., & Matsumoto, I. (2005). Ethanol and matic head injury to neuropsychological deficits in
brain damage. Current Opinion in Pharmacology, alcoholics. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and
5(1), 73–78. Psychiatry, 49, 1348–1353.
Harper, C., Smith, N. A., & Kril, J. J. (1990). The Hilsabeck, R. C., Hassanein, T. I., Carlson, M. D.,
effects of alcohol on the female brain: A neuro- Ziegler, E. A., & Perry, W. (2003). Cognitive func-
pathological study. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 25, tioning and psychiatric symptomatology in patients
445–448. with chronic hepatitis C. Journal of the International
Hata, T., Meyer, J. S., Tanahashi, N., Ishikawa, Y., Imai, Neuropsychological Society, 9(6), 847–854.
A., Shinohara, T., et al. (1987). Three-dimensional Hochla, N. A. N., & Parsons, O. A. (1982). Premature
mapping of local cerebral perfusion in alcoholic aging in female alcoholics: A neuropsychological
encephalopathy with and without Wernicke– study. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 170,
Korsakoff syndrome. Journal of Cerebral Blood 241–245.
Flow and Metabolism, 7, 35–44.
444 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Hoff man, J. J., Hall, R. W., & Bartsch, T. W. (1987). Jernigan, T. L., Butters, N., DiTraglia, G., Schafer,
On the relative importance of “psychopathic” per- K., Smith, T., Irwin, M., et al. (1991a). Reduced
sonality and alcoholism on neuropsychological cerebral grey matter observed in alcoholics
measures of frontal lobe dysfunction. Journal of using magnetic resonance imaging. Alcoholism:
Abnormal Psychology, 96, 158–160. Clinical and Experimental Research, 15,
Horvath, T. B. (1973). Clinical spectrum and epide- 418–427.
miological features of alcoholic dementia. In J. G. Jernigan, T. L., Schafer, K., Butters, N., & Cermak, L. S.
Rankin (Ed.), Alcohol, drugs and brain damage (1991b). Magnetic resonance imaging of alcoholic
(pp. 1–16). Toronto, Ontario: Addiction Research Korsakoff patients. Neuropsychopharmacology, 4,
Foundation. 175–186.
Inglis, J., & Lawson, J. S. (1981). Sex differences in the Jernigan, T. L., Zatz, L. M., Ahumada, A. J., Jr.,
effects of unilateral brain damage on intelligence. Pfefferbaum, A., Tinklenberg, J. R., & Moses, J.
Science, 212, 693–695. A., Jr. (1982). CT measures of cerebrospinal fluid
Irwin, M., Smith, T. L., Butters, N., Brown, S., Baird, volume in alcoholics and normal volunteers.
S., Grant, I., et al. (1989). Graded neuropsycholog- Psychiatry Research, 7, 9–17.
ical impairment and elevated gamma-glutamyl Johnson, J. L., & Rolf, J. E. (1988). Cognitive func-
transferase in chronic alcoholic men. Alcoholism: tioning in children from alcoholic and non-
Clinical and Experimental Research, 13, 99–103. alcoholic families. British Journal of Addiction, 83,
Ishibashi, M., Nakazawa, Y., Yokoyama, T., Koga, Y., 849–857.
Miyahara, Y., Hayashida, N., et al. (1987). Cerebral Jones, B., & Parsons, O. A. (1971). Impaired abstract-
atrophy and slow wave sleep of abstinent chronic ing ability in chronic alcoholics. Archives of
alcoholics. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 19, General Psychiatry, 24, 71–75.
325–332. Jones, B., & Parsons, O. A. (1972). Specific vs gen-
Ishikawa, Y., Meyer, J. S., Tanahashi, N., Hata, T., eralized deficits of abstracting ability in chronic
Velez, M., Fann, W. E., et al. (1986). Abstinence alcoholics. Archives of General Psychiatry, 26,
improves cerebral perfusion and brain volume 380–384.
in alcoholic neurotoxicity without Wernicke– Jones, M. K., & Jones, B. M. (1980). The relationship
Korsakoff syndrome. Journal of Cerebral Blood of age and drinking history to the effects of alco-
Flow and Metabolism, 6, 86–94. hol on memory in women. Journal of Studies on
Jacobson, R. (1986a). The contributions of sex and Alcohol, 41, 179–186.
drinking history to the CT brain scan changes in Joyce, E. M., & Robbins, T. W. (1991). Frontal
alcoholics. Psychological Medicine, 16, 547–559. lobe function in Korsakoff and non-Korsakoff
Jacobson, R. (1986b). Female alcoholics: A controlled alcoholics: Planning and spatial working memory.
CT brain scan and clinical study. British Journal of Neuropsychologia, 29, 709–723.
Addiction, 81, 661–669. Kaseda, Y., Miyazato, Y., Ogura, C., Nakamoto, H.,
Jacobson, R. R., Acker, C. F., & Lishman, W. A. (1990). Uema, T., Yamamoto, K., et al. (1994). Correlation
Patterns of neuropsychological deficit in alcoholic between event-related potentials and MR measure-
Korsakoff ’s syndrome. Psychological Medicine, 20, ments in chronic alcoholic patients. The Japanese
321–334. Journal of Psychiatry and Neurology, 48, 23–32.
Jacobson, R. R., & Lishman, W. A. (1990). Cortical King, M. A., Hunter, B. E., & Walker, D. W. (1988).
and diencephalic lesions in Korsakoff ’s syndrome: Alterations and recovery of dendritic spine den-
A clinical and CT scan study. Psychological sity in rat hippocampus following long-term etha-
Medicine, 20, 63–75. nol ingestion. Brain Research, 459, 381–385.
Janowsky, J. S., Shimamura, A. P., Kritchevsky, M., Kish, G. B., Hagen, J. M., Woody, M. M., & Harvey,
& Squire, L. R. (1989). Cognitive impairment fol- H. L. (1980). Alcoholics’ recovery from cerebral
lowing frontal lobe damage and its relevance to impairment as a function of duration of absti-
human amnesia. Behavioral Neuroscience, 103, nence. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 36, 584–589.
548–560. Kleinknecht, R. A., & Goldstein, S. G. (1972).
Jenkins, R. L., & Parsons, O. A. (1981). Neuropsychological deficits associated with alco-
Neuropsychological effect of chronic alcohol- holism: A review and discussion. Quarterly Journal
ism on tactual-spatial performance and memory of Studies on Alcohol, 33, 999–1019.
in males. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Klisz, D. K., & Parsons, O. A. (1977). Hypothesis
Research, 5, 26–33. testing in younger and older alcoholics. Journal of
Jensen, G. B., & Pakkenberg, B. (1993). Do alco- Studies on Alcohol, 38, 1718–1729.
holics drink their neurons away? Lancet, 342, Kodituwakku, P. W., Kalberg, W., & May, P. A.
1201–1204. (2001). The effects of prenatal alcohol exposure on
The Neurobehavioral Correlates of Alcoholism 445

executive functioning. Alcohol Research & Health, P. E. Nathan, and G. A. Marlatt (Eds.), Addictive
25, 192–198. behaviors: Prevention and early intervention (pp.
Kostandov, E. A., Arsumanov, Y. L., Genkina, O. A., 69–86). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Swets and
Restchikova, T. N., & Shostakovich, G. S. (1982). Zeitlinger.
The effects of alcohol on hemispheric functional Løberg, T., & Miller, W. R. (1986). Personality, cogni-
asymmetry. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 43, tive, and neuropsychological correlates of harmful
411–426. alcohol consumption: A cross-national compar-
Kramer, J. H., Blusewicz, M. J., & Preston, K. A. ison of clinical samples. Annals of the New York
(1989). The premature aging hypothesis: Old Academy of Sciences, 472, 75–97.
before its time? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Long, J. A., & McLachlan, J. F. C. (1974). Abstract rea-
Psychology, 57, 257–262. soning and perceptual–motor efficiency in alco-
Kril, J. J., & Harper, C. G. (1989). Neuronal counts holics: Impairment and reversibility. Quarterly
from four cortical regions of alcoholic brains. Acta Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 35, 1220–1229.
Neuropathologica, 79, 200–204. Ma, X., Coles, C. D., Lynch, M. E., LaConte, S. M.,
Lange, R. T., Iverson, G. L., & Franzen, M. D. (2007). Zurkiya, O., Wang, D., et al. (2005). Evaluation of
Short-term neuropsychological outcome follow- corpus callosum anisotropy in young adults with
ing uncomplicated mild TBI: effects of day-of- fetal alcohol syndrome according to diff usion ten-
injury intoxication and pre-injury alcohol abuse. sor imaging. Alcoholism: Clinical & Experimental
Neuropsychology, 21(5), 590–598. Research, 29(7), 1214–1222.
Leber, W. R., Jenkins, R. L., & Parsons, O. A. (1981). Macciocchi, S. N., Ranseen, J. D., & Schmitt, F.
Recovery of visual–spatial learning and memory in A. (1989). The relationship between neuropsy-
chronic alcoholics. Journal of Clinical Psychology, chological impairment in alcoholics and treat-
37, 192–197. ment outcome at one year. Archives of Clinical
Leber, W. R., Parsons, O. A., & Nichols, N. (1985). Neuropsychology, 4, 365–370.
Neuropsychological test results are related to rat- MacVane, J., Butters, N., Montgomery, K., & Farber,
ings of men alcoholics’ therapeutic progress: A J. (1982). Cognitive functioning in men social
replicated study. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 46, drinkers: A replication study. Journal of Studies on
116–121. Alcohol, 43, 81–95.
Letendre, S. L., Cherner, M., Ellis, R. J., Marquie- Malloy, P., Noel, N., Longabaugh, R., & Beattie,
Beck, J., Gragg, B., Marcotte, T., et al. (2006). The M. (1990). Determinants of neuropsychologi-
effects of hepatitis C, HIV, and methamphetamine cal impairment in antisocial substance abusers.
dependence on neuropsychological performance: Addictive Behaviors, 15, 431–438.
biological correlates of disease. AIDS, 19(Suppl 3), Malloy, P., Noel, N., Rogers, S., Longabaugh, R., &
S72–S78. Beattie, M. (1989). Risk factors for neuropsycho-
Letendre, S., Paulino, A. D., Rockenstein, E., logical impairment in alcoholics: Antisocial per-
Adame, A., Crews, L., Cherner, M., et al. (2007). sonality, age, years of drinking and gender. Journal
Pathogenesis of hepatitis C virus coinfection in of Studies on Alcohol, 50, 422–426.
the brains of patients infected with HIV. Journal Mann, K., Batra, A., Gunthner, A., & Schroth, G.
of Infectious Diseases, 196(3), 361–370. (1992). Do women develop alcoholic brain dam-
Lewohl, J. M., Dodd, P. R., Mayfield, R. D., & Harris, age more readily than men? Alcoholism: Clinical
R. A. (2001). Application of DNA microarrays to and Experimental Research, 16, 1052–1056.
study human alcoholism. Journal of Biomedical Mann, K., Mundle, G., Langle, G., & Petersen, D.
Science, 8(1), 28–36. (1993). The reversibility of alcoholic brain damage
Løberg, T. (1980a). Alcohol misuse and neuropsy- is not due to rehydration: A CT study. Addiction,
chological deficits in men. Journal of Studies on 88, 649–653.
Alcohol, 41, 119–128. Mann, K., Opitz, H., Petersen, D., Schroth, G., &
Løberg, T. (1980b). Neuropsychological deficits in Heimann, H. (1989). Intracranial CSF volumetry
alcoholics: Lack of personality (MMPI) correlates. in alcoholics: studies with MRI and CT. Psychiatry
Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology, Research, 29, 277–279.
126, 797–808. Marchesi, C., De Risio, C., Campanini, G., Maggini,
Løberg, T. (1981). MMPI-based personality subtypes C., Piazza, P., Grassi, M., et al. (1992). TRH test
of alcoholics: Relationships to drinking history, in alcoholics: Relationship of the endocrine results
psychometrics and neuropsychological deficits. with neuroradiological and neuropsychological
Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 42, 766–782. findings. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 27, 531–537.
Løberg, T. (1989). The role of neuropsychology in Marchesi, C., De Risio, C., Campanini, G., Piazza,
secondary prevention. In T. Løberg, W. R. Miller, P., Grassi, M., Chiodera, P., et al. (1994). Cerebral
446 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

atrophy and plasma cortisol levels in alcoholics hippocampal pyramidal cell dendrites resulting
after short or a long period of abstinence. Progress from chronic ethanol consumption and with-
in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological drawal. The Journal of Comparative Neurology,
Psychiatry, 18, 519–535. 225, 111–118.
Martin, P. R., Adinoff, B., Weingartner, H., Melgaard, B., Henriksen, L., Ahlgren, P., Danielsen,
Mukherjee, A. B., & Eckardt, M. J. (1986). U. T., Sørensen, H., & Paulson, O. B. (1990).
Alcoholic organic brain disease: Nosology and Regional cerebral blood flow in chronic alcoholics
pathophysiologic mechanisms. Progress in Neuro- measured by single photon emission computer-
Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 10, ized tomography. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica,
147–164. 82, 87–93.
Martin, P. R., Eckardt, M. J., & Linnoila, M. (1989). Mercer, P. W., & Khavari, K. A. (1990). Are
Treatment of chronic organic mental disorders women drinking more like men? An empirical
associated with alcoholism. Recent Developments examination of the convergence hypothesis.
in Alcoholism, 7, 329–350. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research,
Mathew, R. J., & Wilson, W. H. (1991). Substance 14, 461–466.
abuse and cerebral blood flow. American Journal Miglioni, M., Buchtel, H. A., Campanini, T., & De
of Psychiatry, 148, 292–305. Risio, C. (1979). Cerebral hemispheric lateraliza-
Mathew, R. J., Wilson, W. H., & Tant, S. R. (1986). tion of cognitive deficits due to alcoholism. Journal
Determinants of resting regional cerebral blood of Nervous and Mental Disease, 167, 212–217.
flow in normal subjects. Biological Psychiatry, 21, Miller, R., King, M. A., Heaton, M. B., & Walker,
907–914. D. W. (2002). The effects of chronic ethanol con-
Mattson, S. N., Goodman, A. M., Caine, C., Delis, sumption on neurotrophins and their receptors
D. C., & Riley, E. P. (1999). Executive functioning in the rat hippocampus and basal forebrain. Brain
in children with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure. Research, 950(1–2), 137–147.
Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, Miller, W. R., & Orr, J. (1980). Nature and sequence of
23(11), 1808–1815. neuropsychological deficits in alcoholics. Journal
Mattson, S. N., Riley, E. P., Jernigan, T. L., Ehlers, of Studies on Alcohol, 41, 325–337.
C. L., Delis, D. C., Jones, K. L., et al. (1992). Fetal Miyazato, Y., & Ogura, C. (1993). Abnormalities in
alcohol syndrome: A case report of neuropsycho- event-related potentials: N100, N200 and P300
logical, MRI and EEG assessment of two children. topography in alcoholics. The Japanese Journal of
Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, Psychiatry and Neurology, 47, 853–862.
16, 1001–1003. Molina, J. A., Bermejo, F., del Ser, T., Jimenez-
Mattson, S. N., Jernigan, T. L., & Riley, E. P. (1994). Jimenez, F. J., Herranz, A., Fernandez-Calle, P.,
MRI and prenatal alcohol exposure. Alcohol et al. (1994). Alcoholic cognitive deterioration
Health & Research World, 18(1), 49–52. and nutritional deficiencies. Acta Neurologica
Mattson, S. N., & Riley, E. P. (1995). Prenatal expo- Scandinavica, 89, 384–390.
sure to alcohol: What the images reveal. Alcohol Moss, H. B., Kirisci, L., Gordon, H. W., & Tarter, R. E.
Health & Research World, 19(4), 273–277. (1994). A neuropsychologic profi le of adolescent
Mattson, S. N., Riley, E. P., Gramling, L., Delis, D. alcoholics. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental
C., & Jones, K. L. (1998). Neuropsychological com- Research, 18(1), 159–163.
parison of alcohol-exposed children with or with- Muuronen, A., Bergman, H., Hindmarsh, T., &
out physical features of fetal alcohol syndrome. Telakivi, T. (1989). Influence of improved drink-
Neuropsychology, 12(1), 146–153. ing habits on brain atrophy and cognitive perfor-
McCrady, B. S., & Smith, D. E. (1986). Implications mance in alcoholic patients: A 5-year follow-up
of cognitive impairment for the treatment of alco- study. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental
holism. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 13, 137–141.
Research, 10, 145–149. Nanson, J. L., & Hiscock, M. (1990). Attention defi-
McIntyre, B. M. (1987). Functional neuropsycho- cits in children exposed to alcohol prenatally.
logical impairment and recovery in alcoholics. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research,
Dissertation Abstracts International, 47, 4658–B. 14, 656–661.
McLachlan, J. F. C., & Levinson, T. (1974). Neundorfer, B., Claus, D., & Burkowski, H. (1984).
Improvement in WAIS block design performance Neurological complications of chronic alcoholism.
as a function of recovery from alcoholism. Journal Wiener Klinische Wochenschrift, 96, 576–580.
of Clinical Psychology, 30, 65–66. Nicolas, J. M., Catafau, A. M., Estruch, R., Lomena, F. J.,
McMullen, P. A., Saint-Cyr, J. A., & Carlen, P. L. Salamero, M., Herranz, R., et al. (1993). Regional
(1984). Morphological alterations in rat CA1 cerebral blood flow-SPECT in chronic alcoholism:
The Neurobehavioral Correlates of Alcoholism 447

Relation to neuropsychological testing. Journal of findings. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 40,


Nuclear Medicine, 34, 1452–1459. 397–398.
Nixon, K., & Crews, F. T. (2004). Temporally spe- O’Leary, M. R., Radford, L. M., Chaney, E. F., &
cific burst in cell proliferation increases hippo- Schau, E. J. (1977b). Assessment of cognitive recov-
campal neurogenesis in protracted abstinence ery in alcoholics by use of the Trail-Making Test.
from alcohol. Journal of Neuroscience, 24(43), Journal of Clinical Psychology, 33, 579–582.
9714–9722. O’Neill, J., Cardenas, V. A., & Meyerhoff, D. J. (2001).
Nixon, S. J., Errico, A. L., Parsons, O. A., Leber, W. R., Effects of abstinence on the brain: quantitative
& Kelley, C. J. (1992). The role of instructional magnetic resonance imaging and magnetic res-
set on alcoholic performance. Alcoholism: Clinical onance spectroscopic imaging in chronic alco-
and Experimental Research, 16, 949–954. hol abuse. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental
Nixon, S. J., Kujawski, A., Parsons, O. A., & Yohman, Research, 25(11), 1673–1682.
J. R. (1987). Semantic (verbal) and figural mem- Ogura, C., & Miyazato, Y. (1991). Cognitive dysfunc-
ory impairment in alcoholics. Journal of Clin tions of alcohol dependence using event related
ical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 9, potentials. Japanese Journal of Alcohol and Drug
311–322. Dependence, 26, 331–340.
Nixon, S. J., & Parsons, O. A. (1991). Alcohol-related Oscar-Berman, M. (1987). Alcohol-related ERP
efficiency deficits using an ecologically valid test. changes in cognition. Alcohol, 4, 289–292.
Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, Ozkaragoz, T. Z., & Noble, E. P. (1995).
15, 601–606. Neuropsychological differences between sons of
Nixon, S. J., Parsons, O. A., Schaeffer, K. W., & Hale, R. L. active alcoholic and non-alcoholic fathers. Alcohol
(1988). Subject selection biases in alcoholic sam- and Alcoholism, 30, 115–123.
ples: Effects on cognitive performance. Journal of Page, R. D., & Linden, J. D. (1974). “Reversible”
Clinical Psychology, 44, 831–836. organic brain syndrome in alcoholics: A psycho-
Noble, E. P., Berman, S. M., Ozkaragoz, T. Z., & metric evaluation. Quarterly Journal of Studies on
Ritchie, T. (1994). Prolonged P300 latency in Alcohol, 35, 98–107.
children with the D2 dopamine receptor A1 Page, R. D., & Schaub, L. H. (1977). Intellectual
allele. American Journal of Human Genetics, 54, functioning in alcoholics during six months’
658–668. abstinence. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 38,
Noël, X., Bechara, A., Dan, B., Hanak, C., & Verbanck, 1240–1246.
P. (2007). Response inhibition deficit is involved in Parker, D. A., Parker, E. S., Brody, J. A., & Schoenberg, R.
poor decision making under risk in nonamnesic (1983). Alcohol use and cognitive loss among
individuals with alcoholism. Neuropsychology, employed men and women. American Journal of
21(6), 778–786. Public Health, 73, 521–526.
Noonberg, A., Goldstein, G., & Page, H. A. (1985). Parker, E. S. (1982). Comments on “cognitive func-
Premature aging in male alcoholics: “Accelerated tioning in men social drinkers; A replication
aging” or “increased vulnerability”? Alcoholism: study.” Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 43, 170–177.
Clinical and Experimental Research, 9, 334–338. Parker, E. S., Birnbaum, I. M., Boyd, R. A., & Noble,
O’Connor, S., & Tasman, A. (1990). The applica- E. P. (1980). Neuropsychologic decrements as
tion of electrophysiology to research in alco- a function of alcohol intake in male students.
holism. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research,
Neuroscience, 2, 149–158. 4, 330–334.
Ökvist, A., Johansson, S., Kuzmin, A., Bazov, I., Parker, E. S., & Noble, E. P. (1977). Alcohol consump-
Merino-Martinez, R., Ponomarev, I., et al. (2007). tion and cognitive functioning in social drinkers.
Neuroadaptations in human chronic alcoholics: Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 38, 1224–1232.
dysregulation of the NF-kappa B system. PLoS Parker, E. S., & Noble, E. P. (1980). Alcohol and the
ONE, 2(9), e930. aging process in social drinkers. Journal of Studies
O’Leary, M. R., Donovan, D. M., & Chaney, E. F. on Alcohol, 41, 170–178.
(1977a). The relationship of perceptual field ori- Parker, E. S., Parker, D. A., & Harford T. C. (1991).
entation to measures of cognitive functioning and Specifying the relationship between alcohol use
current adaptive abilities in alcoholics and nonal- and cognitive loss: The effects of frequency of con-
coholics. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, sumption and psychological distress. Journal of
165, 275–282. Studies on Alcohol, 52, 366–373.
O’Leary, M. R., Donovan, D. M., Chaney, E. F., & Parkin, A. J., Blunden, J., Rees, J. E., & Hunkin, N. M.
Walker, R. D. (1979). Cognitive impairment and (1991). Wernicke–Korsakoff syndrome of nonal-
treatment outcome with alcoholics: Preliminary coholic origin. Brain and Cognition, 15, 69–82.
448 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Parkin, A. J., Dunn, J. C., Lee, C., O’Hara, P. F., & alcoholics and animals. Alcohol and Alcoholism,
Nussbaum, L. (1993). Neuropsychological sequelae 21, 137–157.
of Wernicke’s encephalopathy in a 20-year-old Parsons, O. A., Tarter, R. E., & Edelberg, R. (1972).
woman: Selective impairment of a frontal memory Altered motor control in chronic alcoholics.
system. Brain and Cognition, 21, 1–19. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 80, 308–314.
Parsons, O. A. (1983). Cognitive dysfunction and Patterson, B. W., Parsons, O. A., Schaeffer, K. W., &
recovery in alcoholics. Substance and Alcohol Errico, A. L. (1988). Interpersonal problem solv-
Actions/Misuse, 4, 175–190. ing in alcoholics. Journal of Nervous and Mental
Parsons, O. A. (1986a). Alcoholics’ neuropsychologi- Disease, 176, 707–713.
cal impairment: Current findings and conclusions. Patterson, B. W., Sinha, R., Williams, H. L.,
Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 8, 13–19. Parsons, O. A., Smith, L. T., & Schaeffer, K. W.
Parsons, O. A. (1986b). Cognitive functioning in (1989). The relationship between neuropsycho-
sober social drinkers: A review and critique. logical and late component evoked potential mea-
Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 47, 101–114. sures in chronic alcoholics. International Journal
Parsons, O. A. (1987). Intellectual impairment of Neuroscience, 49, 319–327.
in alcoholics: Persistent issues. Acta Medica Patterson, B. W., Williams, H. L., McLean, G. A.,
Scandinavica, 717(Suppl.), 33–46. Smith, L. T., & Schaeffer, K. W. (1987). Alcoholism
Parsons, O. A. (1989). Impairment in sober alco- and family history of alcoholism: Effects on visual
holics’ cognitive functioning: The search for and auditory event-related potentials. Alcohol, 4,
determinants. In T. Løberg, W. R. Miller, P. E. 265–274.
Nathan, and G. A. Marlatt (Eds.), Addictive Pentney, R. J. (1991). Remodeling of neuronal den-
behaviors: Prevention and early intervention (pp. dritic networks with aging and alcohol. Alcohol
101–116). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Swets and and Alcoholism, 1(Suppl.), 393–397.
Zeitlinger. Pentney, R. J., Quackenbush, L. J., & O’Neill, M.
Parsons, O. A., & Fabian, M. S. (1982). Comments on (1989). Length changes in dendritic networks of
“Cognitive functioning in men social drinkers: A cerebellar purkinje cells of old rats after chronic
replication study.” Journal of Studies on Alcohol, ethanol treatment. Alcoholism: Clinical and
43, 178–182. Experimental Research, 13, 413–419.
Parsons, O. A., & Farr, S. P. (1981). The neuropsychol- Peterson, J. B., & Pihl, R. O. (1990). Information
ogy of alcohol and drug use. In S. B. Filskov and processing, neuropsychological function, and
T. S. Boll (Eds.), Handbook of clinical neuropsy- the inherited predisposition to alcoholism.
chology (pp. 320–365). New York: Wiley Press. Neuropsychology Review, 1, 343–369.
Parsons, O. A., & Leber, W. R. (1981). The relationship Pfefferbaum, A., Adalsteinsson, E., & Sullivan, E. V.
between cognitive dysfunction and brain dam- (2006). Dysmorphology and microstructural deg-
age in alcoholics: Causal, interactive, or epiphe- radation of the corpus callosum: Interaction of
nomenal? Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental age and alcoholism. Neurobiology of Aging, 27(7),
Research, 5, 326–343. 994–1009.
Parsons, O. A., & Nixon, S. J. (1993). Neurobehavioral Pfefferbaum, A., Ford, J. M., White, P. M., &
sequelae of alcoholism. Neurologic Clinics, 11, Mathalon, D. (1991). Event-related potentials in
205–218. alcoholic men: P3 amplitude reflects family his-
Parsons, O. A., & Prigatano, G. P. (1977). Memory tory but not alcohol consumption. Alcoholism:
functioning in alcoholics. In I. M. Birnbaum and Clinical and Experimental Research, 15, 839–850.
E. S. Parker (Eds.), Alcohol and human memory Pfefferbaum, A., Lim, K. O., Zipursky, R. B.,
(pp. 185–194). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Mathalon, D. H., Rosenbloom, M. J., Lane, B.,
Parsons, O. A., Schaeffer, K. W., & Glenn, S. W. et al. (1992). Brain gray and white matter volume
(1990a). Does neuropsychological test perfor- loss accelerates with aging in chronic alcoholics: A
mance predict resumption of drinking in post- quantitative MRI study. Alcoholism: Clinical and
treatment alcoholics? Addictive Behaviors, 15, Experimental Research, 16, 1078–1089.
297–307. Pfefferbaum, A., Rosenbloom, M. J., Adalsteinsson,
Parsons, O. A., Sinha, R., & Williams, H. L. (1990b). E., & Sullivan, E. V. (2007). Diff usion tensor imag-
Relationships between neuropsychological test ing with quantitative fibre tracking in HIV infec-
performance and event-related potentials in tion and alcoholism comorbidity: Synergistic
alcoholic and nonalcoholic samples. Alcoholism: white matter damage. Brain, 130(Pt 1), 48–64.
Clinical and Experimental Research, 14, 746–755. Pfefferbaum, A., Rosenbloom, M., Crusan, K., &
Parsons, O. A., & Stevens, L. (1986). Previous alcohol Jernigan, T. L. (1988). Brain CT changes in alco-
intake and residual cognitive deficits in detoxified holics: Effects of age and alcohol consumption.
The Neurobehavioral Correlates of Alcoholism 449

Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, Porjesz, B., & Begleiter, H. (1990). Event-related
12, 81–87. potentials in individuals at risk for alcoholism.
Pfefferbaum, A., & Sullivan, E. V. (2005). Disruption Alcohol, 7, 465–469.
of brain white matter microstructure by exces- Porjesz, B., & Begleiter, H. (1991). Neurophysiological
sive intracellular and extracellular fluid in alco- factors in individuals at risk for alcoholism. Recent
holism: evidence from diff usion tensor imaging. Developments in Alcoholism, 9, 53–67.
Neuropsychopharmacology, 30(2), 423–432. Porjesz, B., & Begleiter, H. (1993). Neurophysiological
Pfefferbaum, A., Sullivan, E. V., Hedehus, M., factors associated with alcoholism. In W. A. Hunt
Adalsteinsson, E., Lim, K. O., & Moseley, M. and S. J. Nixon (Eds.), Alcohol-induced brain dam-
(2000). In vivo detection and functional corre- age (pp. 89–120). Rockville, MD: National Institute
lates of white matter microstructural disruption on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism.
in chronic alcoholism. Alcoholism: Clinical and Porjesz, B., & Begleiter, H. (2003). Alcoholism and
Experimental Research, 24(8), 1214–1221. human electrophysiology. Alcohol Research and
Pfefferbaum, A., Sullivan, E. V., Mathalon, D. H., Health, 27, 153–160.
& Lim, K. O. (1997). Frontal lobe volume loss Portnoff, L. A. (1982). Halstead–Reitan impairment
observed with magnetic resonance imaging in in chronic alcoholics as a function of age of drink-
older chronic alcoholics. Alcoholism: Clinical and ing onset. Clinical Neuropsychology, 4, 115–119.
Experimental Research, 21(3), 521–529. Prange, M. E. (1988). Relapsed versus non-relapsed
Pfefferbaum, A., Sullivan, E. V., Rosenbloom M. J., alcohol abusers: Coping skills, stressful life events
Mathalon, D. H., & Lim, K. O. (1998). A controlled and neuropsychological functioning. Dissertation
study of cortical gray matter and ventricular Abstracts International, 49, 549–B.
changes in alcoholic men over a 5-year interval. Prescott, C. A., & Kendler, K. S. (1999). Age at first
Archives of General Psychiatry, 55(10), 905–912. drink and risk for alcoholism: A noncausal asso-
Phillips, D. E., Krueger, S. K., & Rydquist, J. E. ciation. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental
(1991). Short- and long-term effects of combined Research, 23(1), 101–107.
pre- and postnatal ethanol exposure (three tri- Prigatano, G. P. (1980). Neuropsychological func-
mester equivalency) on the development of mye- tioning of recidivist alcoholics treated with disul-
lin and axons in rat optic nerve. International firam: A follow-up report. International Journal of
Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 9(6), the Addictions, 15, 287–294.
631–647. Rangaswamy, M., Porjesz, B., Chorlian, D. B., Wang, K.,
Plant, M., Sullivan, F. M., Guerri, C., & Abel, E. L. Jones, K., Bauer, L. O., et al. (2002). Beta power in
(1993). Alcohol and pregnancy. In P. M. Verscuren the EEG of alcoholics. Biological Psychiatry, 52,
(Ed.), Health issues related to alcohol consumption 831–842.
(pp. 245–262). New York: ILSI Press. Rangaswamy, M., Porjesz, B., Chorlian, D. B., Choi,
Polich, J., & Bloom, F. E. (1986). P300 and alcohol K., Jones, K. A., Wang, K., et al. (2003). Theta power
consumption in normals and individuals at risk in the EEG of alcoholics. Alcoholism: Clinical and
for alcoholism: A preliminary report. Progress Experimental Research, 27, 607–615.
in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Rangaswamy, M., Porjesz, B., Chorlian, D. B., Wang,
Psychiatry, 10, 201–210. K., Jones, K., Kuperman, S., et al. (2004). Resting
Polich, J., & Bloom, F. E. (1988). Event-related brain EEG in offspring of male alcoholics: Beta frequen-
potentials in individuals at high and low risk cies. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 51,
for developing alcoholism: Failure to replicate. 239–251.
Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, Reed, R. J., & Grant, I. (1990). The long-term neu-
12, 368–373. robehavioral consequences of substance abuse:
Polich, J., Burns, T., & Bloom, F. E. (1988a). P300 Conceptual and methodological challenges for
and the risk for alcoholism: Family history, task future research. NIDA Research Monograph, 101,
difficulty, and gender. Alcoholism: Clinical and 10–56.
Experimental Research, 12, 248–254. Reed, R., Grant, I., & Adams, K. M. (1987). Family
Polich, J., Haier, R. J., Buchsbaum, M., & Bloom, F. E. history of alcoholism does not predict neuropsy-
(1988b). Assessment of young men at risk for alco- chological performance in alcoholics. Alcoholism:
holism with P300 from a visual discrimination Clinical and Experimental Research, 11, 340–344.
task. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 49, 186–190. Reed, R. J., Grant, I., & Rourke, S. B. (1992). Long-
Polich, J., Pollock, V. E., & Bloom, F. E. (1994). Meta- term abstinent alcoholics have normal memory.
analysis of P300 amplitude from males at risk for Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research,
alcoholism. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 55–73. 16, 677–683.
450 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Reitan, R. M. (1972). Verbal problem-solving as male long-term abstinent alcoholics. The Clinical
related to cerebral damage. Perceptual and Motor Neuropsychologist, 7, 343.
Skills, 34, 515–524. Rourke, S. B., & Grant, I. (1995). Neuropsychological
Rhodes, S. S., & Jasinski, D. R. (1990). Learning dis- deficits in male alcoholics: Part II. Using clinical
abilities in alcohol-dependent adults: A prelim- ratings to delineate the effects of interim drinking
inary study. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 23, and length of abstinence on the prevalence of defi-
551–556. cits. Journal of the International Neuropsychological
Richardson, E. D., Malloy, P. F., Longabaugh, R., Society, 1, 327.
Williams, J., Noel, N., & Beattie, M. C. (1991). Rourke S. B., & Grant, I. (1999). The interactive effects
Liver function tests and neuropsychologic impair- of age and length of abstinence on the recovery of
ment in substance abusers. Addictive Behaviors, neuropsychological functioning in chronic male
16, 51–55. alcoholics: A 2-year follow-up study. Journal of
Riley, E. P., & McGee, C. L. (2005). Fetal alcohol the International Neuropsychological Society, 5(3),
spectrum disorders: An overview with emphasis 234–246.
on changes in brain and behavior. Experimental Ruff, R. M., Marshall, L. F., Klauber, M. R., Blunt, B. A.,
Biology and Medicine, 230, 357–365. Grant, I., Foulkes, M. A., et al. (1990). Alcohol abuse
Riley, E. P., McGee, C. L., & Sowell, E. R. (2004). and neurological outcome of the severely head
Teratogenic effects of alcohol: A decade of brain injured. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation,
imaging. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 5, 21–31.
127C, 35–41. Russell, E. W. (1975). A multiple scoring method for
Riley, E. P., Mattson, S. N., Sowell, E. R., Jernigan, T. the assessment of complex memory functions.
L., Sobel, D. F., & Jones, K. L. (1995). Abnormalities Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43,
of the corpus callosum in children prenatally 800–809.
exposed to alcohol. Alcoholism: Clinical and Russell, M., Czarnecki, D. M., Cowan, R., McPherson,
Experimental Research, 19(5), 1198–1202. E., & Mudar, P. J. (1991). Measures of maternal
Roehrich, L., & Goldman, M. S. (1993). Experience- alcohol use as predictors of development in early
dependent neuropsychological recovery and the childhood. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental
treatment of alcoholism. Journal of Consulting and Research, 15, 991–1000.
Clinical Psychology, 61, 812–821. Ryan, C., & Butters, N. (1980a). Further evidence for
Rogers, R. L., Meyer, J. S., Shaw, T. G., & Mortel, K. a continuum-of-impairment encompassing male
F. (1983). Reductions in regional cerebral blood alcoholic Korsakoff patients and chronic alco-
flow associated with chronic consumption of alco- holic men. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental
hol. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 31, Research, 4, 190–198.
540–543. Ryan, C., & Butters, N. (1980b). Learning and
Romani, A., & Cosi, V. (1989). Event-related poten- memory impairments in young and old alcohol-
tials in chronic alcoholics during withdrawal ics: Evidence for the premature-aging hypothesis.
and abstinence. Neurophysiology Clinics, 19, Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research,
373–384. 4, 288–293.
Ron, M. A. (1983). The alcoholic brain: CT scan and Ryan, C., & Butters, N. (1984). Alcohol consumption
psychological findings. Psychological Medicine and premature aging: A critical review. Recent
Monograph, 3, 1–33. Developments in Alcoholism, 2, 223–250.
Ron, M. A. (1987). The brain of alcoholics: An over- Ryan, C., & Butters, N. (1986). The neuropsychology
view. In O. A. Parsons, N. Butters, and P. E. Nathan of alcoholism. In D. Wedding, A. Horton, and J.
(Eds.), Neuropsychology of alcoholism: Implications Webster (Eds.), The neuropsychology handbook
for diagnosis and treatment (pp, 11–20). New York: (pp. 376–409). New York: Springer-Publishing
Guilford Press. Co.
Ron, M. A., Acker, W., Shaw, G. K., & Lishman, W. A. Ryan, C., Butters, N., DiDario, B., & Adinolfi, A.
(1982). Computerized tomography of the brain in (1980). The relationship between abstinence and
chronic alcoholism: A survey and follow-up study. recovery of function in male alcoholics. Journal of
Brain, 105, 497–514. Clinical Neuropsychology, 2, 125–134.
Roth, T., Roehrs, T., & Rosenthal, L. (1995). Ryan, J. J., & Lewis, C. V. (1988). Comparison of nor-
Hypersomnolence and neurocognitive per- mal controls and recently detoxified alcoholics on
formance in sleep apnea. Current Opinion in the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised. The Clinical
Pulmonary Medicine, 1(6), 488–490. Neuropsychologist, 2, 173–180.
Rourke, S. B., Grant, I., Jernigan, T. L., & Reed, R. J. Ryback, R. S. (1971). The continuum and specific-
(1993). Negative computed tomography findings in ity of the effects of alcohol on memory: A review.
The Neurobehavioral Correlates of Alcoholism 451

Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 32, and nonalcoholics. Alcoholism: Clinical and
955–1016. Experimental Research, 8, 347–351.
Sachs, H., Russell, J. A. G., Christman, D. R., & Cook, Schafer, K., Butters, N., Smith, T., Irwin, M., Brown,
B. (1987). Alteration of regional cerebral glucose S., Hanger, P., et al. (1991). Cognitive perfor-
metabolic rate in non-Korsakoff chronic alcohol- mance of alcoholics: A longitudinal evalua-
ism. Archives of Neurology, 44, 1242–1251. tion of the role of drinking history, depression,
Saletu-Zyhlarz, G. M., Arnold, O., Anderer, P., liver function, nutrition, and family history.
Oberndorfer, S., Walter, H., Lesch, O. M., et al. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research,
(2004). Differences in brain function between 15, 653–660.
relapsing and abstaining alcohol-dependent Schau, E. J., O’Leary, M. R., & Chaney, E. F. (1980).
patients, evaluated by EEG mapping. Alcohol Reversibility of cognitive deficit in alcoholics.
Alcohol, 39, 233–240. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 41, 733–740.
Salmon, D. P., & Butters, N. (1987). The etiology Schottenbauer, M. A., Momenan, R., Kerick, M.,
and neuropathology of alcoholic Korsakoff ’s syn- & Hommer, D. W. (2007). Relationships among
drome: Some evidence for the role of the basal aging, IQ, and intracranial volume in alcohol-
forebrain. Recent Developments in Alcoholism, 5, ics and control subjects. Neuropsychology, 21(3),
27–58. 337–345.
Salmon, D. P., Butters, N., & Heindel, W. C. (1993). Schuckit, M. A. (1987). Biological vulnerability to
Alcoholic dementia and related disorders. In alcoholism. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
R. W. Parks, R. F. Zec, and R. S. Wilson (Eds.), Psychology, 55, 301–309.
Neuropsychology of Alzheimer’s disease and other Schuckit, M. A. (1994). A clinical model of genetic
dementias (pp. 186–209). New York: Oxford influences in alcohol dependence. Journal of
University Press. Studies on Alcohol, 55, 5–17.
Samson, Y., Baron, J. C., Feline, A., Bories, J., & Schuckit, M. A., Butters, N., Lyn, L., & Irwin, M.
Crouzel, C. (1986). Local cerebral glucose utili- (1987). Neuropsychologic deficits and the risk for
zation in chronic alcoholics: A positron tomo- alcoholism. Neuropsychopharmacology, 1, 45–53.
graphic study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery Schuckit, M. A., Irwin, M., & Brown, S. A. (1990).
and Psychiatry, 49, 1165–1170. The history of anxiety symptoms among 171 pri-
Sander, A. M., Nixon, S. J., & Parsons, O. A. (1989). mary alcoholics. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 51,
Pretest expectancies and cognitive impairment 34–41.
in alcoholics. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Schuckit, M. A., Irwin, M., & Smith, T. L. (1994).
Psychology, 57, 705–709. One-year incidence rate of major depression and
Schaeffer, K. W., & Parsons, O. A. (1986). Drinking other psychiatric disorders in 239 alcoholic men.
practices and neuropsychological test perfor- Addiction, 89, 441–445.
mance in sober male alcoholics and social drink- Schuckit, M. A., Smith, T. L., Anthenelli, R., & Irwin,
ers. Alcohol, 3, 175–179. M. (1993). Clinical course of alcoholism in 636
Schaeffer, K. W., & Parsons, O. A. (1987). Learning male inpatients. American Journal of Psychiatry,
impairment in alcoholics using an ecologically rel- 150, 786–792.
evant test. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, Schweinsburg, B. C., Alhassoon, O. M., Taylor, M. J.,
175, 213–218. Gonzalez, R., Videen, J. S., Brown, G. G., et al.
Schaeffer, K. W., & Parsons, O. A. (1988). Learning (2003). Effects of alcoholism and gender on brain
and memory test performance in alcoholics as a metabolism. The American Journal of Psychiatry,
function of monetary incentive. International 160, 1180–1183.
Journal of Neuroscience, 38, 311–319. Schweinsburg, B. C., Taylor, M. J., Alhassoon, O. M.,
Schaeffer, K. W., Parsons, O. A., & Errico, A. L. Dager, A. D., Tapert, S. F., Patterson, T. L., et al.
(1988). Abstracting deficits and childhood con- (2002). Spectroscopy and cognitive functioning
duct disorder as a function of familial alcoholism. in alcoholics with withdrawal seizures (abstract).
Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, Journal of the International Neuropsychological
12, 617–618. Society, 8 (2), 252.
Schaeffer, K. W., Parsons, O. A., & Errico, A. L. (1989). Schweinsburg, B. C., Taylor, M. J., Alhassoon, O. M.,
Performance deficits on tests of problem solving in Videen, J. S., Brown, G. G., Patterson, T. L., et al.
alcoholics: Cognitive or motivational impairment? (2001). Chemical pathology in brain white matter
Journal of Substance Abuse, 1, 381–392. of recently detoxified alcoholics: A 1H magnetic
Schaeffer, K. W., Parsons, O. A., & Yohman, J. R. resonance spectroscopy investigation of alcohol-
(1984). Neuropsychological differences between associated frontal lobe injury. Alcoholism, Clinical
male familial and nonfamilial alcoholics and Experimental Research, 25(6), 924–934.
452 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Sharp, J. R., Rosenbaum, G., Goldman, M. S., & Sowell, E. R., Mattson, S. N., Thompson, P. M.,
Whitman, R. D. (1977). Recoverability of psy- Jernigan, T. L., Riley, E. P., & Toga, A. W. (2001).
chological functioning following alcohol abuse: Mapping callosal morphology and cognitive cor-
Acquisition of meaningful synonyms. Journal relates: effects of heavy prenatal alcohol exposure.
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 45, Neurology, 57(2), 235–244.
1023–1028. Sparadeo, F. R., Zwick, W., & Butters, N. (1983).
Shaw, T. G., Mortel, K. F., Meyer, J. S., Rogers, R. L., Cognitive functioning of alcoholic females: An
Hardenberg, J., & Cutaia, M. M. (1984). Cerebral exploratory study. Drug and Alcohol Dependence,
blood flow changes in benign aging and cerebro- 12, 143–150.
vascular disease. Neurology, 34, 855–862. Streissguth, A. P., Barr, H. M., & Sampson, P. D.
Shear, P. K., Jernigan, T. L., & Butters, N. (1994). (1990). Moderate prenatal alcohol exposure:
Volumetric magnetic resonance imaging quanti- Effects on child IQ and learning problems at age
fication of longitudinal brain changes in abstinent 7 1/2 years. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental
alcoholics. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 14, 662–669.
Research, 18, 172–176. Streissguth, A. P., Bookstein, F. L., Sampson, P. D.,
Shelton, M. D., & Parsons, O. A. (1987). Alcoholics’ & Barr, H. M. (1989). Neurobehavioral effects of
self-assessment of their neuropsychological prenatal alcohol: Part III. PLS analyses of neurop-
functioning in everyday life. Journal of Clinical sychologic tests. Neurotoxicology and Teratology,
Psychology, 43, 395–403. 11, 493–507.
Sher, K. J., Walitzer, K. S., Wood, P. K., & Brent, E. E. Streissguth, A. P., Sampson, P. D., Olson, H. C.,
(1991). Characteristics of children of alcoholics: Bookstein, F. L., Barr, H. M., Scott, M., et al.
Putative risk factors, substance use and abuse, and (1994). Maternal drinking during pregnancy:
psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Attention and short-term memory in 14-year-
100, 427–448. old offspring—A longitudinal prospective study.
Sherer, M., Nixon, S. J., Parsons, O. A., & Adams, Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research,
R. L. (1992). Performance of alcoholic and brain- 18, 202–218.
damaged subjects on the Luria Memory Word Test. Stringer, A. Y., & Goldman, M. S. (1988). Experience-
Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 7, 499–504. dependent recovery of block design performance in
Shimamura, A. P., Jernigan, T. L., & Squire, L. R. male alcoholics: Strategy training versus unstruc-
(1988). Korsakoff ’s syndrome: Radiological (CT) tured practice. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 49,
findings and neuropsychological correlates. 406–411.
Journal of Neuroscience, 8, 4400–4410. Sullivan, E. V. (2003). Compromised pontocer-
Silberstein, J. A., & Parsons, O. A. (1979). ebellar and cerebellothalamocortical systems:
Neuropsychological impairment in female alco- Speculations on their contributions to cognitive
holics. In M. Galanter (Ed.), Currents in alcohol- and motor impairment in nonamnesic alcoholism.
ism (pp. 481–495). New York: Grune and Stratton. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research,
Silberstein, J. A., & Parsons, O. A. (1981). 27(9), 1409–1419.
Neuropsychological impairment in female alco- Sullivan, E. V., Fama, R., Rosenbloom, M. J., &
holics: Replication and extension. Journal of Pfefferbaum, A. (2002). A profi le of neuro-
Abnormal Psychology, 90, 179–182. psychological deficits in alcoholic women.
Sinha, R., Bernardy, N., & Parsons, O. A. (1992). Neuropsychology, 16(1), 74–83.
Long-term test–retest reliability of event-related Sullivan, E. V., Rosenbloom, M. J., & Pfefferbaum, A.
potentials in normals and alcoholics. Biological (2000). Pattern of motor and cognitive deficits in
Psychiatry, 32, 992–1003. detoxified alcoholic men. Alcoholism: Clinical and
Smith, J. W., & Layden, T. A. (1972). Changes in Experimental Research, 24(5), 611–621.
psychological performance and blood chemis- Sullivan, E. V., Rosenbloom, M. J., Serventi, K. L.,
try in alcoholics during and after hospital treat- Deshmukh, A., & Pfefferbaum, A. (2003). Effects
ment. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 33, of alcohol dependence comorbidity and antipsy-
379–394. chotic medication on volumes of the thalamus
Smith, K. J., & Eckardt, M. J. (1991). The effects of and pons in schizophrenia. American Journal of
prenatal alcohol on the central nervous system. Psychiatry, 160, 1110–1116.
Recent Developments in Alcoholism, 9, 151–164. Sutker, P. B., & Allain, A. N. (1987). Cognitive
Solomon, D. A., & Malloy, P. F. (1992). Alcohol, abstraction, shift ing, and control: Clinical sample
head injury, and neuropsychological function. comparisons of psychopaths and nonpsychopaths.
Neuropsychology Review, 3, 249–280. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96, 73–75.
The Neurobehavioral Correlates of Alcoholism 453

Tapert, S. F., & Brown, S. A. (1999). Neuropsychological in alcoholics with cirrhosis. Journal of Studies on
correlates of adolescent substance abuse: Four- Alcohol, 47, 74–77.
year outcomes. Journal of the International Tarter, R. E., Jacob, T., & Bremer, D. A. (1989a).
Neuropsychological Society, 5(6), 481–493. Cognitive status of sons of alcoholic men.
Tarter, R. E. (1973). An analysis of cognitive defi- Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research,
cits in chronic alcoholics. Journal of Nervous and 13, 232–235.
Mental Disease, 157, 138–147. Tarter, R. E., Jacob, T., & Bremer, D. L. (1989b). Specific
Tarter, R. E. (1975). Brain damage associated with cognitive impairment in sons of early onset alco-
chronic alcoholism. Diseases of the Nervous holics. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental
System, 36, 185–187. Research, 13, 786–789.
Tarter, R. E. (1980). Brain damage in chronic Tarter, R. E., & Jones, B. M. (1971). Motor impair-
alcoholics: A review of the psychological evi- ment in chronic alcoholics. Diseases of the Nervous
dence. In D. Richter (Ed.), Addiction and brain System, 32, 632–636.
damage (pp. 267–297). Baltimore: University Park Tarter, R. E., McBride, H., Buonpane, N., &
Press. Schneider, D. U. (1977). Differentiation of alco-
Tarter, R. E. (1982). Psychosocial history, mini- holics: Childhood history of minimal brain dys-
mal brain dysfunction and differential drinking function, family history, and drinking pattern.
patterns of male alcoholics. Journal of Clinical Archives of General Psychiatry, 34, 761–768.
Psychology, 38, 867–873. Tarter, R. E., & Parsons, O. A. (1971). Conceptual
Tarter, R. E. (1988). Are there inherited behav- shift ing in chronic alcoholics. Journal of Abnormal
ioral traits that predispose to substance abuse? Psychology, 77, 71–75.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, Tarter, R. E., & Van Thiel, D. H. (1985). Alcohol and
189–196. the brain: Chronic effects. New York: Plenum
Tarter, R. E., & Alterman, A. I. (1984). Press.
Neuropsychological deficits in alcoholics: Tarter, R. E., Van Thiel, D. H., Arria, A. M., Carra, J.,
Etiological considerations. Journal of Studies on & Moss, H. (1988). Impact of cirrhosis on the neu-
Alcohol, 45, 1–9. ropsychological test performance of alcoholics.
Tarter, R. E., & Edwards, K. L. (1986). Multifactorial Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research,
etiology of neuropsychological impairment in 12, 619–621.
alcoholics. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Templer, D. I., Ruff, C. F., & Simpson, K. (1975). Trail
Research, 10, 128–135. making test performance of alcoholics abstinent at
Tarter, R. E., Goldstein, G., Alterman, A., Petrarulo, least a year. International Journal of the Addictions,
E. W., & Elmore, S. (1983). Alcoholic seizures: 10, 609–612.
Intellectual and neuropsychological sequelae. Tivis, L. J., Parsons, O. A., Glenn, S. W., & Nixon,
Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 171, S. J. (1993). Differences in cognitive impairment
123–125. between VA and community treatment center
Tarter, R. E., Hays, A. L., Sandford, S. S., & Van Thiel, alcoholics. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 7,
D. H. (1986a). Cerebral morphological abnor- 43–51.
malities associated with non-alcoholic cirrhosis. Torvik, A. (1987). Brain lesions in alcoholics:
Lancet, 2, 893–895. Neuropathological observations. Acta Medica
Tarter, R. E., Hegedus, A. M., Goldstein, G., Shelly, Scandinavica, 717(Suppl.), 47–54.
C., & Alterman, A. I. (1984). Adolescent sons Torvik, A., Lindboe, C. F., & Rogde, S. (1982).
of alcoholics: Neuropsychological and person- Brain lesions in alcoholics: A neuropathological
ality characteristics. Alcoholism: Clinical and study with clinical correlations. Journal of the
Experimental Research, 8, 216–222. Neurological Sciences, 56, 233–248.
Tarter, R. E., Hegedus, A. M., Van Thiel, D. Trivedi, S., & Raghavan, R. (1989). Cognitive func-
H., Edwards, N., & Schade, R. R. (1987). tioning of alcoholics and its relationship with
Neurobehavioral correlates of cholestatic and prognosis. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 23,
hepatocellular disease: Differentiation acc- 41–44.
ording to disease specific characteristics and Tuck, R. R., & Jackson, M. (1991). Social, neurologi-
severity of the identified cerebral dysfunc- cal and cognitive disorders in alcoholics. Medical
tion. International Journal of Neuroscience, 32, Journal of Australia, 155, 225–229.
901–910. Turner, J., & Parsons, O. A. (1988). Verbal and non-
Tarter, R. E., Hegedus, A. M., Van Thiel, D. H., verbal abstracting: Problem-solving abilities and
Gavaler, J. S., & Schade, R. R. (1986b). Hepatic dys- familial alcoholism in female alcoholics. Journal
function and neuropsychological test performance of Studies on Alcohol, 49, 281–287.
454 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Unkenstein, A. E., & Bowden, S. C. (1991). Predicting Wik, G., Borg, S., Sjogren, I., Wiesel, F. A., Blomqvist,
the course of neuropsychological status in recently G., Borg, J., et al. (1988). PET determination of
abstinent alcoholics: A pilot study. The Clinical regional cerebral glucose metabolism in alcohol-
Neuropsychologist, 5, 24–32. dependent men and healthy controls using 11C-glu-
Victor, M., Adams, R. D., & Collins, G. H. (1989). The cose. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 78, 234–241.
Wernicke–Korsakoff Syndrome. Philadelphia: F. A. Wilde, E. A., Bigler, E. D., Gandhi, P. V., Lowry, C. M.,
Davis Company. Blatter, D. D., Brooks, J., et al. (2004). Alcohol abuse
Vighetto, A., Charles, N., Salzmann, M., Confavreux, and traumatic brain injury: Quantitative magnetic
C., & Aimard, G. (1991). Korsakoff ’s syndrome resonance imaging and neuropsychological out-
as the initial presentation of multiple sclerosis. come. Journal of Neurotrauma, 21(2), 137–147.
Journal of Neurology, 238, 351–354. Wilkinson, D. A. (1982). Examination of alcoholics
Vitiello, M. V., Prinz, P. N., Personius, J. P., Nuccio, M. by computed tomographic (CT) scans: A critical
A., Ries, R. K., & Koerker, R. M. (1987). History of review. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental
chronic alcohol abuse is associated with increased Research, 6, 31–45.
nighttime hypoxemia in older men. Alcoholism: Wilkinson, D. A. (1987). Discussion: CT scan and
Clinical and Experimental Research, 11, 368–371. neuropsychological assessments of alcoholism. In
Volkow, N. D., Hitzemann, R., Wang, G. J., Fowler, O. A. Parsons, N. Butters, and P. E. Nathan (Eds.),
J. S., Burr, G., Pascani, K., et al. (1992). Decreased Neuropsychology of alcoholism: Implications for
brain metabolism in neurologically intact healthy diagnosis and treatment (pp. 76–102). New York:
alcoholics. American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, Guilford Press.
1016–1022. Williams, C. M., & Skinner, A. E. (1990). The cog-
Volkow, N. D., Wang, G. J., Hitzemann, R., Fowler, J. nitive effects of alcohol abuse: A controlled study.
S., Overall, J. E., Burr, G., et al. (1994). Recovery of British Journal of Addiction, 85, 911–917.
brain glucose metabolism in detoxified alcoholics. Winterer, G. A., Kloppel, B., Heinz, A., Ziller, M.,
American Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 178–183. Dufeu, P., Schmidt, L. G., et al. (1998). Quantitative
Vrbaski, S. R., & Ristić, M. (1985). Cerebellum lipids EEG QEEG predicts relapse in patients with
in rats after chronic ethanol treatment. Journal of chronic alcoholism and points to a frontally
Neurochemistry, 44(6), 1868–1872. pronounced cerebral disturbance. Psychiatry
Walker, R. D., Donovan, D. M., Kivlahan, D. R., & Research, 78, 101–113.
O’Leary, M. R. (1983). Length of stay, neuropsycho- Workman-Daniels, K. L., & Hesselbrock, V. M. (1987).
logical performance, and aftercare: Influences on Childhood problem behavior and neuropsycholog-
alcohol treatment outcome. Journal of Consulting ical functioning in persons at risk for alcoholism.
and Clinical Psychology, 51, 900–911. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 48, 187–193.
Wang, G. J., Volkow, N. D., Roque, C. T., Cestaro, Yohman, J. R., & Parsons, O. A. (1985). Intact verbal
V. L., Hitzemann, R. J., Cantos, E. L., et al. (1993). paired-associate learning in alcoholics. Journal of
Functional importance of ventricular enlarge- Clinical Psychology, 41, 844–851.
ment and cortical atrophy in healthy subjects and Yohman, J. R., & Parsons, O. A. (1987). Verbal reason-
alcoholics as assessed with PET, MR imaging, and ing deficits in alcoholics. The Journal of Nervous
neuropsychologic testing. Radiology, 186, 59–65. and Mental Disease, 175, 219–223.
Waugh, M., Jackson, M., Fox, G. A., Hawke, S. H., Yohman, J. R., Parsons, O. A., & Leber, W. R. (1985).
& Tuck, R. R. (1989). Effect of social drinking on Lack of recovery in male alcoholics’ neuropsy-
neuropsychological performance. British Journal chological performance one year after treatment.
of Addiction, 84, 659–667. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 9,
Wechsler, D. (1955). Manual for the Wechsler Adult 114–117.
Intelligence Scale. New York: The Psychological York, J. L., & Biederman, I. (1991). Hand move-
Corporation. ment speed and accuracy in detoxified alcoholics.
Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale- Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research,
Revised Manual. New York: The Psychological 15, 982–990.
Corporation. Zatz, L. M., Jernigan, T. L., & Ahumada, A. J., Jr.
Wechsler, D., & Stone, C. P. (1945). Wechsler Memory (1982). Changes on computed cranial tomography
Scale Manual. New York: The Psychological with aging: Intracranial fluid volume. American
Corporation. Journal of Neuroradiology, 3, 1–11.
Whipple, S. C., Parker, E. S., & Noble, E. P. (1988). An Zipursky, R. B., Lim, K. O., & Pfefferbaum, A. (1989).
atypical neurocognitive profi le in alcoholic fathers MRI study of brain changes with short-term
and their sons. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 49, abstinence from alcohol. Alcoholism: Clinical and
240–244. Experimental Research, 13, 664–666.
19

Neuropsychological Consequences
of Drug Abuse
Raul Gonzalez, Jasmin Vassileva, and J. Cobb Scott

Human beings have used psychoactive sub- Neurobehavioral effects of alcohol use are dis-
stances for thousands of years for spiritual, cussed in a separate chapter of this book.
medicinal, and recreational purposes (Merlin, Understanding the neurobehavioral con-
2003). Such substances affect neurotransmis- sequences of substance use and substance use
sion, may have associated neuropsychological disorders is important from both a research neu-
effects, and often have potential for abuse. With roscience and clinical perspective. For example,
advances in agriculture and technology, cou- functional and structural brain changes that
pled with boundless human curiosity, new psy- occur as a result of acute administration and
choactive substances with potential for abuse chronic use of specific substances can provide
emerge frequently. Use and possession of psy- insights on how modulation of particular neu-
choactive drugs are often legislated due to rotransmitter systems affects the brain and
medical, political, and social influences. Many behavior of human subjects. Insight into the
are available legally (e.g., caffeine, alcohol, nic- neuroscience of human volition and reward
otine), whereas others may only be obtained attribution may be explored through examining
through medical prescriptions (e.g., opiates), or the neural underpinnings of the “loss of con-
are banned outright under most circumstances trol” that typically occurs with substance addic-
(e.g., cannabis, MDMA, LSD). The number of tion. From a clinical standpoint, understanding
substances known to affect neurobehavioral the neuropsychological effects of substance use
functioning are vast, but scientific research can help clinicians discern its impact in the pre-
has generally focused on those most frequently sentation, diagnosis, and treatment planning
used. Preclinical studies, as well as human neu- of patients. Moreover, the sheer prevalence of
ropathological and neuroimaging research, licit and illicit substance use ensures that med-
have been conducted to understand how spe- ical professionals in many fields are likely to
cific drugs affect the brain. Although relevant, encounter patients with history of substance
a thorough discussion of such topics would use or with current substance use, even if they
result in a voluminous work. In this chapter, we are referred for other neurobehavioral or men-
focus on investigations of the neuropsycholog- tal health disorders. Indeed, almost half (46%)
ical sequelae associated with use and misuse of of individuals over 12 years of age in the United
popular psychoactive drugs by human subjects. States reported using illicit drugs in their life-
Neither is it possible to cover all relevant inves- time, with 2.8% of the U.S. population meeting
tigations for each substance we review; thus, criteria for a substance use disorder during
whenever possible we highlight findings from 2005 (Substance Abuse and Mental Health
meta-analyses, large-scale investigations, and/ Services Administration, 2006a). Substance use
or those with particularly illuminating findings. disorders are a significant public health issue,

455
456 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

with economic impact due to disability, acci- disorders, whereas others may focus on sub-
dents, healthcare, and days missed from work. stance users that have consumed a specific sub-
During 1995, the total economic burden of sub- stance a certain number of times (sometimes
stance use was estimated to be $428.1 billion infrequently). These two populations are likely
(Rice, 1999). to be very different in terms of substance use
In order to better contextualize the research severity, comorbid confounds, whether they are
findings we discuss in this chapter, it is prudent seeking treatment, and other factors that may
to first discuss some important methodological influence neuropsychological functioning.
issues that challenge most retrospective studies In this chapter, we will present research find-
on neuropsychological effects of substance use. ings on the neuropsychological sequelae of sub-
Although this list is not exhaustive, the reader stance use grouped by various major drugs and
must consider that: (1) patterns of substance use drug classes. This organizational scheme makes
for any given substance often differ substan- intuitive sense based on the unique pharmaco-
tially across studies; (2) some studies incorpo- logical profiles and actions of these substances
rate samples of substance users that do not meet on neurotransmitters systems and provides a
diagnosis for substance use disorders, whereas logical framework for discussion. However, this
others examine performance in such subjects should not be misconstrued to suggest that the
exclusively; (3) substance users often misuse neuropsychological deficits described for a par-
multiple substances, which may have common ticular drug class are solely the cause of using
as well as unique neuropsychological effects; that substance or are specific only to that sub-
(4) substance users often have other comorbid stance. Use and abuse of multiple illicit drugs
conditions known to affect neuropsycholog- over a lifetime is common among individuals
ical functioning (e.g., psychiatric disorders); with substance dependence. One of the most for-
(5) some neurobehavioral problems may pre- midable obstacles in studying the unique effects
date substance use or be worsened, rather than of drugs of abuse is the extremely high preva-
caused, by substance use. lence of polydrug use among addicts (Darke &
Substance use characteristics of partici- Hall, 1995; Leri et al., 2003). Therefore, it is
pants in research studies differ widely, based difficult to find subject samples that have pre-
on parameters such as length of abstinence, dominantly used only one drug, and when such
amount of polydrug use, and severity of sub- samples are successfully recruited, the ability
stance use (e.g., amount, frequency, duration, to generalize from their findings to most drug
degree of addiction), which may lead to very users is questionable.
discrepant findings across studies. For example, Furthermore, substance use disorders and/or
results may vary depending on participants’ substance addiction, regardless of the substance,
stage of abstinence; that is, whether they are appear to share a common neuropathophysiol-
actively using drugs (acute effects), have very ogy, which involves orbitofrontal cortex and
recently stopped (withdrawal), are postwith- anterior cingulate, as well as parts of basal gan-
drawal but without full brain recovery from the glia and limbic structures (Goldstein & Volkow,
effects of the drug or still have traces of the drug 2002). It has also been noted (Rogers & Robbins,
in their body (residual effects), or if much time 2001) that because all drugs of abuse are known
has elapsed since last use (long term or perma- to act on the mesocorticolimbic dopaminergic
nent effects). Furthermore, it is important to system, one would expect some common neu-
note that being a substance user does not mean rocognitive deficits to be associated with the
invariably that one has a substance use disorder, abuse of various classes of drugs. Yet, because
as most individuals that try or use a particular specific drugs of abuse also differentially affect
drug do not progress to meeting the criteria for other monoaminergic and neuropeptide sys-
a substance use disorder in their lifetime. The tems in the brain, impairments in brain and cog-
DSM-IV recognizes two general types of sub- nitive functioning may differ depending on the
stance use disorders (abuse or dependence) for specific pharmacological properties of the drug.
each of several drug classes. Some studies only Finally, in all but a few of the studies we
include participants that have substance use review, it remains unknown whether the
Neuropsychological Consequences of Drug Abuse 457

observed neuropsychological deficits in sub- in human brain tissue with densest concentra-
stance users predate the onset of substance tion being in basal ganglia, cerebellum, hippo-
use and misuse or whether they are the direct campus, and amygdala (Breivogel & Childers,
result of substance use. There is a large liter- 1998; Glass et al., 1997). We now understand
ature showing genetic and neurobehavioral that cannabis exerts its psychoactive effects by
antecedent risk factors for substance depen- binding to CB1 receptors.
dence, which suggests that some neuropsycho- Cannabis produces its psychoactive effects
logical problems are likely to predate drug use in minutes when smoked, with peak plasma
(e.g., Vanyukov et al., 2003). Without the use of concentrations achieved in 3–10 minutes and
twin studies or longitudinal research designs psychoactive effects lasting approximately 2–3
that follow participants through various stages hours (Grotenhermen, 2003). Acute intoxica-
of drug use (before use, during use, and dur- tion has been shown to affect neuropsycholog-
ing multiple time points after abstinence), ical functioning. The extant literature on acute
these important questions remain difficult to effects of cannabis provide substantial evidence
answer. Despite the challenges presented by the for dose-dependent impairments in verbal and
aforementioned issues, much progress has been nonverbal memory characterized by retrieval
made in the understanding of how substance based deficits and intrusion errors for informa-
use and misuse affects neuropsychological tion presented during intoxication, but no recall
functioning. In addition, it is worth noting that or recognition difficulties with information
even if many neuropsychological abnormalities learned prior to intoxication (Ranganathan &
may predate substance use, it is also likely that D’Souza, 2006). However, conflicting reports
they may be exacerbated with initiation and exist on the magnitude of impairments on other
continuation of drug use. neuropsychological abilities (i.e., processing
speed, reaction time, executive functions).
Understanding the acute effects of canna-
Cannabis
bis on neuropsychological functioning is an
Cannabis is the most commonly used illicit important endeavor; however, it can be argued
substance in the United States, with 40% of that understanding the residual, long-term,
Americans over the age of 12 reporting lifetime or permanent changes in neuropsychological
use during 2005, and 8% reporting use during functioning that are brought about by canna-
the past month (SAMHSA, 2006b). It is esti- bis use is of greater public health significance.
mated that approximately 3.7% of the world’s Numerous studies have been conducted inter-
population has used cannabis—compared to nationally over the last several decades, which
0.3%–0.4% for cocaine and heroin (United have been the subject of several reviews. But,
Nations Office on Drugs and Crimes, 2004). such qualitative reviews have sometimes come
Given its widespread use, it is no wonder that to different conclusions despite substantial over-
the effects of cannabis on neuropsychological lap in the studies surveyed. For example, some
functioning have been so avidly researched and conclude “no evidence that marijuana . . . leads
debated. to functional impairment” (Wert & Raulin,
Neuropsychological research on cannabis 1986), others note “the data support a ‘drug res-
has been partially yoked to developments in idue’ effect . . . but evidence is as yet insufficient
understanding the chemical and neurophar- to support or refute a . . . toxic effect on the CNS”
macological properties of its psychoactive (Pope et al., 1995), and others purport that
ingredients. In 1964, the discovery of delta- “long-term use of cannabis leads to a more sub-
9-tetrahydrocannabinol as the primary psy- tle and selective impairment of cognitive func-
choactive substance in cannabis (Mechoulam & tion” (Solowij et al., 2002).
Gaoni, 1967) incited new research on its effects. Discrepancies in interpretations of study
It was not until 1988 when the first cannabinoid results are in part fueled by substantial hetero-
receptor (CB1) was identified in the mamma- geneity in research designs and by methodo-
lian brain (Devane et al., 1988) and later cloned logical limitations in many of the studies that
(Matsuda et al., 1990). CB1 is widely distributed have been reviewed, which have been previously
458 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

discussed in detail (Gonzalez et al., 2002; Pope, et al., 2007a; Whitlow et al., 2004), including
2002; Pope et al., 1995). In order to overcome on measures of decision making and inhibitory
the limitations associated with qualitative control. However, findings from one investiga-
reviews, Grant et al. (2003) conducted a meta- tion suggested that history of cannabis use may
analysis on studies examining nonacute (i.e., have been neuroprotective in the context of
residual, long-term, permanent) effects of can- methamphetamine use (Gonzalez et al., 2004).
nabis use, including only those studies that met However, like results from the meta-analysis,
a liberal (but essential) set of inclusion criteria. the aforementioned studies cannot establish a
These criteria were chosen to represent minimal causal link between cannabis use and neuro-
scientific standards needed to infer that differ- psychological dysfunction, nor can they con-
ences in neuropsychological performance were fidently determine the course of changes in
associated with cannabis use, rather than other neuropsychological functioning that may occur
potential confounds (e.g., neurological disor- after abstinence.
ders, psychiatric comorbidity, other drug use). A few investigations have employed longi-
Of the 40 studies that were found to examine the tudinal, within-subjects designs with neuro-
question of residual effects of cannabis on neu- psychological tests administered at different
ropsychological functioning (Gonzalez et al., time points after verified abstinence, in order
2002), only 11 met all criteria (Block & Ghoneim, to strengthen causal inferences between can-
1993; Carlin & Trupin, 1977; Croft et al., 2001; nabis use and neuropsychological functioning.
Ehrenreich et al., 1999; Gouzoulis-Mayfrank Pope and colleagues (2001) examined groups
et al., 2000; Hamil, 1996; Pope & Yurgelun-Todd, of former heavy cannabis users, current heavy
1996; Pope et al., 2001; Rodgers, 2000; Solowij, cannabis users, and nonusing controls that
1995; Solowij et al., 2002) and an additional 4 completed thorough neuropsychological assess-
(Deif et al., 1993; Grant et al., 1973; Rochford ments on days 0, 1, 7, and 28 of supervised absti-
et al., 1977; Wig & Varma, 1977) violated only nence. Only current heavy users performed
one criterion. When data from all investigations more poorly than controls (on measures of ver-
were subjected to meta-analysis, there was evi- bal memory); however, these differences were
dence for a small overall detrimental effect of observed only on days 0, 1, and 7, with no sig-
cannabis use history on overall neuropsycho- nificant differences detected by day 28.
logical functioning (d = –.16), with only two Two studies by another research group (Fried
of eight neuropsychological domains (Learning et al., 2002, 2005) examined the neuropsycho-
and Recall/Retention) showing significant, logical performance of a large cohort of longi-
albeit small, effect sizes (d = –.21 and –.27, tudinally followed individuals at ages 9–12 and
respectively). This is consistent with approxi- again at 17–20 years of age, which were divided
mately a quarter-of-a-standard-deviation dif- into four groups based on their cannabis use
ference between cannabis users and nonusers. status at the second assessment (current reg-
No significant effect sizes were observed for ular heavy cannabis smokers, current regular
the domains of simple reaction time, attention, light smokers, former regular smokers, and a
verbal/language, abstraction/executive, percep- nonusing control group). Groups were fairly
tual motor, and motor. Subjects in the studies well matched with adequate control of perti-
included in the meta-analysis varied substan- nent confounds, and all cannabis users reported
tially in length of abstinence from cannabis, abstinence for at least 1 day prior to testing.
and data points were too few to conduct analy- Relative to their baseline, only current heavy
ses examining how length of abstinence affected cannabis users demonstrated a statistically sig-
the magnitude of observed effects. nificant decrease in IQ scores, immediate and
It is worth noting that several studies have delayed memory, and information processing
been published after the meta-analysis which speed. However, effect sizes were fairly small.
report neuropsychological problems among Current light users and former heavy users were
abstinent heavy cannabis users in various abil- not found to differ significantly from controls.
ity areas (Bolla et al., 2002, 2005; Kelleher et al., A noteworthy study that overcomes the issue
2004; Messinis et al., 2006; Verdejo-Garcia of possible premorbid differences between
Neuropsychological Consequences of Drug Abuse 459

cannabis users and nonusers employed a com- America. Currently the two most commonly
prehensive battery of neuropsychological tests used forms of cocaine are cocaine hydrochlo-
to examine 54 monozygotic male twin pairs ride, the powdered form of cocaine typically
discordant for history of cannabis use (Lyons taken intranasally, and crack-cocaine, a free-
et al., 2004). Twin pairs were genetically identi- base form of cocaine which is smoked. Cocaine
cal, raised in the same home, and did not differ is a vasoconstrictor known to cause cerebro-
on history of alcohol, other drug use, or indi- vascular complications of an ischemic nature
ces of achievement (e.g., employment, educa- (Jacobs et al., 1989), resulting in neuronal dam-
tional attainment, school grades, and academic age that may further lead to neuropsychological
difficulties). None of the participants reported impairments. There has been a dramatic increase
using cannabis at least 1 year prior to testing, in the number of cases of cocaine-related ische-
with last regular use occurring about 27 years mic and hemorrhagic strokes, particularly with
ago on average. Of over 50 different indices of the advent of crack-cocaine use in the early
neuropsychological performance examined, 1980s (Levine et al., 1991). Evidence exists show-
statistically significant differences of very small ing greater abuse liability, greater propensity
magnitude were observed only on one measure for dependence, and more severe consequences
of visuoconstructional abilities. when cocaine is smoked (i.e., crack) when com-
Overall, there is ample evidence demonstrat- pared with intranasal use (i.e., cocaine hydro-
ing acute effects of cannabis use on neuropsy- chloride; Hatsukami & Fischman, 1996).
chological functioning, particularly in the areas Structural neuroimaging studies with
of learning and memory. Similarly, deficits cocaine users reveal volume loss primarily in
are also often observed among current, heavy the frontal lobe white matter (Bartzokis et al.,
cannabis users when not intoxicated, with less 2002) and gray matter (Franklin et al., 2002),
frequent users of cannabis often showing mini- but also in the temporal and insular cortices
mal or no deficits. Further, most of the current (Bartzokis et al., 2002; Franklin et al., 2002).
evidence suggests that neuropsychological con- Functional neuroimaging studies with cocaine
sequences of cannabis use appear to dissipate addicts document widespread perfusion and
over time, indicative of no permanent neuro- activation deficits in frontal and limbic areas
psychological effects. (Figure 19–1, see also the color version in the
color insert section; Goldstein & Volkow, 2002;
Volkow et al., 1993) that may persist even after
Cocaine
months of abstinence (Strickland et al., 1993;
The latest United Nations Office on Drugs and Volkow et al., 1992). Typically, acute adminis-
Crime world drug report (UNODC, 2006) tration of cocaine results in higher dopamine
estimates that in 2005, there were 13.4 million (DA) concentrations in limbic but not in frontal
cocaine users worldwide. The United States regions (Goldstein & Volkow, 2002). However,
remains the single largest cocaine market discontinuation of cocaine use leads to a marked
worldwide, accounting for more than 40% of downregulation of dopaminergic activity and
all cocaine users (UNODC, 2006). Within the decreased DA receptor availability, perhaps
United States, there were 2.4 million people related to the reduced glucose metabolism and
estimated to have used cocaine within the cerebral blood flow typically observed in fron-
past month (SAMHSA, 2006b), which repre- tal and limbic areas of abstinent cocaine users
sents an increase of 400,000 more users com- (Goldstein & Volkow, 2002; Volkow et al., 1993).
pared to 2004. Similarly, the number of current In addition, these marked structural and func-
crack users in the United States increased tional brain abnormalities in chronic cocaine
from 467,000 in 2004 to 682,000 in 2005. users have been associated with a variety of
Furthermore, in 2005 there were 1.5 million impairments in neuropsychological function.
Americans who met criteria for abuse of or Acute administration of cocaine has been
dependence on cocaine. associated with improved attentional perfor-
Cocaine is an alkaloid found in the leaves of mance (Johnson et al., 1998), speed of infor-
Erythroxylon coca, a tree indigenous to South mation processing (Higgins et al., 1990), and
460 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Cocaine abuser
Comparison subject A A A A

Figure 19–1. Cocaine abusers tend to show decreased glucose metabolism in areas of prefrontal cortex (A)
relative to healthy controls. (Image courtesy of Dr. Nora Volkow.)

inhibitory control (Fillmore et al., 2006), con- that the neurocognitive deficits associated with
sistent with the acute effects of other stimulant cocaine use may persist even after months of
drugs on neurocognitive function. A review of abstinence (Ardila et al., 1991; Di et al., 2002;
the long-term sequelae of chronic cocaine use O’Malley & Gawin, 1990).
suggests detrimental effects on cognitive func- To our knowledge, there are no meta-analyses
tioning, although the neurocognitive deficits on the long-term neurocognitive effects of
tend to be subtle and specific rather than general. chronic cocaine use; however, a recent quanti-
Consistent with the frontal lobe dysfunction tative review (Jovanovski et al., 2005) compared
seen in neuroimaging studies, the deficits pri- effect sizes across studies and concluded that
marily cluster in the domain of executive func- the most consistent impairments are indeed
tioning (Bolla et al., 1998), which are abilities noted in attention and executive function. The
mediated primarily by the prefrontal cortex and authors analyzed the effect sizes of approxi-
its reciprocal cortical and subcortical connec- mately 140 neuropsychological test indices pro-
tions. However, results across studies of chronic vided by various clinical neuropsychological
cocaine users have often been equivocal. Some measures in 15 studies conducted between 1987
studies of abstinent individuals have noted sig- (when cocaine dependence first appeared as a
nificant deficits in attention (di Schlafani et al., diagnostic category in the DSM-III-R) and 2002.
2002; Roselli & Ardila, 1996), executive func- The total combined sample size included 481
tioning (Beatty et al., 1995; di Schlafani et al., cocaine (predominantly crack) users and 586
2002; Rosselli et al., 2001), psychomotor speed healthy controls. The median effect size for all
(Bauer, 1994; O’Malley & Gawin, 1990; Robinson tests was 0.35. Across individual studies, the
et al., 1999), and visual and verbal memory largest effect sizes were observed on measures
(Bolla et al., 2000; Roselli & Ardila, 1996; van of attention, followed by measures of working
Gorp et al., 1999), whereas others report negli- memory, visual memory, and executive func-
gible effects (Selby & Azrin, 1998; Volkow et al., tion (with the exception of the Wisconsin Card
1992), and yet others report better performance Sorting Test). On the other hand, minimal
in chronic cocaine users relative to healthy con- effect sizes were obtained on tests of language
trols (Bolla et al., 1999). In addition, a number functions including verbal fluency and sensory-
of well-controlled prospective studies indicate perceptual function. However, the results of this
Neuropsychological Consequences of Drug Abuse 461

quantitative review should nonetheless be inter- Task) (Bornovalova et al., 2005). Because most
preted with caution due to the wide variations of these tasks measure various dimensions of
in length of abstinence (e.g., 0–1075 days) across impulsivity and behavioral inhibition, results
the studies included in the analysis, which sug- indicate that nonacute long-term effects of
gests that some studies may have tested indi- chronic cocaine use tends to be manifested pri-
viduals during acute intoxication rather than marily in impaired impulse control and related
during long-term abstinence from cocaine use. functions.
Similarly, several of the studies in the analysis
included subjects with other concurrent sub-
Benzodiazepines
stance use, most notably alcohol.
It is worth noting that alcohol use is indeed Sedatives and tranquilizers are one of the
very common among cocaine users, with upto most common groups of drugs used illicitly
84% of those with cocaine dependence also in the United States, with 12.4% of individuals
being alcohol dependent (Regier et al., 1990). reporting lifetime use without a prescription
The presence of alcohol and cocaine in the (SAMHSA, 2006b). These figures do not take
body creates cocaethylene, a psychoactive sub- into account individuals with prescriptions for
stance which has a half-life three to five times these drugs, who are also at risk for dependence
that of cocaine and carries an 18- to 25-fold and possible neuropsychological sequelae.
increase in the risk of sudden death compared This drug class represents a variety of different
to cocaine alone (Andrews, 1997). However, chemicals of varying potency, bioavailability,
research to date has been inconclusive regard- and half-lives, which makes it difficult to gen-
ing the combined effects of alcohol and cocaine eralize results of studies across all substances
on neuropsychological functioning, with some that can be categorized as sedatives, tranquil-
studies suggesting that the vasodilative effects izers, and hypnotics. During the twentieth
of alcohol may in fact attenuate some of the century, barbiturates were some of the most
vasoconstrictive and neuropsychological effects commonly abused sedatives. Although some
of cocaine (Abi-Saab et al., 2005; Robinson investigations reported deleterious neurocog-
et al., 1999). nitive effects from abuse of these substances
It has been suggested that tasks that simulate (e.g., Grant et al., 1978), results were variable
real-life decision making or inhibitory control (for review see Grant & Mohns, 1975) and could
would be more sensitive to the neurocognitive be attributed in part to the heterogeneity across
impairments associated with drug addiction substances in this drug class. Barbiturates are
(Goldstein et al., 2004). Indeed, more recent still used as analgesics and for treatment of
studies using novel neurocognitive paradigms some disorders, including epilepsy (for review
investigating decision making related to delay of neurocognitive side effects in this context see
and risk (e.g., the Iowa Gambling Task, the Reynolds & Trimble, 1985); however, prescrip-
Rogers Decision-Making Task, the Delayed tion and availability of barbiturates has been
Reward Discounting task, the Balloon Analogue largely replaced by benzodiazepines. Indeed,
Risk Task), as well as tasks assessing the abil- the most commonly used drugs in this class
ity to inhibit behavioral responses such as the currently are the benzodiazepines, which were
Stop-Signal and Go/No-Go paradigms, have reported to have been used illicitly by 8.1% of
begun to provide more conclusive evidence of Americans during 2005, with diazepam, alpra-
the long-term deficits observed in cocaine users. zolam, and lorazepam most often consumed
Recent studies indicate that chronic cocaine use (SAMHSA, 2006b). The neuropsychological
is associated with deficits in response inhibition effects of benzodiazepines have also been the
(Fillmore et al., 2006; Hester et al., 2007), deci- most extensively studied. Some authors posit
sion making assessed with the Iowa Gambling that benzodiazepines have strong potential for
Task (Bechara et al., 2001; Verdejo-Garcia & abuse, dependence, and addiction based on
Perez-Garcia, 2007), delayed reward discount- animal and human studies (Busto & Sellers,
ing (Kirby & Petry, 2004), and risk-taking 1991); however, others point out that benzodiaz-
propensity (using the Balloon Analogue Risk epine abuse usually takes place in the context of
462 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

other substance use and is generally not a drug Salzman et al., 1992; Tata et al., 1994; Tonne
of choice (O’Brien, 2005). Regardless, during et al., 1995; Vignola et al., 2000). Among users
2005, 0.2% of Americans met a substance use in the combined data set, benzodiazepines
disorder diagnosis for tranquilizers (compare were used for an average of 9.9 years, at an
with 0.1% for heroin) (SAMHSA, 2006b). Given average daily dose of 17.2 mg (diazepam equiv-
their prevalence of use and potential for abuse, alent), with the last daily dose taken ranging
combined with the availability of controlled from 18 days to 4 hours prior to evaluation.
investigations and well-executed quantitative The vast majority of studies included in the
reviews, we focus only on the neuropsycholog- meta-analysis excluded individuals with other
ical effects of benzodiazepines in the section drug use or heavy alcohol use. Despite using a
below. conservative approach to calculate effect sizes
Benzodiazepines exert their mind-altering, in each study, the authors found statistically
and therapeutic, effects through their action on significant moderate to large effects, indicat-
GABA receptors (Haefely, 1978). Acute subjec- ing poorer neuropsychological performance
tive effects of benzodiazepine administration by benzodiazepine users across all 12 domains
are well known and include sedation, slowing, for which data were compiled.
relaxation, and anterograde amnesia, which A second meta-analysis was conducted to
clearly have implications for performance determine if published studies showed recov-
on neuropsychological tests (Barker et al., ery of neuropsychological functioning after
2003; Buffett-Jerrott & Stewart, 2002). Indeed, abstinence from benzodiazepines. Using sim-
benzodiazepines are clinically used to induce ilar study inclusion criteria and the same
anterograde amnesia in patients undergoing grouping of effect sizes into 12 neuropsycho-
surgery. Acutely, benzodiazepines have been logical domains, Barker and colleagues (2004b)
associated with neuropsychological deficits in examined if neuropsychological functioning
almost every cognitive domain that is typically improves among “long-term” benzodiazepine
assessed clinically, including processing speed, users after withdrawal with data from 12 inves-
explicit and implicit memory, attention, and tigations (Bergman et al., 1980, 1989; Birzele,
executive functions (Buffett-Jerrott & Stewart 1992; Curran, 1992; Gorenstein et al., 1994,
2002). 1995; Petursson et al., 1983; Rickels et al., 1999;
Barker, Greenwood, Jackson, and Crowe Sakol & Power, 1988; Salzman et al., 1992; Tata
(2004a, 2004b) conducted two meta-analyses et al., 1994; Tonne et al., 1995). Overall, sub-
to quantitatively compile results from peer- jects used benzodiazepines for approximately
reviewed studies published between 1980– 10 years, with an average daily dose of 15.3 mg
2000 that examined neuropsychological effects (diazepam equivalent). A median of 3 months
of “long-term” (at least 1 year) benzodiazepine (range 1–65) elapsed between initial neuro-
use. In their fi rst meta-analysis (Barker et al., psychological assessment and postwithdrawal
2004a), they calculated and compiled effect assessment. Small to moderate statistically sig-
sizes representing differences between groups nificant effect sizes, indicating improvements
of “long-term” benzodiazepine users and in performances, were observed for 5 of the
controls for each of 12 separate neuropsycho- 11 cognitive domains (visuospatial, attention/
logical domains (sensory processing, nonver- concentration, general intelligence, psychomo-
bal memory, speed of processing, attention/ tor speed, and nonverbal memory). A second
concentration, general intelligence, working set of analyses with all but one of these stud-
memory, psychomotor speed, visuospatial, ies revealed that even after the postwithdrawal
problem solving, verbal memory, motor con- assessment, previous benzodiazepine users still
trol, and verbal reasoning) across the 15 stud- demonstrated significant impairments of small
ies that met their inclusion criteria (Bergman to large effect sizes in 8 ability areas (verbal
et al., 1980, 1989; Birzele, 1992; Curran, 1992; memory, psychomotor speed, speed of process-
Gorenstein et al., 1994, 1995; Hendler et al., ing, motor control, visuospatial, general intel-
1980; Lucki & Rickels, 1986; Petursson et al., ligence, attention/concentration, and nonverbal
1983; Rickels et al., 1999; Sakol & Power, 1988; memory) compared to controls.
Neuropsychological Consequences of Drug Abuse 463

Barker et al. (2003, 2004a, 2004b) have noted evidence, it is prudent to expect that long-term
that many of the investigations conducted to use of benzodiazepines alone is likely to impart
examine the long-term effects of benzodiaz- deficits on neuropsychological assessments con-
epines (including many of the studies in their ducted in such patients. However, it is difficult
meta-analyses) suffer from significant limita- to generalize what nonacute effect on neuro-
tions, which include inadequate control for psychological functioning would be bestowed
other substance use and lack of a “high anxi- by intermittent recreational use of benzodiaz-
ety” non-benzodiazepine using control group epines. Recreational users of benzodiazepines
in addition to a group of healthy controls. They are likely to consume higher doses than those
addressed these issues by examining the neu- reported in the above studies. One can specu-
ropsychological performance of three groups late that chronic, long-term use at doses that
of carefully recruited (to exclude potential con- are at least in the range of the studies reviewed
founds), well-matched subjects: normal healthy would yield similar effects.
controls, individuals with anxiety disorders
but no benzodiazepine use (ANX Group), and
Methamphetamine
a group of individuals with anxiety disorders
(Ice, Crank, Crystal, Meth)
and history of using benzodiazepines for at
least 12 months (BZD Group). The group of Methamphetamine (MA) is a potent, addictive
subjects with history of benzodiazepine use psychostimulant that has marked effects on the
reported on an average approximately 9 years of central nervous system (CNS) for up to 12 hours
use with a mean length of abstinence of approx- and may be administered in a variety of dif-
imately 42 months, with all reporting more ferent ways (e.g., injection, snorting, or smok-
than 6 months of withdrawal from benzodiaz- ing). MA use has been increasingly prevalent
epine use. Participants completed several neu- in recent years, in part due to its easy and cost-
ropsychological measures assessing five ability efficient synthesis in clandestine laboratories
areas (verbal memory, motor control, nonverbal with inexpensive, over-the-counter ingredients.
memory, visuospatial, and attention/concentra- Recent estimates indicate that approximately
tion). The BZD group performed significantly 10 million people (around 4.5% of the U.S. pop-
worse than the ANX and normal control groups ulation) have tried MA at some point in their
on verbal memory and motor control domains, lives, while 1.3 million persons used MA in the
as well as on one measure of nonverbal mem- past year (SAMHSA, 2006b). In addition, dra-
ory, despite no significant differences between matic increases in both MA-related emergency
the BZD and ANX group on self-reported lev- room visits and MA abusers seeking treatment
els of anxiety symptoms (both of these groups have occurred from the mid-1990s until the pre-
reported significantly greater levels of anxiety sent (SAMHSA, 2006a, 2006c). While some data
than controls). No significant differences on suggest that MA use in the United States may
neuropsychological measures were observed have, at minimum, plateaued, MA use remains
between the ANX and healthy controls. Thus, a significant problem in many areas, including
differences in neuropsychological performance the Western region of the United States and
of benzodiazepine users could not be attributed Southeast Asia (SAMHSA, 2006b).
to anxiety. The acute effects of MA use can include
Thus, there is compelling evidence to sug- euphoria, increased respiration, vasoconstric-
gest that long-term use of benzodiazepines tion, tachycardia, appetite suppression, enhanced
cause significant and widespread neuropsy- energy, increased libido, insomnia, irritabil-
chological impairments that persist even after ity, and even paranoid psychosis. Chronic use
many months of abstinence. The majority of of MA is associated with a host of adverse
participants in the studies reviewed above were psychosocial (e.g., interpersonal, financial),
long-term users of therapeutic doses of benzo- psychiatric (e.g., depression, psychosis), and
diazepines, not unlike many patients who may physical sequelae, including potent effects on
present for neuropsychological assessments CNS functioning due to its rapid transport
in clinical settings. In light of the available across the blood–brain barrier (Barr et al.,
464 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

2006). Long-term MA use results in significant but current estimates suggest that approxi-
neurotoxicity, which may occur via a number mately 40% of persons with MA dependence
of processes (e.g., oxidative stress and hyper- demonstrate global neuropsychological impair-
thermia; Cadet et al., 2005; Davidson et al., ment (i.e., deficits in at least two cognitive
2001). This neurotoxicity is evident in several domains; Rippeth et al., 2004). Commensurate
neurotransmitter systems, but is perhaps most with the hypothesized neurotoxicity to dopa-
notable on nigrostriatal dopaminergic projec- minergic frontostriatal circuits, MA users evi-
tions, thus altering the function of the dopa- dence impairment in cognitive processes highly
mine rich fronto-striato-thalamo-cortical loops dependent upon these circuits, including epi-
(Cass, 1997). To this end, neuroimaging stud- sodic memory, complex information-processing
ies have demonstrated various structural (e.g., speed, attention/working memory, response
Thompson et al., 2004), cerebral blood flow inhibition, decision making, and novel problem
(e.g., Chang et al., 2002; Figure 19–2, see also solving. Comorbid disease conditions such as
the color version in the color insert section), HIV (Rippeth et al., 2004), Hepatitis C infec-
and metabolic (e.g., Ernst et al., 2000) abnor- tion (Cherner et al., 2005), or Attention-Deficit/
malities in the prefrontal cortex and striatum Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD; Sim et al.,
of MA dependent persons, and postmortem 2002) may further increase the risk of cogni-
studies have found reductions in levels of dopa- tive impairment. Whether the abuse of other
mine and its metabolites in the striatum of MA substances such as alcohol or cocaine in combi-
abusers (Moszczynska et al., 2004; Wilson et al., nation with MA imparts additive risk remains
1996). unanswered by the literature; however, comor-
Although administration of moderate doses bid marijuana use may not exacerbate impair-
of MA has been shown to enhance certain cog- ment (Gonzalez et al., 2004).
nitive abilities, such as attention and speed of One of the most marked impairments seen
information processing (e.g., Johnson et al., with MA abuse is in verbal episodic memory
2005; Mohs et al., 1980), chronic MA use is (Hoffman et al., 2006; Rippeth et al., 2004). The
often associated with mild to moderate neu- severity of this deficit persists into both early
ropsychological impairment. Interpretation of (Jaffe et al., 2005; Kalechstein et al., 2003) and
the literature remains difficult due to varying sustained (Johanson et al., 2006) MA absti-
methodologies and participant characteristics, nence. In addition, episodic memory may be

B
Methamphetamine

A
user

100%

C
Normal
control

Figure 19–2. Decreased rCBF in putamen (A) and frontal (B) white matter of a methamphetamine user
compared to a healthy control. Increased rCBF in a methamphetamine user compared to a healthy control in
parietal brain regions (C). (Image courtesy of Dr. Linda Chang.)
Neuropsychological Consequences of Drug Abuse 465

particularly susceptible to the effects of MA and/or impulsive choices (Gonzalez et al., 2007;
relapse (Simon et al., 2004). Woods and col- Paulus et al., 2002). Moreover, hypoactivation
leagues (2005) recently investigated the nature in prefrontal, parietal, and insular cortex dur-
of MA-associated verbal learning deficits, find- ing a decision-making task, potentially signi-
ing that inefficiencies in the strategic (i.e., exec- fying reduced processing resources, has been
utive) components of encoding and memory shown to predict incidence of relapse (Paulus
likely underlie the impairments observed, sup- et al., 2005).
porting a model of MA-associated neurotoxic- MA dependent patients are frequently noted
ity in frontostriatal circuits. to have variable attention, slowed process-
Executive dysfunction is also commonly ing, and increased distractibility. Deficits are
observed in MA-abusing populations, and most apparent on tests of complex processing,
may contribute to maintenance of drug-seek- especially when working memory is taxed or
ing behaviors (Bechara & Damasio, 2002). some degree of decision making is required,
Significant impairments are evident in the while basic attentional and processing abili-
ability to switch attentional sets or inhibit ties appear unaffected (Chang et al., 2002). For
responses as assessed by the Stop-Signal Task example, deficits in MA abusers are uncommon
(Monterosso et al., 2005) and the interference on tasks such as the Trail-Making Test, Part A
condition of the Stroop task (Salo et al., 2002; or simple digit recognition, but are more pro-
Simon et al., 2000, 2002; cf. Hoff man et al., nounced on tests such as the n-back task or
2006). However, a recent study identified a dis- Digit Symbol (Johanson et al., 2006; Kalechstein
sociation between set-shifting and inhibition in et al., 2003; Simon et al., 2002). Sustained atten-
an MA dependent population, finding inhibi- tion may also be particularly susceptible to
tion selectively compromised (Salo et al., 2005), MA-associated damage (London et al., 2005).
a conclusion bolstered by lack of impairment on Reduced psychomotor abilities have also
another task examining set-shifting abilities, been noted in chronic MA abusers, although
Trail-Making Test, Part B (Chang et al., 2005; the incidence has been less than what might
Kalechstein et al., 2003; Rippeth et al., 2004; be expected given the vulnerability of the
cf. Simon et al., 2000). Interestingly, deficits in striatum to MA-associated neurotoxicity
selective inhibition in MA abusers may be asso- (Moszczynska et al., 2004). Indeed, only a
ciated with reduced levels of N-acetyl aspartate few studies have found impairment in motor
(NAA), a brain metabolite commonly regarded functioning, mostly assessed via the Grooved
as a marker of neuronal integrity, in the anterior Pegboard test (Chang et al., 2002; Volkow et al.,
cingulate (Salo et al., 2007). 2001b). Nevertheless, preclinical and case stud-
In relation, MA dependent individuals ies suggest the presence of motor abnormalities
may have marked difficulties with impulsiv- in MA abusers, and it is possible that the mea-
ity, decision making, and conceptualization. sures employed have not been sensitive enough
While some studies have shown deficits in to detect the subtle nature of impairment
abstract reasoning and conceptualization (e.g., (Caligiuri & Buitenhuys, 2005). Alternatively, it
Simon et al., 2002), others have shown equiv- is possible that MA-associated damage to dopa-
ocal results (e.g., Rippeth et al., 2004). Similar minergic systems is more pronounced in cog-
to other substance dependent individuals (e.g., nitive (i.e., caudate) than motor (i.e., putamen)
Coffey et al., 2003), MA dependent individuals areas of the striatum (Caligiuri & Buitenhuys,
evidence elevated rates of “delay discounting,” 2005; Moszczynska et al., 2004).
which ostensibly reflects decision-making abil- It remains unresolved whether the extent of
ity and impulsivity by examining a participant’s neuropsychological impairment is related to
sensitivity to delayed versus immediate rewards MA use variables and/or extent of dependence.
(Hoffman et al., 2006; Monterosso et al., 2006). Some studies have reported that rates of cog-
In addition, MA dependent individuals may nitive impairment correlate with frequency
evidence fundamental dysfunction during deci- (Simon et al., 2000) or amount (Monterosso
sion-making tasks, including stimulus-driven et al., 2005) of MA use, while other studies have
behavior and elevated rates of disadvantageous found no associations with measures assessing
466 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

the severity of MA dependence or MA use fre- contain no MDMA at all (Tanner-Smith, 2006),
quency, amount, or duration (Chang et al., 2002; though MDMA is most often present (Parrott,
Hoffman et al., 2006; Johanson et al., 2006; 2004). In the sections below, we will use the
Rippeth et al., 2004). This ambiguity may be terms MDMA and “ecstasy” interchangeably,
due to the unreliability of self-report substance acknowledging the varying amount of MDMA
use data, or it may reflect the influence of other, that may have been consumed by participants
often unmeasured factors (e.g., psychiatric in retrospective studies.
symptomatology). MDMA produces its psychoactive effects pri-
Although a number of studies have shown marily through its effects on serotonin—but
at least partial recovery of both dopamine ter- also dopamine and norepinephrine—producing
minal function and brain metabolism with initial effects in humans after oral ingestion in
extended abstinence (e.g., Volkow et al., 2001a; about a half-hour to an hour and lasting about
Wang et al., 2004), it is still unclear whether this 3 to 4 hours (Green et al., 2003). Preclinical
recovery results in any substantive changes in studies suggest that MDMA may produce sub-
neuropsychological function. In early stages of stantial neurotoxic damage to serotonergic
abstinence, deficits are, at the least, equivalent nerve terminals (Green et al., 2003), though
to those seen in currently abusing individuals, its generalizability to recreational MDMA use
especially in the domains of learning/memory by humans has been the subject of substantial
and complex working memory (Hoff man et al., controversy (Baumann et al., 2007; Gouzoulis-
2006; Kalechstein et al., 2003; Simon et al., 2004; Mayfrank & Daumann, 2006a). Nonetheless,
cf. Chang et al., 2005). Volkow and colleagues many investigators have sought to document
(2001a) found that deficits in psychomotor and neuropsychological impairments that may arise
memory performance continued to persist after as a result of MDMA use.
9 months of abstinence, albeit in a less severe Dumont and Verkes (2006) conducted a
form, despite a significant degree of recovery in systematic semiqualitative review of 29 stud-
dopamine terminal function. Longer periods ies examining acute effects of MDMA using
of abstinence may lead to further recovery of well operationalized criteria for study inclu-
cognitive function, although some deficits still sion, such as: (1) MDMA must be administered
remain, most notably on episodic memory and to healthy volunteers with known and veri-
cognitive inhibition (Johanson et al., 2006). fied dose; (2) studies must employ a placebo-
controlled design; and (3) acute effects must be
measured. Many studies examined subjective
MDMA (Ecstasy)
effects of naïve and experienced users, with
MDMA (3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine), the most common effects (reported in more
or “ecstasy,” was first synthesized and patented than half of studies) being euphoria, extrover-
in the early 1900s, but did not gain popularity sion, improved mood, confusion, liking for the
until the late 1980s and early 1990s as a rec- drug, hallucination, and a “drug effect.” Studies
reational drug (Parrott, 2001). The chemical examining acute effects of MDMA on neuro-
structure of MDMA shares properties of both psychological functions were too few to war-
stimulants and hallucinogens—partly because rant generalization for most cognitive ability
it does not fit neatly into either of these drug cat- areas. However, a sufficient number of studies
egories and because of its social and emotional were available that examined performance on
effects, it was classified with similar substances measures of attention (Cami et al., 2000; Farre
(i.e., MBDB, MDA, MDAE) as an “entactogen” et al., 2004; Gamma 2000; Hernandez-Lopez
(Nichols et al., 1986). During 2005, 4.7% of et al., 2002; Lamers et al., 2003; Vollenweider
Americans over 12 years of age reported using 1998), although none found significant effects
“ecstasy” (SAMHSA, 2006b). Tablets of ecstasy of MDMA. Other studies have reported impair-
are expected to contain MDMA; however, they ments in memory, but these effects appear to be
may contain MDMA combined with other transient (Kuypers & Ramaekers, 2005, 2007).
psychoactive substances (e.g., mescaline, her- Substantially more literature is available
oin, caffeine, dextromethorphan, MA) or may on residual effects of MDMA use, which have
Neuropsychological Consequences of Drug Abuse 467

primarily been examined using retrospective illicit substances with known neuropsycho-
designs. Recently, this work has been synthe- logical sequelae, have significant psychiatric
sized in two separate meta-analyses that had comorbidity, and may have neuropsycholog-
substantial overlap in the studies they included. ical deficits that predate drug use, factors that
Verbaten (2003) synthesized data from between- are difficult to address and severely limit infer-
group studies examining neuropsychological ences (Gouzoulis-Mayfrank & Daumann, 2006b;
effects of recreational MDMA use published Gouzoulis-Mayfrank et al., 2006a). Similarly,
between 1975 and 2002. The only other criterion data provided by meta-analyses are only as
for including a study in their analysis was that good as the studies they examine, and most of
subjects must be free of MDMA or other illicit the investigations included in the meta-analyses
drugs for at least 1week prior to their evalua- described above employed samples of MDMA
tion. The analyses, which were based on a pool users who also used other illicit substances,
of 14 studies (Bhattachary & Powell, 2001; Croft making it difficult to attribute the deficits
et al., 2001; Fox et al., 2001; Gouzoulis-Mayfrank observed specifically to MDMA use and not
et al., 2000; McCann et al., 1999; Morgan, polysubstance use in general.
1998, 1999; Morgan et al., 2002; Parrott et al., Stronger causal inferences can be made from
1998; Reneman et al., 2001a, 2001b; Rodgers, longitudinal investigations of changes in neu-
2000; Verkes et al., 2001; Wareing et al., 2000), ropsychological functioning among MDMA
indicated that MDMA users demonstrated users after abstinence or continued use, although
significant impairments relative to controls such studies have reported mixed findings.
on measures of short-term memory, long- One study compared subjects after 18 months
term memory, and attention. More recently, of abstinence from MDMA and noted no
Kalechstein et al. (2007), conducted a meta- changes in cognitive function (Gouzoulis-
analysis of similar scope with studies published Mayfrank et al., 2005). In contrast, other stud-
until 2004. A liberal criteria that included any ies have found stable (or improved) memory
study on neuropsychological effects of MDMA performances in abstinent users while show-
with matched controls yielded 23 studies ing persistent declines in memory performance
for synthesis, whereas a stricter criteria that among continued users at a 1-year (Zakzanis &
required studies to match groups on demo- Young, 2001) and 2-year follow-up (Zakzanis &
graphic and premorbid factors and ensured that Campbell, 2006). However, these studies also
MDMA users were abstinent at assessment and suffered from samples of MDMA users who
not treatment seeking winnowed the dataset to also used varied other illicit substances.
11 investigations (Bhattachary & Powell, 2001; In order to avoid issues of polydrug use
Fox et al., 2001; Gouzoulis-Mayfrank et al., among MDMA users and to better isolate
2000; McCardle et al., 2004; Morgan, 1999; effects of MDMA on neuropsychological
Morgan et al., 2002; Parrott et al., 1998; Reneman functioning, Halpern et al. (2004) carefully
et al., 2000; Rodgers, 2000; Verkes et al., 2001; recruited subjects with a very restrictive crite-
Wareing et al., 2000). Findings indicated signif- ria to obtain two small groups of rave attendees
icantly poorer performance by MDMA users, (one group consisting of fairly “pure” users of
with generally moderate effect sizes observed MDMA) that were very well matched on pre-
on all domains (i.e., attention/concentration, morbid and demographic factors. MDMA users
verbal learning and memory, nonverbal learn- were negative on urine toxicology testing for
ing and memory, motor/psychomotor speed, various substances and reported at least 10 days
and executive functions). of abstinence from MDMA. Comparisons of
Although most published MDMA investiga- both groups showed few significant differences
tions make valiant attempts to soundly establish across measures on a very thorough neuropsy-
relationships between MDMA use and neuro- chological battery, though MDMA users were
psychological functions, most are also limited more likely to perform more poorly. However,
by the many confounds that are inherent in stratifying MDMA users into moderate (22–50
retrospective studies of drug use. Specifically, lifetime uses) and heavy (60–450 lifetime uses)
MDMA users are often frequent users of other groups revealed that the heavy group was
468 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

significantly worse on some (but not all) mea- criteria for dependence or abuse of prescription
sures of processing speed and executive func- opioid medications, which is about the same as
tions. Although this investigation has made the number of Americans classified with depen-
one of the best efforts to date in controlling dence or abuse of cocaine.
for important confounds common in other Opiate is an extract derived from the seeds
MDMA studies, their findings have also been of the opium poppy plant which has been
criticized due to their very small sample size cultivated by ancient civilizations in Persia,
and likelihood of Type-I errors given the few Mesopotamia, and Egypt. However, although
statistically significant results found among the opiate drugs have been used for centuries, it was
many neuropsychological tests they conducted only in recent decades that their mode of action
to examine between-group differences (Lyvers began to be elucidated. In 1973, three different
& Hasking, 2004). laboratories (Pert & Snyder, 1973; Simon et al.,
At this time, preclinical studies appear to 1973; Terenius, 1973) almost simultaneously
substantiate neurotoxicity from MDMA admin- reported the first evidence of the existence of
istration. However, investigations on neuro- stereospecific binding sites or receptors for
psychological deficits of recreational MDMA opiates in the brain. Currently, three types of
use have been difficult and inconclusive, par- opioid receptors have been identified (μ- [mu],
tially due to confounds inherent in the man- κ- [kappa], and δ- [delta] opioid receptors).
ner this drug is often used; that is, usually in The presence of such receptors in animal and
small infrequent quantities and/or in the con- human brain further suggested that endoge-
text of substantial other drug use. Evidence is nous opiate-like substances most likely exist in
persuasive for neuropsychological impairments the body. This led to the discovery of the endog-
among heavy MDMA users; however, whether enous opioid neuropeptides (Hughes et al.,
the deficits observed can be specifically attrib- 1975), commonly referred to as endorphins (a
uted to MDMA or other confounds remains to contraction of “endogenous” and “morphine”).
be untangled. Neuroimaging findings reveal a plethora of
abnormalities related to chronic opiate use.
Structural neuroimaging studies of opiate
Opiates
abusers typically show general cortical atrophy
Opiates account for the largest proportion of (Pezawas et al., 1998; Strang & Gurling, 1989)
people in drug treatment services worldwide and enlargement of external and internal CSF
(UNODC, 2006). According to the UNODC, spaces (Danos et al., 1998; Kivisaari et al., 2004;
in 2005 almost 16 million people were esti- Pezawas et al., 1998). Functional neuroimaging
mated to abuse opiates worldwide. Within the studies investigating the acute effects of opiates
United States, an estimated 136,000 Americans on brain functioning generally reveal decreases
were current heroin users in 2005 (SAMHSA, in blood flow or glucose utilization (Forman
2006b). Compared to almost 15 million cur- et al., 2004; London et al., 1990; Martin-Soelch
rent marijuana users and 2.4 million current et al., 2001), with some studies (Ersche et al.,
cocaine users in the United States, the number 2006; Pezawas et al., 2002) reporting increased
of current heroin users does not appear to be activations in various regions of the prefrontal
as high, yet opiate addiction is associated with cortex. Similarly, most neuroimaging studies
some of the most devastating health and social of the withdrawal / abstinence effects of opiates
consequences. typically reveal decreased activation in frontal
In addition to heroin, the past few years (Gerra et al., 1998; Krystal et al., 1995; Rose et al.,
have seen a marked increase in the use of pre- 1996), parietal (Krystal et al., 1995; Rose et al.,
scription opioid medications such as Vicodin 1996), and temporal cortices (Rose et al., 1996).
and OxyContin, with more than 4.7 million There is some indication that the decreased
Americans using such medications (SAMHSA, frontal activations are reversible with increased
2006b), more than the number abusing cocaine, length of abstinence (Rose et al., 1996).
heroin, inhalants, and hallucinogens com- Available evidence indicates that chronic
bined. Alarmingly, 1.5 million Americans meet opiate addiction is also associated with
Neuropsychological Consequences of Drug Abuse 469

neuropsychological impairments across mul- administration effects of prescription opi-


tiple cognitive domains. Yet, in comparison oids such as OxyContin (Zacny & Gutierrez,
with cannabis, cocaine, and other stimulants, 2003) and hydrocodone (Zacny, 2003) using
there has been considerably less research into a brief cognitive battery have revealed minor
the neuropsychological impairments related to impairments in attention and psychomotor
opiate use. Early investigations examining the functioning, but these effects were much less
effects of narcotics addiction on intelligence pronounced and short-lasting than the effects
testing reported no deficits (Isbell & Fraser, of benzodiazepines.
1950; Pfeffer & Ruble, 1948). However, research The long-term residual effects of opiates
into opiate effects did not become more com- appear to cluster in the domain of execu-
mon in the US until the 1970s due to the wide- tive functioning (Ersche et al., 2006; Lyvers &
spread heroin use by Vietnam veterans. Earlier Yakimoff, 2003) and most typically include
studies (e.g., Fields & Fullerton, 1975; Hill & impairments in working memory (Papageoriou
Mikhael, 1979; Korin, 1974) typically used et al., 2003) and various dimensions of impul-
traditional neuropsychological batteries con- sivity, such as decision making (Fishbein et al.,
sisting of the Bender-Gestalt, Halstead–Reitan, 2007) and delayed reward discounting (Kirby &
or the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. Results Petry, 2004; Kirby et al., 1999). With regard to
were somewhat inconsistent, with some stud- the temporal stability of these impairments,
ies (Hill & Mikhael, 1979; Grant et al., 1978; the literature suggests that there tends to be
Korin, 1974) reporting impaired performance improvement of cognitive functioning over time
of heroin addicts, whereas another (Fields & with abstinence (Guerra et al., 1987). The num-
Fullerton, 1974) found that heroin addicts ber of well-designed studies investigating the
performed better than a control group. long-term neurocognitive effects of prescrip-
More recent studies on the acute effects of tion opioid medication is still limited. A recent
opiates have typically been conducted with review (Chapman et al., 2002) of their effects in
patients on methadone maintenance therapy patients with chronic pain or cancer concluded
(MMT) and reveal a wide variety of cognitive that impairments tend to be mild, short-lived
deficits including impairments in attention and are most often present on simple measures
(Darke et al., 2000; Specka et al., 2000), work- of psychomotor speed but not on more com-
ing memory (Mintzer & Stitzer, 2002; Specka plex cognitive tasks. It should be noted that not
et al., 2000), memory (Darke et al., 2000), psy- all patients in the studies were abstinent at the
chomotor speed (Darke et al., 2000; Mintzer & time of testing and that the participants were
Spitzer, 2002; Specka et al., 2000), problem typically clinical populations with many con-
solving (Darke et al., 2000), and decision mak- founding variables other than pain.
ing (Mintzer & Spitzer, 2002; Rotheram-Fuller A number of more recent studies have begun
et al., 2004). Of these, the impairments in psy- to compare the neuropsychological functioning
chomotor speed and decision making seem to of current heroin addicts to that of individuals
be the most reliable findings. Other investi- addicted to other substances such as stimulants
gators (Guerra et al., 1987; Hill & Mikhael, or cocaine (Bornovalova et al., 2005; Ersche
1979; Korin, 1974; Rounsaville et al., 1982; et al., 2006; Kirby & Petry, 2004; Ornstein et al.,
Strang & Gurling, 1989) have tested neuro- 2000; Rogers et al., 1999; Verdejo-Garcia &
psychological functioning in current heroin Perez-Garcia, 2007; Verdejo-Garcia et al., 2007b).
users (not on MMT) and have revealed gener- In general, most studies reveal that the neu-
ally mixed results. For example, some studies rocognitive impairments in stimulant users are
(e.g., Rounsaville et al., 1982) reported no neu- more severe and extensive than those in heroin
ropsychological impairments, whereas others users. Studies comparing neuropsychological
noted impairments in attention (Guerra et al., functioning between users of different drug
1987; Strang & Gurling, 1989), short-term classes are particularly informative in reveal-
memory (Guerra et al., 1987), verbal fluency ing some of the unique effects of specific drugs
(Guerra et al., 1987), and memory (Strang & on cognition. Overall, the effects of opiates on
Gurling, 1989). Studies investigating acute neurocognitive function are variable and not
470 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

as consistent as those present in users of other to other time points afterwards, the important
classes of drugs and need to be addressed by question of whether some neuropsychological
meta-analytic studies, which may bring greater disturbances predated substance use can be
clarity of the type of neurocognitive impair- examined.
ments associated with opiate use. Carlin and O’Malley (1996) also suggested
that examining the effects of substance use
on neuropsychological functioning was not
Discussion
an actively pursued research topic at the time.
Use of illicit substances is a common practice, Currently, research on effects of substance use
and abuse of such substances is often associated and addiction on brain functioning is thriv-
with neuropsychological deficits that may vary ing, and the published manuscripts on the topic
in pattern, severity, and duration. Although have dramatically increased. This is likely in
many methodological challenges have served part due to major advances in the understand-
to obfuscate our understanding of how specific ing of the neuropathophysiology of drug addic-
substances affect neuropsychological function- tion, which have been made in recent years.
ing, significant improvements in the quality However, many other factors that differ across
and quantity of such research has occurred substances have also likely served to stimu-
in the last decade. In the previous edition of late research. For example, increase in MA
this book, one of the conclusions arrived at research has been concomitant with increase in
by Carlin and O’Malley (1996) was that litera- use of the drug in the United States. Similarly,
ture on neuropsychological effects of substance MDMA abuse did not receive widespread noto-
use had many shortcomings at the time. They riety until its use became more widespread, and
noted such issues as small sample sizes, lack many empirical investigations on its neuropsy-
of well-characterized participant groups, and chological effects have followed thereafter. For
absence of longitudinal investigations. Further, cannabis, a significant surge in studies was in
they identified meta-analysis as a useful tech- part due to characterization of an endogenous
nique that deals with the issue of small sample cannabinoid signaling system with implications
sizes and provides better consensus on avail- for therapeutic applications. Furthermore, the
able research findings. Since then, the clarity renewed debate on medicinal use of cannabis
of our knowledge has improved and many of and its legal status have strengthened the rele-
these issues have been addressed, but progress vance of understanding its neuropsychological
has been somewhat uneven across substances. effects. Scientific discoveries and sociopoliti-
For example, meta-analyses have furthered our cal factors will likely continue to steer research
understanding on the nonacute neuropsycho- priorities on topics of substance use, hopefully
logical effects of cannabis, benzodiazepines, resulting in persistent increases in the quality of
and MDMA. Unfortunately, to our knowledge, such studies for decades to come.
meta-analyses have not been conducted on
effects of cocaine or opiates. Typically, meta-
analyses have included only studies that meet References
important methodological criteria to minimize Abi-Saab, D., Beauvais, J., Mehm, J., Brody, M.,
effects of confounds and improve confidence in Gottschalk, C., & Kosten, T. R. (2005). The effect
associations observed between substance use of alcohol on the neuropsychological functioning
and neuropsychological functioning. Similarly, of recently abstinent cocaine-dependent subjects.
longitudinal investigations examining long- American Journal on Addictions, 14, 166–178.
Andrews, P. (1997). Cocaethylene toxicity. Journal of
term effects of substances have emerged for
Addictive Diseases, 16, 75–84.
cannabis, benzodiazepines, and MDMA, but Ardila, A., Roselli, M., & Strumwasser, S. (1991).
remain relatively few. Such studies are criti- Neuropsychological deficits in chronic cocaine
cal to our understanding on the persistence of abusers. International Journal of Neuroscience, 57,
neuropsychological changes due to drug use. 73–79.
Moreover, when participants are tested prior Barker, M. J., Greenwood, K. M., Jackson, M., &
to initiation of substance use and compared Crowe, S. F. (2004a). Cognitive effects of long-term
Neuropsychological Consequences of Drug Abuse 471

benzodiazepine use: A meta-analysis. CNS Drugs, Birzele, H. J. (1992). Benzodiazepine induced amne-
18, 37–48. sia after long-term medication and during with-
Barker, M. J., Greenwood, K. M., Jackson, M., & drawal. European Review of Applied Psychology,
Crowe, S. F. (2004b). Persistence of cognitive 42, 277–283.
effects after withdrawal from long-term benzodi- Block, R. I., & Ghoneim, M. M. (1993). Effects of
azepine use: A meta-analysis. Archives of Clinical chronic marijuana use on human cognition.
Neuropsychology, 19, 437–454. Psychopharmacology, 110, 219–228.
Barker, M. J., Jackson, M., Greenwood, K. M., & Bolla, K. I., Brown, K., Eldreth, D., Tate, K., & Cadet,
Crowe, S. F. (2003). Cognitive effects of benzodi- J. L. (2002). Dose-related neurocognitive effects of
azepine use: A review. Australian Psychologist, 38, marijuana use. Neurology, 59, 1337–1343.
202–213. Bolla, K. I., Cadet, J., & London, E. D. (1998). The
Barr, A. M., Panenka, W. J., MacEwan, G. W., neuropsychiatry of chronic cocaine abuse. Journal
Thornton, A. E., Lang, D. J., Honer, W. G., et al. of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 10,
(2006). The need for speed: An update on meth- 280–289.
amphetamine addiction. Journal of Psychiatry and Bolla, K. I., Eldreth, D. A., Matochik, J. A., & Cadet,
Neuroscience, 31(5), 301–313. J. L. (2005). Neural substrates of faulty decision-
Bartzokis, G., Beckson, M., Lu P. H., Edwards, N., making in abstinent marijuana users. Neuroimage.,
Bridge, P., & Mintz, J. (2002). Brain matura- 26, 480–492.
tion may be arrested in chronic cocaine addicts. Bolla, K. I., Funderburk, F. R., & Cadet, J. L. (2000).
Biological Psychiatry, 51, 605–611. Differential effects of cocaine and cocaine + alco-
Bauer, L. O. (1994). Vigilance in recovering cocaine- hol on neurocognitive performance. Neurology,
dependent and alcohol-dependent patients: A pro- 54, 2285–2292.
spective study. Addictive Behaviors, 19, 599–607. Bolla, K. I., Rothman, R. B., & Cadet, J. L. (1999).
Baumann, M. H., Wang, X., & Rothman, R. B. Dose-related neurobehavioral effects of chronic
(2007). 3,4-Methylenedioxymethamphetamine cocaine use. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and
(MDMA) neurotoxicity in rats: A reappraisal of Clinical Neurosciences, 11, 361–369.
past and present findings. Psychopharmacology, Bornovalova, M. A., Daughters, S. B., Hernandez,
189, 407–424. G. D., Richards, J. B., & Lejuez, C. W. (2005).
Beatty, W. W., Katzun, V. M., Moreland, V. J., & Differences in impulsivity and risk-taking pro-
Nixon, S. J. (1995). Neuropsychological performance pensity between primary users of crack cocaine
of recently abstinent alcoholics and cocaine abus- and primary users of heroin in a residential sub-
ers. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 37, 247–253. stance-use program. Experimental and Clinical
Bechara, A., & Damasio, H. (2002). Decision-making Psychopharmacology, 13, 311–318.
and addiction (part I): Impaired activation of Breivogel, C. S., & Childers, S. R. (1998). The func-
somatic states in substance dependent individu- tional neuroanatomy of brain cannabinoid recep-
als when pondering decisions with negative future tors. Neurobiology of Disease, 5, 417–431.
consequences. Neuropsychologia, 40, 1675–1689. Buffett-Jerrott, S. E., & Stewart, S. H. (2002).
Bechara, A., Dolan, S., Denburg, N., Hindes, A., Cognitive and sedative effects of benzodiazepine
Anderson, S. W., & Nathan, P. E. (2001). Decision- use. Current Pharmaceutical Design, 8, 45–58.
making deficits, linked to a dysfunctional ven- Busto, U. E., & Sellers, E. M. (1991). Anxiolytics and
tromedial prefrontal cortex, revealed in alcohol sedative/hypnotics dependence. British Journal of
and stimulant abusers. Neuropsychologia, 39, Addiction, 86, 1647–1652.
376–389. Cadet, J. L., Jayanthi, S., & Deng, X. (2005).
Bergman, H., Borg, S., Engelbrektson, K., & Vikander, Methamphetamine-induced neuronal apop-
B. (1989). Dependence on sedative-hypnotics: tosis involves the activation of multiple death
Neuropsychological impairment, field depen- pathways. Review. Neurotoxicity Research, 8,
dence and clinical course in a 5-year follow-up 199–206.
study. British Journal of Addiction, 84, 547–553. Caligiuri, M. P., & Buitenhuys, C. (2005). Do pre-
Bergman, H., Borg, S., & Holm, L. (1980). clinical findings of methamphetamine-induced
Neuropsychological impairment and exclusive motor abnormalities translate to an observable
abuse of sedatives or hypnotics. American Journal clinical phenotype? Neuropsychopharmacology,
of Psychiatry, 137, 215–217. 30, 2125–2134.
Bhattachary, S., & Powell, H. (2001). Recreational Cami, J., Farre, M., Mas, M., Roset, P. N., Poudevida, S.,
use of 3, 4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine Mas, A., et al. (2000). Human pharmacology of 3,
(MDMA) or “ecstasy”: Evidence for cognitive 4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (“ ecstasy”):
impairment. Psychological Medicine, 31, 647–658. Psychomotor performance and subjective effects.
472 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 20, Darke, S., Sims, J., McDonald, S., & Wickes, W.
455–466. (2000). Cognitive impairment among methadone
Carlin, A. S., & Trupin, E. W. (1977). The effect of maintenance patients. Addiction, 95, 687–695.
long-term chronic marijuana use on neuropsycho- Davidson, C., Gow, A. J., Lee, T. H., & Ellinwood, E. H.
logical functioning. International Journal of the (2001). Methamphetamine neurotoxicity: Necrotic
Addictions, 12, 617–624. and apoptotic mechanisms and relevance to
Carlin, A. S., & O’Malley, S. (1996). Neuropsychological human abuse and treatment. Brain Research
consequences of drug abuse. In I. Grant and Reviews, 36(1), 1–22.
K. M. Adams (Eds.), Neuropsychological assess- Deif, A., El, S. A., & Fawzy, R. K. (1993). Neurological,
ment of neuropsychiatric disorders (2nd ed.). psychiatric and C.T. evaluation of chronic can-
New York: Oxford University Press. nabis smokers. Journal of the Medical Research
Cass, W. A. (1997). Decreases in evoked overflow of Institute, 14, 151–160.
dopamine in rat striatum after neurotoxic doses of Devane, W. A., Dysarz, F. A., Johnson, M. R., Melvin,
methamphetamine. Journal of Pharmacology and L. S., & Howlett, A. C. (1988). Determination and
Experimental Therapeutics, 280(1), 105–113. characterization of a cannabinoid receptor in rat-
Chang, L., Cloak, C., Patterson, K., Grob, C., brain. Molecular Pharmacology, 34, 605–613.
Miller, E. N., & Ernst, T. (2005). Enlarged striatum Di, S. V., Tolou-Shams, M., Price, L. J., & Fein, G.
in abstinent methamphetamine abusers: A possi- (2002). Neuropsychological performance of indi-
ble compensatory response. Biological Psychiatry, viduals dependent on crack-cocaine, or crack-
57(9), 967–974. cocaine and alcohol, at 6 weeks and 6 months of
Chang, L., Ernst, T., Speck, O., Patel, H., DeSilva, abstinence. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 66,
M., Leonido-Yee, M., et al. (2002). Perfusion MRI 161–171.
and computerized cognitive test abnormalities Di Sclafani, V., Tolou-Shams, M., Price, L. J., & Fein, G.
in abstinent methamphetamine users. Psychiatry (2002). Neuropsychological performance of indi-
Research, 114(2), 65–79. viduals dependent on crack-cocaine, or crack-
Chapman, S. L., Byas-Smith, M. G., & Reed, B. A. cocaine and alcohol, at 6 weeks and 6 months of
(2002). Effects of intermediate and long-term use abstinence. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 66,
of opioids on cognition in patients with chronic 161–171.
pain. The Clinical Journal of Pain, 18, S83–S90. Dumont, G. J., & Verkes, R. J. (2006). A review of
Cherner, M., Letendre, S., Heaton, R. K., Durelle, J., acute effects of 3,4-methylenedioxymetham-
Marquie-Beck, J., Gragg, B., et al. (2005). Hepatitis C phetamine in healthy volunteers. Journal of
augments cognitive deficits associated with HIV Psychopharmacology, 20, 176–187.
infection and methamphetamine. Neurology, 64(8), Ehrenreich, H., Rinn, T., Kunert, H. J., Moeller, M.
1343–1347. R., Poser, W., Schilling, L., et al. (1999). Specific
Coffey, S. F., Gudleski, G. D., Saladin, M. E., & attentional dysfunction in adults following early
Brady, K. T. (2003). Impulsivity and rapid dis- start of cannabis use. Psychopharmacology, 142,
counting of delayed hypothetical rewards in 295–301.
cocaine-dependent individuals. Experimental and Ersche, K. D., Fletcher, P. C., Roiser, J. P., Fryer,
Clinical Psychopharmacology, 11(1), 18–25. T. D., London, M., Robbins, T. W., et al. (2006).
Croft, R. J., Mackay, A. J., Mills, A. T. D., & Differences in orbitofrontal activation during
Gruzelier, J. G. H. (2001). The relative contribu- decision-making between methadone-maintained
tions of ecstasy and cannabis to cognitive impair- opiate users, heroin users and healthy volunteers.
ment. Psychopharmacology, 153, 373–379. Psychopharmacology, 188, 364–373.
Curran, H. V. (1992). Memory functions, alertness Ernst, T., Chang, L., Leonido-Yee, M., & Speck, O.
and mood of long-term benzodiazepine users: A (2000). Evidence for long-term neurotoxicity asso-
preliminary investigation of the effects of a normal ciated with methamphetamine abuse: A 1H MRS
daily dose. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 6, 69. study. Neurology, 54(6), 1344–1349.
Danos, P., Van Roos, D., Kasper, S., Bromel, T., Farre, M., de la Torre, R., Mathúna, B. O., Roset, P. N.,
Broich, K., Krappel, C., et al. (1998). Enlarged Peiró, A. M., Torrens, M., et al. (2004). Repeated
cerebrospinal fluid spaces in opiate-dependent doses administration of MDMA in humans:
male patients: A stereological CT study. Neuropsy- Pharmacological effects and pharmacokinetics.
chobiology, 38, 80–83. Psychopharmacology, 173, 364–375.
Darke, S., & Hall, W. (1995). Levels and corre- Fields, S., & Fullerton, J. (1975). Influence of her-
lates of polydrug use among heroin users and oin addiction in neuropsychological functioning.
regular amphetamine users. Drug and Alcohol Journal of Clinical and Consulting Psychology,
Dependence, 39, 231–235. 43, 114.
Neuropsychological Consequences of Drug Abuse 473

Fillmore, M. T., Rush, C. R., & Hays, L. (2006). Acute the frontal cortex. American Journal of Psychiatry,
effects of cocaine in two models of inhibitory 159, 1642–1652.
control: Implications of non-linear dose effects. Gonzalez, R., Bechara, A., & Martin, E. M. (2007).
Addiction, 101, 1323–1332. Executive functions among individuals with
Fishbein, D. H., Krupitsky, E., Flannery, B. A., methamphetamine or alcohol as drugs of choice:
Langevin, D. J., Bobashev, G., Verbitskaya, E., Preliminary observations. Journal of Clinical and
et al. (2007). Neurocognitive characterizations Experimental Neuropsychology, 29(2), 155–159.
of Russian heroin addicts without a significant Gonzalez, R., Carey, C., & Grant, I. (2002). Nonacute
history of other drug use. Drug and Alcohol (Residual) neuropsychological effects of cannabis
Dependence, 90, 25–38. use: A qualitative analysis and systematic review.
Forman, S. D., Dougherty, G. G., Casey, B. J., Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 42, 48S–57S.
Siegle, G. J., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., et al. (2004). Gonzalez, R., Rippeth, J. D., Carey, C. L.,
Opiate addicts lack error-dependent activation of Heaton, R. K., Moore, D. J., Schweinsburg, B. C.,
rostral anterior cingulate. Biological Psychiatry, et al. (2004). Neurocognitive performance of
55, 531–537. methamphetamine users discordant for his-
Fox, H. C., Parrott, A. C., & Turner, J. J. (2001). tory of marijuana exposure. Drug and Alcohol
Ecstasy use: Cognitive deficits related to dosage Dependence, 76, 181–190.
rather than self-reported problematic use of the Gorenstein, C., Bernik, M. A., & Pompeia, S. (1994).
drug. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 15, 273. Differential acute psychomotor and cognitive
Franklin, T. R., Acton, P. D., Maldjian, J. A., Gray, J. D., effects of diazepam on long-term benzodiazepine
Croft, J. R., Dackis, C. A., et al. (2002). Decreased users. International Clinical Psychopharmacology,
gray matter concentration in the insular, orbitof- 9, 145–153.
rontal, cingulate, and temporal cortices of cocaine Gorenstein, C., Bernik, M. A., Pompeia, S., &
patients. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 134–142. Marcourakis, T. (1995). Impairment of perfor-
Fried, P., Watkinson, B., James, D., & Gray, F. (2002). mance associated with long-term use of benzodi-
Current and former marijuana use: Preliminary azepines. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 9, 313.
findings of a longitudinal study of effects on IQ Gouzoulis-Mayfrank, E. & Daumann, J. (2006a).
in Young adults. Canadian Medical Association Neurotoxicity of methylenedioxyamphetamines
Journal, 166, 887–891. (MDMA; ecstasy) in humans: How strong is the
Fried, P. A., Watkinson, B., & Gray, R. (2005). evidence for persistent brain damage? Addiction,
Neurocognitive consequences of marihuana—a 101, 348–361.
comparison with pre-drug performance. Neuro- Gouzoulis-Mayfrank, E., & Daumann, J. (2006b).
toxicology and Teratology, 27, 231–239. The confounding problem of polydrug use in rec-
Gamma, A. (2000). 3,4-Methylenedioxymethamphet- reational ecstasy/MDMA users: A brief overview.
amine (MDMA) modulates cortical and limbic brain Journal of Psychopharmacology, 20, 188–193.
activity as measured by[H 215 O]-PET in healthy Gouzoulis-Mayfrank, E., Daumann, J., Tuchtenhagen,
humans. Neuropsychopharmacology, 23, 388–395. F., Pelz, S., Becker, S., Kunert, H. J., et al. (2000).
Gerra, G., Calbiani, B., Zaimovic, A., Sartori, R., Impaired cognitive performance in drug free
Ugolotti, G., Ippolito, L., et al. (1998). Regional users of recreational ecstasy (MDMA). Journal
cerebral blood flow and comorbid diagnosis in of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 68,
abstinent opioid addicts. Psychiatry Research, 83, 719–725.
117–126. Gouzoulis-Mayfrank, E., Fischermann, T., Rezk, M.,
Glass, M., Dragunow, M., & Faull, R. L. M. (1997). Thimm, B., Hensen, G., & Daumann, J. (2005).
Cannabinoid receptors in the human brain: A Memory performance in polyvalent MDMA
detailed anatomical and quantitative autora- (ecstasy) users who continue or discontinue
diographic study in the fetal, neonatal and adult MDMA use. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 78,
human brain. Neuroscience, 77, 299–318. 317–323.
Goldstein, R. Z., Leskovjan, A. C., Hoff, A. L., Grant, I., Gonzalez, R., Carey, C. L., Natarajan, L., &
Hitzemann, R., Bashan, F., Khalsa, S. S., et al. Wolfson, T. (2003). Non-acute (residual) neurocog-
(2004). Severity of neuropsychological impairment nitive effects of cannabis use: A meta-analytic
in cocaine and alcohol addiction: Association study. Journal of the International Neuropsy-
with metabolism in the prefrontal cortex. chological Society, 9, 679–689.
Neuropsychologia, 42, 1447–1458. Grant, I., Adams, K. M., Carlin, A. S., Rennick, P. M.,
Goldstein, R. Z., & Volkow, N. D. (2002). Drug addic- Judd, L. L., & Schooff, K. (1978). The collabora-
tion and its underlying neurobiological basis: tive neuropsychological study of polydrug users.
Neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of Archives of General Psychiatry, 35, 1063–1074.
474 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Grant, I. & Mohns, L. (1975). Chronic Cerebral performance and physiology. Psychopharmacology,
Effects of Alcohol and Drug Abuse. Substance Use 102, 451–458.
& Misuse, 10, 883–920. Hill, S. Y., & Mikhael, M. A. (1979). Computerized
Grant, I., Rochford, J., Fleming, T., & Stunkard, A. transaxial tomographic and neuropsychological
(1973). A neuropsychological assessment of the evaluations in chronic alcohol and heroin abusers.
effects of moderate marihuana use. Journal of American Journal of Psychiatry, 136, 598–602.
Nervous and Mental Disease, 156, 278–280. Hoff man, W. F., Moore, M., Templin, R., McFarland,
Green, A. R., Mechan, A. O., Elliott, J. M., O’Shea, E., & B., Hitzemann, R. J., & Mitchell, S. H. (2006).
Colado, M. I. (2003). The pharmacology and clini- Neuropsychological function and delay discount-
cal pharmacology of 3,4-methylenedioxymetham- ing in methamphetamine-dependent individuals.
phetamine (MDMA, “ecstasy”). Pharmacology Psychopharmacology, 188(2), 162–170.
Review, 55, 463–508. Hughes, J., Smith, T. W., Kosterlitz, H. W., Fothergill,
Grotenhermen, F. (2003). Pharmacokinetics and L. A., Morgan, B. A., & Morris, H. R. (1975).
pharmacodynamics of cannabinoids. Clinical Identification of two related pentapeptides from
Pharmacokinetics, 42, 327–360. the brain with potent opiate agonist activity.
Guerra, D., Sole, A., Cami, J., & Tobena, A. (1987). Nature, 258, 577–580.
Neuropsychological performance in opiate addicts Isbell, H., & Fraser, H. F. (1950). Addiction to anal-
after rapid detoxification. Drug and Alcohol gesics and barbiturates. Pharmacological Reviews,
Dependence, 20, 261–270. 2, 355–397.
Haefely, W. E. (1978). Central actions of benzodiaz- Jacobs, I. G., Roszler, M. H., Kelly, J. K., Klein, M. A., &
epines: General introduction. British Journal of Kling, G. A. (1989). Cocaine abuse: Neurovascular
Psychiatry, 133, 231–238. complications. Radiology, 170, 223–227.
Halpern, J. H., Pope, H. G., Jr., Sherwood, A. R., Jaffe, C., Bush, K. R., Straits-Troster, K., Meredith,
Barry, S., Hudson, J. I., & Yurgelun-Todd, D. (2004). C., Romwall, L., Rosenbaum, G., et al. (2005). A
Residual neuropsychological effects of illicit comparison of methamphetamine-dependent
3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) inpatients childhood attention deficit hyperactiv-
in individuals with minimal exposure to other ity disorder symptomatology. Journal on Addictive
drugs. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 75, 135–147. Disorders, 24(3), 133–152.
Hamil, W. L. (1996). Auditory learning and mem- Johanson, C. E., Frey, K. A., Lundahl, L. H., Keenan,
ory performance among veterans with a his- P., Lockhart, N., Roll, J., et al. (2006). Cognitive
tory of stimulant abuse. Dissertation Abstracts function and nigrostriatal markers in abstinent
International: Section B: The Sciences and methamphetamine abusers. Psychopharmacology,
Engineering, 56, 5806. 185(3), 327–338.
Hatsukami, D. K., & Fischman, M. W. (1996). Crack Johnson, B. A., Roache, J. D., Ait-Daoud, N., Wallace,
cocaine and cocaine hydrochloride. Are the differ- C., Wells, L. T., & Wang, Y. (2005). Effects of isra-
ences a myth or reality? Journal of the American dipine on methamphetamine-induced changes in
Medical Association, 276, 1580–1588. attentional and perceptual-motor skills of cogni-
Hendler, N., Cimini, C., & Ma, T. (1980). A com- tion. Psychopharmacology, 178(2–3), 296–302.
parison of cognitive impairment due to benzo- Johnson, B., Overton, D., Wells, L., Kenny, P.,
diazepines and to narcotics. American Journal of Abramson, D., Dhother, S., et al. (1998). Effects of
Psychiatry, 137, 828–830. acute intravenous cocaine on cardiovascular func-
Hernandez-Lopez, C., Farre, M., Roset, P. N., tion, human learning, and performance in cocaine
Menoyo, E., Pizarro, N., Ortun o, J., et al. (2002). addicts. Psychiatry Research, 77, 35–42.
3,4-Methylenedioxymethamphetamine (Ecstasy) Jovanovski, D., Erb, S., & Zakzanis K. K. (2005).
and alcohol interactions in humans: Psychomotor Neurocognitive deficits in cocaine users: A quan-
performance, subjective effects, and pharmacoki- titative review of the evidence. Journal of Clinical
netics. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental and Experimental Neuropsychology, 27, 189–204.
Therapeutics, 300, 236–244. Kalechstein, A. D., De La, G. R., Mahoney, J. J., III,
Hester, R., Simoes-Franklin, C., & Garavan, H. (2007). Fantegrossi, W. E., & Newton, T. F. (2007). MDMA
Post-error behavior in active cocaine users: Poor use and neurocognition: A meta-analytic review.
awareness of errors in the presence of intact per- Psychopharmacology, 189, 531–537.
formance adjustments. Neuropsychopharmacology, Kalechstein, A. D., Newton, T. F., & Green, M. (2003).
32, 1974–1984. Methamphetamine dependence is associated with
Higgins, S. T., Bickel, W. K., Hughes, J. R., Lynn, neurocognitive impairment in the initial phases
M., Capeless, M. A., & Fenwick, J. W. (1990). of abstinence. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and
Effects of intranasal cocaine on human learning, Clinical Neurosciences, 15(2), 215–220.
Neuropsychological Consequences of Drug Abuse 475

Kelleher, L. M., Stough, C., Sergejew, A. A., & Rolfe, vigilance in recently abstinent methamphetamine
T. (2004). The effects of cannabis on informa- abusers. Biological Psychiatry, 58(10), 770–778.
tion-processing speed. Addictive Behaviors, 29, London, E. D., Broussolle, E. P., Links, J. M., Wong,
1213–1219. D. F., Cascella, N. G., Dannals, R. F., et al. (1990).
Kirby, K. N., & Petry, N. M. (2004). Heroin and Morphine-induced metabolic changes in human
cocaine abusers have higher discount rates for brain. Studies with positron emission tomography
delayed rewards than alcoholics or non-drug-us- and [fluorine 18] fluorodeoxyglucose. Archives of
ing controls. Addiction, 9, 461–471. General Psychiatry, 47, 73–81.
Kirby, K. N., Petry, N. M., & Bickel, W. K. (1999). Lucki, I., & Rickels, K. (1986). The behavioral effects
Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for of benzodiazepines following long-term use.
delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 22, 424–433.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, Lyons, M. J., Bar, J. L., Panizzon, M. S., Toomey, R.,
78–87. Eisen, S., Xian, H., et al. (2004). Neuropsychological
Kivisaari, R., Kakhonen, S., Puuskari, V., Jokela, consequences of regular marijuana use: A twin
O., Rapeli, P., & Autti, T. (2004). Magnetic reso- study. Psychological Medicine, 34, 1239–1250.
nance imaging of severe, long-term, opiate-abuse Lyvers, M., & Hasking, P. (2004). Have Halpern et al.
patients without neurologic symptoms may show (2004) detected “residual neuropsychological
enlarged cerebrospinal spaces but no signs of brain effects” of MDMA? Not likely. Drug and Alcohol
pathology of vascular origin. Archives of Medical Dependence, 75, 149–152.
Research, 35, 395–400. Lyvers, M., & Yakimoff, M. (2003). Neuropsychological
Korin, H. (1974). Comparison of psychometric mea- correlates of opiate dependence and withdrawal.
sures in psychiatric patients using heroin and Addictive Behaviors, 28, 605–611.
other drugs. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 83, Martin-Soelch, C., Chevalley, A. F., Kunig, G.,
208–212. Missimer, J., Magyar, S., Mino, A., et al. (2001).
Krystal, J. H., Woods, S. W., Kosten, T. R., Rosen, M. Changes in reward-induced brain activation i n
I., Seibyl, J. P., van Dyck, C. C., et al. (1995). Opiate opiate addicts. European Journal of Neuroscience,
dependence and withdrawal: Preliminary assess- 14, 1360–1368.
ment using single photon emission computerized Matsuda, L. A., Lolait, S. J., Brownstein, M. J., Young,
tomography (SPECT). American Journal of Drug A. C., & Bonner, T. I. (1990). Structure of a canna-
and Alcohol Abuse, 21, 47–63. binoid receptor and functional expression of the
Kuypers, K. P., & Ramaekers, J. G. (2005). Transient cloned cDNA. Nature, 346, 561–564.
memory impairment after acute dose of 75mg 3.4- McCann, U. D., Mertl, M., Eligulashvili, V., &
Methylene-dioxymethamphetamine. Journal of Ricaurte, G. A. (1999). Cognitive performance
Psychopharmacology, 19, 633–639. in (¦) 3, 4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine
Kuypers, K. P., & Ramaekers, J. G. (2007). Acute dose (MDMA, “ecstasy”) users: A controlled study.
of MDMA (75 mg) impairs spatial memory for loca- Psychopharmacology, 143, 417–425.
tion but leaves contextual processing of visuospa- McCardle, K., Luebbers, S., Carter, J. D., Croft , R.
tial information unaffected. Psychopharmacology J., & Stough, C. (2004). Chronic MDMA (ecstasy)
(Berl), 189, 557–563. use, cognition and mood. Psychopharmacology,
Lamers, C. T. J., Ramaekers, J. G., Muntjewerff, N. 173, 434–439.
D., Sikkema, K. L., Riedel, W. J., Samyn, N., et al. Mechoulam, R., & Gaoni, Y. (1967). The absolute con-
(2003). Dissociable effects of a single dose of ecstasy figuration of delta-1-tetrahydrocannabinol, the
(MDMA) on psychomotor skills and attentional major active constituent of hashish. Tetrahedron
performance. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 17, Letters, 12, 1109–1111.
379. Merlin, M. D. (2003). Archaeological evidence for
Leri, F., Bruneau, J., & Stewart, J. (2003). the tradition of psychoactive plant use in the old
Understanding polydrug use: Review of heroin world. Economic Botany, 57, 295–323.
and cocaine co-use. Addiction, 98, 7–22. Messinis, L., Kyprianidou, A., Malefaki, S., &
Levine, S. R., Brust, J. C., Futrell, N., Brass, L. M., Papathanasopoulos, P. (2006). Neuropsychological
Blake, D., Fayad, P., et al. (1991). A comparative deficits in long-term frequent cannabis users.
study of the cerebrovascular complications of Neurology, 66, 737–739.
cocaine: Alkaloidal versus hydrochloride-a review. Mintzer, M. Z., & Stitzer, M. L. (2002). Cognitive
Neurology, 41, 1173–1177. impairment in methadone maintenance patients.
London, E. D., Berman, S. M., Voytek, B., Simon, S. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 67, 41–51.
L., Mandelkern, M. A., Monterosso, J., et al. (2005). Mohs, R. C., Tinklenberg, J. R., Roth, W. T., & Kopell,
Cerebral metabolic dysfunction and impaired B. S. (1980). Sensitivity of some human cognitive
476 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

functions to effects of methamphetamine and Parrott, A. C. (2004). Is ecstasy MDMA? A review


secobarbital. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 5(2), of the proportion of ecstasy tablets containing
145–150. MDMA, their dosage levels, and the changing
Monterosso, J. R., Ainslie, G., Xu, J., Cordova, perceptions of purity. Psychopharmacology (Berl),
X., Domier, C. P., & London, E. D. (2006). 173, 234–241.
Frontoparietal cortical activity of methamphet- Parrott, A. C., Lees, A., Garnham, N. J., Jones, M., &
amine-dependent and comparison subjects per- Wesnes, K. (1998). Cognitive performance in
forming a delay discounting task. Human Brain recreational users of MDMA or “ecstasy”: Evidence
Mapping, 28, 383–393. for memory deficits. Journal of Psychophar-
Monterosso, J. R., Aron, A. R., Cordova, X., Xu, J., macology, 12, 79.
& London, E. D. (2005). Deficits in response inhi- Paulus, M. P., Hozack, N. E., Zauscher, B. E.,
bition associated with chronic methamphetamine Frank, L., Brown, G. G., Braff, D. L., et al. (2002).
abuse. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 79(2), Behavioral and functional neuroimaging evidence
273–277. for prefrontal dysfunction in methamphetamine-
Morgan, M. (1998). Recreational use of Ecstasy dependent subjects. Neuropsychopharmacology,
(MDMA) is associated with elevated impulsivity. 26(1), 53–63.
Neuropsychopharmacology, 19, 252–264. Paulus, M. P., Tapert, S. F., & Schuckit, M. A. (2005).
Morgan, M., McFie, L., Fleetwood, L., & Robinson, Neural activation patterns of methamphetamine-
J. (2002). Ecstasy (MDMA): Are the psycholog- dependent subjects during decision making pre-
ical problems associated with its use reversed by dict relapse. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(7),
prolonged abstinence? Psychopharmacology, 159, 761–768.
294–303. Pert, C. B., & Snyder, S. H. (1973). Opiate receptor:
Morgan, M. J. (1999). Memory deficits associated Demonstration in nervous tissue. Science, 179,
with recreational use of “ecstasy” (MDMA). 1011.
Psychopharmacology, 141, 30–36. Petursson, H., Gudjonsson, G. H., & Lader, M. H.
Moszczynska, A., Fitzmaurice, P., Ang, L., (1983). Psychometric performance during with-
Kalasinsky, K. S., Schmunk, G. A., Peretti, F. J., drawal from long-term benzodiazepine treatment.
et al. (2004). Why is parkinsonism not a feature Psychopharmacology, 81, 345–349.
of human methamphetamine users? Brain, 127(Pt Pezawas, L. M., Fischer, G., Diamant, K., Schneider,
2), 363–370. C., Schindler, S. D., Thurnher, M., et al. (1998).
Nichols, D. E., Hoff man, A. J., Oberlender, R. A., Cerebral CT findings in male opioid-dependent
Jacob, P., III, & Shulgin, A. T. (1986). Derivatives patients: Stereological, planimetric and linear
of 1-(1,3-benzodioxol-5-yl)-2-butanamine: measurements. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging
Representatives of a novel therapeutic class. Section, 83, 139–147.
Journal of Medicinal Chemistry, 29, 2009–2015. Pezawas, L., Fischer, G., Podreka, I., Schindler, S.,
O’Brien, C. P. (2005). Benzodiazepine use, abuse, Brucke, T., Jagsch, R., et al. (2002). Opioid addic-
and dependence. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, tion changes cerebral blood flow symmetry.
66(Suppl 2), 28–33. Neuropsychobiology, 45, 67–73.
O’Malley, S. S., & Gawin, F. H. (1990). Abstinence Pfeffer, A. Z., & Ruble, D. C. (1948). Chronic psy-
symptomatology and neuropsychological impair- chosis and addiction to morphine. Archives of
ment in chronic cocaine abusers. NIDA Research Neurology and Psychiatry, 56, 665–672.
Monographs, 101, 179–190. Pope, H. G., Jr. (2002). Cannabis, cognition, and
Ornstein, T. J., Iddon, J. L., Baldacchino, A. M., residual confounding. Journal of the American
Sahakian, B. J., London, M., Everitt, B. J., et al. Medical Association, 287, 1172–1174.
(2000). Profi les of cognitive dysfunction in Pope, H. G., Gruber, A. J., & Yurgelun-Todd, D.
chronic amphetamine and heroin abusers. (1995). The residual neuropsychological effects of
Neuropsychopharmacology, 23, 113–126. cannabis—the current status of research. Drug
Papageorgiou, C., Rabavilas, A., Liappas, I., & Stefanis, and Alcohol Dependence, 38, 25–34.
C. (2003). Do obsessive-compulsive patients and Pope, H. G., Gruber, A. J., Hudson, J. I., Huestis, M. A.,
abstinent heroin addicts share a common psycho- & Yurgelun-Todd, D. (2001). Neuropsychological
physiological mechanism? Neuropsychobiology, performance in long-term cannabis users. Archives
47, 1–11. of General Psychiatry, 58, 909–915.
Parrott, A. C. (2001). Human psychopharmacol- Pope, H. G., Jr., & Yurgelun-Todd, D. (1996). The
ogy of Ecstasy (MDMA): A review of 15 years of residual cognitive effects of heavy marijuana use in
empirical research. Human Psychopharmacology: college students. Journal of the American Medical
Clinical and Experimental, 16, 557–577. Association, 275, 521–527.
Neuropsychological Consequences of Drug Abuse 477

Ranganathan, M., & D’Souza, D. C. (2006). The acute Rogers, R. D., Everitt, B. J., Baldacchino, A.,
effects of cannabinoids on memory in humans: A Blackshaw, A. J., Swainson, R., Wynne, K., et al.
review. Psychopharmacology, 188, 425–444. (1999). Dissociable deficits in the decision-making
Regier, D. A., Farmer, M. E., Rae, D. S., Locke, B. Z., cognition of chronic amphetamine abusers, opiate
Keith, S. J., Judd, L. L., et al. (1990). Comorbidity abusers, patients with focal damage to prefrontal
of mental disorders with alcohol and other drug cortex, and tryptophan-depleted normal volun-
abuse. Results from the Epidemiologic Catchment teers: Evidence for monoaminergic mechanisms.
Area (ECA) Study. Journal of the American Medical Neuropsychopharmacology, 20, 322–339.
Association, 264, 2511–2518. Rogers, R. D., & Robbins, T. W. (2001). Investigating
Reneman, L., Booij, J., Schmand, B., van den the neurocognitive deficits associated with chronic
Brink, W., & Gunning, B. (2000). Memory dis- drug misuse. Current Opinions in Neurobiology,
turbances in Ecstasy users are correlated with 11, 250–257.
an altered brain serotonin neurotransmission. Rose, J. S., Branchey, M., Buydens-Branchey, L.,
Psychopharmacology, 148, 322–324. Stapleton, J. M., Chasten, K., Werrell, A., et al.
Reneman, L., Lavalaye, J., Schmand, B., de Wolff, F. A., (1996). Cerebral perfusion in early and late opiate
van den Brink, W., den Heeten, G. J., et al. (2001a). withdrawal: A technetium-99m-HMPAO SPECT
Cortical serotonin transporter density and ver- study. Psychiatry Research, 31, 39–47.
bal memory in individuals who stopped using Roselli, M., & Ardila, A. (1996). Cognitive effects of
3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA or cocaine and polydrug abuse. Journal of Clinical
“Ecstasy”): Preliminary findings. Archives of General and Experimental Neuropsychology, 18, 122–135.
Psychiatry, 58, 901–906. Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., Lubomski, M., Murray, S., &
Reneman, L., Majoie, C. B. L. M., Schmand, B., King, K. (2001). Personality profi le and neuropsy-
van den Brink, W., & den Heeten, G. J. (2001b). chological test performance in chronic cocaine-
Prefrontal N-acetylaspartate is strongly associated abusers. International Journal of Neuroscience,
with memory performance in (abstinent) ecstasy 110, 55–72.
users: Preliminary report. Biological Psychiatry, Rotheram-Fuller, E., Shoptaw, S., Berman, S. M., &
50, 550–554. London, E. D. (2004). Impaired performance in
Reynolds, E. H., & Trimble, M. R. (1985). Adverse a test of decision-making by opiate-dependent
neuropsychiatric effects of anticonvulsant drugs. tobacco smokers. Drug and Alcohol Dependence,
Drugs, 29, 570–581. 73, 79–86.
Rice, D. P. (1999). Economic costs of substance abuse, Rounsaville, B. J., Jones, C., Novelly, R. A., & Kleber, H.
1995. Proceedings of the Association of American (1982). Neuropsychological functioning in opiate
Physicians, 111, 119–125. addicts. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disorders,
Rickels, K., Lucki, I., Schweizer, E., Garcia-Espana, F., 170, 209–216.
& Case, W. G. (1999). Psychomotor performance Sakol, M. S. & Power, K. G. (1988). The effects of
of long-term benzodiazepine users before, during, long-term benzodiazepine treatment and graded
and after benzodiazepine discontinuation. Journal withdrawal on psychometric performance. Psycho-
of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 19, 107–113. pharmacology, 95, 135–138.
Rippeth, J. D., Heaton, R. K., Carey, C. L., Marcotte, Salo, R., Nordahl, T. E., Moore, C., Waters, C.,
T. D., Moore, D. J., Gonzalez, R., et al. (2004). Natsuaki, Y., Galloway, G. P., et al. (2005). A
Methamphetamine dependence increases risk dissociation in attentional control: Evidence
of neuropsychological impairment in HIV from methamphetamine dependence. Biological
infected persons. Journal of the International Psychiatry, 57(3), 310–313.
Neuropsychological Society, 10(1), 1–14. Salo, R., Nordahl, T. E., Natsuaki, Y., Leamon,
Rochford, J., Grant, I., & LaVigne, G. (1977). Medical M. H., Galloway, G. P., Waters, C., et al. (2007).
students and drugs: Further neuropsychologi- Attentional control and brain metabolite levels in
cal and use pattern considerations. International methamphetamine abusers. Biological Psychiatry,
Journal of the Addictions, 12, 1057–1065. 61, 1272–1280.
Robinson, J. E., Heaton, R. K., & O’Malley, S. S. (1999). Salo, R., Nordahl, T. E., Possin, K., Leamon, M.,
Neuropsychological functioning in cocaine abus- Gibson, D. R., Galloway, G. P., et al. (2002).
ers with and without alcohol dependence. Journal Preliminary evidence of reduced cognitive inhi-
of the International Neuropsychological Society, 5, bition in methamphetamine-dependent individu-
10–19. als. Psychiatry Research, 111(1), 65–74.
Rodgers, J. (2000). Cognitive performance amongst Salzman, C., Fisher, J., Nobel, K., Glassman, R.,
recreational users of “ecstasy”. Psychopharma- Wolfson, A., & Kelley, M. (1992). Cognitive
cology, 151, 19–24. improvement following discontinuation in elderly
478 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

nursing home residents. International Journal of Emergency Department Visits (DAWN Series
Geriatric Psychiatry, 7, 89–93. D-28, DHHS Publication No. SMA 06–4143).
Selby, M. J., & Azrin, R. L. (1998). Neuropsychological Rockville, MD: Office of Applied Studies.
functioning in drug abusers. Drug and Alcohol Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services
Dependence, 50, 39–45. Administration. (2006b). Results from the 2005
Sim, T., Simon, S. L., Domier, C. P., Richardson, K., National Survey on Drug Use and Health: National
Rawson, R. A., & Ling, W. (2002). Cognitive Findings (Office of Applied Studies, NSDUH Series
deficits among methamphetamine users with H-30, DHHS Publication No. SMA 06–4194).
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symp- Rockville, MD: Office of Applied studies.
tomatology. Journal of Addictive Diseases, 21(1), Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services
75–89. Administration. (2006c). Treatment Episode Data
Simon, S. L., Dacey, J., Glynn, S., Rawson, R., & Set (TEDS): 1994–2004 (DASIS Series: S-33, DHHS
Ling, W. (2004). The effect of relapse on cognition Publication No. SMA 064180). Rockville, MD:
in abstinent methamphetamine abusers. Journal Office of Applied Studies.
of Substance Abuse Treatment, 27(1), 59–66. Tanner-Smith, E. E. (2006). Pharmacological content
Simon, S. L., Domier, C., Carnell, J., Brethen, P., of tablets sold as “ecstasy”: Results from an online
Rawson, R., & Ling, W. (2000). Cognitive impair- testing service. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 83,
ment in individuals currently using metham- 247–254.
phetamine. American Journal on Addictions, 9(3), Tata, P. R., Rollings, J., Collins, M., Pickering, A., &
222–231. Jacobson, R. R. (1994). Lack of cognitive recovery
Simon, S. L., Domier, C. P., Sim, T., Richardson, K., following withdrawal from long-term benzodiaz-
Rawson, R. A., & Ling, W. (2002). Cognitive per- epine use. Psychological Medicine, 24, 203–213.
formance of current methamphetamine and Terenius, L., (1973). Stereospecific interaction
cocaine abusers. Journal of Addictive Diseases, between narcotic analgesics and a synaptic plasma
21(1), 61–74. membrane fraction of rat brain cortex. Acta
Simon, E. J., Hiller, J. M., & Edelman, I. (1973). Pharmacologica et Toxicologica, 32, 317.
Stereospecific binding of the potent narcotic United Nations Office on Drugs and Crimes. (2004).
analgesic [3H]etorphine to rat brain homogenate. World Drug Report. United Nations Publications.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, Thompson, P. M., Hayashi, K. M., Simon, S. L., Geaga,
70, 1947. J. A., Hong, M. S., Sui, Y., et al. (2004). Structural
Specka, M., Finkbeiner, T., Lodemann, E., Leifert, K., abnormalities in the brains of human subjects who
Kluwig, J., & Gastpar, M. (2000). Cognitive-motor use methamphetamine. Journal of Neuroscience,
performance of methadone-maintained patients. 24(26), 6028–6036.
European Addiction Research, 6, 8–19. Tonne, U., Hiltunen, A. J., Vikander, B.,
Solowij, N. (1995). Do cognitive impairments recover Engelbrektsson, K., Bergman, H., Bergman, I.,
following cessation of cannabis use? Life Sciences, et al. (1995). Neuropsychological changes during
56, 2119–2126. steady-state drug use, withdrawal and abstinence
Solowij, N., Stephens, R. S., Roff man, R. A., Babor, in primary benzodiazepine-dependent patients.
T., Kadden, R., Miller, M., et al. (2002). Cognitive Acta Psychiatrica Scandanivica, 91, 299–304.
functioning of long-term heavy cannabis users Van Gorp, W. G., Wilkins, J. N., Hinkin, C. G.,
seeking treatment. Journal of the American Moore, L. H., Hull, J., Horner, M. D., et al. (1999).
Medical Association, 287, 1123–1131. Declarative and procedural memory functioning
Strang, J., & Gurling, H. (1989). Computerized in abstinent cocaine abusers. Archives of General
tomography and neuropsychological associa- Psychiatry, 56, 85–89.
tions in long term heroin addicts. Addiction, 85, Vanyukov, M. M., Tarter, R. E., Kirisci, L., Kirillova,
1011–1019. G. P., Maher, B. S., & Clark, D. B. (2003). Liability
Strickland, T. L., Mena, I., Villanueva-Meyer, J., to substance use disorders: 1. Common mecha-
Miller, B. L., Cummings, J., Mehringer, C. M., et al. nisms and manifestations. Neurosciences and
(1993). Cerebral perfusion and neuropsychologi- Biobehavioral Reviews, 27, 507–515.
cal consequences of chronic cocaine use. Journal Verbaten, M. N. (2003). Specific memory deficits
of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 5, in ecstasy users? The results of a meta-analysis.
419–427. Human Psychopharmacology, 18, 281–290.
Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Verdejo-Garcia, A., Benbrook, A., Funderburk, F.,
Administration. (2006a). Drug Abuse Warning David, P., Cadet, J. L., & Bolla, K. I. (2007a). The
Network, 2004: National Estimates of Drug-Related differential relationship between cocaine use and
Neuropsychological Consequences of Drug Abuse 479

marijuana use on decision-making performance recovery of brain metabolism in methamphet-


over repeat testing with the Iowa Gambling Task. amine abusers after protracted abstinence.
Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 90, 2–11. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161(2), 242–248.
Verdejo-Garcia, A., & Perez-Garcia, M. (2007). Wareing, M., Fisk, J. E., & Murphy, P. N. (2000).
Profi le of executive deficits in cocaine and her- Working memory deficits in current and previ-
oin polysubstance users: Common and differ- ous users of MDMA (ecstasy). British Journal of
ential effects on separate executive components. Psychology, 91, 181–188.
Psychopharmacology, 190, 517–530. Wert, R. C., & Raulin, M. L. (1986). The chronic
Verdejo-Garcia, A. J., Perales, J. C., & Perez-Garcia, cerebral effects of cannabis use. II. Psychological
M. (2007b). Cognitive impulsivity in cocaine and findings and conclusions. International Journal of
heroin polysubstance abusers. Addictive Behaviors, Addictions, 21, 629–642.
32, 950–966. Whitlow, C. T., Liguori, A., Livengood, L. B.,
Verkes, R. J., Gijsman, H. J., Pieters, M. S. M., Hart, S. L., Mussat-Whitlow, B. J., Lamborn,
Schoemaker, R. C., de Visser, S., Kuijpers, M., et al. C. M., et al. (2004). Long-term heavy marijuana
(2001). Cognitive performance and serotonergic users make costly decisions on a gambling task.
function in users of ecstasy. Psychopharmacology, Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 76, 107–111.
153, 196–202. Wig, N. N., & Varma, V. K. (1977). Patterns of
Vignola, A., Lamoureux, C., Bastien, C. H., & Morin, long-term heavy cannabis use in North India
C. M. (2000). Effects of chronic insomnia and use of and its effects on cognitive functions: A prelim-
benzodiazepines on daytime performance in older inary report. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 2,
adults. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psycholo- 211–219.
gical Sciences and Social Sciences, 55, 54–62. Wilson, J. M., Kalasinsky, K. S., Levey, A. I.,
Volkow, N. D., Chang, L., Wang, G. J., Fowler, J. S., Bergeron, C., Reiber, G., Anthony, R. M., et al.
Franceschi, D., Sedler, M., et al. (2001a). Loss of (1996). Striatal dopamine nerve terminal mark-
dopamine transporters in methamphetamine ers in human, chronic methamphetamine users.
abusers recovers with protracted abstinence. Nature Medicine, 2(6), 699–703.
Journal of Neuroscience, 21(23), 9414–9418. Woods, S. P., Rippeth, J. D., Conover, E., Gongvatana,
Volkow, N. D., Chang, L., Wang, G. J., Fowler, J. S., A., Gonzalez, R., Carey, C. L., et al. (2005).
Leonido-Yee, M., Franceschi, D., et al. (2001b). Deficient strategic control of verbal encoding and
Association of dopamine transporter reduction retrieval in individuals with methamphetamine
with psychomotor impairment in methamphet- dependence. Neuropsychology, 19(1), 35–43.
amine abusers. American Journal of Psychiatry, Zacny, J. P. (2003). Characterizing the subjective,
158(3), 377–382. psychomotor, and physiological effects of a hydro-
Volkow, N. D., Fowler, J. S., Wang, G. J., Hitzemann, codone combination product (Hycodan) in non-
R., Logan, J., Schlyer, D. J., et al. (1993). Decreased drug-abusing volunteers. Psychopharmacology,
dopamine D2 receptor availability is associated 165, 146–156.
with reduced frontal metabolism in cocaine abus- Zacny, J. P., & Gutierrez, S. (2003). Characterizing
ers. Synapse, 14, 169–177. the subjective, psychomotor, and physiological
Volkow, N. D., Hitzemann, R., Wang, G. J., Fowler, effects of oral oxycodone in non-drug-abusing
J. S., Wolf, A. P., Dewey, S. L., et al. (1992). Long- volunteers. Psychopharmacology, 170, 242–254.
term frontal brain metabolic changes in cocaine Zakzanis, K. K., & Campbell, Z. (2006). Memory
abusers. Synapse, 11, 184–190. impairment in now abstinent MDMA users
Vollenweider, F. (1998). Psychological and cardiovas- and continued users: A longitudinal follow-up.
cular effects and short-term sequelae of MDMA Neurology, 66, 740–741.
(Ecstasy) in MDMA-naive healthy volunteers. Zakzanis, K. K., & Young, D. A. (2001). Memory
Neuropsychopharmacology, 19, 241–251. impairment in abstinent MDMA (“Ecstasy”)
Wang, G. J., Volkow, N. D., Chang, L., Miller, E., users: A longitudinal investigation. Neurology, 56,
Sedler, M., Hitzemann, R., et al. (2004). Partial 966–969.
20

Neuropsychological, Neurological, and


Neuropsychiatric Correlates of Exposure
to Metals
Roberta F. White and Patricia A. Janulewicz

The neurotoxic properties of metals have been or occupational exposures to metals and had
known for centuries. For example, hatters’ use symptoms of central nervous system (CNS)
of mercury, with associated behavioral anoma- dysfunction but did not reach criteria for a clin-
lies was known in the 1800s, and the behavioral ical diagnosis of intoxication, and in persons
changes and confusional states associated with who appeared to be asymptomatic with such
occupational and environmental lead expo- exposures (Baker, Jr. et al., 1983; Haenninen
sure were described even earlier. In the fi rst et al., 1978; Valciukas & Lilis, 1980). For the
half of the twentieth century, the syndrome of purposes of epidemiological research on indi-
lead poisoning among children with environ- viduals with occupational and environmental
mental exposures to paint was well described exposures to toxicants such as heavy metals,
and treated, and dementia and death associ- neuropsychological test methods offered sen-
ated with lead exposure resulting from drink- sitive indicators of brain function with out-
ing bootleg liquor produced in lead-bearing comes that were based on standardized test
equipments were well documented. In the administration and scoring rules, validated
1960s and 1970s, methylmercury poisoning with regard to the underlying brain–behavior
was identified in Minamata (Japan) and Iraq relationships revealed by the tests, and associ-
following environmental exposures of contam- ated with normative data that allowed estimate
inated food, resulting in clinical descriptions of effect magnitude and clinical relevance of
of the associated neurological symptoms and exposure-associated decrements in test scores.
in well-defi ned descriptions of the neuropath- This quantification of subtle brain function was
ological manifestations of methylmercury in key to uncovering the neurotoxic properties of
the brains of children and adults (Choi, 1989). many toxicants, including metals, whose neu-
These instances of metal toxicity were identi- rotoxicity was previously unknown.
fied from obvious symptoms and sequelae of In addition to their research applications,
very large exposures (both acute and chronic), neuropsychological test methods slowly became
with intoxication/poisoning and clear-cut accepted as valid clinical indicators of brain
clinical encephalopathy. dysfunction allowing the diagnosis of enceph-
In the latter half of the twentieth century, the alopathy among individuals with well-defined
application of neuropsychological test method- exposures to neurotoxic metals.
ology to the study of metal neurotoxicity cre- Research since the early 1900s, particularly
ated an explosion of new knowledge regarding using animal models (Feldman, 1999), has led
effects of metals on brain function in patients to increased knowledge about the neuropath-
with clinical manifestations of encephalopathy, ological, biochemical, and cellular effects of
in persons who had histories of environmental metal exposures. However, a great deal remains

480
Correlates of Exposure to Metals 481

unknown concerning the focal and diff use coal, and other manufacturing processes. A
effects of such exposures on the human brain, common form of organic mercury exposure
especially at low levels of exposure. Newly avail- (methylmercury) is related to consumption
able magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) meth- of seafood containing mercury that has been
ods such as automated quantification of specific biotransformed into an organic (methylated)
brain areas in structural MRI, MR spectros- form. Seafood consumption is probably the
copy (MRS), diff usion weighted tensor imaging most common source of environmental mer-
and functional MRI (fMRI) (Janulewicz et al., cury exposure (see below).
2006), positron emission tomography (PET) Arsenic is also a well-established neuro-
(Janulewicz et al., 2006), and near infra-red toxicant, though studies of exposure effects
imaging scans (NIRS), may shed more light on on human development are just beginning to
these issues in the next decades. While expen- emerge. Well-water exposures to arsenic are
sive and difficult to apply in epidemiologic set- common in the United States and other parts
tings, effective application of these methods in of the world, and exposure can also result from
targeted situations and with well-defined popu- pesticides, pigments, some paints, smelter pro-
lations may shed important light on the neuro- cesses, and manufacturing processes such as
pathological underpinnings of the CNS effects electroplating and semi-conductor production.
of such exposures in humans. Manganese is similarly emerging as a neurotox-
icant of primary research interest. Exposures
may be related to iron and steel industry pro-
Metals with Known Neurotoxicity
cesses, welding, manufacture of fireworks and
Exposure to several metals (and their associated matches, dry-cell battery production, fertiliz-
compounds) has been linked to encephalopathy. ers, and welding operations.
It is important to note that most of these sub- Thallium, which is used in rodenticides, fun-
stances are found naturally on earth, leading to gicides and photoelectric cells, is also found
potential exposure through soil, water, and air. naturally in rock formations and mica. Tin
Exposure to them has occurred across centuries compounds, particularly organotins, are also
of human evolution, and metals have been found neurotoxic. Chemists and chemical laboratory
in archeological sites and remains of humans workers are especially likely to be exposed to
buried centuries ago. This section briefly lists these compounds. Nickel and aluminum are
some of the most common neurotoxic metals also known to have neurotoxic properties.
and their sources of exposures. Clinical features
of exposure to them are described later in this
Issues in Behavioral Toxicology
introduction.
Lead and mercury (and their organic forms) How are metals defined as neurotoxic versus
have been investigated most extensively with non-neurotoxic? Such determinations vary by
regard to their clinical, neurological, neurobe- the individual and/or agency posing the ques-
havioral, neuropathological, and cellular effects tion. For some agencies, the essential criteria
across the lifespan. Industrial and domestic include animal and/or tissue models demon-
applications of lead that can result in exposure strating neurotoxic effects of experimental
include solders, lead shot/bullets, insecticides, exposures and evidence of effects in humans
batteries, smelting, paints, leaded glass, pipes from epidemiologic or experimental research.
and other lead-containing metal structures, In many cases, the initial evidence suggesting
and a number of manufacturing processes neurotoxicity has emanated from clinical case
(some of which use lead in its organic forms- reports on individuals or groups of patients
see below). Elemental mercury exposure can with substantial exposure and clinical evidence
result from dental amalgams (in dentists and of CNS disease. These reports may then lead to
technicians as well as patients), scientific and the development of animal and tissue models of
other instruments that use mercury indicators, exposure and to epidemiologic investigations of
fluorescent lights, electroplating, gold mining, individuals with acute and chronic exposures to
felt making, electrical power production from the metal in question.
482 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

An important concept in understanding the neuropsychological data reflect abnormalities


effects of metals and other neurotoxicants is that within domains that are known to be related
of clinical versus subclinical or preclinical mani- to the exposure of interest and that the results
festations of toxicant effects. Clinical manifesta- cannot be explained by some other factor such
tions of exposure result in obvious disease, with as a neurological, psychiatric or developmen-
signs, symptoms and/or abnormal laboratory tal disorder, motivational factors, or premorbid
results evident in the context of a well- demon- cognitive status. When evaluating the effects
strated exposure. At the other end of the scale of neurotoxic insult, it is easy to overestimate
is preclinical or subclinical evidence of neuro- exposure effects in persons with borderline pre-
toxicity, in which evidence of subtle CNS dys- morbid intellectual skills and to underestimate
function can be discerned in population studies such effects in very bright patients. A nomencla-
linking an outcome to a range of exposures. An ture has been developed for diagnosing toxicant-
example of the latter is the evidence for decline induced encephalopathies that considers effects
in Wechsler Intelligence Quotient (IQ) among of acute versus chronic exposure separately
children with lead exposure: a statistically sig- and evaluates the severity of the encephalop-
nificant mean 3-point decline in IQ related to athy (from mild, reversible to severe, irrevers-
low-level exposure might not appear to be “clin- ible brain damage) (White, 1992). In addition
ically significant” because it is not large enough to the diagnosis of encephalopathy, some met-
to diagnose a clinical decline in an individual als exposures result in clinical syndromes. For
patient. However, such a finding has great pub- example, manganese exposure is associated
lic health importance. First, it demonstrates with a parkinsonian syndrome (Feldman, 1999)
that lead affects CNS function. Furthermore, and neurological disorders such as amyotropic
it has tremendous implications for popula- lateral sclerosis and multiple sclerosis have been
tions of individuals: a 3-point shift downward linked in epidemiologic research to exposure to
in IQ within a population increases the num- lead, zinc and other metals (Beal, 1992; Garruto
ber of individuals with borderline IQ and et al., 1985).
decreases the number in the very superior range
(Bellinger, 2007; Bellinger & Bellinger, 2006).
Identifying and Quantifying
Such findings also have implications for regu-
Subclinical Effects of Metal
latory agencies in setting standards for accept-
Exposures
able exposures based on lowest level exposure
effects. Finally, evidence of sub-clinical effects Epidemiologic methods have been critical to
gives greater credence to patients’ symptom public health research on metal neurotoxic-
complaints in the absence of clinically obvious ity. Epidemiologic approaches include cross-
disease: they may be experiencing subtle expo- sectional, longitudinal, retrospective, and
sure effects that cannot be confirmed diagnosti- prospective study designs. Population and case-
cally with currently available technology. control methodology are both employed in these
approaches. Generally, greater credibility is
given to prospective data that clearly link expo-
Clinical Manifestations of Metal
sure (ideally presented as a continuous variable
Intoxication
and measured objectively in body tissue such as
Clinical evidence of neurotoxicity often rests blood or urine or in environmental samples of
on evidence of encephalopathy within exposed air, water or soil) to CNS outcomes. The dem-
individuals. Encephalopathies resulting from onstration of dose–effect relationships between
lead and mercury exposures are described in exposure and outcome is considered to be the
the sections on these metals that follow. These gold standard in such research. Comparisons
kinds of disorders are usually described in rela- between exposed and unexposed populations
tion to the causative agent (e.g., lead encepha- (with no clear quantification of exposure inten-
lopathy). Often, abnormal neuropsychological sity) may produce suggestive or preliminary evi-
test results are used to support these diagnoses. dence of effects of a neurotoxicant but are rarely
In such cases, it is essential that the supporting considered to be conclusive. This is partially due
Correlates of Exposure to Metals 483

to the likelihood of confounding in such stud- can also be apparent clinically in individuals,
ies: membership in the exposed or control group these sources of evidence exist despite the fact
may be systematically linked to another variable that there is great interindividual susceptibility
that better explains the outcomes than exposure to exposure to specific neurotoxicants. The same
itself. Of course, confounding can also be seen in dosage exposure can produce clinical disease in
research that examines dose–effect relationships one person while it has no apparent effects in
if intensity of exposure is systematically related someone else, individuals seem to vary in the
to a confounding variable. When neuropsycho- sites of brain lesions following exposure, and
logical test outcomes are used, critical potential exposures to some toxicants seem to be protec-
confounders or effect modifying variables that tive against development of neurological disease
must be considered when analyzing outcome data or degeneration. For example, if two siblings
include age, education, history of developmental carry the same genetic risk for Parkinson’s dis-
disorder of learning and/or attention, gender, ease (PD) and eventually develop PD, the sibling
ethnicity, socioeconomic class, history of neu- who smokes is likely to have later onset of the
rological disorder or insult, and medications. In disease (Myers, 2006). This illustration of gene–
the case of exposures occurring in adulthood, an environment interactions in protection likely
estimate of premorbid intellectual level is essen- applies to the issue of sites of action of toxicants
tial, and estimates for both the verbal and spatial and susceptibility to them. Furthermore, gene–
domains are preferable (personal communica- environment–gene interactions and other more
tions). These are generally based on “hold test” complex relationships between exposures and
scores on academic skills tests or Wechsler sub- CNS outcomes are almost certainly very impor-
tests. Other variables that sometimes impact test tant, at least for some diseases and some indi-
performance that should be considered include viduals. The exploration of these relationships
rural versus urban upbringing, familiarity with is in its earliest, most exploratory stages. But,
computers, parental education and/or vocation, studies of this kind will likely advance the field
and stimulation in the home. Although affective of behavioral toxicology dramatically in the
measures such as “depression” are often used as future and may shed light on the genetic and
potential confounders in research in behavioral environmental risk factors at play in the devel-
toxicology, it is important to consider such vari- opment of some neurodegenerative disorders
ables quite carefully and critically, since affective such as Alzheimer’s disease, cerebrovascular
changes occur frequently following exposures disease, Amyotropic Lateral Sclerosis (ALS),
to neurotoxicants in a dose-dependent manner and Parkinson’s disease.
(Baker, Jr. et al., 1983; White, 1992). Although
the use of data transformed through normative
Lead and Mercury
values to standard scores might appear to be an
attractive means of controlling for some poten- This chapter focuses on the two neurotoxic
tial confounders, we have consistently found that metals that have been studied the most inten-
use of raw score outcomes in data analysis, with sively—lead and mercury. We will begin with
statistical control of confounding variables is a review of the neurotoxicity of these metals
the most accurate and sensitive approach. Study in their various forms and the neuropatholog-
populations are often unique to themselves, ical effects of exposure in adults and children.
U.S. normative values can introduce unneces- We will emphasize neuropsychological find-
sary noise into the data even for Americans, and ings associated with exposures to these metals,
appropriate norms are not available for many though other evidence of neurotoxicity will be
study populations. mentioned briefly.

Gene–Environment Interactions Lead


Although large population studies can identify Lead is an ubiquitous metal found naturally (and
dose–effect relationships that confirm the neu- abundantly) in the earth. It is used extensively
rotoxic character of metals and neurotoxicity in manufacturing and as an additive to various
484 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

industrial chemicals and compounds, including Bone is the long-term storage compartment in
gasoline, paints, and solders. Both inorganic and the body for lead. A technique called x-ray fluo-
organic forms of lead have neurotoxic properties. roscopy (XRF) applied to the patella or tibia
Forms of inorganic lead include metallic lead as can be used to assess lifetime Lead exposure
well as compounds such as lead acetate, azide, (Hu et al., 1990, 1995). Bone storage of lead is
chloride, chlorate, chromate, fluoride, iodate, an important feature of assessing effects of the
nitrate, oxide, sulfate, and sulfide. These lead metal, since lead acquired early in life can be
compounds occur in crystal, powder, and solid mobilized from bone storage later on, result-
forms. Organic lead compounds include tetra- ing in acute symptoms. This can occur during
ethyllead and tetramethyllead, both of which pregnancy, with development of osteoporosis,
are liquids. Occupational exposure to inorganic or in other unusual circumstances. For exam-
lead compounds may occur during smelting, ple, a patient who developed thyroid disease in
welding, Lead paint manufacture, mining, jew- adulthood showed symptoms of lead intoxica-
elry and leaded glass production, automobile tion, high levels of lead on XRF, and abnormal
manufacture, lead foundry work, de-leading neuropsychological test findings that appeared
operations, and discharge of firearms using lead to reflect childhood lead exposure (Weisskopf
bullets. Environmental exposures to inorganic et al., 2004a).
lead can occur through occupational sources Blood lead levels are generally expressed in
(e.g., industrial output contaminating soil, micrograms per deciliter (ug/dl). The back-
water or air; exposure of family members from ground U.S. lead level is less than 5 ug/dl.
contaminated clothing of lead-exposed work- Childhood blood lead level recommendations
ers). It can also result from lead paint exposure have steadily dropped as research evidence
in older homes, lead pipes carrying water, and has accumulated. Currently, it appears that a
contamination of soil and water from natural no-effect level (NOEL) does not exist for child-
sources of lead. The latter can result in dietary hood lead exposure: dose–effect relationships
exposure when food is grown in contaminated between exposure and cognitive outcomes are
soil or drinks are made from contaminated significant and, in fact the most steep, at <10 ug/
water. Occupational and environmental expo- dl (Canfield et al., 2003). Occupational expo-
sure to organic lead compounds was more com- sures in the United States are now considered to
mon in the United States when they were used be acceptable at <40 ug/dl, also representing a
as additives in gasoline and continue to occur steady decline from 60 ug/dl in 1965.
in countries that permit use of leaded gasoline Developmental stage at the time of exposure
products. Occupational exposures to organic is a key factor influencing the pervasiveness and
lead continue to occur in some manufactur- severity of exposure effects. While acute and
ing operations and during cleaning of storage chronic exposures to lead in adults affect spe-
tanks that have held leaded gasoline. Pollution cific domains of cognitive function, generally
from industrial organic Lead processes can con- sparing language (White, 1992), prenatal and
taminate seafood and organic lead compounds childhood exposures result in a much more dif-
sometimes enter the food chain by contaminat- fuse presentation of deficits. Because childhood
ing plant life, resulting in organic lead expo- lead exposures generally occur at the highest
sure. Ingestion of alcohol appears to enhance dosages at ages 1–2, when mouthing and crawl-
the effects of lead exposure. For a detailed ing are dominant behaviors, lead effects have
description of inorganic and organic lead com- been most extensively documented at these
pounds, exposure sources, and bioavailability, ages. Given the same dosage exposure, earlier
see Feldman (Feldman, 1999). exposure is associated with more profound
Lead exposure can be assessed through lab- deficits in a wider range of behavioral domains.
oratory measurements of lead in several body It has been shown that early childhood expo-
media (urine, blood, hair, dentin in teeth, bone). sures affect general intelligence, acquisition of
Urine and blood measures most clearly assess academic knowledge and school completion,
recent exposures, while chronic or remote expo- personality development, social competency,
sures can be detected in hair and tooth dentin. and occupational success (Bellinger & Dietrich,
Correlates of Exposure to Metals 485

1994; Needleman, 2004; Needleman & Bellinger, descriptions of clinical intoxications with case
1981; Needleman et al., 1982; White et al., examples).
1993a). Furthermore, children who are genet-
ically inclined to develop cognitive deficits in Overview of the Neuropsychological Literature
specific domains such as attention, language or on Inorganic Lead Neurotoxicity. As men-
visuospatial skills, appear to develop especially tioned earlier, lead has received intense exam-
prominent deficits in the vulnerable domain(s) ination as a neurotoxicant. This scrutiny has
following exposure. It should be noted that, in included extensive application of neuropsycho-
this context, the developing brain is especially logical and psychometric test techniques. More
susceptible to lead effects through adolescence than any other neurotoxicant, there is abundant
and into early adulthood during brain myelina- literature concerning effects of lead on both
tion and development of prefrontal brain areas. adults and children, which we will consider
Therefore, exposure effects in adolescence separately.
resemble those in early childhood more than
they do in those experienced by adults (i.e., they Adult Lead Exposures. Table 20–1 summa-
tend to be more diff use and expressed in vul- rizes methods and findings from 18 investiga-
nerable cognitive domains). While it has been tions on adult lead exposure effects, divided
hypothesized that aging may represent another into occupational and environmental studies.
vulnerable period for lead effects on cognition In the 1970s, Helena Haenninen, a Finish
and that lead exposure early in life may be a risk clinician and psychologist, was one of the first
factor for the development of neurodegenera- investigators to apply clinical psychometric
tive disorders such as Alzheimer’s disease, data tests to investigate the effects of occupational
on these issues remain sparse at present. exposures to lead and other toxicants on brain
Inorganic lead poisoning from acute and function and behavior. Her 1978 paper is impor-
chronic exposures is characterized by gastro- tant for a number of reasons. First, it identifies
intestinal (constipation, vomiting, anorexia), dose–effect relationships between quantified
CNS (headache, light-headedness, irritability, biomarkers of lead exposure and degree of cog-
fatigue, mood changes), autonomic nervous nitive change on objective, domain-specific
system (ANS) (sweating) and musculoskeletal neuropsychological measures, demonstrat-
(myalgia, arthralgia) symptoms. As enceph- ing lawful decrements in performance related
alopathy develops, seizures, coma, increased to increased lead level (in this case seen in the
intracranial pressure with edema, and death absence of statistically significant differences
may occur (Feldman, 1999). Peripheral neu- between exposed subjects and the control
ropathy is also seen following chronic expo- group). Second, her findings are seen among
sure. Organic lead poisoning was common in lead-exposed workers who did not appear to be
the United States before the removal of lead clinically sick—that is, she demonstrated sub-
from gasoline, when intoxication was seen in or preclinical effects of lead exposure. Finally,
petroleum workers and among individuals who Hanninen paid attention to the behavioral and
sniffed gasoline as a means of becoming high. affective consequences of exposure to neurotox-
Symptoms of organic lead poisoning following icants such as lead, including the fact that there
acute exposures include prominent behavioral are dose–effect relationships between exposure
manifestations (hallucinations, irritability, agi- and affective/behavioral change that cannot be
tation, sleep disturbances, nightmares), gastro- explained as mere indicators of “functional”
intestinal signs (nausea or vomiting, anorexia), disorders or psychiatric disease. These prin-
and motor disturbances (tremulousness, ataxia). ciples have been replicated repeatedly in occu-
If untreated or severe enough, the patient may pational studies of lead neurotoxicity over the
develop seizures and die. Chronic exposure to decades since this seminal paper.
organic lead results in similar symptoms but The table demonstrates that many neuro-
may include cognitive complaints, abdominal psychological measures have been employed
pain, nystagmus, chorea, seizures and hyper- in epidemiologic research addressing lead tox-
active reflexes (see Feldman, 1999) for detailed icity. Domain-specific tests have been used in
Table 20–1. Neuropsychological Research: Occupational and Environmental Lead Exposure in Adults
Authors Study site Study type Sample characteristics Study assessment battery Data analytic approach Positive findings

Occupational Studies
Haenninen Finland Cross- 49 Pb-exposed workers WAIS: Sim, PC, BD, DSp; Exposed versus con- No significant
et al., 1978 sectional (mean BLL = 11–32 ug/ Benton VOT; Santa trols; dose–effect differences exposed
dL); 24 controls Ana pegboard; reaction w/in exposed grp versus unexposed; in
time; WMS exposed grp, higher
Pb assoc w/lower
WAIS BD, DSp;
WMS Vis Rep; Santa
Ana
Valciukas & New York City, Cross- 4 groups w/occupational WAIS BD, DSy; Dose–effect Higher BLL associated
Lilis, 1980 Michigan sectional Pb exposure (N = 141) Embedded figures with worse scores on
(mean Pb = 27.5– all 3 tests
50.1ug/dL); 265 controls
Hogstedt et al., Swedish Lead Cross- 40 Lead exposed; 27 Synonyms, logical func- Exposed versus Exposed < controls,
1983 registry sectional controls tions, perceptual speed, unexposed 11/14 tests; no dose–
psychomotor function, effect relationship
memory, learning, seen
reaction time
Baker et al., U.S. foundry, Longitudinal Year 1: N = 106 expe- WAIS-Voc, Sim, DSp, BD, Dose–effect; score Year 1: Higher BLL
1984, 1985 factory cohort study rience workers, 65 DSy; WMS; Benton change over time associated with
controls; Year 2: N = 43 VOT; CPT; Santa Ana; worse WAIS Voc,
experience, 34 controls; POMS Sim, DSp; WMS
Year 3: N = 39 experi- PAL, Vis Rep; POMS;
ence, 19 controls Year 3: Decline in
BLL associated with
improved POMS
Jeyaratnam Singapore Cross- 49 Pb-exposed work- WAIS DSy, DSp; Santa Exposed versus Exposed worse on
et al., 1986 sectional ers (polyvinyl chlo- Ana; reaction time; controls WAIS DSy; TMT-A;
ride manufacture), 36 TMT; mood Santa Ana; mood
matched controls
Parkinson, Pennsylvania Cross- 288 Pb-exposed workers, WAIS-Inf, PC, Sim, BD, Exposed versus None
1986 sectional 181 controls DSy, DSp; Benton VOT; nonexposed;
Embedded Figures; dose–effect
Grooved Pegboard
Yokoyama Japan Prospective 17 gun metal factory WAIS- DSy, PC, BD, PA, Dose–effect; change Year 1: higher BLL
et al., 1988 longitudinal workers tested 2x; BLL OA over 2 years associated with lower
= 30–64 ug/dL year 1, (w/exposure WAIS-PC; year 3: PC
24–39 year 3; 12 con- decrement) score improved in
trols (BLL = 20 year 1, high BLL group
14 year 3)
Schwartz E. Penn. Longitudinal 535–543 former chemi- WAIS-Inf, BD; ROCF; Tibial Lead-test Higher peak tibial
et al., 2002; cohort study cal workers exposed to RAVLT; serial digit performance Lead associated with
Schwartz organic Lead; XRF used learning; WAIS-R outcomes; peak worse serial digit
et al., 1999 to measure bone Lead; DSy; TMT; Purdue tibial Lead-score learning, RAVLT,
118 controls pegboard; Stroop; FTT; changes TMT-B, FTT, Purdue
CES-D; SCL-90 pegboard, Stroop;
peak tibial Lead
predicted decline in
performance over
time
Osterberg Swedish sec- Cross- 38 male Pb-exposed Cognitive Scanner Battery High exposure Negative study
et al., 1997 ondary sectional workers, N = 19 high (face recognition test, versus low, low
smelter bone Lead (median number learning, figure exposure versus
32 ug/g); N = 19 low drawing, pen to point, controls
bone Lead (median 16 parallelogram, continu-
ug/g); N = 19 controls ous graphics, cancel-
(median bone Lead 4 lation, CPT, mood
ug/g) assessment)
Lucchini et al., N. Italy Cross- 71 Pb-exposed workers Swedish Performance Exposed versus None in SPES or Luria
2000 sectional (mean BLL = 28 ug/ Evaluation System controls
dL); 86 controls (mean (SPES); 5 Luria-
BLL = 8) Nebraska motor
subtests
Kumar et al., India Cross- 60 Pb-exposed battery WAIS DSy, DSp; Exposed versus Exposed had worse
2002 sectional workers (mean BLL Cancellation; RPM controls scores than controls
= 55.6 ug/dL), 30 con- on virtually all mea-
trols (mean BLL = 6) sures; NB: exposure
was above U.S. occu-
pational standards

continued
Table 20–1. continued
Authors Study site Study type Sample characteristics Study assessment battery Data analytic approach Positive findings

Schwartz South Korea Longitudinal 576 Lead workers tested Reaction time; TMT; DSp; Dose–effect, cross- Cross-sectional: higher
et al., 2005 cohort study 3x/2.2 years; mean BLL DSy; Purdue pegboard; sectional and BLL associated with
= 31 ug/dL Benton VOT; RCPM; longitudinal lower scores on
CES-D executive and motor
tasks; longitudinal:
few + findings
Chen, Taiwan Cross- 33 highly exposed workers NES2 computerized test Exposed versus Higher exposure asso-
Dietrich, sectional (BLL = 40–80 ug/dL), battery controls ciated with worse
Ware, 28 workers w/”low” scores on NES2
Radcliffe, exposure (<40), 62 FTT, symbol digit,
and Rogan, controls pattern comparison,
2005 reaction time, visual
digit spans, asso-
ciate learning, mood
scales
Chuang et al., Taiwan Longitudinal 27 Lead glaze workers fol- Chinese NES2 Change in BLL— Decreased Pb associ-
2005a cohort study lowed for 4 years; mean effect on test ated with improved
Pb declined from 26.3 score FTT, Pattern com-
ug/dL to 8.3 parison reaction
time, memory
Bleecker et al., Canada Occupational 254 workers median cur- RAVLT Dose response TWA, IBL contributed
2005b cohort study rent BLL = 27.7 ug/dL; to variance in encod-
time weighted average ing and retrieval
(TWA) BLL (30 ug/dL);
integrated BLL index
(IBL)
Environmental Studies
Muldoon Baltimore, Cross- 530 women enrolled in MMSE, TMT-B, WAIS-R Dose–effect In one sample, higher
et al., 1996 Monogahela sectional Study of Osteoporotic DSy, grooved pegboard, BLL associated
Valley Fractures (SOF), mean reaction time with worse TMT-B,
age 70.5 years, geomet- WAIS-R DSy
ric mean BLL = 4.8
ug/dL
Nordberg Kungsholmen Longitudinal 762 subjects (583 women, MMSE Dose–effect No association between
et al., 2000 district, cohort study 179 men); average age BLL and MMSE
Stockholm 88.4 years; mean BLL
= 3.7 ug/dL
Weisskopf Boston, MA Closed cohort 446 veterans in MMSE Dose–effect A one-interquartile
et al., 2004b study Normative Aging range (20 ug/g of
Study; mean age 67.4; bone mineral) asso-
patella median Lead ciated with –0.24
= 27 ug/g., tibia MMSE score
median Lead = 21 ug/g
Krieg et al., National Cross- Adults aged 20–59; BLLs Simple reaction time, Dose–effect None
2005 Health and sectional 0.7–41.8 ug/dL; geo- symbol-digit substi-
Nutrition metric mean = 2.51 ug/ tution, serial digit
Evaluation dL.; arithmetic mean learning
Survey = 3.3 ug/dL
(NHANES
III) United
States

BLL = blood Lead level; Pb = Lead; exp. = exposed; ug/dL = micrograms per deciliter; ug/g = micrograms per gram
Benton VOT = Benton Visual Organization Test; CPT = continuous performance test; CES-D = Centers for Disease Control Epidemiology Scale of Depression; FTT = fi nger tapping
test; NES, NES2 = Neurobehavioral Evaluations System; MMSE = Mini-Mental Status Examination; POMS = Profi le of Mood States; RAVLT = Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test;
RPM = Raven Progressive Matrices; RCPM = Raven Colored Progressive Matrices; SCL-90 = Symptom Checklist-90 TMT = Trail-making Test (TMT-A = Part A, TMT-B, Part B);
WAIS, (WAIS-R) = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (Revised) (subtests: Inf = Information; RCOF = Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure; Voc = Vocabulary; Sim = Similarities; Com
= Comprehension; DSp = Digit Span; BD = Block Design; DSy = Digit Symbol; PC = Picture Completion; PA = Picture Arrangement; OA = Object Assembly); WMS = Wechsler
Memory Scale (PAL = Paired Associate Learning, Vis Rep = Visual Reproduction); Santa Ana = Santa Ana Form Board Test
490 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

order to gain clues about the likely brain sites exposure metric in occupational settings where
of the neurotoxic actions of lead. It has been workers have experienced chronic exposures.
demonstrated repeatedly that lead exposure in Since the 1990s, some studies have used XRF
adulthood affects executive skills, visuospatial methodology to estimate long-term chronic
abilities, manual motor speed, short-term mem- dose effects based on lead uptake in the patella
ory (both learning and retention), and mood or tibia (Schwartz et al., 1999, 2002). These
state. Linguistic abilities appear to be spared. studies have been better able to define chronic
These consistent findings suggest that lead pref- effects of lead exposure than have investigations
erentially affects the functioning of sub-cortical relying on multiple BLLs collected over a period
and limbic areas, with primary or secondary of time.
frontal lobe dysfunction (White, 1992). These In recent years, attention has also been
possible localizations of action receive some focused on the effects of organic lead exposures,
support from recent imaging research. One though disentangling the effects of inorganic
study documented hippocampal effects of lead and organic forms of the metal has been chal-
exposure in elderly men on proton MRS noting lenging (Schwartz et al., 2002).
higher myoinositol-to-creatine ratios in hippo- Attention has also recently been focused on
campus (Weisskopf et al., 2007). A case report effects of low-level lead exposure in the elderly,
described white matter lesions in the structural who may be especially vulnerable to lead neu-
brain MRIs of lead-exposed 71-year-old mono- rotoxicity. Studies of this issue are sparse,
zygotic twins, one of whom had more severe Contradictory results have been observed with
exposure than the other. Neuropsychological regard to effects on the Mini-Mental Status
findings included executive/working memory Examination (MMSE) (Nordberg et al., 2000;
deficits in both twins, while the twin with Weisskopf et al., 2004a). One study reported
higher exposure demonstrated much worse relationships between poorer performance on
performance on short-term memory tasks than Trail-making Test-Part B (TMT-B) and Wechsler
the sibling. The MRS and neuropsychological Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R)
findings were interpreted as suggesting hippo- Digit Symbol (DSy) scores in women whose
campal and frontal lobe dysfunction. In addi- average BLL was very low (4.8 ug/dL) (Muldoon
tion, the structural MRIs on the twins identified et al., 1996). Participants in these studies were
possible micro-infarcts, suggesting cerebrovas- members of medical cohort studies, not occupa-
cular consequences of lead exposure that would tionally exposed workers. It is difficult to deter-
be consistent with the known effects of lead on mine when the critical lead exposures may have
cardiovascular function (e.g., increased blood occurred developmentally. It remains possible
pressure) (Weisskopf et al., 2004b). that lead levels late in life are markers for expo-
When the epidemiologic research summa- sure that occurred earlier in adulthood or even
rized in this table began in the 1970s–1980s, a in childhood, resulting in cognitive changes
blood lead level (BLL) < 60 ug/dL was considered that are maintained through a lifetime.
to be acceptable for workers with occupational
exposure. By 1985, it was clear that chronic Childhood Exposures. Table 20–2 summarizes
occupational lead exposure is associated with data from an extensive epidemiologic literature
neurocognitive changes in the 40–60 ug/dL BLL on lead neurotoxicity in children. Though this
range (Baker et al., 1984, 1985). The standard for body of work has employed domain-specific
exposure was lowered to 40 ug/dL. It has since neuropsychological tests, it has relied more
become apparent that effects are seen as low as heavily on the use of omnibus tests of intelli-
20–30 ug/dL in occupational settings (Bleecker gence and academic skills to systemically inves-
et al., 2005a; Chuang et al., 2005b). However, tigate the threshold values of lead exposure
the threshold BLL for chronic occupational lead required to produce observable exposure-out-
exposure effects on CNS function has yet to be come relationships. These tests have included
clearly delineated. the Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children
Although BLL is most accurate as a measure of (WISC, WISC-R, WISC-III), the McCarthy
recent and acute exposure, it is often used as an scale, the Bayley Scales of Infant Development
Table 20–2. Neuropsychological Research: Childhood Lead Exposure
Authors Study site Study type Sample Study assessment Data analytic Positive findings
characteristics battery approach

Needleman, Boston area Cross-sectional 58 high (>24 ppm) and WISC-R, Piagetian con- High versus low High exposure children worse than
1979 100 low (<6 ppm) versation, Peabody, exposure group low exposure group (p < .1) on:
dentine Pb levels Sentence repetition, comparison WISC-R FSIQ, PIQ, VIQ; WISC
groups; mean Token, Seashore subtests (I, Voc, DSp, Com, PC),
age = 7.3 years Rhythm, VMI, Seashore Rhythm test, Sentence
Frostig, reaction time, Repetition and reaction time. Rated
H-R motor tasks. more distractible and more behav-
Teacher behavior rat- ioral symptoms by teachers
ing scale
Needleman & Re-analysis of Re-categorized 158 working class WISC-R Dose–response Dentine Pb > 20 ppb associated with
Bellinger, Needleman exposure children decreased IQ
1981 et al., 1979 groups into
0–9.9 ppm,
10.0–19.9 ppm,
20.0–29.9 ppm,
and 30.0–39.9
ppm
Bellinger Boston area Cross-sectional 141 children. 22 ele- Teacher ratings. Dose–response Higher dentine associated with poorer
et al., 1984 child vated (>20 ppm), 71 Pupil rating scale. school performance, grade retention
cohort f/u midrange (10.0–19.9 Classroom behavior most strongly associated with Lead
ppm), 48 low (<10 questionnaire levels. Teacher ratings lower with
ppm) dentine levels high exposure
Needleman Boston area Longitudinal 132 young adults, NES subtests; CVLT; High (>24ppm) Higher dentine Lead levels associ-
et al., 1990 child follow-up mean age 18.4 years. BNT; ROCF; word versus low (<6 ated with worse vocabulary and
cohort f/u identification test ppm) exposure; high grammatical reasoning, finger
exposure (>20), tapping, hand-eye coordination,
medium (10–19.9) reaction time, reading scores
and low (<10)
Needleman U.S. children Cross-sectional 2335 children, grades Same as above, Dose–response Controlling for 5/39 possible con-
et al., 1982 1 & 2, median den- Needleman et al., founders related to exposure level,
tine Pb level = 12 1979 Pb related to lower WISC-R FS, P
ppm and VIQs; Seashore , sentence repe-
tition, Token, reaction time
continued
Table 20–2. Continued
Authors Study site Study type Sample Study assessment Data analytic Positive findings
characteristics battery approach
Bellinger Boston Prospective Infants; 76 high BSID, MDI Dose–response High cord BLL associated with lower
et al., cohort umbilical BLL MDI at 6 and 12 months
1986, (mean = 14.6 ug/
1991 dL), 88 medium
(mean = 6.5), 85
low (mean = 1.8)
Stiles & Boston Prospective 148 children recruited WISC-R, K-TEA, CVLT, Dose–response Highest BLL(>10 ug/dL) at 24 months
Bellinger, neonate cohort at birth, tested at WCST, ROCF, Story associated with significantly lower
1993 10 years. Exposure recall, FTT, pegboard FSIQ, VIQ
defined as BLL at
24 months: low (<5
ug/dL), medium
(5–9.9), high (>10)
Hansen et al., Denmark Cross-sectional 162 children; aver- WISC, Bender Gestalt High versus controls High Lead exposure related to lower
1989 age dentin Pb level Test, Seashore, TMT, WISC VIQ + FSIQ, and Bender
= 10.7 ug/g. High sentence repetition
exposed (dentine test, CPT
Pb >18.7 ug/dL)
matched by sex and
SES to control chil-
dren (<5 ug/dL)
Wigg et al., Port Pirie Prospective 723 children BLL at 6, BSID Dose–response BLL level at all ages negatively corre-
1988 Cohort cohort 15, and 24 months lated with mental development at 24
Study, (geometric means months
Southern 14.3, 20.8 and 21.2
Australia ug/dL respectively)
McMichael Port Pirie Prospective 548 4-year old McCarthy GCI Dose–response Increases in BLL from 10 ug/dL to 30
et al., 1992 birth cohort children associated with 8.3 point decrement
cohort, in GCI in girls, 0.8 in boys
follow-up
Baghurst Port Pirie Prospective 494 seven year old McCarthy GCI Dose–response Inverse relations btw IQ at 7 and
et al., 1992 cohort, cohort children both antenatal and postnatal BLL;
follow-up increase from 10 ug/dL to 30 = 5.3
IQ point decrease
Tong et al., Port Pirie, Prospective Mean BLL= 21.2 ug/ BSID, MDI, GCI Dose–response Changes in IQ + decline in BLL from
2000 follow-up cohort dL at age 2, 7.9 ug/ age 7 to age13 years suggest slightly
children dL at age 11–13 better cognition in children whose
years BLL declined the most
Ernhart & U.S. inner city Cross-sectional 359 infants recruited Receptive and expres- Dose–response No statistically significant results
Greene, at birth, tested at sive language, taped
1990 2 years; mean cord speech sample at 2
blood 6.56 ug/dL; years
mean 6 month =
10.1; 2 year = 16.7; 3
year =16.7 ug/dL
Dietrich Cincinnati Prospective 258 urban, inner-city K-ABC Dose–response Weak inverse association between
et al., 1991 Lead Study cohort children tested at postnatal BLL and performance on
cohort age 4 years. Mean K-ABC visual-spatial and visual-
lifetime Pb levels by motor tasks
quartiles 7.9, 11.4,
15.2, and 23.7 ug/dL
Dietrich Lead Study Prospective 259 urban, inner-city K-ABC Dose–response Higher postnatal BLL associated with
et al., 1992 cohort— cohort children tested at poorer K-ABC,all subtests; adjust-
follow up age 5 years; mean ment for home environment +
BLL a 5 years = 11.9 maternal IQ left few statistically
ug/dL significant results
Dietrich Cincinnati Prospective 253 children tested at WISC-R Dose–response Higher postnatal BLL associated with
et al., 1993 Lead Study cohort age 6.5 years; 3 exp. lower FSIQ and PIQ, average life-
cohort groups by mean time BLL >20 ug/dL associated with
lifetime BLL 0–10, 7-point decrease in IQ, compared to
>10–15, >15–20, >20 average lifetime BLL = 10 ug/dL
ug/dL
Coscia et al., Cincinnati Longitudinal 196 children, mean WISC-R, WISC-3 (BD, Dose–response 15 year olds with higher BLL had lower
2003 Cohort cohort prenatal BLL = 8.53 Voc) verbal comprehension scores and
Study ug/dl, mean BLL greater decline in vocabulary
age 1–6, 13.46 ug/dL
Fergusson New Zealand Longitudinal Children 8–12 years; New Zealand Revision Categorical groups Children with elevated BLL (8+, 4–7
et al., 1993 cohort mean dentine Pb of the Burt Wood (dentine Pb levels ppm) 4–6 months behind children
levels at 6–8 years Reading Test 0–3, 4–7, 8+ ppm) with 0–3 ppm on word recognition
= 6.2 ppm
continued
Table 20–2. Continued
Authors Study site Study type Sample Study assessment Data analytic Positive findings
characteristics battery approach
White et al., Boston Cross-sectional 33 middle-aged indi- WAIS, WMS, TMT, Lead poisoned ver- Exposed worse than controls on all
1993a viduals diagnosed COWAT, RPM, sus controls except 3 tests (WMS, Inf, Orien,
with Lead poison- FTT, POMS WAIS DSp); 3 subtests significantly
ing before age of lower in exposed: WAIS PC, WMS
4; 20 controls with LM, RPM
no Lead poisoning
diagnosis
Wasserman Yugoslavia Prospective 332 children tested at McCarthy GCI Dose–response Increase in BLL from 10 to 25 ug/dL
et al., 1994 cohort age 4 years. BLL = associated with 3.8 decrease in GCI
39.9 ug/dL (exposed
smelter), and 9.6 ug/
dL (controls)
Shen et al., Yangpu Prospective Newborn children fol- BSID—MDI, PDI High versus low High exposure group lower MDI
1998 District, Cohort lowed 0–12 months, comparison scores at 3, 6, and 12 months
China mean cord BLL high groups
group = 13.4 ug/dL,
low = 5.3
Stokes et al., Silver Valley, Cohort Young adults with Pb Grooved pegboard, Exposed versus Exposed group worse hand-eye coordi-
1998 WA exposure (n = 281), dynamometer, Santa unexposed nation, simple reaction time, TMT-
without Pb expo- Ana, FTT, simple B, symbol digit latency, serial digit
sure (n = 289) reaction time, TMT,
NES, RPM
Mendelsohn New York, Cross-sectional 68 children age 6–36 BSID, 2nd ed. Group 1 (BLL 0–9.9 BLL 10–24.9 ug/dL associated with
et al., 1998 mos; BLL <25 ug/ ug/dL) com- mean MDI score 6.2 points lower
dL pared, to group 2 than BLL 0–9.9
(10–24.9)
Chiodo et al., Detroit, Longitudinal 246 African WISC-III, CPT, Talland Dose–response Higher BLL associated with worse
2004 Michigan cohort American, inner- digit cancellation, FSIQ, VIQ + PIQ, CPT, Seashore,
city children. BLL at WCST, Tower of WCST, VMI, pegboard, WRAML
7.5 years London, Seashore,
WRAML, grooved
pegboard, Cosi, VMI
Hu et al., Mexico City, Longitudinal 146 mother–infant BSID Dose–response BLL in the first trimester (7.1–>10 ug/
2006 Mexico cohort pairs recruited at dL predicts lower MDI scores
prenatal clinic
Tellez-Rojo Mexico City, Prospective 294 children, BLL at BSID Dose–response Higher BLL 24 months associated with
et al., 2006 Mexico cohort 12 and 24 months lower MDI and PDI; BLL 12 months
<10 ug/dL associated with 24-months PDI only
Gomaa et al., Mexico City, Prospective 197 mother– infant BSID Dose–response Higher maternal tibia + umbilical
2002 Mexico cohort pairs. Maternal cord BLL associated with lower MDI
patella and tibial scores; 2-fold increase in umbilical
Pb post delivery BLL associated with 3.1 point MDI
(mean patella Pb = decrease
17.9 ug/dL; mean
tibia Pb = 11.5 ug/
dL). Umbilical cord
levels (mean = 6.7
ug/dL)
Kordas et al., Torreon, Cross-sectional 602 children mean Math Achievement Test, Dose–response For every 1 ug/dL increase in BLL,
2006 Mexico BLL = 10.2 ug/dL PPVT, WISC-RM, decreases were seen: 0.17 on Math
TMT, Sternberg, achievement ; –0.36 PPVT; –0.05
Figure matching, Sternberg; –0.05 figure matching
Figure design, Visual
Search, CAT, stimulus
discrimination, visual
memory span
Lanphear United Cross-sectional 4853 children aged WRAT; WISC-R (BD, Dose–response Lower cognitive and academic skills
et al., States Meta-analysis 6–16; geometric DSp) Four groups associated with BLL <5 ug/dL
2000 mean BLL = 1.9 <10 ug/dL, <7.5
ug/dL. 172 (2.1%) ug/dL, <5 ug/
BLL >10 ug/dL dL, <2.5 ug/dL
BLL = blood Lead level; Pb = Lead; exp.=exposed; ug/dL = micrograms per deciliter; ug/g = micrograms per gram
BNT = Boston Naming Test; BSID = Bayley Scales lof Infant Development (MDI = Mental Development Index; PDI = Psychomotor Development Index); CAT = Child Assessment
Test; COWAT = Controlled Word Association Test; CPT = continuous performance test; CVLT = California Verbal Learning Test; FTT = fi nger tapping test; H-R = Halstead Reitan;
K-ABC = Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children; K-TEA = Kaufman Test of Educational Achievement; McCarthy GCI = McCarthy Scales Global Cognitive Index; NES, NES2 =
Neurobehavioral Evaluation System; POMS = Profi le of Mood States; PPVT = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; ROCF = Rey–Osterreith Complex Figure; RPM = Raven Progressive
Matrices; RCPM = Raven Colored Progressive Matrices; TMT = Trail-making Test (TMT-A = Part A, TMT-B, part B); VMI = Beery Visual Motor Integration test; WCST = Wisconsin
Card Sorting Test; WAIS = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (DSp = Digit Span); WISC, WISC-R, WISC-III = Wechsler Scales of Intelligence for Children (Revised, III) (subtests: BD =
Block Design; DSp = Digit Span; Voc = Vocabulary); WMS = Wechsler Memory Scale (LM = Logical Memory); WRAML = Wide Range Assessment of Learning and Memory; WRAT
= Wide Range Achievement Test; Santa Ana = Santa Ana Form Board Test
496 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

(BSID, BSID-2), the Kaufman Assessment section, we will provide a brief background on
Battery for Children (K-ABC), and a myriad of elemental, inorganic and organic mercury, fol-
tests of academic achievement and knowledge. lowed by a detailed discussion on the effects of a
In addition, the consequences of lead expo- form of organic mercury (methylmercury) that
sure effects on behavior have been investigated has received considerable scientific attention
with regard to promotion through grades in due to its public health importance as a seafood
school, completing high school, or serving jail contaminant.
time (Bellinger et al., 1984; Needleman, 1979). Elemental mercury can be found in ther-
Several important cohorts have been followed mometers and other scientific and medical
longitudinally for many years as a part of this instruments; it is utilized in the manufacture
research, including a group of Boston first and of fluorescent light bulbs and in gold mining
second graders (Needleman, 1979), Boston operations. It is highly lipophilic, easily crosses
newborns (Bellinger et al., 1984, 1986), a birth the blood brain barrier, and appears to remain
cohort from Port Pirie, Australia (Baghurst in the brain for very long periods of time once
et al., 1992; McMichael et al., 1988; Tong et al., it accumulates there. Mild effects of acute ele-
2000; Wigg et al., 1988), and the Cincinnati lead mental mercury exposure can include tremor,
Study cohort (Dietrich et al., 1991, 1992, 1993). irritability, emotional lability, depression, and
Paralleling the progression of adult lead confusion. In cases of serious intoxication, acute
research, childhood studies began demonstrat- exposure to elemental mercury vapor can result
ing dose-related neurotoxicity at levels that in metal fever syndrome. The initial phase of this
would be considered to be unsafe by today’s syndrome is marked by respiratory symptoms,
standards, confirming effects at lower and including pneumonitis, bronchiolitis and bron-
lower dosages and providing detailed data on chitis, accompanied by nausea and vomiting,
the shape of the effect curve of lead exposure on malaise, chills and fever. Onset of neurological
intelligence. This line of research culminated in symptoms can occur within 24 hours (tremor,
the meta-analysis of data from several studies delirium, coma). If the initial phase of the syn-
involving almost 5000 children that demon- drome is not fatal, it is followed by an interme-
strated IQ effects well below BLLs of 10 ug/dL (<5 diate phase, which includes renal symptoms.
ug/dL) (Lanphear et al., 2000). In fact, it appears In patients who survive this phase, a late phase
that the steepest slope of the lead effect curve on may occur that is remarkable for persistence of
IQ occurs at <10 ug/dL, with continuing dose– neurological symptoms after the other systemic
effect relationships above that level (Canfield manifestations have resolved. Chronic exposure
et al., 2003). In these studies, each increase of 10 to elemental mercury can result in micromercu-
µg/dL in the lifetime average BL concentration rialism, which is notable for tremor, dysphoria,
was associated with a 4.6 decrease in IQ. As the and diarrhea, which may progress to gingivitis,
blood lead concentration increased from 1 to 10 cognitive complaints, psychotic symptoms, and
µg/dL the IQ declined by 7.4 points. worsened tremor. Exposure to ethanol appears
Demonstration of lead effects through IQ to decrease the effects of elemental mercury
measures, which have face validity as measures exposure by inhibiting its uptake in the red
of cognition among lay people and clinicians blood cells.
and has resulted in the elegant demonstration Inorganic mercury compounds are used in
of lead-associated effects on outcomes, is con- plating processes, photography, cosmetics, and
sidered by many to be a major public health embalming. Systemic health effects of acute and
achievement of the latter half of the twentieth chronic exposure to inorganic mercury are sim-
century. ilar to those seen in elemental mercury exposure
and most likely represent effects of breakdown
of the compounds into the elemental form
Mercury
(Feldman, 1999). A case study of elemental mer-
As in the case of lead, the degree of neurotox- cury poisoning effects in a thermometer worker
icity and specific CNS effects of exposure to showed cognitive deficits in several domains
mercury depend upon its chemical form. In this (executive, visuospatial, motor, and mood/
Correlates of Exposure to Metals 497

behavior, sparing language), accompanied by and deposited in sites remote from initial
cortical atrophy and white matter lesions on release. Degree of methylmercury contamina-
MRI (White et al., 1993b). tion of seafood varies widely by geographic area
Organic mercurial compounds can be found and sometimes within different bodies of water
in several forms that vary widely in neurotox- in the same geographic region. Persons who
icity, perhaps related to the ease with which the consume a great deal of fish are at increased risk
compound is metabolized into its inorganic to experience symptoms of methylmercury poi-
form (Feldman, 1999). Some forms of organic soning, and cases of methylmercury intoxica-
mercury are so neurotoxic that even minute tion have been reported in California related to
quantities of exposure can result in progressive consuming large amounts of sushi (Hightower
neurologic and systemic decline and death. An & Moore, 2003).
interesting characteristic of organic mercury is The clinical syndrome of methylmercury
the occurrence of delayed effects of exposure, intoxication has been well described due to con-
with symptoms occurring some time after ces- tamination of Minamata Bay in Japan from a
sation of exposure that can progress to severe chloralkali plant that used mercury in manufac-
illness and even death. Ethylmercury is a form ture, dumping the metal in Minama Bay, where
of organic mercury that is found in thimero- it was transformed through the methylation
sol, a preservative that was used widely in the process described above into methylmercury
past in childhood vaccines and is still present and contaminated the local food supply of fish.
in some types of medications and inoculations. Called Minamata disease, this form of metal
This form of mercury has received recent atten- poisoning is remarkable for several features. It
tion as a possible explanation of the increase in has a clear-cut developmental trajectory: pre-
diagnosed autism in the United States (Clifton, natal exposure to the compound can result in
2007; “Thimerosal: Updated statement. An cerebral palsy, blindness, and death; early child-
advisory committee statement (acs),” 2007) and hood exposure can produce severe neurological
has been removed from many vaccines. and sensory deficits; exposure in adulthood may
Methylmercury is an organic mercury com- result in no obvious symptoms (even in mothers
pound that is of substantial public health impor- carrying children born with severe deficits) or
tance. This compound can be manufactured may be limited to mild sensory disturbances,
and exposure has occurred through acciden- cognitive change or peripheral neuropathy.
tal contamination of food or grains. However, These effects of methylmercury were confirmed
exposure most often results from a process in in a second epidemic of methylmercury poison-
which inorganic mercury contamination of the ing caused by contaminated grain in Iraq in the
environment (air, soil, water) leads to methyla- 1970s (Myers et al., 2000) and in the case history
tion by microorganisms in sediment and water. of a farming family who consumed pork from
These microorganisms, now contaminated by pigs to which they had fed grain accidentally
methylmercury enter the food chain through contaminated by the compound. In all of these
plants and fish. Organisms higher on the food instances, brain autopsy data and MRI studies
chain which are large and long-lived carry the revealed severe brain damage in children with
greatest risk of methylmercury contamina- prenatal exposure (including small brain size,
tion. Worldwide, the greatest problem resulted incomplete development and failure of neu-
from contamination of tuna, swordfish, shell- rons to migrate properly during development),
fish, other seafood and land animals that ingest widely diff use multifocal lesions in children
contaminated plants or fish. The environmen- with postnatal exposure, and a relatively focal
tal pollution leading to this process can occur picture of posterior brain damage (especially
secondary to manufacturing processes that use visual cortex) in adults (Choi, 1989).
mercury, gold mining operations, and electrical Methylmercury is known to affect the ANS
power production. Sometimes the pollution is and is related to increased heart rate variabil-
localized and affects a particular area (e.g., gold ity and to increased blood pressure in a dose–
mining on portions of the Amazon River), but effect manner. Thus, it is likely related to stroke
mercury residue can also be carried by winds and has been linked to increased risk of heart
498 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

disease and myocardial infarction (Jain et al., maternal hair collected when their cohort chil-
2007). dren were born; they found that higher maternal
Subtle effects of methylmercury , especially hair MeHg was associated with poorer perfor-
preclinical effects of exposure in the absence mance on the Denver Developmental Screening
of obvious clinical disease, have been the topic Test (DDST) at age 4 (Kjellstrom et al., 1986) and
of intense research in the past 15 years. These on WISC-R PIQ and FSIQ, McCarthy perceptual
studies have sought to determine No Observable and motor outcomes, and the Test of Language
Effect Level (NOEL) of methylmercury effect, Development (TOLD) at age 6 (Kjellstrom et al.,
especially for prenatal and early childhood 1989). A team of investigators from the Faroe
exposures. Islands, Denmark, Japan and the United States
examined a large birth cohort of Faroese chil-
Overview of the Neuropsychological Litera- dren (n = 1022) at ages 6.5–7.5 and at 14; they
ture on Methylmercury Neurotoxicity Table reported that higher levels of prenatal MeHg
20–3 summarizes the epidemiologic evidence exposure (especially cord blood measures) were
concerning methylmercury (MeHg) neuro- associated with poorer performance on tests of
toxicity in children using neurocognitive out- attention, memory and naming at age 6.5–7.5
comes. Systematic epidemiologic behavioral (Grandjean et al., 1997) and on attention, motor,
toxicology research on this compound to date visuospatial and naming tests at age 14 (Debes
has largely focused on prenatal exposures, due et al., 2006b). However, very different results
to the remarkable vulnerability of the develop- have been reported for the Seychelles cohort.
ing CNS to this toxicant, which was so clearly This study linked prenatal MeHg exposure, as
identified in the poisoning epidemics in Japan measured by maternal hair levels at parturition,
and Iraq. to performance on a variety of omnibus and
Beginning in the 1980s, investigators became domain-specific cognitive tests administered at
concerned with question of subtle, low-dose several points in development (6.5, 19, 29, 66,
effects of MeHg on the developing nervous sys- and 109 mos.). Essentially, no positive findings
tem. Birth cohorts of children with documented were reported (Davidson et al., 2000). A study in
prenatal exposure were developed in New Great Britain also found no relationship between
Zealand, the Faroe Islands and the Seychelles DDST performance at age 18 mos. and cord tis-
Islands, where seafood and fish are an impor- sue Hg (Daniels et al., 2004), though exposure in
tant part of the local diet. Although the New this cohort was extremely low.
Zealand and Seychelles studies have relied A number of explanations have been offered
heavily on omnibus tests of intelligence and for these contradictory findings, especially
achievement, other studies of MeHg, includ- between the two large on-going cohort studies
ing the Faroese investigations, have relied on (Faroes and Seychelles). These have included
domain-specific neuropsychological tests. The possible confounding from exposure to poly-
greater reliance on more focused cognitive chlorinated biphenyls (PCBs) in the Faroes,
outcomes in the MeHg literature compared to though these were well documented and ruled
Lead research likely reflects greater knowledge out as explaining the findings (Daniels et al.,
about the structural neuropathological effects 2004; Grandjean et al., 1997). Other differ-
of MeHg on the brain, at least at higher dose ences between the studies include source of die-
exposures. tary exposure (ocean fish in Seychelles versus
The literature on developmental MeHg neu- whale, ocean fish and shellfish in the Faroes),
rotoxicity in individuals without evidence of test administrators (testing was done by clinical
clinical poisoning contains some fascinating specialists in the Faroes, nurses and students in
contradictions. The New Zealand and Faroese the Seychelles), and exposure assessment dif-
studies clearly demonstrate dose–effect rela- ferences (the Faroese study used cord blood
tionships between measures of prenatal MeHg and maternal hair, finding better associations
exposure and poorer performance on neurocog- with cord blood; the Seychelles investigators
nitive tests during development. Kjellstrom and used only maternal hair). Interestingly, a recent
colleagues measured prenatal exposure using meta-analysis of the New Zealand, Seychelles
Table 20–3. Neuropsychological Research: Childhood Methylmercury Exposure
Authors Study site Study type Sample Study assessment Data analytic Positive findings
characteristics battery approach
Kjellstrom et al., New Zealand Longitudinal 31 high prenatal exposure DDST High versus low Lower DDST scores
1986 birth cohort children (maternal Hg in in high exp. grp.
hair at birth > 6 ug/g);
31 low prenatal exposure
(maternal hair 0–5); tested
at age 4
Kjellstrom et al., New Zealand 61 high exp., 3 low exp. con- WISC-R, TOLD, Dose–effect Maternal hair Hg
1989 cohort f/u trol grps McCarthy associated with
WISC-R PIQ,
FSIQ; McCarthy
percepual,
motoric; TOLD
Myers et al., Seychelle Longitudinal 740 children a. DDST-R, Fagan test of a. Dose–effect a. None
1995a, 1995b Islands birth cohort a. tested at 6.5 months, prena- visual memory b. Dose–effect b. None
tal exp. measured by mater- b. DDST-R
nal hair levels (0.5–26.7
ppm, median = 5.9)
b. tested again at 19 months
29 months
Davidson et al., Seychelles 711 children tested at 66 McCarthy, Bender, W-J Dose–effect None
1998; Myers cohort f/u months Letter & Word Rec.; (using prena-
& Davidson, Preschool Language tal + postnatal
2000 Test; CBC measures)
Davidson et al., Seychelles 87 children tested at 109 WISC-III (Inf, Voc, BD, Dose–effect None
2000 cohort f/u months DSp, Coding), CVLT,
BNT, VMI, WRAML
design memory,
Grooved pegboard,
TMT, FTT

continued
Table 20–3. continued
Authors Study site Study type Sample Study assessment Data analytic Positive findings
characteristics battery approach
Grandjean et al., Faroe Islands Longitudinal 917 chidlren tested at 7 years; NES (FTT, Hand-eye Dose–effect Higher prena-
1997 birth cohort prenatal exp. assessed in coor, Animal CPT), tal cord Hg
cord blood, maternal hair TPT, Bender, CVLT, BNT, associated with
WISC-R (DSp,Sim, worse CPT, DSp,
BD), NAPOMS Bender recall,
CVLT, BNT
Debes et al., Faroe Islands 878 children tested at age 14 NES (FTT, CPT), BNT, Dose–effect Higher cord blood
2006a cohort f/u years S-B Copying Test, Hg associated
CVLT, WISC-R (Sim, with worse FTT,
DSp), CPT, Spatial
WISC-R + WAIS BD, span, S-B
WMS-III Spatial Span Copying, BNT
Grandjean et al., Brazil, Cross-sectional 351 children from 4 villages, FTT, Santa Ana pegbrd., Dose–effect Higher hair Hg
1999 Tapajos tested age 7–12 years., WISC-III DSp forward; associated with
River Basin maternal and child’s Hg S-B (Copying, Bead worse scores on
assessed in hair (mean = Memory) S-B Copying,
3.8 ug/g–25.4 in 4 villages) Santa Ana
Cordier et al., French Cross-sectional 206 children tested age 5–12 FTT, S-B (Copying, Bead Dose–effect Higher maternal
2002 Guiana years, maternal hair Hg memory), McCarthy hair Hg associ-
measured; (3 exp. grps: DSp ated with lower
≤=5, 5–10, >10 ug/g) S-B Copying
scores
Daniels et al., Great Britain Birth cohort 1054 children tested 15 and MacArthur Dose–effect None
2004 18 months; cord tissue Hg Communication
(0–76.5; median = 0.01 Development Inventory
ug/g) (15 months); DDST (18
months)
BNT = Boston Naming Test; CBC = Child Behavior Checklist; CPT = continuous performance test; CVLT = California Verbal Learning Test; DDST = Denver Developmental Screening
Test; FTT = fi nger tapping test; H-R = Halstead Reitan; NAPOMS = Nonverbal Analogue Profi le of Mood States; NES, NES2 = Neurobehavioral Evaluation System; S-B = Stanford Binet
Scale of Intelligence-4; TMT = Trail-making Test; TOLD = Test of Language Development; TMT; VMI = Beery Visual Motor Integration; WAIS = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale;
WISC-R, WISC-III = Wechsler Scales of Intelligence for Children (Revised, III) (subtests: Inf = Information, BD = Block Design; DSp = Digit Span; Voc = Vocabulary, Sim-Similarities);
W-J = Woodcock Johnson; WMS-III = Wechsler Memory Scale-III; WRAML = Wide Range Assessment of Learning and Memory; WRAT = Wide Range Achievement Test; Santa Ana
= Santa Ana Form Board Test
Correlates of Exposure to Metals 501

and Faroese data used WISC subtest scores to Although the reversibility of effects of pre-
generate IQ measures, finding an overall effect natal exposure is unknown, some investigators
of about –0.18 IQ points per 1ppm increase in believe that CNS effects are permanent (Debes
maternal hair mercury. The authors suggested et al., 2006b; Murata et al., 2004). A functional
that a neurotoxic MeHg effect is, in fact, appar- imaging pilot study of 16-year-old Faroese
ent in the data from the Seychelles as well as the cohort members indicated significant fMRI dif-
other two studies (Axelrad et al., 2007). ferences between boys with high and low pre-
Despite the contradictory results, some have natal exposures, with highly exposed children
called for a reduction in recommended degree having increased activation in more brain areas
of maternal exposure to methylmercury dur- than children with low exposure (Janulewicz
ing pregnancy (Gilbert & Grant-Webster, 1995). et al., 2006). Figure 20–1 illustrates one exam-
There has also been considerable controversy in ple of the findings from this research (see also
the general press and public health circles about the color version in the color insert section).
the benefits of fish consumption versus poten- Another debate involves effects of postna-
tial risks due to MeHg exposure. tal versus prenatal MeHg exposure on CNS

a. High MeHg and high PCB exposure

20

15

10

0
b. Low MeHg and low PCB exposure

12
10
8
6
4
2
0
c. Subtraction of activation of the low exposure group from the high exposure group
14
12
10
8
6
4
2
0

Figure 20–1. Activation during a Task of Photic Stimulation (a) The figure shows activation for the high
exposure group during a task of photic stimulation predominantly in the primary occipital cortex bilaterally
(Brodmann’s area 17). (b) The low exposure group shows bilateral activation primarily in the occipital associa-
tion cortex (Brodmann’s areas 18 and 19). (c) When the two groups are compared there is greater activation in
the primary occipital cortex in the high exposure group than the low exposure group, representing recruitment
of different neuronal resources in the two groups.
502 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

function. In the Faroese cohort, postnatal Behavioral neurotoxicologists have a great


measures of exposure at the time of testing deal of work to do—and neuropsychological
were largely unrelated to cognitive outcomes. assessment methods will certainly play a role in
Studies of children living along the Amazon these efforts going forward.
have suggested relationships between current
hair mercury level and cognitive performance
in children for whom prenatal exposure esti- References
mates were unavailable (Cordier et al., 2002; Axelrad, D. A., Bellinger, D. C., Ryan, L. M., &
Grandjean et al., 1999). However, these inves- Woodruff, T. J. (2007). Dose–response relationship
tigators speculate that mercury levels in hair of prenatal mercury exposure and IQ: An integra-
taken at the time of testing in these populations tive analysis of epidemiologic data. Environmental
is a marker for prenatal as well as postnatal Health Perspectives, 115(4), 609–615.
exposure. Obviously, it is difficult to disentangle Baghurst, P. A., McMichael, A. J., Wigg, N. R.,
pre- versus postnatal effects when data are not Vimpani, G. V., Robertson, E. F., Roberts, R. J.,
available on mercury exposure levels at birth. et al. (1992). Environmental exposure to lead and
children’s intelligence at the age of seven years.
The Port Pirie cohort study. New England Journal
Discussion of Medicine, 327(18), 1279–1284.
Epidemiologic research linking neuropsycho- Baker, E. L., Feldman, R. G., White, R. A., Harley,
logical outcomes to exposures to metals has been J. P., Niles, C. A., Dinse, G. E., et al. (1984).
Occupational lead neurotoxicity: A behavioural
remarkable for positive findings of subtle effects
and electrophysiological evaluation. Study design
of low-level exposures on brain function. Such and year one results. British Journal of Industrial
findings provide an obvious call to action for Medicine, 41(3), 352–361.
preventive measures that will reduce exposure Baker, E. L., Jr., Feldman, R. G., White, R. F., Harley, J.
and ultimately result in healthier CNS function P., Dinse, G. E., & Berkey, C. S. (1983). Monitoring
in adults and children. In this setting, neuropsy- neurotoxins in industry: Development of a neu-
chological outcomes have had a powerful public robehavioral test battery. Journal of Occupational
health impact. As imaging techniques improve Medicine, 25(2), 125–130.
in resolution and sophistication, it is likely that Baker, E. L., White, R. F., Pothier, L. J., Berkey, C. S.,
they will be applied to many of the same issues Dinse, G. E., Travers, P. H., et al. (1985). Occupational
described in this chapter with, perhaps, new lead neurotoxicity: Improvement in behavioural
effects after reduction of exposure. British Journal
insights into structural and metabolic mecha-
of Industrial Medicine, 42(8), 507–516.
nisms underlying the neurocognitive changes Beal, M. F. (1992). Does impairment of energy
that have already been defined. metabolism result in excitotoxic neuronal death in
Fascinating questions remain. What gene–en- neurodegenerative illnesses? Annals of Neurology,
vironment or gene–environment–gene interac- 31(2), 119–130.
tions underlie metal effects on cognition? Does Bellinger, D., & Dietrich, K. N. (1994). Low-level
early metal exposure lead to later development lead exposure and cognitive function in children.
of neurodegenerative disorders? If so, which Pediatric Annals, 23(11), 600–605.
metals and which disorders? Are metal effects Bellinger, D., Leviton, A., Rabinowitz, M., Needleman,
on the developing of adult brain truly revers- H., & Waternaux, C. (1986). Correlates of low-level
ible? Are there possible treatments for them? lead exposure in urban children at 2 years of age.
Pediatrics, 77(6), 826–833.
How can subtle changes in cognition and CNS
Bellinger, D., Sloman, J., Leviton, A., Rabinowitz,
function associated with low-level exposures be M., Needleman, H. L., & Waternaux, C. (1991).
more accurately diagnosed and defined clini- Low-level lead exposure and children’s cognitive
cally in individuals? Which of the metals besides function in the preschool years. Pediatrics, 87(2),
lead and mercury will show similar effects and at 219–227.
what developmental stages? Is aging as vulner- Bellinger, D. C. (2007). Lead neurotoxicity in chil-
able a developmental stage for low-level metal dren: Decomposing the variability in dose–effect
effects as childhood, or at least more vulnerable relationships. American Journal of Industrial
than young adulthood and middle age? Medicine, 50, 720–728.
Correlates of Exposure to Metals 503

Bellinger, D. C., & Bellinger, A. M. (2006). Childhood ylmercury exposed children in French Guiana.
Lead poisoning: The torturous path from science Environmental Research, 89(1), 1–11.
to policy. Journal of Clinical Investigation, 116(4), Coscia, J. M., Ris, M. D., Succop, P. A., & Dietrich, K.
853–857. N. (2003). Cognitive development of lead exposed
Bellinger, D. C., Needleman, H. L., Leviton, A., children from ages 6 to 15 years: An application
Waternaux, C., Rabinowitz, M. B., & Nichols, of growth curve analysis. Child Neuropsychology,
M. L. (1984). Early sensory-motor development 9(1), 10–21.
and prenatal exposure to Lead. Neurobehavioral Daniels, J. L., Longnecker, M. P., Rowland, A. S., &
Toxicology and Teratology, 6(5), 387–402. Golding, J. (2004). Fish intake during pregnancy
Bleecker, M. L., Ford, D. P., Lindgren, K. N., Hoese, and early cognitive development of offspring.
V. M., Walsh, K. S., & Vaughan, C. G. (2005a). Epidemiology, 15(4), 394–402.
Differential effects of lead exposure on com- Davidson, P. W., Myers, G. J., Cox, C., Axtell, C.,
ponents of verbal memory. Occupational and Shamlaye, C., Sloane-Reeves, J., et al. (1998).
Environmental Medicine, 62(3), 181–187. Effects of prenatal and postnatal methylmercury
Bleecker, M. L., Ford, D. P., Vaughan, C. G., Lindgren, exposure from fish consumption on neurode-
K. N., Tiburzi, M. J., & Walsh, K. S. (2005b). Effect velopment: Outcomes at 66 months of age in the
of lead exposure and ergonomic stressors on seychelles child development study. Journal of the
peripheral nerve function. Environmental Health American Medical Association, 280(8), 701–707.
Perspectives, 113(12), 1730–1734. Davidson, P. W., Palumbo, D., Myers, G. J., Cox, C.,
Canfield, R. L., Henderson, C. R., Jr., Cory-Slechta, D. Shamlaye, C. F., Sloane-Reeves, J., et al. (2000).
A., Cox, C., Jusko, T. A., & Lanphear, B. P. (2003). Neurodevelopmental outcomes of seychellois chil-
Intellectual impairment in children with blood dren from the pilot cohort at 108 months follow-
lead concentrations below 10 microg per decili- ing prenatal exposure to methylmercury from a
ter. New England Journal of Medicine, 348(16), maternal fish diet. Environmental Research, 84(1),
1517–1526. 1–11.
Chen, A., Dietrich, K. N., Ware, J. H., Radcliffe, J., Debes, F., Budtz-Jorgensen, E., Weihe, P., White,
& Rogan, W. J. (2005). IQ and blood lead from 2 R. F., & Grandjean, P. (2006a). Impact of prena-
to 7 years of age: Are the effects in older children tal methylmercury exposure on neurobehavioral
the residual of high blood lead concentrations in function at age 14 years. Neurotoxicology and
2-year-olds? Environmental Health Perspectives, Teratology, 28(3), 363–375.
113(5), 597–601. Debes, F., Budtz-Jorgensen, E., Weihe, P., White,
Chiodo, L. M., Jacobson, S. W., & Jacobson, J. L. R. F., & Grandjean, P. (2006b). Impact of prena-
(2004). Neurodevelopmental effects of postnatal tal methylmercury exposure on neurobehavioral
lead exposure at very low levels. Neurotoxicology function at age 14 years. Neurotoxicology and
and Teratology, 26(3), 359–371. Teratology, 28(5), 536–547.
Choi, B. H. (1989). The effects of methylmercury on Dietrich, K. N., Berger, O. G., & Succop, P. A. (1993).
the developing brain. Progress in Neurobiology, Lead exposure and the motor developmental sta-
32(6), 447–470. tus of urban six-year-old children in the cincin-
Chuang, H. Y., Chao, K. Y., & Tsai, S. Y. (2005a). nati prospective study. Pediatrics, 91(2), 301–307.
Reversible neurobehavioral performance with Dietrich, K. N., Succop, P. A., Berger, O. G.,
reductions in blood lead levels—a prospec- Hammond, P. B., & Bornschein, R. L. (1991).
tive study on lead workers. Neurotoxicology and Lead exposure and the cognitive development
Teratology, 27(3), 497–504. of urban preschool children: The cincinnati lead
Chuang, H. Y., Chao, K. Y., & Wang, J. D. (2005b). study cohort at age 4 years. Neurotoxicology and
Estimation of burden of lead for offspring of Teratology, 13(2), 203–211.
female lead workers: A quality-adjusted life Dietrich, K. N., Succop, P. A., Berger, O. G., & Keith,
year (qaly) assessment. Journal of Toxicology R. W. (1992). Lead exposure and the central audi-
and Environmental Health. Part A, 68(17–18), tory processing abilities and cognitive devel-
1485–1496. opment of urban children: The Cincinnati lead
Clifton, J. C., 2nd. (2007). Mercury exposure and study cohort at age 5 years. Neurotoxicology and
public health. Pediatric Clinics of North America, Teratology, 14(1), 51–56.
54(2), 237–269, viii. Ernhart, C. B., & Greene, T. (1990). Low-level lead
Cordier, S., Garel, M., Mandereau, L., Morcel, H., exposure in the prenatal and early preschool
Doineau, P., Gosme-Seguret, S., et al. (2002). periods: Language development. Archives of
Neurodevelopmental investigations among meth- Environmental Health, 45(6), 342–354.
504 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Feldman, R. G. (1999). Occupational and environmen- exposure. Archives of Environmental Health,


tal neurotoxicology. Philidelphia, PA: Lippincott- 45(6), 335–341.
Raven Publishers. Hu, H., Tellez-Rojo, M. M., Bellinger, D., Smith,
Fergusson, D. M., Horwood, L. J., & Lynskey, M. T. D., Ettinger, A. S., Lamadrid-Figueroa, H., et al.
(1993). Early dentine lead levels and subsequent (2006). Fetal lead exposure at each stage of preg-
cognitive and behavioural development. Journal nancy as a predictor of infant mental develop-
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied ment. Environmental Health Perspectives, 114(11),
Disciplines, 34(2), 215–227. 1730–1735.
Garruto, R. M., Swyt, C., Fiori, C. E., Yanagihara, R., Jain, N. B., Potula, V., Schwartz, J., Vokonas, P. S.,
& Gajdusek, D. C. (1985). Intraneuronal deposi- Sparrow, D., Wright, R. O., et al. (2007). Lead levels
tion of calcium and aluminium in amyotropic lat- and ischemic heart disease in a prospective study
eral sclerosis of guam. Lancet, 2(8468), 1353. of middle-aged and elderly men: The va normative
Gilbert, S. G., & Grant-Webster, K. S. (1995). aging study. Environmental Health Perspectives,
Neurobehavioral effects of developmental meth- 115(6), 871–875.
ylmercury exposure. Environmental Health Janulewicz, P. A., White, R. F., & Palumbo, C. (2006).
Perspectives, 103(Suppl 6), 135–142. Role of neuroimaging. New York: Taylor and
Gomaa, A., Hu, H., Bellinger, D., Schwartz, J., Tsaih, Francis.
S. W., Gonzalez-Cossio, T., et al. (2002). Maternal Jeyaratnam, J., Boey, K. W., Ong, C. N., Chia, C. B.,
bone lead as an independent risk factor for fetal & Phoon, W. O. (1986). Neuropsychological stud-
neurotoxicity: A prospective study. Pediatrics, ies on lead workers in Singapore. British Journal of
110(1 Pt 1), 110–118. Industrial Medicine, 43(9), 626–629.
Grandjean, P., Weihe, P., White, R. F., Debes, F., Kjellstrom, T., Kennedy, P., Wallis, S., & Mantel, C.
Araki, S., Yokoyama, K., et al. (1997). Cognitive (1986). Physical and mental development of chil-
deficit in 7-year-old children with prenatal expo- dren with prenatal exposure to mercury from fish.
sure to methylmercury. Neurotoxicology and Stage 1: Preliminary tests at age 4. (report 3080).
Teratology, 19(6), 417–428. Stockholm: National Swedish Environmental
Grandjean, P., White, R. F., Nielsen, A., Cleary, D., Protection Board.
& de Oliveira Santos, E. C. (1999). Methylmercury Kjellstrom, T., Kennedy, P., Wallis, S., & Mantel, C.
neurotoxicity in amazonian children down- (1989). Physical and mental development of chil-
stream from gold mining. Environmental Health dren with prenatal exposure to mercury from fish.
Perspectives, 107(7), 587–591. Stage 2: Interviews and psychological tests at age
Haenninen, H., Hernberg, S., Mantere, P., Vesanto, R., 6. (report 3642). Stockholm: National Swedish
& Jalkanen, M. (1978). Psychological performance Environmental Protection Board.
of subjects with low exposure to lead. Journal of Kordas, K., Canfield, R. L., Lopez, P., Rosado, J. L.,
Occupational Medicine, 20(10), 683–689. Vargas, G. G., Cebrian, M. E., et al. (2006). Deficits
Hansen, O. N., Trillingsgaard, A., Beese, I., Lyngbye, in cognitive function and achievement in Mexican
T., & Grandjean, P. (1989). A neuropsychological first-graders with low blood lead concentrations.
study of children with elevated dentine lead level: Environmental Research, 100(3), 371–386.
Assessment of the effect of lead in different socio- Krieg, E. F., Jr., Chrislip, D. W., Crespo, C. J.,
economic groups. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, Brightwell, W. S., Ehrenberg, R. L., & Otto, D.
11(3), 205–213. A. (2005). The relationship between blood lead
Hightower, J. M., & Moore, D. (2003). Mercury lev- levels and neurobehavioral test performance in
els in high-end consumers of fish. Environmental NHANES III and related occupational studies.
Health Perspectives, 111(4), 604–608. Public Health Reports, 120(3), 240–251.
Hogstedt, C., Hane, M., Agrell, A., & Bodin, L. (1983). Kumar, P., Husain, S. G., Murthy, R. C., Srivastava,
Neuropsychological test results and symptoms S. P., Anand, M., Ali, M. M., et al. (2002).
among workers with well-defined long-term expo- Neuropsychological studies on lead battery work-
sure to lead. British Journal of Industrial Medicine, ers. Veterinary and Human Toxicology, 44(2),
40(1), 99–105. 76–78.
Hu, H., Aro, A., & Rotnitzky, A. (1995). Bone lead Lanphear, B. P., Dietrich, K., Auinger, P., & Cox, C.
measured by x-ray fluorescence: Epidemiologic (2000). Cognitive deficits associated with blood
methods. Environmental Health Perspectives, lead concentrations <10 microg/dl in us children
103(Suppl 1), 105–110. and adolescents. Public Health Reports, 115(6),
Hu, H., Milder, F. L., & Burger, D. E. (1990). 521–529.
X-ray fluorescence measurements of lead bur- Lucchini, R., Albini, E., Cortesi, I., Placidi, D.,
den in subjects with low-level community lead Bergamaschi, E., Traversa, F., et al. (2000).
Correlates of Exposure to Metals 505

Assessment of neurobehavioral performance as a Needleman, H. L. (2004). Low level lead exposure


function of current and cumulative occupational and the development of children. The Southeast
lead exposure. Neurotoxicology, 21(5), 805–811. Asian Journal of Tropical Medicine and Public
McMichael, A. J., Baghurst, P. A., Vimpani, G. V., Health, 35(2), 252–254.
Robertson, E. F., Wigg, N. R., & Tong, S. L. (1992). Needleman, H. L., & Bellinger, D. C. (1981). The epi-
Sociodemographic factors modifying the effect of demiology of low-level lead exposure in child-
environmental lead on neuropsychological devel- hood. Journal of the American Academy of Child
opment in early childhood. Neurotoxicology and Psychiatry, 20(3), 496–512.
Teratology, 14(5), 321–327. Needleman, H. L., Leviton, A., & Bellinger, D. (1982).
McMichael, A. J., Baghurst, P. A., Wigg, N. R., Lead-associated intellectual deficit. New England
Vimpani, G. V., Robertson, E. F., & Roberts, R. Journal of Medicine, 306(6), 367.
J. (1988). Port Pirie cohort study: Environmental Needleman, H. L., Schell, A., Bellinger, D., Leviton,
exposure to lead and children’s abilities at the age A., & Allred, E. N. (1990). The long-term effects
of four years. New England Journal of Medicine, of exposure to low doses of Lead in childhood. An
319(8), 468–475. 11-year follow-up report. New England Journal of
Mendelsohn, A. L., Dreyer, B. P., Fierman, A. H., Medicine, 322(2), 83–88.
Rosen, C. M., Legano, L. A., Kruger, H. A., et al. Nordberg, M., Winblad, B., Fratiglioni, L., & Basun,
(1998). Low-level lead exposure and behavior in H. (2000). Lead concentrations in elderly urban
early childhood. Pediatrics, 101(3), E10. people related to blood pressure and mental per-
Muldoon, S. B., Cauley, J. A., Kuller, L. H., Morrow, formance: Results from a population-based study.
L., Needleman, H. L., Scott, J., et al. (1996). Effects American Journal of Industrial Medicine, 38(3),
of blood lead levels on cognitive function of older 290–294.
women. Neuroepidemiology, 15(2), 62–72. Osterberg, K., Borjesson, J., Gerhardsson, L., Schutz,
Murata, K., Weihe, P., Budtz-Jorgensen, E., Jorgensen, A., & Skerfving, S. (1997). A neurobehavioural
P. J., & Grandjean, P. (2004). Delayed brainstem study of long-term occupational inorganic lead
auditory evoked potential latencies in 14-year-old exposure. The Science of the Total Environment,
children exposed to methylmercury. The Journal 201(1), 39–51.
of Pediatrics, 144(2), 177–183. Parkinson, A. (1986). A picture of psychiatric/mental
Myers, G. J., & Davidson, P. W. (2000). Does meth- health nursing. Pro Re Nata PRN, 8(6), 11.
ylmercury have a role in causing developmental Schwartz, B. S., Lee, B. K., Bandeen-Roche, K.,
disabilities in children? Environmental Health Stewart, W., Bolla, K., Links, J., et al. (2005).
Perspectives, 108(Suppl 3), 413–420. Occupational lead exposure and longitudi-
Myers, G. J., Davidson, P. W., Cox, C., Shamlaye, C., nal decline in neurobehavioral test scores.
Cernichiari, E., & Clarkson, T. W. (2000). Twenty- Epidemiology, 16(1), 106–113.
seven years studying the human neurotoxic- Schwartz, B. S., Stewart, W., & Hu, H. (2002).
ity of methylmercury exposure. Environmental Neurobehavioural testing in workers occu-
Research, 83(3), 275–285. pationally exposed to lead. Occupational and
Myers, G. J., Davidson, P. W., Cox, C., Shamlaye, Environmental Medicine, 59(9), 648–649.
C. F., Tanner, M. A., Marsh, D. O., et al. (1995a). Schwartz, B. S., Stewart, W. F., Todd, A. C., & Links,
Summary of the seychelles child development J. M. (1999). Predictors of dimercaptosuccinic acid
study on the relationship of fetal methylmercury chelatable lead and tibial lead in former organo-
exposure to neurodevelopment. Neurotoxicology, lead manufacturing workers. Occupational and
16(4), 711–716. Environmental Medicine, 56(1), 22–29.
Myers, G. J., Marsh, D. O., Davidson, P. W., Cox, C., Shen, X. M., Yan, C. H., Guo, D., Wu, S. M., Li, R.
Shamlaye, C. F., Tanner, M., et al. (1995b). Main Q., Huang, H., et al. (1998). Low-level prenatal
neurodevelopmental study of seychellois children lead exposure and neurobehavioral development
following in utero exposure to methylmercury of children in the first year of life: A prospective
from a maternal fish diet: Outcome at six months. study in Shanghai. Environmental Research, 79(1),
Neurotoxicology, 16(4), 653–664. 1–8.
Myers, R. H. (2006). Considerations for genom- Stiles, K. M., & Bellinger, D. C. (1993).
ewide association studies in Parkinson disease. Neuropsychological correlates of low-level lead
American Journal of Human Genetics, 78(6), exposure in school-age children: A prospec-
1081–1082. tive study. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 15(1),
Needleman, H. L. (1979). Lead levels and children’s 27–35.
psychologic performance. New England Journal of Stokes, L., Letz, R., Gerr, F., Kolczak, M., McNeill,
Medicine, 301(3), 163. F. E., Chettle, D. R., et al. (1998). Neurotoxicity in
506 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

young adults 20 years after childhood exposure resonance spectroscopic evidence of glial effects
to lead: The bunker hill experience. Occupational of cumulative lead exposure in the adult human
and Environmental Medicine, 55(8), 507–516. hippocampus. Environmental Health Perspectives,
Tellez-Rojo, M. M., Bellinger, D. C., Arroyo-Quiroz, 115(4), 519–523.
C., Lamadrid-Figueroa, H., Mercado-Garcia, A., Weisskopf, M. G., Wright, R. O., Schwartz, J., Spiro,
Schnaas-Arrieta, L., et al. (2006). Longitudinal A., 3rd, Sparrow, D., Aro, A., et al. (2004b).
associations between blood lead concentrations Cumulative lead exposure and prospective change
lower than 10 microg/dl and neurobehavioral in cognition among elderly men: The va normative
development in environmentally exposed children aging study. American Journal of Epidemiology,
in Mexico city. Pediatrics, 118(2), e323–330. 160(12), 1184–1193.
Thimerosal: Updated statement. An advisory White, R. F. (1992). Clinical syndromes in adult
committee statement (acs). (2007). Canada neuropsychology: The practitioner’s handbook.
Communicable Disease Report, 33(ACS-6), 1–13. Neurobehavioral effects of toxic exposures (Chapter
Tong, S., McMichael, A. J., & Baghurst, P. A. (2000). 1). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Interactions between environmental lead expo- White, R. F., Diamond, R., Proctor, S., Morey, C., &
sure and sociodemographic factors on cognitive Hu, H. (1993a). Residual cognitive deficits 50 years
development. Archives of Environmental Health, after lead poisoning during childhood. British
55(5), 330–335. Journal of Industrial Medicine, 50(7), 613–622.
Valciukas, J. A., & Lilis, R. (1980). Psychometric tech- White, R. F., Feldman, R. G., Moss, M. B., & Proctor,
niques in environmental research. Environmental S. P. (1993b). Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI),
Research, 21(2), 275–297. neurobehavioral testing, and toxic encephalop-
Wasserman, G. A., Graziano, J. H., Factor-Litvak, P., athy: Two cases. Environmental Research, 61(1),
Popovac, D., Morina, N., Musabegovic, A., et al. 117–123.
(1994). Consequences of lead exposure and iron sup- Wigg, N. R., Vimpani, G. V., McMichael, A. J.,
plementation on childhood development at age 4 Baghurst, P. A., Robertson, E. F., & Roberts, R. J.
years. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 16(3), 233–240. (1988). Port Pirie cohort study: Childhood blood
Weisskopf, M. G., Hu, H., Mulkern, R. V., White, R., lead and neuropsychological development at age
Aro, A., Oliveira, S., et al. (2004a). Cognitive defi- two years. Journal of Epidemiology and Community
cits and magnetic resonance spectroscopy in adult Health, 42(3), 213–219.
monozygotic twins with lead poisoning. Yokoyama, K., Araki, S., & Aono, H. (1988).
Environmental Health Perspectives, 112(5), 620–625. Reversibility of psychological performance in sub-
Weisskopf, M. G., Hu, H., Sparrow, D., Lenkinski, clinical lead absorption. Neurotoxicology, 9(3),
R. E., & Wright, R. O. (2007). Proton magnetic 405–410.
21

Clinical Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia


Philip D. Harvey and Richard S. E. Keefe

People with schizophrenia have been known interventions for people with schizophrenia. Of
since the initial definitions of the condition to further relevance is the level of detail and length
manifest an array of cognitive impairments. of assessment required to gain suitable informa-
These impairments appear to be generally mod- tion from an assessment of NP performance.
erate to severe in nature and to be accompanied
by somewhat smaller changes in global intel-
Course of Cognitive Impairments
lectual functioning. It appears as if the major-
ity, if not all, people with schizophrenia show Cognitive impairments in schizophrenia are
evidence of impairments in performance on clearly detectable at the time of the first psy-
neuropsychological (NP) tests and that these chotic episode (Bilder et al., 2000; Mohamed
impairments are present over the entire course et al., 1999). These impairments are similar in
of the illness. These impairments are not caused severity to those seen in patients with an estab-
by medications or psychotic symptoms. NP lished course of illness and a history of antipsy-
impairments in schizophrenia are function- chotic treatments (Saykin et al., 1994). These
ally relevant, in that, consistent with other data suggest, consistent with several different
conditions, the severity of cognitive deficits is follow-up studies of first episode patients (e.g.,
also associated with impairments in a variety Hoff et al., 1999), that cognitive impairments
of everyday living skills, including vocational, do not show progressive change in the period
social, role, and independent living skills. immediately after the onset of the illness.
In trying to understand NP impairments and Research on individuals prior to the devel-
their importance in schizophrenia, there are sev- opment of schizophrenia has also suggested
eral important issues. These include the course that NP impairments are already present at this
of impairment in NP functioning, including time. Impairments in a wide array of cognitive
when the impairments can be detected and functions are also present in individuals who are
changes over the lifespan, the profi le of impair- identified as meeting clinical criteria for being in
ment and whether these impairments are simi- a prodromal period prior to the onset of schizo-
lar to other conditions where brain dysfunction phrenia (Lencz et al., 2006). While most of the
is present, the severity of impairment, the prev- NP ability areas that are impaired in people who
alence of impairments and whether any indi- meet diagnostic criteria for schizophrenia are
viduals with the illness are spared, and whether also impaired in prodromal cases, it has been
these impairments are related in general vs. spe- argued that there is some variation in the sever-
cific ways to functional deficits. Each of these ity impairments prior to the onset of psychosis.
issues is important for the assessment of func- Verbal memory and olfactory function may be
tional potential and development of treatment particularly impaired in those individuals who

507
508 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

will later develop a psychotic illness (Brewer on NP tests does not decline, on either a group-
et al., 2003). Of further interest is the finding mean or case-by-case basis between the period
that NP impairments and functional deficits are of the first episode until quite later in life. As
correlated in cases who do not yet meet diag- reviewed by Rund (1998), studies with a vari-
nostic criteria for schizophrenia. As shown by ety of NP assessments, younger patient popula-
Cornblatt et al. (2007), social abnormalities and tions, and follow-up periods have not revealed
impairments in NP performance are intercor- substantial evidence of change in NP perfor-
related, while neither of these domains of func- mance between the first episode and late life.
tioning are correlated with the level of severity These data have led some to the conclusion that
of “subpsychotic” symptoms shown by these cognitive impairments in schizophrenia resem-
putatively prodromal cases. ble the consequences of a static encephalopathy,
Studies that have followed cases who were at least during the early to middle years of adult
selected for increased risk for the development of life (Goldberg et al., 1993).
schizophrenia, such as having an affected parent, In contrast to these findings of stability of
have also reported NP impairment detectable performance in early to middle adulthood, there
years before the onset symptoms (Seidman et al., is evidence from a variety of sources indicating
2006). In specific, impairments in attentional and that at least a subset of patients with schizophre-
working memory functions are detectable in chil- nia show evidence of decline exceeding normal
dren whose parents have schizophrenia as early aging influences later in their life (Arnold et al.,
as the pre-teen years (Cornblatt & Erlenmeyer- 1995; Davidson et al., 1995; Harvey et al., 1999).
Kinling, 1985) and some evidence has suggested The majority of the replicated evidence regard-
that the prevalence of wide-ranging attentional ing cognitive decline beyond normal aging stan-
deficits identifies those vulnerable children who dards has come from patients with a chronic
are most likely to develop schizophrenia later in course of illness, extreme functional disability,
life (Cornblatt et al., 1999). and extended stays in chronic psychiatric hos-
Population-based studies that have related pitals, nursing homes, or other full-care facil-
the development of schizophrenia later in life to ities. These patients also appear to be affected
late-adolescent NP performance have also sug- by treatment-resistant psychotic symptoms,
gested that a significant number of individuals and there is some tentative evidence that the
who are examined at age 16 and are apparently ongoing severity of psychosis is related to risk
healthy at that time have cognitive changes that for decline in NP performance over follow-up
predict later schizophrenia. In a series of stud- periods ranging from 18 to 72 months (Harvey
ies based on the Israeli draft board assessments, et al., 2003). Even these patients appear to mani-
Davidson and colleagues have reported on a fest NP decline only after age 65 or so (Friedman
number of NP impairments that predict the et al., 2001), suggesting that with standard NP
later development of schizophrenia (Davidson assessment methods, cognitive decline appears
et al., 1999). These include intellectual decline to be detectable only in later life. An exception
across assessments (Reichenberg et al., 2005), to these findings are those of Granholm et al.
excessive variation across different cognitive (2000), who noted that patients in the forties
ability areas (Reichenberg et al., 2006), and gen- and fifties showed evidence of requiring consid-
eralized cognitive impairments (Weiser et al., erably more processing resources than healthy
2004). While these impairments do not have controls (HC) of the same age to perform infor-
high levels of sensitivity to the eventual devel- mation processing tasks; in people with schizo-
opment of schizophrenia, they do suggest that, phrenia, these declines were age related.
for a substantial number of people who even- In summary of evidence regarding the course
tually develop schizophrenia, NP impairments of cognitive impairments in schizophrenia, the
are detectable when no other signs of illness are majority of the evidence suggests that impair-
immediately apparent. ments are detectable prior to the onset of the ill-
Turning to the course of cognitive impair- ness, that these impairments at the time of the
ments after the development of the illness, the first episode appear similar in severity and pro-
bulk of the evidence suggests that performance fi le to those seen in more chronic patients, and
Clinical Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia 509

that change over the course of the lifespan is a more direct examination of structural and
modest until later life. Later life patients appear functional regional brain dysfunction, partic-
to be more vulnerable to cognitive decline if ularly when paired with evocative cognitive
they have persistent psychosis and lifelong func- procedures, but interest in the profiles of cog-
tional disability. nitive impairments has persisted. This interest
While these findings have been interpreted as is focused on several different dimensions: the
reflecting a static encephalopathy, there is con- notion of general versus specific performance
siderable neuroimaging evidence (starting with deficits, the ability to identify differential perfor-
DeLisi et al., 1997) to suggest that there are active mance on potentially separable cognitive ability
cortical changes occurring over the lifespan in domains, and the question of whether perfor-
patients with schizophrenia, beginning with mance deficits in schizophrenia resemble those
the first episode. These declines are detected seen in disorders with known brain dysfunc-
in cross-sectional studies of patients across the tion (e.g., frontal–striatal abnormalities, cortical
lifespan, particularly during periods of time dementia, and amnestic syndromes).
when NP performance fails to decline, such The idea of identification of differential per-
as immediately after the first episode (DeLisi formance deficits is an old one in both clinical
et al., 2004). Treatment with atypical antipsy- neuropsychology and schizophrenia research.
chotic medications has been reported to be Often referred to as a “double-dissociation,” , the
associated with less cortical change (Lieberman identification of differential patterns of spared
et al., 2005), as has good treatment response with and impaired performance across different abil-
conventional medications, resulting in minimal ity areas as a clue to information about origins
residual negative symptoms (Lieberman et al., of neuropathology has long been an assessment
2001). For most patients, however, these data goal. Similarly, cognitively oriented schizophre-
suggest that the encephalopathy in schizophre- nia research has focused on the identification of
nia is not static and raise questions about the rea- “differential deficits” for at least the last 30 years
sons for the failure to detect changes on NP tests (Chapman & Chapman, 1978). This goal has been
in these same patients. It is possible that either based on the notion that people with schizophre-
the NP tests are not sensitive to these changes nia often have deficits in performance on multi-
or that these changes do not induce any mean- ple cognitive ability domains, so that assessment
ingful cognitive burden in the short term. The of a limited number of ability domains may lead
patients who show the greatest cortical changes to spurious conclusions. For instance, if global
in short-term studies earlier in life (Davis et al., intellectual performance, episodic memory,
1998) are the same treatment refractory, consis- and attentional performance are all impaired
tently disabled patients who show the greatest compared to healthy people, then assessment of
cognitive declines later. Possibly, some elements episodic memory alone could lead to the errone-
of cognitive reserve are protecting patients from ous conclusion of a specific ability deficit when
showing NP functional declines in the presence episodic memory is no more impaired than any
of cortical compromise. Later research using other ability that would have been assessed.
a mix of NP and experimental neuroscience This problem is also potentially compounded
tests will be required in order to discriminate by the fact that intellectual performance is com-
between these possibilities. promised in schizophrenia (David et al., 1997).
Since most other cognitive abilities are corre-
lated with scores on intelligence tests, general
Profile of Cognitive Impairment
declines in intellectual functioning could pro-
Since NP tests were designed to detect brain dys- duce poor scores on a whole array of different
function and there is some evidence of specific cognitive abilities. Patients with schizophrenia
relations between NP performance deficits and are also apparently heterogenous in the extent
regional brain dysfunction, there has been con- of their intellectual decline, with some patients
siderable interest in the use of profi les of impaired showing psychometric evidence of decline
performance to detect possible CNS substrates while others appear to have generally preserved
of impairment. Neuroimaging techniques provide functioning (Weickert et al., 2000). Thus, the
510 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

challenge in assessment of people with schizo- reported that the single largest correlate of over-
phrenia is not in the area of detection of impair- all cognitive performance in schizophrenia is
ment, but rather identification of ability areas a measure of the speed of cognitive processing
that are relatively spared and hence serve as a (see Dickinson et al., 2007). For instance, in the
reference point for differential deficit inferences baseline results (Keefe et al., 2006) of the large
(Harvey et al., 2000). (n = 1035 with full assessments) CATIE study,
Many studies have used factor analysis to processing speed as measured by WAIS-III
identify dimensions of NP performance that digit symbol accounted for 61% of the variance
could then be evaluated for their differential in the total NP composite score, with an addi-
levels of impairment compared to HC. Recent tional measure of psychomotor speed (grooved
studies have yielded inconsistent and provoca- pegboard) accounting for an additional 7%. In
tive results. Factor analyses of NP performance contrast, measures of executive functioning
on the part of people with schizophrenia have (Wisconsin Card Sorting Test—WCST), atten-
yielded factor solutions ranging from 1 (Keefe tion (Continuous Performance Test—CPT), and
et al., 2006) to 6 (Gladsjo et al., 2004) factors. spatial working memory accounted for 2% each
These studies cannot be faulted for small sample of the variance in total score.
sizes (the study with the single-factor solution These data suggest more careful evaluation
had over 1331 participants), narrow selection of of the long-held assumption that schizophre-
test batteries, or other major confounds. nia is marked by a profile of discriminable per-
Sophisticated studies using confirmatory formance deficits across several critical ability
modeling procedures have been applied to the areas (executive functioning, episodic memory,
issue of the dimensional structure of NP deficits working memory, attention, and processing
in schizophrenia. Perhaps the most sophisticated speed). The possibility has been raised that
of those studies (Dickinson et al., 2004), using the profi le of impairments seen is actually due
a broad assessment including complete WAIS to a global performance deficits, combined
and WMS batteries in addition to other tests, with the differential ability of different cogni-
demonstrated that the differences in NP perfor- tive measures to detect impairment in general.
mance between HC and schizophrenia patients Differential sensitivity to performance deficits,
are best explained by a single “severity” dimen- due to reliability, administration issues, and
sion. This dimension did not reflect simple intel- other factors, combined with a generalized per-
lectual impairment, but rather tapped a number formance deficit, might lead to the detection of
of apparently independent ability domains. apparent profiles of impairment.
The fit of the unifactorial impairment dimen- Related to the emerging questions about
sion was considerably better than a multifacto- whether cognition in schizophrenia truly
rial approach to the differences in performance reflects a profi le of variably impaired abilities
between schizophrenic and healthy samples. as compared to a single global deficit are the
A question that arises, when the overall pro- questions as to whether performance deficits on
file of performance in schizophrenia and the the part of people with schizophrenia resemble
dimensions of relative impairment compared to the profi le of impairments seen in people with
HC are found to be unidimensional, is whether identifiable CNS lesions and resulting cognitive
there is a single aspect of impairment that changes. Among the three possibilities proposed
accounts for all other measured deficits. Several to describe the impairments seen in people with
such impairments have been proposed previ- schizophrenia are frontostriatal (i.e., subcorti-
ously, including working memory impairments cal) conditions, cortical dementias, and focal
(Goldman-Rakic, 1994), overall information amnestic syndromes. Each will be considered
processing resource availability (Harvey et al., briefly below.
2006b), and processing (i.e., psychomotor) speed
(Dickinson et al., 2007). Even these ability areas
Frontostriatal Conditions
may be interrelated, however, and these domains
may not be as separable as they would seem from These conditions, with the prototype being
a superficial examination. Multiple studies have Huntington’s disease, involve a profi le of
Clinical Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia 511

impairment with reductions in executive func- State Examination (MMSE) scores or not, they
tioning, attention, processing speed, and spe- routinely outperform AD patients on delayed
cific changes in episodic memory (Paulsen et al., recall memory. Interestingly, in two separate
1995b). Given the prominence of executive, studies (Davidson et al., 1996; Heaton et al.,
attention, and processing speed abnormali- 1994) with very different populations of patients
ties, the often-reported sparing of recognition with schizophrenia, the global level of impair-
memory in people with schizophrenia has led ment across ability areas was greater in people
to the suggestion of resemblance to fronto- with schizophrenia than those with AD, other
striatal conditions, in that studies of people than for delayed recall memory, suggesting that
with schizophrenia (e.g., Paulsen et al., 1995a; the cortical dementia descriptor does not cap-
Turetsky et al., 2002) have suggested that a ture the signature of impairment in people with
frontostriatal memory profile is more common schizophrenia.
than a cortical profi le (marked by no sparing of
recognition memory) or unimpaired profile in
Amnesia
patients with schizophrenia. However, it also
seems likely that in schizophrenia this profi le People with schizophrenia have grossly impaired
may also be affected by global severity; when episodic memory and may perform in the same
geriatric schizophrenia patients are followed range on some tests as people with amnestic
up, a large proportion of patients with fronto- conditions. Some studies have suggested that
striatal profi le develop a cortical profi le over impairments in episodic memory are particu-
time (Bowie et al., 2004). Thus, as suggested in larly extreme compared to other ability areas
the dementia literature, frontostriatal profi les (e.g., Saykin et al., 1991). These results need to
may simply be produced by the presence of less be considered in the context of the Dickinson
severe global impairments. Further, there was et al. results, which were based on a much
markedly little internal consistency between broader meta-analysis. Similar to amnesia,
putative markers of the frontostriatal impair- patients with schizophrenia show procedural
ment in patients with schizophrenia in the one memory deficits that are modest compared to
study that examined this issue. In patients with their other impairments (Heinrichs & Zakzanis,
frontostriatal conditions, recall memory and 1998). At the same time, it is rare to see a patient
verbal fluency are typically impaired while rec- with schizophrenia with an isolated impair-
ognition memory and confrontation memory ment in episodic memory. It is our conclusion
are often spared. Spared recognition memory that schizophrenia is more similar to conditions
did not predict spared confrontation naming in with wide-ranging cognitive deficits and not to
a study with 239 schizophrenia patients, leading patients with isolated impairments in episodic
to only 19% overlap between memory-derived memory.
and language-derived frontostriatal profi les Another important issue is whether the pro-
(Harvey et al., 2002). fi le of NP deficits in schizophrenia reflects spe-
cific impairments in regional brain functions.
For instance, it has been argued that the cog-
Cortical Dementia
nitive impairments seen in schizophrenia are
Patients with schizophrenia show pervasive comparable to those seen in cases with specific
cognitive deficits and most meet the behavioral frontal lobe lesions. This argument is based both
criteria for dementia. In addition, some pro- on NP test performance and on evidence from
portion of patients with schizophrenia, both neuroimaging studies suggesting reduced fron-
younger and older, show evidence of a corti- tal lobe activity (i.e., hypofrontality) during cog-
cal profi le of impairment, with no evidence of nitive challenge procedures (e.g., Callicott et al.,
recognition memory being spared compared 1999). While many aspects of NP impairment in
to delayed recall (see above). Yet, when people schizophrenia are apparently similar to frontal
with schizophrenia are compared to patients lobe dysfunction, including deficits in abstrac-
with Alzheimer’s disease (AD), whether they tion and problem solving (Palmer & Heaton,
are younger or older, matched on Mini-Mental 2000), working memory (Kim et al., 2004), and
512 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

attention (Bryson & Bell, 2003), some evidence processing capacity seen in schizophrenia is not
from clinical NP studies of patients with schizo- clear from these results, but it appears that the
phrenia fail to show evidence of consistent pat- brain response to processing load is an indica-
terns of frontal lobe-like dysfunctions. For tor, not a cause, of alterations in performance.
instance, Gambini et al. (2003) reported that
there were four different clusters of patients that
Summary
were detected when performance was measured
on a battery of frontal-type tests, suggesting a The true nature of the profi le of cognitive
more global profile of performance. Further, impairments in schizophrenia is still essentially
questions have been raised about the specific- unresolved. It could be concluded that people
ity of classical NP tests to frontal lobe dysfunc- with schizophrenia do not show marked simi-
tions, with considerable data suggesting that larities to people with known neurological dis-
deficits may be produced by lesions in a variety orders or with specific regional brain lesions in
of cortical and subcortical areas (Anderson et al., the profi le of their cognitive impairments and
1991). that to compare schizophrenia to identified
Recent intriguing findings from the clini- conditions is unproductive. The dimensional
cal neuroscience literature show how complex structure of symptoms is an open question, and
the relationships between schizophrenia, brain it is not clear if statistical approaches will pro-
activity, and NP test performance are. Research vide the answer. As far as global impairment is
by Callicott and colleagues (Callicott et al., 1999, considered, it seems quite important to deter-
2000) has employed a parametric examination mine why processing speed accounts for such
of cognitive processing demands, the “n-back” a substantial amount of the overall impair-
working memory test. In this assessment, sub- ment seen in schizophrenia, and why this area
jects are required to maintain information in accounts for impairments in other areas. Study
working memory and respond to information of processing speed abnormalities seems clearly
that was presented in the immediately prior important and will need to be pursued in order
stimulus, or separated from the current stimu- to determine if this should be a primary treat-
lus by 1, 2, 3, or more items. People with schizo- ment goal.
phrenia find this test particularly challenging,
and, at 2-back conditions, show evidence of
Severity of Impairments
grossly diminished activation of the anterior
frontal cortex compared to HC performing Despite our concerns about the statistical and
the same test, much like their performance on neurobiological validity of different elements
other frontal-dependent tests such as WCST. of cognitive impairments in schizophrenia, we
Interestingly, when HC are asked to perform a present our discussion of the severity of impair-
3-back version of the test, their cortical activa- ments using standard ability area definitions.
tion looks like the performance of people with We do this because ability areas are defined by
schizophrenia at the 2-back condition, with tests and tests are the metric with which cog-
reduced anterior activation and excessive acti- nitive performance data are collected. That
vation of posterior regions. When people with said, we note that the severity of impairments
schizophrenia perform the 1-back test, their detected by different tests may be as much a
brain activation looks like healthy individuals function of the performance of the tests as that
performing at 2-back. of the patient. These issues are addressed else-
These findings indicate that altered cor- where in this volume.
tical activation under increased processing In this section we consider performance defi-
demands is not specific to schizophrenia, but cits relative to healthy individuals who are rea-
rather an apparently normative response. sonably similar in demographic characteristics
“Hypofrontality” appears not to be a diagnos- that affect cognitive functioning. As has been
tically sensitive indicator, but rather a behav- noted for decades, the changes in educational
ioral index of processing demands and capacity attainment associated with schizophrenia are
(Cannon et al., 2005). The origin of the reduced substantial, although considerable evidence
Clinical Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia 513

Table 21–1. Level of Impairment in Cognitive Abilities in Schizophrenia


Mild Moderate Severe
Long-Term Factual Memory X
Perceptual Skills X
Manual Dexterity X
Attention
Sustained Attention X
Selective Attention X
Working Memory
Spatial Working Memory X
Verbal Working Memory X
Episodic Memory
Verbal Learning X
Nonverbal memory (Spatial X
Memory)
Delayed Recall X
Delayed Recognition X
Procedural Memory X
Executive Functions X
Processing Speed X
Verbal Skills
Naming X
Verbal Fluency X

from large-scale studies shows that the corre- performance with consistency across ability
lation between educational attainment and NP areas.
performance is quite similar in HC and schizo- As we mentioned above, there is cognitive
phrenia samples (e.g., Davidson et al., 1995). decline at the time of the first diagnosis of schizo-
Thus, “matching” on education between HC and phrenia. Thus, the concept of “premorbid” intellec-
schizophrenia samples is a mistake; the natural tual functioning refers to the idea that functioning
history of schizophrenia leads to reduced edu- before the onset of the illness is less impaired than
cational attainment. Several alternative strate- after. Thus, comparison of HC and schizophrenia
gies have been proposed. They include “relative patients needs to consider both the current level
matching,” which would select HC and schizo- of performance of the schizophrenia patients
phrenia patients as yoked comparisons based (likely reduced from premorbid functioning)
on their position in the respective education and their impairment compared to their perfor-
distribution (Davidson et al., 1996), matching mance prior to the development of the illness (i.e.,
on parental education (Keefe et al., 2005), and their premorbid functioning). Thus, the impor-
using education as a covariate rather than a tant aspects of cognitive impairment in schizo-
rigid matching variable. phrenia are those that are impaired compared to
An additional question in the domain of premorbid functioning and reflect deterioration
defining levels of impairment (this is an issue in performance subsequent to the development of
that will return full-force in the next section the illness. For instance, it is possible to identify
on normal performance) is how is impairment patients with schizophrenia who appear to vary
defined? Clearly a population of people with in their level of intellectual decline (Weickert
mental retardation would perform poorly on et al., 2000).
the tests of classical cognitive abilities in schizo- When people with schizophrenia are compared
phrenia, such as episodic memory and execu- to HC subjects, their performance is consistently
tive functioning. Yet, would they be performing impaired, regardless of the standards used. Table
worse than their IQ would predict? Even if 21–1 shows the relative level of impairment across
they performed more poorly than HC samples, these samples based on several different sources
this would not reflect a decline in their perfor- of data (as previously summarized by Gold and
mance in these abilities, but a decline in their Harvey [1993] and Bowie and Harvey [2005]). In
514 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

this table, data regarding conceptual ability areas NP normality in people with schizophrenia. For
are provided. For a detailed assessment look- example, estimates of the proportion of neurop-
ing at individual tests (which is a more desir- sychologically impaired patients have varied
able strategy) please see Heinrichs and Zakzanis from 11% (Torrey et al., 1994) to approximately
(1998). We define mild impairments as those that 73% (Palmer et al., 1997), and up to 98% (Keefe
are about 0.5 SD compared to expected, mod- et al., 2005) or 100% (Wilk et al., 2005). These
erate impairment as 0.5–1.5 SD compared to inconsistencies are due, in part, to the different
expected scores, and severe are more than 1.5 SD criteria used to classify normality. There have
compared to standards. These impairments are, been at least three previously published classi-
whenever possible, indexed relative to premorbid fication methods. Using the Individual Profi le
standards, but this is not possible in all cases. Rating (IPR) procedure presented by Kremen
Premorbid intellectual functioning is often et al. (2000), performance on each ability area
estimated by performance on various tests of is computed as the mean of the z-scores of the
old learning such as word recognition reading individual measures comprising the ability area.
(Gladsjo et al., 1999), as considerable evidence Individual NP profi les were then rated for sever-
suggests that performance on these tests is least ity of impairment using the classification crite-
vulnerable to decline, prior to or during the ill- ria of Kremen et al. (2000). A profile is generally
ness (Harvey et al., 2000). While a discussion of considered abnormal when at least two func-
the validity of this process could fi ll this entire tions were more than 2 SDs below the norma-
chapter, there are several studies providing evi- tive mean. However, a profi le with only a single
dence regarding the congruence of performance impaired function could be rated as abnormal if
on these measures both with data collected that function was extremely impaired (i.e., >3
prior to illness (Tracy et al., 1996) and evidence SDs below the normative mean). Sizable dis-
regarding resistance to decline (Harvey et al., crepancies between domains of function were
2006a). These studies have shown that regard- also considered indicating compromised NP
less of whether the follow-up assessments are function, even if neither function was more
performed immediately after the development than 2 SDs below the normative mean. Patients
of the illness (Reichenberg et al., 2005) and are classified into four groups: “neuropsy-
compared to recent test scores or are compared chologically normal,” “borderline neuropsy-
to relative declines in older patients in the pro- chologically normal,” “neuropsychologically
cess of deterioration (Harvey et al., 2006a), these abnormal,” or “neuropsychologically severely
scores have the same characteristics. impaired.” The IPR procedure allows assess-
As can be seen in the table, the general level of ment of both the absolute level of performance
impairment is moderate to severe and there are in each ability area, and the extent of within-
very few mildly impaired areas. Interestingly, subject variability across domains, analogous to
the same areas that are moderately to severely the way in which one would clinically evaluate
impaired are those that are most consistently individual NP profiles.
related to impairments in everyday functional Palmer et al. (1997) suggested that in keep-
skills (Green, 1996). Most abilities are performed ing with accepted definitions of general or clin-
at levels consistent with those seen in cases with ically significant cognitive impairment (CSCI),
traumatic brain injury or dementia, which sug- impairment had to be observed in at least two
gests substantial and functionally relevant levels specific ability areas in order for patients to be
of impairment. While there is some variability classified as “neuropsychologically impaired.”
across cases, most people with schizophrenia have These authors classified impairment as perfor-
impairments in several different ability areas. mance of 1 SD or more below the general popu-
lation mean based on corrected scores.
Another perspective is the Global Deficit
Prevalence of Impairment and NP
Score (GDS) approach for classifying NP
Normality
impairment (Heaton et al., 1994). The GDS
There has been an ongoing discussion of the approach begins by converting T-scores to def-
prevalence of NP impairments and, conversely, icit scores that reflect presence and severity of
Clinical Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia 515

impairment. T-scores greater than 40 represent and impairments compared to HC with similar
no impairment (deficit score = 0); whereas a intellectual performance using a criteria of sta-
deficit score of 1 reflects mild impairment tistical significance, essentially all patients meet
(T-score = 39 – 35); deficit score of 2 reflects mild the criteria. When more substantial relative
to moderate impairment (T-score = 34 to 30); 3 impairments are required, particularly when
reflects moderate impairment (T-score = 29 to indexed against general population standards
25); 4 reflects moderate to severe impair- and not controlled for individual intellectual
ment (T-score = 24 to 20), and 5 reflects severe performance, the prevalence of impairment is
impairment (T-score < 20). Deficit scores on considerably reduced.
all tests are then averaged to create the GDS. In summary, cognitive impairments are gen-
A GDS greater than or equal to 0.5 indicates that, erally moderate to severe across ability areas;
on average, an individual was mildly impaired with a comprehensive cognitive assessment,
on half of the NP test measures in the battery. few patients are spared from having cogni-
A final approach was recently suggested to tive impairments. Level of stringency of the
consider the possibility that using global pop- definition of NP abnormality and reliance on
ulation means may result in biased estimates population norms, instead of directly index-
of individual patient’s profi les of impairment. ing performance against premorbid functions,
In this alternate approach, performance on leads to reduced estimates of impairment.
current ability areas is compared to the per-
formance of HC matched to the schizophrenia
Functional Relevance of NP
patients on the basis of the same level of intel-
Impairment in Schizophrenia
lectual performance (Wilk et al., 2005). Thus,
current performance is compared on an indi- One likely reason for the increased interest in
vidual basis to expected performance based cognition in schizophrenia in the past 15 years
on intellectual intactness and the extent of has been the increased appreciation of the
impaired performance is inferred from discrep- functional relevance of NP impairments. Poor
ancies between expected and achieved levels of performance on NP in tests is associated with
performance. In this study, matched pairs of greater disability in everyday outcomes, includ-
patients and controls, including patients whose ing social functioning, independent living and
current IQ scores were greater than 110, were self-maintenance skills, and a variety of role
then compared on other aspects of cognitive functioning including work, school, and par-
functioning. Even patients whose current IQs enting. While the issues involved in the rela-
were 110 and above manifested impaired per- tionships between functional disability and NP
formance in several ability domains, compared performance across a variety of conditions are
to their matched controls. Thus, these data sug- considered elsewhere in the volume, there are at
gested that even in cases with evidence of intel- least issues that are somewhat specific to schizo-
lectual intactness, NP abnormalities could still phrenia: whether there are specific relationships
be detected. Further, this procedure might still between components of NP impairments and
underestimate cognitive impairments, as those different domains of everyday living skills and
schizophrenia patients with IQs more than 100 whether the treatment of NP deficits would lead
might have still deteriorated from higher, better to reduction in functional disability.
level of performance. In terms of the specificity of the relation-
It is not surprising that these different stan- ships between domains of NP and functional
dards for judging impairment in performance dysfunctions, initial suggestions (Green, 1996)
have led to variable estimates of the prevalence that there may be some specificity of relation-
of cognitive abnormalities in schizophrenia. ships have not been consistently replicated.
What is clear, however, is that the stringency of Most studies find a relatively general relation-
the definitions of impairment is directly related ship between measures of everyday living out-
to the prevalence of impairments detected. In comes and most NP ability areas (Evans et al.,
the Wilk et al. definition, where deterioration 2003; Harvey et al., 1997; Twamley et al., 2002).
from current functioning is directly indexed Further, the most substantial correlations in
516 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

most studies have been between global cogni- participants in both the studies were receiving
tive ability scores and everyday living domains specialty employment intervention services.
(Harvey et al., 1998). When functional disabil- Receipt of these services not only delivers a level
ity is measured in terms of functional poten- of support and encouragement absent from the
tial, such as with performance-based measures case management of many people with schizo-
of social and functional skills, there is mod- phrenia, but also presupposes a motivation to
est evidence for somewhat greater specificity seek and maintain employment. It is not at all
(McClure et al., 2007), but no clear-cut corre- clear that similar cognitive remediation inter-
lations between deficits in cognitive and func- ventions, even with cognitive efficacy, would
tional performance measures. lead to changes in employment outcomes with-
When treatment of cognitive impairments out the ongoing support provided by additional
is considered as a strategy to reduce functional specialty interventions.
disability, the level of relationship between NP Cognitive impairments are functionally rel-
performance and functional disability must be evant and their treatment, when the cognitive
considered. The correlation between everyday intervention has efficacy and when supports are
outcomes and global scores on NP assessment in place, can lead to functional gains. It is likely
batteries has been shown to share about 25% an error to assume that cognitive enhancement
variance, while individual ability areas share in isolation would lead to substantial functional
between 4% and 16%. Changes in single ability gains. It is also unclear what level of improve-
areas might not be expected to exert substantial ment is required to be functionally relevant and
effects on everyday outcomes, although changes what the details are of the interaction between
in variables that are related this strongly to each support and enhancement. Would greater levels
other might lead to changes with clinical signifi- of cognitive change (e.g., complete normaliza-
cance if certain critical threshold levels of perfor- tion of functioning) require less concurrent sup-
mance were crossed during successful treatment. port? Conversely, would a modest enhancement
Correlations between performance-based mea- signal be facilitated by consistent and appropri-
sures and NP performance measures are greater ate concurrent supportive interventions? These
than those with everyday outcomes (Bowie are empirical questions that will be addressed
et al., 2006), and as a result, successful cognitive for years to come.
enhancement might lead to greater correlated
changes in these types of measures. Other influ-
Assessment Strategies in
ences on everyday outcomes, such as motivation,
Schizophrenia
opportunities, and various incentives and disin-
centives toward performance need to be consid- The history of clinical neuropsychology has been
ered as well (Rosenheck et al., 2006). marked by controversies over the best approaches
Cognitive remediation interventions have to assessment. Differential merits of targeted
demonstrated detectable and potentially versus battery-based assessments are discussed
clinically meaningful changes in everyday elsewhere in this volume. In schizophrenia,
outcomes. In two separate studies using ran- like in other neuropsychiatric conditions where
domized clinical trials methods, the addition motivation and cooperation are affected in some
of cognitive remediation therapy to the ongo- cases, understanding the optimum level of detail
ing provision of supported employment led to required to perform an adequate NP assessment
gains in the actively treated group compared to is important. This concern is sharpened by the
the comparison sample (McGurk et al., 2005; findings, presented above, that cognitive impair-
Wexler & Bell, 2005). These improvements had ments in schizophrenia may reflect a single, uni-
long-term persistence, with differences still evi- factorial severity dimension that can be captured
dent after 3 years (McGurk et al., 2007). These with considerable accuracy using a relatively
results make two important points. The first simple assessment. Given the lack of evidence
is that NP performance can be altered and supporting the idea of (1) potential differential
these alterations can have direct functional brain dysfunctions or subtypes of the illness
benefits. Second, however, is the point that all defined by different profiles of cognitive deficits
Clinical Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia 517

or (2) differential functional importance across schizophrenia is not reliably associated with
most NP ability areas, extensive NP assessments improvements in cognitive functioning. It had
may not be needed to characterize the majority been proposed that newer antipsychotic medi-
of patients with schizophrenia. cations, referred to as atypical antipsychot-
The issue of the level of detail required in ics, were superior to conventional medications
assessment may be particularly relevant for for cognitive enhancement in schizophrenia
certain types of NP research, such as cognitive (Woodward et al., 2006). Many of these stud-
enhancement. In the large schizophrenia trial for ies have methodological limitations, including
the Clinical Antipsychotic Trials of Intervention small sample sizes, nonrandomized methods,
Effectiveness (CATIE) project, an extensive NP and high doses of the conventional comparator
assessment was performed at up to three assess- medications (Harvey & Keefe, 2001). Further,
ment points. Not only was the majority of the the effect sizes of changes in cognitive perfor-
variance in composite scores accounted for by a mance with atypical medications, averaged
few measures, all of them paper and pencil, but across several meta-analytic studies, was about
several of the computerized tests had close to 20% 0.25 SD, which is a relatively small effect size
missing data per assessment (Keefe et al., 2006). compared to the level of deficit described earlier
While this may not be an issue for completion in this chapter. Finally, the results of the large-
of assessments by experienced sites and testers, scale (N > 1000 patients) CATIE trial (Keefe
clearly clinical trials require assessments that are et al., 2007) indicated that the effects of atypi-
easily completed. Another interesting issue in cal medications was consistent with the levels
the CATIE trial was that some of the briefer tests detected in previous meta-analyses, about 0.25
accounted for substantial variance in the com- SD, but was also not reliably different from the
posite NP score, and a set of five tests accounted cognitive benefits of low doses of the conven-
for almost 90% of the variance. The briefer tests tional antipsychotic medication (perphenazine)
were also more likely to be completed by more employed in the study. The general impression
subjects. Direct comparisons of abbreviated and is that atypical antipsychotic medications do
extensive assessments in schizophrenia have not provide substantial added benefits for most
suggested that longer NP assessments add as patients from a cognitive standpoint.
little as 1% sensitivity to the detection of cogni- In contrast, studies of the effects of cognitive
tive impairments in schizophrenia (Keefe et al., remediation interventions have suggested not
2004) and that composite scores on brief batter- only that these interventions are quite effec-
ies are just as strongly related to functional dis- tive in the short term (e.g., McGurk et al., 2005;
ability as those derived from longer assessment Wexler & Bell, 2005), but also that these inter-
procedures (Keefe et al., 2006). Several different ventions have long-term functional benefits that
batteries with suitable psychometric characteris- are substantial. For instance, in the McGurk
tics are available (e.g., Velligan et al., 2004; Wilk et al. sample, a three-year follow-up indicated
et al., 2004). Given our understanding of the NP that the patients randomized to cognitive
characteristics of schizophrenia, we find it diffi- remediation earned 1100% more money over
cult to recommend extensive assessments unless the follow-up period than similarly employ-
an evaluation for additional comorbidities is ment seeking patients who were randomized
being performed. Even then, the signal arising to treatment as usual (McGurk et al., 2007).
from cognitive impairments in schizophrenia is These results, while few in number, do suggest
so extensive that it is possible that many concur- that there is a substantial benefit for cognitive
rent comorbidities might not even be detectable remediation treatment, one that extends even to
against the background of schizophrenia. chronic patients (Lindenmeyer et al., 2008) and
is statistically significant in meta-analyses.
Pharmacological cognitive enhancement
Treatments for Cognitive
has been less successful. Treatment failures
Impairments
have been reported for cholinesterase inhibi-
It is well understood that successful treat- tors (Keefe et al., 2008), atomoxetine (Friedman
ment of psychotic symptoms in patients with et al., 2008), selective serotonin reuptake
518 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

inhibitor (SSRI) antidepressants (Friedman et al., Initial characterization and clinical correlates.
2005), and glutamatergic agents (Buchanan et American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 549–559.
al., 2007). While this may appear disappoint- Bowie, C. R., & Harvey P. D. (2005). Cognition in
ing, there are multiple additional large-scale schizophrenia: impairments, determinants, and
functional importance. Psychiatric Clinics of
trials in process and, unless there are factors
North America, 28, 613–633.
associated with other treatments for schizo-
Bowie, C. R., Reichenberg, A., Patterson, T. L., Heaton,
phrenia that interfere with the effectiveness of R. K., & Harvey, P. D. (2006). Determinants of real
pharmacological agents, this is an area where world functional performance in schizophrenia:
new positive developments may be expected in Correlations with cognition, functional capacity,
the future. and symptoms. American Journal of Psychiatry,
163, 418–425.
Bowie, C. R., Reichenberg, A., Reichmann, N.,
Conclusions Parrella, M., White, L., & Harvey, P. D. (2004).
Schizophrenia is marked by an extensive and Stability and functional correlates of memo-
functionally relevant set of NP abnormalities ry-based classification in older schizophrenia
patients. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry,
that persist over the lifespan. In some patients
12, 376–386.
there is evidence of worsening in functioning
Brewer, W. J., Wood, S. J., McGorry P. D., Francey,
with aging, most likely in patients with a life- S. M., Phillips, L. J., Yung, A. R., et al. (2003).
long history of poor response to treatment and Impairment of olfactory identification ability in
chronic institutional stay. These impairments individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis who
are present on many of the tests used to exam- later develop schizophrenia. American Journal of
ine other neuropsychiatric conditions, but the Psychiatry, 160, 1790–1794.
impairment profile in schizophrenia does not Bryson, G., & Bell, M. D. (2003). Initial and final
confirm clearly to a pattern consistent with work performance in schizophrenia: Cognitive
either focal deficits or profi les of impairment and symptom predictors. Journal of Nervous and
seen in other conditions such as cortical demen- Mental Disease, 19, 87–92.
Buchanan, R. W., Javitt, D. C., Marder, S. R.,
tias. A current controversy exists as to whether
Schooler, N. R., Gold, J. M., McMahon, R. P., et
NP impairment in schizophrenia is generalized
al. (2007). The Cognitive and Negative Symptoms
or specific and as to whether extensive cogni- in Schizophrenia Trial (CONSIST): The efficacy
tive assessments provide more information of glutamatergic agents for negative symptoms
than abbreviated evaluations. What is clearly and cognitive impairments. American Journal of
agreed upon is that cognitive impairment in Psychiatry, 164, 1593–1602.
schizophrenia is present in most to all cases and Callicott, J. H., Mattay, V. S., Bertolino, A., Finn, K.,
that these impairments are factors that interfere Coppola, R., Frank, J. A., et al. (1999). Physiological
greatly with the life functioning of people with characteristics of capacity constraints in working
schizophrenia. memory as revealed by functional MRI. Cerebral
Cortex, 9, 20–26.
Callicott. J. H., Bertolino, A., Mattay, V. S,
References Langheim, F. J., Duyn, J., Coppola, R., et al. (2000).
Physiological dysfunction of the dorsolateral pre-
Anderson, S. W., Damasio, H., Jones, R. D., & Tranel, frontal cortex in schizophrenia revisited. Cerebral
D. (1991). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test perfor- Cortex, 10, 1078–1092.
mance as a measure of frontal lobe damage. Journal Cannon, T. D., Glahn D. C., Kim, J., Van Erp, T.
of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 13, G., Karlsgodt, K., Cohen, M. S., et al. (2005).
909–922. Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activity during
Arnold, S. E., Gur, R. E., Shapiro, R. M., Fisher, maintenance and manipulation of information in
K. R., Moberg, P. J., Gibney, M. R., et al. (1995). working memory in patients with schizophrenia.
Prospective clinicopathologic studies of schizo- Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 1071–1080.
phrenia: Accrual and assessment of patients. Chapman, L. J., & Chapman, J. P. (1978). The measure-
American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 731–737. ment of differential deficit. Journal of Psychiatric
Bilder, R. M., Goldman, R. S., Robinson, D., Research, 14, 303–311.
Reiter, G., Bell, L., Bates, J. A., et al. (2000). Cornblatt B. A., Auther, A. M., Niendam, T., Smith,
Neuropsychology of first-episode schizophrenia: C. W., Zinberg, J., Bearden, C. E., et al. (2007).
Clinical Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia 519

Preliminary findings for two new measures of relationship of neuropsychological abilities to


social and role functioning in the prodromal specific domains of functional capacity in older
phase of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, schizophrenia patients. Biological Psychiatry, 53,
33, 688–702. 422–430.
Cornblatt B. A., & Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L. (1985). Friedman, J. I., Carpenter, D., Lu, J., Fan, J., Tang, C.
Global attentional deviance as a marker of risk for Y., White, L., et al. (2008). A pilot study of adjunc-
schizophrenia: Specificity and predictive validity. tive atomoxetine treatment to second-generation
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 94, 470–486. antipsychotics for cognitive impairment in schizo-
Cornblatt, B., Obuchowski, M., Roberts, S., Pollack phrenia. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology,
S., & Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L. (1999). Cognitive 28, 59–63.
and behavioral precursors of schizophrenia. Friedman J. I., Harvey P. D., Coleman T., Moriarty, P.
Developmental Psychopathology, 11, 487–508. J., Bowie, C., Parrella, M., et al. (2001). A six-year
David, A. S., Malmberg, A., Brandt, L., Albeck, P., follow-up study of cognitive and functional status
& Lewis G. (1997). IQ and risk for schizophrenia: across the life-span in schizophrenia: A compar-
A population-based cohort study. Psychological ison with Alzheimer’s disease and normal aging.
Medicine, 27, 1311–1323. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 1441–1448.
Davidson M., Harvey P. D., Powchick P., Parrella, M., Friedman, J. I., Ocampo, R., Elbaz, Z., Parrella, M.,
White, L., Knobler, H. Y., et al. (1995). Severity of White, L., Bowler, S., et al. (2005). The effect of
symptoms in chronically institutionalized geri- citalopram adjunctive treatment added to atypical
atric schizophrenic patients. American Journal of antipsychotic medications for cognitive perfor-
Psychiatry, 152, 197–205. mance in patients with schizophrenia. Journal of
Davidson, M., Harvey, P. D., Welsh, K., Powchik, P., Clinical Psychopharmacology, 25, 237–242.
Putnam, K. M., & Mohs, R. C. (1996). Cognitive Gambini, O., Campana, A., Garghentini, G., &
impairment in old-age schizophrenia: A compara- Scarone, A. (2003). No evidence of a homogenous
tive study of schizophrenia and Alzheimer’s disease. frontal neuropsychological profi le in a sample of
American Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 1274–1279. schizophrenic subjects. Journal of Neuropsychiatry
Davidson, M., Reichenberg, A., Rabinowitz, J., Weizer, and Clinical Neurosciences, 15, 53–57.
M., Kaplan, Z., & Mark M. (1999). Behavioral and Gladsjo, J. A., Heaton, R. K., Palmer, B. W., Taylor,
intellectual markers for schizophrenia in appar- M. J., & Jeste, D. V. (1999). Use of oral reading to
ently healthy male adolescents. American Journal estimate premorbid intellectual and neuropsycho-
of Psychiatry, 156, 1328–1335. logical functioning. Journal of the International
Davis, K. L., Buchsbaum, M. S., Shihabuddin, L., Neuropsychological Society, 5, 247–254.
Spiegel-Cohen, J., Metzger, M., Frecska, E., et al. Gladsjo, J. A, McAdams, L. A., Palmer, B. A., Moore,
(1998). Ventricular enlargement in poor outcome D. J., Jeste, D. V., & Heaton R. K. (2004). A six-factor
schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 43, 783–793. model of cognition in schizophrenia and related
Delisi, L., Sakuma, M., Mauricio, A. M., Relja, M., psychotic disorders: Relationships with clinical
& Hoff, A. L. (2004). Cerebral ventricular change symptoms and functional capacity. Schizophrenia
over the first 10 years after the onset of schizo- Bulletin, 30, 739–754.
phrenia. Psychiatry Research, 130, 57–70. Gold, J., & Harvey, P. D. (1993). Cognitive deficits in
Delisi, L., Sakuma, M., Tew, W., Kushrer, M., Hoff, A., schizophrenia. In: Peter Powchik and S. C. Schultz
& Grimson, R. (1997). Schizophrenia as a chronic (Eds.), Psychiatric clinics of North America (pp.
active brain process: A study of progressive brain 295–312). Philadelphia: William B. Saunders.
structural change subsequent to the onset of psy- Goldberg, T. E., Hyde, T. M., Kleinman, J. E., &
chosis. Psychiatry Research Brain Imaging, 74, Weinberger, D. R. (1993). Course of schizophrenia:
129–140. Neuropsychological evidence for a static encepha-
Dickinson, D., Iannone, V. N., Wilk, C. M., & Gold, lopathy. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 19, 797–804.
J. M. (2004). General and specific cognitive defi- Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1994). Working mem-
cits in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 55, ory dysfunction in schizophrenia. Journal of
826–833. Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 6,
Dickinson, D., Ramsey, M. E., & Gold, J. M. (2007). 348–357.
Overlooking the obvious: A meta-analytic com- Granholm, E., Morris, S., Asarnow, R. F., Chock, D., &
parison of digit symbol coding tasks and other Jeste, D. V. (2000). Accelerated age-related decline
cognitive measures in schizophrenia. Archives of in processing resources in schizophrenia: Evidence
General Psychiatry, 64, 532–542. from pupillary responses recorded during the span
Evans, J. D., Heaton, R. K., Paulsen, J. S., Palmer, of apprehension task. Journal of the International
B. W., Patterson, T., & Jeste, D. V. (2003). The Neuropsychological Society, 6, 30–43.
520 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Green, M. F. (1996). What are the functional Heaton, R. K., Paulsen, J. S., McAdams, L. A.,
consequences of neurocognitive deficits in Kuck, J., Zisook, S., Braff, D., et al. (1994).
schizophrenia? American Journal of Psychiatry, Neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenics:
153, 321–330. Relationship to age, chronicity, and dementia.
Harvey, P. D., Bertisch, H., Friedman, J. I., Marcus, S., Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 469–476.
Parrella, M., White, L., et al. (2003). The course Heinrichs, R. W., & Zakzanis, K. K. (1998).
of functional decline in geriatric patients with Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: A quan-
schizophrenia: Cognitive-functional and clinical titative review of the evidence. Neuropsychology,
symptoms as determinants of change. American 12, 426–444.
Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 11, 610–619. Hoff, A. L., Sakuma, M., Wieneke, M., Horon, R.,
Harvey, P. D., Friedman, J. I., Bowie, C., Reichenberg, Kushner, M., & DeLisi, L. E. (1999). Longitudinal
A., McGurk, S. R., Parrella, M., et al. (2006a). neuropsychological follow-up study of patients
Validity and stability of performance-based esti- with first-episode schizophrenia. American
mates of premorbid educational functioning in Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 1336–1341.
older patients with schizophrenia. Journal of Keefe, R. S. E., Bilder, R. M., Harvey, P. D., Davis,
Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28, S. M., Palmer, B. W., Gold, J. M., et al. (2006).
178–192. Baseline neurocognitive deficits in the CATIE
Harvey, P. D., Howanitz, E., Parrella, M., White, L., schizophrenia trial. Neuropsychopharmacology,
Davidson, M., Mohs, R. C., et al. (1998). Symptoms, 31, 2033–2046.
cognitive functioning, and adaptive skills in geri- Keefe, R. S. E., Bilder, R. M., Davis, S. M., Harvey,
atric patients with lifelong schizophrenia: A com- P. D., Palmer, B. W., Gold, J. M., et al. (2007).
parison across treatment sites. American Journal Neurocognitive effects of antipsychotic medi-
of Psychiatry, 155, 1080–1086. cations in patients with chronic schizophrenia.
Harvey, P. D., & Keefe, R. S. (2001). Studies of cog- Archives of General Psychiatry, 64, 633–647.
nitive change in patients with schizophrenia fol- Keefe, R. S. E., Eesley, C. E., & Poe, M. P. (2005).
lowing novel antipsychotic treatment. American Defining a cognitive function decrement in schizo-
Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 176–184. phrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 688–691.
Harvey P. D., Silverman J. M., Mohs R. C., Parrella, Keefe, R. S., Goldberg, T. E., Harvey, P. D., Gold,
M., White, L., Powchik, P., et al. (1999). Cognitive J. M., Poe, M. P., & Coughenour, L. (2004). The
decline in late-life schizophrenia: A longitudinal brief assessment of cognition in schizophrenia:
study of geriatric chronically hospitalized patients. Reliability, sensitivity, and comparison with a
Biological Psychiatry, 45, 32–40. standard neurocognitive battery. Schizophrenia
Harvey, P. D., Sukhodolsky, D., Parrella, M., White, Research, 68, 283–297.
L., & Davidson, M. (1997). The association between Keefe, R. S. E., Malhotra, A. K., Meltzer, H. Y.,
adaptive and cognitive deficits in geriatric chronic Kane, J. M., Buchanan, R. W., Murthy, A., et
schizophrenic patients. Schizophrenia Research, al. (2008). Efficacy and safety of donepezil in
27, 211–218. patients with schizophrenia or schizoaffective
Harvey, P. D., Moriarty, P. J., Bowie, C., Friedman, disorder: Significant placebo/practice effects in
J. I., Parrella, M., White, L., et al. (2002). Cortical a 12-week, randomized, double-blind, placebo-
and subcortical cognitive deficits in schizophre- controlled trial. Neuropsychopharmacology, 33(6),
nia: Convergence of classifications based on lan- 1217–1228.
guage and memory skill areas. Journal of Clinical Keefe, R. S. E., Poe, M., Walker, T. M., & Harvey, P.
and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24, 55–66. D. (2006). The Schizophrenia Cognition Rating
Harvey, P. D., Moriarty, P. J., Friedman, J. I., Scale SCoRS: Interview based assessment and its
White, L., Parrella, M., Mohs, R. C., et al. (2000). relationship to cognition, real world functioning
Differential preservation of cognitive functions and Functional Capacity. American Journal of
in geriatric patients with lifelong chronic schizo- Psychiatry, 163, 426–432.
phrenia: Less impairment in reading compared Kim, J., Glahn, D. C., Nuechterlein, K. H., & Cannon,
with other skill areas. Biological Psychiatry, 47, T. D. (2004). Maintenance and manipulation of
962–968. information in schizophrenia: Further evidence
Harvey, P. D., Reichenberg, A., Romero, M., for impairment in the central executive compo-
Granholm, E., & Siever, L. J. (2006b). Dual-task nent of working memory. Schizophrenia Research,
information processing in schizotypal personality 68, 173–187.
disorder: Evidence of impaired processing capac- Kremen, W. S., Seidman, L. J., Faraone, S. V.,
ity. Neuropsychology, 20, 453–460. Toomey, R., & Tsuang, M. T. (2000). The paradox
Clinical Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia 521

of normal neuropsychological function in schizo- of the International Neuropsychological Society, 1,


phrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 88–99.
743–752. Paulsen, J. S., Salmon, D. P., Monsch, A., Butters,
Lencz, T., Smith, C. W., McLaughlin, D., Auther, A., N., Swenson, M. R., & Bondi, M. W. (1995).
Nakayama, E., Hovey, L., et al. (2006). Generalized Discrimination of cortical from subcortical demen-
and specific neurocognitive deficits in prodromal tias on the basis of memory and problem-solving
schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 59, 863–871. tests. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51, 48–58.
Lieberman, J., Chakos, M., Wu, H., Alvir, J., Hoff man, Reichenberg A., Weiser, M., Rapp, Rabinowitz, J.,
E., Robinson, D., et al. (2001). Longitudinal study Caspi, A., Schmeidler, J., et al. (2005). Elaboration
of brain morphology in first episode schizophre- on premorbid intellectual performance in schizo-
nia. Biological Psychiatry, 49, 487–499. phrenia: Premorbid intellectual decline and risk
Lieberman, J. A., Tollefson, G. D., Charles, C., for schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry,
Zipursky, R., Sharma, T., Kahn, R. S., et al. (2005). 62, 1297–1304.
Antipsychotic drug effects on brain morphology Reichenberg A., Weiser, M., Rapp, M. A., Rabinowitz,
in first-episode psychosis. Archives of General J., Caspi, A., Schmeidler, J., et al. (2006). Premorbid
Psychiatry, 62, 361–370. intra-individual variability in intellectual perfor-
Lindenmayer, J. P., McGurk, S. R., Mueser, K. T., mance and risk for schizophrenia: A population-
Khan, A., Wance, D., Hoff man, L., et al. (2008). A based study. Schizophrenia Research, 85, 49–57.
randomized controlled trial of cognitive remedia- Rosenheck, R., Leslie, D., Keefe, R. S., McEvoy, J.,
tion among inpatients with persistent mental ill- Swartz, M., Perkins, D., et al. (2006). Barriers
ness. Psychiatric Services, 59, 241–247. to employment in people with schizophrenia.
McClure, M. M., Bowie, C. R., Patterson, T. L., American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 411–417.
Heaton, R. K., Weaver, C., Anderson, H., et Rund, B. R. (1998). A review of longitudinal studies
al. (2007). Correlations of functional capacity of cognitive functions in schizophrenia patients.
and neuropsychological performance in older Schizophrenia Bulletin, 24, 425–435.
patients with schizophrenia: Evidence for speci- Saykin, A. J., Gur, R. C., Gur, R. E., Mozeley,
ficity of relationships? Schizophrenia Research, 89, D., Mozeley, L. H., & Resnick, S. M. (1991).
330–338. Neuropsychological function in schizophrenia:
McGurk, S. R., Mueser, K. T., Feldman, K., Wolfe, Selective impairment in memory and learning.
R., & Pascaris, A. (2007). Cognitive training for Archives of General Psychiatry, 48, 618–623.
supported employment: 2–3 year outcomes of a Saykin, A. J., Shtasel, D. L., Gur, R. E., Kester,
randomized controlled trial. American Journal of D. B., Mozley, L. H., Stafi niak, P., et al. (1994).
Psychiatry, 164, 437–441. Neuropsychological deficits in neuroleptic naive
McGurk, S. R., Mueser, K. T., & Pascaris, A. (2005). patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Archives
Cognitive training and supported employment of General Psychiatry, 51, 124–131.
for persons with severe mental illness: One- Seidman, L. J., Guiliano, A. J., Smith, C. W.,
year results from a randomized controlled trial. Stone, W. S., Glatt, S. J., Meyer, E., et al. (2006).
Schizophrenia Bulletin, 31, 898–909. Neuropsychological functioning in adolescents
Mohamed, S., Paulsen, J. S., O’Leary, D. Arndt, S., and young adults at genetic risk for schizophrenia
& Andreasen N. (1999). Generalized cognitive and affective psychoses: Results from the Harvard
deficits in schizophrenia: a study of fi rst-epi- and Hillside Adolescent High Risk Studies.
sode patients. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, Schizophrenia Bulletin, 32, 507–524.
749–754. Torrey, E. F., Bowler, A. E., Taylor, E. H., & Gottesman,
Palmer, B. W., & Heaton, R. K. (2000). Executive I. I. (1994). Schizophrenia and manic-depressive
dysfunction in schizophrenia. In: T. Sharma and disorder: The biological roots of mental illness as
P. D. Harvey (Eds.), Cognition in schizophrenia: revealed by the landmark study of identical twins.
Impairments, importance and treatment strategies New York: Basic Books.
(pp. 51–72). New York: Oxford University Press. Tracy, J. I., McCrory, A. C., Josiassen, R. C., &
Palmer, B. W., Heaton, R. K., Paulsen, J. S., Kuck, J., Monaco, C. A. (1996). A comparison of reading
Braff, D., Harris, M. J., et al. (1997). Is it possible and demographic based estimates of premor-
to be schizophrenic yet neuropsychologically nor- bid intelligence in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia
mal? Neuropsychology, 11, 437–446. Research, 22, 103–109.
Paulsen, J. S., Heaton, R. K., Sadek, J. R., Perry, W., Turetsky, B. I., Moberg, P. J., Mozley, L. H., Moelter,
& Jeste, D. V. (1995). The nature of learning and S. T., Agrin, R. N., Gur, R. C., et al. (2002).
memory impairments in schizophrenia. Journal Memory-delineated subtypes of schizophrenia:
522 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Relationship to clinical, neuroanatomical, and performance of male adolescents is lower than


neurophysiological measures. Neuropsychology, controls across psychiatric disorders: A popula-
16, 481–490. tion-based study. Acta Psychatrica Scandinavica,
Twamley, E. W., Doshi, R. R., Nayak, G. V., Palmer, 110, 471–475.
B. W., Golshan, S., Heaton, R. K., et al. (2002). Wexler, B. E., & Bell, M. D. (2005). Cognitive reme-
Generalized cognitive impairments, ability to per- diation and vocational rehabilitation for schizo-
form everyday tasks, and level of independence in phrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 31, 931–941.
community living situations of older patients with Wilk, C. M., Gold, J. M., Humber, K., Dickerson, F.,
psychosis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, Fenton, W. S., & Buchanan, R. W. (2004). Brief
2013–2020. cognitive assessment in schizophrenia: Normative
Velligan, D. I., DiCocco, M., Bow-Thomas, C. C., data for the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment
Cadle, C., Glahn, D. C., Miller, A. L., et al. (2004). A of Neuropsychological Status. Schizophrenia
brief cognitive assessment for use with schizophre- Research, 70, 175–186.
nia patients in community clinics. Schizophrenia Wilk, C. M., Gold, J. M., McMahon, R. P., Humber,
Research, 71, 273–283. K., Iannone, V. N., & Buchanan, R. W. (2005).
Weickert, T. W., Goldberg, T. E., Gold, J. M., Bigelow, No, it is not possible to be schizophrenic yet neu-
L. B., Egan, M. F., & Weinberger D. R. (2000). ropsychologically normal. Neuropsychology, 19,
Cognitive impairments in patients with schizo- 778–786.
phrenia displaying preserved and compromised Woodward, N. D., Purdon, S. E., Meltzer, H. Y., & Zald,
intellect. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, D. H. (2005). A meta-analysis of neuropsycholog-
907–913. ical change to clozapine, olanzapine, quetiapine,
Weiser, M., Reichenberg A., Rabinowitz, J., Knobler, and risperidone in schizophrenia. International
H. Y., Lubin, G., Yazvitzky, R., et al. (2004). Cognitive Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 8, 457–472.
22

Neuropsychology of Depression and Related


Mood Disorders
Scott A. Langenecker, H. Jin Lee, and Linas A. Bieliauskas

Tremendous strides have been made toward The present chapter follows several previous
understanding the neuropsychological, neuro- chapters on depression and depression-related
anatomical, and neuroimaging findings associ- cognitive difficulties by King and Caine (1996)
ated with depression. Whereas understanding and Caine (1986) in earlier editions of this book.
of the neuroanatomical networks involved in As King and Caine note in the previous edition of
depression and related mood disorders remains this book, study of cognition in mood disorders
in an adolescent phase, neuropsychological has moved from being a nuisance when study-
findings in depression and related mood disor- ing late-life dementias, also known as pseudo-
ders are fairly well codified at this point. To be dementia, to a full fledged area of inquiry. The
sure, there are a number of clinical and demo- underlying assumptions about mood disorders
graphic features that substantially impact cog- as being functional and not organic disorders
nitive performance in the context of a mood have now been assailed on a number of different
disorder, including later age of onset, polyphar- fronts (Tucker et al., 1990). In fact, it is now best
macy/substance abuse, medical complications, to consider mood disorders under a category of
greater severity, and resistance to traditional “potentially reversible cognitive decrements”
treatments. Whereas the “causes” for depres- (Sobow et al., 2006), although the careful reader
sion-associated cognitive difficulties are het- of this chapter will come to appreciate that for
erogeneous, the co-occurrence of features of some individuals, the cognitive difficulties that
depression and cognitive difficulties of specific co-occur with depression are in no way revers-
types suggests a common set of neural networks ible. This characterization may sit poorly with
that may be adversely affected, including medial many in a field christened in the tradition of
and ventral frontal, limbic, and basal ganglia degenerative dementias, wherein dysfunction
structures. The present chapter is intended to is the inevitable, and currently irreversible,
provide the reader with a blend of traditional sequelae of neurological disease. However, as
neuropsychological investigations of depression studies of disorders such as multiple sclerosis
and related mood disorders, hereafter referred and Huntington’s disease have aptly informed
to generally as mood disorders, in addition to the field, assumptions about “functional” dis-
integrating the latest cognitive and affective orders can be largely misplaced (Ghaffar &
neuroscience research. We will review the mod- Feinstein, 2007). This is by no means to asso-
erating impact of age of onset, effort, subtypes of ciate the frequently present, moderate cognitive
depression, and medications on cognitive func- inefficiencies observed in depression to those
tioning in mood disorders, as well as research more severe, highly prevalent, and persistent
using cognitive measures as predictors of treat- difficulties observed in degenerative condi-
ment response. tions. Rather, the purpose is to place the field

523
524 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

and the reader in the proper pose of humility assessment of mood disorders. The chapter
when making inferences about the current state closes with several compelling studies that have
of knowledge in brain–behavior relationships in used neuropsychological and cognitive/affec-
mood disorders. Mood disorders, like any other tive neuroscience measures to predict treatment
psychological phenomena, have the central ner- response, a summary, and some recommenda-
vous system (CNS) as the wellspring for their tions for what a “core” battery might be com-
existence, and many of the same tracts and sys- prised of when assessing a patient with a mood
tems implicated in neurological disorders are disorder.
similarly implicated in mood and other psychi-
atric disorders (Cummings, 1993).
Traditional Studies and
The burgeoning fields of affective and cog-
Cognitive Profile
nitive neuroscience, as well as the expanding
body of knowledge on mood disorders from Major depressive disorder (MDD) is associated
traditional neuropsychological investigations, with inefficiencies in various cognitive domains
together make a chapter on the neuropsychol- (Austin et al., 2001, p. 57; Miller, 1975; for reviews
ogy of mood disorders no small endeavor. There see Elliott, 1998; Rogers et al., 2004) including
are many allied areas of inquiry that are not attention (Cornblatt et al., 1989; Porter et al.,
seamlessly integrated into one theory, model, 2003; Weiland-Fiedler et al., 2004), psychomotor
or basis for understanding of the brain–be- speed (Sobin & Sackeim, 1997), executive func-
havior relationships most typically observed in tion (Grant et al., 2001; Paelecke-Habermann
mood disorders. Given this challenge, we have et al., 2005), and memory (Austin et al., 1992;
attempted to balance specific and important Brown et al., 1994; Burt et al., 1995; Elliott et al.,
areas of inquiry within mood disorders with the 1996). It is well known that intragroup heteroge-
more general purview that one might typically neity of cognitive decrements exist, though the
expect from a book chapter. We make no pre- reasons are still not completely clear. Possible
tense of being exhaustive in breadth or depth, mediating factors, some of which are addressed
but do hope that the present chapter provides an later in this chapter, include the age of onset,
ample overview of general areas along with very age of participants, premorbid level of cognitive
specific subtopics that we feel the reader might function, symptom severity, number of previ-
most benefit from. We begin with an overview ous depressive episodes, effort and motivation,
of what might be considered a typical cognitive medication status, patient status (inpatient or
profi le of a depressed patient, based largely on outpatient), state of the individual (i.e., remit-
the traditional neuropsychological studies of ted or depressed), and history of hospitalization
depression. We move from these to discuss neu- (Elliott, 1998; Fossati et al., 2002; Gualtieri et al.,
roanatomical and neuroimaging evidence cur- 2006). As such, drawing conclusions regarding
rently available in mood disorders research. The cognitive decrements in MDD is challenging
neuroimaging data largely rely upon cognitive because studies often utilize a depressed group
and affective neuroscience research. We then of heterogeneous individuals varying in afore-
delve into critical mediating factors in under- mentioned characteristics. Furthermore, many
standing brain–behavior relationships in mood such studies are powered only to detect large
disorders. This is a necessary and, we believe, and very large effect sizes and also typically use
very valuable overview of factors that directly tasks designed to assess for obvious brain injury
relate to what extent cognitive difficulties might or neurodegenerative conditions.
be expected to occur in mood disorders. We In this section, we review cognitive decre-
will also highlight some specific subtypes of ments in adults who are under the age of 65
mood disorders that illustrate potential brain– as other neurological factors often contribute
behavior relationships, or where greater preva- in the disease process in geriatric or late-on-
lence or severity of cognitive difficulties might set depression. The specific topic of late-onset
be expected. We review potential medication depression is discussed in the Age of Onset sec-
effects in depression, as this is likely a frequent tion in this chapter. Readers can also find sev-
question that arises in the neuropsychological eral excellent articles on neuropsychological
Neuropsychology of Depression and Related Mood Disorders 525

dysfunction in geriatric depression (Bieliauskas, difficult). By way of background, the automatic


1993; Elderkin-Thompson et al., 2007; Marcos effortful hypothesis states that depressed indi-
et al., 2005; Sheline et al., 2006; Wright & viduals should incrementally show poorer per-
Persad, 2007). We focus on domains of atten- formance as the degree of effort required on any
tion, psychomotor speed, executive function, given task increases (Hasher & Zacks, 1979).
and memory, which are the areas that have been Similarly, Rose and Ebmeier (2006) examined
most researched and found to be pertinent to working memory in 20 patients with MDD (in
MDD (Bulmash et al., 2006; Gualtieri et al., or outpatients, most of who were on antide-
2006; Landro et al., 2001; Porter et al., 2003). We pressant medications) and 20 healthy controls
also understand that sorting neuropsychologi- by using a different version of n-back task. The
cal measures into different constructs may be researchers found slower reaction times and
arbitrary as one measure usually taps more than reduced accuracy in the MDD group compared
one cognitive domain. Furthermore, attention, to the control group in a linear fashion, again,
working memory, processing speed, and execu- with no disproportionately slower reaction time
tive function mainly involve the common struc- and decreased accuracy with increasing cogni-
tures in the frontal–subcortical neural networks tive load, suggesting a more uniform pattern
(Cummings, 1995; Mega & Cummings, 1994). of weakness rather than an automatic/effortful
continuum.
In tasks of sustained attention, individuals
Attention
with MDD were shown to make more omission
Research on attention in MDD suggests that errors (Langenecker et al., 2007a, 2007b; Porter
although simpler attentional processes may not et al., 2003; Sevigny et al., 2003) and commis-
be affected in adult depressed patients (Harvey sion errors (Farrin et al., 2003; Porter et al.,
et al., 2004; Ravnkilde et al., 2003), more com- 2003) on continuous performance tests (CPTs).
plex attentional processes such as working In addition, individuals who were in euthymic
memory or sustained attention are often com- or remitted states were also shown to demon-
promised in patients with MDD (Porter et al., strate persistent difficulty with sustained atten-
2003; Rose & Ebmeier, 2006) and even in remit- tion. Recently, Weiland-Fiedler et al. (2004)
ted individuals in some cases (Weiland-Fiedler found decrements in 28 fully remitted, unmed-
et al., 2004). Hartlage et al. (1993) noted that icated individuals with a history of MDD com-
depressed individuals have more difficulty on pared to 23 healthy controls on the Rapid Visual
tasks that require effortful processing com- Information Processing Task of the Cambridge
pared to tasks that are simpler or require Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery
automatic processing. Indeed, some studies of (CANTAB). Paelecke-Habermann et al. (2005)
attentional processing in MDD have shown also found continued attention difficulty in
that short-term attention such as the forward remitted individuals with MDD. Although the
digit span is generally unaffected compared to aforementioned studies demonstrated atten-
more difficult working memory tasks (Harvey tional inefficiencies in euthymic and depressed
et al., 2004; Ravnkilde et al., 2003). For exam- states, Grant et al. (2001) did not find differ-
ple, Harvey et al. (2004) found that short-term ences on the digit span task and a CPT in 123
attention on forward and backward digit span nonchronic, MDD outpatients and 36 healthy
and forward spatial span did not differ between controls. The dependent measures reported by
22 young inpatients with MDD and 22 normal Grant and colleagues were different from the
controls, while poorer performance was found measures reported by other researchers (Harvey
in the patient group on a backward spatial span et al., 2004; Porter et al., 2003; Sevigny et al.,
task and a verbal n-back task. However, their 2003).
results did not fully support an automatic ver-
sus effortful processing hypothesis as they
Psychomotor Speed
failed to find a group and complexity interac-
tion (i.e., the patient group did not do dispro- Research on psychomotor speed in mood dis-
portionately worse as the task became more orders have shown inconsistent findings. Some
526 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

studies have shown slowed psychomotor speed assessments involve various functions including
in individuals with mood disorders (Bulmash concept formation, set-shifting, planning, inhi-
et al., 2006; Sobin & Sackeim, 1997) while other bition, working memory, and fluency (Brown
studies have not found any difference between et al., 1994; Miyake et al., 2000). Some of the
depressed individuals and healthy controls commonly used tests to assess executive dys-
(Grant et al. 2001; Porter et al., 2003). In general, function both in research and clinical settings
psychomotor slowing has been more commonly include the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST;
associated with melancholic depression (Austin Channon, 1996; Grant et al., 2001), tests of ver-
et al., 1999) and/or in older individuals with bal fluency (for a review see Henry & Crawford,
depression (Beats et al., 1996; Nebes et al., 2005), the Stroop interference test (Harvey
2000). In medication-free younger individuals, et al., 2004; Markela-Lerenc et al., 2006), Trail-
normal performance was found on the Digit Making Test Part B (Grant et al., 2001; Harvey
Symbol Substitution Test on the Wechsler Adult et al., 2004), the Tower of London test (Naismith
Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R) and on a et al., 2006; Porter et al., 2003), and the Halstead–
CPT in terms of respond latency (Porter et al., Reitan Category test (Grant et al., 2001).
2003). In addition, Harvey et al. (2004) also found Individuals with depressive symptoms have
no difference between 22 inpatients with MDD been shown to complete fewer categories and
and 22 healthy controls on the Trail-Making Test make more perseverate responses on the WCST
Part A. Perhaps, the level of difficulty or required (Channon, 1996; Grant et al., 2001), although
effort has an effect on psychomotor slowing. inconsistently (Fossati et al., 2001; Ravnkilde
Hammar et al. (2003) examined automatic and et al., 2003). Grant et al. (2001) compared 123
effortful processing by using a visual search par- outpatients with MDD to 36 healthy controls
adigm in 21 individuals with MDD on antide- on the WCST and found that the MDD group
pressant medications and healthy controls. The completed a fewer number of categories, made
results indicated no difference in reaction time more perseverative responses and errors, and
between the two groups on the task that was more often failed to maintain set and learn.
thought to only require automatic processing No difference was found, however, on the other
(one distractor task). Nevertheless, reaction time measures of executive functioning including
was slower in the MDD group when the task the Halstead–Reitan Categories Test, letter flu-
required effortful processing (two distractor ency, and Trail-Making Test Part B. Ravnkilde
task). Another study of 26 patients with MDD et al. (2003) found no difference on the WCST
reported positive relationships between choice between 40 inpatients with MDD and a group of
reaction time on the California Computerized 49 controls. The researchers did find that Stroop
Assessment Package clinical depression rat- word reading, color naming, and interference
ing scales (Egeland et al., 2005). Bulmash et al. were all poorer in the MDD group compared to
(2006) examined 18 unmedicated individuals the healthy control group. Harvey et al. (2004)
with MDD and 29 controls on a driving simula- reported that the depressed group did not differ
tor for four 30-minute trials throughout the day. from the control group on the word reading con-
The MDD group demonstrated overall slowed dition of the Stroop test while they performed
steering reaction time and increased number worse on the color naming and interference
of crashes compared to the control group even conditions. The researchers also found slower
after age and sleepiness were controlled. completion times on the Trail-Making Test Part
B. Fossati et al. (2001) did not find a significant
difference between 22 depressed inpatients and
Executive Functioning
22 healthy controls on the modified WCST but
Research has shown that individuals with MDD found a difference on the spontaneous condi-
demonstrate decrements on tasks that are pre- tion of the California Card Sorting Test (Delis
sumed to measure executive function (Grant et al., 1992). Individuals with MDD were found
et al. 2001; Langenecker et al., 2005; Paelecke- to generate fewer words on verbal fluency tasks
Habermann et al., 2005; Porter et al. 2003). (i.e., a lexical fluency test and on the ‘exclude
Executive function tasks in neuropsychological letter’ fluency test) in some studies (Porter et al.,
Neuropsychology of Depression and Related Mood Disorders 527

2003) but not in others (Harvey et al., 2004; utilized recall versus recognition memory tasks
Naismith et al., 2006). No difference was found (presuming that the recall tasks are more diffi-
between the MDD and normal controls on the cult than recognition tasks) to examine Hasher
Tower of London test (Naismith et al., 2006; and Zacks’ hypothesis. The researchers found
Porter et al., 2003). that the only difference between the medica-
tion-free MDD group and the normal group on
the RAVLT was the distractor list recall with-
Memory
out any evidence of proactive interference. In
Although many studies have shown decreased contrast, the researchers found that the MDD
memory function in patients with MDD (Austin group performed poorer on measures of recog-
et al., 1992; Bornstein et al., 1991; Brown et al., nition memory (memory for visual information
1994; Burt et al., 1995; Elliott et al., 1996), the such as patterns and spatial locations on the
findings again do not show consistent patterns CANTAB compared to the control group).
or severity of memory impairment (Basso & As mentioned, the number of recurrent
Bornstein, 1999; Brand et al., 1992; Fossati depressive episodes and inpatient status may be
et al., 2002). In addition, the initial acquisi- associated with poorer memory function. Basso
tion process seems to be more affected than and Bornstein (1999) studied 20 single-episode
retrieval in depressed individuals, as evidenced depressed inpatients and 46 recurrent depressed
by decreased initial recall as well as decreased inpatients and found a greater memory dysfunc-
delayed recall and recognition (Austin et al., tion in the recurrent group compared to single-ep-
1992; Basso & Bornstein, 1999; Ravnkilde isode patients on the California Verbal Learning
et al., 2003). This is likely associated with the Test-Second Edition (CVLT-II) measures includ-
affected attentional processes that are inter- ing total initial recall, learning curve, short-de-
fering with the encoding process. Austin et al. lay cued recall, long-delay free and cued recall,
(1992) indicated poorer performance on initial and recognition discrimination. Ravnkilde et al.
acquisition and delayed recall and recognition (2003) did not find a difference in learning and
portions of the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning memory on the Luria Verbal Learning Test but
Test (RAVLT) in two depressed patient groups found a difference on the immediate recall of the
(“endogeneous” and “neurotic”) compared to Logical Memory Test of the WAIS and immedi-
controls. In contrast, Grant et al. (2001) failed ate and delayed recall on the Visual Reproduction
to find differences in memory function in 123 Test of the WMR-R between 40 medicated
MDD outpatients compared to controls on the depressive inpatients and 49 normal controls.
Hopkins Verbal Learning Test (HVLT) and on More positively, memory decrements appear to
the Visual Reproduction subtest of the WAIS-R. subside in remitted individuals. Weiland-Fiedler
Hasher and Zacks (1979) proposed that memory et al. (2004) did not find any difference in mem-
dysfunction in depression is likely demonstrated ory acquisition on the CVLT-II between a group
with more complex tasks that require effortful that consisted of 28 fully remitted, unmediated
encoding rather than less effortful or automatic individuals with a history of MDD and a group
encoding processes. Rohling and Scogin (1993) of 23 healthy participants.
examined 30 depressed patients (21 outpatients
and 9 inpatients), 20 psychiatric controls, and
Neuroanatomy of Mood Disorders
30 normal controls on measures that appeared
to be more challenging such as paired-associate Limbic, frontal, and subcortical areas have been
learning or free recall tasks versus measures that strongly implicated in depression and related
were presumed to be easier such as memory for mood disorders (Alexopoulos, 2002; Carroll
frequency of occurrence or spatial locations. et al., 1976; Drevets & Raichle, 1992; Goldapple
The researchers noted that as there were no sig- et al., 2004; Mayberg et al., 1994; Videbech,
nificant correlations between the measures of 2000), although it should be noted that these
depression severity and the measures of effortful areas are implicated in a number of disorders for
memory, their results failed to support Hasher which dysfunctions in mood and affect are not
and Zacks’ hypothesis. Porter et al. (2003) always present (Nilsson et al., 2002; Owen, 2004;
528 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Van Praag et al., 1975). The relationship of mood incorporated within these circuits and are con-
and cognitive disturbance to neuroanatomical sistent with what was previously described in this
abnormalities remains somewhat tenuous, as few book by King and Caine (1996).
studies have examined these relationships with
sufficient sample size and measurement speci-
Structural Imaging
ficity. This section will review several structures
shown to be affected in mood disorders but will Structural imaging studies of MDD and related
not exhaustively review the many studies that mood disorders, like most morphologic studies,
have been completed of morphometry in mood are limited primarily by region of interest (ROI)
disorders. There is specific focus on studies with approaches. For example, many studies only
larger samples, better clinical characterization, examine one ROI within total brain volume, or a
and attempts to look at the relationship between subsection of total brain volume. Th is approach
cognitive or affective variables and brain mor- can provide for a single disassociation, but does
phometry when possible. Functional imaging not address the possibility of entire systems
studies are reviewed in a subsequent section being disrupted in mood disorders. It does not
below. Figure 22.1 illustrates brain regions enable one to understand other parts of the lim-
referenced throughout this section, whereas bic and frontal networks that may be implicated
Brodmann areas can be found elsewhere (Kolb & in depression, nor does it address whether there
Whishaw, 1996). (See also the color version in the is a global problem or if other systems are unaf-
color insert section.) By and large, these areas are fected. More recently, voxel-based approaches
all part of a ventral cingulate–medial prefrontal have been used to do full brain comparisons,
circuit, as described by Cummings (1993) and but these are limited by heterogeneity in anat-
further subdivided by Rolls (Rolls, 1999) in lateral omy across subjects and limitations in strategies
and medial orbital-frontal circuits. There is also for anatomical warping to address the hetero-
a dorso-lateral prefrontal circuit that is impli- geneity. There are a number of brain regions
cated in some of the research described below. implicated in the neuroanatomy of mood disor-
We further note that subcortical structures are ders, illustrated in Figure 22.1.

Regions of Interest in Mood


Disorders
1. Insula
2. Dorsolateral Prefrontal
3. Anterior Temporal
4. Orbital Frontal
5. Putamen
6. Hippocampal Formation
7. Amygdaloid Complex
8. Posterior Cingulate
9. Dorsal Anterior Cingulate
10. Caudate
11. Medial Prefrontal
12. Nucleus Accumbens
13. Thalamus &
Hypothalamus
14. Raphe
15. Subgenual Anterior
Cingulate

Figure 22–1. Frequent Regions of Interest Reported in Structural and Imaging Studies Relevant to
Understanding Depression and Related Psychiatric Disorders. Numbers indicate center of foci, although some
foci are collapsed across the left-right axis to reduce the number of images necessary to display these foci.
Neuropsychology of Depression and Related Mood Disorders 529

The importance of the hippocampus in mood education matched controls indicated a rela-
disorders is now accepted, though the specificity tionship between right hippocampal volume
and reliability of this relationship has yet to be and persisting memory difficulty 6 months after
fully explicated (Caetano et al., 2004; Campbell & the initial assessment (O’Brien et al., 2004). Of
MacQueen, 2004; Lee et al., 2002; Lopez course, it is unknown if smaller hippocampi
et al., 1998; Mervaala et al., 2000; Sapolsky, placed one at greater risk for mood disorders
2001; Stockmeier et al., 2004). There are sev- and reduced cognition due to increased vulner-
eral reviews of studies of hippocampal (and ability, or if these are the result of length and
amygdalar—see below) volume and depres- severity of illness.
sion available and we refer the reader to these There is continued debate about whether
(Sheline, 2003; Videbech & Ravnkilde, 2004). A the amygdala is involved in mood disorders,
voxel-based morphometry study of 20 patients whereas it is well-established that the amyg-
with treatment resistant depression (TRD), dala is critical in anxiety disorders. There is a
compared to 20 remitted and 20 control sub- mixture of studies reporting increased as com-
jects revealed atrophy in bilateral hippocam- pared to decreased amygdala volume (Sheline,
pal structures, as well as other areas described 2003). A study of 30 first-episode patients with
in subsequent sections (Shah et al., 2002). MDD demonstrated increased bilateral hip-
Their TRD group had a significantly greater pocampal volume compared to the match
number of hospitalizations compared to the control group of 30 patients and 27 recurrent
remitted depressives and all had received at depressed patients and was stable over 1 year
least six sessions of electroconvulsive therapy (Frodl et al., 2003). A larger study of 47 female
(ECT). An earlier study by this group dem- twins pairs exploring amygdala volumes sug-
onstrated reduced Auditory Verbal Learning gested that there were no differences between
performance in treatment resistant depression those with MDD, those at high risk for MDD,
patients compared to remitted patients (Shah and the control subjects, although there was a
et al., 1998) and a positive correlation between significant relationship between twin amygdala
left hippocampal volume and AVLT perfor- volumes suggesting a genetic influence (Munn
mance. A study by Vythlingham and colleagues et al., 2007). Reductions in amygdala volumes
failed to demonstrate differences in hippocam- have been associated with the short form of the
pal volume between 38 depressed and 33 con- 5-HTTLPR polymorphism in one large study
trol subjects, but the study did show trend level of 114 subjects using voxel-based morphometry
correlations between hippocampal volume and (Pezawas et al., 2005), but with the long form in
visual immediate and visual delayed recall on another study of 247 young adult female twins
the Selective Reminding Test (Vythilingam (Chorbov et al., 2007). The data available on
et al., 2004). In 37 moderate to severe traumatic relationships between amygdala volume, cog-
brain injury (TBI) patients with co-occurring nition, and affect measures is limited in mor-
depression, smaller hippocampal volume was phometry data and may be further complicated
associated both with development of mood dis- by comorbidity between depressed and anxiety
orders and poorer vocational outcome at 1 year, subjects in some studies. Whereas there are ani-
suggesting a strong link between hippocampal mal and theoretical models of mood disorders
integrity and affective and cognitive function- that would suggest an important role for the
ing (Pournajafi-Nazarloo et al., 2007). A study amygdala in the etiology and maintenance of the
of 34 inpatients with depression and 34 control mood disorders, the present state of knowledge
subjects reported reduced hippocampal vol- using morphological techniques is equivocal in
ume in MDD and a relationship between lower this regard. Importantly, there is now some sug-
hippocampal volume and poorer performance gestion that amygdala volumes may be enlarged
on the WCST, but not the AVLT (Frodl et al., in first onset of mood disorder, yet smaller or
2006). Furthermore, there was no association not different in size with recurrence of depres-
of cognitive measures with frontal volumes. sion and/or treatment with medication.
A longitudinal study of 61 depressed patients The frontal lobes have long been implicated
over the age of 60, compared with 40 age and in mood disorders (Harlow, 1868; Moniz, 1954).
530 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

However, notes of caution for the reader include volume (Steffens et al., 2003). These studies
the following: first, the frontal lobes encom- suggest that the frontal lobes are indeed rele-
pass 33% of the cortical surface, second, there vant when understanding the pathophysiology
are five, possibly six, frontosubcortical circuits of depression and the potential for concurrent
regulating everything from eye movements to neuropsychological decrements.
emotional functioning, and third, the zeitgeist Other brain regions, and related affect and
remains focused on frontal pathology as a cause, cognitive measures, have received very lit-
or result, of mood disorders. As such, one can- tle research focus thus far in mood disorders.
not conclude that the frontal lobes are uniquely Studies of the basal ganglia are mixed, and
involved in mood disorders. Methodological there appears to be a distinction in volume
challenges in measuring specific frontal regions between simply depressed (reduced) and bipo-
are formidable, whether by Brodmann area or lar (increased) patients with mood disorders
by specific landmarks. The subgenual anterior (Anand & Shekhar, 2003; Krishnan et al., 1992).
cingulate has been demonstrated to be smaller One study of the thalamus with 25 bipolar and
in several studies (Botteron et al., 2002; Drevets 17 unipolar patients showed no differences
et al., 1997; Ongur et al., 1998, also Coryell et al., in thalamic volume from 39 control subjects
2005) with one of the negative studies showing (Caetano et al., 2001). The insula has also been
a functional abnormality in the same region implicated in mood disorders, although vari-
(Pizzagalli et al., 2004). One other group studied ability in measurement boundaries likely have
both anterior and posterior cingulate volume in precluded obtaining strong reliability in mor-
21 unremitted and 10 remitted patients with phologic measurements, thus reducing reports
unipolar depression compared to 31 control in the literature about this region (Nagai et al.,
subjects, showing reduced volumes in all four 2007). Voxel-based morphometry studies,
(right, left by anterior, posterior) ROIs in those which are limited in structural specificity, have
with unremitted depression, but only in the left shown more structural abnormalities in the
anterior cingulate in the subset with remitted insula in schizophrenia, and not in mood disor-
depression, both compared to the control group ders (Nagai et al., 2007).
(Caetano et al., 2006). The remitted and unre- Of course, the relevance of subcortical and
mitted groups did not differ in ROI volumes, periventricular hyperintensities in mood dis-
suggesting that reduced power in the subset orders are of increasing interest, particularly in
analysis may have masked differences between elderly or middle-age onset mood disorders, and
the patient groups, as well as between the remit- these patients are more likely to exhibit cogni-
ted patient and control groups. A study of 44 tive difficulties (Bhalla et al., 2006; King et al.,
elderly patients with depression showed larger 1998; O’Brien et al., 2004; Sobow et al., 2006).
lateral ventricles in those depressed patients One large study of 48 depressed patients and 73
with prior ECT compared to those without depressed inpatients demonstrated an increas-
ECT (Simpson et al., 2001). Furthermore, there ing odds ratio of 5.32 for subcortical hyperin-
were multiple correlations of neuropsychologi- tensities in those with depression (Coffey et al.,
cal measures with frontal (reverse digit span), 1993). Another of 37 patients found that white
temporal (Trial 1 of the RAVLT, perseverative matter hyperintensities were predictors of con-
errors on the WCST, copy of the Rey–Osterrieth version to dementia syndromes and/or persist-
Complex Figure Test, and memory for the Rey– ing cognitive difficulties (Hickie et al., 1997).
Osterrieth Complex Figure Test), and parietal A more recent study of 41 MDD patients and
(Digit Symbol Substitution Test, Trial 1 of the 41 control subjects reported inverse relation-
RAVLT, and reverse digit span) lobe volumes ships between bilateral orbital frontal volumes
but not volume of the lateral ventricle. Another and subcortical gray matter lesions severity, but
study compared 30 elderly depressives and 40 not deep white matter lesion severity (Lee et al.,
controls, demonstrating a negative relationship 2003). One very large study of 2546 subjects
between perseverative errors and a positive rela- between age 60 and 64 did not find an associa-
tionship with total correct on the Benton Visual tion between cognitive measures and hippo-
Memory Test with left orbital frontal cortical campal or amygdalar volume, APOE4, or white
Neuropsychology of Depression and Related Mood Disorders 531

matter hyperintensities, but rather found these more challenging. Finally, due to current funding
decrements to be associated with psychiatric structures, recruitment difficulties given restric-
symptoms, poor physical health, and personal- tive exclusion criteria for imaging protocols, and
ity factors (Jorm et al., 2004). A subset analysis the expense of imaging technology, most, if not
of this sample suggested that degree of white all, imaging studies are underpowered, limiting
matter hyperintensities in 475 elders was related convergence across functional imaging studies,
to depressive symptoms, potentially mediated particularly in a biologically heterogeneous set
by smoking and physical disability (Jorm et al., of mood disorders. This section is subdivided
2005). The disruption of networks important for into a brief review of neurotransmitter stud-
cognitive and affective processing is the likely ies, resting glucose/blood flow studies, affective
consequence of white matter hyperintensities, challenges, and cognitive challenges.
even if the underlying pathology behind white Before beginning to review functional imag-
matter hyperintensities remains under debate ing studies in mood disorders, it is valuable to
(Cummings & Benson, 1984; Lamberty & review the affective neuroscience literature, as
Bieliauskas, 1993). much of this data was the genesis for imaging
paradigms described below. In fact, in future
chapters on mood disorders, emotion process-
Functional Neuroimaging
ing may more appropriately be placed under
and Cognitive and Affective
the “traditional” neuropsychological profile
Neuroscience Research
of mood disorders. Herein, we differentiate
Tremendous progress has been achieved in between emotion perception and processing as
functional imaging studies of mood disor- they might differ from emotional experience.
ders in the last decade. Indeed, the field has The former denotes a cognitive process in the
progressed to the point of trials for deep brain same vein as visual perception and process-
stimulation with subgenual cingulate, ventral ing, whereby, the latter refers more broadly to a
striatal and left inferior frontal targets for treat- gestalt of experience. Behavioral studies of emo-
ment resistant depression (Mayberg et al., 2005; tion processing abnormalities in mood disorders
Schlaepfer et al., 2007), as well as in use of repet- have often focused on aspects of behavior such as
itive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) mood-congruent memory biases (Gotlib et al.,
(McLoughlin et al., 2007). In addition, imag- 2004, 2005; Rude et al., 2002), negative process-
ing data are being used to prospectively predict ing biases (Danion et al., 1991, 1995; Watkins
treatment response, highlighting both the het- et al., 1992, 1996; White et al., 1992), memory
erogeneity in mood disorders and the specificity priming (Bradley et al., 1995; Mogg et al., 1993),
of actual brain function to illness and treatment and interference effects. These studies indicate
parameters (Brody et al., 1999; Langenecker a proclivity for processing and remembering
et al., 2007b; Mayberg et al., 1997; Pizzigalli negative stimuli, and better delayed memory
et al., 2001). Herein, we will review very briefly for such stimuli, in those with mood disorders.
functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), Facial emotion stimuli have also been used to
positron emission tomography (PET), and sin- determine whether depressed individuals expe-
gle photon emission computed tomography rience difficulty with processing and classifying
(SPECT) studies of mood disorders, again with the emotion expressed in these faces. A major-
specific focus on those studies using larger sam- ity of these studies indicates that depressed
ple sizes and directly assessing relationships of individuals have difficulty in correctly classify-
activation with clinical and cognitive variables. ing emotions in facial stimuli (Bouhuys et al.,
We note that the time and spatial sensitivity for 1996, 1999; Gur et al., 1992; Langenecker et al.,
PET/SPECT and fMRI differ, as well as the cogni- 2005; Mikhailova et al., 1996; Nandi et al., 1982,
tive/affective/rest paradigms used, which might Persad & Polivy, 1993). These data have formed
easily explain the apparent discrepancy between a basis for exploring the functional underpin-
PET/SPECT and fMRI results. In addition, arti- nings of affective irregularities in mood disor-
fact and signal voids in orbital and medial tem- ders, primarily in fMRI (below), but also in PET
poral regions in fMRI make imaging these areas and SPECT studies.
532 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Imaging with transmitter-specific ligands of MDD, prefrontal cortex, basal ganglia, and
using SPECT and PET suggests decreased medial temporal areas are perhaps more con-
5HT1-A and 2-A binding in medial tempo- sistently shown to deviate from controls using
ral structures in mood disorders, although the blood flow and glucose studies with PET (Liotti
data are not consistent, with affects of gender, et al., 2000; Milak et al., 2005; Videbech, 2000).
age of onset, current age, and illness severity all One PET study of 40 patients with depression
playing a potential role in these discrepancies showed diminished rCBF-to-cognitive mea-
(Kennedy & Zubieta, 2004). Studies with dopa- sures relationships when compared to similar
mine ligands, such as DR D2 and endogenous relationships in 49 healthy controls (Ravnkilde
opiods, have received relatively less research et al., 2003). One unique study used acute tryp-
focus in mood disorders (Kennedy et al., 2006; tophan (precursor to serotonin) depletion in
Larisch et al., 1997), but these studies also sug- eight remitted men, showing diminished ven-
gest relatively decreased binding in mood dis- tral anterior cingulate and orbital frontal cortex
orders. There is some suggestion that dopamine H2(15)O PET after depletion associated with
binding may differ by mood disorder subtype. increased depression symptoms (Smith et al.,
In one small study, those with psychomotor 1999). Those who exhibited related slowing in
retardation and anhedonia associated with verbal fluency had diminished anterior cingu-
decreased DR D2 binding, whereas those with late activity.
impulsivity showed normal binding in left cau- Functional imaging studies with cognitive
date (Martinot et al., 2001). A more recent study and affective challenges in mood disorders have
of a mixed group of 12 suicide attempters, most exploded, particularly within the last 5 years.
with mood disorders, suggested higher impul- In addition, increasing sophistication in mea-
sivity was related to lower serotonin (5HTT) sure design, combined with tighter recruitment
and dopamine (DAT) binding potential, with protocols and relatively larger sample sizes, has
relationships most strongly located in the basal resulted in exciting new findings within the
ganglia, and extending into insula for serotonin. field. As before, we will highlight studies, with
Unfortunately, there is mixed evidence related larger sample sizes and attempts to relate func-
to whether treatment of depression results in a tional abnormalities in mood disorders with
significant change in dopamine and serotonin clinical and cognitive variables, with the goal of
concentrations (Argyelan et al., 2005; Moses- increasing confidence about brain regions and
Kolko et al., 2007). Important for the purposes systems that are affected in mood disorders.
of this chapter, these neurotransmitters are rep- Affective neuroscience studies in mood dis-
resented quite densely in the same anatomical orders using fMRI focused heavily on experien-
areas discussed up to this point: frontal, ante- tial aspects of emotion processing, for example,
rior and medial temporal, insular, and subcor- passive viewing and resting activation studies
tical areas. (Bench et al., 1993; Dolan et al., 1994; Kalin et al.,
Fluorodeoxyglucose (FDG), H2(15)O, and 1997; Sheline et al., 2001; Whalen, 1998; Wright
resting cerebral blood flow (rCBF) PET stud- et al., 2001). Stimuli have included faces, com-
ies in depression implicate a network of fron- plex visual scenes, and more recently semantic
tal and limbic areas involved, much like that stimuli, some of which are rated as to personal
reported in the section on ‘Neuroanatomy of relevance (Canli et al., 2004; Fossati et al.,
Mood Disorders’. Subgenual anterior cingulate 2003; George et al., 1993, 1994, 1996; Gilboa-
abnormalities in rest and affective paradigms Schechtman et al., 2002; Gorno-Tempini et al.,
have been reported, with hypometabolism in 2001; Iidaka et al., 2001; Kensinger & Corkin,
MDD and in the depressed phase of bipolar 2004; Maddock et al., 2003; Ochsner et al.,
disorder (Drevets et al., 1997; Kennedy et al., 2004; Phan et al., 2003; Siegle et al., 2002, see
1997; Kegeles et al., 2003; Mayberg et al., 1994). Figure 22.2 for an example from our own work,
The abnormalities in subgenual anterior cin- see also the color version in the color insert
gulate resolve with effective treatment (Brody section). Physiological reactivity to emotional
et al., 2001; Kennedy et al., 2007). The same stimuli and emotion induction is distinctly dif-
brain regions implicated in anatomical studies ferent in depressed compared to control groups
A B C Neg-Neut

–5 15 21

D E F Post-Neut

–9 18 29 CON>MDD
MDD>CON

G H Brodmann Area 9/46 Activation by Group and Contrast


1.5
CON MDD
1.2
0.9
C D
0.6
0.3
0
–0.3

–0.6 Left Pos–Neut


–0.9 Left Neg–Neut
Right Pos–Neut
–1.2 Right Neg–Neut

Figure 22–2. Activation to emotionally salient stimuli for those with Major Depressive Disorder (MDD, n =
13) compared to control (CON, n = 15) participants. Mean Hamilton Depression Rating Scale-17 Item score
for the MDD group was 19.2 (SD = 7.6). The Emotion Word Stimulus Test is nine blocks each of positive (Pos),
Negative (Neg) and Neutral (Neut) words (from the Affective Norms for Emotional Words set; Bradley and
Lang, 1999) presented to participants during 3 Tesla functional MRI (GE Scanner). Images were analyzed with
Statistical Parametric Mapping 2 (SPM2; Friston, 1996, thresholds p<.001, minimum cluster size = 120 mm3)
and Region of Interest (ROI) posthoc analyses were conducted using the MARSBAR tool from SPM2 (Brett
et al., 2002). Panels A–F (radiological orientation) represent group differences with red indicating CON>MDD
and blue indicating MDD>CON. Panels A–C illustrate group comparisons for Neg-Neut and panels D–F
represent contrasts for Pos-Neut. There was an area of increased activation for both contrasts, MDD>CON
in right middle frontal gyrus (MFG—Brodmann area 9/46, Panels C, F) that was further explored in posthoc
analyses. Panel G illustrates the spherical ROI created in the right MFG (in green). An identical spherical ROI
was created in the left MFG to test the laterality theory of MDD. The theory states that there will be increased
activation in right MFG for Neg-Neut and decreased activation in left MFG for Pos-Neut in MDD compared
to CON (Davidson, 2002). This theory is not supported (Panel H bars) with increased right MFG activation for
both emotion contrasts (MDD>CON: left bar set = CON; right bar set = MDD). Left MFG was not different
between groups in any contrast. We interpret these findings as increased emotion regulatory demand for emo-
tional words in MDD irrespective of Pos or Neg valence.

533
534 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

(Kalin et al., 1997; Ketter et al., 1996; Kumari has been termed the default network (includ-
et al., 2003; Paradiso et al., 1999; Phillips et al., ing medial, rostral, and posterior cingulate) and
2001; Thomas et al., 2001). This disruption in more task-oriented structures in depression
neurophysiological reactions to emotion stimuli (lateral prefrontal and parietal, Anand et al.,
is reversible with successful treatment (Brody 2005; Greicius et al., 2007), although the spec-
et al., 2001; Davidson et al., 1999; Davidson ificity and clinical meaning of these findings
et al., 2003; Fu et al., 2004; Kalin et al., 1997; remain to be seen.
Sheline et al., 2001; see example of our own In summary, morphological and functional
work in Figure 22.2 below). Although compel- imaging are strongly suggesting that limbic,
ling, these studies have just begun to explore frontal, and subcortical regions are involved
the nature of emotion processing difficulties in in mood disorders (see Figure 22.1), converg-
depression. ing nicely with the traditional neuropsycholog-
Cognitive neuroscience studies of mood dis- ical profi le described earlier in the chapter. At
orders in fMRI are perhaps the most intriguing present, the nature of the relationship is largely
in recent years, with several recent studies indi- correlational—it is unclear if these abnormal-
cating increased frontal activation in mood dis- ities precede onset of the mood disorder and
orders relative to comparison groups. One study may place individuals at higher risk for devel-
of interference resolution reported increased opment of a mood disorder, or if they are the
activation for MDD patients (e.g., left DLPFC, result of neurobiological changes concurrent
AC) compared to the control group (Wagner with, or resulting from, experience of the mood
et al., 2006). Studies utilizing working memory disorder. Nonetheless, imaging studies of fron-
and verbal fluency tasks have generally reported tal, medial temporal, and subcortical regions
more prominent activation in the control will likely continue in mood disorders. There
groups in frontal, basal ganglia, and parietal is currently debate about relative increases and
areas compared to MDD patients (Audenaert decreases in lateral activation bias (right over
et al., 2002; Barch et al., 2003; Elliott et al., left), or cortical/subcortical (under activation
1997; Holmes et al., 2005; Hugdahl et al., 2003, of cortical, overactivation of subcortical/lim-
2004; Matsuo et al., 2002; Okada et al., 2003), bic), but the body of research has not yet con-
while three other studies (examining working verged on a consistently replicable pattern of
memory, attention, and interference control, findings (Davidson et al., 2002; Northoff et al.,
respectively) have noted greater activation in 2000; Phillips et al., 2003). We strongly urge
frontal areas for the MDD groups compared to researchers to more routinely include analysis
the control groups in the context of preserved of structure/function to affective/cognitive rela-
behavioral performance (Harvey et al., 2005; tionships, particularly with neuropsychological
Holmes et al., 2005; Wagner et al., 2006). In variables. These can provide better grounding
this same vein, our group has used a parametric of the significance of any differences between
Go/No-Go test in 20 patients with MDD com- those with mood disorders and control groups.
pared to 22 control subjects (Langenecker et al.,
2007b). The MDD patients exhibited decreased
Critical Mediating Factors in
attention and increased inhibitory control accu-
Cognitive Dysfunction related
racy performance in a significant interaction,
to Depression
relative to the control group. The MDD group
also exhibited greater subgenual cingulate and
Age of Onset
bilateral ventral frontal activation compared to
the control group during correct rejections of There does not appear to be a gold standard for
No-Go stimuli (inhibition). determining age of onset for depression due to
More recently, resting functional connectivity difficulties with individual patient memories,
and ROI-linked functional connectivity studies inconsistency of report from one query to the
are now on the forefront of studies of mood dis- next, and variability in depressive symptoms
orders. There appears to be a disruption in the (Knauper et al., 1999). Authors have reported
core, low-frequency “cross-talk” between what that early-onset major depression has a mean age
Neuropsychology of Depression and Related Mood Disorders 535

of 13.7 (SD = 5.0) and late-onset depression has abnormalities. This finding was subsequently
a mean age of 33.5 (SD = 9.5) (Klein et al., 1999). confirmed in a large longitudinal (Paterniti
Early-onset depression is considered more malig- et al., 2002) and neuropathologic study (Sweet
nant, persistent, and resistant to treatment. et al., 2004). Similarly, depression in old age
The cognitive changes associated with is associated with generalized atherosclerosis
this adult onset depression are felt to include (Vinkerset al., 2005) and with increased car-
decreased attention on tasks that require diovascular and noncardiovascular mortality
increased effort, decreased initial acquisition (Vinkers et al., 2004).
of stimuli, and decreased retrieval of infor- In sum, while primary depression, generally
mation that is encoded (Caine, 1986). Caine first occurring in young adulthood, is asso-
(1981) describes these changes as being simi- ciated with cognitive dysfunction related to
lar to a subcortical dementia. Interestingly, in attention, learning, and recall, the initial occur-
their description of the syndrome of subcortical rence of depressive symptoms in the elderly is
dementia, Cummings and Benson (1984) note very likely an indicator of an underlying degen-
depression to be a prominent feature. The cog- erative or cerebrovascular neurological pro-
nitive changes, however, are coincident with the cess. For each depressive symptom observed
severity of the depression and many are lessened in the elderly, the rate of cognitive decline has
if the severity of depressive symptoms is thera- been observed to increase by about 5% pre-
peutically ameliorated (Bieliauskas, 1993). dicting cognitive decline in old age (Wilson
When symptoms of depression occur for the et al., 2004), though depressive symptoms are
first time in older adults, they are more likely not necessarily increased during the prodro-
to indicate underlying neurological change mal phase of Alzheimer’s disease itself (Wilson
than to be manifestations of primary depres- et al., 2008). From a quality-of-life perspective,
sion (Bieliauskas, 1993). In their review of the late-life depression is treatable by conventional
literature, Lamberty and Bieliauskas (1993) pharmacotherapies, though careful dosing
report that most late-onset depressive symp- must be observed (Sadavoy, 2004). The positive
toms are accompanied by abnormal findings change in cognitive efficiency that is observed
when neuroimaging is employed. In 1992, the when primary depression is treated, however, is
NIH Consensus Conference on Diagnosis and not likely to be seen in the treatment of late-life-
Treatment of Depression in Late Life stated: onset depressive symptoms (Bieliauskas, 1993).

There is some evidence to suggest that late-onset


depression is associated with a lower frequency of Severity
family history of depression but a higher frequency It is currently unclear whether severity, as defined
of cognitive impairment, cerebral atrophy, deep
by clinician rating or self-report obtained, has
white matter changes, recurrences, medical comor-
any relation to the extent of neuropsychologi-
bidity, and mortality. (NIH Consensus Development
Panel on Depression in Late Life, 1992, p. 1019) cal decrements. There is a suggestion that being
seen on an inpatient psychiatry ward is an index
Multiple later studies have confirmed the same. of disease severity that would portend greater
Kumar et al. (2000) report that atrophy and cognitive difficulties (Burt et al., 1995), as would
high-intensity lesions represent independent recurrence of depressive episodes. This section
pathways to late-life depression, with patients will review studies prototypic of those examin-
with major depression having larger whole ing severity according to objective psychometric
brain lesion volumes than controls and smaller measures and/or location of treatment service/
frontal lobe volumes. recruitment. We acknowledge that depression
As a corollary, depression in the elderly also severity gradations assume a dimensional dis-
appears to be predictive of subsequent cognitive tribution of cognitive decrements, related in a
decline. Nussbaum et al. (1995) report that 23% linear fashion. A categorical perspective, such
of a sample of depressed patients showed cogni- as that used between treatment responders
tive decline over a 25-month period, along with and treatment nonresponders, also provides an
increased white matter MRI, CAT, and EEG index of severity and is reviewed below in a later
536 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

section. Further, there are some mood disorders in significant cognitive decrements and under-
(e.g., bipolar disorder) that are often viewed as lying neurological dysfunction (as a chronolog-
greater in severity relative to others (MDD). ical sequelae of depression).
These sorts of subtype gradations, such as com- A history of inpatient hospitalization and/
paring MDD to Bipolar disorder or comparing or the number of previous depressive episodes
melancholic and nonmelancholic depression, appear to be associated with the executive and
are also conducted in a separate section below. memory decrements in some studies, both dif-
One large study of elderly patients with ferent indices of clinical severity. Purcell et al.
minor (N = 32) and major depression (N = (1997) examined 20 depressed patients (19 out-
63) indicated poorer performance compared patients and 1 inpatient; 12 medicated) and 20
to a matched control group of 71 participants age and education matched normal controls on
in initial learning on trial 1 of the CVLT and the CANTAB and found attentional set-shifting
on List B, as well as in number of correct cat- decrements in the depressed group. On closer
egories for WCST (minor depression only), examination, the researchers found that a his-
and Letter–Number Sequencing (major only tory of inpatient hospitalization was associated
(Elderkin-Thompson et al., 2007)). There were no with poorer performance on the set-shifting
significant effects of severity using this classifi- task. Paelecke-Habermann et al. (2005) stud-
cation. A very large study of 385 elderly patients ied 40 euthymic patients with a history of MDD
showed an increase in disruption on the Stroop diagnosis (20 individuals with one to two epi-
test between mild and moderate depression, but sodes and 20 individuals with three or more
not with severe depression (Baune et al., 2006). episodes) and 20 healthy controls on tasks of
A smaller study of 26 severely depressed patients executive function (Behavioral Assessment of
demonstrated a significant positive relationship the Dysexecutive Syndrome, word fluency, and
between severity of depression and measures of backward visuospatial span) and found worse
simple and complex reaction time, or process- performance in the MDD group. Furthermore,
ing speed (Egeland et al., 2005) and one mea- the severe MDD group performed worse
sure of memory, with a surprising number of than the mild group, suggesting that indi-
significant negative correlations between 8 a.m. viduals with recurrent episodes demonstrate
cortisol levels and memory and executive func- greater decrements than those with one or two
tioning measures. episodes.
As is probably well known, those with Studies of the impact of severity of illness
severe depression have poorer long-term treat- as they relate to significance of cognitive dec-
ment prognosis (Elkin et al., 1995; Saghafi rements are limited in many respects due to
et al., 2007), which may in fact be mediated nonuniformity in neuropsychological measures
by extent of neuropsychological decrements employed, clinical measures of severity utilized,
in performance. There have been attempts to and the broader issue of heterogeneity in mood
use chronicity as a measure of severity, includ- disorders by subtype. Future studies will likely
ing structured interviews to code number of have to further subdivide into different cogni-
days ill. However, it should be noted that recall tive domains, different depression subtypes,
bias may significantly affect patient reports of with unique mediating factors.
length of illness and number of episodes (Riso
et al., 2002). One interesting study used retro-
Effort
spective recall of days of untreated depression
to predict hippocampal volume loss in women Effort, motivation, and abulia continue to be
with recurrent depression (Sheline et al., 2003). key concepts in understanding mood disor-
It is important to conduct longitudinal studies ders and related cognitive decrements (King
to determine whether severity of symptoms is & Caine, 1996). Indeed, the effort-automatic
related to cognitive performance in a meaning- hypothesis of depression had direct roots in
ful way (e.g., reflective of permanent underlying beliefs about how depression might affect moti-
disruption of neural networks as an etiological vation and effort. For example, early studies
risk factor), or whether depression itself results indicated that memory tasks that were more
Neuropsychology of Depression and Related Mood Disorders 537

automatic (recognition) tended to be performed et al., 2004; Egeland et al., 2005). We have also
just fine by depressed patients, whereas more recently found that depression, as measured by
effortful, difficult tasks (recall) tended to be DSM-IV criteria and Geriatric Depression Scale
tasks that were more difficult for those with (GDS; Yesavage), was not related to very basic
mood disorders (Cohen et al., 1982; Roy-Byrne measures of effort such as the Rey 15-Item Test
et al., 1986; Weingartner et al., 1981). As noted (Lee et al., 1992; Rey, 1964) or the Kaufman
in chapter 9 by King and Caine (1996) in the Hand Movements Test (Bowen & Littell, 1997;
previous edition of this book, there are other Kaufman & Kaufman, 1983) in a population of
theories accounting for cognitive decrements in elderly medical inpatients (Vadnal, 2005).
mood disorders, including decreased resource These differences in failed versus passed
availability, increased distraction through inef- effort tests in “depressed” samples highlight
ficient inhibitory process, and mood congruent the importance of using formal or informal, but
memory biases, though we will not go into these validated, effort measures when assessing those
in detail herein. with mood disorders, especially when the pos-
More recently, there has been a strong and sibility of primary and secondary gain becomes
healthy debate about the genesis of cognitive crucially important in interpreting the relevance
decrements in patients with depression, largely of poor test performance. When no primary or
revolving around the old concepts of “func- secondary gain can be found, the clinician can
tional” and “organic” brain-based cognitive correlate self-reported energy levels, depres-
difficulties, particularly within legal contexts sion severity, psychomotor retardation, and so
(Rohling et al., 2002). In a recent study of pri- on with observed inefficiencies in performance
marily legal cases, over 40% of subjects recruited (everyday functioning), but estimates of opti-
were excluded because of documented poor mal functioning levels or inference of underly-
effort using standardized measures (Rohling ing neuronal dysfunction in these contexts need
et al., 2002). Interestingly, the excluded group to be carefully interpreted.
had significantly higher depression scores on
the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HDRS)
Subtypes of Mood Disorders
compared to the group that was not excluded,
and performed significantly worse on multi- There are a number of different subtypes of
ple cognitive measures including learning and depression, wherein cognitive function may be
memory. A smaller percentage (15%, 8%) of greater (e.g., cerebrovascular, bipolar disorder).
patients fi ling for workers compensation with Furthermore, there are certain subtype-by-
depressive symptoms failed formal effort mea- cognitive function distinctions, most nota-
sures in a study of 233 patients (Sumanti et al., bly the prevalence of psychomotor retardation
2006). Because anergia and amotivation are in melancholic but not in atypical depression.
key symptoms of depression and these symp- A rapidly emerging set of data is in cognitive
toms can adversely affect effort, it is reason- decrements with co-occurring depression and
able to assume that for a minority of depressed medical conditions. Cushing’s disease is a rare
patients, “amotivation” may result in a failed disease with high prevalence of depression,
effort test. One possible interpretation of this thought to be mediated by hypercortisolism
finding is that significant primary depression and epitomizing the glucocorticoid cascade
may not result in permanent brain dysfunction, hypothesis of depression. Review of these select
but rather that insufficient engagement of moti- subtypes exemplifies the variability of cognitive
vational drive (evident in depression) can mimic dysfunction in mood disorders.
cognitive decrements on challenging tasks.
However, two recent studies have not shown
Cerebrovascular
any difficulty in depressed patients or those
with anxiety, in passing formal effort measures, The increased prevalence of depression among
suggesting that depression symptoms may patients with cerebrovascular accidents (CVAs)
have very different etiologies and purposes in has long been established, with between 50%
legal as opposed to clinical settings (Ashendorf and 68% manifesting symptoms of depression
538 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

(Eastwood et al., 1989). These estimates ren- the normal course of illness of the other. In the
dered the occurrence of “melancholia in up to past, depression associated with a medical ill-
25% of patients,” with minor or masked depres- ness was considered to be a psychological reac-
sion in 75%–95% of cases (Wiart, 1997). A more tion to having a medical illness and thought to
recent study has demonstrated that patients with have a less severe course (Boland et al., 2006).
ischemic stroke have double the risk of depres- However, it is now clear that depression in the
sion compared with those without stroke (11.2% medically ill may be more difficult to treat com-
versus 5.2%) with a greater incidence of depres- pared to depression in individuals without a
sion corresponding to more severe CVAs, par- comorbid medical condition (Boland et al.,
ticularly in vascular territories supplying limbic 2006). These individuals generally report more
structures (Desmond et al., 2003). Though there medical symptoms even when the severity of
has been speculation that the distribution of the medical disorder is taken into account (for
CVAs, especially as related to syndromes of a review, see Katon et al., 2007). Depression can
aphasia, may specifically relate to depressive be detrimental in the medically ill in that these
symptoms, more recent evidence suggests that individuals are three times less likely to adhere
screening for aphasia across studies is highly to medical treatment recommendations com-
inconsistent and that conclusions as to relation- pared to nondepressed medically ill individu-
ships between aphasia subtypes and depression als (Dimatteo et al., 2000). In addition, when
are not justified (Townend et al., 2007). Laska the depression is not properly treated, it could
et al. (2007) alternatively suggest that depres- negatively impact the morbidity and mortality
sion diagnosis and severity can be reliably made of the medical illness, especially in older adults
during the acute phases of aphasia and suggest (Boland, 2006; Covinsky et al., 1999; Frasure-
that depression can be identified in at least 24% Smith et al., 1993; Ganzini et al., 1997; Katon,
of such patients. 2003; Lesperance & Frasure-Smith, 1996). For
Nevertheless, there remains little doubt that example, Frasure-Smith et al. followed 222
depression is common following CVA. There patients who were hospitalized for myocardial
has been the suggestion of a vascular depression infarction for 6 months and found that MDD
hypothesis, that is, that cerebrovascular disease was an independent risk factor for mortal-
may predispose, precipitate, or perpetuate some ity. Overall, MDD can be identified in 7–17%
geriatric depressive syndromes, It is hypothe- of patients with chronic medical conditions
sized that prefrontal systems are disrupted with (Egede, 2007). In contrast, when depression is
associated executive dysfunction (Alexopoulos treated, medical symptoms decrease regardless
et al., 1997), mechanisms discussed in the sec- of the improvement in the medical disease sta-
tion on ‘Neuroanatomy of Mood Disorders’. tus (Borson et al., 1992).
The cognitive profi le of the medically ill indi-
viduals with depression should not differ from
Secondary to Medical Condition
the traditional profi le described above. However,
Depression is a common comorbid condition in additional physical symptoms including pain
medical illnesses including cancer (Chochinov, and increased fatigue, which are more com-
2001; McDaniel et al., 1995; Spiegel, 2001), dia- mon in certain medical populations including
betes (Anderson et al., 2001; Ciechanowski cancer, chronic pain, and fibromyalgia need to
et al., 2003), heart disease (Ruo et al., 2003), be taken into consideration when interpreting
asthma (Eisner et al., 2005), chronic obstructive test results. Neuropsychological investigations
pulmonary disease (Yohannes et al., 2000), obe- in the non-neurological, medically ill individu-
sity (Thomsen et al., 2006), migraine headaches als with depression are scarce with only a few
(Molgat & Patten, 2005), chronic fatigue syn- that have focused on fibromyalgia and chronic
drome (Patten et al., 2005), fibromyalgia (Ahles fatigue syndrome (Johnson et al., 1997; Landro
et al., 1991; Patten et al., 2005), and chronic pain et al., 1997; Suhr, 2003). Nonetheless, it appears
(McWilliams et al., 2003). A bidirectional rela- that cognitive inefficiencies are generally associ-
tion between depression and medical illnesses ated with the individual’s presenting symptoms
appears to exist, with each possibly altering such as fatigue or depression. For example, Suhr
Neuropsychology of Depression and Related Mood Disorders 539

investigated 28 individuals with fibromyalgia, improvements in memory are positively asso-


27 individuals with chronic pain, and 21 healthy ciated with posttreatment volume increases
controls on measures of intellect, memory, exec- in the hippocampus, whereas improvements
utive functioning, attention, and psychomotor in mood are associated with increases in the
speed, as well as on self-reported measures of caudate (Starkman et al., 1999, 2003, 2007).
depression, pain, fatigue, and cognitive com- Cushing’s disease provides a model for how
plaints. The fibromyalgia group reported more excessive stress hormones can have potentially
memory complaints, fatigue, pain, and depres- reversible cognitive and affective sequelae,
sion than the other groups. Although no dif- highlighting the deleterious affects of stress
ference in performance was found on cognitive hormones, the link between brain morphome-
measures when fatigue, pain, and depression try and cognitive and affective symptoms, and
were taken into account, the severity of depres- the potential for plasticity in brain and neuro-
sion was associated with memory performance psychological functioning in mood disorders
and fatigue was associated with psychomotor (McEwen, 2002). Although focus has been pri-
speed. marily on amygdala, hippocampus, and cau-
date thus far, future studies by our group are
targeted toward effects of hypercortisolism on
Cushing’s Disease and
the entire medial frontal–subcortical circuit.
Hypercortisolemic Mood Disorders
Studies of hypercortisolemia in other mood
One very well-understood model for describ- disorders have also helped build the connec-
ing the neurobiological sequelae of mood tion between cortisol/ACTH levels and cogni-
disorders is the glucocorticoid cascade tive dysfunction. This may be in part due to the
hypothesis (Sapolsky, 2000). In this model, heterogeneity in depressive samples and relative
the increased stress often observed in mood variability of neuroendocrine measurements
disorders results in excessive release of CRF, across subjects. Hypercortisolemia occurs in
signaling release of ACTH, resulting in subse- approximately 50% of individuals with depres-
quent release of cortisol. In 33% of individu- sion, and treatment with agents to reduce cortisol
als with MDD, an abnormal ACTH response levels can result in improved cognitive perfor-
to the steroid dexamethasone is indicative of mance (Golomb et al., 1993; Young et al., 1994,
failed feedback mechanism, putatively a result 2001, 2004). One study of 23 severely depressed
of chronic excessive glucocorticoids (Sonino patients found significant negative correlations
& Fava, 1996; Young et al., 1991). Cushing’s between executive and memory functions and
disease is a rare disorder often thought to 8 a.m. cortisol levels (Egeland et al., 2005). In
typify the glucocorticoid model, as there is a study of 102 female outpatients with remit-
sustained, elevated cortisol levels for as long ted MDD, those with recurrent depression had
as several years before the condition is diag- higher cortisol levels than the nonrecurrent
nosed and treated (1961). The excess cortisol women (Bos et al., 2005). Elevated cortisol but
levels have been associated with a 60% preva- not ACTH levels have also been reported in a
lence of mood irregularities, typically depres- study of 29 participants with psychotic major
sion. Further, treatment of Cushing’s disease depression compared to 26 healthy controls
typically results in alleviation of depressive and to 24 participants with nonpsychotic major
symptoms (Sonino et al., 1993; Starkman et al., depression (Gomez et al., 2006). Some modest
1981, 1986). The remission of cognitive diffi- correlations were reported between measures
culties thought to be secondary to Cushing’s of executive functioning, processing speed, and
disease is more equivocal in nature, with some memory with cortisol levels. Furthermore, in a
studies showing no improvement, and others study of 17 patients with bipolar disorder, euthy-
showing improvement in memory and execu- mic phase, and 16 matched control subjects,
tive functioning (Dorn & Cerrone, 2000; Dorn post-dexamethasone cortisol was positively
et al., 1997; Forget et al., 2002; Heald et al., correlated with number of errors on a working
2004; Hook et al., 2007; Mauri et al., 1993; memory task (Watson et al., 2006). It is unclear
Starkman et al., 2001). One study reported that whether hypercortisolemia mediates cognitive
540 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

decrements in mood disorders, and the relation- individuals on measures of learning and mem-
ships between HPA axis measures and cognition ory, executive function, and psychomotor speed
are not always evident (Caine et al., 1984). As (Ferrier et al., 1999; Martinez-Aran et al., 2004;
noted before, underlying neural networks that Rubinsztein et al., 2000; Zubieta et al., 2001).
control stress responses and assist in certain Research on individuals with BD often indi-
aspects of cognition are negatively affected in cates a similar pattern of cognitive inefficien-
mood disorders, but variability in presence and cies that is found in individuals with MDD,
degree of disruption, when combined with small with more decrements in general (Borkowska &
sample sizes and variability in clinical char- Rybakowski, 2001; for reviews see Olley et al.,
acterization and classification, might explain 2005; Quraishi & Frangou, 2002). For example, a
inconsistencies within the literature. study examining unmedicated individuals with
MDD or BD during acute depressive episodes
indicated worse performance in the BD group
Melancholic Depression
on the Performance IQ portion of the WAIS-R
Apart from the studies of psychotic major and on tests of executive function (Borkowska
depression in the context of hypercortisolemia, & Rybakowski, 2001). Specifically, the BD group
there are also a subset of studies comparing performed worse on the Stroop test (word read-
melancholic and nonmelancholic depression ing and color–word interference parts), the
(Cornell et al., 1984). One study demonstrated letter fluency test, and the WCST (more perse-
difficulty in digit symbol and perseverative verative errors and fewer completed categories).
errors on the WCST on a group of specifically However, contradicting evidence also exists,
defined patients with melancholic depression, indicating no difference between MDD and
and not on the nonmelancholic group (Austin BD patients, at least in depressed states. Fossati
et al., 1999). A more recent study comparing 11 et al. (2004) investigated memory performance
melancholic with 11 nonmelancholic depressed on a verbal learning task in patients with a first
patients matched on HDRS symptoms, age, age depressive episode, MDD, and BD in depressed
of onset, and education reported poorer perfor- states. The researchers found poorer first trial
mance in intra/extradimensional set-shifting free recall in both MDD and BD groups but not
from the CANTAB battery, but not in paired in the first-episode group and concluded that
associates learning or stockings of Cambridge the repetition of depressive episodes affect ver-
(Michopoulos et al., 2006). A smaller study of bal memory performance in acute depressive
seven melancholic, eight nonmelancholic, and phases regardless of the subtype of depression.
nine control participants demonstrated slow- Similarly, Bearden et al. (2006) did not find
ing on several attention, executive function- any difference in verbal memory performance
ing, and processing speed tasks in the patients between the MDD and BD groups although
with melancholic depression but not in the both groups performed worse than the normal
nonmelancholic group (Rogers et al., 2004). It control group. The pattern of cognitive decre-
has been hypothesized that frontal dysfunction ments in BD patients has also been compared to
is greater in melancholic depression (Austin that of patients with schizophrenia and found
et al., 1999), but no imaging studies to date have to have similarities, although in less severity
tested this hypothesis. Recent neuroimaging (Schretlen et al., 2007).
studies focusing on anhedonia more specifi- The clinical state or phase of illness in BD
cally have reported decreased ventral striatal/ appears to affect the neuropsychological func-
nucleus accumbens response to positive stim- tion with mixed/manic states showing the
uli in depression (Epstein et al., 2006; Keedwell greatest decrements and euthymic states dis-
et al., 2005; Knutson et al., 2008). playing the least. Sweeney et al. (2000) exam-
ined 35 BD patients (14 in mixed or manic state
and 21 in depressed state), 59 MDD patients,
Bipolar
and 51 healthy controls on the CANTAB.
Individuals with bipolar disorder (BD) often Decrements in executive function, episodic
demonstrate worse performance than healthy memory, and spatial span were demonstrated
Neuropsychology of Depression and Related Mood Disorders 541

in BD patients in mixed/manic states but only Zubieta et al., 2001). However, recently Torrent
episodic memory decrement was shown in BD et al. (2006) examined 38 individuals with BD I
patients in depressed states and patients with in euthymic states, 33 individuals with bipolar II
MDD. A recent meta-analysis of BD individu- in euthymic states, and 35 healthy controls. The
als in euthymic states (Robinson et al., 2006) two BD groups showed worse performance on
revealed greater decrements on measures of tasks of attention and working memory, execu-
executive function and memory compared to tive function, and verbal memory compared to
measures of attention and processing speed. the control group, but the type II group was less
Specifically, large effect sizes were found for impaired on tasks of verbal memory and exec-
category fluency, backward digit span, and total utive function. Future studies need to carefully
learning score on the RAVLT and the CVLT; consider subtype of bipolar disorder, as well as
medium effect sizes were found for Stroop a consideration of mediating factors such as
Color–Word Inference Test, Trail-Making Test phase of illness and history of substance abuse
B, WCST (perseverative errors and catego- and suicide attempts.
ries), and short- and long-delay free recall of
the memory tests; and a small effect size was
Medication Effects
found for letter fluency and forward digit span.
In a recent review, Robinson and Ferrier (2006) The understanding of medication effects in
indicated that factors influencing the severity mood disorders poses several challenges in
of neuropsychological inefficiencies in euthy- the assessment and interpretation of cognitive
mic bipolar patients include the number of and affective data above and beyond the effects
previous manic episodes, hospitalizations, and of the mood disorder itself. As there is consid-
length of illness. A consistent fi nding was the erable variability on cognitive functioning in
negative relation between the number of manic mood disorders, and considerable severity of
episodes and performance on tasks of verbal illness in these disorders, understanding any
memory and executive function. Specifically, affects of medication becomes quite complex.
regarding memory function, although encod- For example, those with more severe illness may
ing is more commonly impaired than retention be more likely to receive medications that will
in euthymic BD individuals, the increasing affect cognitive functioning, and perhaps even
number of manic episodes was associated with higher doses of these medications than those
poorer retention. A recent study by Malhi with less severe illness. As medications are not
et al. (2007) followed 25 patients with BD over randomly assigned to patients, there is a poten-
30 months and assessed them in hypomanic, tial for medication by severity interactions that
depressed, and euthymic phases of illness. may confound interpretation of either one of
Decrements in executive functioning, memory, these alone. As such, the clinician should bal-
and attention were seen in both hypomanic ance knowledge of the potential cognitive side
and depressed states with additional fine motor effects of medication with those that could be a
deficit in the depressed state. In the euthymic result of the disorder itself. In some cases, it may
phase, mild attention and memory decrements be advisable to use medication washouts to bet-
were found. Taken together, although euthy- ter determine medication as opposed to illness
mic BD patients show relatively less cognitive effects on cognition, while also being aware of
inefficiency, persisting decrements, albeit mild, greater potential for relapse and/or complica-
appear to be in components of executive func- tions in such instances.
tion and memory.
It is worth mentioning that the type of BD
Anticholinergics
also has an effect on cognitive function with
BD type I being associated with worse cog- Medications with anticholinergic effects are
nitive outcome than compared to BD type II. frequently prescribed for depression, particu-
Relatively few studies have specified the type of larly tricyclic antidepressants, though this class
BD and a fewer studies have examined the differ- also includes selective serotonin reuptake inhibi-
ences between the two types (Malhi et al., 2007; tors (SSRIs) such as Paroxetine and atypical
542 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

antidepressants such as Venlafaxine. Caution though measures of anxiety and depression did
is urged about use of drugs with anticholin- not differ between the two groups (Kudoh et al.,
ergic properties, especially in the elderly, due 2004). A comprehensive review of benzodiaz-
to potentially exaggerated side effects such epine effects is available, with expected effects
as confusion, and memory and concentra- of sedation and cognitive decrements in atten-
tion decrements (Sadavoy, 2004). Use of SSRIs tion, executive functioning, and memory to be
may result in subjective memory difficulties, at expected (Buffett-Jerrott & Stewart, 2002). The
least in those with significant psychopathology effects of benzodiazepines, by way of increasing
(Wadsworth et al., 2005). Particular attention to sleepiness, have been linked to thalamic glucose
dosing in older patients is recommended, with metabolism in nine healthy control subjects
some ranges based on ¼–½ of the general adult (Volkow et al., 1995). Further, tolerance to detri-
dose. We recently found that when careful dos- mental memory effects of benzodiazepines does
ing with drugs with anticholinergic properties not appear to occur with long-term use whereas
in elderly inpatients is followed, significant cog- some tolerance to psychomotor effects of diaz-
nitive side effects do not appear to be of major epam was present (Gorenstein et al., 1994).
concern (Harik et al., 2008). Indeed, by and This was demonstrated using 10-mg diazepam
large successful treatment may in fact increase administration on memory in 28 long-term
memory, psychomotor, and attentional func- diazepam users with anxiety. Thus, benzodiaz-
tioning (Brooks & Hoblyn, 2007). epines can affect attention, executive function-
ing, and memory, while increasing sedation,
perhaps further effecting cognitive functioning
Benzodiazepines
in those with mood disorders.
Patients with mood disorders, perhaps more
often those with significant anxiety or insom-
Opiates
nia, are frequently prescribed a benzodiazepine
in addition to a primary mood medication. The Given the high rate of mood disorders in indi-
impact of benzodiazepines on cognitive func- viduals with chronic pain or cancer, the evalua-
tioning is fairly well understood and is one of tion of opioid medications, which are commonly
the reasons why they are typically used at night used in these medical populations, on cognitive
or as needed. For example, on-drug driving functioning is necessary (Haythornthwaite
performance on a simulator in a double-blind, et al., 1998; Turk & Brody, 1992). The concern for
crossover study of 18 healthy volunteers was possible adverse effects of opioid use has been
significantly poorer than nondrug performance associated with the presence of opiate recep-
with both extended and immediate release tors in the neural structures that are involved in
alprazolam (Leufkens et al., 2007). There were attention, learning, and memory (Payne, 1990).
also effects of immediate release alprazolam The complexities of research in medical popula-
on divided attention reaction time at 1-, 2.5-, tions using opioids including varying types of
and 5-hours postadministration, for Stop RT opioid medication and doses, interactions with
on a stop signal task at the same intervals, and other medications, and participant variables
for delayed recall on a word-learning test at 1 make drawing any firm conclusions regard-
hour. Effects for extended release were present ing the effects of opioids on cognition difficult.
on cognitive tests at 1- and 2.5-hour intervals However, there is evidence that short-term use
compared to placebo. Lorazepam, and not of opioid medication in healthy individuals can
chlorpromazine, administration to 72 healthy affect psychomotor performance on tasks such
adults resulted in impaired free recall and as Digit Symbol Substitution Test, reaction time,
word-completion in an earlier study (Danion and finger tapping (Kerr et al., 1991; Zacny et al.,
et al., 1992). Furthermore, in a comparison of 1994). Some studies suggest that when individu-
328, 65–80 year olds, 57 of whom were chronic als with chronic pain and healthy individuals
benzodiazepine users, chronic benzodiaze- are compared on cognitive tasks after taking
pine use resulted in higher rates of postoper- opioids, the drug has a less deleterious effect
ative confusion (26% compared to 13%), even on cognition in individuals with chronic pain,
Neuropsychology of Depression and Related Mood Disorders 543

likely due to pain relief and/or the pain possibly improved with large clinical trials using demo-
counteracting the possible effects of opioids in graphic, clinical, and neuropsychological mea-
the CNS (Haythornthwaite et al., 1998; Lorenz sures to predict treatment response, followed
et al., 1997; Sjogren et al., 2000; Twycross, 1994; up by studies to initiate alternative treatment
Vainio et al., 1995). In patients who take opi- strategies for those with poor prognosis for
oid medications on a long-term basis, cognitive positive response to traditional treatments.
inefficiencies are suggested to occur during the Screening instruments for use in psychiatry
first few hours after a dose is given and dur- and family medicine clinics are available and
ing the first few days of use (for reviews, see can be readily integrated into clinical care set-
Chapman et al., 2002; Zacny, 1995). A recent tings (Gualtieri et al., 2006; Gur et al., 2001a,
study (Byas-Smith et al., 2005) evaluated driv- 2001b; Langenecker et al., 2007a).
ing ability in a predetermined route in a com- A significant limitation of this section is that
munity; performance on the Test of Variables of most studies of treatment response predictors
Attention and on the Digit Symbol Substitution are much too small in sample size to rule out pre-
Test in 21 patients with chronic pain with stable dictors as being irrelevant in estimating poten-
regimens of opioid analgesics, 11 patients with tial for treatment response. This would suggest,
chronic pain without opioid use, and 50 healthy however, that any convergence across these small
controls were evaluated. No significant differ- studies is worthy of further and more detailed
ence in driving ability, sustained attention, or consideration and study. A study of 53 inpatients
psychomotor speed were found, suggesting that with depression who were treated with com-
at least some individuals with chronic pain who bined pharmacological and psychotherapeutic
regularly use opioids do not display significantly treatments used cognitive styles (e.g., dysfunc-
diminished cognitive abilities. tional attitude and extreme thinking) to predict
treatment response at 6 months, noting that
less changes in both measures predicted a more
Predicting Treatment Response
rapid return of depressive symptoms (Beevers
in Mood Disorders
et al., 2007). In a relatively larger study of 55
There is an emerging corpus of data suggesting depressed patients, better verbal fluency, and
that neuropsychological and cognitive/affective poorer Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test,
neuroscience techniques may be valuable in Benton Visual Retention Test, WAIS-R arith-
prospectively predicting treatment response in metic, Block Design, and Similarities, as well as
mood disorders. There is, however, an uneasy poorer Hooper Visual Organization Test per-
and nonproductive tension between insurance formance, were significant predictors of depres-
companies mindful of increasing health-care sion remission in HDRS symptoms to SSRIs at
costs, a relative paucity of treatment stud- 6 weeks of treatment (Kampf-Sherf et al., 2004).
ies examining alternative treatment predic- Another study of open label SSRI treatment in
tive models in mood disorders, and a general 35 patients with MDD demonstrated that bet-
unwillingness to integrate neuropsychological ter verbal fluency performance was a signifi-
techniques into psychiatric clinics. This is per- cant, positive prognostic indicator of eventual
haps a left-over of the remaining stigma toward treatment response (Taylor et al., 2006); this
mental illnesses, as opposed to the “hard” neu- study was supported by a smaller study of 14
rological syndromes where permanent, irre- middle-aged patients with depression (Dunkin
versible, and far more severe cognitive sequelae et al., 2000). In the study by Dunkin et al., an
are more frequently observed. It should not omnibus measure of executive functioning
be lost on the reader that perhaps one of the including verbal fluency was poorer in the non-
greatest areas for impact and improvement in responders (n = 6) compared to the responders
health care could be the timely and judicious (n = 8). Another study of 45 depressed elderly
application of neuropsychological evaluations patients failed to show any differences between
in psychiatry clinics. The current trial and responders and nonresponders on the Mattis
error, “cheapest or newest drug first” treat- Dementia Rating Scale (Butters et al., 2000),
ment model for mood disorders can likely be although this instrument may not be sensitive
544 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

enough to difficulties observed in mood disor- as retrospectively, valuable in predicting and


ders. A larger study of 112 elderly patients with understanding treatment response in mood
depression showed poorer Stroop interference disorders. Herein, the correlation between
and Initiation/Perseveration scores from the structural, functional, and behavioral abnor-
Dementia Rating Scale in the nonresponder malities in mood disorders is extended to some
group (n = 44) compared to the responders extent to include real outcome variables. Future
(Alexopoulos et al., 2005). studies can look at correlation of these different
Brain imaging studies, including morpho- dependent variables as they relate to changes
logic, EEG, PET, and fMRI have also dem- with treatment, in order to assess static (trait)
onstrated promise in predicting treatment versus phasic (state) abnormalities and decre-
response in mood disorders, most commonly ments in function. There is great hope that these
depression. For example, a study comparing measures can be used within clinical settings to
20 subjects with a lengthy depressive episode inform clinicians about prognosis for treatment
(greater than 2 years) demonstrated reduced response to standard pharmacotherapies, has-
verbal memory performance and decreased tening alternative treatments, and aiding in the
left temporal and hippocampal volume when development of novel treatments.
compared to 20 remitted and 20 control sub-
jects (Shah et al., 1998). In a larger PET study
Conclusions
of 39 outpatients with MDD, decreasing ventral
anterior cingulate and anterior insula activ- Neuropsychology, along with cognitive and
ity was associated with improvement in anx- affective neuroscience research, has taken part
iety and tension symptoms, increasing dorsal in a dramatic transformation in the apprecia-
anterior cingulate activity was associated with tion for, and understanding of, the neurobio-
improvement in psychomotor retardation, and logical systems affected in mood disorders. It
increasing dorsolateral prefrontal activation is increasingly accepted that mood disorders
was associated with improvement in cognitive frequently co-occur with cognitive difficul-
symptoms, all from the HDRS and Profi le of ties and further that such difficulties may be
Mood States (Brody et al., 2001). In our own reversible in some individuals, but not in oth-
study of 20 patients with MDD using parametric ers, based on certain characteristics outlined
Go/No-Go task during fMRI, activation during herein (e.g., age of onset, co-occurring medi-
correct rejections of No-Go stimuli at pretreat- cal conditions). Further, genetic and biolog-
ment in bilateral inferior frontal, right middle ical risks for developing mood disorders may
frontal, left amygdala and nucleus accumbens, co-occur with cognitive weaknesses that are
and subgenual anterior cingulate was highly evident long before evidence of an affective
predictive of eventual response to s-citalopram disorder emerges and are even present to some
in 15 completers (Langenecker et al., 2007b). An extent in relatives of those affected with mood
fMRI study using an emotion processing probe disorders.
also demonstrated that amygdala activation In the present chapter, we have demonstrated
was predictive of treatment response to cogni- a neuroanatomical network that is affected
tive behavioral therapy (Siegle et al., 2006). One in a majority of individuals with mood disor-
other PET and an EEG study demonstrated ders, including amygdala, hippocampus, basal
that subgenual and rostral anterior cingulate ganglia, thalamus, anterior temporal, insular,
activity, respectively, were positive predictors ventral, medial, and dorsal prefrontal areas.
of treatment response to SSRIs (Mayberg et al., It should not be lost on the reader that most
1997; Pizzagalli et al., 2004). of these areas are all part of a ventral cingu-
The neuropsychological and brain imaging late–medial prefrontal circuit, as described by
studies predicting treatment response strongly Cummings (1993) and further subdivided by
confirm several recurrent themes within this Rolls (Rolls, 1999) in lateral and medial orbital–
chapter. Disruptions in frontal and limbic acti- frontal circuits. The exception is dorsolateral
vation and in tasks thought to be dependent prefrontal cortex, which is a distinct dorsal
on these brain circuits are prospectively, as well anterior cingulate–dorsal prefrontal circuit that
Neuropsychology of Depression and Related Mood Disorders 545

does interact with the corticolimbic circuit. The Recommendations


cognitive and affective findings in mood disor-
We also wish to share with the reader the types
ders overlay nicely onto the idea of disruption of
of assessment strategies and instruments that
these three frontosubcortical circuits, including
will be most beneficial in different settings.
disruptions in emotion processing, attention,
Given the traditional and more recent findings
executive functioning, memory, and psychomo-
in mood disorders, at a minimum we recom-
tor speed. It is also not surprising that measures
mend that an assessment of depression-related
from these five cognitive/affective domains
cognitive functioning include the following:
are implicated in understanding mechanistic
issues in mood disorders, such as severity and
1. Estimate of baseline functioning less
prognosis for successful treatment response.
dependent on effort and memory retrieval.
Medication effects and comorbid medical con-
2. Direct or validated indirect measures of
ditions also play a critical role in understanding
effort, regardless of the possibility of primary or
the impact of mood disorders on neuropsy-
secondary gain.
chological functioning and there are certainly
3. Memory measures that rely on repeated
subtypes of mood disorders with greater dis-
exposure to the same stimuli such that a learn-
ease burden and chronicity. In essence, neuro-
ing curve can be established.
psychological probes can be used to understand
4. Measures of executive functioning that
disease burden and potential for response to
tap into processing speed, short-term memory,
traditional treatments. More importantly, these
sustained attention, and problem-solving areas.
indexes of disease burden can be ascertained at
5. Fine motor dexterity and speed.
first episode, thus shifting the treatment plan
6. Emotion perception and processing.
dependent on risk of recurrence of mood dis-
7. Objective measures of mood and
orders or prognosis of poor response to “front
psychopathology.
line” treatments.
Neuropsychologists should appreciate that
Assessments with younger adults should also
the distinction between “organic” and “func-
be attentive to the higher incidence of mood
tional” disorders is increasingly becoming
disorders in those with learning and attention
minimized as our understanding of neurobio-
deficit disorders, and will likely benefit from
logical mechanisms of brain function improve.
judicious use of achievement tests. Evaluations
A challenge remains in distinguishing poor
with older adults may benefit from using remote
effort, decreased goal-directed behavior,
memory measures and cued recall techniques
decreased efficiency in planning, as secondary
that can help distinguish between mood and
to mood disorders, from the same observed
amnestic disorders (Dierckx et al., 2007).
phenomena in legal or compensation contexts
Finally, the neuropsychologist should be leery
where primary and secondary gain are highly
of profi les associated with any given psychiat-
significant factors. There will be greater expec-
ric condition, as at best there is overlap between
tation that neuropsychologists appreciate the
many different psychiatric conditions, with no
impact of psychiatric and mood symptoms
validated stable and unique elements for any
in cognitive functioning. Neuropsychologists
given mood disorder.
can function effectively as consultants in
psychiatry and even in primary care clinics,
wherein the goal is to understand illness bur- Acknowledgments
den and prognosis for treatment success, such
that alternative and more effective treatments We acknowledge the National Alliance for
can be more efficiently and rapidly applied. Research on Schizophrenia and Depression
Finally, neuropsychologists should continue (SAL) and NIH K-12 Clinical Research Career
to be mindful of the relevance of, and need Development Award (SAL). We thank Jon-Kar
for, applied knowledge in clinical, legal, and Zubieta, MD, PhD and Sara L. Wright, PhD
research settings wherein mood disorders will for their review of this chapter and helpful
continue to be prevalent. comments.
546 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

References implications for functional neuropathology.


British Journal of Psychiatry, 178, 200–206.
Depression in Late Life; NIH Consensus Development Austin, M. P., Ross., M., Murray, C., O’Carroll, R.E.,
Conference on the Diagnosis and Treatment of Ebmeier, K.P., Goodwin, G.M. (1992). Cognitive
Depression in Late Life. (1992). National Institutes dysfunction in major depression. Journal of
of Mental Health. Affective Disorders, 25, 21–29.
Ahles, T. A., Khan, S. A., Yunus, M. B., & Spiegel, Austin, M.-P., Wilhelm, K., Parker, G., Hickie, I.,
D. A. (1991). Psychiatric status of patients with Brodaty, H., Chan, J. et al. (1999). Cognitive func-
primary fibromyalgia, patients with rheumatoid tion in depression: A distinct pattern of fron-
arthritis, and subjects without pain: A blind com- tal impairment in melancholia? Psychological
parison of DSM-III diagnoses. American Journal Medicine, 29, 73–85.
of Psychiatry, 148(12), 1721–1726. Barch, D. M., Sheline, Y. I., Csernansky, J. G., &
Alexopoulos, G. S. (2002). Frontostriatal and lim- Snyder, A. Z. (2003). Working memory and pre-
bic dysfunction in late-life depression. American frontal cortex dysfunction: Specificity to schizo-
Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 10, 687–695. phrenia compared with major depression.
Alexopoulos, G. S., Kiosses, D. N., Heo, M., Murphy, Biological Psychiatry, 53, 376–384.
C. F., Shanmugham, B., & Gunning-Dixon, F. Basso, M. R., & Bornstein, R. A. (1999). Relative
(2005). Executive dysfunction and the course of memory deficits in recurrent versus first-episode
geriatric depression. Biological Psychiatry, 58, major depression on a word-list learning task.
204–210. Neuropsychology, 13(4), 557–563.
Alexopoulos, G. S., Meyers, B. S., Young, R. C., Baune, B. T., Suslow, T., Engelien, A., Arolt, V., &
Campbell, S., Silbersweig, D., & Charlson, M. Berger, K. (2006). The association between depres-
(1997). “Vascular depression” hypothesis. Archives sive mood and cognitive performance in an elderly
of General Psychiatry, 54, 915–922. general population—the MEMO Study. Dementia
Anand, A., Li, Y., Wang, Y., Wu, J. W., Gao, S. J., Geriatrics and Cognitive Disorders, 22, 142–149.
Bukhari, L., et al. (2005). Activity and connectiv- Bearden, C. E., Glahn, D. C., Monkul, E. S., Barrett,
ity of brain mood regulating circuit in depression: J., Najt, P., Villarreal, V., et al. (2006). Patterns of
A functional magnetic resonance study. Biological memory impairment in bipolar disorder and uni-
Psychiatry, 57, 1079–1088. polar major depression. Psychiatry Research, 142,
Anand, A., & Shekhar, A. (2003). Brain imaging 139–150.
studies in mood and anxiety disorders—Special Beats, B. C., Sahakian, B. J., & Levy, R. (1996).
emphasis on the amygdala. Amygdala in Brain Cognitive performance in tests sensitive to fron-
Function: Bacic and Clinical Approaches, 985, tal lobe dysfunction in the elderly depressed.
370–388. Psychological Medicine, 26(3), 591–603.
Anderson, R. J., Freedland, K. E., Clouse, R. E., & Beevers, C. G., Wells, T. T., & Miller, I. W. (2007).
Lustman, P. J. (2001). The prevalence of comorbid Predicting response to depression treatment: The
depression in adults with diabetes: A meta-analy- role of negative cognition. Journal of Consulting
sis. Diabetes Care, 24(6), 1069–1078. and Clinical Psychology, 75, 422–431.
Argyelan, M., Szabo, Z., Kanyo, B., Tanacs, A., Bench, C. J., Friston, K. J., Brown, R. G., Frackowiak,
Kovacs, Z., Janka, Z., et al. (2005). Dopamine R. S., & Dolan, R. J. (1993). Regional cerebral
transporter availability in medication free and blood flow in depression measured by positron
in bupropion treated depression: A 99mTc- emission tomography: The relationship with
TRODAT-1 SPECT study. Journal of Affective clinical dimensions. Psychological Medicine, 23,
Disorders, 89, 115–123. 579–590.
Ashendorf, L., Constantinou, M., & McCaff rey, R. J. Bhalla, R. K., Butters, M. A., Mulsant, B. H., Begley,
(2004). The effect of depression and anxiety A. E., Zmuda, M. D., Schoderbek, B., et al. (2006).
on the TOMM in community-dwelling older Persistence of neuropsychologic deficits in the
adults. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19, remitted state of late-life depression. American
125–130. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 14, 419–427.
Audenaert, K., Goethals, I., Van, L. K., Lahorte, P., Bieliauskas, L. A. (1993). Depressed or not depressed?
Brans, B., Versijpt, J., et al. (2002). SPECT neu- That is the question. Journal of Clinical and
ropsychological activation procedure with the Experimental Neuropsychology, 15(1), 119–134.
Verbal Fluency Test in attempted suicide patients. Boland, R., Stein, D. J., Kupfer, D. J., & Schatzberg,
Nuclear Medicine Communications, 23, 907–916. A. F. (2006). Depression in Medical Illness
Austin, M.-P., Mitchell, P., & Goodwin, G. M. (Secondary Depression). In D.J. Stein, D>J. Kupfer,
(2001). Cognitive deficits in depression: Possible & A.F. Schatzberg (Eds.) The American Psychiatric
Neuropsychology of Depression and Related Mood Disorders 547

Publishing Textbook of Mood Disorders. (pp. 639– brain metabolic changes in patients with major
652). Washington DC: US: American Psychiatric depression treated with either paroxetine or inter-
Publishing, Inc. personal therapy: Preliminary findings. Archives of
Borkowska, A., & Rybakowski, J. K. (2001). General Psychiatry: Special Issue, 58, 631–640.
Neuropsychological frontal lobe tests indicate that Brooks, J. O., & Hoblyn, J. C. (2007). Neurocognitive
bipolar depressed patients are more impaired than costs and benefits of psychotropic medications in
unipolar. Bipolar Disorders, 3, 88–94. older adults. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and
Bornstein, R. A., Baker, G. B., & Douglass, A. B. Neurology, 20, 199–214.
(1991). Depression and memory in major depres- Brown, R. G., Scott, L., Bench, C. & Dolan, R.J. (1994).
sive disorder. Journal of Neuropsychiatry & Clinical Cognitive function in depression: Its relationship
Neurosciences, 3, 78–80. to the presence and severity of intellectual decline.
Borson, S., McDonald, G. J., Gayle, T., Deffebach, Psychological Medicine, 24, 829–847.
M., Lakshminarayan, S., & VanTuinen, C. (1992). Buffett-Jerrott, S. E., & Stewart, S. H. (2002).
Improvement in mood, physical symptoms, and Cognitive and sedative effects of benzodiazepine
function with nortriptyline for depression in use. Current Pharmaceutical Design, 8, 45–58.
patients with chronic obstructive pulmonary dis- Bulmash, E. L., Moller, H. J., Kayumov, L., Shen, J.,
ease. Psychosomatics, 33(2), 190–201. Wang, X., & Shapiro, C. M. (2006). Psychomotor
Bos, E. H., Bouhuys, A. L., Geerts, E., Van Os, T. W., disturbance in depression: Assessment using a
Van, d. S. I., Brouwer, W. H., et al. (2005). Cognitive, driving simulator paradigm. Journal of Affective
physiological, and personality correlates of recur- Disorders, 93, 213–218.
rence of depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, Burt, D. B., Zembar, M. J., & Niederehe, G. (1995).
87, 221–229. Depression and memory impairment: A meta-
Botteron, K., Raichle, M., Drevets, W., Heath, A., & analysis of the association, its pattern, and speci-
Todd, R. (2002). Volumetric reduction in the left ficity. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 285–305.
subgenual prefrontal cortex in early onset depres- Butters, M. A., Becker, J. T., Nebes, R. D., Zmuda,
sion. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 342–344. M. D., Mulsant, B. H., Pollock, B. G., et al. (2000).
Bouhuys, A. L., Geerts, E., Mersch, P. P. A., & Jenner, Changes in cognitive functioning following treat-
J. A. (1996). Nonverbal interpersonal sensitivity ment of late-life depression. American Journal of
and persistence of depression: Perception of emo- Psychiatry, 157, 1949–1954.
tions in schematic faces. Psychiatry Research, 64, Byas-Smith, M. G., Chapman, S.L., Reed, B., &
193–203. Cotsonis, G. (2005). The effect of opioids on driv-
Bouhuys, A. L., Geerts, E., & Gordijn, M. C. M. ing and psychomotor performance in patients
(1999). Depressed patients’ perceptions of facial with chronic pain. Clinical Journal of Pain, 21(4),
emotions in depressed and remitted states are 345–352.
associated with relapse: A longitudinal study. Caetano, S. C., Hatch, J. P., Brambilla, P., Sassi, R.
Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease, 187, B., Nicoletti, M., Mallinger, A. G., et al. (2004).
595–602. Anatomical MRI study of hippocampus and amyg-
Bowen, M., & Littell, C. (1997). Discriminating adult dala in patients with current and remitted major
normals, patients, and claimants with a pediatric depression. Psychiatry Research-Neuroimaging,
test: A brief report. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 11, 132, 141–147.
433–435. Caetano, S. C., Kaur, S., Brambilla, P., Nicoletti, M.,
Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., Millar, N., & White, J. (1995). Hatch, J. P., Sassi, R. B., et al. (2006). Smaller cin-
Selective processing of negative information: gulate volumes in unipolar depressed patients.
Effects of clinical anxiety, concurrent depression, Biological Psychiatry, 59, 702–706.
and awareness. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Caetano, S. C., Sassi, R., Brambilla, P., Harenski, K.,
104, 532–536. Nicoletti, M., Mallinger, A. G., et al. (2001). MRI
Brand, A. N., Jolles, J., & Gispen-de Wied, C. (1992). study of thalamic volumes in bipolar and unipo-
Recall and recognition memory deficits in depres- lar patients and healthy individuals. Psychiatry
sion. Journal of Affective Disorders, 25, 77–86. Research, 108, 161–168.
Brody, A., Saxena, S., Silverman, D., Alborzian, S., Caine, E.D., (1981). Pseudodementia. Current con-
Fairbanks, L., Phelps, M., et al.. (1999). Brain met- cepts and future directions. Archives of General
abolic changes in major depressive disorder from Psychiatry, 38, 1359–1364.
pre- to post-treatment with paroxetine. Psychiatric Caine, E. D., Yerevanian, B. I., & Bamford, K. A.
Research, 91, 127–139. (1984). Cognitive function and the dexameth-
Brody, A. L., Saxena, S., Stoessel, P., Gillies, L. A., asone suppression test in depression. American
Fairbanks, L. A., Alborzian, S., et al. (2001). Regional Journal of Psychiatry, 141, 116–118.
548 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Caine, E.D., (1986). The Neuropsychology depres- Coryell, W., Nopoulos, P., Drevets, W., Wilson, T.,
sion The pseudodementia syndrome In I. Grant & Andreasen, N. C. (2005). Subgenual prefron-
and K. M. Adams (Eds.), Neuropsychological tal cortex volumes in major depressive disorder
assessment of neuropsychiatric disorders (1st ed., and schizophrenia: Diagnostic specificity and
pp. 221–243). New York: Oxford. prognostic implications. American Journal of
Campbell, S., & MacQueen, G. (2004). The role of Psychiatry, 162, 1706–1712.
the hippocampus in the pathophysiology of major Covinsky, K. E., Kahana, E., Chin, M. H., Palmer,
depression. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience, R. M., Fortinsky, R. H., & Landefeld, C. S. (1999).
29, 417–426. Depressive symptoms and 3-year mortality in
Canli, T., Sivers, H., Thomason, M. E., Whitfield- older hospitalized medical patients. Annals of
Gabrieli, S., Gabrieli, J. D. E., & Gotlib, I. H. Internal Medicine, 130(7), 563–569.
(2004). Brain activation to emotional words in Cummings, J. L. (1993). The neuroanatomy of depres-
depressed vs healthy subjects. Neuroreport, 15, sion. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 54, 14–20.
2585–2588. Cummings, J. L. (1995). Anatomic and behavioral
Carroll, B. J., Curtis, G. C., & Mendels, J. (1976). aspects of fronto-subcortical circuits. Annals of
Neuroendocrine regulation in depression I. Limbic the New York Academy Sciences, 769, 1–13.
system-adrenocortical dysfunction. Archives of Cummings, J.L., & Benson, D.F. (1984). Subcortical
General Psychiatry, 33, 1039–1044. dementia. Review of an emerging concept. Archives
Channon, S. (1996). Executive dysfunction in depres- fof Neurology, 41, 874–879.
sion: The Wisconsin card Sorting Test. Journal of Danion, J. M., Kauff mann-Muller, F., & Grange, D.
Affective Disorders, 39, 107–114. (1995). Affective valence of words, explicit and
Chapman, S. L., Byas-Smith, M. G., & Reed, B. A. implicit memory in clinical depression. Journal of
(2002). Effects of intermediate- and long-term use Affective Disorders, 34, 227–234.
of opioids on cognition in patients with chronic Danion, J. M., Kauff mann-Muller, F., & Grange, D.
pain. Clinical Journal of Pain, 18(4), S83–S90. (1995). Affective valence of words, explicit and
Chochinov, H. H. M. (2001). Depression in cancer implicit memory in clinical depression. Journal of
patients. The Lancet Oncology, 2(8), 499–505. Affective Disorders, 34, 227–234.
Chorbov, V. M., Lobos, E. A., Todorov, A. A., Heath, Danion, J. M., Peretti, S., Grange, D., Bilik, M., Imbs,
A. C., Botteron, K. N., & Todd, R. D. (2007). J. L., & Singer, L. (1992). Effects of chlorpromazine
Relationship of 5-HTTLPR genotypes and depres- and lorazepam on explicit memory, repetition
sion risk in the presence of trauma in a female twin priming and cognitive skill learning in healthy
sample. American Journal of Medical Genetics-B volunteers. Psychopharmacology (Berlin), 108,
Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 144B, 830–833. 345–351.
Ciechanowski, P. S., Katon, W. J., Russo, J. E., & Danion, J. M., Willard-Schroeder, D., Zimmermann,
Hirsch, I. B. (2003). The relationship of depres- M. A., Grange, D., Schlienger, J. L., & Singer, L.
sive symptoms to symptom reporting, self-care (1991). Explicit memory and repetition priming
and glucose control in diabetes. General Hospital in depression, preliminary findings. Archives of
Psychiatry, 25(4), 246–252. General Psychiatry, 48, 707–711.
Coffey, C., Wilkinson, W., Weiner, R., Parashos, I., & Davidson, R., Abercrombie, H., Nitschke, J., &
Djang, W. (1993). Quantitative cerebral anatomy Putnam, K. (1999). Regional brain function, emo-
in depression. A controlled magnetic resonance tion and disorders of emotion. Current Opinion in
study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 7–16. Neurobiology, 9, 228–234.
Cohen, R. M., Weingartner, H., Smallberg, S. A., Davidson, R., Pizzigalli, D., Nitschke, J., & Putnam,
Pickar, D., & Murphy, D. L. (1982). Effort and F. (2002). Depression: Perspectives from affective
cognition in depression. Archives of General neuroscience. Annual Review of Psychology, 53,
Psychiatry, 39, 593–597. 545–574.
Cornblatt, B. A., Lenzenweger, M. F., & Erlenmeyer- Davidson, R. J., Irwin, W., Anderle, M. J., & Kalin,
Kimling, L. (1989). The continuous performance N. H. (2003). The neural substrates of affective
test, identical pairs version: II. Contrasting atten- processing in depressed patients treated with
tional profi les in schizophrenic and depressed venlafaxine. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160,
patients. Psychiatric Research, 29, 65–85. 64–75.
Cornell, D. G., Saurex, R., & Berent, S. (1984). Delis, D. C., Squire, L.R., Bihrley, A., & Massman, P.
Psychomotor retardation in melancholic and non- (1992). Componential analysis of problem-solving
melancholic depression: Cognitive and motor ability: Performance of patients with frontal lobe
components. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 932, damage and amnesic patients on a new sorting
150–157. test. Neuropsychologia, 30, 683–697.
Neuropsychology of Depression and Related Mood Disorders 549

Desmond, D. W., Remien, R. H., Moroney, J. T., Stern, Eisner, M. D., Katz, P. P., Lactao, G., & Iribarren, C.
Y., Sano, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (2003). Ischemic (2005). Impact of depressive symptoms on adult
stroke and depression. Journal of the International asthma outcomes. Annals of Allergy, Asthma and
Neuropsychological Society, 9, 429–439. Immunology, 94, 566–574.
Dierckx, E., Engelborghs, S., De, R. R., De Deyn, P. P., Elderkin-Thompson, V., Mintz, J., Haroon, E.,
& Ponjaert-Kristoffersen, I. (2007). Differentiation Lavretsky, H., & Kumar, A. (2007). Executive
between mild cognitive impairment, Alzheimer’s dysfunction and memory in older patients with
disease and depression by means of cued recall. major and minor depression. Archives of Clinical
Psychological Medicine, 37, 747–755. Neuropsychology, 22, 261–270.
Dimatteo, M. R., Lepper, H. S., & Croghan, T. W. Elkin, I., Gibbons, R. D., Shea, M. T., Sotsky, S. M.,
(2000). Depression is a risk factor for noncompli- Watkins, J. T., Pilkonis, P. A., et al. (1995). Initial
ance with medical treatment: Meta-analysis of the severity and differential treatment outcome in the
effects of anxiety and depression on patient adher- National Institute of Mental Health Treatment
ence. Archives of Internal Medicine, 160, 2101–2107. of Depression Collaborative Research Program.
Dolan, R. J., Bench, C. J., Brown, R. G., Scott, L. C., Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 63,
& Frackowiak, R. S. (1994). Neuropsychological 841–847.
dysfunction in depression: The relationship Elliott, R. (1998). The neuropsychological profi le in
to regional cerebral blood flow. Psychological unipolar depression. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
Medicine, 24, 849–857. 2(11), 447–454.
Dorn, L. D., Burgess, E. S., Friedman, T. C., Dubbert, Elliott, R., Baker, S. C., Rogers, R. D., O’Leary, D. A.,
B., Gold, P. W., & Chrousos, G. P. (1997). The lon- Paykel, E. S., Frith, C. D., et al. (1997). Prefrontal
gitudinal course of psychopathology in Cushing’s dysfunction in depressed patients performing a
syndrome after correction of hypercortisolism. complex planning task: A study using positron
Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, emission tomography. Psychological Medicine, 27,
82, 912–919. 931–942.
Dorn, L. D. & Cerrone, P. (2000). Cognitive function Elliott, R., Sahakian, B. J., McKay, A. P., Herrod,
in patients with Cushing’s syndrome: A longitu- J. J., Robbins, T. W., & Paykel, E. S. (1996).
dinal perspective. Clinical Nursing Research, 9, Neuropsychological impairments in unipolar
420–440. depression: The influence of perceived failure on
Drevets, W. C., Price, J., & Simpson, J. (1997). subsequent performance. Psychological Medicine
Subgenual prefrontal cortex abnormalities in 26, 975–989.
mood disorders. Nature, 386, 824–827. Epstein, J., Pan, H., Kocsis, J. H., Yang, Y., Butler,
Drevets, W. C., & Raichle, M. E. (1992). T., Chusid, J., et al. (2006). Lack of ventral striatal
Neuroanatomical circuits in depression: response to positive stimuli in depressed versus
Implications for treatment mechanisms. normal subjects. American Journal of Psychiatry,
Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 28, 261–274. 163, 1784–1790.
Dunkin, J. J., Leuchter, A. F., Cook, I. A., Kasl-Godley, Farrin, L., Hull, L., Unwin, C., Wykes, T., & David,
J. E., Abrams, M., & Rosenberg-Thompson, S. A. (2003). Effects of depressed mood on objective
(2000). Executive dysfunction predicts nonre- and subjective measures of attention. The Journal
sponse to fluoxetine in major depression. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences,
of Affective Disorders, 60, 16–23. 15(1), 98–104.
Eastwood, M. R., Rifat, S. L., Nobbs, H., & Ruderman, Ferrier, I. N., Stanton, B. R., Kelly, T. P., & Scott, J.
J. (1989). Mood disorder following cerebrovas- (1999). Neuropsychological function in euthymic
cular accident. British Journal of Psychiatry, 154, patients with bipolar disorder. British Journal of
195–200. Psychiatry, 175, 246–251.
Egede, L. E. (2007). Major depression in individuals Forget, H., Lacroix, A., & Cohen, H. (2002). Persistent
with chronic medical disorders: Prevalence, cor- cognitive impairment following surgical treatment
relates, and association with health resource utili- of Cushing’s syndrome. Psychoneuroendocrinology,
zation, lost productivity, and functional disability. 27, 367–383.
General Hospital Psychiatry, 29, 409–416. Fossati, P., Coyette, F., Ergis, A. M., & Allilaire, J. F.
Egeland, J., Lund, A., Landro, N. I., Rund, B. R., (2002). Influence of age and executive functioning
Sundet, K., Asbjornsen, A., et al.. (2005). Cortisol on verbal memory of inpatients with depression.
level predicts executive and memory function Journal of Affective Disorders, 68, 261–271.
in depression, symptom level predicts psycho- Fossati, P., Ergis, A., & Allilaire, J. (2001). Problem-
motor speed. Acta Psychiatria Scandinavia, 112, solving abilities in unipolar depressed patients:
434–441. Comparison of performance on the modified
550 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

version of the Wisconsin and the California sort- developments. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 20,
ing tests. Psychiatry Research, 104, 145–156. 278–285.
Fossati, P., Harvey, P. -O., Bastard, G. L., Ergis, Gilboa-Schechtman, E., Erhard-Weiss, D., &
A. -M., Jouvent, R., & Allilaire, J. -F. O. (2004). Jeczemien, P. (2002). Interpersonal deficits meet
Verbal memory performance of patients with a cognitive biases: Memory for facial expressions
first depressive episode and patients with unipo- in depressed and anxious men and women.
lar and bipolar recurrent depression. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 113, 279–293.
Psychiatric Research, 38(2), 137–144. Goldapple, K., Segal, Z., Garson, C., Lau, M.,
Fossati, P., Hevenor, S., Graham, S. J., Grady, C., Bieling, P., Kennedy, S., et al. (2004). Modulation
Keightley, M. L., Craik, F., et al. (2003). In search of cortical–limbic pathways in major depres-
of the emotional self: An fMRI study using pos- sion—treatment-specific effects of cognitive
itive and negative emotional words. American behavior therapy. Archives of General Psychiatry,
Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 1938–1945. 61, 34–41.
Frasure-Smith, N., Lesperance, F., & Talajic, M. Golomb, J., deLeon, M. J., Kluger, A., George, A. E.,
(1993). Depression following myocardial infarc- Tarshish, C., & Ferris, S. H. (1993). Hippocampal
tion. Impact on 6-month survival. JAMA, 270(15), atrophy in normal aging an association with recent
1819–1825. memory impairment. Archives of Neurology, 50,
Frodl, T., Meisenzahl, E. M., Zetzsche, T., Born, C., 967–973.
Jager, M., Groll, C., et al. (2003). Larger amygdala Gomez, R. G., Fleming, S. H., Keller, J., Flores,
volumes in first depressive episode as compared B., Kenna, H., DeBattista, C., et al.. (2006). The
to recurrent major depression and healthy control neuropsychological profi le of psychotic major
subjects. Biological Psychiatry, 53, 338–344. depression and its relation to cortisol. Biological
Frodl, T., Schaub, A., Banac, S., Charypar, M., Jager, Psychiatry, 60, 472–478.
M., Kummler, P., et al. (2006). Reduced hippocam- Gorenstein, C., Bernik, M. A., & Pompeia, S. (1994).
pal volume correlates with executive dysfunction- Differential acute psychomotor and cognitive
ing in major depression. Journal of Psychiatry & effects of diazepam on long-term benzodiazepine
Neuroscience, 31, 316–323. users. International Clinical Psychopharmacology,
Fu, C. H. Y., Williams, S. C. R., Cleare, A. J., 9, 145–153.
Brammer, M. J., Walsh, N. D., Kim, J., et al. (2004). Gorno-Tempini, M., Pradelli, S., Serafini, M.,
Attenuation of the neural response to sad faces in Pagnoni, G., Baraldi, P., Porro, C., et al. (2001).
major depression by antidepressant treatment—A Explicit and incidental facial expression process-
prospective, event-related functional magnetic ing: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 14, 465–473.
resonance imaging study. Archives of General Gotlib, I. H., Krasnoperova, E., Yue, D. N., &
Psychiatry, 61, 877–889. Joormann, J. (2004). Attentional biases for nega-
Ganzini, L., Smith, D. M., Fenn, D. S., & Lee, M. A. tive interpersonal stimuli in clinical depression.
(1997). Depression and mortality in medically ill Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 127–135.
older adults. Journal of the American Geriatrics Gotlib, I. H., Traill, S. K., Montoya, R. L., Joormann,
Society, 45(3), 307–312. J., & Chang, K. (2005). Attention and memory
George, M. S., Ketter, T., Gill, D., Haxby, J., Ungerleider, biases in the offspring of parents with bipolar dis-
L., Herscovitch, P., et al. (1993). Brain regions order: indications from a pilot study. Journal of
involved in recognizing facial emotion or identity: Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 84–93.
An oxygen-15 PET study. Journal of Neuropsychiatry Grant, M. M., Thase, M. E., & Sweeney, J. A. (2001).
and Clinical Neuroscience, 5, 384–394. Cognitive disturbance in outpatient depressed
George, M. S., Ketter, T. A., Parekh, P. I., Herscovitch, younger adults: Evidence of modest impairment.
P., & Post, R. M. (1996). Gender differences in Biological Psychiatry, 50, 35–43.
regional cerebral blood flow during transient Greicius, M. D., Flores, B. H., Menon, V., Glover,
self-induced sadness or happiness. Biological G. H., Solvason, H. B., Kenna, H., et al. (2007).
Psychiatry, 40, 859–871. Resting-state functional connectivity in major
George, M. S., Ketter, T. A., Parekh, P. I., Rosinsky, depression: Abnormally increased contributions
N., Ring, H., Casey, B. J., et al. (1994). Regional from subgenual cingulate cortex and thalamus.
brain activity when selecting a response despite Biological Psychiatry, 62, 429–437.
interference: An H215O PET study of the Stroop Gualtieri, C. T., Johnson, L. G., & Benedict, K. B.
and an emotional Stroop. Human Brain Mapping, (2006). Neurocognition in depression: Patients
1, 194–209. on and off medication versus healthy compari-
Ghaffar, O., & Feinstein, A. (2007). The neuropsy- son subjects 501. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and
chiatry of multiple sclerosis: A review of recent Clinical Neurosciences, 18, 217–225.
Neuropsychology of Depression and Related Mood Disorders 551

Gur, R. C., Edwin, R., Gur, R., Zwil, A., Heimberg, with successfully treated Cushing’s disease.
C., & Kraemer, H. (1992). Facial emotion dis- Clinical Endocrinology, 61, 458–465.
crimination: II Behavioral findings in depression. Henry, J. D., & Crawford, J. R. (2005). A meta-an-
Psychiatry Research, 42, 241–251. alytic review of verbal fluency deficits in depres-
Gur, R. C., Ragland, D., Moberg, P., Bilker, W., sion. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Kohler, C., Siegel, S., et al. (2001a). Computerized Neuropsychology, 27, 78–101.
neurocognitive scanning: II The profi le of schizo- Hickie, I., Scott, E., & Wilhelm, K. (1997). Subcortical
phrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology, 25, 777–788. hyperintensities on magnetic resonance imaging
Gur, R. C., Ragland, D., Moberg, P., Turner, T., Bilker, in patients with severe depression-longitudinal
W., Kohler, C., et al. (2001b). Computerized neu- evaluation. Biological Psychiatry, 42, 367–374.
rocognitive scanning: I Methodology and valida- Holmes, A. J., Macdonald, A., Carter, C. S., Barch, D.
tion in healthy people. Neuropsychopharmacology, M., Stenger, V. A., & Cohen, J. D. (2005). Prefrontal
25, 766–776. functioning during context processing in schizo-
Hammar, A., Lund, A., & Hugdahl, K. (2003). Selective phrenia and major depression: An event-related
impairment in effortful information processing fMRI study. Schizophrenia Research, 76, 199–206.
in major depression. Journal of the International Hook, J. N., Giordani, B., Schteingart, D. E., Guire,
Neuropsychological Society, 9, 954–959. K., Giles, J., Ryan, K., et al. (2007). Patterns of cog-
Harik, L., Pica, A., Wright, S., Lee, J., & Bieliauskas, nitive change over time and relationship to age fol-
L. (2008). Cognitive deficits associated with anti- lowing successful treatment of Cushing’s disease.
cholinergic therapy and drugs with anticholiner- Journal of the International Neuropsychological
gic properties in community dwelling veterans. Society, 3, 21–29.
Journal of the International Neuropsychological Hugdahl, K., Rund, B. R., Lund, A., Asbjornsen, A.,
Society, 14(Suppl S1), 106. Egeland, J., Ersland, L., et al. (2004). Brain activa-
Harlow, J. (1868). Recovery from the passage of an tion measured with fMRI during a mental arith-
iron bar through the head. Publications of the metic task in schizophrenia and major depression.
Massachussetts Medical Society, 2, 327–347. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 286–293.
Hartlage, S., Alloy, L. B., Vazquez, C., & Dykman, Hugdahl, K., Rund, B. R., Lund, A., Asbjornsen, A.,
B. (1993). Automatic and effortful processing Egeland, J., Landro, N. I., et al. (2003). Attentional
in depression. Psychological Bulletin, 113(2), and executive dysfunctions in schizophrenia and
247–278. depression: Evidence from dichotic listening per-
Harvey, P. O., Le Bastard, G., Poshon, J. B., Levy, R., formance. Biological Psychiatry, 53, 609–616.
Allilaire, J. F., Dubois, B., et al. (2004). Executive Iidaka, T., Omori, M., Murata, T., Kosaka, H.,
functions and updating of the contents of work- Yonekura, Y., Okada, T., et al. (2001). Neural
ing memory in unipolar depression. Journal of interactions of the amygdala with the prefron-
Psychiatric Research, 38, 567–576. tal and temporal cortices in the processing of
Harvey, P. O., Fossati, P., Pochon, J-B., Levy, R., Le facial expressions as revealed by fMRI. Journal of
Bastard, G., et al., (2005). Cognitive control and Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 1035–1047.
brain resources in major depression: An fMRI Johnson, S. K., Lange, G., DeLuca, J., Korn, L. R., &
study using the n-back task. Neuroimage, 26, Natelson, B. (1997). The effects of fatigue on neu-
860–869. ropsychological performance in patients with
Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1979). Automatic and effort- chronic fatigue syndrome, multiple sclerosis,
ful processes in memory. Journal of Experimental and depression. Applied Neuropsychology, 4(3),
Psychology: General, 108(3), 356–388. 145–153.
Hatfield, R., Langenecker, S. A., Smet, I., Maixner, D., & Jorm, A. F., Anstey, K. J., Christensen, H., de Plater,
Giordani, B. (2005). Memory functioning and white G., Kumar, R., Wen, W., et al. (2005). MRI hyper-
matter disease in Treatment Resistant Depression. intensities and depressive symptoms in a com-
Presented at the International Neuropsychological munity sample of individuals 60–64 years old.
Society meeting in St. Louis, MO. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 699–704.
Haythornthwaite, J. A., Menefee, L. A., Quatrano- Jorm, A. F., Butterworth, P., Anstey, K. J., Christensen,
Piacentini, A. L., & Pappagallo, M. (1998). H., Easteal, S., Maller, J., et al. (2004). Memory
Outcome of chronic opioid therapy for non-cancer complaints in a community sample aged 60–64
pain. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management, years: Associations with cognitive functioning,
15, 185–194. psychiatric symptoms, medical conditions, APOE
Heald, A. H., Ghosh, S., Bray, S., Gibson, C., genotype, hippocampus and amygdala volumes,
Anderson, S. G., Buckler, H., et al. (2004). Long- and white-matter hyperintensities. Psychological
term negative impact on quality of life in patients Medicine, 34, 1495–1506.
552 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Kalin, N., Davidson, R., Irwin, W., Warner, G., Orendi, valence and arousal. Proceedings of the National
J., Sutton, S., et al. (1997). Functional magnetic res- Academy of Sciences of the United States of
onance imaging studies of emotional processing in America, 101, 3310–3315.
normal and depressed patients: Effects of venlafax- Kerr, B. B., Hill, H. H., Coda, B. B., Calogero, M.
ine. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 58, 32–39. M., Chapman, C. C. R., Hunt, E. E., et al. (1991).
Kampf-Sherf, O., Zlotogorski, Z., Gilboa, A., Concentration-related effects of morphine on
Speedie, L., Lereya, J., Rosca, P., et al. (2004). cognition and motor control in human subjects.
Neuropsychological functioning in major depres- Neuropsychopharmacology, 5(3), 157–166.
sion and responsiveness to selective seretonin Ketter, T. A., Andreason, P. J., George, M. S., Lee, C.,
reuptake inhibitors antidepressants. Journal of & Gill, D. S. (1996). Anterior paralimbic mediation
Affective Disorders 82, 453–459. of procaine-induced emotional and psychosen-
Katon, W., Lin, E. H. B., & Kroenke, K. (2007). The sory experiences. Archives of General Psychiatry,
association of depression and anxiety with med- 53, 59–69.
ical symptom burden in patients with chronic King, D. A., & Caine, E. D. (1996). Cognitive impair-
medical illness. General Hospital Psychiatry, 29(2), ment and major depression. In I. Grant and K. M.
147–155. Adams (Eds.), Neuropsychological assessment of
Katon, W. J. (2003). Clinical and health services rela- neuropsychiatric disorders (2nd ed., pp. 200–217).
tionships between major depression, depressive New York: Oxford.
symptoms, and general medical illness. Society of King, D. A., Cox, C., Lyness, J. M., Conwell, Y., &
Biological Psychiatry, 54, 216–226. Caine, E. D. (1998). Quantitative and qualitative
Kaufman, A., & Kaufman, N. (1983). Kaufman differences in the verbal learning performance of
assessment battery for children; administration elderly depressives and healthy controls. Journal
and scoring manual. Circle Pines, MA: American of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4,
Guidance Service. 115–126.
Keedwell, P., Andrew C., Williams, S. C. R., Klein, D., Schatzberg, A.F., McCullough, J.P.,
Bramner, M. J., & Phillips, M. L. (2005). A double Dowling, F., Goodman, D. et al., (1999). Age of
dissociation of ventromedial prefrontal cortical onset in chronic major depression: relation to
responses to sad and happy stimuli in depressed demographic and clinical variables, family his-
and healthy individuals. Biological Psychiatry, tory, and treatment response . Journal of Affective
58, 495–503. Disorders , 55 , 149 - 157
Kegeles, L. S., Malone, K. M., Slifstein, M., Ellis, S. Knauper, B. Cannell, CF. Schwarz, N, Bruce, M.L.,
P., Xanthopoulos, E., Keilp, J. G., et al. (2003). Kessler, R. (1999). Improving the accuracy of major
Response of cortical metabolic deficits to sero- depression age of onset reports in the US National
tonergic challenge in familial mood disorders. Comorbidity Survey. International Journal of
American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 76–82. Methods in Psychiatric Research,8:39–48.
Kennedy, S. E., Koeppe, R. A., Young, E. A., & Knutson, B., Bhanji, J. P., Cooney, R. E., Atlas, L. Y.,
Zubieta, J. K. (2006). Dysregulation of endog- & Gotlib, I. H. (2008). Neural responses to mon-
enous opioid emotion regulation circuitry in etary incentives in major depression. Biological
major depression in women. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63, 686–692.
Psychiatry, 63, 1199–1208. Kolb, B., & Whishaw, I. (1996). Human neuropsychol-
Kennedy, S. E., & Zubieta, J. K. (2004). Neuroreceptor ogy. New York: W. H. Freeman and Company.
imaging of stress and mood disorders. CNS Krishnan, K. R., McDonald, W. M., Escalona, P. R.,
Spectrums, 9, 292–301. Doraiswamy, P. M., Na, C., Husain, M. M., et al.
Kennedy, S. H., Javanmard, M., & Vaccarino, F. J. (1992). Magnetic resonance imaging of the cau-
(1997). A review of functional neuroimaging in date nuclei in depression. Archives of General
mood disorders: Positron emission tomography Psychiatry, 49, 553–557.
and depression. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, Kudoh, A., Takase, H., Takahira, Y., & Takazawa, T.
42, 467–475. (2004). Postoperative confusion increases in elderly
Kennedy, S. H., Konarski, J. Z., Segal, Z. V., Lau, long-term benzodiazepine users. Anesthesia and
M. A., Bieling, P. J., McIntyre, R. S., et al. (2007). Analgesia, 99, 1674–1678.
Differences in brain glucose metabolism between Kumar, A., Bilker, W., Jin, Z., Udupa, J. (2000). Atrophy
responders to CBT and venlafaxine in a 16-week and high intensity lesions: complementary neurobi-
randomized controlled trial 6. American Journal ological mechanisms in late-life major depression.
of Psychiatry, 164, 778–788. Neuropsychopharmacology, 22, 264–274.
Kensinger, E. A., & Corkin S. (2004). Two routes to Kumari, V., Mitterscifft haler, M., Teasdale, J., Malhi,
emotional memory: Distinct neural processes for G., Brown, R., Giampietro, V., et al. (2003). Neural
Neuropsychology of Depression and Related Mood Disorders 553

abnormalities during cognitive generation of Lesperance, F., & Frasure-Smith, N. (1996). Negative
affect in treatment-resistant depression. Biological emotions and coronary heart disease: Getting to
Psychiatry, 54, 777–791. the heart of the matter. Lancet, 347, 414–415.
Lamberty, G. J., & Bieliauskas, L. A. (1993). Leufkens, T. R., Vermeeren, A., Smink, B. E., van,
Distinguishing between depression and demen- R. P., & Ramaekers, J. G. (2007). Cognitive, psy-
tia in the elderly: A review of neuropsychological chomotor and actual driving performance in
findings. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 8, healthy volunteers after immediate and extended
149–170. release formulations of alprazolam 1 mg.
Landro, N. I., Stiles, T. C., & Sletvold, H. (1997). Psychopharmacology (Berl), 191, 951–959.
Memory functioning in patients with primary Liotti, M., Mayberg, H. S., Brannan, S. K., McGinnis,
fibromyalgia and major depression and healthy S., Jerabek, P., & Fox, P. T. (2000). Differential lim-
controls. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 42(3), bic–cortical correlates of sadness and anxiety in
297–306. healthy subjects: Implications for affective disor-
Landro, N. I., Stiles, T. C., & Sletvold, H. (2001). ders. Biological Psychiatry, 48, 30–42.
Neurological function in nonpsychotic unipolar Lopez, J. F., Chalmers, D. T., Little, K. Y., & Watson,
major depression. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, S. J. (1998). Regulation of serotonin 1A, glucocor-
& Behavioral Neurology, 14(4), 233–240 ticoid, and mineralocorticoid receptor in rat and
Langenecker, S. A., Bieliauskas, L. A., Rapport, L. human hippocampus: Implications for the neu-
J., Zubieta, J. K., Wilde, E. A., & Berent, S. (2005). robiology of depression. Biological Psychiatry, 43,
Face emotion perception and executive function- 547–573.
ing deficits in depression. Journal of Clinical and Lorenz, J., Beck, H., & Bromm, B. (1997). Cognitive
Experimental Neuropsychology, 27, 320–333. performance, mood and experimental pain
Langenecker, S. A., Caveney, A. F., Young, E. A., before and during morphine-induced analgesia
Giordani, B., Nielson, K. A., Rapport, L. J., et al. in patients with chronic nonmalignant pain. Pain,
(2007a). The psychometric properties and sensitiv- 73, 369–375.
ity of a brief computer-based cognitive screening Maddock, R. J., Garrett, A. S., & Buonocore, M. H.
battery in a depression clinic. Psychiatric Research, (2003). Posterior cingulate cortex activation by
152, 143–154. emotional words: fMRI evidence from a valence
Langenecker, S. A., Kennedy, S. E., Guidotti, L., decision task. Human Brain Mapping, 18, 30–41.
Briceno, E., Own, L., Hooven, T., et al. (2007b). Malhi, G. S., Ivanovski, B., Hadzi-Pavlovic, D.,
Frontal and limbic activation during inhibitory Mitchell, P. B., Vieta, E., & Sachdev, P. (2007).
control predicts treatment response in Major Neuropsychological deficits and functional
Depressive Disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 53, impairment in bipolar depression, hypomania and
46–53. euthymia. Bipolar Disorders, 9, 114–125.
Larisch, R., Klimke, A., Vosberg, H., Loffler, S., Marcos, T., Portella, M. J., Navarro, V., Gasto, C., Rami,
Gaebel, W., & Muller-Gartner, H. W. (1997). In L., Lazaro, L., et al. (2005). Neuropsychological
vivo evidence for the involvement of dopamine-D2 prediction of recovery in late-onset major depres-
receptors in striatum and anterior cingulate gyrus sion. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry,
in major depression. NeuroImage, 5, 251–260. 20, 790–795.
Laska, A. C., Martensson, B., Kahan, T., von Arbin, Markela-Lerenc, J., Kaiser, S., Fiedler, P., Weisbrod,
M., & Murray, V. (2007). Recognition of depres- M., & Mundt, C. (2006). Stroop performance in
sion in aphasic stroke patients. Cerebrovascular depressive patients: A preliminary report. Journal
Diseases, 24, 74–79. of Affective Disorders, 94, 261–267.
Lee, A., Ogle, W., & Sapolsky, R. M. (2002). Stress Martinez-Aran, A., Vieta, E., Colom, F., Torrent, C.,
and depression: Possible links to neuron death in Sanchez-Moreno, J., Reinares, M., et al. (2004).
the hippocampus. Bipolar Disorders, 4, 117–128. Cognitive impairment in euthymic bipolar
Lee, G. P., Loring, D. W., & Martin, R. C. (1992). patients: Implications for clinical and functional
Rey’s 15-Item Visual Memory Test for the detec- outcome. Bipolar Disorders, 6(3), 224–232.
tion of malingering: Normative observations on Martinot, M. L. P., Bragulat, V., Artiges, E., Dolle,
patients with neurological disorders. Psychological F., Hinnen, F., Jouvent, R., et al. (2001). Decreased
Assessment, 4, 43–46. presynaptic dopamine function in the left caudate
Lee, S. -H., Payne, M. E., Steffens, D. C., McQuoid, of depressed patients with affective flattening and
D. R., Lai, T. -J., Provenzale, J. M., et al. (2003). psychomotor retardation. American Journal of
Subcortical lesion severity and orbitofrontal cor- Psychiatry, 158, 314–316.
tex volume in geriatric depression. Biological Matsuo, K., Kato, N., & Kato, T. (2002). Decreased
Psychiatry, 54, 529–533. cerebral haemodynamic response to cognitive and
554 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

physiological tasks in mood disorders as shown Disorder and Schizotypal Personality Disorder.
by near-infrared spectroscopy. Psychological Biological Psychiatry, 40, 697–705.
Medicine, 32, 1029–1037. Milak, M. S., Parsey, R. V., Keilp, J., Oquendo, M. A.,
Mauri, M., Sinforiani, E., G., B., Vignati, F., Berselli, Malone, K. M., & Mann, J. J. (2005). Neuroanatomic
M. E., Attanasio, R., et al. (1993). Memory impair- correlates of psychopathologic components of
ment in Cushing’s disease. Acta Neurologica major depressive disorder. Archives of General
Scandinavia, 87, 52–55. Psychiatry, 62, 397–408.
Mayberg, H. S., Brannan, S., Mahurin, R., Jerabek, Miller, W. R. (1975). Psychological deficit in depres-
P., Brickman, J., Tekell, J., et al. (1997). Cingulate sion. Psychological Bulletin, 82(2), 238–260.
function in depression: A potential predictor of Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki,
treatment response. Neuroreport, 8, 1057–1061. A. H., & Howerter, A. (2000). The unity and diver-
Mayberg, H. S., Lewis, P., Regenold, W., & Wagner, sity of executive functions and their contributions
H. (1994). Paralimbic hypoperfusion in unipo- to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable
lar depression. Journal of Nuclear Medicine, 35, analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100.
929–934. Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Williams, R., & Mathews,
Mayberg, H. S., Lozano, A. M., Voon, V., McNeely, A. (1993). Subliminal processing of emotional
H. E., Seminowicz, D., Hamani, C., et al. (2005). information in anxiety and depression. Journal of
Deep brain stimulation for treatment-resistant Abnormal Psychology, 102, 304–311.
depression. Neuron, 45, 651–660. Molgat, C. V., & Patten, S. B. (2005). Comorbidity
McDaniel, J. S., Musselman, D. L., Porter, M. R., & of major depression and migraine—a Canadian
Reed, D. A. (1995). Depression in patients with Population-Based Study. The Canadian Journal
cancer: Diagnosis, biology, and treatment. Archives of Psychiatry/La Revue canadienne de psychiatrie,
of General Psychiatry, 52(2), 89–99. 50(13), 832–837.
McEwen, B. S. (2002). Editorial: Cortisol, Cushing’s Moniz, E. (1954). I succeeded in performing the
syndrome, and a shrinking brain-new evidence prefrontal leukotomy. Journal of Clinical and
for reversibility. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology Experimental Psychopathology, 15, 373–379.
and Metabolism, 87, 1947–1948. Moses-Kolko, E. L., Price, J. C., Thase, M. E.,
McLoughlin, D. M., Mogg, A., Eranti, S., Pluck, G., Meltzer, C. C., Kupfer, D. J., Mathis, C. A., et al.
Purvis, R., Edwards, D., et al. (2007). The clinical (2007). Measurement of 5-HT1A receptor bind-
effectiveness and cost of repetitive transcranial ing in depressed adults before and after antide-
magnetic stimulation versus electroconvulsive pressant drug treatment using positron emission
therapy in severe depression: A multicentre tomography and [11C]WAY-100635. Synapse, 61,
pragmatic randomised controlled trial and eco- 523–530.
nomic analysis. Health Technology Assessment, Munn, M. A., Alexopoulos, J., Nishino, T., Babb, C.
11, 1–54. M., Flake, L. A., Singer, T., et al. (2007). Amygdala
McWilliams, L. A., Cox, B. J., & Enns, M. W. (2003). volume analysis in female twins with major
Mood and anxiety disorders associated with depression. Biological Psychiatry, 62(5), 415–422.
chronic pain: An examination in a nationally rep- Nagai, M., Kishi, K., & Kato, S. (2007). Insular cortex
resentative sample. Pain, 106(1), 127–133. and neuropsychiatric disorders: A review of recent
Mega, M. S., & Cummings, J. L. (1994). Fronto- literature. European Psychiatry, 22(6), 387–394.
subcortical circuits and neuropsychiatric disor- Naismith, S. L., Hickie, I. B., Ward, P. B., Scott, E.,
ders. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical & Little, C. (2006). Impaired implicit sequence
Neurosciences, 6, 358–370. learning in depression: A probe for frontostriatal
Mervaala, E., Fohr, J., Kononen, M., Valknen- dysfunction? Psychological Medicine, 36, 313–323.
Korhonen, M., & NVainio, P. (2000). Quantitative Nandi, D., Saha, G., Bhattacharya, B., & Mandal,
MRI of the hippocampus and amygdala in severe M. (1982). A study on perception of facial expres-
depression. Psychological Medicine, 30, 117–25. sion of emotions in depression. Indian Journal of
Michopoulos, I., Zervas, I. M., Papakosta, V. M., Psychiatry, 24, 284–290.
Tsaltas, E., Papageorgiou, C., Manessi, T., et al. Nebes, R. D., Butters, M. A., Mulsant, B. H., Pollock,
(2006). Set shift ing deficits in melancholic vs. non- B. G., Zmuda, M. D., Houck, P. R., et al. (2000).
melancholic depression: Preliminary findings. Decreased working memory and processing speed
European Psychiatry, 21, 361–363. mediate cognitive impairment in geriatric depres-
Mikhailova, E., Vladimirova, T., Iznak, A., sion. Psychological Medicine, 30, 679–691.
Tsusulkovskaya, E., & Sushko, N. (1996). Nilsson, F. M., Kessing, L. V., Sorenson, T.
Abnormal recognition of facial expression of emo- M., Anderson, P. K., & Bolwig, T. G. (2002).
tions in depressed patients with Major Depression Major depressive disorder in Parkinson’s
Neuropsychology of Depression and Related Mood Disorders 555

disease: A register-based study. Acta Psychiatrica Strength of association for specific conditions in
Scandinavica, 106, 202–211. the general population. The Canadian Journal of
Northoff, G., Richter, A., Gessner, M., Schlagenhauf, Psychiatry/La Revue canadienne de psychiatrie,
F., Fell, J., Baumgart, F., et al. (2000). Functional 50(4), 195–202.
dissociation between medial and lateral prefrontal Payne, R. (1990). Medication-induced performance
cortical spatiotemporal activation in negative and deficits: Analgesics and narcotics. Journal of
positive emotions: A combined fMRI/MEG study. Occupational Medicine, 32, 362–369.
Cerebral Cortex, 10, 93–107. Persad, S., & Polivy, J. (1993). Differences between
Nussbaum, P. D., Kaszniak, A. W., Allender, J. A., & depressed and nondepressed individuals in
Rapcsak, S. Z. (1995). Depression and cognitive the recognition of and response to facial emo-
deterioration in the elderly: A follow-up study. The tional cues. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102,
Clinical Neuropsychologist, 9, 101–111. 358–368.
O’Brien, J. T., Lloyd, A., McKeith, I., Gholkar, A., Pezawas, L., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Drabant, E. M.,
& Ferrier, N. (2004). A longitudinal study of hip- Verchinski, B. A., Munoz, K. E., Kolachana, B. S.,
pocampal volume, cortisol levels, and cognition et al. (2005). 5-HTTLPR polymorphism impacts
in older depressed subjects. American Journal of human cingulate-amygdala interactions: A genetic
Psychiatry, 161, 2081–2090. susceptibility mechanism for depression. Nature
Ochsner, K., Ray, R. D., Cooper, J. C., Robertson, E. Neuroscience, 8, 828–834.
R., Chopra, S., Gabrielli, J. D. E., et al. (2004). For Phan, K. L., Taylor, S. F., Welsh, R. C., Decker, L. R.,
better or worse: Neural systems supporting the Noll, D., Nichols, T. E., et al. (2003). Activation
cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative of the medial prefrontal cortex and extended
emotion. NeuroImage, 23, 483–499. amygdala by individual ratings of emotional
Okada, G., Okamoto, Y., Morinobu, S., Yamawaki, arousal: A fMRI study. Biological Psychiatry, 53,
S., & Yokota, N. (2003). Attenuated left prefrontal 211–215.
activation during a verbal fluency task in patients Phillips, M., Drevets, W., Rauch, S., & Lane, R.
with depression. Neuropsychobiology, 47, 21–26. (2003). Neurobiology of emotion perception II:
Olley, A., Malhi, G. S., Mitchell, P. B., Batchelor, Implications for major psychiatric disorders.
J., Lagopoulos, J., & Austin, M. V. (2005). When Biological Psychiatry, 54, 515–528.
euthymia is just not good enough. Nervous and Phillips, M. L., Medford, N., Young, A. W., Williams,
Mental Disease, 193(5), 323–330. L., Williams, S. C. R., Bullmore, E. T., et al.
Ongur, D., Drevets, W. C., & Price, J. L. (1998). (2001). Time courses of left and right amygdalar
Glial reduction in the subgenual prefrontal cor- responses to fearful facial expressions. Human
tex in mood disorders. Proceedings of the National Brain Mapping: Special Issue, 12, 193–202.
Academy of Sciences of the United States of Pizzagalli, D. A., Oakes, T. R., Fox, A. S., Chung, M.
America, 95, 13290–13295. K., Larson, C. L., Abercrombie, H. C., et al. (2004).
Owen, A. M. (2004). Cognitive dysfunction in Functional but not structural subgenual prefron-
Parkinson disease: The role of frontostriatal cir- tal cortex abnormalities in melancholia. Molecular
cuitry. Neuroscientist, 10, 525–537. Psychiatry, 9, 393–405.
Paelecke-Habermann, Y., Pohl, J., & Leplow, B. Pizzigalli, D., Pascual-Marqui, R. D., Nitschke, J.,
(2005). Attention and executive functions in Oakes, T. R., Larson, C. L., Abercrombie, H., et al.
remitted major depression patients. Journal of (2001). Anterior cingulate activity as a predictor
Affective Disorders, 89, 125–135. of degree of treatment response in major depres-
Paradiso, S., Johnson, D., Andreasan, N., O’Leary, sion: Evidence from brain Electrical Tomography
D., Watkins, G., Ponto, L., et al. (1999). Cerebral Analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158,
blood flow changes associated with attribution of 405–415.
emotional valence to pleasant, unpleasant, and Porter, R. J., Gallagher, P., Thompson, J. M., & Young,
neutral visual stimuli in a PET study of normal A. H. (2003). Neurocognitive impairment in drug-
subjects. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, free patients with major depressive disorder.
1618–1629. British Journal of Psychiatry, 182, 214–220.
Paterniti S, Verdier-Taillefer MH, Dufouil C, Pournajafi-Nazarloo, H., Carr, M. S., Papademeteriou,
Alpérovitch A. (2002). Depressive symptoms and E., Schmidt, J. V., & Cushing, B. S. (2007). Oxytocin
cognitive decline in elderly people. Longitudinal selectively increases ERalpha mRNA in the neo-
study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 181, 406–410. natal hypothalamus and hippocampus of female
Patten, S. B., Beck, C. A., Kassam, A., Williams, J. prairie voles. Neuropeptides, 41, 39–44.
V. A., Barbui, C., & Metz, L. M. (2005). Long- Purcell, R., Maruff, P., Kyrios, M., & Pantelis,
term medical conditions and major depression: C. (1997). Neuropsychological function in
556 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

young patients with unipolar major depression. Ruo, B., Rumsfeld, J. S., Hlatky, M. K., Liu, H.,
Psychological Medicine, 27, 1277–1285. Browner, W. S., & Whooley, M. A. (2003).
Quraishi, S., & Frangou, S. (2002). Neuropsychology Depressive symptoms and health-related quality
of bipolar disorder: A review. Journal of Affective of life: The heart and soul study. JAMA: Journal
Disorders, 72, 209–226. of the American Medical Association, 290(2),
Ravnkilde, B., Videbech, P., Clemmensen, K., 215–221.
Egander, A., Rasmussen, N. A., Gjedde, A., et al. Sadavoy, J. (2004). Psychotropic drugs and the elderly.
(2003). The Danish PET/depression project: New York: Norton, & Company.
Cognitive function and regional cerebral blood Saghafi, R., Brown, C., Butters, M. A., Cyranowski,
flow. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 108, 32–40. J., Dew, M. A., Frank, E., et al. (2007). Predicting
Rey, A. (1964). L’examen Clinicque en Psychologie 6-week treatment response to escitalopram phar-
[The Clinical Examination in Psychology]. Paris: macotherapy in late-life major depressive disor-
Presses Universitaires de France. der. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry,
Riso, L. P., Miyatake, R. K., & Thase, M. E. (2002). 11, 1141–1146.
The search for determinants of chronic depres- Sapolsky, R. (2000). Glucocorticoids and hippocam-
sion: A review of six factors. Journal of Affective pal atrophy in neuropsychiatric disorders. Archives
Disorders, 70, 103–115. of General Psychiatry, 57, 925–935.
Robinson, L. J., & Ferrier, I. N. (2006). Evolution of Sapolsky, R. M. (2001). Depression, antidepressants,
cognitive impairment in bipolar disorder: A sys- and the shrinking hippocampus. Proceedings of
tematic review of cross-sectional evidence. Bipolar National Academy of the Sciences of the United
Disorders, 8, 103–116. States of America, 98, 12320–12322.
Robinson, L. J., Thompson, J. M., Gallagher, P., Schlaepfer, T. E., Cohen, M. X., Frick, C., Kosel,
Goswami, U., Young, A. H., Ferrier, I. N., et al. M., Brodesser, D., Axmacher, N., et al. (2007).
(2006). A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in Deep brain stimulation to reward circuitry alle-
euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. Journal viates anhedonia in refractory major depression.
of Affective Disorders, 93, 105–115. Neuropsychopharmacology, 33, 368–377.
Rogers, M. A., Bellgrove, M. A., Chiu, E., Mileshkin, Schretlen, D. J., Cascella, N.G., Meyer, S. M., Kingery,
C., & Bradshaw, J. L. (2004). Response selection L. R., Testa, S. M., Munro, C. A., et al. (2007).
deficits in melancholic but not nonmelancholic Neuropsychological functioning in bipolar disor-
unipolar major depression. Journal of Clinical and der and schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 62,
Experimental Neuropsychology, 26, 169–179. 179–186.
Rohling, M. L., Green, P., Allen, L., & Iverson, G. L. Sevigny, M., Everett, J., & Grondin, S. (2003).
(2002). Depressive symptoms and neurocognitive Depression, attention, and time estimation. Brain
test scores in patients passing symptom valid- and Cognition, 53, 351–353.
ity tests. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 17, Shah, P., Ebmeier, K., Glabus, M., & Goodwin, G.
205–222. (1998). Cortical grey matter reductions associated
Rohling, M. L., Scogin, F. (1993). Automatic and with treatment-resistant chronic unipolar depres-
effortful memory processes in depressed persons. sion: Controlled magnetic resonance imaging
The Journals of Gerontology, 48, 87–95. study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 172, 527–532.
Rolls, E. (1999). The brain and emotion. Oxford, UK: Shah, P. J., Glabus, M. F., Goodwin, G. M., & Ebmeier,
Oxford University Press. K. P. (2002). Chronic, treatment-resistant depres-
Rose, E. J., & Ebmeier, K. P. (2006). Pattern of sion and right fronto-striatal atrophy. British
impaired working memory during major depres- Journal of Psychiatry, 180, 434–440.
sion. Journal of Affective Disorders, 90, 149–161. Sheline, Y. (2003). Neuroimaging studies of mood
Roy-Byrne, P. P., Weingartner, H., Bierer, L. M., disorder effects on the brain. Biological Psychiatry,
Thompson, K., & Post, R. M. (1986). Effortful 54, 338–352.
and automatic cognitive processes in depression. Sheline, Y., Barch, D., Donnelly, J., Ollinger, J.,
Archives of General Psychiatry, 43, 265–267. Snyder, A., & Mintun, M. A. (2001). Increased
Rubinsztein, J. S., Michael, A., Paykel, E. S., amygdala response to masked emotional faces in
& Sahakian, B. J. (2000). Cognitive impair- depressed subjects resolves with antidepressant
ment in remission in bipolar affective disorder. treatment: An fMRI study. Biological Psychiatry,
Psychological Medicine, 30(5), 1025–1036. 50, 651–658.
Rude, S. S., Wentzlaff, R. M., Gibbs, B., Vane, J., & Sheline, Y. I., Barch, D. M., Garcia, K., Gersing, K.,
Whitney, T. (2002). Negative processing biases pre- Pieper, C., Welsh-Bohmer, K., et al. (2006). Cognitive
dict subsequent depressive symptoms. Cognition function in late life depression: Relationships to
& Emotion, 16, 423–440. depression severity, cerebrovascular risk factors,
Neuropsychology of Depression and Related Mood Disorders 557

and processing speed. Biological Psychiatry, 60, Decrease in cortisol reverses human hippocampal
58–65. atrophy following treatment of Cushing’s disease.
Sheline, Y. I., Gado, M. H., & Kraemer, H. C. (2003). Biological Psychiatry, 46, 1595–1602.
Untreated depression and hippocampal vol- Starkman, M. N., Giordani, B., Gebarski, S. S., &
ume loss. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, Schteingart, D. E. (2003). Improvement in learning
1516–1518. associated with increase in hippocampal forma-
Siegle, G. J., Carter, C. S., & Thase, M. E. (2006). tion volume. Biological Psychiatry, 53, 233–238.
Use of fMRI to predict recovery from unipolar Starkman, M. N., Giordani, B., Gebarski, S. S., &
depression with cognitive behavior therapy 145. Schteingart, D. E. (2007). Improvement in mood
American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 735–738. and ideation associated with increase in right cau-
Siegle, G. J., Steinhauer, S. R., Thase, M. E., Stenger, date volume. Journal of Affective Disorders, 101,
A., & Carter, C. S. (2002). Can’t shake that feeling: 139–147.
Event-related fMRI assessment of sustained amyg- Starkman, M. N., Schteingart, D. E., & Schork, M.
dala activity in response to emotional information A. (1981). Depressed mood and other psychi-
in depressed individuals. Biological Psychiatry, 51, atric manifestations of Cushing’s syndrome:
693–707. Relationship to hormone levels. Psychosomatic
Simpson, S. W., Baldwin, R. C., Burns, A., & Jackson, Medicine, 43, 3–18.
A. (2001). Regional cerebral volume measure- Starkman, M. N., Schteingart, D. E., & Schork, M.
ments in late-life depression: Relationship to clin- A. (1986). Cushing’s syndrome after treatment:
ical correlates, neuropsychological impairment Changes in cortisol and ACTH levels, and ame-
and response to treatment. International Journal lioration of the depressive syndrome. Psychiatry
of Geriatric Psychiatry, 16, 469–476. Research, 19, 177–188.
Sjogren, P., Olsen, A. K., Thomsen, A. B., & Dalberg, Steffens, D. C., McQuoid, D. R., Welsh-Bohmer, K.
J. (2000). Neuropsychological performance in A., & Krishnan, K. R. (2003). Left orbital frontal
cancer patients: The role of oral opioids, pain and cortex volume and performance on the benton
performance status. Pain, 86, 237–245. visual retention test in older depressives and con-
Smith, K. A., Morris, J. S., Friston, K. J., Cowen, P. J., trols. Neuropsychopharmacology, 28, 2179–2183.
& Dolan, R. J. (1999). Brain mechanisms associated Stockmeier, C. A., Mahajan, G. J., Konick, L. C.,
with depressive relapse and associated cognitive Overholser, J. C., Jurjus, G. J., Meltzer, H. Y., et al.
impairment following acute tryptophan deple- (2004). Cellular changes in the postmortem hippo-
tion. British Journal of Psychiatry, 174, 525–529. campus in major depression. Biological Psychiatry,
Sobin, C., & Sackeim, H. A. (1997). Psychomotor 56, 640–650.
symptoms of depression. American Journal of Suhr, J. A. (2003). Neuropsychological impairment
Psychiatry, 154, 4–17. in fibromyalgia: Relation to depression, fatigue,
Sobow, T., Wojtera, M., & Kloszewska, I. (2006). and pain. Journal of Psychosomatic Research,
Prevalence of potentially reversible cognitive 55(4), 321–329.
function disorders in patients of a memory dys- Sumanti, M., Boone, K. B., Savodnik, I., & Gorsuch,
function clinic. Psychiatria Polska, 40, 845–854. R. (2006). Noncredible psychiatric and cognitive
Sonino, N., & Fava, G. A. (1996). Serotonin, symptoms in a workers’ compensation “stress” claim
Cushing’s disease, and depression. Psychotherapy sample. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 20, 754–765.
and Psychosomatics, 65, 63–65. Sweeney, J. A., Kmiec, J. A., & Kupfer, D. J. (2000).
Sonino, N., Fava, G. A., Belluardo, P., Girelli, M. Neuropsychologic impairments in bipolar and
E., & Boscaro, M. (1993). Course of depression unipolar mood disorders on the CANTAB neu-
in Cushings-Syndrome—response to treatment rocognitive battery. Biological Psychiatry, 48,
and comparison with Graves-Disease. Hormone 674–685.
Research, 39, 202–206. Sweet, R.A., Hamilton, R.L., Butters, M.A., Mulsant,
Spiegel, D. (2001). Mind matters: Coping and cancer B.H., Pollock, B.G. et al. (2004) Neuropathologic
progression. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, correlates of late-onset major depression.
50(5), 287–290. Neuropschopharmacology, 29, 2242–2250.
Starkman, M. N., Giordani, B., Berent, S., Schork, Taylor, B. P., Bruder, G. E., Stewart, J. W., McGrath,
M. A., & Schteingart, D. E. (2001). Elevated corti- P. J., Halperin, J., Ehrlichman, H., et al. (2006).
sol levels in Cushing’s disease are associated with Psychomotor slowing as a predictor of fluoxetine
cognitive decrements. Psychosomatic Medicine, nonresponse in depressed outpatients. American
63, 985–993. Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 73–78.
Starkman, M. N., Giordani, B., Gebarski, S. S., Berent, Thomas, K., Drevets, W., Dahl, R. E., Ryan, N.,
S., Schork, M. A., & Schteingart, D. E. (1999). Birmaher, B., Eccard, C., et al. (2001). Amygdala
558 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

response to fearful faces in anxious and depressed Volkow, N. D., Wang, G. J., Hitzemann, R., Fowler, J. S.,
children. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, Pappas, N., Lowrimore, P., et al. (1995). Depression
1057–1063. of thalamic metabolism by lorazepam is associ-
Thomsen, A. F., Kvist, T. K., Andersen, P. K., & ated with sleepiness. Neuropsychopharmacology,
Kessing, L. V. (2006). Increased relative risk of 12, 123–132.
subsequent affective disorders in patients with a Vythilingam, M., Vermetten, E., Anderson, G. M.,
hospital diagnosis of obesity. International Journal Luckenbaugh, D., Anderson, E. R., Snow, J., et al.
of Obesity, 30(9), 1415–1421. (2004). Hippocampal volume, memory, and corti-
Torrent, C., Martinez-Aran, A., Daban, C., Sanchez- sol status in major depressive disorder: Effects of
Moreno, J., Comes, M., Goikolea, J. M., et al. treatment. Biological Psychiatry, 56, 101–112.
(2006). Cognitive impairment in bipolar II disor- Wadsworth, E. J., Moss, S. C., Simpson, S. A., & Smith,
der. British Journal of Psychiatry, 189, 254–259. A. P. (2005). SSRIs and cognitive performance in a
Townend, E., Brady, M., & McLaughlan, K. (2007). working sample. Human Psychopharmacology, 20,
Exclusion and inclusion criteria for people with 561–572.
aphasia in studies of depression after stroke: A Wagner, G., Sinsel, E., Sobanski, T., Kohler, S.,
systematic review and future recommendations. Marinou, V., Mentzel, H. J., et al. (2006). Cortical
Neuroepidemiology, 29, 1–17. inefficiency in patients with unipolar depression:
Tucker, G., Popkin, M., Caine, E., Folstein, M., & Grant, An event-related MRI study with the Stroop task.
I. (1990). Reorganizing the “organic” disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 59, 958–965.
Hospital & Community Psychiatry, 41, 722–724. Watkins, P. C., Mathews, A., Williamson, D. A., &
Turk, D. C., & Brody, M. C. (1992). What position Fuller, R. D. (1992). Mood-congruent memory in
do APS’s physician members take on chronic pain depression—emotional priming or elaboration.
opioid therapy? APS Bulletin, 2, 1–5. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 581–586.
Twycross, R. G. (1994). Opioids. In P. D. Wall and R. Watkins, P. C., Vache, K., Verney, S. P., Muller, S.,
Melzack (Eds.), Textbook of pain (3rd ed., pp. 943– & Mathews, A. (1996). Unconscious mood-con-
962). Edinburgh, Scotland: Churchill Livingsone. gruent memory bias in depression. Journal of
Vadnal, V. L. (2005). Depression and effort in Abnormal Psychology, 105, 34–41.
the elderly. Unpublished senior honor’s thesis, Watson, S., Thompson, J. M., Ritchie, J. C., Ferrier,
University of Michigan. I. N., & Young, A. H. (2006). Neuropsychological
Vainio, A., Ollila, J., Matikainen, E., Rosenberg, P., & impairment in bipolar disorder: The relationship
Kalso, E. (1995). Driving ability in cancer patients with glucocorticoid receptor function. Bipolar
receiving long-term morphine analgesia. Lancet, Disorders, 8, 85–90.
346, 667–670. Weiland-Fiedler, P., Erickson, K., Waldeck, T.,
Van Praag, H. M., Korf, J., Lakke, J., & Schut, T. Luckenbaugh, D. A., Pike, D., Bonne, O., et al.
(1975). Dopamine metabolism in depression, psy- (2004). Evidence for continuing neuropsychologi-
choses, and Parkinson’s disease: The problem of cal impairments in depression. Journal of Affective
specificity of biological variables in behavior dis- Disorders, 82, 253–258.
orders. Psychological Medicine, 5, 138–146. Weingartner, H., Cohen, R. M., Murphy, D. L., Martello,
Videbech, P. (2000). PET measurements of brain glu- J., & Gerdt, C. (1981). Cognitive processes in depres-
cose metabolism and blood flow in major depres- sion. Archives of General Psychiatry, 38, 42–47.
sive disorder: A critical review. Acta Psychiatrica Whalen, J. (1998). Masked presentation of emo-
Scandinavica, 101, 11–20. tional facial expressions modulate amygdala
Videbech, P., & Ravnkilde, B. (2004). Hippocampal activity without explicit knowledge. Journal of
volume and depression: A meta-analysis of MRI Neuroscience, 18, 411–419.
studies. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161, White, J., Davison, G. C., Haaga, D. A., & White, K.
1957–1966. (1992). Cognitive bias in the articulated thoughts of
Vinkers, D.J., Stek, M.L., Gussekloo, J., Van Der Mast, depressed and nondepressed psychiatric patients.
R.C., Westendorp, R.G. (2004). Does depression in Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease, 180, 77–81.
old age increase only cardiovascular mortality? Wiart, L. (1997). Post-cerebrovascular stroke depres-
The Leiden 85-plus Study. International Journal of sion. Encephale, 23, 51–54.
Geriatric Psychiatry, 19, 852–857. Wilson, R.S., Mendes De Leon, C.F., Bennett, D.A.,
Vinkers, D.J., Stek, M.L., van der Mast, R.C., de Bienias, J.L., Evans, D.A. (2004). Depressive symp-
Craen, A.J., Le Cessie, S. (2005). Generalized ath- toms and cognitive decline in a community pop-
erosclerosis, cognitive decline, and depressive ulation of older persons. Journal of Neurology,
symptoms in old age. Neurology, 65, 107–112. Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 75, 126–129.
Neuropsychology of Depression and Related Mood Disorders 559

Wilson, R.S., Arnold, S.E., Beck, T.L., Bienias, J.L., Young, E. A., Carlson, N., & Brown, M. B. (2001).
Bennett, D.A. (2008). Change in depressive symp- Twenty-four hour ACTH and cortisol pulsatility
toms during the prodromal phase of Alzheimer in depressed women. Neuropsychopharmacology,
disease. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65, 25, 267–276.
439–445. Young, E. A., Haskett, R. F., Grunhaus, L., Pande, A.,
Wright, C. I., Fischer, H., Whalen, P. J., McInerney, S. Weinberg, V. M., Watson, S. J., et al. (1994). Increased
C., Shin, L. M., & Rauch, S. L. (2001). Differential circadian activation of the hypothalamic pituitary
prefrontal cortex and amygdala habituation adrenal axis in depressed patients in the evening.
to repeatedly presented emotional stimuli. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 701–707.
Neuroreport: For Rapid Communication of Young, E. A., Haskett, R. F., Murphy-Weinberg, V.,
Neuroscience Research, 12, 379–383. Watson, S. J., & Akil, H. (1991). Loss of gluco-
Wright, S. L., & Persad, C. (2007). Distinguishing corticoid fast feedback in depression. Archives of
between depression and dementia in older per- General Psychiatry, 48, 693–699.
sons: Neuropsychological and neuropathologi- Zacny, J. P. (1995). A review of the effects of opi-
cal correlates. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and oids on psychomotor and cognitive function-
Neurology, 20(4), 189–198. ing in humans. Experimental and Clinical
Yohannes, A. M., Baldwin, R. C., & Connolly, M. J. Psychopharmacology, 3(4), 432–466.
(2000). Mood disorders in elderly patients with Zacny, J. P., Lichtor, J.L., & Thapar, P. (1994). Comparing
chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. Reviews the subjective, psychomotor and physiological
in Clinical Gerontology, 10(2), 193–202. effects of intravenous butorphanol and morphine
Young, A. H., Gallagher, P., Watson, S., Del-Estal, D., in healthy volunteers. Journal of Pharmacology
Owen, B. M., & Ferrier, I. N. (2004). Improvements and Experimental Therapeutics, 270, 579–588.
in neurocognitive function and mood following Zubieta, J., Huguelet, P., O’Neil, R. L., & Giordani, B.
adjunctive treatment with mifepristone (RU-486) J. (2001). Cognitive function in euthymic bipolar
in bipolar disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology, I disorder. Psychiatry Research, 102, 9–20.
29, 1538–1545.
23

The Neuropsychology of Memory


Dysfunction and Its Assessment
David P. Salmon and Larry R. Squire

Introduction phenomenology of memory dysfunction and its


underlying causes.
Memory impairment is a common consequence
of neurological injury or disease (Strub & Black,
1977) and is often reported in association with The Amnesic Syndrome
affective (Sternberg & Jarvik, 1976; Stromgren, Although memory dysfunction most often
1977) and other psychiatric (Aleman et al., occurs as one of a constellation of disorders of
1999; Chapman, 1966) disorders. It may also intellectual function, as in depression or demen-
occur as a side effect of treatments such as psy- tia, it can sometimes occur as a relatively pure
chotropic drugs and electroconvulsive therapy entity. When this occurs, patients with memory
(ECT) (Sackeim, 2000; Squire, 1986). Given the dysfunction due to neurological injury or dis-
pervasive and pernicious nature of memory ease can have severe deficits in the ability to
dysfunction in neuropsychiatric disorders, it learn and retain new information (i.e., antero-
is not surprising that memory impairment has grade amnesia) but can appear cognitively nor-
been the subject of extensive clinical research. mal to casual observation. In conversation, they
This research has shown that, when memory can exhibit appropriate social skills and exhibit
disorders are evaluated with quantitative meth- normal language ability. They can also have
ods, it becomes possible to identify the vari- normal intelligence as measured by conven-
ous disorders that can occur, to understand tional IQ tests and, importantly, a normal abil-
the similarities and differences between them, ity to hold information in immediate memory
and to follow their course reliably in individ- (Butters & Cermak, 1980; Shrager et al., 2008).
ual patients (for reviews, see Butters et al., 1995; These characteristics of the amnesic syndrome
Mayes, 1988; Squire & Schacter, 2002; Verfaellie are illustrated in one of the best known cases
& O’Connor, 2000). These possibilities have of circumscribed amnesia, patient H.M., a man
been realized largely through the application who developed severe anterograde amnesia after
of principles developed in the field of cognitive neurosurgery carried out to relieve severe epi-
neuroscience. Thus, the purpose of this chap- lepsy (Scoville & Milner, 1957). Following bilat-
ter is twofold. First, to elucidate a rationale for eral medial temporal lobe resection, H.M. was
the neuropsychological evaluation of memory unable to retain memory for events and informa-
dysfunction that is grounded in basic cognitive tion encountered after his surgery. Despite this
neuroscience research. Second, to identify and profound anterograde amnesia, H.M. retained
describe neuropsychological tools of memory an above-average IQ, intact immediate mem-
testing that are widely used in clinical settings. ory, and normal problem-solving and language
Before discussing these issues, we describe the functions, including the ability to detect various

560
The Neuropsychology of Memory Dysfunction and Its Assessment 561

kinds of linguistic ambiguity (e.g., “Racing cars closed head injury. The method revealed the
can be dangerous” or “Charging tigers should approximate duration of both anterograde and
be avoided” [Lackner, 1974; also see Schmolck retrograde amnesia, and showed how the def-
et al., 2000]). H.M. also had acute awareness of icit changed with the passage of time. In this
his memory deficit and insight into its nature. particular case, anterograde amnesia affected
In an often-quoted passage, he expressed his about 2 ½ months postinjury and remained
own experience of his memory disorder: fi xed for this period of time, even after memory
capacities had largely recovered. Presumably,
Right now, I’m wondering. Have I done or said any- this reflected the time when memories for
thing amiss? You see, at this moment everything
events could not be formed in the normal way
looks clear to me, but what happened just before?
That’s what worries me. It’s like waking from a
due to the neurologic injury. Retrograde amne-
dream; I just don’t remember. (Milner, 1970, p. 37) sia was initially severe and extensive but grad-
ually shrank to 2 weeks with oldest memories
Another important feature of the amnesic recovering first.
syndrome exhibited by H.M. was an inability Patient interviews revealed a similar relation-
to remember events that occurred prior to the ship between anterograde and retrograde amne-
onset of his amnesia (Corkin, 1984). This ret- sia in severely depressed patients undergoing a
rograde amnesia extended back several years prescribed course of bilateral ECT. Thirty-one
prior to his surgery at age 27 but was time-lim- patients were interviewed about their memory
ited, as he was capable of recalling well-formed before ECT, and again 7 months and 3 years
autobiographical episodes from his adolescence. after treatment. Before treatment, patients on
Subsequent research has shown that retrograde average reported having difficulty remembering
amnesia can be relatively circumscribed and the 5 months prior to ECT, presumably because
limited to just a few months or years, or it can be of their depressive illness. Seven months after
extensive and affect many decades prior to the ECT, patients reported difficulty remember-
onset of amnesia (Bayley et al., 2006; Squire & ing events that occurred during the 3 months
Bayley, 2007). In addition, retrograde amnesia after treatment and during the 2 years preced-
is typically temporally graded with older mem- ing. Three years later, retrograde amnesia had
ories less affected than more recent memories. shrunk to about pre-ECT level and anterograde
It should be noted, however, that the temporal amnesia remained fi xed at 2 months (Squire &
characteristics of retrograde amnesia are diffi- Slater, 1983).
cult to determine in memory disorders that have Although these examples show that there
a gradual onset (e.g., dementia of Alzheimer’s can be recovery of memory function over
disease) because the distinction between antero- time following certain amnesia-precipitating
grade and retrograde amnesia is always blurred. events (e.g., ECT, traumatic brain injury), in
In these cases, it becomes difficult to determine many cases extensive anterograde and retro-
if information is unavailable because it was not grade amnesia are permanent. The profound
acquired in the first place (anterograde amne- anterograde amnesia and time-limited retro-
sia) or because it was acquired initially and then grade amnesia exhibited by patient H.M., for
later lost as the result of the onset of amnesia example, has not abated after more than 50
(retrograde amnesia). years (Corkin, 2002). Indeed, it appears that
Because many patients with memory dys- if specific brain structures thought to underlie
function have insight into their condition, a memory processes are destroyed, the amne-
useful way to obtain information about the sic syndrome will be a permanent condition.
scope of their memory loss and the relationship If these structures are made temporarily dys-
between anterograde and retrograde amnesia is functional by ECT or vascular deficiencies or
to construct a time line of the deficit with the only partially or reversibly damaged by a closed
patient’s assistance. Th is technique was used to head injury, some recovery of memory func-
good advantage by Barbizet (1970) as a way of tion can occur. The characteristics of different
identifying what past time periods were affected causes of amnesia are described further in the
in an amnesic patient who had suffered a severe following section.
562 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Causes of Amnesia Subsequent studies of patients with more


restricted medial temporal lobe damage have
Amnesia can occur for a variety of reasons provided additional information about the
that include temporal lobe surgery, chronic role of the hippocampus itself in memory (e.g.,
alcohol abuse, head injury, anoxia or ischemia, Zola-Morgan et al., 1986). Patient R.B. became
encephalitis, epilepsy, tumor, or vascular acci- amnesic in 1978 at the age of 52 after an episode
dent (for review, see Kopelman, 2002). In addi- of global ischemia that occurred as a compli-
tion, amnesia is typically a prominent and early cation of open-heart surgery. Extensive neu-
sign of dementia associated with Alzheimer’s ropsychological assessment during the next 5
disease and other neurodegenerative disorders years revealed significant memory impairment
(for review, see Salmon, 2000). Although the list in the absence of other cognitive dysfunction
of various etiologies for memory dysfunction is (see Figure 23–1). Upon his death in 1983, histo-
long, they have in common their adverse effect logical examination of R.B.’s brain revealed a
on a core set of neurological structures that are discrete bilateral lesion that involved the full
critical for memory, as described below. rostrocaudal extent of the CA1 field of the hip-
pocampus. Other medial temporal lobe struc-
Medial Temporal Lobe Amnesia tures and other brain regions were unaffected.
The profound amnesia experienced by patient This was the first reported case of human amne-
H.M. following bilateral medial temporal lobe sia documented by extensive neuropsychologi-
resection to treat severe epilepsy provided the cal testing that occurred as a consequence of
first evidence that structures within this brain neuropathologically verified damage restricted
region are critical for human memory. Bilateral to the hippocampus. Subsequent neurohisto-
hippocampal damage was often identified as the logical studies have confirmed the link between
critical factor leading to his amnesia, but the memory dysfunction and bilateral hippocampal
medial temporal lobe resection was extensive damage (Gold & Squire, 2006; Rempel-Clower
and damaged not only the hippocampus but et al., 1996; Victor & Agamanolis, 1990).
also the amygdala and adjacent medial tempo- The results of these studies indicate that the
ral cortex (Scoville & Milner, 1957). The relative hippocampus is a critical structure for memory
importance of these structures for his memory and that damage limited to the hippocampus
impairment could not be determined. is sufficient to produce easily detectable and

R.B. R.B. Control

Figure 23–1. Performance of patient R.B. on two separate administrations of the Rey–
Osterreith Complex Figure Test. R.B. was first asked to copy the figure (top portions of the
left and center panels) and then to reproduce the figure from memory 10–20 minutes later
(bottom portion of left and center panels). The two administrations of the test were 6 months
(left panel) and 23 months (center panel) after the onset of his amnesia. In both instances,
R.B.’s delayed recall of the figure was severely impaired relative to the performance of an age-
matched normal control subject (right panel). (Adapted from Zola-Morgan et al., 1986).
The Neuropsychology of Memory Dysfunction and Its Assessment 563

clinically significant memory impairment. It relative preservation of older remote memo-


is important to note, however, that none of the ries). Neuropathological studies have shown
patients with damage restricted to the hippo- that the syndrome is associated with bilateral
campus was as severely amnesic as patient H.M. damage along the diencephalic midline, espe-
This suggests that medial temporal lobe struc- cially hemorrhagic lesions in the dorsomedial
tures outside of the hippocampus itself must be thalamic and mammillary nuclei (Mair et al.,
important for memory function. This notion 1979; Mayes, 1988). Additional studies with
was confirmed in studies of an animal model of humans and experimental animals confirm
human amnesia in the monkey (Squire & Zola- the importance of medial thalamic structures,
Morgan, 1991). This work identified impor- such as the mediodorsal nucleus, the anterior
tant structures in addition to the hippocampal nucleus, and the connections and structures
region itself. (The hippocampal region includes within the internal medullary lamina (Gold &
the hippocampus proper, the dentate gyrus, Squire, 2006; Markowitsch, 1988; Zola-Morgan
and the subiculum). The additional important & Squire, 1993).
structures are the adjacent entorhinal, para- The amnesic syndrome associated with
hippocampal, and perirhinal cortices. The Korsakoff ’s syndrome is similar to that of medial
amygdala is not a component of this memory temporal lobe amnesia in many respects (Butters
system. & Cermak, 1980). Although early studies raised
the possibility that differences might exist in
the rate of forgetting within long-term memory,
Diencephalic Amnesia
more recent studies of patients with radiologi-
Damage to the midline of the diencephalon cally confirmed lesions indicated that forgetting
can produce severe amnesia. The most widely from long-term memory occurs at an equivalent
studied example of diencephalic amnesia is rate for patients with diencephalic and medial
Korsakoff ’s syndrome, which can develop temporal lobe amnesia (Kopelman & Stanhope,
after many years of chronic alcohol abuse and 1997; McKee & Squire, 1992) (see Figure 23–2).
nutritional deficiency (Victor et al., 1989). This finding is entirely compatible with the well-
Korsakoff ’s syndrome is characterized by pro- known observation that in amnesia information
found anterograde amnesia and extensive, is rapidly lost as it moves from short-term to
temporally graded retrograde amnesia (with long-term memory (e.g., Isaac & Mayes, 1999).

100

90
Percent correct

80

70

60

50
10 min 2 hr 30–32 hr
Log retention interval

Figure 23–2. The performance of patients with medial temporal lobe amnesia (solid triangles; n = 5),
patients with diencephalic amnesia (solid squares; n = 6), and normal control subjects (open circles;
n = 10) at three retention delays averaged across four different visual recognition memory tests. The
two amnesic patient groups exhibited equivalent rates of forgetting within long-term memory after
performance was equated at the shortest delay. (Adapted from McKee & Squire, 1992).
564 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

In this sense, all the “organic” amnesias are syn- functions. However, patients are left with a gap
dromes of rapid forgetting. At present, there is in their memory for events that occurred during
no compelling basis for separating medial tem- the period of anterograde amnesia, presumably
poral lobe and diencephalic amnesia according because usable long-term memory was not being
to behavioral findings. Certainly, it is reason- formed. They may also have a permanent retro-
able to suppose that the medial temporal lobe grade amnesia for events that occurred from a
and the diencephalic midline should make few hours to a day or two just prior to the epi-
different contributions to normal memory. sode (Kritchevsky, 1989). Although the precise
However, each region may also be an essential cause of TGA is not known, it generally occurs
component in a larger functional system such in persons over age 50 (the annual incidence is
that similar amnesia results from damage to 23.5 per 100,000 for persons older than 50) and
any component. is thought to be related to vascular factors that
Although diencephalic and medial temporal affect the medial temporal lobe or diencephalic
lobe damage appear to produce similar memory memory system (Bettermann, 2006; Hodges &
deficits, there are ways in which Korsakoff ’s syn- Warlow, 1990). However, patients who have had
drome differs from other forms of amnesia. As an episode of TGA do not have an increased risk
one of the first serious students of Korsakoff ’s of developing permanent memory dysfunction
syndrome, Talland (1965) wrote that memory or of having a subsequent stroke.
dysfunction in these patients “does not pre-
sent simply a derangement in memory” (p. 108).
Electroconvulsive Therapy
Similarly, Zangwill (1977) suggested that “other
and more extensive psychological dysfunction Transient amnesia also occurs after ECT, which
must coexist with amnesia for the classic pic- is sometimes prescribed for severe depres-
ture of Korsakoff ’s syndrome to emerge” (p. 113). sive illness (for reviews, see Fraser et al., 2008;
These observations were confirmed in subse- Sackeim, 2000; Squire, 1986). Anterograde
quent neuropsychological studies that showed amnesia associated with ECT can be quite
signs of frontal lobe impairment in these severe, particularly in patients who receive
patients that influenced memory test perfor- bilateral treatments. Retrograde amnesia can
mance (Moscovitch, 1982; Oscar-Berman et al., also be quite severe and temporally graded with
2004; Schacter, 1987; Shimamura et al., 1991; more recent memories most affected (Fraser
Squire, 1982). These findings suggest that neu- et al., 2008). New learning capacity recovers
ropsychological examination of patients with within the months following ECT, and persist-
memory impairment must take into account ing impairment in new learning is not detected
the idea that etiologically distinct forms of after this time. Retrograde amnesia also
amnesia can present with nonmemory deficits resolves by 6 to 9 months after ECT, although
that are superimposed on primary memory memory for events that occurred during the
dysfunction. weeks just preceding ECT can be permanently
affected and there may be some spotty memory
loss for even earlier time periods. Although
Transient Global Amnesia
the biological basis of ECT-associated memory
Amnesia is not always permanent. Transient impairment is not known with certainty, it is
global amnesia (TGA) is a disorder quite similar likely to be due to transient electrophysiologi-
to the neurological amnesia just discussed that cal abnormalities in the medial temporal lobe
usually begins suddenly and lasts for only sev- structures important for memory function
eral hours (Hodges & Ward, 1989; Kritchevsky, (Abrams & Essman, 1982; Lerer et al., 1984;
1989; for review, see Jager et al., 2008). The Malitz & Sackeim, 1986).
memory impairment is characterized by severe
anterograde amnesia and a temporally graded
Alzheimer’s Disease
retrograde amnesia that extends over as much
as 20 years prior to the onset episode. After Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is an age-related
the episode, there is full recovery of memory degenerative brain disorder that is characterized
The Neuropsychology of Memory Dysfunction and Its Assessment 565

by neocortical atrophy, neuron and synapse prior to the onset of dementia (e.g., Bäckman
loss, and abnormal deposition of senile plaques et al., 2001). These and similar findings led to
and neurofibrillary tangles (Terry & Katzman, the development of formal criteria for amnes-
1983; Terry et al., 1991). These neuropathologi- tic Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI; Peterson
cal changes are thought to usually begin in the et al., 1995), a predementia condition in elderly
hippocampus and entorhinal cortex, and grad- individuals that is characterized by both subjec-
ually spread to association cortices of the fron- tive and objective memory impairment, which
tal, temporal, and parietal lobes (Braak & Braak, occurs in the face of relatively preserved general
1991; Hyman et al., 1984). Consistent with this cognition and functional abilities (for reviews,
neuropathology, the primary clinical mani- see Albert & Blacker, 2006; Peterson et al., 2001).
festation of AD is a profound global dementia Presumably, the onset of this circumscribed
characterized by severe amnesia and additional amnesia corresponds to the development of AD
deficits in language, “executive” functions, pathology in medial temporal lobe structures
attention, and visuospatial and constructional (Devanand et al., 2007).
abilities (see Bondi et al., this volume). Because
AD is estimated to affect approximately 10% of
Frontal Lobe Memory Dysfunction
the population over the age of 65 (Evans et al.,
1989), it is the most common cause of severe Damage to the prefrontal association cortex
memory dysfunction. can result in memory dysfunction (for review,
Despite the global nature of the cognitive see Baldo & Shimamura, 2002). The memory
dysfunction evident in AD, a disorder of mem- deficit usually reflects poor planning and orga-
ory is clearly the most prevalent and promi- nization that leads to inefficient learning and
nent feature of the early stages of the disease. retrieval strategies, increased susceptibility to
The memory deficit is similar in many respects interference, and poor retrieval of context (e.g.,
to that associated with circumscribed amne- temporal order) or “source” of to-be-remem-
sia due to medial temporal lobe damage. For bered information (Janowsky et al., 1989b).
example, neuropsychological testing of AD There are also deficits in attention and working
patients reveals significant declines in their memory (i.e., a limited-capacity memory sys-
ability to learn and retain new verbal or non- tem in which information that is the immedi-
verbal information (i.e., anterograde amne- ate focus of attention can be temporarily held
sia), as well as deficits in the ability to recollect and manipulated; Baddeley, 1986). The severe
information from their recent past (i.e., retro- deficit in retention that characterizes medial
grade amnesia) (Salmon, 2000). Unlike patients temporal lobe and diencephalic amnesia is not
with medial temporal lobe amnesia, however, present in frontal lobe memory dysfunction
memory for concepts, meaning of words, and (Kopelman & Stanhope, 1997). Furthermore,
remotely acquired factual information may patients with frontal lobe damage often show
also be impaired in patients with AD, even in dispropor tionate deficits in free recall com-
the relatively early stages of the disease. In addi- pared to recognition (Janowsky et al., 1989a;
tion, patients with AD have early deficits in cer- Shimamura et al., 1990; Wheeler et al., 1995).
tain aspects of implicit memory (see following For example, Janowsky and colleagues (1989a)
section on memory systems) that are intact in found that patients with frontal lobe lesions
patients with circumscribed amnesia (Salmon performed normally on the recognition version
et al., 1988; Shimamura et al., 1987). of a word list-learning task despite impaired
A number of prospective longitudinal stud- performance on the free-recall version of the
ies of cognitive function in nondemented older same task (see Figure 23–3). Patients with dien-
adults have shown that a decline in memory cephalic or medial temporal lobe damage, in
often occurs prior to the emergence of the obvi- contrast, were impaired at both recall and rec-
ous cognitive and behavioral changes required ognition. These and similar results suggest that
for a clinical diagnosis of AD (for review, see the memory impairment associated with frontal
Twamley et al., 2006). In some cases, memory lobe damage does not represent a fundamental
impairment becomes apparent many years amnesic syndrome (Kopelman, 2002).
566 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

A B
100

80 F-CON F-CON
F
60 F
Chance Chance

40

20
Recall Recognition Recognition with delay
0
1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 7 day delay
Trials Trials

C
100

CON
80 ALC

60
Chance
40 AMN
KOR
20
Recall Recognition
0
1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5

Figure 23–3. The performance of patients with frontal lobe lesions (F; n = 7) and
their controls (F-CON; n = 11) on the recall and recognition components of the Rey
Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT) (Panel A: recall and recognition; Panel B: a sec-
ond recognition test that includes a 7-day delay). Although free recall was impaired,
recognition memory was not impaired. For comparison, the performance of patients
with Korsakoff ’s syndrome (KOR; n = 7) or other amnesia (AMN; n = 5) and their
controls (alcoholic controls, ALC; n = 8, and healthy controls, CON; n = 8, respec-
tively) are also shown (Panel C). The amnesic Korsakoff patients and the other amne-
sic patients were impaired on both the recall and recognition components of the test.
(Adapted from Janowsky et al., 1989a).

Traumatic Brain Injury


The immediate aftermath of TBI is usually char-
Memory dysfunction is common following acterized by a period of posttraumatic amnesia
traumatic brain injury (TBI), but usually occurs (PTA) that includes confusion, disorientation,
in conjunction with other cognitive deficits that impaired attention, and an inability to learn new
may include a reduction in general intellectual information or remember day-to-day events
capacity, disorders of language and visual per- (Brooks, 1984). PTA can last from a few hours
ception, apraxia, impairment of attention, and to several weeks but gradually clears. In some
personality change (for reviews, see Levin et al., cases, however, residual memory impairment
1982; Newcombe, 1983; Vakil, 2005). The pres- may persist for a number of years. According
ence of multiple disorders is consistent with to Newcombe (1983), approximately 1% of TBI
the variable and widespread pattern of cerebral patients have persisting signs of impairment.
damage that can result from severe head injury. The duration of PTA is the best available index
The Neuropsychology of Memory Dysfunction and Its Assessment 567

of severity of injury and is a good predictor of Retrograde amnesia associated with TBI is
eventual recovery (Jennett, 1976; Russell & thought to be quite prevalent (Carlesimo et al.,
Smith, 1961). 1998) but has not been widely studied. In one of
A number of studies have examined the long- the few group studies reported, Carlesimo and
term effect (beyond the episode of PTA) of mod- colleagues (1998) found that 20 patients with
erate-to-severe TBI on learning and memory TBI were impaired relative to 20 healthy control
(for review, see Schacter & Crovitz, 1977; Vakil, subjects in recalling remote autobiographical
2005). In general, these studies have shown that, information and well-known public events and
relative to healthy control subjects or control people. This retrograde amnesia was extensive
subjects with nonbrain traumatic injury (e.g., but did not follow a temporal gradient typical
spinal cord injury), patients with TBI have defi- of medial temporal lobe or diencephalic amne-
cits in both verbal and visual immediate mem- sia. Russell and Nathan (1946) in their classic
ory, a decreased rate of learning across trials, study of memory impairment associated with
and a faster rate of forgetting over a delay inter- head injury make several points about the rela-
val. These findings are illustrated in a study by tionship between anterograde and retrograde
Zec and colleagues (2001), which compared the amnesia. First, on the basis of 972 of cases of
performances of patients with TBI (an average head injury, where information was available
of 10 years post-TBI), healthy control subjects about memory loss, it was found that retrograde
and control subjects with spinal cord injury amnesia was typically brief, covering a period
on a battery of memory tests that included the of less than 30 minutes in 90% of the cases.
Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised (WMS-R) and Second, the longer the anterograde amnesia,
the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT). the longer the retrograde amnesia. Third, retro-
The TBI patients were impaired relative to the grade amnesia was more severe in closed (con-
control groups on the Verbal Memory, Visual cussive) head injury than in gunshot wounds
Memory, General Memory, and Delayed or other cases of penetrating brain injury.
Memory indices of the WMS-R, and on learn- Fourth, during the period of amnesia, neither
ing rate and delayed recall measures from the the anterograde nor retrograde component of
Rey AVLT. Other studies have shown that rate memory loss could be influenced significantly
of learning is impaired in patients with TBI by hypnosis or barbiturate drugs. This latter
when measured by free recall, but normal when finding suggests that the memory impairment
assessed using cued recall procedures (Vakil & has a neurological rather than functional basis
Oded, 2003). Studies using the Buschke-Fuld (e.g., compensation neurosis).
Selective Reminding Test indicate that the
learning deficit may be due to inconsistent recall
Functional Amnesia
(Levin et al., 1979). Thus, inefficient organiza-
tion and learning strategies may be the primary Functional amnesia, also known as dissociative
reason for the learning impairment exhibited by amnesia, is a dissociative psychiatric disorder
patients with TBI (Vakil, 2005). Although faster that involves alterations in consciousness and
forgetting in patients with TBI compared to identity (for review, see Brandt & van Gorp,
controls has been reported in numerous studies 2006). Although no particular brain structure
(e.g., Carlesimo et al., 1997; Crosson et al., 1988; or brain system is implicated in functional
Vakil et al., 1992; Vanderploeg et al., 2001), it amnesia, the cause of the disorder must be due
may not be apparent if groups are equated for to abnormal brain function of some kind. Its
initial level of performance in the immediate presentation varies considerably from individ-
memory condition (DeLuca et al., 2000). These ual to individual, but in most cases functional
latter findings have led some investigators to amnesia is preceded by physical or emotional
suggest that at least a subset of patients with TBI trauma and occurs in association with some
have memory deficits similar to those associ- prior psychiatric history (Kritchevsky et al.,
ated with prefrontal association cortex damage 2004; Schacter et al., 1982). In many cases, the
rather than fundamental anterograde amnesia patient is in a confused or frightened state at
(Vakil, 2005). the time of hospitalization. Functional amnesia
568 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

is usually characterized by severe retrograde


A Cognitive Neuroscience
amnesia, especially for autobiographical infor-
Framework for the Assessment
mation and personal identity. Semantic or
of Memory Disorders
factual knowledge about the world is often pre-
served, even though factual information about Memory is among the most studied and the
the patient’s daily life is unavailable. Despite best understood of the higher cortical functions
profound impairment in the ability to recall (e.g., perception, attention, language, memory,
information about the past, the ability to learn and action). Although still very incomplete,
new information is usually intact. The disorder our understanding of memory has grown sig-
sometimes clears and the lost memories return, nificantly over the past several decades at both
but it can be long lasting with sizeable pieces of the cellular level in terms of cellular events and
the past remaining unavailable. synaptic change and the systems (or neuropsy-
Distinguishing between the “organic” (i.e., chological) level in terms of brain systems and
those based in physical brain damage) and the organization (for review, see Squire & Kandel,
functional amnesias is rather straightforward 2009). Accordingly, a good deal of relevant
because in the “organic” amnesias anterograde information about normal memory, and how it
amnesia is severe, but loss of personal identity is organized in the brain, can be brought to bear
does not occur. The opposite is true in func- when addressing memory disorders from the
tional amnesia (Kopelman, 1995; Nemiah, 1980) perspective of clinical neuropsychology. Two
(see Figure 23–4). Moreover, in “organic” amne- important concepts from cognitive neurosci-
sia retrograde amnesia is usually temporally ence and cognitive psychology that have had a
graded, and public event memory and autobio- large impact on the assessment of memory dis-
graphical memory are similarly affected. In orders are the identification of neurologically
functional amnesia, retrograde memory func- distinct memory systems and the idea that there
tion is determined more by memory content are psychologically distinct component pro-
than by when the information was acquired. cesses involved in remembering information.

30 80
Percent of total memories recalled

Controls (N=5)
FA (N=8)
Mean score for 10 items

60
20

40

10
* 20
*
0 0
No probe Probe No probe Probe 0–9 10 months 11–20 years >20 years
months –10 years
FA (N=9) Controls (N=5)
Age of memory

Figure 23–4. Patients with functional amnesia (FA) were impaired relative to
healthy controls in their ability to recollect 10 autobiographical episodes (left panel).
The quality of the recollections were scored (0 to 3 points) before (No Probe) and after
(Probe) encouragement to produce as specific a recollection as possible (left panels;
black circles indicate performance of individual subjects). In addition, patients with
FA recalled most of their memories (i.e., those scored 3) from recent time periods (0–9
months after the onset of amnesia) and very few from the more distant past (right
panel). This pattern is opposite to the temporal gradient often observed in patients
with “organic” amnesia. (Adapted from Kritchevsky et al., 2004).
The Neuropsychology of Memory Dysfunction and Its Assessment 569

Memory Systems One organizational scheme for memory that


has generated considerable theoretical and
One of the major contributions of cognitive empirical interest is the distinction between
neuroscience to the study of memory dysfunc- declarative (or explicit) and nondeclarative
tion and its assessment is the recognition that (or implicit or procedural) forms of memory
memory is not a unitary process mediated by (Cohen & Squire, 1980; Squire & Zola, 1996;
a single neuroanatomical system, but rather a see Figure 23–5). Declarative memory refers
compilation of various subsystems that differ to knowledge of episodes and facts that can be
from one another in their rules of operation, in consciously recalled and related (i.e., declared)
the types of information they process, and in by the rememberer. It has been characterized
their neuroanatomical substrates (for review, as “knowing that” and includes such things
see Squire, 2004). These memory subsystems as memory for the words on a recently pre-
interact but are thought to be relatively inde- sented list and knowledge that a dog is an ani-
pendent and can be differentially impaired by mal. Nondeclarative memory, in contrast, is
disease or injury. Investigations of memory described as “knowing how” and pertains to
in impaired patient populations have served an unconscious form of remembering that is
not only to validate this viewpoint but also to expressed only through the performance of the
identify the neuroanatomical structures asso- specific operations comprising a particular task.
ciated with different memory systems. Based The use of nondeclarative memory is indicated
on these studies, neuropsychological models of by the performance of a newly acquired motor,
memory have been developed that provided a perceptual, or cognitive skill, for example, and
powerful conceptual framework for the clinical by the unconscious facilitation in processing a
assessment and classification of the distinctive stimulus that occurs following its previous pre-
patterns of memory impairment observed in sentation (i.e., priming).
different neuropsychiatric disorders.

Long-term memory

Declarative Nondeclarative
(Explicit) (Implicit)

Facts Events Skills & Priming & Simple Nonassociative


habits perceptual classical learning
learning conditioning

Emotional Skeletal

Medial temporal lobe Neocortex Cerebellum


diencephalon Striatum Amygdala Reflex
pathways

Figure 23–5. Classification of memory. Declarative (explicit) memory refers to conscious rec-
ollection of facts and events. Nondeclarative (implicit) memory refers to a heterogeneous col-
lection of abilities that are expressed through performance rather than recollection. Experience
alters behavior nonconsciously without providing access to memory content. (Adapted from
Squire & Zola, 1996).
570 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

The distinction that has been drawn between affected by disease or injury in specific brain
declarative and nondeclarative memory is based regions. Research in this area involves the exam-
not only on conceptual grounds but also on the ination of the component processes used in per-
dissociation of these two forms of memory in forming episodic memory tasks (e.g., encoding,
patients with circumscribed amnesia. Despite storage, retrieval) and the relationship between
severe deficits in declarative memory, amne- episodic memory and other cognitive func-
sic patients demonstrate relatively normal lex- tions (e.g., “executive” functions or seman-
ical and semantic priming (Graf et al., 1984; tic memory). Two patients who superficially
Levy et al., 2004; Shimamura, 1986; Tulving appear to display equivalent memory impair-
& Schacter, 1990) and a preserved ability to ments according to a standardized test can have
learn and retain a variety of motor, perceptual, strikingly different processing deficits under-
and cognitive skills (e.g., Brooks & Baddeley, lying their impairments. For example, Butters
1976; Cohen & Squire, 1980; Corkin, 1968; for and colleagues (1985) showed that impairment
review, see Squire, 2004). The preservation of in different processes may underlie the memory
nondeclarative memory in patients with cir- deficits associated with circumscribed amnesia
cumscribed amnesia indicates that this form of and Huntington’s disease (HD), a genetic neu-
memory does not rely on the same medial tem- rodegenerative disorder producing basal gan-
poral and diencephalic brain structures that glia atrophy that results in a movement disorder
are thought to mediate declarative memory. (i.e., chorea) and dementia.
Various forms of nondeclarative memory may Compared to normal control subjects,
be mediated by diverse brain structures, such as patients with HD and patients with amnesia
the neostriatum, the amygdala, the cerebellum, were equally impaired on a word list-learning
and the neocortex, structures that are directly task when free recall was required, but patients
involved in the initial processing and perfor- with HD were significantly less impaired than
mance of particular tasks (Squire, 2004). the amnesic patients when memory was tested
Declarative memory has been further dichot- in a recognition format (see Figure 23–6). These
omized into episodic and semantic forms results suggest that the memory deficit in HD
(Tulving, 1983). Episodic memory refers to the patients may strongly involve an inability to
storage and recollection of temporally dated initiate appropriate and systematic retrieval
autobiographical events that depend upon tem- strategies (the need for which is reduced by rec-
poral and/or spatial contextual cues for their ognition testing procedures), whereas the deficit
retrieval. Examples of episodic memory include in the amnesic patients, which is not ameliorated
the ability to recall activities from the previous by recognition procedures, may involve prob-
day and the ability to remember learning a list lems with encoding and consolidation. Indeed,
of words presented 10 minutes earlier. Semantic a number of studies have shown that patients
memory, in contrast, refers to our general fund with medial temporal lobe amnesia have sim-
of knowledge, which consists of overlearned ilar degrees of impairment (relative to healthy
facts and concepts that are not dependent upon controls) on both recall and recognition tasks
contextual cues for retrieval. Knowledge of the consistent with an encoding/consolidation def-
meanings of words, the ability to name com- icit (Kopelman et al., 2007; Manns et al., 2003;
mon objects, or to recollect well-known geo- Wixted & Squire, 2004). As these examples
graphical, historical, and arithmetical facts are show, experimental neuropsychological studies
all examples of semantic memory. designed within the theoretical paradigms of
cognitive psychology have yielded substantial
insights into the processes involved in memory
Memory Processes
deficits. With these successes in mind, the con-
Another major contribution from cognitive psy- struction of a number of standardized memory
chology and neuroscience to the neuropsycho- tests such as the California Verbal Learning
logical assessment of memory disorders is the Test (Delis et al., 1987), the Selective Reminding
notion that “memory” consists of a number of Tests (Buschke & Fuld, 1974), and the Wechsler
component processes that can be differentially Memory Scale-Revised and Wechsler Memory
The Neuropsychology of Memory Dysfunction and Its Assessment 571

Free recall Recognition


14 8
NC
HD
12 7
Mean number of words recalled

AMN

Mean d’measure of recognition


6
10
5
8
4
6
3
4
2

2 1

0 0
1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5
Trials Trials

Figure 23–6. The performance of patients with circumscribed amnesia (AMN), patients with
Huntington’s disease (HD), and normal control (NC) subjects on the free-recall (left panel) and
recognition (right panel) components of the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test. The HD and AMN
patients were similarly impaired in the recall condition, but the HD patients were less impaired than
the AMN patients in the recognition condition. This finding suggests that ineffective retrieval (as
opposed to encoding or storage deficits) plays a greater role in the memory impairment of the HD
patients than in the patients with amnesia. (Adapted from Butters et al., 1985).

Scale-III (Wechsler, 1987, 1997) have been influ- they have the same tendencies that we all have
enced by current cognitive models of memory. (but against the backdrop of reduced retentive
In the next section, we illustrate how currently capacity) to remember more reliably things that
available standardized memory tests can be seem important compared to things that seem
used to identify distinct processing deficits. trivial.
When objectively assessing a memory dis-
Neuropsychological Assessment order, two general considerations apply: (1) the
of Memory Dysfunction: General need to exceed immediate memory capacity that
Considerations remains normal in patients with circumscribed
amnesia, and (2) the possibility of a material-
Formal neuropsychological testing of memory specific deficit in memory that could arise from
permits one to compare objectively the scores lateralized brain damage. These considerations
of a given patient to a known group average so are discussed below.
that the scope and severity of memory dysfunc-
tion can be ascertained. Objective testing with
standardized procedures also protects against
Exceeding Immediate Memory Capacity
the natural tendency to rationalize, minimize,
or forget clinical observations that do not con- The critical feature of memory tasks that
form to expectation, or to be influenced by a accounts for their sensitivity to amnesia is that
patient’s ability to remember information that information presented to the patient exceeds
might be particularly salient to them. People immediate memory capacity. Even severely
with memory dysfunction exercise the same amnesic patients can have a normal digit span
denial, suppression, and selection that we all are and a normal ability to report back the rela-
subject to while learning and remembering, and tively small amount of information that can be
572 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

maintained in “conscious awareness.” William rehearsal and to reveal a deficit if one is present.
James (1890) termed this capacity “primary If the delay is short (e.g., seconds or minutes),
memory”: a formal distraction procedure is needed to
prevent rehearsal and expose a deficit. The fol-
An object in primary memory . . . was never lost;
its date was never cut off in consciousness from lowing observation of patient H.M. makes this
that of the immediately present moment. In fact it point:
comes to us as belonging to the rearward portion Forgetting occurred the instant his focus of attention
of the present space of time, and not to the genuine shifted, but in the absence of distraction his capacity
past . . . Secondary memory, as it might be styled, is for sustained attention was remarkable. Thus he was
the knowledge of a former state of mind after it had able to retain the number 584 for at least 15 minutes,
already once dropped from consciousness . . . It is by continuously working out mnemonic schemes.
brought back, recalled, fished up, so to speak, from When asked how he had been able to retain the num-
a reservoir in which, with countless other objects, it ber for so long, he replied: “It’s easy. You just remem-
lay buried and lost from view. (pp. 646–648) ber 8. You see, 5, 8, and 4, add to 17. You remember
This concept of primary memory remains 8, subtract it from 17 and it leaves 9. Divide 9 in half
quite useful in understanding the nature of the and you get 5 and 4, and there you are: 584. Easy.”
(Milner, 1970, p. 37)
memory impairment in amnesia. An interest-
ing study of five amnesic patients, including Given that “primary memory” is preserved in
H.M. (Drachman & Arbit, 1966), illustrates patients with circumscribed amnesia, it is easy
this point in a formal way. Patients and control to understand that the hallmark of the disorder
subjects were asked to repeat back digit strings is considered to be impaired performance on
of increasing length until an error occurred. A tests of delayed recall (with interpolated dis-
string of digits of the same length was then given traction). These tests form the cornerstone of
repeatedly until it was reproduced correctly or any thorough neuropsychological assessment of
until 25 repetitions of the same digit string had memory functions.
been given. Each time a correct response was
given, a new string of digits was presented that
Material-Specific Memory Dysfunction
was one digit longer than the preceding string.
and Its Assessment
With this procedure, normal subjects were able
to increase their digit span to at least 20 digits. In addition to addressing the issue of severity,
Amnesic patients, however, had great difficulty objective neuropsychological testing of memory
once their digit span capacity (i.e., the digit must also address the fact that memory dys-
string length when their first error occurred) function can differ depending on whether the
had been reached. Even after 25 repetitions underlying neurological injury or disease is
of the same digit string, H.M. was unable to bilateral or left or right unilateral. The effect on
increase his digit span by one digit beyond his memory follows from the asymmetry of hemi-
primary memory capacity of six digits. spheric function with respect to language: verbal
As this example shows, the performance of impairment from left-sided damage, nonverbal
amnesic patients on tests involving immedi- impairment from right-sided damage, “global”
ate recall depends on whether the amount of impairment from bilateral damage. This point
information to be remembered exceeds “pri- has been best demonstrated in the thorough
mary memory” or “immediate memory capac- work of Brenda Milner on temporal lobe func-
ity.” It should also be noted that even when the tion (Milner, 1958, 1971). She observed that
amount of information to be remembered is patients with left medial temporal lobe resec-
within immediate memory capacity, memory tions (which were carried out to relieve epilepsy)
performance will be poor if active rehearsal is complain that they cannot remember what they
prevented by interposing a delay fi lled with dis- have read, and they do poorly on verbal memory
traction between learning and retention testing tests such as delayed recall of short prose pas-
(Cowan et al., 2004). If the delay is very long sages. Patients who have sustained right medial
(e.g., an hour or more), the natural distrac- temporal lobe resections, in contrast, complain
tion of ongoing activity is sufficient to prevent that they do not remember where they have put
The Neuropsychology of Memory Dysfunction and Its Assessment 573

things, and do poorly on tests of memory for clustering strategies so that these abilities can
faces, spatial relationships, and other stimuli be assessed. Several of the more common verbal
that are not readily encoded in words. Milner memory tests that employ these techniques are
termed these conditions material-specific dis- described below.
orders (Milner, 1968) to signify that the side of
the brain that is affected determines the kind of
Delayed Recall of Prose Materials
material that is difficult to learn and remember.
Interestingly, material-specific memory disor- There are several formal neuropsychological
ders are ordinarily not dependent on the sen- tests of delayed recall that require memory of
sory modality through which material is learned prose passages. In general, patients are asked in
(e.g., auditory, visual, or tactile). For example, in these tasks to repeat back a short story immedi-
the case of left medial temporal lobe injury, a ately after hearing it and then again after some
short prose passage will be difficult to remem- delay (Milner, 1958). One of the most widely
ber regardless of whether the patient reads the used of these tasks is the Logical Memory Test
story or hears it read. from the Wechsler Memory Scales (WMS;
These material-specific effects have been dem- WMS-Revised; WMS-III). The Logical Memory
onstrated for left or right temporal lobe surgical Test (as used in the WMS-R; Wechsler, 1987) is
lesions (Milner, 1971); epileptic foci of the left a standardized test that assesses free recall of
or right temporal lobe (Delaney et al., 1980); left two short stories, which are read aloud to the
or right unilateral ECT (in which the two elec- patient. Each story contains 25 bits of informa-
trodes are applied to the same side of the head, tion. In the immediate recall condition, the two
in contrast to bilateral ECT, in which one elec- stories are presented sequentially with free recall
trode is applied to each temple) (Cohen et al., elicited after each presentation. Free recall of the
1968); unilateral diencephalic lesions (Michel two stories is elicited again after a 30-minute
et al., 1982; Speedie & Heilman, 1982; Squire period filled with unrelated testing. The total
et al., 1989); and unilateral diencephalic brain numbers of bits of information recalled imme-
stimulation (Ojemann, 1971). The existence of diately and after the delay interval are recorded.
material specific memory deficits indicates the A “savings” score that reflects consolidation (or
need for careful assessment of both verbal and conversely, forgetting) is computed by dividing
nonverbal memory functions in patients with the total score achieved during delayed recall
memory disorders. by the total score achieved during immediate
recall (Troster et al., 1989).
Tests of delayed recall of prose passages are
Neuropsychological Assessment
quite sensitive to verbal memory deficits. This
of Anterograde Amnesia: Verbal
principle is illustrated in a study that assessed
Memory Tests
the performance of 15 patients prescribed a
A number of techniques have been developed to series of bilateral ECT using a test of this type
assess verbal memory dysfunction. These tech- (not the Wechsler Memory Scale) with imme-
niques include tasks that require the recall of diate and 24-hour delayed recall (reported in
meaningful prose passages, word list-learning Weiner et al., 1984) (see Figure 23–7). Memory
tasks, and verbal paired-associates learning was assessed before ECT and then again, with
tasks. Many of these tasks were developed with an alternate form, 6–10 hours after the fift h
an eye toward the cognitive neuroscience princi- treatment of the series. Results showed that
ples of memory function discussed above. Thus, ECT had no effect on immediate recall of the
they often include both immediate and delayed prose passage, and by this measure one might
recall components so that consolidation and have supposed memory functions to be nor-
forgetting can be assessed. They may include mal. However, delayed recall tests showed that
both free-recall and recognition components memory functions were markedly impaired.
so that retrieval and encoding/consolidation Whereas patients tested before their prescribed
deficits can be distinguished, and they may be series of ECT showed considerable retention 24
structured to encourage semantic encoding and hours after hearing the prose passage, only 2 of
574 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

15 patients could recall any part of the prose Word List-Learning Tasks
passage after a delay following ECT. The abil-
One of the most powerful techniques for detect-
ity to learn and retain prose material recovered
ing verbal memory dysfunction is the word
6–9 months after the completion of ECT treat-
list-learning task. These tasks are particularly
ment, as shown in a separate group of patients
effective when they are designed to assess com-
(see Figure 23–7).
ponent processes involved in remembering.
The pattern of deficits exhibited by patients
Thus, many of the most effective word list-learn-
on the prose recall test immediately after ECT
ing tasks incorporate stimulus lists or proce-
(i.e., impaired delayed recall with unimpaired
dures that encourage deep semantic encoding,
immediate recall) helps to distinguish the
allow examination of semantic or serial clus-
“organic” memory disorders from memory dis-
tering during recall, assess learning over trials
orders caused by depression. Rapid (i.e., greater
as well as short-delay and long-delay recall, and
than normal) forgetting revealed by contrast-
contrast free recall with cued recall or recogni-
ing immediate and delayed recall is a sensitive
tion in order to separate encoding/storage and
marker of neurological dysfunction, whereas
retrieval deficits. There are a large number of
depression affects immediate memory (pre-
standardized word list-learning tests that are
sumably because patients are preoccupied or
used in the neuropsychological assessment of
inattentive) but does not affect delayed recall
memory, including the Rey Auditory Verbal
beyond what would be expected from the level
Learning Test (AVLT; Rey, 1964), the California
of immediate recall. That is, depression is not
Verbal Learning Test (CVLT; Delis et al., 1987),
associated with rapid forgetting (Cronholm &
the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test (HVLT;
Ottosson, 1961; Sternberg & Jarvik, 1976).
Brandt, 1991), the CERAD Word-List-Learning
Test (Welsh et al., 1991), the Selective Reminding
Test (Buschke & Fuld, 1974), the Free and Cued
Selective Reminding Test (Grober et al., 1997),
8 and the Double Memory Test (Buschke et al.,
6 months 1997), among others. Several of the most widely
Mean number of segments recalled

7
Before ECT used of these tests are described below.
6 After ECT

5 Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (Rey, 1964). The


AVLT is a list-learning task that assesses multi-
4
ple cognitive parameters associated with learn-
3 ing and memory. On each of 5 learning trials,
15 unrelated words (all nouns) are presented
2 orally at the rate of one word per second and
1 immediate free recall of the words is elicited
after each list presentation. The number of cor-
0 rectly recalled words on each trial is recorded.
No delay 1 day 2 weeks
Following a 20-minute delay filled with unre-
Figure 23–7. Delayed recall of a short prose pas- lated testing, free recall of the original 15-word
sage by psychiatric patients administered bilateral list is elicited. Finally, a yes/no recognition test is
electroconvulsive therapy (ECT) for depressive ill- administered using a new word list. After each
ness. Testing occurred before any treatment (n = 12), of 5 list presentations, the original 15 words and
6–10 hours after the fi ft h treatment (n = 15), and, for 15 interspersed distracter words are presented.
a different group of 16 patients, 6–9 months after The number of target “hits” and false positive
treatment. Patients were severely impaired at retain- responses are recorded. The Rey AVLT has been
ing information over a delay when they were tested used in a number of studies that demonstrate the
during the course of treatment, but this ability had severity of memory impairment in patients with
recovered by 6 months after treatment. (Adapted amnesia (e.g., Janowsky et al., 1989a; Squire &
from Squire & Chace, 1975). Shimamura, 1986; see Figure 23–3).
The Neuropsychology of Memory Dysfunction and Its Assessment 575

The California Verbal Learning Test recall and cued-recall tests are repeated for
(Delis et al., 1987). The California Verbal List A, followed by a yes/no recognition test
Learning Test (CVLT) is a standardized ver- consisting of the 16 List A items and 28 ran-
bal memory test that was explicitly devel- domly interspersed distractor items. Using this
oped to assess a variety of memory processes procedure, the CVLT assesses and provides
identified in cognitive psychological studies multiple indices for rate of learning, retention
of normal memory. In the test, subjects are after short- and long-delay intervals, seman-
given five presentations/free-recall trials for a tic encoding ability, recognition (i.e., discrim-
list (List A) of 16 words (4 words in each of 4 inability), intrusion and perseverative errors
semantic categories) and are then given a sin- (i.e., susceptibility to proactive interference),
gle interference trial for a second, different list and response biases. Performance profi les for
(List B) of 16 items. Immediately after the List amnesic patients with Korsakoff ’s syndrome,
B trial, subjects are given a free-recall test and patients with Alzheimer’s disease, and patients
then a cued-recall test (utilizing the names of with Huntington’s disease have been identified
the four categories) for the items on the initial (Delis et al., 1991) (see Figure 23–8). The CVLT
list (List A). Twenty minutes later, the free- was recently updated (as the CVLT-II; Delis

Initial learning Free recall Recognition


0

–1
Mean Z-score

–2

–3 *

–4

–5 KOR
AD
HD
50
100

*
Mean free recall intrusions

40
Mean delayed recall savings

80

60 30

40 20

*
20 10

0 0

Figure 23–8. The pattern of performance on the California Verbal Learning Test exhibited by amnesic patients
with Korsakoff ’s syndrome (KOR; n = 8), patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD; n = 20), and patients with
Huntington’s disease (HD; n = 20). The three groups performed similarly on the initial learning and free-recall
measures, but HD patients exhibited significantly (*) less impaired recognition (i.e., discriminability), higher
savings, and fewer free-recall intrusions than the other two groups. The AD and KOR patients performed simi-
larly on all of the measures. (Adapted from Delis et al., 1991).
576 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

et al., 2000) with new word categories, more perhaps the most sensitive is paired-associates
accurate indices of recognition memory, and learning. Paired-associates learning tests have
expanded normative data. been used experimentally for many years and
standardized versions have been incorporated
into clinical memory test batteries (e.g., all
Selective Reminding Test (Buschke & Fuld,
versions of the Wechsler Memory Scale). In
1974). The selective reminding test is a verbal
general, these tests involve the initial presen-
list-learning task that provides information on
tation of pairs of semantically related or unre-
storage, retention, and retrieval. It was one of
lated words with the instructions to remember
the fi rst standardized verbal memory tests to
which words go together. Immediately follow-
be designed on the basis of memory processes
ing presentation of the list of word pairs, or
identified in experimental cognitive psycho-
after a delay fi lled with unrelated testing, the
logical studies of normal memory. On the ini-
fi rst word of each pair is presented and the
tial trial of a widely used version of the task,
patient must generate the word with which it
patients are read 10 unrelated words and then
was paired. The sensitivity of verbal paired-
asked for immediate recall of the entire list. On
associates learning tests to memory impair-
the second trial, patients are read only those
ment is illustrated in a study (Janowsky et al.,
words they failed to recall on the fi rst trial (i.e.,
1989a) that had three kinds of patients with
selectively reminded) and again are asked for
memory problems attempt to learn 10 noun-
recall of the entire list. Th is same procedure,
noun word pairs (e.g., army-table, door-sky).
presenting only those words not recalled on
In this case, three consecutive presentations
the preceding trial, is followed for a total of
of the word pairs were given, and after each
six trials. With this procedure and the scor-
presentation subjects were asked to try to pro-
ing methods described by the test authors,
duce the second word of the pair upon hearing
it is possible to derive measures of the num-
the fi rst. As Figure 23–9 shows, patients with
ber of items in short-term storage (i.e., items
alcoholic Korsakoff ’s syndrome and other
recalled in the trial immediately after remind-
amnesic patients all performed poorly on this
ing), the number of items in long-term storage
test, obtaining an average score of less than
(i.e., items recalled even without reminding
four correct responses across all 30 learn-
in the immediately preceding trial), random
ing trials. Patients with frontal lobe lesions
retrieval (i.e., items recalled inconsistently),
and control subjects performed much better.
and the total number of items recalled during
In this same test, patient H.M. was unable to
testing. In addition, the patient’s performance
produce any correct responses after three tri-
may be analyzed for intrusion errors (i.e., the
als, even when he was instructed in the use of
production of words that were not on the list)
imagery techniques for associating the words
that might be indicative of susceptibility to
in each pair (Jones, 1974). The sensitivity of
proactive interference. Several additional tests
this test derives from the fact that the mate-
using selective reminding procedures have
rial to be remembered is too extensive to be
been developed, including the Free and Cued
retained within immediate memory, the cor-
Selective Reminding Test (Grober et al., 1997),
rect associations between the various words
which uses a semantic encoding procedure
must be retained, and the test requires recall
that has patients identify the semantic cate-
rather than recognition of the paired item. A
gory of each to-be-remembered item during
drawback to using paired-associates learn-
list presentation, and then uses the category
ing, however, is that poor performance could
name as a cue at the time of recall (in addition
result from factors other than circumscribed
to uncued free recall).
memory loss (e.g., depression, inattention, or
dementia). Accordingly, the greatest value of
the test may be to rule out memory impair-
Verbal Paired-Associates Learning
ment in persons who perform well. If perfor-
Among tests specialized for the detection mance is poor, then further tests are needed to
and quantification of memory dysfunction, interpret the poor performance.
The Neuropsychology of Memory Dysfunction and Its Assessment 577

Paired associate learning The Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure


25
(Osterrieth, 1944; Rey, 1941). In this task
the subject is asked to copy the Rey–Osterrieth
20 Complex Figure and then to reconstruct it from
Score (3 trials)

memory without forewarning. The reconstruc-


15
tion can be requested immediately or after a
10 delay fi lled with unrelated (preferably verbal)
testing. Alternate forms of the figure and var-
5
ious standardized scoring systems have been
0 developed (Lezak et al., 2004; Milner & Teuber,
KOR AMN F ALC CON F-CON 1968). The sensitivity of the Rey–Osterrieth
Complex Figure test to amnesia is shown in the
Figure 23–9. The performance of patients with
poor performance of patient R.B. discussed ear-
Korsakoff ’s syndrome (KOR; n = 7), other patients
lier (see Figure 23–1).
with amnesia (AMN; n = 5), patients with frontal
lobe lesions (F; n = 7), and their controls (alcoholic
Visual Reproduction Test (Wechsler,
controls, ALC; n = 8, healthy controls, CON; n = 8,
1945, 1987, 1997). A version of the Visual
and frontal controls, F-CON; n = 11, respectively)
Reproduction Test is included in all editions of
on a test of paired-associates learning that required
the Wechsler Memory Scales. On each of the
learning 10 noun-noun word pairs across three
three trials of the original version of this test
trials. The amnesic patients were severely impaired.
(i.e., in the WMS), the subject must reproduce a
(Adapted from Janowsky et al., 1989a).
complex geometric figure from memory imme-
diately following a 10-second study period.
Three increasingly complex stimuli containing
from 4 to 10 components are presented on suc-
Neuropsychological Assessment of cessive trials. The subject’s reproductions are
Anterograde Amnesia: Nonverbal scored according to how many components are
Memory Tests produced from the original stimulus drawings.
Two classes of nonverbal memory tests can be To increase the sensitivity of the test to memory
used in clinical assessment: those that require dysfunction, two modifications of the original
retention and free recall of geometric designs version of the test are often incorporated. First,
that are drawn or reconstructed by the patient as a measure of long-term retention, subjects
and those that require recognition of previ- are asked after 30 minutes of unrelated testing
ously presented visual stimuli such as nonsense to again reproduce the figures from memory.
figures or unfamiliar faces or objects. Second, the subject is asked to simply copy the
stimulus figures in order to assess any visuoper-
ceptual dysfunction or apraxia that may be con-
Delayed Recall of Nonverbal Material taminating the ability to reproduce the figures
from memory. The delayed recall measure from
There are a number of tests of nonverbal mem- the Visual Reproduction Test has been shown
ory that require the patient to draw or recon- to be quite sensitive to memory impairment in
struct relatively complex geometric designs that cases of circumscribed amnesia (Troster et al.,
had been studied earlier. The integrity of the 1989) and dementia (Salmon et al., 2002), and
reproduced design is usually scored in terms of this measure was incorporated into the newer
correctly completed components. Three of the versions of the test that are included in the WMS-
most widely used tests of free recall of nonver- Revised and the WMS-III. These latter versions
bal material are the Rey–Osterrieth Complex also modified the number and design of the
Figure Test, the Visual Reproduction Test from to-be-remembered geometric figures.
the Wechsler Memory Scales (WMS, WMS-
Revised, WMS-III), and the Benton Visual Benton Visual Retention Test. This test con-
Retention Test. sists of a series of 10 stimulus cards that contain
578 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

geometric figures that must be remembered and memory tests is the memory for faces compo-
drawn by the patient. The first two stimulus nent of the Recognition Memory Test.
cards contain a single figure, and the remain-
ing eight cards contain three figures (generally Recognition Memory Test (RMT; Warrington,
two large and one small) arranged in a row. In 1984). This test consists of two forced-choice
the initial administration format (Form A) each recognition tests—one for words and one for
stimulus card is presented for 10 seconds, and faces. Memory for words and faces are tested
drawing begins immediately after its removal. separately. For each test, 50 stimuli are pre-
The second administration format (Form B) is sented sequentially, at the rate of 3 seconds
identical to the first (with a different set of fig- each, and the patient must make a judgment of
ures), but each stimulus card is presented for “pleasant” or “unpleasant” to assure attention
only 5 seconds. The third administration format and encoding. Immediately following the study
(Form C) requires the patient to simply copy the phase, each stimulus is paired with a new one,
figures on each stimulus card (again with a dif- and subjects are asked to indicate which of the
ferent set of figures). In the final administration two was presented earlier. The score is the num-
format (Form D), each stimulus card is presented ber correct out of 50 for both words and faces.
for 10 seconds, and drawing begins 15 seconds The RMT is useful for assessing different levels
after its removal. The score for each condition is of severity among memory-impaired patients,
the number of correctly reproduced three-figure and an even more sensitive assessment can be
displays (for a maximum of 10 points). An error obtained by administering the memory test one
score can also be computed on the basis of the day after the study phase (Reed & Squire, 1997;
presence of six error types: omissions, distor- Squire & Shimamura, 1986; see Figure 23–10).
tions, perseverations, rotations, misplacements, Comparison of the faces and words conditions
and incorrect size. Extensive normative data for allows evaluation of potential material-specific
the Benton Visual Retention Test are available memory deficits (Warrington, 1984). A modi-
(Mitrushina et al., 2005). fied version of the faces component of the RMT
is included in the WMS-III. In this version,
there are 24 target faces and both immediate and
Delayed Recognition of Nonverbal
30-minute delayed recognition is assessed using
Material
a yes/no format rather than the two-alternative
Nonverbal memory tests that require patients forced choice format of the RMT.
to draw or reconstruct geometric designs can
be adversely influenced by visuoconstructive
Neuropsychological Assessment
deficits (i.e., apraxia) or other nonmemory
of Anterograde Amnesia: Memory
problems that preclude drawing. To avoid this
Test Batteries
potential problem, some tests require delayed
recognition of nonverbal stimuli. These rec- A number of comprehensive memory test bat-
ognition tests have been independently devel- teries have been developed and standardized
oped as new instruments or have evolved from for clinical assessment of memory dysfunc-
the free-recall tests described above with the tion (for review, see Lezak et al., 2004). These
addition of a delayed recognition component batteries usually include an array of tests that
in which each target stimulus must be chosen evaluate various memory functions such as ori-
from among an array of similar foils. Examples entation, learning, and retention, while taking
of the latter include recognition versions of into account the possibility of modality-specific
the Visual Reproduction Test and the Benton deficits. A benefit of standardized test batteries
Visual Retention Test (Amieva et al., 2006). for clinical use is that they are often grounded
Other nonverbal recognition memory tests in extensive normative data that allow the pres-
require identification of faces (Warrington, ence and severity of memory impairment to be
1984) or unfamiliar objects (e.g., front doors of reliably identified across the age span, and they
houses; Baddeley et al., 1994). One of the most may provide index scores that allow straight-
widely used freestanding nonverbal recognition forward comparisons among various aspects of
The Neuropsychology of Memory Dysfunction and Its Assessment 579

50

LJ
45
GD LM
Mean number correct LM PH
LJ
40 AB
WH

PH
35 GD
PH
LJ
PH LM
AB LM AB
AB
30 WH LJ
WH GD

25 GD
WH

20
Words Faces Words Faces
immed immed delay delay

Figure 23–10. The performance of amnesic patients (open bars) and


healthy control subjects (dark bars) on the Words and Faces components of
the Warrington Recognition Memory Test. Recognition memory was tested
immediately (Immed) and in separate tests after a 24-hour delay (Delay).
The means (+SEM) are indicated by the bars (maximum possible score =
50), and individual scores of the amnesic patients are indicated by dots and
their initials. (Adapted from Reed & Squire 1997).

memory performance and general intellectual it became possible and popular to evaluate the
abilities. Two of the most widely used memory severity of memory dysfunction in a patient by
test batteries are the Wechsler Memory Scale comparing their MQ and IQ scores. In both
and its various updates (Wechsler, 1945, 1987, clinical and experimental analyses of memory
1997) and the Rivermeade Behavioral Memory dysfunction, a large IQ-MQ difference (usually
Test Battery (deWall et al., 1994; Wilson et al., a difference greater than 15 or 20 points) was
1989). taken as evidence of a significant and selective
amnesic disorder.
As knowledge of the nature of amnesia grew
The Wechsler Memory Scale
from experimental studies (e.g., Butters &
The original Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS; Cermak, 1980; Squire & Shimamura, 1986), it
Wechsler, 1945) was among the first standard- became apparent that the WMS was inadequate
ized memory batteries used to clinically assess for characterizing memory disorders. The scale
memory function. The WMS included seven was limited because (1) scores were combined
distinct subtests that measured general orienta- into one general MQ score that masked disso-
tion, immediate memory, and verbal and non- ciations in various memory functions, (2) there
verbal long-term memory. A memory quotient was a preponderance of verbal memory tests,
(MQ) could be calculated on the basis of per- (3) there were no measures of delayed mem-
formance scores for the various subtests. The ory retention, and (4) some subtests measured
MQ was designed to parallel the IQ from the functions not typically affected in amnesia
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) and, (e.g., digit span memory and general orien-
therefore, was standardized in the normal pop- tation). The Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised
ulation to a mean of 100 with a standard devi- (WMS-R) was introduced in 1987 with the
ation of 15. With the development of the MQ intention of overcoming some of the limitations
580 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

of the original test battery (Wechsler, 1987). The several shortcomings remained. For example,
WMS-R consists of 13 subtests, which are com- the WMS-R does not provide index scores that
bined in various ways to produce five indices: fall below 50, and many amnesic patients would
Attention/Concentration, General Memory, obtain scores below this level, particularly on
Verbal Memory, Visual Memory, and Delayed the Delayed Recall Index. Thus, the WMS-R is
Recall. Each index is normalized to yield a mean not suited for discriminating among patients
score of 100 for normal subjects and a standard with severe memory impairment. In addition,
deviation of 15. Moreover, for each verbal sub- the WMS-R does not test memory in a recog-
test (e.g., digit span, prose recall, verbal paired- nition format, and that prevents a comparison
associates learning), there is a corresponding of retrieval versus encoding/storage processes
nonverbal subtest (e.g., visual memory span, by measuring the difference between free-recall
visual reproduction, visual paired-associates and recognition test performance.
learning). Several verbal and nonverbal tests are The newest version of the WMS, the WMS-
administered after a delay of about 30 minutes. III (Wechsler, 1997), was developed to address
The WMS-R has significant advantages over these shortcomings and to provide a balanced
its predecessor and it provides perhaps the approach to memory assessment. While adding
best general assessment of memory dysfunc- measures of recognition memory, the WMS-
tion among readily available test instruments. III reduced the number of core subtests to six
Among its advantages are distinct indices for (Logical Memory, Verbal Paired-Associates,
verbal and nonverbal memory performance Spatial Span, Letter-Number Sequencing, Face
that can elucidate material-specific defi- Recognition Memory, and Family Pictures) and
cits in patients with unilateral brain damage made other subtests optional (Information and
(Chelune & Bornstein, 1988), a delayed memory Orientation, Mental Control, Digit Span, Visual
index that is particularly sensitive to the rapid Reproduction, Word List Learning patterned
forgetting that characterizes many amnesic after the Rey AVLT). Eight primary memory
disorders (Butters et al., 1988), and the devel- indices that have an expanded lower range can
opment of a separate Attention/Concentration now be generated: Auditory Immediate Memory,
index that incorporates performance scores Visual Immediate Memory, Immediate Memory,
for subtests that do not depend on the kind of Auditory Delayed Memory, Visual Delayed
memory that is impaired in amnesia (digit span, Memory, Auditory Recognition Memory,
visuospatial span, and mental control). Indeed, General Memory, and Working Memory (for-
Janowsky and colleagues (1989a) found that merly Attention/Concentration). It should be
amnesic patients exhibited normal scores on the noted that the change from verbal memory
Attention/Concentration index of the WMS-R, indices in the WMS-R to auditory memory
despite severe impairment on the scale’s mem- indices in the WMS-III shifts the focus of these
ory indices. Patients with frontal lobe lesions, in measures from ones that purportedly measure
contrast, exhibited low scores on the Attention/ material-specific memory (with a correspond-
Concentration index but normal scores on ing implication for clinical/anatomical correla-
memory indices (Janowsky et al., 1989a). The tions) to ones that measure modality-specific
WMS-R has also been useful in characteriz- memory (based on the sensory modality of the
ing dementia. Patients with Alzheimer’s disease stimulus presentation) (Lezak et al., 2004). As
or Huntington’s disease exhibited significant mentioned previously, material-specific mem-
impairment on the Attention/Concentration ory disorders are ordinarily not dependent on
index as well as on the memory indices, whereas the sensory modality through which material is
patients with amnesic disorders (e.g., patients learned (e.g., auditory, visual, or tactile) (Milner,
with Korsakoff ’s syndrome, hypoxia, or herpes 1968). Because of this change, and because sev-
encephalitis) exhibited significant impairment eral sensitive measures have been given optional
only on the memory indices (Butters et al., status (e.g., Visual Reproductions) and sev-
1988). eral new, untried subtests have been included
Although the WMS-R represented a sig- (e.g., Family Pictures Recall and Recognition),
nificant advance over the original WMS, it is not clear that the WMS-III represents a
The Neuropsychology of Memory Dysfunction and Its Assessment 581

significant advance beyond the WMS-R for the or famous people from specified times in the
assessment of memory deficits in patients with past to ensure that the information is verifiable
amnesia (Lezak et al., 2004). and potentially accessible to all subjects; and (2)
tests based on an individual patient’s autobio-
graphical information from various episodes
Rivermead Behavioral Memory Test
(e.g., a wedding) and periods of life.
The Rivermead Behavioral Memory Test Both types of tests of have provided impor-
(RBMT) is a test battery that assesses a vari- tant information about the nature of remote
ety of everyday tasks of memory (deWall et al., memory loss in amnesic patients. For example,
1994; Wilson et al., 1989). Subjects are asked to tests of remote memory for past public events in
perform tasks that include associating a name patients who became amnesic on a known cal-
to a face, remembering the location of a hid- endar day showed that (1) extensive, temporally
den object, recalling a spatial route, and distin- graded retrograde amnesia covering more than
guishing previously presented faces from new a decade occurs in patients with medial tem-
ones. In addition, tasks involving “remember- poral lobe amnesia, just as it does in patients
ing to remember” (i.e., prospective memory) with diencephalic amnesia (e.g., Korsakoff ’s
are given, such as remembering to ask a certain syndrome); (2) the impairment reflects a loss
question when an alarm sounds. The test battery of usable knowledge, not a simple difficulty in
is unique among other clinical memory tests in retrieving an intact memory store that can be
its emphasis on memory demands that are asso- overcome given sufficient opportunities for
ciated with everyday functioning. Accordingly, retrieval; and (3) very remote memory for fac-
it has the advantage that it is strongly corre- tual information can be intact in amnesia, even
lated with the clinical prognosis for rehabil- when the tests are made so difficult that normal
itation (Wilson, 1987). Three forms of the test subjects can answer only 20% of the questions
battery have been constructed so that an indi- (Squire et al., 1989). Similar patterns of retro-
vidual patient can be assessed at different times grade amnesia have been shown for autobio-
after brain injury or disease. The RBMT is par- graphical information (Kopelman et al., 1989).
ticularly well suited to assess progress during Despite their ability to provide significant
rehabilitation of memory impairment (Wilson, information about the nature of retrograde
2008). It may be less well suited for neuropsy- amnesia, both types of remote memory tests
chological studies of memory function, how- have limitations in their usefulness as clinical
ever, because task performance appears to be tests for individual patients. Tests based on pub-
quite sensitive to the level of attention and the lic events and famous people presume that the
ability to apply mnemonic strategies. Thus, poor individual patient was exposed to the informa-
performance on the RMBT may be indicative of tion in question—more specifically, exposed to
deficits in attention or general intellectual abil- it only at the time that it occurred. This may not
ities rather than being indicative specifically of be the case for specific types of information that
memory impairment. some people ignore (e.g., sporting events) or for
certain events or famous people that are reex-
posed repeatedly over time (e.g., the attacks of
Neuropsychological Assessment
September 11, 2001). Tests of autobiographical
of Retrograde Amnesia
knowledge have a different limitation. They rely
The severity and extent of retrograde amnesia on information that is often not easily corrobo-
is difficult to assess in a quantitative, standard- rated, although a spouse or relative can some-
ized fashion because individuals who develop times verify reported events and dates. Asking
memory disorders differ in their life experiences the patient to redate the memories they had
and in their past exposure to memorable peo- recalled and dated in an earlier test session can
ple, events, and information. There have been also provide a check against outright fabrication
two approaches to the development of neuro- (Schacter et al., 1982). In addition, both types
psychological tests for retrograde amnesia: (1) of retrograde amnesia tests may be suscepti-
tests based on information about public events ble to the contaminating effects of anterograde
582 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

amnesia, particularly in patients with an insidi- that had been reported in the news from 1950
ous onset of memory dysfunction (e.g., patients to 1985. Testing was first conducted in a free-
with Alzheimer’s disease). Only when amnesia recall format (e.g., “what was the name of the
has a very recent or precise time of onset can first satellite to be launched?”) and later in a
one know which test questions assess retro- four-alternative forced-choice format (e.g., “Was
grade amnesia and which assess anterograde it Discover, Explorer, Sputnik, or Telstar?”). The
amnesia. test was administered to amnesic patients with
Because of these limitations, prospective Korsakoff ’s syndrome and their alcoholic con-
studies with experimental animals have been trols, and to non-Korsakoff amnesic patients (i.e.,
needed to reveal the precise shape of the tempo- patients with medial temporal lobe damage) and
ral gradient of retrograde amnesia. Such studies their healthy normal controls. The results showed
have shown that information acquired remote that both patient groups had temporally graded
from surgery (e.g., hippocampal ablation) is retrograde amnesia with greater impairment
remembered significantly better than informa- for more recent past memories (Squire et al.,
tion acquired closer to surgery (Cho et al., 1993; 1989). This pattern of remote memory loss was
Kim & Fanselow, 1992; Winocur, 1990; Zola- observed with both free-recall and recognition
Morgan & Squire, 1990). These results indicate test formats (see Figure 23–11). Similar results
that information that initially depends on the have been obtained with a variety of other tests
integrity of medial temporal lobe/diencephalic of memory for past public events that employed
brain structures for its storage and retrieval can free-recall or recognition formats (Albert et al.,
eventually become independent of these struc- 1979; Hodges & Ward, 1989; Kopelman, 1989;
tures through some process of consolidation or McCarthy & Warrington, 1990).
reorganization that results in a more permanent
memory (Squire & Alvarez, 1995). These find- Famous Faces Test (Albert et al., 1979). Albert
ings with experimental animals provide strong and colleagues (1979) developed a test of the
confirmation for the reality of temporal gradi- ability to name or otherwise identify pictured
ents of retrograde amnesia, an idea proposed people who were famous at different time peri-
more than 100 years ago (Ribot, 1881). ods in the past. In its original form, the test
asked for the identification of 8–10 people who
were famous in each decade from the 1920s to
Tests of Public Knowledge
the 1970s. The test was subsequently updated to
The first remote memory tests to explore objec- extend into the early 1990s (Hodges et al., 1993;
tively the nature of memory disorders in amnesic Sadek et al., 2004). On each trial, free recall of
patients consisted of questions about persons or the name of the pictured person is attempted.
events that had been in the news in Great Britain Semantic and phonemic cues are provided if
or involved photographs of famous people who free recall is unsuccessful. Faces are presented
had been prominent in Great Britain at different in a specified order with items from the vari-
times in the past (Sanders & Warrington, 1971). ous decades rotating from earliest to latest. In
By asking questions or presenting faces that cov- an attempt to provide some degree of equiva-
ered the past several decades, it was possible to lency of difficulty across the items from the var-
obtain a sampling of an individual’s knowledge ious decades, the test items were chosen so that
of past events. Subsequently, similar tests based healthy normal control subjects would correctly
on public events or famous faces were developed recall the name of about 75% of the faces in each
for use in the United States (Albert et al., 1979; decade. Using this test, Albert and colleagues
Seltzer & Benson, 1974; Squire, 1974). Examples (1979) showed the patients with Korsakoff ’s
of public event and famous face identification syndrome have an extensive, temporally graded
tests are described below. retrograde amnesia with information from the
distant past better retained than information
Public Events Test. Squire and colleagues from the more recent past.
(Squire et al., 1989) developed a 92-item pub- Using an updated version of this task, Squire
lic events questionnaire that covered events and colleagues (1989) asked amnesic Korsakoff ’s
Public events

100 Recall Recognition

80 KOR (N=7)

Percent correct
ALC (N=9)
60

40

20

0
80 70 60 50 80 70 60 50

Famous faces

100 Recall Recognition

80
Percent correct

60

40

20

0
80 70 60 50 40 80 70 60 50 40
Decade Decade

Public events

100 Recall Recognition

80 AMN (N=5)
Percent correct

CON (N=8)
60

40

20

0
80 70 60 50 80 70 60 50

Famous faces

100 Recall Recognition

80
Percent correct

60

40

20

0
80 70 60 50 40 80 70 60 50 40
Decade Decade

Figure 23–11. Performance of patients with Korsakoff ’s syndrome and


alcoholic control subjects (upper panels), and non-Korsakoff amnesic
patients and their control subjects (lower panels), on the recall and recog-
nition versions of the public events test and the famous faces test. (dashed
lines indicate chance performance). (Adapted from Squire et al., 1989).

583
584 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

patients and their alcoholic controls, and non- capacity of a single patient. Despite these limi-
Korsakoff amnesic patients and their controls, tations, the television test successfully identi-
to identify 117 photographs of famous peo- fied temporally graded retrograde amnesia in
ple who came into the news between 1950 and patients receiving ECT (Squire et al., 1975).
1985. Both free-recall and recognition versions
of the task were administered. The results were
Tests of Autobiographical Knowledge
consistent with those of Albert and colleagues
(1979) in showing temporally graded retrograde Although autobiographical memory tests do not
amnesia in patients with Korsakoff ’s syndrome, have the advantage of being based on verifiable
and extended these results to patients with and publicly accessible information, they can
non-Korsakoff ’s amnesia (i.e., patients with be of considerable value in the neuropsycho-
medial temporal lobe damage). In addition, the logical assessment of memory disorders. Two
results paralleled those obtained with the pub- of the most widely used tests of remote autobio-
lic events questionnaire (Squire et al., 1989) (see graphical knowledge are the Crovitz procedure
Figure 23–11). (sometimes referred to as the Crovitz Test) and
It should be noted that remote memory tests the Autobiographical Memory Interview (AMI)
cannot be assumed to satisfy the criterion for (Kopelman et al., 1989).
equivalence just because normal subjects obtain
the same score (e.g., 75% correct) across all The Crovitz Test (Crovitz & Schiffman,
time periods sampled by the test. As discussed 1974). Inspired by the early quantitative stud-
in detail elsewhere (Squire & Cohen, 1982), the ies of Galton (1879), the Crovitz Test is designed
events or faces selected from more remote time to obtain autobiographical remote memories
periods may initially have been more salient about specific past episodes of a patient’s life.
and more widely known than those selected Patients are given standard cue words (e.g., win-
from more recent time periods. In addition, dow, tree, ticket, bird) and are asked in each case
the events and faces from more remote periods to recall a specific memory from the past that
could have been forgotten more slowly. Thus, involves the word. After recalling a memory for
such tests may not be particularly well suited to a given cue word, subjects are asked to date the
compare performance across time periods. memory as best they can. The responses can be
scored on the basis of the quality and quantity
Television Test (Squire & Slater, 1975). of recalled information, and they can provide
Another remote memory test that has found useful information about the time periods from
useful application in the study of memory dis- which recall is possible.
orders is a test of former one-season television Even amnesic patients who obtain normal
programs (Squire, 1989; Squire & Slater, 1975). scores for recall may draw their memories from
It was designed to overcome an important different time periods than normal subjects.
limitation of other available remote memory This was true in the case of patients with alco-
tests—the difficulty of comparing scores across holic Korsakoff syndrome who obtained normal
different past time periods. To make valid com- or near-normal scores on the recall test but drew
parisons across time periods, the items selected their memories from 10 years earlier than their
must satisfy the criterion of equivalence; that is alcoholic control subjects (Zola-Morgan et al.,
they must sample past time periods in an equiv- 1983). Similarly, patients who became amne-
alent way so that the events from different time sic as the result of anoxia or ischemia, and had
periods are likely to have been learned to about presumed or confirmed bilateral damage to the
the same extent, and then forgotten at similar hippocampal formation, drew fewer memories
rates. The television test appears to satisfy this from the more recent time period than control
criterion (Squire, 1989). However, the television subjects (MacKinnon & Squire, 1989). Under
test is limited by the relatively short time span some conditions, the amnesic patients were
that it can reliably cover (about 20 years), and by able to recollect fully formed autobiographical
the fact that it yields variable results when used episodes from their earlier life, which could not
clinically to explore the retrograde memory be distinguished qualitatively or quantitatively
The Neuropsychology of Memory Dysfunction and Its Assessment 585

from the recollections of normal subjects. This impaired on certain nondeclarative memory
finding was recently replicated with the Crovitz tasks (Shimamura et al., 1987; for review, see,
procedure (Bayley et al., 2005). Salmon, 2000). In addition, these tests may be
particularly useful with patient groups that have
Autobiographical Memory Interview a primary memory deficit that involves non-
(Kopelman et al., 1989). The AMI is a semi- declarative forms of memory, such as patients
structured interview that assesses a patient’s with Huntington’s disease (Butters et al., 1990;
ability to recall facts (personal semantic mem- Heindel et al., 1989; Saint-Cyr & Taylor, 1992).
ory) and specific events (autobiographical The nondeclarative memory tests of most
incidents) from past life. Recall of both types interest for assessment are those that have been
of information is assessed with three ques- used to demonstrate intact or near-intact per-
tions for each of three time periods: childhood formance in amnesic patients. These include
(e.g., “what was the name of your elementary tasks involving learning simple conditioned
school?”), early adulthood (e.g., “what was your responses, perceptuomotor skills such as mir-
first job?”), and the recent past (e.g., “who was a ror drawing and motor pursuit, perceptual
recent visitor to your home?”). Prompts are pro- skills like reading mirror-reversed words, cog-
vided when the patient cannot give a response. nitive skills, adaptation-level effects, probabi-
Responses are scored for clarity and specificity listic classification learning, artificial grammar
on a 0–3 scale. The AMI is sensitive to remote learning, speeded reading of repeated prose
memory loss in patients with circumscribed passages, and several kinds of priming (for
amnesia. Kopelman and colleagues (Kopelman, review, see Squire, 2004). Despite normal per-
1989; Kopelman et al., 1989), for example, found formance on these nondeclarative memory
that patients with amnesia of various etiologies tasks, memory-impaired patients often show
scored significantly below normal control sub- profound amnesia for the declarative memory
jects on all components of the AMI, with par- aspects of the learning situation. For example,
ticularly great impairment for the recent past patients with circumscribed amnesia exhibit
items in both the autobiographical and personal progressive learning and 24-hour retention of a
semantic memory categories. This temporally conditioned eye blink response, but they cannot
graded retrograde amnesia revealed by the AMI describe the apparatus used in their learning
was observed in several additional studies (with sessions or how it had been used (Daum et al.,
some using a variant of the AMI) that assessed 1989; Weiskrantz & Warrington, 1979).
patients with Korsakoff ’s syndrome (Kopelman Although a description of the various experi-
et al., 1999) or amnesia related to temporal lobe mental tasks that have been used to assess non-
damage (Bayley et al., 2006; Bright et al., 2006; declarative memory in patients with amnesia
Kirwan et al., 2008; Reed & Squire, 1998). is beyond the scope of the present discussion,
two types of priming tests, word-stem com-
pletion and associative priming, will be briefly
Neuropsychological Tests of
described because these are the tests most often
Nondeclarative Memory
used in a quasi-clinical fashion.
Although tests of nondeclarative memory are
not usually used in clinical settings, they can
Word-Stem Completion Priming
sometimes provide useful information that is
not obtained with conventional memory tests. One of the most widely used methods to elicit
As mentioned previously, patients with cir- priming is the word-stem completion task
cumscribed amnesia arising from diencephalic (Graf et al., 1984). In this task, words are pre-
or medial temporal lobe damage perform nor- sented to the subject (e.g., MOTEL) in the guise
mally on these tests, so impairment would sug- of a “likeability” rating task (not as a memory
gest the involvement of brain systems beyond task), and later three-letter word stems (e.g.,
the declarative memory system. Indeed, patients MOT) are presented. Half of the stems can
with more global cortical involvement, such as be completed with one of the previously rated
those with early stage Alzheimer’s disease, are words, and half are novel (to assess baseline
586 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

guessing rates). Subjects are asked to say the subjects. In contrast, when subjects were asked
first word that comes to mind that completes to use the word stems as cues to recollect words
each word stem. In a study by Graf and col- from the study session, the control subjects
leagues (1984), the probability of producing performed better than amnesic patients. The
previously presented words was increased from ability of the word-stem completion priming
about 10% (baseline word-stem completion) to test to differentiate between patients with cir-
about 50% for both amnesic patients and con- cumscribed amnesia or Alzheimer’s disease was
trol subjects. The priming effect indicates that first shown in a study by Shimamura and col-
words appear to “pop” into mind, and amnesic leagues (1987; see Figure 23–12). Although both
patients exhibit this effect as strongly as control patient groups had significant and equal deficits

60

50
Words completed (%)

40

30

20

10

0
AD CON HD CON KS CON

100

90
HD-CON
80 AD-CON
KS-CON
70
Percent correct

60

50 Chance
HD
40
AD
30 KS

20

10
0
1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5
Recall trials Recognition trials
Figure 23–12. The performance of patients with Alzheimer’s disease
(AD), Huntington’s disease (HD), or Korsakoff ’s syndrome (KS), and their
respective controls (CON), on the word-stem completion task (top panel).
All groups exhibited priming, as evidenced by the fact that they completed
word stems to form previously presented words at above baseline guessing
rates (the darkened portion of the bars), but priming was impaired in patients
with AD. In contrast, all three patient groups exhibited significant impair-
ment relative to controls on the explicit recall and recognition components
of the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (bottom panel). (Adapted from
Shimamura et al., 1987).
The Neuropsychology of Memory Dysfunction and Its Assessment 587

in explicit memory measured on the Rey AVLT, obtaining a profi le of performance from single
the patients with amnesia exhibited normal lev- patients. Retrograde amnesia can be objectively
els of priming above baseline completion rates. assessed with tests of remote memory for public
Patients with Alzheimer’s disease, in contrast, events, but more subjective methods based on
showed less priming than their controls or the questions about autobiographical knowledge
amnesic patients. or a time line of memory loss are also useful.
When assessing the patient with memory dys-
function, more than one test of each type can be
Associative Priming (Levy et al., 2004;
used if the findings in any one area are ambigu-
Shimamura & Squire, 1984)
ous; additional tests can be used as needed once
One version of the associative priming task the clinician becomes familiar with the status of
employs a paired-associates procedure in which the patient’s memory functions.
subjects are first asked to judge the degree of Neuropsychological testing of memory is
relatedness of categorically or functionally most informative when memory tests are supple-
related word pairs (e.g., BIRD-ROBIN, NEEDLE- mented with tests of other cognitive functions.
THREAD) and later to “free-associate” to the Additional tests establish valuable reference
first words of the previously presented pairs points that help in interpreting memory test
and to words that were not presented as part of scores, and can identify or rule out other kinds
the paired-associates. Shimamura and Squire of cognitive impairment that could influence
(1984) showed that amnesic patients and con- memory test performance. For example, the
trol subjects exhibit a similar bias for using WAIS-R or WAIS-III IQ test can be very helpful
recently presented words (i.e., the second word because it can identify the general cognitive sta-
of the presented word pair) when performing tus and test-taking ability of the patient. Tests
the free association task. In contrast, Salmon that assess language comprehension, confron-
and colleagues (1988) found that patients with tational naming, verbal fluency, constructional
Alzheimer’s disease were significantly less ability, and executive functions (i.e., frontal lobe
likely to produce the second word of the seman- functions) can also be valuable in this regard,
tically related pair than were control subjects because deficits in any of these abilities can
or patients with Huntington’s disease. Similar influence performance on many memory tests
associative priming deficits in patients with (Moscovitch, 1982; Shimamura et al., 1990;
Alzheimer’s disease were found by using a var- for review, see Lezak et al., 2004). The Mattis
iant of the free-association procedure (Brandt Dementia Rating Scale (Mattis, 1988) is partic-
et al., 1988). Thus, tests of associative priming ularly useful in assessing the basic cognitive sta-
may be particularly useful in differentiating tus of patients with suspected dementia because
memory impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease it evaluates a wide range of abilities with mea-
from other causes. sures that avoid floor effects in performance.
Having considered the available methods
for testing memory, it is worth noting that
Summary and Recommendations
improvement in neuropsychological testing
The preceding sections considered the issues methods has depended importantly on basic
involved in undertaking a clinical neuropsy- research in cognitive psychology and neu-
chological evaluation of memory functions. roscience. An enormous amount has been
Specialized tests are available for assessing mem- learned over the past two decades about how
ory dysfunction, including tests of new learning the brain accomplishes memory storage. An
capacity, remote memory, and nondeclarative inventory of memory tests has been one of the
memory. Of the readily available tests to assess fruits of this enterprise. The neuropsycholog-
anterograde amnesia, the WMS-R and WMS- ical, brain-systems study of memory is part
III provide a useful general-purpose assessment of a broader program of research aimed at
of new learning ability, the RMT allows effective understanding the biology of memory at all
assessment of the severity of memory impair- levels of analysis—from cellular and synap-
ment, and the CVLT (or CVLT-II) is useful for tic events to the complex behavior of animals
588 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

and humans. A broad, basic research approach Bettermann, K. (2006). Transient global amnesia:
to the study of memory and the brain should The continuing quest for a source. Archives of
continue to inform us about mechanisms Neurology, 63, 1336–1337.
and organization, and at the same time yield Braak, H., & Braak, E. (1991). Neuropathological
staging of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta
improved methods for the assessment of
Neuropathologica, 82, 239–259.
patients with memory dysfunction. These same
Brandt, J. (1991). The Hopkins Verbal Learning Test:
research efforts can be expected to suggest development of a new memory test with six equiva-
strategies for developing treatments for mem- lent forms. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 5, 125–142.
ory dysfunction and methods for evaluating Brandt, J., Spencer, M., McSorley, P., & Folstein, M.
those treatments. F. (1988). Semantic activation and implicit mem-
ory in Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Disease and
Associated Disorders, 2, 112–119.
References Brandt, J., & van Gorp, W. G. (2006). Functional
Abrams, R., & Essman, W. B. (1982). Electroconvulsive (“psychogenic”) amnesia. Seminars in Neurology,
therapy: Biological foundations and clinical appli- 26, 331–340.
cations. New York: Spectrum Publications Inc. Bright, P., Buckman, J., Fradera, A., Yoshimasu,
Albert, M. S., & Blacker, D. (2006). Mild cognitive H., Colchester, A. C., & Kopelman, M. D. (2006).
impairment and dementia. Annual Review of Retrograde amnesia in patients with hippocampal,
Clinical Psychology, 2, 379–388. medial temporal, temporal lobe, or frontal pathol-
Albert, M.S., Butters, N., & Levin, J. (1979). Temporal ogy. Learning & Memory, 13, 545–557.
gradients in the retrograde amnesia of patients Brooks, D. N. (1984). Cognitive deficits after head
with alcoholic Korsakoff ’s disease. Archives of injury. In D. N. Brooks (Ed.), Closed head injury:
Neurology, 36, 211–216. Psychological, social, and family consequences.
Aleman, A., Hijman, R., de Haan, E. H. F., & Kahn, R. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
S. (1999). Memory impairment in schizophrenia: Brooks, D. N., & Baddeley, A. D. (1976). What can
A meta-analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, amnesic patients learn? Neuropsychologia, 14,
156, 1358–1366. 111–122.
Amieva, H., Gaestel, Y., & Dartigues, J. (2006). The mul- Buschke, H., & Fuld, P. A. (1974). Evaluating storage,
tiple-choice formats (form F and G) of the Benton retention, and retrieval in disordered memory and
Visual Retention Test as a tool to detect age-related learning. Neurology, 24, 1019–1025.
memory changes in population-based studies and Buschke, H., Sliwinski, M. J., Kuslansky, G., & Lipton,
clinical settings. Nature Protocols, 1, 1936–1938. R. B. (1997). Diagnosis of early dementia by the
Backman, L., Small, B. J., & Fratiglioni, L. (2001). double memory test. Neurology, 48, 989–997.
Stability of the preclinical episodic memory deficit Butters, N., & Cermak, L. S. (1980). Alcoholic
in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain, 124, 96–102. Korsakoff ’s syndrome: An information processing
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: approach. New York: Academic Press.
Claredon Press. Butters, N., Delis, D. C., & Lucas, J. A. (1995). Clinical
Baddeley, A. D., Emslie, H., & Nimmo-Smith, I. assessment of memory disorders in amnesia.
(1994). Doors and People: a test of visual and ver- Annual Review of Psychology, 46, 493–523.
bal recall and recognition. Bury St. Edmunds, Butters, N., Heindel, W. C., & Salmon, D. P. (1990).
England: Thames Valley Test Company. Dissociation of implicit memory in demen-
Baldo, J. V., Shimamura, A. P. (2002). Frontal lobes tia: Neurological implications. Bulletin of the
and memory. In A. D. Baddeley, M. D. Kopelman, Psychonomic Society, 28, 359–366.
and B. A. Wilson (Eds.), Handbook of memory dis- Butters, N., Salmon, D. P., Cullum, C. M., Cairns,
orders (2nd ed., pp. 363–380). Chichester UK: John P., Troster, A. I., Jacobs, D., et al. (1988).
Wiley. Differentiation of amnesic and demented patients
Barbizet, J. (1970). Human memory and its pathology. with the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised. The
San Francisco: WH Freeman and Company. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 2, 133–148.
Bayley, P. J., Gold, J. J., Hopkins, R. O., & Squire, L. Butters, N., Wolfe, J., Martone, M., Granholm, E.,
R. (2005). The neuroanatomy of remote memory. & Cermak, L. S. (1985). Memory disorders asso-
Neuron, 46, 799–810. ciated with Huntington’s disease: Verbal recall,
Bayley, P. J., Hopkins, R. O., & Squire, L. R. (2006). verbal recognition and procedural memory.
The fate of old memories after medial tempo- Neuropsychologia, 6, 729–744.
ral lobe damage. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, Carlesimo, G. A., Sabbadini, M., Bombardi, P., Di
13311–13317. Porto, E., Loasses, A., & Caltagirone, C. (1998).
The Neuropsychology of Memory Dysfunction and Its Assessment 589

Retrograde memory deficits in severe closed head epilepsy: A comparison of non-surgical, unilateral
injury patients. Cortex, 34, 1–23. temporal lobe and frontal lobe samples. Cortex,
Carlesimo, G. A., Sabbadini, M., Loasses, A., & 16, 103–117.
Caltagirone, C. (1997). Forgetting from long-term Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B. A.
memory in severe closed head injury patients: (1987). The California verbal learning test. New
Effect of retrieval conditions and semantic orga- York: Psychological Corporation.
nization. Cortex, 33, 131–142. Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B.
Chapman, J. (1966). The early symptoms of schizo- A. (2000). The California verbal learning test, 2nd
phrenia. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 112, edition (CVLT-II). San Antonio: Psychological
225–251. Corporation.
Chelune, G. J., & Bornstein, R. A. (1988). WMS-R pat- Delis, D. C., Massman, P. J., Butters, N., Salmon, D.
terns among patients with unilateral brain lesions. P., Cermak, L. S., & Kramer, J. H. (1991). Profi les of
The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 2, 121–132. demented and amnesic patients on the California
Cho, Y. H., Beracochea, D., & Jaffard, R. (1993). Verbal Learning Test: Implications for the
Extended temporal gradient for the retrograde assessment of memory disorders. Psychological
and anterograde amnesia produced by ibote- Assessment, 3, 19–26.
nate entorhinal cortex lesions in mice. Journal of DeLuca, J., Schultheis, M. T., Madigan, N. K,
Neuroscience, 13, 1759–1766. Christodoulou, C., & Averill, A. (2000). Acquisition
Cohen, B. D., Noblin, C. D., Silverman, A. J., & versus retrieval deficits in traumatic brain injury:
Penick, S. B. (1968). Functional asymmetry of the Implications for memory rehabilitation. Archives of
human brain. Science, 162, 475–477. Physical Medicine and Rehabilation, 81, 1327–1333.
Cohen, N., & Squire, L. R. (1980). Preserved learning Devanand, D. P., Pradhaban, G., Liu, X., Khandji, A.,
and retention of pattern analyzing skills in amne- De Santi, S., Seagal, S., et al. (2007). Hippocampal
sia: Dissociation of knowing how and knowing and entorhinal atrophy in mild cognitive impair-
that. Science, 210, 207–210. ment: prediction of Alzheimer disease. Neurology,
Corkin, S. (1968). Acquisition of motor skill 68, 828–836.
after bilateral medial temporal lobe excision. deWall, C., Wilson, B. A., & Baddeley, A. D. (1994).
Neuropsychologia, 6, 255–265. The extended Rivermead Behavioral Memory
Corkin, S. (1984). Lasting consequences of bilat- Test: A measure of everyday memory performance
eral medial temporal lobectomy: Clinical course in adults. Memory, 2, 149–166.
and experimental findings in H.M. Seminars in Drachman, D. A., & Arbit, J. (1966). Memory and the
Neurology, 4, 249–259. hippocampal complex. II. Is memory a multiple
Corkin, S. (2002). What’s new with the amnesic process? Archives of Neurology, 15, 52–61.
patient H.M.? Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 3, Evans, D. A., Funkenstein, H. H., Albert, M. S.,
153–160. Scherr, P. A., Cook, N. R., Chown, M. J., et al.
Cowan, N., Beschin, N., & Della Sala, S. (2004). Verbal (1989). Prevalence of Alzheimer’s disease in a
recall in amnesics under conditions of diminished community population of older persons. Higher
retroactive interference. Brain, 127, 825–834. than previously reported. JAMA, 262, 2551–2556.
Cronholm, B., & Ottosson, J. O. (1961). Memory Fraser, L. M., O’Carroll, R. E., & Ebmeier, K. P.
functions in endogenous depression. Archives of (2008). The effect of electroconvulsive therapy on
General Psychiatry, 5, 101–107. autobiographical memory: a systematic review.
Crosson, B., Novack, T. A., Trenerry, M. R., & Craig, The Journal of ECT, 24, 10–17.
P. L. (1988). California Verbal Learning Test Galton, F. (1879). Psychometric experiments. Brain,
(CVLT) performance in severely head-injured 2, 149–162.
and neurologically normal adult males. Journal Gold, J. J., & Squire, L. R. (2006). The anatomy of
of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 10, amnesia: Neurohistological analysis of three new
754–768. cases. Learning & Memory, 13, 699–710.
Crovitz, H. F., & Schiff man, H. (1974). Frequency Graf, P., Squire, L. R., & Mandler, G. (1984). The
of episodic memories as a function of their age. information that amnesic patients do not forget.
Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 4, 517–518. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Daum, I., Channon, S., & Canavan, A. (1989). Classical Memory, and Cognition, 10, 164–178.
conditioning in patients with severe memory Grober, E., Merling, A., Heimlich, T., & Lipton, R.
problems. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and B. (1997). Free and cued selective reminding and
Psychiatry, 52, 47–51. selective reminding in the elderly. Journal of
Delaney, R. C., Rosen, A. J., Mattson, R. H., & Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 19,
Novelly, R. A. (1980). Memory function in focal 643–654.
590 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Heindel, W. C., Salmon, D. P., Shults, C. W., Walicke, Kopelman, M. D. (1989). Remote and autobiographi-
P. A., & Butters, N. (1989). Neuropsychological cal memory, temporal context memory, and fron-
evidence for multiple implicit memory systems: tal atrophy in Korsakoff and Alzheimer patients.
A comparison of Alzheimer’s, Huntington’s, Neuropsychologia, 27, 437–460.
and Parkinson’s disease patients. Journal of Kopelman, M. D. (1995). The assessment of psycho-
Neuroscience, 9, 582–587. genic amnesia. In A. D. Baddeley, B. A. Wilson,
Hodges, J. R., Salmon, D. P., & Butters, N. (1993). and F.N. Watts (Eds.), Handbook of memory disor-
Recognition and naming of famous faces in ders (pp. 427–448). Chichester UK: John Wiley.
Alzheimer’s disease: A cognitive analysis. Kopelman, M. D. (2002). Disorders of memory.
Neuropsychologia, 31, 775–788. Brain, 125, 2152–2190.
Hodges, J. R., & Ward, C. D. (1989). Observations Kopelman, M. D., Bright, P., Buckman, J., Fradera,
during transient global amnesia: A behavioral and A., Yoshimasu H., Jacobson C., & Colchester C. F.
neuropsychological study of five cases. Brain, 112, (2007). Recall and recognition memory in amnesia:
595–620. Patients with hippocampal, medial temporal, tem-
Hodges, J. R., & Warlow, C. P. (1990). The etiology of poral lobe, or frontal pathology. Neuropsychologia,
transient global amnesia: A case-control study of 45, 1232–1246.
114 cases with prospective follow-up. Brain, 113, Kopelman, M. D., & Stanhope, N. (1997). Rates of
639–657. forgetting in organic amnesia following tempo-
Hyman, B. T., Van Hoesen, G. W., Damasio, A. R., ral lobe, diencephalic, and frontal lobe lesions.
& Barnes, C. L. (1984). Alzheimer’s disease: Cell Neuropsychology, 11, 343–356.
specific pathology isolates the hippocampal for- Kopelman, M. D., Stanhope, N., & Kingsley, D. (1999).
mation. Science, 225, 1168–1170. Retrograde amnesia in patients with diencephalic,
Isaac, C. L., & Mayes, A. R. (1999). Rate of forgetting temporal lobe or frontal lesions. Neuropsychologia,
in amnesia: Recall and recognition of prose. 37, 939–958.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Kopelman, M. D., Wilson, B. A., & Baddeley, A. D. (1989).
Memory, and Cognition, 25, 942–962. The Autobiographical Memory Interview: A new
Jager, T., Bazner, H., Kliegel, M., Szabo, K., & assessment of autobiographical and personal seman-
Hennerici, M. G. (2008). The transience and nature tic memory in amnesic patients. Journal of Clinical
of cognitive impairments in transient global and Experimental Neuropsychology, 11, 724–744.
amnesia: A meta-analysis. Journal of Clinical and Kritchevsky, M. (1989). Transient global amnesia.
Experimental Neuropsychology, 3, 1–12. In F. Boller and J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of
James, W. (1890). Principles of psychology. New York: neuropsychology (3rd ed., pp. 167–182). New York:
Holt. Elsevier.
Janowsky, J. S., Shimamura, A. P., Kritchevsky, M., & Kritchevsky, M., Chang, J., & Squire, L. R. (2004).
Squire, L. R. (1989a). Cognitive impairment follow- Functional amnesia: Clinical description and
ing frontal lobe damage and its relevance to human neuropsychological profi le of 10 cases. Learning &
amnesia. Behavioral Neuroscience, 103, 548–560. Memory, 11, 213–226.
Janowsky, J. S., Shimamura, A. P., & Squire, L. R. Lackner, J. R. (1974). Observations on the speech
(1989b). Source memory impairment in patients processing capabilities of an amnesic patient:
with frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia, 27, Several aspects of H.M.’s language function.
1043–1056. Neuropsychologia, 12, 199–207.
Jennett, B. (1976). Assessment of the severity of head Lerer, B., Weiner, R. D., & Belmaker, R. H. (1984).
injury. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and ECT: Basic mechanisms. London: John Libbey.
Psychiatry, 39, 647–655. Levin, H. S., Benton, A. L., & Grossman, R. G. (1982).
Jones, M. K. (1974). Imagery as a mnemonic aid after Neurobehavioral consequences of closed head
left temporal lobectomy: Contrasts between mate- injury. New York: Oxford University Press.
rial-specific and generalized memory disorders. Levin, H. S., Grossman, R. G., Rose, J. E., & Teasdale,
Neuropsychologia, 12, 21–30. G. (1979). Long-term neuropsychological outcome
Kim, J. J., & Fanselow, M. S. (1992). Modality-specific of closed head injury. Journal of Neurosurgery, 50,
retrograde amnesia of fear. Science, 256, 675–677. 412–422.
Kirwan, C. B., Bayley, P. J., Galvan, V. V., & Squire, L. Levy, D. A., Stark, C. E. L., & Squire, L. R. (2004).
R. (2008). Detailed recollection of remote autobio- Intact conceptual priming in the absence of declar-
graphical memory after damage to the medial tem- ative memory. Psychological Science, 15, 680–685.
poral lobe. Proceedings of the National Academy Lezak, M., Howieson, D. B., & Loring, D. W. (2004).
of Sciences of the United States of America, 105, Neuropsychological assessment. New York: Oxford
2676–2680. University Press
The Neuropsychology of Memory Dysfunction and Its Assessment 591

MacKinnon, D., & Squire, L. R. (1989). Autobio- Moscovitch, M. (1982). Multiple dissociations of
graphical memory in amnesia. Psychobiology, 17, function in amnesia. In L. Cermak (Ed.), Human
247–256. memory and amnesia (pp. 337–370). Hillsdale NJ:
Mair, W. G. P., Warrington, E. K., & Weiskrantz, L. Lawrence Erlbaum.
(1979). Memory disorder in Korsakoff ’s psycho- Nemiah, J. C. (1980). Dissociative disorders. In
sis: A neuropathological and neuropsychological H. Kaplan, A. Freedman, and B. Sadock (Eds.),
investigation of two cases. Brain, 102, 749–783. Comprehensive textbook of psychiatry (3rd ed., pp.
Malitz, S., & Sackeim, H. A. (1986). Electroconvulsive 1544–1561). Baltimore: Williams and Wilkens.
therapy: Clinical and basic research issues. New Newcombe, F. (1983). The psychological conse-
York: The New York Academy of Sciences. quences of closed head injury: Assessment and
Manns, J. R., Hopkins, R. O., Reed, J. M., Kitchener, rehabilitation. Injury, 14, 111–136.
E. G., & Squire, L. R. (2003). Recognition mem- Ojemann, G. A. (1971). Alteration in nonver-
ory and the human hippocampus. Neuron, 37, bal short term memory with stimulation in the
171–180. region of the mammillothalamic tract in man.
Markowitsch, H. (1988). Diencephalic amnesia: Neuropsychologia, 9, 195–201.
A reorientation towards tracts? Brain Research Oscar-Berman, M., Kirkley, S. M., Gansler, D. A., &
Reviews, 13, 351–370. Couture, A. (2004). Comparisons of Korsakoff and
Mattis, S. (1988). Dementia Rating Scale. Odessa, FL: non-Korsakoff alcoholics on neuropsychological
Psychological Assessment Resources. tests of prefrontal brain functioning. Alcoholism,
Mayes, A. (1988). Human organic memory disorders. Clinical and Experimental Research, 28, 667–675.
New York: Oxford University Press. Osterrieth, P. A. (1944). Le test de copie d’une figure
McCarthy, R. A., & Warrington, E. K. (1990). Actors complexe. Archiv für Psychologie, 30, 206–256.
but not scripts: The dissociation of people and Petersen, R. C., Doody, R., Kurz, A., Mohs, R. C.,
events in retrograde amnesia. Neuropsychologia, Morris, J. C., et al. (2001). Current concepts in
30, 633–644. mild cognitive impairment. Archives of Neurology,
McKee, R. D., & Squire, L. R. (1992). Equivalent for- 58, 1985–1992.
getting rates in long-term memory for dience- Petersen, R. C., Smith, G. E., Ivnik, R. J., Tangalos,
phalic and medial temporal lobe amnesia. Journal E. G., Schaid, D. J., et al. (1995). Apolipoprotein
of Neuroscience, 12, 3765–3772. E status as a predictor of the development of
Michel, D., Laurent, B., Foyatier, N., Blanc, A., Alzheimer’s disease in memory-impaired indi-
& Portafaix, M. (1982). Infarctus thalamique viduals. JAMA, 273, 1274–1278.
paramedian gauche. Revue Neurologique, 138, Ribot, T. (1881). Les Maladies de la Mémoire [English
533–550. translation: Diseases of Memory]. New York:
Milner, B. (1958). Psychological deficits produced Appleton-Century-Croft.
by temporal lobe excision. Research Publications Reed, L. J., & Squire, L. R. (1997). Impaired recogni-
– Association for Research in Nervous and Mental tion memory in patients with lesions limited to the
Disease, 36, 244–257. hippocampal formation. Behavioral Neuroscience,
Milner, B. (1968). Disorders of memory after brain 111, 667–675.
lesions in man. Preface: Material-specific and Reed, L. J., & Squire, L. R. (1998). Retrograde amne-
generalized memory loss. Neuropsychologia, 6, sia for facts and events: Findings from four new
175–179. cases. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 3943–3954.
Milner, B. (1970). Memory and the medial temporal Rempel-Clower, N., Zola, S. M., Squire, L. R., &
regions of the brain. In K. H. Pribram and D. E. Amaral, D. G. (1996). Three cases of enduring
Broadbent (Eds.), Biology and memory (pp. 29–50). memory impairment following bilateral damage
New York: Academic Press. limited to the hippocampal formation. Journal of
Milner, B. (1971). Interhemispheric differences in the Neuroscience, 16, 5233–5255.
localization of psychological processes in man. Rey, A. (1941). L’examen psychologique dans les
British Medical Bulletin, 27:272–277. d’encephalopathie traumatique. Archiv für
Milner B., & Teuber H. L. (1968). Alteration of per- Psychologie, 28, 286–340.
ception and memory in man: Reflection on meth- Rey, A. (1964). L’examen Clinique Psychologie. Paris:
ods. In L. Weiskrantz (Ed.), Analysis of behavioral Presses Universitaires de France.
change (pp. 268–375). New York: Harper and Row. Russell, W. R., & Nathan, P. W. (1946). Traumatic
Mitrushina, M., Boone, K. B., Razani, J., & D’Elia, L. amnesia. Brain, 69, 280–300.
F. (2005). Handbook of normative data for neuro- Russell, R., & Smith, N. (1961). Post-traumatic amne-
psychological assessment, 2nd edition. New York: sia in closed head injury. Archives of Neurology, 5,
Oxford University Press. 16–29.
592 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Sackeim, H. (2000). Memory and ECT: From polar- Shimamura, A. P., Janowsky, J. S., & Squire, L. R.
ization to reconciliation. Journal of ECT, 16, (1991). What is the role of frontal lobe dam-
87–96. age in memory disorders? In H. D. Levin, H. M.
Sadek, J. R., Johnson, S. A., White, D. A., Salmon, Eisenberg, and A. L. Benton (Eds.), Frontal lobe
D. P., Taylor, K. I., Delapena, J. H., et al. (2004). function and dysfunction (pp. 173–195). New York:
Retrograde amnesia in dementia: Comparison of Oxford University Press.
HIV-associated dementia, Alzheimer’s disease, Shimamura, A. P., Salmon, D. P., Squire, L. R., &
and Huntington’s disease. Neuropsychology, 18, Butters, N. (1987). Memory dysfunction and word
692–699. priming in dementia and amnesia. Behavioral
Saint-Cyr, J. A., & Taylor, A. E. (1992). The mobiliza- Neuroscience, 101, 347–351.
tion of procedural learning: The “key signature” Shimamura, A .P. & Squire, L. R. (1984). Paired-
of the basal ganglia. In L. R. Squire and N. Butters associate learning and priming effects in amnesia: a
(Eds.), Neuropsychology of memory (2nd ed., pp. neuropsychological study. Journal of Experimental
188–202). New York: Guilford Press. Psychology: General, 113, 556–570.
Salmon, D. P. (2000). Disorders of memory in Schmolck, H., Stefanacci, L., & Squire, L. R. (2000).
Alzheimer’s disease. In L. S. Cermak (Ed.), Detection and explanation of ambiguity are unaf-
Handbook of neuropsychology, 2nd edition (Vol. fected by hippocampal lesions but are impaired
2): Memory and its disorders (pp. 155–195). by larger temporal lobe lesions. Hippocampus, 10,
Amsterdam: Elsevier. 759–770.
Salmon, D. P., Shimamura, A. P., Butters, N., & Smith, Shrager, Y., Levy, D. A., Hopkins, R. O., & Squire,
S. (1988). Lexical and semantic priming deficits L. R. (2008). Working memory and the organiza-
in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of tion of brain systems. Journal of Neuroscience, 28,
Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 10, 4818–4822.
477–494. Speedie, L. J., & Heilman, K. M. (1982). Amnestic
Salmon, D. P., Thomas, R. G., Pay, M. M., Booth, disturbance following infarction of the left dorso-
A., Hofstetter, C. R., Thal, L. J., et al. (2002). medial nucleus of the thalamus. Neuropsychologia,
Alzheimer’s disease can be accurately diagnosed 20, 597–604.
in very mildly impaired individuals. Neurology, Squire, L. R. (1974). Remote memory as affected by
59, 1022–1028. aging. Neuropsychologia, 12, 429–435.
Sanders, H. I., & Warrington, E. K. (1971). Memory Squire, L. R. (1982). Comparisons between forms of
for remote events in amnesic patients. Brain, 94, amnesia: Some deficits are unique to Korsakoff ’s
661–668. syndrome. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Schacter, D. L. (1987). Implicit memory: History and Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 8, 560–571.
current status. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Squire, L. R. (1986). Memory functions as affected by ECT.
Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 501–518. In S. Malitz and H. Sackeim (Eds.), Electroconvulsive
Schacter, D., & Crovitz, H. F. (1977). Memory func- therapy: Clinical and basic issues (pp. 307–314). New
tion after closed head injury: A review of the quan- York: New York Academy of Sciences.
titative research. Cortex, 13, 150–176. Squire, L. R. (1989). On the course of forgetting in
Schacter, D., Wang, P. L., Tulving, E., & Freedman, P. very long-term memory. Journal of Experimental
C. (1982). Functional retrograde amnesia: A quan- Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15,
titative case study. Neuropsychologia, 20, 523–532. 241–245.
Scoville, W. B., & Milner, B. (1957). Loss of recent Squire, L. R. (2004). Memory systems of the brain: A
memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. brief history and current perspective. Neurobiology
Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, of Learning and Memory, 82, 171–177.
20, 11–21. Squire, L. R., & Alvarez, P. (1995). Retrograde amne-
Seltzer, B., & Benson, D. F. (1974). The temporal pat- sia and memory consolidation: A neurobiological
tern of retrograde amnesia in Korsakoff ’s disease. perspective. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 5,
Neurology, 24, 527–530. 169–177.
Shimamura, A. P. (1986). Priming effects in amne- Squire, L. R., Amaral, D. G., Zola-Morgan, S. M.,
sia: Evidence for a dissociable memory function. Kritchevsky, M., & Press, G. (1989). Description
The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: of brain injury in the amnesic patient N.A. based
Human Experimental Psychology, 38, 619–644. on magnetic resonance imaging. Experimental
Shimamura, A. P., Janowsky, J. S., & Squire, L. R. Neurology, 105, 23–35.
(1990). Memory for the temporal order of events Squire, L. R., & Bayley, P. J. (2007). The neurosci-
in patients with frontal lobe lesions and amnesic ence of remote memory. Current Opinion in
patients. Neuropsychologia, 28, 803–814. Neurobiology, 17, 185–196.
The Neuropsychology of Memory Dysfunction and Its Assessment 593

Squire, L. R., & Chace, P. M. (1975). Memory func- Terry, R. D., Masliah, E., Salmon, D. P., Butters, N.,
tions six to nine months after electroconvul- DeTeresa, R., Hill, R., et al. (1991). Physical basis
sive therapy. Archives of General Psychiatry, 32, of cognitive alterations in Alzheimer’s disease:
1557–1564. Synapse loss is the major correlate of cognitive
Squire, L. R., & Cohen, N. J. (1982). Remote memory, impairment. Annals of Neurology, 30, 572–580.
retrograde amnesia, and the neuropsychology of Troster, A. I., Jacobs, D., Butters, N., Cullum, M., &
memory. In L. Cermak (Ed.), Human memory and Salmon, D. P. (1989). Differentiating Alzheimer’s
amnesia (pp. 275–303). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence disease from Huntington’s disease with the
Erlbaum. Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised. Clinics in
Squire, L. R., Haist, F., & Shimamura, A. P. (1989). The Geriatric Medicine, 5, 611–632.
neurology of memory: Quantitative assessment Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of episodic memory. New
of retrograde amnesia in two groups of amnesic York: Oxford University Press.
patients. Journal of Neuroscience, 9, 828–839. Tulving, E., & Schacter, D. L. (1990). Priming and
Squire, L. R., & Kandel, E. R. (2009). Memory: From human memory systems. Science, 247, 301–306.
mind to molecules. 2nd edition. Greenwood Village Twamley, E. W., Ropacki, S. A., & Bondi, M. W.
CO: Roberts & Company. (2006). Neuropsychological and neuroimaging
Squire, L. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2002). The neuropsy- changes in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Journal
chology of memory, 3rd ed. New York: Guilford of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12,
Press. 707–735.
Squire, L. R., & Shimamura, A. P. (1986). Vakil, E. (2005). The effect of moderate to severe
Characterizing amnesic patients for neurobehav- traumatic brain injury (TBI) on different
ioral study. Behavioral Neuroscience, 100, 866–877. aspects of memory: A selective review. Journal of
Squire, L. R., & Slater, P. C. (1975). Forgetting in very Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 27,
long-term memory as assessed by an improved 977–1021.
questionnaire technique. Journal of Experimental Vakil, E., Arbell, N., Gozlan, M., Hoofien, D., &
Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 104, Blachstein, H. (1992). Relative importance of
50–54. informational units and their role in long-term
Squire, L. R., & Slater, P. C. (1983). Electroconvulsive recall by closed-head-injured patients and con-
therapy and complaints of memory dysfunction: A trol groups. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
prospective three-year follow-up study. The British Psychology, 60, 802–803.
Journal of Psychiatry, 142, 1–8. Vakil, E., & Oded, Y. (2003). Comparison between
Squire, L. R., Slater, P. C., & Chace, P. M. (1975). three memory tests: Cued recall, priming and
Retrograde amnesia: temporal gradient in very savings in closed-head injured patients and
long term memory following electroconvulsive controls. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
therapy. Science, 187, 77–79. Neuropsychology, 25, 274–282.
Squire, L. R., & Zola-Morgan, S. (1991). The medial Vanderploeg, R. D., Crowell, T. A., & Curtiss, G.
temporal lobe memory system. Science, 253, (2001). Verbal learning and memory deficits in
1380–1386. traumatic brain injury: Encoding, consolidation,
Squire, L. R., & Zola, S. M. (1996). Structure and and retrieval. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
function of declarative and nondeclarative mem- Neuropsychology, 23, 185–195.
ory systems. Proceedings of the National Academy Verfaellie, M., & O’Connor, M. (2000). A neuro-
of Sciences of the United States of America, 93, psychological analysis of memory and amnesia.
13515–13522. Seminars in Neurology, 20, 455–462.
Sternberg, D. E., & Jarvik, M. E. (1976). Memory Victor, M., Adams, R. D., & Collins, C. (1989). The
functions in depression. Archives of General Wernicke-Korsakoff syndrome and related neuro-
Psychiatry, 33, 219–224. logic disorders due to alcoholism and malnutrition.
Stromgren, L. S. (1977). The influence of depression Philadelphia: FA Davis.
on memory. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 56, Victor, M., & Agamanolis, J. (1990). Amensia
109–128. due to lesions confi ned to the hippocampus: A
Strub, R. L., & Black, F. W. (1977). The mental status clinical-pathological study. Journal of Cognitive
examination in neurology. Philadelphia: FA Davis. Neuroscience, 2, 246–257.
Talland, G. A. (1965). Deranged memory. New York: Warrington, E. K. (1984). Recognition memory test.
Academic Press. Windsor England: NFER-Nelson Publishing
Terry, R. D., & Katzman, R. (1983). Senile dementia Company LTD.
of the Alzheimer type. Annals of Neurology, 14, Wechsler, D. (1945). A standardized memory scale
497–506. for clinical use. Journal of Psychology, 19, 87–95.
594 Neuropsychiatric Disorders

Wechsler, D. (1987). The Wechsler memory scale-re- everyday memory problems. Journal of Clinical
vised. New York: Psychological Corporation. and Experimental Neuropsychology, 11, 855–870.
Wechsler, D. (1997). The Wechsler Memory Scale, Third Winocur, G. (1990). Anterograde and retrograde
Edition (WMS-III) Administration and Scoring amnesia in rats with dorsal hippocampal or
Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological dorsomedial thalamic lesions. Behavioral Brain
Corporation. Research, 38, 145–154.
Weiner, R. D., Rogers, H. J., Welch, C. A., Davidson, Wixted, J. T., & Squire, L. R. (2004). Recall and recog-
J. R. T., Miller, R. D., Weir, D., et al. (1984). ECT nition are equally impaired in patients with selec-
stimulus parameters and electrode placement: tive hippocampal damage. Cognitive, Affective &
Relevance to therapeutic and adverse effects. In B. Behavioral Neuroscience, 4, 58–66.
Lerer, R. D. Weiner, and R. H. Bellmaker (Eds.), Zangwill, O. L. (1977). The amnesic syndrome. In C.
ECT: Basic Mechanisms (pp. 139–147). London: W. M. Whitty and O. L. Zangwill (Eds.), Amnesia:
John Libbey and Company. clinical, psychological, and medicolegal aspects (pp.
Weiskrantz, L., & Warrington, E. K. (1979). 104–117). London: Butterworths.
Conditioning in amnesic patients. Zec, R. F., Zellers, D., Belman, J., Miller, J., Matthews,
Neuropsychologia, 17, 187–194. J., Ferneau-Belman, D., et al. (2001). Long-term
Welsh, K., Butters, N., Hughes, J., Mohs, R., & consequences of severe closed head injury on epi-
Heyman, A. (1991). Detection of abnormal mem- sodic memory. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
ory decline in mild cases of Alzheimer’s disease Neuropsychology, 23, 671–691.
using CERAD neuropsychological measures. Zola-Morgan, S., Cohen, N. J., & Squire, L. R. (1983).
Archives of Neurology, 48, 278–281. Recall of remote episodic memory in amnesia.
Wheeler, M. A., Stuss, D. T., & Tulving, E. (1995). Neuropsychologia, 21, 487–500.
Frontal lobe damage produces episodic mem- Zola-Morgan, S., & Squire, L. R. (1990). The primate
ory impairment. Journal of the International hippocampal formation: Evidence for a time-lim-
Neuropsychological Society, 1, 525–536. ited role in memory storage. Science, 250, 288–290.
Wilson, B. A. (1987). Rehabilitation of memory. New Zola-Morgan, S., & Squire, L. R. (1993). Neuroanatomy
York: Guilford Press. of memory. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 16,
Wilson, B. A. (2008). Neuropsychological rehabil- 547–563.
itation. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4, Zola-Morgan, S., Squire, L. R., & Amaral, D. G.
141–162. (1986). Human amnesia and the medial temporal
Wilson, B. A., Cockburn, J., Baddeley, A. D., & region: Enduring memory impairment following
Hiorns, R. (1989). The development and valida- a bilateral lesion limited to field CA1 of the hippo-
tion of a test battery for detecting and monitoring campus. Journal of Neuroscience, 6, 2950–2967.
Part III

Psychosocial Consequences of
Neuropsychological Impairment
This page intentionally left blank
24

Neurobehavioral Consequences
of Traumatic Brain Injury
Sureyya Dikmen, Joan Machamer, and Nancy Temkin

Traumatic brain injury (TBI) is of major pub- Figure 24–4 shows the rates by age, gender,
lic health significance, affecting approximately and external cause. Falls are most frequent in
1.4 million people in the United States each year children and in those older than 75. Moving
(Langlois et al., 2004; Thurman et al., 1999). Of vehicle accidents and assaults are relatively even
these about 50,000 die and 230,000 are admitted across age groups with perhaps higher rates
to an acute care hospital and survive until dis- for ages 10–35. While head injuries can affect
charge (Thurman et al., 1999). About 1.1 million anyone, the rates are higher in those with less
are seen in emergency rooms and discharged, education, minority status, and lower socioeco-
and countless others are either not reported or nomic status. Alcohol intoxication is involved in
are seen in private doctor’s offices. An estimated 1/3–1/2 of the hospitalized cases of TBI. History
80,000–90,000 persons are left with long-term of substance abuse predating the injury is also
disability (Sosin et al., 1995) (See Figure 24–1). high (Bombardier et al., 2003; Corrigan, 1995;
Hospitalization rates for TBI have decreased Dikmen et al., 1995a).
steadily since 1980s as seen in Figure 24–2. It is Perhaps the definition of consequences of TBI
noteworthy that the most dramatic decline has as formulated by the Consensus Conference in
been for those with mild TBI. Although various 1998 captures best the enormity and variety of
factors may be responsible for the decline seen consequences:
for those with mild TBI, such as lower incidence
Regardless of severity, consequences rarely are
rates due to prevention efforts, it appears that this limited to one set of symptoms, clearly delineated
decline might be primarily due to changes in hos- impairments or a disability that affects only part
pital admission policies (Thurman et al., 1999). of a person’s life. Rather, consequences of TBI often
Of all TBIs, approximately 75%–80% are affect human functions along a continuum: from
considered mild (Centers for Disease Control altered physiological functions of cells; through
and Prevention, 2004—www.cdc.gov/injury) neurological, neuropsychological and psychological
While the majority of individuals with a mild impairments; to medical problems and disabilities
brain injury recover within days to months that affect the individual with TBI, family, friends,
after injury, others, including a fraction of the community as well as society in general. (Consensus
milds and those with more severe TBIs, expe- conference. Rehabilitation of persons with traumatic
brain injury. NIH Consensus Development Panel
rience long-term impairments that affect their
on Rehabilitation of Persons with Traumatic Brain
ability to resume their prior social roles and Injury, 1999)
responsibilities.
Figure 24–3 shows rates of TBI per 100,000 Given the rate of mortality and disabilities,
by age and gender. Males outnumber females in TBI costs are very high both economically and
all age groups except perhaps after age 75. in terms of the suffering engendered. Max et al.

597
598 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

50,000
deaths

235,000 At least I.4 million TBI's occur in


hospitalizations the United States Each Year

1,111,000
emergency department visits

???
receiving other medical care or no care

Figure 24–1. Average annual number of traumatic brain injury-related emergency department visits, hos-
pitalizations, and deaths, United States, 1995–2001. (From: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2004
[http://www.cdc.gov/ncipc/pub-res/TBI_in_US_04/TBI in the US_Jan_2006.pdf]).

140
Mild Moderate
Hospitalization rate per 100,000 people

120 Severe Unknown

100

80

60

40

20

0
1980 1983 1986 1989 1992 1995
Year

Figure 24–2. Incidence rates of TBI-related hospitalizations in the United States by category of severity,
1980–1995. (From: National Hospital Discharge Survey, National Center for Health Statistics, Centers for
Disease Control and Prevention from: Thurman D, Guerrero J. [1999]. Trends in hospitalization associated
with traumatic brain injury. JAMA, 282, 954–957. Copyright © 1999, American Medical Association. All rights
reserved.)

(1991) estimated the lifetime costs of head inju- of impairments, disabilities, and participation
ries sustained in 1985 to be $44 billion in 1988 problems that occur in the survivors of such
dollars. The majority of the cost (54%) was not injuries. This chapter is a selective review of
for direct health care but was rather incurred by neuropsychological and psychosocial outcome
lost productivity and dependency of survivors. following TBI and some of the factors that con-
More recent estimates put the cost at $60 billion tribute to these outcomes with emphasis on the
in 2000 (Finkelstein et al., 2006). results of our studies. We will also briefly review
Over the last 20–25 years considerable infor- selected topics of recent interest: genetic factors
mation has accumulated regarding the nature in relation to outcome, whether TBI presents a
Neurobehavioral Consequences of Traumatic Brain Injury 599

1,600

1,400 In almost every age group,


TBI rates are higher for
1,200 males than females

1,000

800
Male
600 Female

400

200

0
4

5
0–

5–

–1

–1

–2

–3

–4

–5

–6

–7

–>7
10

15

20

25

35

45

55

65
Figure 24–3. Average annual traumatic brain injury-related rates for emergency department visits, hospital-
izations, and deaths, by age group and sex, United States, 1995–2001. (From: Centers for Disease Control and
Prevention, 2004 [http://www.cdc.gov/ncipc/pub-res/TBI_in_US_04/TBI in the US_Jan_2006.pdf]).

700
Falls are the leading cause
600 of TBI; rates are highest
among those ages 0 to 4 and
500 those aged 75 and older

400
Falls
300

200

100
Motor vehicle
0 Assault
4

5
0–

5–

–1

–1

–2

–3

–4

–5

–6

–7

–>7
10

15

20

25

35

45

55

65

Figure 24–4. Average annual traumatic brain injury-related rates for emergency department visits, hospitaliza-
tions, and deaths, by age group and external cause, United States, 1995–2001. (From: Centers for Disease Control
and Prevention, 2004 [http://www.cdc.gov/ncipc/pub-res/TBI_in_US_04/TBI in the US_Jan_2006.pdf]).

risk factor for later neurodegenerative diseases, determined by a host of factors including sever-
and aging with TBI. ity of the brain injury, characteristics of the per-
son injured, time since injury, and a number
of factors after the injury that may facilitate or
Outcome and Determinants of Outcome
hinder recovery.
Outcome following TBI ranges from death to A brief review of indices used to assess sever-
full recovery. Time to maximal recovery may ity of injury is in order. Severity of closed/blunt
range from days or weeks to years, depending injuries most often sustained in civilian TBIs is
on the severity of injury, the age at the time typically measured by various indices of degree
of injury, and the type of function in ques- of altered state of consciousness, reflecting the
tion, among other factors. The outcome is degree of diff use brain injury. Glasgow Coma
600 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

Table 24–1. Glasgow Coma Scale


Best Eye Opening Best Motor
Code Code
4 Spontaneous 6 Obeys commands
3 To verbal stimulation 5 Localizes to pain
2 To pain 4 Abnormal withdrawal to pain
1 None 3 Flexion to pain
2 Extension to pain
1 No response to pain
Best Verbal TBI Classification by Total GCS
Code
5 Oriented Mild 13–15
4 Confused Moderate 9–12
3 Inappropriate words Severe 3–8
2 Unintelligible sounds Complicated
1 No verbalization Mild 13–15 with CT abnormalities

Scale (GCS; Teasdale & Jennett, 1974), the memory to more normal levels. Time to fol-
most commonly used measure, is an index of low commands is the time from injury to the
depth of coma at a specified time after injury point when the individual is able to follow
(see Table 24–1). Th is scale assesses eye open- simple commands consistently as defined by
ing, verbal responses, and motor responses to the motor component of the GCS (Teasdale &
stimulation, with scores ranging from 3 to 15. Jennett, 1974). This index can be often obtained
Severe injuries are considered to be those with from careful review of nursing notes during
GCS of 8 or less. Moderate TBI is considered acute hospitalization.
when the GCS is between 9 and 12, and mild Other indices of severity of brain injury
TBI when the GCS is 13 or greater. Generally, that have been examined include CT abnor-
the lowest score obtained in the fi rst 24 hours malities and pupilary size and reactivity.
after resuscitation is used for classification Secondary insults such as hypoxia, hypoten-
purposes. Patients with GCS of 13–15 with CT sion, and increased intracranial pressure also
abnormalities are considered as complicated relate to outcome. TBIs manifest themselves in
mild and thought to have outcomes similar to various combinations of these severity indica-
those with moderate injuries (Williams et al., tors. Unfortunately, there is no higher-order
1990), although more research is needed in classifications system developed to date that
this area. takes into account some combination of these
Measures of duration of impaired conscious- severity indices and relates them to outcome,
ness typically are better predictors of outcome in a clinician user friendly manner. An excep-
than measures of depth of coma such as the tion is the work of the International Mission
GCS. These measures include posttraumatic on Prognosis and Analysis of Clinical Trials in
amnesia (PTA) and time from injury to con- Traumatic Brain Injury (IMPACT), which uses
sistently follow commands. PTA is the period multiple factors including demographics and
of time from injury to the time when regular severity indicators to provide predictions of
memory for day-to-day events return. PTA outcomes for individual cases through a Web
can be determined retrospectively (Levin, site. However, the outcome predicted is a very
1995) or prospectively (Russell & Smith, 1961). global index of overall functioning at 6 months
Prospectively assessed PTA may be a better postinjury (e.g., dead versus alive; unfavorable
predictor but much more difficult to assess, (dead, vegetative, and severe disability) versus
or less likely to be available to the clinician as favorable outcome (moderate disability and
it requires daily monitoring of the return of good recovery).
Neurobehavioral Consequences of Traumatic Brain Injury 601

functioning postinjury based on the preinjury


Challenges to the Determination
intellectual or educational level of the person
of TBI-Related Losses in Civilian
with TBI. They compared the change in intel-
Injuries
lectual function on the AGCT from pre- to post-
injury to see if those who had low educational
Unavailability of Baseline Information levels or performed poorly on the AGCT before
One of the biggest challenges to studying neu- the injury showed a greater decline than those
robehavioral outcomes of civilian TBI is the who did better initially. They found no evidence
absence of adequate information (i.e., baseline) to support a relationship between higher level
about the condition in which the individual of functioning preinjury and less decline after
started prior to the injury. Important questions the injury.
regarding outcomes of TBI include the determi- In the case of civilian TBIs, obtaining base-
nation of the nature and severity of the losses line information has been possible in sports
sustained, the recovery trajectory, and, ulti- injuries. In these studies players are examined
mately, the nature and degree of residual deficits preseason. This design has allowed research-
if any and how to predict them. These questions ers to examine those injured versus those
can be more accurately and efficiently answered who have not been injured, consider change
if one has good information about where the in performance from pre- to postinjury in the
individual started from. Unfortunately, for the two groups, and, by monitoring performance
majority of the civilian injuries such informa- over days, ascertain when the results of those
tion is unavailable. injured normalize. A fair amount of literature
Two injury circumstances where baseline has accumulated regarding neuropsycholog-
information has been available and have shown ical recovery from sports injuries. Please see
the value of such information are those of war Belanger and Vanderploeg (2005) and Cernich
and sports injuries. In the case of war injuries, et al., (2007) for reviews. These studies suggest
the Army General Classification Test (AGCT) that recovery of neuropsychological deficits
scores used in World War II and the later devel- from mild sports injuries seem to occur over
oped Armed Forces Qualification Test (AFQT), about 7–14 days. The most significant finding is
which are correlated with indices of general that those sustaining TBI seem to fail to bene-
intelligence and were administered at induction fit from practice effects expected from repeated
into the army, were used as baseline information exposure to the tests.
(Grafman et al., 1986; Schwab et al., 1993). The While the war injuries or sports injuries ben-
results of these studies have elucidated impor- efit from the availability of baseline informa-
tant information about the role of preinjury tion, they represent very specialized subgroups
intelligence on outcome from head injury. To of TBI. Those studied with injuries from previ-
give a couple of examples, Grafman et al. (1986) ous conflicts have had penetrating injuries with
studied Vietnam veterans with penetrating head major and distinct localized brain damage.
injuries of either right or left hemisphere lesions Sports injuries tend to be very mild, and in both
and compared structural lesion location, total cases the population tends to be principally
brain tissue volume loss, and preinjury AFQT healthy young adults.
scores with postinjury intellectual functioning.
Their results showed that preinjury intelligence Choice of Control Groups
was a more significant predictor of tests requir-
ing a number of complementary cognitive pro- In the absence of baseline information, per-
cesses than either the volume of brain tissue loss formance of a comparison group allows deter-
or lesion location, highlighting the importance mination of deficits in a group with TBI. The
of baseline cognitive functioning in explain- choice of comparison group to be used is
ing postinjury cognitive outcome. Weinstein important. One needs to consider compara-
and Teuber (1957) provided information about bility on a number of dimensions not only on
a different question. They examined whether age, education, and gender but also on some-
there was a differential decline in intellectual times difficult-to-define temperamental and
602 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

sociodemographic factors that may predispose effects. Those more likely to be lost to follow-up,
or characterize those injured. Such groups not unexpectedly, tend to be those with lower
may include friends or family members of the education and problematic psychosocial his-
injured based on the assumption that “birds of tories such as drug and alcohol abuse. Results
a feather flock together” (Dikmen et al., 1986; based on those that can be followed may not pro-
Dikmen & Temkin, 1987; McLean et al., 1993). vide an accurate picture of head injury effects.
Another type of comparison group is a gen- Although these challenges are formidable,
eral trauma group, which includes persons who they are not insurmountable, and considerable
sustained injury to other body parts but not to effort has been put into obtaining useful out-
the brain. This kind of a group, although not as come information for the broad class of civilian
good as a baseline in some respects, may take injuries. Choice of control group and adequate
into account lifestyle similarities as well as inju- sample size are critical. The 1800 TBI cases and
ries to other body parts which could impact 230 controls in our data repository is the largest
neurobehavioral functioning. Because of dif- series of cases enrolled at injury and followed
ferences in the populations, use of normative longitudinally with neuropsychological and
samples to judge patient results in clinical work functional status measures of outcome and will
can be also misleading, particularly when the provide the basis for much of what follows.
patient has sustained a mild TBI and the conse-
quences are mild and subtle. Normal variation
Neuropsychological Outcomes
in performance can mimic or mask TBI effects
(Dikmen et al., 2001). Neuropsychological impairments refer to
cognitive difficulties such as problems with
attention, working memory, episodic mem-
Representativeness of the Cases
ory, information-processing speed, executive
with TBI Studied
functions, language, and visual-spatial skills.
If we are interested in the nature and magnitude Neuropsychological difficulties are sensitive to
of limitations due to TBI, the samples studied the effects of TBI and have received considerable
need to be representative of persons with TBI. research attention because of their importance
Enrollment in the study needs to be based on in compromising the functioning of individuals
the characteristics of the injury, rather than on in everyday life and their quality of life. Various
outcome. For instance, a clinic population seen factors influence the nature and severity of cog-
for impairments and disabilities associated with nitive impairment, such as the characteristics of
TBI is inappropriate. The latter type of a sample the person injured, severity of the brain injury,
may provide very useful information about the and time from injury to the time of testing,
type of problems that may happen after injury which reflects recovery.
but will inflate the magnitude of the problems The severity of the brain injury clearly has a
as was seen in earlier mild TBI studies (e.g., decisive effect on neuropsychological outcome.
Rimel et al., 1981). Severity indices such as coma length (Dikmen
et al., 1995b), coma depth (Teasdale & Jennet,
1974), one or both nonreactive pupils, and pres-
Loss to Follow-up
ence and severity of mass lesions have signifi-
Finally, another big challenge is performing lon- cant impact on outcome.
gitudinal studies with minimal loss to follow-up. In one of our studies (Dikmen et al., 1995b) we
This is a big problem in TBI studies as the pop- examined the neuropsychological functioning
ulation injured tend to be young and mobile. of a group of 436 persons with TBI on a compre-
Enrolling and maintaining subjects in the stud- hensive battery of neuropsychological measures
ies is a difficult, labor-intensive, and expensive 1 year after injury. These patients were nonselect,
yet critical endeavor because loss to follow-up is representative cases enrolled in the study at the
not random (Barber, 2003; Corrigan et al., 2003), time of injury and followed to 1 year with a high
and thus, if the loss to follow-up rate is high, the follow-up rate of 85%. Functions assessed ranged
results could over- or underestimate head injury from motor to higher-level executive functions
Neurobehavioral Consequences of Traumatic Brain Injury 603

and included finger tapping speed, sustained in explaining the difficulties in everyday life of
and divided attention, memory and learning, persons with TBI. In the employment literature,
verbal and performance intelligence, process- simple and complex motor speed measures have
ing speed, reasoning, and overall neuropsycho- been shown as being important in the employ-
logical competency index. The performances of ability of both head-injured persons and those
these subjects, as a group, and also subdivided with epilepsy (Dikmen & Morgan, 1980; Fraser
by various severity indices, were compared with et al., 1988).
a group of general trauma subjects who had sus- Dose–response relationships can be observed
tained injury but not to the head. on other measures of brain injury severity.
Table 24–2 shows the performances of the Figure 24–5 shows the relationship between
TBI cases divided by time from injury to follow overall neuropsychological impairment
simple commands (TFC), an index of length of (Halstead Impairment Index) and several brain
impaired consciousness. Overall, the TBI sub- injury severity indices including GCS (an index
jects as a group performed significantly more of depth of coma), neurosurgical interven-
poorly than the general trauma group. The tion for evacuation of space-occupying lesions,
impairments were not restricted to attention, and number of nonreactive pupils. Halstead
memory, or speed of information processing, Impairment index, which is based on perfor-
abilities thought to be sensitive to TBI. Rather, mance on seven measures, ranges from 0 to
the impairments were diff use, and also involved 1, with 0 indicating that none of the measures
motor skills, general intellectual functions, new was in the impaired range and 1 that all were.
problem solving, and overall impairments. The Dose–response relationship between this index
magnitude and the pervasiveness of impair- and the various severity indicators are obvious.
ments, however, depended on severity of the What is noteworthy is the range of variabil-
brain injury. There was clearly a relationship ity in performance within each of the severity
between TFC and level of cognitive impair- steps across the different severity indicators.
ments 1 year after injury. As a group, the per- Those with the least or less severity are quite
formances of the subgroup with TFC less than homogenous in their performances, similar to
1 hour were comparable to those of the general the trauma controls. The performances of those
trauma group. Selective impairments on mea- with greater severities are quite heterogeneous,
sures of attention and memory start to emerge with the exception of the very severe group
with longer TFC of 1–24 hours. With longer as represented with TFC of 1 month or more.
TFC lengths such as 1–2 weeks or more, nearly These results indicate that although there is a
all measures are impaired, with pronounced and dose–response relationship between severity
consistent impairments observed in the groups and neuropsychological performance, there is
with 2–4 weeks or more. It is interesting to note great variability within each of the severity levels
that more consistent differences between the TBI indicating that other factors are at play to either
subgroups and the control group are on mea- exacerbate or mitigate the impact of severity on
sures of speed (simple and psychomotor) and performance or the recovery potential.
composite measures (Performance Intelligence The degree of neuropsychological impair-
Quotient—PIQ, Halstead Impairment Index) ments is greater soon after injury with recov-
rather than more specific measures of attention ery occurring over days, weeks, months, and
and memory. While the constructs of attention maybe years. Degree of improvement and
and memory are clearly impacted by TBI, mea- degree of residuals relate to degree of original
sures of these constructs are not very reliable loss. Figure 24–6 shows recovery of WAIS-PIQ
(Dikmen et al., 1999). Composite measures may (Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Performance
derive their significance because of increased Intelligence Quotient) from 1 to 12 months in
measurement reliability due to the multiplic- subgroups of patients divided on the basis of
ity of the observations they are based on. The time to follow commands. Those with more
construct of speed (or processing speed) holds severe brain injury show greater loss at 1 month
promise as an important area of investigation compared to trauma controls. Those with
from both theoretical and clinical perspectives greater loss have more room to improve with the
604 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

Table 24–2. Median Scores for Trauma Controls and Subjects with TBI Divided by Time to Follow
Commands.a
Measure Trauma <1 1–24 25 7–13 14–28 ≥29 ri
controlsb hourc hoursd hours–6 daysf daysg daysh
dayse
Motor Functions
Finger Tapping, dominant 53 51 52 47*** 49 ** 42*** 11*** –.51
hand
Finger Tapping, nondomi- 50 48 48 44*** 45** 38*** 16*** –.47
nant hand
Namewriting, dominant 0.50 0.50 0.53 0.60 0.60 * 0.69 *** 3.15*** .49
hand
Namewriting, nondominant 1.30 1.41 1.44 1.87* 1.64*** 1.82*** 3.60 *** .43
hand
Attention and flexibility
Seashore Rhythm Test 27 27 26 27 26 24*** 16*** –.42
Trail Making Test, Part A 23 22 25 27** 26* 40 *** 101*** .57
Trail Making Test, Part B 56 57 72* 63 71 132*** 301*** .50
Stroop Color and Word Test, 42 42 46 44 48*** 70 *** 151*** .51
Part 1
Stroop Color and Word Test, 96 98 108 107 122*** 162*** 300 *** .49
Part 2
Memory
WMS-LM 11 10.5 10 10 9 8** 0.50 *** –.39
WMS-VR 11 12 10 11 11 9* 4*** –.38
SR-RCL 89 89 84* 86* 75*** 69 *** 24*** –.53
WMS-LM, delayed 9 9 8 7* 7* 6*** 0 *** –.46
WMS-VR, delayed 10 11 10 9 10 6*** 0 *** –.45
SR-RCL 30-minute delay 9 8 8* 8 7*** 5*** 0 *** –.45
SR-RCL, 4-hour delay 8 8 7 8 5*** 4*** 0 *** –.46
Verbal
WAIS-VIQ 106 106 101 101 98 92*** 57*** –.45
Performance Skills
WAIS-PIQ 112 110 106 102*** 102** 90 *** 56*** –.55
TPT-T 0.36 0.35 0.40 0.61*** 0.51*** 1.10 *** 5.75*** .61
Reasoning
Category Test 24 22 28 41 32 72*** 112*** .54
Overall
Halstead Impairment Index 0.1 0.1 0.3 0.4*** 0.4** 0.7*** 1.0 *** .59
Notes: WMS-LM = Wechsler Memory Scale Logical Memory. WMS-VR = Wechsler Memory Scale Visual Reproduction.
SR-RCL = Selective Reminding Test Total Recall. TPT-T = Tactual Performance Test time per block. WAIS-VIQ = Wechsler
Adult Intelligence Scale Verbal Intelligence Quotient. WAIS-PIQ = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale- Performance
Intelligence Quotient.
a
For example, the median WAIS-PIQ for subjects with TBI who took less than 1 hour to follow commands is 110. The
median decreases systematically as time to follow commands increases and subjects with TBI -injured who took 29 days or
more to follow commands have a median WAIS-PIQ of 56.
b
n = 121. cn = 161. dn = 100. en = 52. f n = 37. gn = 32. hn = 53; median is untestable, lowest observed score recorded.
i
All significant at p < .001.
*
p < .05. **p < .01 ***p < .001.
Source: Dikmen et al. (1995b). Published by APA, reprinted with permission.

slopes of improvement appearing to be propor- due to floor effects of the measure. Those with
tional to the degree of initial loss. An exception milder injuries have lost less and, thus, have less
is the most severe group (i.e., TFC greater than room to improve. In spite of greater improve-
29 days). This group also showed improvement, ment, those with greater initial loss end up with
but the improvement is difficult to observe greater impairments 1 year after injury. These
Neurobehavioral Consequences of Traumatic Brain Injury 605

1.2 1.2

1 1

0.8 0.8

0.6 0.6

0.4 0.4

0.2 0.2

0 0
TC 13–15 9–12 6–8 3–5 TC < 1 hr 1– 25 hr– 7– 14– 29 days +
(n=120) (n=243) (n=73) (n=77) (n=35) 24 hr 6 days 13 days 28 days
(n=120) (n=160) (n=100) (n=51) (n=37) (n=31) (n=53)
Glasgow coma scale score Time to follow commands
1.2 1.2

1 1

0.8 0.8

0.6 0.6

0.4 0.4

0.2 0.2

0 0
TC None EDH only SDH TC 0 1 2
(n=120) (n=331) (n=23) (n=78) (n=120) (n=377) (n=21) (n=33)
Neurosurgical intervention
# Non-reactive pupils

Figure 24–5. Halstead Impairment Index by neurological severity indices. (From: Dikmen et al. [1995].
Neuropsychological outcome at 1-year post head injury. Neuropsychology, 9(1), 80–90. Published by APA,
reprinted with permission.)
Note: TC = Trauma Control, EDH = Epidural Hematoma, SDH = Subdural Hematoma. Halstead Impairment
Index = 1.1 is the value assigned to untestable subjects.

results suggest that the degree of initial deficit adjusting for normal aging. There was evidence
is a significant determinant of the subsequent of a greater negative effect of age in more severe
amount of recovery and the residual deficits injuries. This was seen in two ways. The differ-
(Dikmen et al., 1983). ence in performance between young and older
Of the demographic factors, age has a major individuals was greater in more severe inju-
impact on outcome. The clearest evidence for ries, and the youngest age at which one could
age effects comes from studies based on severe begin to see worse performance was lower with
TBI and using global outcomes, as will be increased severity. The age effect on neuropsy-
reviewed later under psychosocial outcomes. chological functioning may be the result of the
Neuropsychological studies showing differ- fact that head injury effects on an older brain
ential neuropsychological impairments as a are comparatively worse than on a younger one,
function of age have been fewer perhaps due to or because of greater plasticity of the younger
small sample sizes in any given study because of brain to recover compared with an older brain
higher mortality among older individuals and (Dikmen & Machamer, 1995).
difficulties in obtaining appropriate controls. While no one would doubt the negative
We have examined the impact of age on an neuropsychological consequences of moder-
extensive battery of neuropsychological mea- ate or more severe TBIs, the neuropsychologi-
sures in a group of 219 subjects with TBI (Ross cal sequelae of mild traumatic brain have been
et al., 1994). There is clear evidence for differen- much more controversial. Interest and contro-
tial head injury effect as a function of age after versy regarding mild TBI derives from the fact
606 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

120

<1 hour
100 1–24 hours
25 hours – 6 days
80
7–13 days
PIQ

60

14–28 days
40
29+days

20

0
1–month 1–year

Figure 24–6. Median PIQ score by time to follow command groups. (From: Dikmen et al. [1995].
Neuropsychological outcome at 1-year post head injury. Neuropsychology, 9(1), 80–90. Published by APA,
reprinted with permission.)
Note: Time to follow commands is the time from injury to when the individual is able to follow simple com-
mands consistently as defi ned by the motor component of the Glasgow Coma Scale. Time to follow commands
group < 1 hour n = 161, 1–24 hours n = 100, 25 hours to 6 days n = 52, 7–13 days n = 37, 14–28 days n = 32,
≥ 29 days n = 53.

that they constitute the majority of the injured, Interested readers are referred to other publica-
and determination of whether or not a mild TBI tions (Belanger et al., 2005; Carroll et al., 2004;
has occurred is often based on subtle findings Cassidy et al., 2004).
and self-reports. Also, oftentimes the disabil-
ity is not easily explained on the basis of objec-
tive findings. Based on information available Psychosocial/Functional
in the literature, most would agree that mild Status Outcomes
head injuries are indeed associated with early TBIs can leave those who survive them with
neuropsychological difficulties that resolve by various limitations or disabilities in everyday
1–3 months postinjury in the majority of the life. These limitations may involve basic areas of
cases (Binder et al., 1997; Dikmen et al., 1986; functioning such as personal care and ambula-
Gronwall & Wrightson, 1974; Levin et al., 1987; tion to higher-level functions such as social rela-
Macciocchi et al., 1996; Ponsford et al., 2000). tionships, work, and leisure. While the nature
After conducting meta-analysis of published of the disabilities is multifarious, some impor-
data Binder et al. (1997) concluded that the tant questions are how often do these problems
effect size of mild head injury on neuropsy- occur and what are the predictors of those who
chological measures is small. This is the case in are likely to have them?
single uncomplicated head injury in previously The most commonly used index to describe
healthy young people. Less agreement exists, overall outcome in TBI is the Glasgow Outcome
however, regarding the persistence of neuro- Scale (GOS; Jennett & Bond, 1975) and, more
psychological impairments, their cause if they recently, its revised version, the GOSE (Wilson
persist, and whether or not they are able to et al., 1998). The GOS has five categories: death,
explain long-term disabilities. persistent vegetative state, severe disability,
The topic of mild TBI is too rich and broad moderate disability, and good recovery. The
to be adequately covered in this chapter. differentiation between the last two categories
Neurobehavioral Consequences of Traumatic Brain Injury 607

is based on the patient’s dependence on others hospital admission such as glucose, platelets,
for self-care activities and the patient’s ability and hemoglobin levels (McHugh et al., 2007;
to participate in normal social life. The GOSE Murray et al., 2007; Van Beek et al., 2007). The
subdivides the three better GOS categories into IMPACT study Web site (http://mgzlx4.eras-
upper and lower good recovery, moderate dis- musmc.nl/impact/index.php?id=1,0,0,1,0,0)
ability, and severe disability categories. The best allows one to calculate global outcome for indi-
data on overall outcome and its predictors in vidual cases based on demographic and severity
patients with severe head injury based on the variables.
GOS are those contributed by three large mul- More detailed information on outcome for
ticenter studies: the International Coma Data survivors of TBI, and with a broader spec-
Bank, the Pilot Phase of the National Coma trum of TBI severity, is shown in Figure 24–7
Data Bank, and the Full-Phase Coma Data as assessed by the Sickness Impact Profi le
Bank (Chesnut et al., 2000; Foulkes et al., 1991; (Dikmen et al., 1995c). The subjects were 410
Marshall et al., 1991). Severe head injury was hospitalized adults with a brain injury sever-
defined as a GCS of 8 or less at 6 hours of injury. ity ranging from mild to severe and prospec-
On the basis of large series of cases with severe tively followed to 1 year .The SIP results of this
TBI, approximately 45% of patients die early group were compared with a group of friends
and half of those surviving sustain sufficiently and a group of trauma controls (i.e., a group
severe impairments to make them totally or sig- who had sustained an injury that spared the
nificantly dependent on others. Even the good head). The results on this measure represent
recovery category of the GOS, which includes percent dysfunction in various areas of every-
approximately 50% of the surviving patients, day life the subject endorses as a result of health
does not assure close to preinjury level of func- or injury. Thus, higher scores mean worse per-
tioning (Tate et al., 1989). It is important to point formance. As can be seen, friend controls in this
out that persons with severe head injuries who age group do not endorse much health-related
survive long enough to be hospitalized consti- dysfunction. In contrast, TBI cases do endorse
tute approximately 20% of all hospitalized cases problems in almost all areas, especially in the
for TBI. The rest have a spectrum of less severe areas of employment, recreation, and cognitive
injuries. functioning. Note, however, that trauma con-
Factors that predict outcome on the GOS have trols also endorse problems but not to the same
recently been examined in the IMPACT study, extent, which suggests that some of the disabili-
which combined data collected over the past ties seen in TBI cases might be related to other
20 years on TBI into one large database of over injuries sustained in the same accident.
9000 patients (Marmarou et al., 2007a). These The effects of these injuries can be long last-
data came from eight clinical trials and three ing and permanent in the more severely injured
epidemiological studies, and consist of infor- persons. Long-term outcome at 3–5 years after
mation collected from time of injury to postre- injury was examined in a group of adults with
suscitation with outcome on the GOS evaluated moderate to severe TBI using the Functional
at 6 months postinjury. The results of a series Status Examination (Dikmen et al., 2003).
of univariate and multivariate analyses revealed The Functional Status Examination, through
that poorer outcome on the GOS is related to a structured interview, assigns a rating rang-
age at the time of injury (as age increases, out- ing from no difference as compared to prein-
come gets worse), low GCS scores, especially jury to difficulty performing the activity but
the motor score, one or more nonreactive independent, and needing partial or total help.
pupils, and CT findings including traumatic Figure 24–8 shows the limitations reported
subarachnoid hemorrhage, swelling, midline by the subjects. Significant limitations were
shift, and presence of mass lesions (Marmarou reported by subjects in nearly all areas of daily
et al., 2007b; Maas et al., 2007; Murray et al., living assessed. Recovery to preinjury levels
2007; Mushkudiani et al., 2007). Other vari- ranged from 65% in personal care to about 40%
ables related to poor outcome on the GOS were in cognitive competency, major activity, and lei-
hypotension, hypoxia, and laboratory values at sure and recreation. The degree of limitations,
608 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

12% 6%
Ambulation
Sleep/rest 10% 6%
1%

12% Alertness 16%


Emotional p < .001
10% behavior 9%
behavior
1%

Body care/ 4% 7%
TBI Communication
movement 3% 5%
(n=410)

TC
Home 8% 16%
(n=124)
management 8% Recreation 12%
FC 1%
(n=88)
5% p < .05 1%
Mobility Eating
2% 1%

Social 10% p < .01 23%


Work p < .001
interaction 7% 14%

0% 5% 10% 15% 20% 25% 0% 5% 10% 15% 20% 25%


% Dysfunction at 1–year % Dysfunction at 1–year

Figure 24–7. Sickness impact profi le (SIP): Mean percent dysfunction at 1-year. (Adapted from: Dikmen et al.
[1995]. One year psychosocial outcome in head injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society,
1, 67–77. Reprinted with permission.)
Note: TBI = Traumatic Brain Injury, TC = Trauma Control, FC = Friend Control. Significance levels refer to
the results of the comparisons between TBI and TC on individual Scales.

however, was related to the severity of injury GCS, global functional status on the GOS, and
(Dikmen et al., 2003). performance on neuropsychological measures
A functional area that has received consider- at one month after injury as seen in Table 24–3.
able attention is employment. Return to work is For example, the relative risk of unemployment
clearly compromised after the injury due to the was about 6 times as high as expected for those
various impairments and disabilities resulting with GCSs of 3–8, and about 3.5 for those with
from the injury to the brain as well as injuries GCSs of 13–15. With respect to functional sta-
sustained to other parts of the body in the same tus at 1 month, those with good recovery were
accident. 1.83 more likely while those with severe dis-
Doctor et al. (2005) examined, among those ability were 6.79 times more likely to be unem-
working preinjury, the risk of unemployment 1 ployed at 1 year. Table 24–3 also shows different
year after TBI relative to expected risk of unem- levels of performance on WAIS-PIQ at 1 month
ployment for the sample under a validated risk- in relation to employment status at 1 year. Note
adjusted econometric model of employment in the increasing levels of risk of unemployment at
the US population. The sample included 418 1 year as compared to expected with increasing
preinjury workers hospitalized for a head injury, levels of difficulties on PIQ at 1 month.
with the severity ranging from mild to severe. The importance of the same factors was dem-
The results indicated that for the overall group onstrated, not only in the rates of unemploy-
42% of TBI cases were unemployed versus 9% ment at 1 year but also in the timing of return
expected. After accounting for underlying risk to work in another study (Dikmen et al., 1994),
of unemployment in the general population, using some of the same subjects and analyzed
the relative risk of unemployment varied as a with survival methodology. Figure 24–9 shows
function of demographics. For example, it was rates of return to work over time as a function of
higher for males. It also varied as a function of severity of injury as evaluated by the GCS, pre-
severity of the brain injury as determined by injury job stability, peripheral extremity injury
Neurobehavioral Consequences of Traumatic Brain Injury 609

100%

90%

80%

70%
Same as before
60%
Difficulty
50%
Some help
40%
Complete dependence
30%

20%

10%

0%
re

ing

nt

cy

on

n
ve

nc

vit
tio

tio
ca

me

ten

i
Tra
lIv

rat

cti
de
ula

rea
al

ge

pe

teg

ra
en
of
n

rec
na

om
rso

Am

ep

ajo
l in
ard

ma

&
Pe

ind

ec

M
cia
nd

re
me

itiv
Sta

ial

So

isu
Ho

gn
nc

Le
Co
a
Fin

Figure 24–8. Functional Status Examination endorsements at 3–5 years after TBI. (Reprinted from Archives
of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 84(10), Dikmen et al., Outcome 3 to 5 years after moderate to severe
traumatic brain injury, 1449–1457, Copyright © 2003, with permission from Elsevier.)
Note: The percentage of cases at different levels of functioning within each area of everyday life is shown.

Table 24–3. Relative and Excess Risk Values as a Function of Glasgow Coma Scale, GOS, and PIQ
Actual Expected Risk Difference (%) Relative Risk (95% CI)
Unemployed (%) Unemployed (%) (95% CI)
Glasgow Coma Score
13–15 31.1 8.8 22.3 (16.3, 28.7) 3.46 (2.87, 4.28)
9–12 46.4 9.6 36.8 (25.8, 48.0) 4.85 (3.71, 6.02)
3–8 62.1 10.4 51.7 (40.6, 61.8) 5.98 (4.92, 6.96)
GOS at 1 month
Good 15.6 8.5 7.1 (0.8, 15.2) 1.83 (1.10, 2.79)
Moderate 39.8 8.3 31.5 (21.4, 42.1) 4.81 (3.60, 6.10)
Severe 66.7 9.8 56.8 (48.0, 64.9) 6.79 (5.89, 7.61)
PIQ at 1 month
110 and over 18.7 6.8 11.9 (4.0, 22.2) 2.76 (1.60, 4.28)
100–109 18.1 8.7 9.4 (1.2, 20.1) 2.08 (1.15, 3.33)
90–99 41.2 9.9 31.2 (20.7, 42.4) 4.14 (3.08, 5.27)
80–89 38.6 8.7 29.9 (15.6, 45.8) 4.44 (2.80, 6.26)
Below 80 61.4 11.0 50.3 (34.4, 64.6) 5.57 (4.13, 6.86)
Source: Adapted from Doctor et al. (2005). Reprinted with the permission of Cambridge University Press.

and neuropsychological status at 1 month after work at least for a short time by 1-year postin-
injury assessed by the Halstead Impairment jury, compared to 25% of TBI with severe inju-
Index, a composite measure of performance on ries represented by GCS of 3–8. Note also that
Halstead’s Neuropsychological Test Battery. As the rate of return to work is faster for the mild as
shown in Figure 24–9, the rate of return to work compared to the severe TBIs. The rate of return
varies systematically by severity of injury. About to work is slightly better for trauma comparison
80% of TBI with GCS of 13–15 have returned to subjects than for the mild TBI subjects. Preinjury
610 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

stability of employment, severity of extremity from complicated mild (GCS 13–15 with CT
injury, and neuropsychological competency at 1 abnormalities) to severe, with the majority of
month after injury—all these impact the rate of the cases having complicated mild injuries.
return to work, as well as the fraction who have Work stability definitions included amount of
returned at least briefly by 1 or 2 years postin- time worked (amount of time worked divided
jury. Many additional factors influence the rate by time observed postinjury) and maintenance
and timing of return to work, such as age, edu- of uninterrupted employment once a person
cation, and gender (Dikmen et al., 1994). These returned to work. Amount of time worked was
results are consistent with findings by others significantly and systematically related to brain
regarding the importance of injury severity, injury severity, neuropsychological functioning
preinjury productivity, educational levels, and at 1-month postinjury, and preinjury charac-
early cognitive status on postinjury employ- teristics such as prior work stability and earn-
ment (Boake et al., 2001; Nakase-Richardson ings. For example, all of the neuropsychological
et al., 2007; Sherer et al., 2002). measures examined showed highly significant
Although substantial information exists differences with amount of time worked postin-
about factors related to who returns, and time jury. On the other hand, once persons returned
taken to return to work after TBI, less is known to work, the ability to maintain uninterrupted
about the stability of the work experience after employment was largely related to premorbid
the injury. Machamer et al. (2005) studied 165 characteristics such as older age, higher income
preinjury workers with TBI, who were followed before the injury, or a preinjury job with benefits.
for 3–5 years postinjury. Injury severity ranged It was also related to higher neuropsychological

A Control and GCS subgroups B Preinjury job stability


100 100
90 Control 90
80 GCS, 13–15 80 Stable
Returing to work, %

Returing to work, %

70 70
GCS, 9–12
60 60 Unstable
50 50
40 GCS, 3–8 40
30 30
20 20
10 10
0 0
6 12 18 24 6 12 18 24
Time, months Time, months

C AIS extremities D Halstead impairment index


100 100
HII, 0 –.2
90 90
80 AIS, 1–2 80
Returing to work, %

HII, .3–.4
Returing to work, %

70 AIS, 0 70 HII, .5–.7


60 60
AIS, 3–5
50 50
HII, .8–1.0
40 40
30 30
20 20
10 10
0 0
6 12 18 24 6 12 18 24
Time, months Time, months

Figure 24–9. Time to return-to-work by various severity and demographic measures. (From: Dikmen et al.
[1994]. Employment following traumatic head injuries. Archives of Neurology, 51, 177–186. Copyright © 1994,
American Medical Association. All rights reserved.)
Note: GCS = Glasgow Coma Scale, AIS = Abbreviated Injury Scale, HII = Halstead Impairment Index.
Neurobehavioral Consequences of Traumatic Brain Injury 611

functioning 1-month postinjury (reflecting the results have not been uniformly positive in
combined effects of premorbid functioning and fi nding this association at 6 months postinjury
TBI severity), but not related to neurologic indi- on the GOS/GOSE (Diaz-Arrastia et al., 2003;
ces of severity (Machamer et al., 2005). Another Nathoo et al., 2003). Teasdale et al., (2005) in
study examined job stability post-TBI by defin- a follow-up study involving 984 consecutive
ing stability as employment at each of three eval- head injury admissions failed to fi nd their
uation points 1, 2, and 3 or 4 years postinjury. earlier reported association between apoE4
They also reported that job stability was related and poor outcome on the GOS at 6 months.
to multiple factors, including TBI severity, age, They did report an interaction between age
and functional outcome ratings (Kreutzer et al., and apoE4 on outcome such that children and
2003). young adults with apoE4 had worse outcome.
Lack of an association has also been reported
for long-term outcomes, on average, 18 years
Areas of Research of
postinjury (Millar et al., 2003) and for other
Current Interest
6-month outcomes following mild to moder-
ate TBI, including neuropsychological func-
Genetic Factors (APOE4)
tioning, emotional distress, depression, global
and Outcome
functioning, and psychosocial outcome and
Recent evidence has indicated that genetic fac- symptoms (Chamelian et al., 2004).
tors may play a role in outcome following TBI. Undoubtedly, this is an area of great interest
One of the most studied is the apolipoprotein to many. However, more research is needed to
E gene that is involved with transporting lipids unravel the role of apoE4 and other genetic fac-
within the central nervous system. There are tors on outcome following TBI.
three allelic variants in humans, apoE2, apoE3,
and apoE4. The apoE4 variant has been associ-
Aging with Traumatic Brain Injury
ated with poor outcome following TBI (Teasdale
et al., 1997). The reason why apoE4 should be This is a topic of current interest although only
associated with worse outcome is not known, a few studies have addressed it. The issue here is
but is hypothesized to have something to do whether or not the effect of aging is accelerated
with the neural response to injury that in some or abnormal in a previously injured brain. This
way results in increased neurodegeneration kind of study is extremely difficult and costly
(Diaz-Arrastia & Baxter, 2006). Teasdale and to do since it requires a lengthy longitudinal
colleagues studied 89 prospectively recruited investigation and has been most typically car-
traumatic brain-injured subjects admitted to ried out in military populations (Corkin et al.,
a neurosurgery unit and representing a range 1989; Walker & Blumer, 1989). Corkin et al.
of brain injury severity. They compared the (1989) presented some of the first evidence of
presence or absence of apoE4 with outcome on exacerbation of cognitive decline in World War
the GOS at 6 months postinjury. The fi ndings II veterans with penetrating head injuries. They
showed a significant difference in outcome compared 57 head-injured and 27 veterans with
(p < .01) with 57% of the participants with peripheral nerve injuries on measures of cog-
apoE4 having poor outcome defi ned as death, nitive functioning first assessed in the 1950s
vegetative state, or severe disability versus only (approximately 10 years after injury) and again
27% with poor outcome in the group without 30 years later. The veterans with head injuries
apoE4. Th is fi nding remained significant after showed significantly greater decline in cogni-
controlling for age, GCS, and CT scan fi nd- tive functioning between testing at time 1 and
ings. Other studies, conducted later have also 2 compared to those with peripheral nerve
found poor outcome with apoE4 although they injuries. However, subsequent studies in mili-
have all had small sample sizes (Ariza et al., tary (Newcombe, 1996) and civilian (Wood &
2006; Chiang et al., 2003; Crawford et al., Rutterford, 2006) populations have not found
2002; Friedman et al., 1999; Lichtman et al., evidence for late accelerated cognitive decline
2000; Sundstrom et al., 2004). However, the after TBI. Conflicting results across studies may
612 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

in part be due to the sample of cases available documented cases of TBI that occurred from
for long-term follow-up. Assessments that occur 1935 to 1984 in Olmsted County, Minnesota,
decades after the injury will likely have sample found no association between TBI and
attrition due to death and may also suffer from Alzheimer’s disease. However, time to onset of
other biases such as loss of subjects with better Alzheimer’s was less than expected, which sug-
or worse late outcome potentially influencing gested that TBI may reduce the time to develop
the findings. For example, Wood and Rutterford dementia. Other prospective cohort studies
(2006) were only able to perform late evaluations found no significantly increased risk of dementia
on 21% of their original sample. Another pos- for those with a history of TBI (Katzman et al.,
sible reason for discrepant findings is whether 1989; Mehta et al., 1999; Williams et al., 1991).
the follow-up period is long enough to capture Follow-up periods that were too short and/or an
differential age-related changes in functioning. average age of sample that was lower than the
Potentially, even a lengthy follow-up period of expected age of dementia onset, in some of the
20–25 years may not be long enough if subjects studies, may help account for discrepant find-
are injured in their teens or young adult years. ings (Starkstein & Jorge, 2005).
A separate but related area of research is The association between TBI and Alzheimer’s
the possible association between TBI and later disease has also been examined using case-con-
development of Alzheimer’s disease. Whether trol studies, which have generally supported
or not TBI is a risk factor for Alzheimer’s disease this association. In this design, subjects with
is not clear although there have been numer- Alzheimer’s disease are identified, a matched
ous studies to investigate this association. One control group without dementia is recruited,
of the common ways this association has been risk factors are examined retrospectively, and
explored is with prospective cohort studies. odds ratios are calculated to determine relative
These studies follow a group of healthy older risk. Many of these studies have found a signif-
people longitudinally and compare risk factors icant association between TBI and Alzheimer’s
between those who develop dementia and those disease (French et al., 1985; Graves et al., 1990;
who do not. The advantage of this type of study Heyman et al., 1984; Kondo et al., 1994; Mayeuz
is that it is less likely to suffer from recall bias et al., 1993; Rasmusson et al., 1995; Sullivan
since history of TBI is evaluated before demen- et al., 1987) with the odds of prior head injury
tia occurs. The results of these studies have ranging from 2 (Sullivan et al., 1987) to 13.75
generally shown that TBI is a significant risk (Rasmusson et al., 1995). The Canadian Study
factor for Alzheimer’s disease (Luukinen et al., of Health and Aging (1994) examined a large
2005; Plassman et al., 2000; Schofield et al., population-based sample of Alzheimer’s dis-
1997). For example, Schofield et al. (1997) fol- ease and controls aged 65 years and older in
lowed 271 people for 5 years, and 39 cases devel- ten Canadian provinces. Their results indicate
oped probable or possible Alzheimer’s disease. that head injury was a borderline significant
Alzheimer’s disease was significantly associ- risk factor for Alzheimer’s disease. Other case-
ated with a history of TBI, and those reporting control studies have failed to find a significant
loss of consciousness of more than 5 minutes association (Amaducci et al., 1986; Broe et al.,
were at significantly increased risk. Plassman 1990; Chandra et al., 1989; Chandra et al., 1987;
et al. (2000) studied 1776 World War II veter- Mayeux et al., 1995). Possible reasons for the dis-
ans who had sustained either a nonpenetrating crepant findings may be the result of methodo-
head injury or an unrelated condition resulting logical problems including varied definitions
in hospitalization during their military service. of head injury across studies, the likelihood of
These veterans were contacted approximately misdiagnosed Alzheimer’s disease since this
50 years later and evaluated for dementia. The condition can only be verified at autopsy, and
results found that both moderate and severe recall bias. The history of TBI is determined
head injury was associated with significantly in these studies by recall, with limited or no
increased risk of dementia. On the other hand, means of verification. It is possible that relatives
Nemetz et al. (1999), using the resources of the of dementia patients may have overreported a
Rochester Epidemiology Project to examine all history of TBI. Finally, another problem of some
Neurobehavioral Consequences of Traumatic Brain Injury 613

of the case-control studies may have involved Prediction of global outcome and employment
inadequate statistical power to detect a signif- are two areas that have received more attention
icant association between TBI and Alzheimer’s and where more gains have been made.
disease (Lye & Shores, 2000; Starkstein & Jorge, While no one would doubt the negative neu-
2005). Meta-analyses of case-control studies ropsychological and psychosocial consequences
(Fleminger et al., 2003; Mortimer et al., 1991) of moderate or more severe TBIs, the sequelae
had a high level of statistical power, and the of mild traumatic brain have been much more
results showed a significant association between controversial. Less agreement exists regarding
TBI and Alzheimer’s disease in men (but not the persistence of neuropsychological impair-
women). However, the problem of possible ments, their cause if they persist, and whether
recall bias remains. or not they are able to explain long-term dis-
It may also be possible that some of the abilities in those with mild injuries.
inconsistencies between studies are the results Areas of more recent research interest in TBI
of factors that are poorly understood and thus include the effects of genetic factors on outcome
uncontrolled. For example, the role of the (apolipoprotein E gene, which is the most stud-
apoE4 allele and the association of TBI and ied), and whether TBI accelerates normal aging
Alzheimer’s disease are not clear. Mayeux et al. or may be a harbinger for Alzheimer’s disease.
(1995) reported that a history of head injury These areas of research are in their infancy, and
and the apoE4 allele increased the risk of their full value for understanding outcome of
Alzheimer’s tenfold, while head injury without TBI is not yet clear.
the apoE4 allele did not significantly increase
the risk of Alzheimer’s. Plassman et al. (2000),
on the other hand, found only a nonsignificant References
trend for the apoE4 allele. This is another area
Amaducci, L. A., Fratiglioni, L., Rocca, W. A.,
of research that is going to continue and, hope- Fieschi, C., Livrea, P., Pedone, D., et al. (1986).
fully, produce more definitive results. Risk factors for clinically diagnosed Alzheimer’s
disease: A case-control study of an Italian popula-
tion. Neurology, 36(7), 922–931.
Conclusions
Ariza, M., Pueyo, R., Matarin, M. d. M., Junque, C.,
Traumatic brain injury represents a significant Mataro, M., Clemente, I., et al. (2006). Influence
public health problem. The importance of this of APOE polymorphism on cognitive and behav-
problem derives from the high incidence rates, ioural outcome in moderate and severe traumatic
young previously healthy people compromising brain injury. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery,
and Psychiatry, 77, 1191–1193.
a large proportion of those injured, survivors
Barber, J. (2003). Adjusting for bias caused by miss-
having normal life spans, and, depending on ing outcomes in studies of traumatic brain injury:
the severity of injury, the possibility of a variety An application and evaluation of three methods.
of persistent impairments and disabilities that Unpublished Master, University of Washington.
compromise the quality of life of those who sur- Belanger, H. G., Curtiss, G., Demery, J. A., Lebowitz,
vive. Over the last 25 years considerable infor- B. K., & Vanderploeg, R. D. (2005). Factors mod-
mation has accumulated regarding the nature erating neuropsychological outcomes following
of impairments, disabilities, and participation mild traumatic brain injury: A meta-analysis.
problems that occur in the survivors of such Journal of the International Neuropsychological
injuries. Much has also been learned about fac- Society, 11(3), 215–227.
tors that relate to outcome, such as severity of Belanger, H. G., & Vanderploeg, R. D. (2005). The neu-
ropsychological impact of sports-related concus-
the brain injury, the characteristics of the per-
sion: A meta-analysis. Journal of the International
son injured (in particular, age), time since injury Neuropsychological Society, 11(4), 345–357.
(reflecting recovery), and the type of function Binder, L. M., Rohling, M. L., & Larrabee, G. J. (1997).
in question. Although various factors that are A review of mild head trauma. Part I: Meta-analytic
important with respect to outcome have been review of neuropsychological studies. Journal of
identified, prediction on an individual level is Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 19,
limited due to much unexplained variability. 421–431.
614 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

Boake, C., Millis, S. R., High, W. M., Jr., Delmonico, with Traumatic Brain Injury. (1999). Journal of the
R. L., Kreutzer, J. S., Rosenthal, M., et al. (2001). American Medical Association, 282(10), 974–983.
Using early neuropsychologic testing to predict Corkin, S., Rosen, T. J., Sullivan, E. V., & Clegg, R.
long-term productivity outcome from traumatic A. (1989). Penetrating head injury in young adult-
brain injury. Archives of Physical Medicine and hood exacerbates cognitive decline in later years.
Rehabilitation, 82(6), 761–768. The Journal of Neuroscience, 9(11), 3876–3883.
Bombardier, C. H., Temkin, N. R., Machamer, J., & Corrigan, J. D. (1995). Substance abuse as a mediat-
Dikmen, S. S. (2003). The natural history of drink- ing factor in outcome from traumatic brain injury.
ing and alcohol-related problems after traumatic Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation,
brain injury. Archives of Physical Medicine and 76(4), 302–309.
Rehabilitation, 84(2), 185–191. Corrigan, J. D., Harrison-Felix, C., Bogner, J., Dijkers,
Broe, G. A., Henderson, A. S., Creasey, H., McCusker, M., Terrill, M. S., & Whiteneck, G. (2003). Systematic
E., Korten, A. E., Jorm, A. F., et al. (1990). A case- bias in traumatic brain injury outcome studies
control study of Alzheimer’s disease in Australia. because of loss to follow-up. Archives of Physical
Neurology, 40(11), 1698–1707. Medicine and Rehabilitation, 84(2), 153–160.
Carroll, L. J., Cassidy, J. D., Peloso, P. M., Borg, J., Crawford, F. C., Vanderploeg, R. D., Freeman, M. J.,
von Holst, H., Holm, L., et al. (2004). Prognosis Singh, S., Waisman, M., Michaels, L., et al. (2002).
for mild traumatic brain injury: Results of the APOE genotype influences acquisition and recall
WHO Collaborating Centre Task Force on Mild following traumatic brain injury. Neurology, 58(7),
Traumatic Brain Injury. Journal of Rehabilitation 1115–1118.
Medicine, February (43 Suppl), 84–105. Diaz-Arrastia, R., & Baxter, V. K. (2006). Genetic
Cassidy, J. D., Carroll, L. J., Peloso, P. M., Borg, J., von factors in outcome after traumatic brain injury:
Holst, H., Holm, L., et al. (2004). Incidence, risk What the human genome project can teach us
factors and prevention of mild traumatic brain about brain trauma. Journal of Head Trauma
injury: Results of the WHO Collaborating Centre Rehabilitation, 21(4), 361–374.
Task Force on Mild Traumatic Brain Injury. Diaz-Arrastia, R., Gong, Y., Fair, S., Scott, K. D.,
Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine, February (43 Garcia, M. C., Carlile, M. C., et al. (2003). Increased
Suppl), 28–60. risk of late posttraumatic seizures associated with
Cernich, A., Reeves, D., Sun, W., & Bleiberg, J. (2007). inheritance of APOE epsilon4 allele. Archives of
Automated Neuropsychological Assessment Neurology, 60(6), 818–822.
Metrics sports medicine battery. Archives of Dikmen, S., & Machamer, J. E. (1995). Neurobehavioral
Clinical Neuropsychology, 22(Suppl 1), S101–S114. outcomes and their determinants. Journal of Head
Chamelian, L., Reis, M., & Feinstein, A. (2004). Six- Trauma Rehabilitation, 10(1), 74–86.
month recovery from mild to moderate Traumatic Dikmen, S., Machamer, J., & Temkin, N. (2001).
Brain Injury: The role of APOE-epsilon4 allele. Mild head injury: Facts and artifacts. Journal of
Brain, 127(Pt 12), 2621–2628. Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23(6),
Chandra, V., Philipose, V., Bell, P. A., Lazaroff, A., & 729–738.
Schoenberg, B. S. (1987). Case-control study of late Dikmen, S., Machamer, J. E., Winn, H. R., & Temkin,
onset “probable Alzheimer’s disease”. Neurology, N. R. (1995b). Neuropsychological outcome at 1-year
37(8), 1295–1300. post head injury. Neuropsychology, 9(1), 80–90.
Chandra, V., Kokmen, E., Schoenberg, B. S., & Beard, Dikmen, S., McLean, A., & Temkin, N. (1986).
C. M. (1989). Head trauma with loss of conscious- Neuropsychological and psychosocial conse-
ness as a risk factor for Alzheimer’s disease. quences of minor head injury. Journal of Neurology,
Neurology, 39(12), 1576–1578. Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 49(11), 1227–1232.
Chesnut, R. M. (2000). Evolving models of neu- Dikmen, S., & Morgan, S. (1980). Neuropsychological
rotrauma critical care: An analysis and call to factors related to employability and occupational
action. Clinical Neurosurgery, 46, 185–195. status in persons with epilepsy. Journal of Nervous
Chiang, M. F., Chang, J. G., & Hu, C. J. (2003). and Mental Disease, 168(4), 236–240.
Association between apolipoprotein E genotype Dikmen, S., Reitan, R. M., & Temkin, N. R. (1983).
and outcome of traumatic brain injury. Acta Neuropsychological recovery in head injury.
Neurochir (Wien), 145(8), 649–653; discussion Archives of Neurology, 40(6), 333–338.
653–644. Dikmen, S., & Temkin, N. (1987). Determination of
Consensus conference. Rehabilitation of persons the effects of head injury and recovery in behav-
with traumatic brain injury. NIH Consensus ioral research. In H. S. Levin, J. Grafman, and H.
Development Panel on Rehabilitation of Persons M. Eisenberg (Eds.), Neurobehavioral recovery
Neurobehavioral Consequences of Traumatic Brain Injury 615

from head injury (pp. 73–88). New York: Oxford poor outcome in survivors of traumatic brain
University Press. injury. Neurology, 52(2), 244–248.
Dikmen, S. S., Heaton, R. K., Grant, I., & Temkin, N. Grafman, J., Salazar, A., Weingartner, H., Vance,
R. (1999). Test-retest reliability and practice effects S., & Amin, D. (1986). The relationship of brain-
of expanded Halstead- Reitan Neuropsychological tissue loss volume and lesion location to cognitive
Test Battery. Journal of the International deficit. The Journal of Neuroscience, 6(2), 301–307.
Neuropsychological Society, 5(4), 346–356. Graves, A. B., White, E., Koepsell, T. D., Reifler, B.
Dikmen, S. S., Machamer, J. E., Donovan, D. M., V., van Belle, G., Larson, E. B., et al. (1990). The
Winn, H. R., & Temkin, N. R. (1995a). Alcohol use association between head trauma and Alzheimer’s
before and after traumatic head injury. Annals of disease. American Journal of Epidemiology, 131(3),
Emergency Medicine, 26(2), 167–176. 491–501.
Dikmen, S. S., Machamer, J. E., Powell, J. M., & Gronwall, D., & Wrightson, P. (1974). Delayed recov-
Temkin, N. R. (2003). Outcome 3 to 5 years ery of intellectual function after minor head
after moderate to severe traumatic brain injury. injury. Lancet, 2(7881), 605–609.
Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Heyman, A., Wilkinson, W. E., Stafford, J. A., Helms,
84(10), 1449–1457. M. J., Sigmon, A. H., & Weinberg, T. (1984).
Dikmen, S. S., Ross, B. L., Machamer, J. E., & Alzheimer’s disease: A study of epidemiological
Temkin, N. R. (1995c). One year psychosocial out- aspects. Annals of Neurology, 15(4), 335–341.
come in head injury. Journal of the International Jennett, B., & Bond, M. (1975). Assessment of outcome
Neuropsychological Society, 1(1), 67–77. after severe brain damage. Lancet, i, 480–484.
Dikmen, S. S., Temkin, N. R., Machamer, J. E., Katzman, R., Aronson, M., Fuld, P., Kawas, C., Brown,
Holubkov, A. L., Fraser, R. T., & Winn, H. R. T., Morgenstern, H., et al. (1989). Development of
(1994). Employment following traumatic head dementing illnesses in an 80-year-old volunteer
injuries. Archives of Neurology, 51(2), 177–186. cohort. Annals of Neurology, 25(4), 317–324.
Doctor, J. N., Castro, J., Temkin, N. R., Fraser, R. T., Kondo, K., Niino, M., & Shido, K. (1994). A case-
Machamer, J. E., & Dikmen, S. S. (2005). Workers’ control study of Alzheimer’s disease in Japan—
risk of unemployment after traumatic brain injury: significance of life-styles. Dementia, 5(6), 314–326.
A normed comparison. Journal of the International Kreutzer, J. S., Marwitz, J. H., Walker, W., Sander, A.,
Neuropsychological Society, 11(6), 747–752. Sherer, M., Bogner, J., et al. (2003). Moderating fac-
Finkelstein, E., Corso, P., Miller, T., & associates. tors in return to work and job stability after trau-
(2006). The incidence and economic burden of matic brain injury. The Journal of Head Trauma
injuries in the United States. New York: Oxford Rehabilitation, 18(2), 128–138.
University Press. Langlois, J. A., Rutland-Brown, W., & Thomas, K. E.
Fleminger, S., Oliver, D. L., Lovestone, S., Rabe- (2004). Traumatic brain injury in the United States:
Hesketh, S., & Giora, A. (2003). Head injury as a Emergency department visits, hospitalizations, and
risk factor for Alzheimer’s disease: The evidence 10 deaths. Atlanta (GA): Centers for Disease Control
years on; a partial replication. Journal of Neurology, and Prevention, National Center for Injury
Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 74(7), 857–862. Prevention and Control.
Foulkes, M. A., Eisenberg, H. M., Jane, J. A., Levin, H. S., Mattis, S., Ruff, R. M., Eisenberg, H.
Marmarou, A., Marshall, L. F., & Traumatic Coma M., Marshall, L. F., Tabaddor, K., et al. (1987).
Data Bank Research Group. (1991). The Traumatic Neurobehavioral outcome following minor
Coma Data Bank: Design, methods, and base- head injury: A three-center study. Journal of
line characteristics. Journal of Neurosurgery, 75, Neurosurgery, 66, 234–243.
S8–S13. Levin, H. S. (1995). Neurobehavioral outcome of
Fraser, R., Dikmen, S., McLean, A., Jr., Miller, B., & closed head injury: Implications for clinical trials.
Temkin, N. (1988). Employability of head injury Journal of Neurotrauma, 12(4), 601–610.
survivors: First year post-injury. Rehabilitation Lichtman, S. W., Seliger, G., Tycko, B., & Marder, K.
Counseling Bulletin, 31, 276–288. (2000). Apolipoprotein E and functional recovery
French, L. R., Schuman, L. M., Mortimer, J. A., Hutton, from brain injury following postacute rehabilita-
J. T., Boatman, R. A., & Christians, B. (1985). A case- tion. Neurology, 55(10), 1536–1539.
control study of dementia of the Alzheimer type. Luukinen, H., Viramo, P., Herala, M., Kervinen,
American Journal of Epidemiology, 121(3), 414–421. K., Kesaniemi, Y. A., Savola, O., et al. (2005).
Friedman, G., Froom, P., Sazbon, L., Grinblatt, Fall-related brain injuries and the risk of demen-
I., Shochina, M., Tsenter, J., et al. (1999). tia in elderly people: A population-based study.
Apolipoprotein E-epsilon4 genotype predicts a European Journal of Neurology, 12(2), 86–92.
616 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

Lye, T. C., & Shores, E. A. (2000). Traumatic brain Mehta, K. M., Ott, A., Kalmijn, S., Slooter, A. J., van
injury as a risk factor for Alzheimer’s disease: A Duijn, C. M., Hofman, A., et al. (1999). Head trauma
review. Neuropsychology Review, 10(2), 115–129. and risk of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: The
Maas, A. I., Steyerberg, E. W., Butcher, I., Dammers, Rotterdam Study. Neurology, 53(9), 1959–1962.
R., Lu, J., Marmarou, A., et al. (2007). Prognostic Millar, K., Nicoll, J. A., Thornhill, S., Murray, G. D., &
value of computerized tomography scan charac- Teasdale, G. M. (2003). Long term neuropsycholog-
teristics in traumatic brain injury: Results from ical outcome after head injury: Relation to APOE
the IMPACT study. Journal of Neurotrauma, 24(2), genotype. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and
303–314. Psychiatry, 74(8), 1047–1052.
Macciocchi, S. N., Barth, J. T., Alves, W., Rimel, R. Mortimer, J. A., van Duijn, C. M., Chandra, V.,
W., & Jane, J. A. (1996). Neuropsychological func- Fratiglioni, L., Graves, A. B., Heyman, A., et al.
tioning and recovery after mild head injury in col- (1991). Head trauma as a risk factor for Alzheimer’s
legiate athletes. Neurosurgery, 39(3), 510–514. disease: A collaborative re-analysis of case-control
Machamer, J., Temkin, N., Fraser, R., Doctor, J., & studies. EURODEM Risk Factors Research Group.
Dikmen, S. (2005). Stabililty of employment after International Journal of Epidemiology, 20(Suppl
traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International 2), S28–S35.
Neuropsychological Society, 11(7), 807–816. Murray, G. D., Butcher, I., McHugh, G. S., Lu, J.,
Marmarou, A., Lu, J., Butcher, I., McHugh, G. S., Mushkudiani, N. A., Maas, A. I., et al. (2007).
Mushkudiani, N. A., Murray, G. D., et al. (2007a). Multivariable prognostic analysis in traumatic
IMPACT database of traumatic brain injury: brain injury: Results from the IMPACT study.
Design and description. Journal of Neurotrauma, Journal of Neurotrauma, 24(2), 329–337.
24(2), 239–250. Mushkudiani, N. A., Engel, D. C., Steyerberg, E. W.,
Marmarou, A., Lu, J., Butcher, I., McHugh, G. S., Butcher, I., Lu, J., Marmarou, A., et al. (2007).
Murray, G. D., Steyerberg, E. W., et al. (2007b). Prognostic value of demographic characteristics in
Prognostic value of the Glasgow Coma Scale and traumatic brain injury: Results from the IMPACT
pupil reactivity in traumatic brain injury assessed study. Journal of Neurotrauma, 24(2), 259–269.
pre-hospital and on enrollment: An IMPACT Nakase-Richardson, R., Yablon, S. A., & Sherer, M.
analysis. Journal of Neurotrauma, 24(2), 270–280. (2007). Prospective comparison of acute con-
Marshall, L. F., Gautille, T., Klauber, M. R., Eisenberg, fusion severity with duration of post-traumatic
H. M., Jane, J. A., Luerssen, T. G., et al. (1991). The amnesia in predicting employment outcome after
outcome of severe closed head injury. Journal of traumatic brain injury. Journal of Neurology,
Neurosurgery, 75, S28–S36. Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 78(8), 872–876.
Max, W., MacKenzie, E. J., & Rice, D. P. (1991). Head Nathoo, N., Chetry, R., van Dellen, J. R., Connolly,
injuries: Costs and consequences. Journal of Head C., & Naidoo, R. (2003). Apolipoprotein E poly-
Trauma Rehabilitation, 6(2), 76–91. morphism and outcome after closed traumatic
Mayeux, R., Ottman, R., Tang, M. X., Noboa-Bauza, brain injury: Influence of ethnic and regional dif-
L., Marder, K., Gurland, B., et al. (1993). Genetic ferences. Journal of Neurosurgery, 98(2), 302–306.
susceptibility and head injury as risk factors for Nemetz, P. N., Leibson, C., Naessens, J. M., Beard, M.,
Alzheimer’s disease among community-dwelling Kokmen, E., Annegers, J. F., et al. (1999). Traumatic
elderly persons and their first-degree relatives. brain injury and time to onset of Alzheimer’s dis-
Annals of Neurology, 33(5), 494–501. ease: A population-based study. American Journal
Mayeux, R., Ottman, R., Maestre, G., Ngai, C., Tang, of Epidemiology, 149(1), 32–40.
M. X., Ginsberg, H., et al. (1995). Synergistic Newcombe, F. (1996). Very late outcome after focal
effects of traumatic head injury and apolipopro- wartime brain wounds. Journal of Clinical and
tein-epsilon 4 in patients with Alzheimer’s dis- Experimental Neuropsychology, 18(1), 1–23.
ease. Neurology, 45(3 Pt 1), 555–557. Plassman, B. L., Havlik, R. J., Steffens, D. C., Helms,
McHugh, G. S., Engel, D. C., Butcher, I., Steyerberg, M. J., Newman, T. N., Drosdick, D., et al. (2000).
E. W., Lu, J., Mushkudiani, N., et al. (2007). Documented head injury in early adulthood and
Prognostic value of secondary insults in traumatic risk of Alzheimer’s disease and other dementias.
brain injury: Results from the IMPACT study. Neurology, 55(8), 1158–1166.
Journal of Neurotrauma, 24(2), 287–293. Ponsford, J., Willmott, C., Rothwell, A., Cameron,
McLean, A., Jr., Dikmen, S., & Temkin, (1993). P., Kelly, A.M., Nelms, R., et al. (2000). Factors
Psychosocial recovery after head injury. Archives influencing outcome following mild traumatic
of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 74, brain injury in adults. Journal of the International
1041–1046. Neuropsychological Society, 6(5), 568–579.
Neurobehavioral Consequences of Traumatic Brain Injury 617

Rasmusson, D. X., Brandt, J., Martin, D. B., & of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 52,
Folstein, M. F. (1995). Head injury as a risk factor 1128–1134.
in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Injury, 9(3), 213–219. Teasdale, G., & Jennett, B. (1974). Assessment of
Rimel, R. W., Giordani, B., Barth, J. T., Boll, T. J., & coma and impaired consciousness: A practical
Jane, J. A. (1981). Disability caused by minor head scale. Lancet, ii, 81–84.
injury. Neurosurgery, 9(3), 221–228. Teasdale, G. M., Murray, G. D., & Nicoll, J. A. (2005).
Ross, B., Temkin, N., & Dikmen, S. (1994). The The association between APOE epsilon4, age and
effects of aging on neuropsychological outcome outcome after head injury: A prospective cohort
following traumatic head injury. Paper presented study. Brain, 128(Pt 11), 2556–2561.
at the International Neuropsychological Society, Teasdale, G. M., Nicoll, J. A., Murray, G., & Fiddes,
Cincinnati, Ohio. M. (1997). Association of apolipoprotein E poly-
Russell, W. R., & Smith, A. (1961). Post-traumatic morphism with outcome after head injury. Lancet,
amnesia in closed head injury. Archives of 350(9084), 1069–1071.
Neurology, 5, 4–17. The Canadian Study of Health and Aging: Risk fac-
Schofield, P. W., Tang, M., Marder, K., Bell, K., tors for Alzheimer’s disease in Canada. (1994).
Dooneief, G., Chun, M., et al. (1997). Alzheimer’s Neurology, 44(11), 2073–2080.
disease after remote head injury: An incidence Thurman, D. J., Alverson, C., Dunn, K. A., Guerrero,
study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and J., & Sniezek, J. E. (1999). Traumatic brain injury
Psychiatry, 62(2), 119–124. in the United States: A public health perspective.
Schwab, K., Grafman, J., Salazar, A. M., & Kraft, J. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 14(6),
(1993). Residual impairments and work status 15 602–615.
years after penetrating head injury: Report from Van Beek, J. G., Mushkudiani, N. A., Steyerberg, E.
the Vietnam Head Injury Study. Neurology, 43(1), W., Butcher, I., McHugh, G. S., Lu, J., et al. (2007).
95–103. Prognostic value of admission laboratory param-
Sherer, M., Sander, A. M., Nick, T. G., High, W. M., eters in traumatic brain injury: Results from the
Jr., Malec, J. F., & Rosenthal, M. (2002). Early IMPACT study. Journal of Neurotrauma, 24(2),
cognitive status and productivity outcome after 315–328.
traumatic brain injury: Findings from the TBI Walker, A. E., & Blumer, D. (1989). The fate of World
model systems. Archives of Physical and Medical War II veterans with posttraumatic seizures.
Rehabilitation, 83(2), 183–192. Archives of Neurology, 46(1), 23–26.
Sosin, D. M., Sniezek, J. E., & Waxweiler, R. J. (1995). Weinstein, S., & Teuber, H. L. (1957). The role of pre-
Trends in death associated with traumatic brain injury education and intelligence level in intellec-
injury, 1979 through 1992. Success and failure. tual loss after brain injury. Journal of Comparative
Journal of the American Medical Association, and Physiological Psychology, 50, 535–539.
273(22), 1778–1780. Williams, D. B., Annegers, J. F., Kokmen, E., O’Brien,
Starkstein, S. E., & Jorge, R. (2005). Dementia P. C., & Kurland, L. T. (1991). Brain injury and
after traumatic brain injury. International neurologic sequelae: A cohort study of dementia,
Psychogeriatrics, 17(Suppl 1), S93–S107. Parkinsonism, and amyotrophic lateral sclerosis.
Sullivan, P., Petitti, D., & Barbaccia, J. (1987). Head Neurology, 41(10), 1554–1557.
trauma and age of onset of dementia of the Williams, D. H., Levin, H. S., & Eisenberg, H. M.
Alzheimer type. Journal of the American Medical (1990). Mild head injury classification. Journal of
Association, 257(17), 2289–2290. Neurosurgery, 27(3), 422–428.
Sundstrom, A., Marklund, P., Nilsson, L. G., Cruts, Wilson, J. T., Pettigrew, L. E., & Teasdale, G. M. (1998).
M., Adolfsson, R., Van Broeckhoven, C., et al. Structured interviews for the Glasgow Outcome
(2004). APOE influences on neuropsychological Scale and the extended Glasgow Outcome Scale:
function after mild head injury: Within-person Guidelines for their use. Journal of Neurotrauma,
comparisons. Neurology, 62(11), 1963–1966. 15(8), 573–585.
Tate, R. L., Lulham, J. M., Broe, G. A., Strettles, B., Wood, R. L., & Rutterford, N. A. (2006). Long-term
& Pfaff, A. (1989). Psychosocial outcome for sur- effect of head trauma on intellectual abilities:
vivors of severe blunt head injury: The results A 16-year outcome study. Journal of Neurology,
from a consecutive series of 100 patients. Journal Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 77(10), 1180–1184.
25

Neuropsychiatric, Psychiatric, and


Behavioral Disorders Associated with
Traumatic Brain Injury
George P. Prigatano and Franziska Maier

present is very complex” (p. 245). He suggested


Introduction and Historical
that “the personality structure is disturbed,
Background
particularly by lesions of the frontal lobes, the
Numerous clinical and research reports have parietal lobes, and the insula Reili, but it is also
emphasized that changes in patients’ emotional disturbed by diff use damage to the cortex . . .”
and motivational characteristics, as well as (pp. 246–247). However, he noted that a basic
their behavioral reactions after traumatic brain cognitive disturbance—“impairment of the
injury (TBI) are especially distressful to family abstract attitude”—was responsible for some
members (Hawley et al., 2003; Winstanley et al., of the psychiatric and behavioral disturbances
2006) and pose the most serious barriers to seen in these patients. Impulsive responding,
successful (neuropsychological) rehabilitation emotional lability, and the lack of “joy” were all
(Prigatano, 1999). These disturbances are often related to the inability of the patient to grasp the
associated with changes in cognitive function- complexity of the situation and to tolerate nor-
ing (Goldstein, 1952; Max et al., 2000), but often mal tensions associated with problem solving.
cannot be purely explained on the basis of such His eloquent description of the “catastrophic
impairments. Understanding the mechanisms reaction” has been especially helpful to rehabili-
responsible for these disturbances following tationists when teaching patients how to under-
TBI has attracted much attention over the years. stand and manage their emotional reactions
Yet our knowledge remains incomplete. (Klonoff & Lage, 1991; Prigatano et al., 1986).
At the turn of the last century, Adolf Meyer Later assessment of World War II soldiers who
(1904), the “father of American psychiatry,” had suffered TBIs also emphasized the complex
described the “anatomical facts” underlying relationship between lesion location, lesion size,
the “traumatic insanities.” Paul Schilder (1934), changes in cognition, and the emergence of
the psychoanalyst, described the “deep dis- various neuropsychiatric disorders (Lishman,
turbances in the emotional attitude” observed 1968). This later led Lishman (1978) to identify
in TBI patients “which we cannot measure” eleven factors (i.e., mental constitution, premor-
(p. 159). Kurt Goldstein (1952), a neurologist bid personality, emotional impact of the injury,
and psychiatrist who was actively involved in circumstances, setting and repercussions of
the rehabilitation of soldiers who had suffered injury, iatrogenic factors, environmental fac-
war-induced brain injuries, emphasized the tors, compensation and litigation, response to
important distinction between a direct versus intellectual impairments, development of epi-
indirect symptom. Influenced by the work of lepsy, amount of brain damage incurred, and
John Hughlings Jackson, Goldstein (1952) noted location of brain damage incurred) that poten-
that “the symptomatology which these patients tially could contribute to the psychiatric and

618
Disorders Associated with Traumatic Brain Injury 619

Causes
Multiple interacting factors such as
Genes Gene expression Viruses Toxins Nutrition Birth injury Personal experience

Brain structure and function


(e.g., brain development and degeneration, plastic changes in response to experience, brain chemistry,
changes in response to medications, changes in response to psychotherapy)

Mind functions
(e.g., memory, emotion, language, attention, arousal, consciousness)

The unique person in a specific social world


(i.e., individual behavior and response in a specific personal and social environment)

A specific mental illness


(e.g., schizophrenia, mood disorders, dementias, anxiety disorders)

Figure 25–1. A synthetic model of the development of mental illness (reprinted with permission from
Andreasen, 2001).

behavioral disturbances observed following description of individual patients has given way
head (brain) trauma. to the use of standardized questionnaires and
Since that time, several reviews have appeared structured interviews to understand the psychi-
on the neuropsychiatric aspects of traumatic atric and behavioral problems of children (Max
brain injury (e.g., Jorge, 2005; Silver et al., 2005). et al., 2001) and adults (Dikmen et al., 2004)
These papers emphasized the complexity of the after TBI. In this chapter, we will review find-
problem (something Goldstein noted over 65 ings from this approach.
years ago) and have suggested that we apply the Due to the extensive literature that has
technology of the time to help unravel it. In her appeared over the last 10–15 years, selected
insightful book, Andreasen (2001) reminds us papers will be reviewed for the following distur-
that specific psychiatric disorders or mental ill- bances: depression, anxiety, paranoid ideation,
ness reflect a complex, multifactorial outcome. a lack of empathy, irritability, impulsiveness,
Brain injury (type, size, distribution, loca- and socially inappropriate comments. Due to
tion, etc.) potentially interacts with multiple space limitations, we will not address other
variables to produce the symptom picture (see important topics such as posttraumatic stress
Figure 25–1). disorders or pathological laughter and crying
Our present technological advances include after TBI (Tateno et al., 2004). While the focus
static and dynamic neuroimaging of the brain will be on the TBI literature, studies with other
and our capacity to apply molecular biology to patient groups that have specific bearing on
map the genome. Thus, the analysis of emotional, topics to be discussed will be briefly considered.
motivational, and behavioral disturbances after Before reviewing these papers, a few method-
TBI has become progressively more “biological” ological problems facing this research will be
and “neurological” in nature. Careful clinical considered.
620 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

method of recording unresponsiveness to the


Methodological Problems
environment following brain trauma, this sys-
Recent studies have used neuroimaging to relate tem might be oversimplified at times. It has pro-
changes of brain structure (as recorded by com- gressively become clear that GCS scores need to
puted tomography [CT] or magnetic resonance be supplemented with neuroimaging findings
imaging [MRI]) to psychiatric disturbances to obtain a clearer picture of the severity of TBI
after TBI. To date they repeatedly have failed to (Prigatano et al., 2008).
show a strong relationship between brain neu- A true neuropsychiatry of TBI must wrestle
roimaging findings and DSM-IV diagnoses that with these realities and conduct studies that
are applied to TBI patients. Even when neuroim- allow one to specifically relate neuropatholog-
aging techniques try to relate dynamic brain ical changes with neuropsychological changes
activity, as reflected by functional MRI (fMRI), that can change with time and occur with vary-
positron emission tomography (PET), or sin- ing degrees of severity of injury. Premorbid
gle photon emission computed tomography characteristics of the patients, as well as their
(SPECT) studies to psychiatric disturbances, psychosocial and cultural backgrounds, need to
the findings are often difficult to interpret. It is be considered (Yeates et al., 1997).
seldom that a specific brain lesion and/or a spe- Finally, clinicians and researchers should
cific metabolic finding correlates with specific share a common terminology when at all possi-
neuropsychiatric disorders. ble. We would suggest the following definitions
We believe this is due to several fundamental be utilized. The term neuropsychiatric refers to
problems underlying research in this area. First, changes in the patient’s psychiatric status that
researchers seldom, if ever, explicitly discuss the is a direct effect (i.e., caused) of brain dysfunc-
important distinction of a direct versus indirect tion. The term psychiatric refers to disturbances
effect of brain dysfunction on the psychiat- in the mental or emotional (and motivational)
ric disorders seen in TBI patients. Second, the features of the person that either preexisted the
American Psychiatric Association Diagnostic brain disorder (i.e., are characterological) or are
and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders in reaction to the limitations a person experi-
(DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, ences in their functional capacities following
1994) may be helpful when classifying tra- brain injury. The term behavioral is used to
ditional psychiatric disturbances, but is lim- identify changes in responding that are not a
ited when describing the varied emotional and part of a constellation of symptoms that would
motivational disturbances that can be witnessed warrant a specific psychiatric diagnosis.
after TBI. Thus, studies that classify TBI patients
using DSM-IV terminology may not capture
the most relevant psychiatric features that have
Depression
the potential to be related to specific neuropsy- Depressive mood is common among patients
chologic, neuroanatomical, neurophysiologic, with a history of moderate-to-severe TBI, but
or neurotransmitter disturbances observed in estimates of the percentage of TBI patients who
TBI patients. A third major difficulty that faces present with acute versus chronic major depres-
researchers in this field was recently empha- sive disorder (MDD) as defined by the DSM-IV
sized by Jorge (2005). He notes: “The neuropsy- vary. Jorge and Starkstein (2005) suggested,
chiatry of TBI encompasses a broad spectrum however, that “major depression is present in
of cognitive, emotional, and behavioral distur- about 40% of patients hospitalized for a TBI” (p.
bances occurring during the acute, post-acute, 482). Fedoroff et al. (1992) demonstrated a sig-
and chronic phases of the illness” (p. 291). “TBI nificant relationship between location of brain
is characterized by widespread neuropatholog- lesions and the development of major depres-
ical changes in diverse cortical areas, subcorti- sion in acute TBI patients using CT scans of the
cal nuclei, and white matter tracts . . .” (p. 293). brain within the first 24 hours, and again 1–2
Fourth, although the classification of TBI sever- weeks postinjury. “The presence of left dorso-
ity by the Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS) (Teasdale lateral frontal lesions and/or left basal ganglia
& Jennett, 1974) is an objective, easily applied lesions and, to a lesser extent, parietal, occipital,
Disorders Associated with Traumatic Brain Injury 621

and right hemisphere lesions was associated preinjury personality disturbances and neuro-
with an increased probability of developing pathological/neuropsychological disturbances
major depression” (p. 918). observed in TBI patients.
The same sample of patients was then fol- Dikmen et al. (2004) using a behavioral
lowed at 3, 6, and 12 months postinjury (Jorge approach documented that “the rates of mod-
et al., 1993). Using the previous CT scan find- erate to severe depression ranged from 31% at
ings, these researchers failed to reveal a signif- 1 month to 17% at 3 to 5 years” (p. 1457) post-
icant relationship between lesion location and trauma. Their study also noted that premorbid
the presence of major depression during these factors such as a history of mood disorders and
time frames. The strength of the relationship unstable work history was related to postinjury
between depression and regional brain lesions depression. They note, as others have (see Silver
seemed to alter with time. et al., 2005), that “these data do not support a
In a further comparison, Jorge et al. (1993) simple dose-response relation between TBI
specifically compared a TBI patient group that severity and severity of depressive symptoms”
was depressed in the hospital, but whose symp- (p. 1461). They go on to remark “that educa-
toms improved to a nondepressive state by the tional, vocational, and psychologic factors do
3-month follow-up (N = 7), with TBI patients predict depressive symptom severity, especially
who remained depressed. The acute-onset tran- at 1 year after injury” (p. 1461). This conclusion
sient depressed patients demonstrated not only is in keeping with the clinical observations that
significantly higher injury severity but also psychotherapy and medications are both helpful
significantly higher rates of left anterior lesion in treating depression in postacute TBI patients
locations than the other patient group. (Prigatano, 2005; Prigatano & Summers, 1997).
In perhaps the first comprehensive prospec- Levin et al. (2005) conducted the first pro-
tive study on major depression following TBI, spective study on the development of a major
Jorge et al. (2004) reports several interesting depressive episode (MDE) following mild TBI
findings. Major depression was observed in in adults (i.e., admitting GCS score 13–15 on
approximately 33% of 91 patients during the arrival to the hospital with no later deterioration
first year following brain trauma. A history below a GCS score of 13). They report that the
of premorbid mood disturbance and anxi- presence of an abnormal brain CT finding cou-
ety disorders was common in these patients. pled with these GCS scores increased the odds
Depressed TBI patients tended to perform of an MDE by over 7 to 1. They demonstrate that
worse on neuropsychological tests, especially a combination of GCS scores and neuroimag-
measures sampling “executive functions.” Their ing findings may be helpful in predicting later
volumetric MRI findings were perhaps the most MDE, even following mild TBI.
interesting of all findings reported. TBI patients Not all neuroimaging studies, however, have
with major depression “showed significant been able to relate abnormal findings to depres-
decreased left lateral frontal cortex volumes” (p. sion after TBI. The positive findings observed
47). “This difference was owing to significantly often relate to the acute period following trauma.
smaller left inferior frontal gyrus volumes . . . as Studies that examined patients several years
well as small left superior frontal gyrus vol- post-TBI often do not report these correlations.
umes . . . and left middle frontal gyrus volumes” Koponen et al. (2006) attempted to relate MRI
(p. 47). They later cautioned, however, that “it findings to Axis I and Axis II psychiatric disor-
is unclear if the reduced prefrontal volumes ders following TBI 30 years posttrauma. Major
observed in patients with major depressive dis- depression was surprisingly related to an absence
order are the result of the pathophysiological of cerebral contusions. Citing Fann et al. (2004)
mechanisms initiated by TBI or they constitute epidemiological study, these authors suggest that
a preexistent trait associated with an increased the risk of depression several years posttrauma
risk to develop mood disorders. Brain atrophic may be associated with milder TBIs.
changes can be observed among patients with As interesting as these (generally) well-de-
chronic mood disorders . . .” (p. 48). These stud- signed studies are, they have not substantially
ies clearly document the interaction effects of brought us any closer to understanding MDD
622 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

in postacute TBI patients. A history of mood or Anxiety after Traumatic Brain


psychiatric disorders plus an unstable work his- Injury
tory may be as predictive of post-TBI depression
The prevalence of a generalized anxiety disor-
as any neuroimaging correlate found to date. The
der (GAD) in the population has been estimated
patients’ perception of how the injury affected
to be approximately 5.1% (see Hiott & Labbate,
them and their assessment of the extent and
2002). The prevalence of GAD after TBI is
nature of their neuropsychological impairment
unknown. Some studies report a low prevalence
are seldom, if ever, described in these technically
(2.5%; Deb et al., 1999) while others report a
well-designed studies. The patients’ experiences
higher prevalence (10.2%; see Hiott & Labbate,
are often excluded from the data analysis, leaving
2002).
the clinician unclear as to what role the premor-
Like depression, anxiety disorders encompass
bid factors and the patients’ present existential
a wide range of behavioral difficulties. There is
situation contribute to their major depression.
often a sense of “panic” and excessive worrying
Before leaving the topic of neuroimaging cor-
associated with restlessness, difficulties concen-
relates of mood disorders after TBI, a recent
trating, easy fatigability, and irritability. The
study by Jorge et al. (2007) examined the asso-
later problems are especially common after mild
ciation between mood disorders following TBI
TBI and often compose part of a post-concus-
and lesions of the hippocampus. Hippocampal
sion syndrome (Ruff & Richards, 2003). Efforts
atrophy is common after severe TBI (Bigler,
to relate neuropathological changes in the brain
2005). In a sample of 37 TBI patients, 19 were
to anxiety disorders have repeatedly run into
diagnosed with major depression or mixed fea-
difficulties (Hiott & Labbate, 2002), with a lack
tures during the first 3 months after injury. MRI
of consensus regarding how different lesions of
scans at the 3-month follow-up showed that TBI
the brain may affect this complicated psychiat-
patients who had developed a mood disorder had
ric problem.
significantly lower right and left hippocampal
Clinically, anxiety often seems to be associ-
volumes than TBI patients without mood disor-
ated with the patients’ awareness that there has
ders. They also note that reduced hippocampal
been some disturbance in their functioning and
volume was related to poor vocational outcome,
their inability to return to preinjury role activi-
after controlling for severity of TBI and the pres-
ties. Concerns about the future and inability to
ence of mood disorders. It does not appear, how-
support oneself and one’s family after TBI may
ever, that the authors specifically assessed the
result in increased self-reports of anxiety.
impact of vocational outcome on depression. If
A pathological absence of anxiety, however,
major depression after TBI is a direct outcome
may reflect a disturbance of frontal lobe func-
of hippocampal volume, and not an indirect
tion in which individuals do not experience
result of poor vocational outcome, the severity
tension over a given situation when they should
of depressive symptoms should correlate with
(see Luria, 1948/1963, for a discussion of this
hippocampal volume when vocational outcome
important problem). For example, Vasa et al.
is held constant in statistical analyses. One could
(2004) noted that in children with TBI “greater
also ask the question, Are the patients with low
volume and number of OFC (or orbitofrontal
hippocampal volume depressed even if they are
cortex) lesions correlated with decreased risk
able to return to a productive lifestyle? Research
for anxiety” (p. 208). This is in keeping with
on the effects of TBI on depression has still not
Luria’s (1948/1963) earlier observations. To
adequately separated the potential direct versus
date, no specific brain lesion has been shown
indirect effects of the TBI on this psychiatric dis-
to substantially increase anxiety after varying
turbance. We would predict that those patients
levels of severity of TBI. Following Goldstein’s
who are able to maintain a productive lifestyle
(1952) observation, anxiety often appears to be
several months after TBI will have lower rates of
a part of the catastrophic reaction. It may reflect
depression despite their neuroimaging findings
a patient’s inability to cope with the environ-
(Prigatano et al., 1986). A key moderating vari-
mental demands secondary to reduced cogni-
able will be, however, whether depression was a
tive capacity.
major problem prior to the TBI.
Disorders Associated with Traumatic Brain Injury 623

Factor 1
Depressed mood Apathy Factor 2
Anhedonia Lack of concentration
Self-concept Cognitive inefficiency

Anxiety

Factor 3
Disinhibition
Impulsivity
Hyperactivity

Figure 25–2. Factor structure of psychiatric symptoms elicited by the Present State Examination for persons
with traumatic brain injury (reprinted with permission from Jorge & Starkstein, 2005).

Jorge and Starkstein (2005) recently reported Sachdev, 2003). Epidemiological studies suggest
data and a theoretical model that links anxi- schizophrenic-like disorders are 2–3 times more
ety after TBI with depression, as well as other common when the patient has had a history of
commonly observed cognitive and behavioral TBI (Arciniegas et al., 2003; Silver et al., 2001).
difficulties. Using patients’ subjective reports A consistent and as yet unanswered question is,
of their present status, they note that reports of Does TBI put a person at risk for psychosis or
anxiety and depression coexist in approximately can it cause psychosis several years posttrauma?
60% of the TBI subjects that they studied. They At the heart of this problem has been the
analyzed the factor structure of the psychiatric failure to find a direct neuropathological link
symptoms reported by persons with TBI (see between a person’s TBI and his or her psycho-
Figure 25–2 taken from their work). The first sis. McAllister and Ferrell (2002) shed light,
factor accounted for 27% of the variance and however, on the key observation relative to this
comprised mainly symptoms associated with controversy:
depression. The second factor accounted for
12% of the variance. It reflected primarily sub- Although no single brain region has been identified
as the site or cause of schizophrenia, several brain
jective complaints of cognitive difficulty. The
regions appear to play important roles in the gene-
third factor accounted for 11% of the variability sis and phenomenology of this disorder, and these
and appeared to reflect the problem of psycho- regions overlap with those vulnerable to injury in the
motor agitation and disinhibition. The figure typical TBI. (p. 361)
highlights the important clinical observation
that many of these symptoms overlap in a given A number of clinical reports have implicated
patient, as observed in group data as well. damage to the left hemisphere of the brain,
particularly involving the temporal lobes, with
psychotic-like behavior, including paranoid
Psychosis and Paranoid
ideation (see McAllister & Ferrell, 2002). Acute
Ideation after TBI
frontal lobe damage, however, has been consis-
While uncommon, psychotic thought processes tently linked to confusional disorders (DeLuca
and behavior have been observed by a num- & Cicerone, 1991), and may play a role in late
ber of investigators several months and years onset of delusions.
post-severe TBI (Fujii et al., 2004; Hillbom, Prigatano (1988) suggested that unresolved
1960; Lishman, 1968; Thomsen, 1984; Zhang & anosognosia or impaired self-awareness (ISA)
624 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

may contribute to paranoid ideation several raw score of 84 out of 150 points. These scores
years post-TBI. The senior author has followed are highly reliable from both the patient’s and
TBI patients who have developed frank para- mother’s perspective. Despite the patient’s insis-
noid ideation several years posttrauma. One tence that he could do everything well 13 years
patient was a 26-year-old male who suffered a postinjury, his Wechsler Verbal IQ score was
severe TBI with an admitting GCS score of 6. 84 with a Performance IQ of 64. He was clearly
He was first examined at age 31, approximately cognitively impaired but could not explain his
5 years posttrauma. He demonstrated during difficulties in everyday life. His conclusion was
the clinical interview, as well as on neuropsy- that others were mistreating him and this was
chological tests, significant neurocognitive the basis of his personal failures and misery.
impairment. He insisted, however, that he was Koponen et al. (2002) noted that, in the
perfectly normal in all areas except for motor 30-year follow-up study, paranoid ideation was
difficulties. He insisted he was able to work. observed in 8.3% of their study sample. These
He reported no major difficulties in his cogni- authors report that delusions are often asso-
tive functioning or changes in his personality. ciated with bilateral brain lesions frequently
Using the Patient Competency Rating Scale involving the thalamus. Mizrahi et al. (2006)
(PCRS) (Prigatano et al., 1986), the patient also have reported a relationship between ano-
reported performing almost all tasks with lit- sognosia and delusions in Alzheimer patients.
tle or no difficulty. His total score on the PCRS
was 143 out of 150 possible points. (A score of
Loss of Empathy After Traumatic
150 points means the patient is stating that of
Brain Injury
30 items he can do each activity with absolutely
no difficulty.) In contrast, his mother rated him The DSM-IV (1994) does not classify loss of
on the PCRS-Relative’s form with a score of 85 empathy, commonly observed in clinical prac-
out of 150 points, reflecting significant restric- tice, as a specific neuropsychiatric disorder. A
tions in his day-to-day life. recent study by Woods and Williams (2008)
The patient was then seen 13 years later found that individuals with TBI reported a sig-
because of the onset of paranoid ideation. He nificantly higher proportion of low emotional
continued to insist that he was perfectly normal empathy (60.7%; 54 of 89 participants) than nor-
in his ability to remember and to think logically. mal controls (31%; 26 of 84 participants). This
He viewed himself as having no major limita- problem can be especially devastating because
tions that would preclude him from returning it substantially interferes with one’s ability to
to work. He explained his failure to return to sustain interpersonal relationships. Early severe
work on the basis that others did not give him TBI may have especially negative consequences
appropriate opportunities. He explained his for the individual (Prigatano, 1999). Eslinger,
social isolation and lack of friends on the basis Parkinson and Shamay (2002) note that empa-
that he lived with his mother in a rural town thy “. . . has been described as a ‘binding force’
outside of the Greater Phoenix area. He did that permits individuals to share experiences
not present with any delusions of grandeur or and exchange understanding, a fundamental
believe that he had any special role to play in building block for most interpersonal relation-
life. He was not plagued by hearing voices that ships and social groups” (p. 95). When there is a
urged him to carry out certain actions. He did loss of empathy, the individual is often described
not have an elaborate set of beliefs that under- as “self-centered” and insensitive to the needs
lined his paranoid ideation as is sometimes seen of others. As reviewed by Eslinger et al. (2002),
in true schizophrenia. a series of studies suggest that damage to the
On the PCRS-Patient’s form, his total score prefrontal cortex and the right posterior corti-
was 138 out of 150 points. The only change in cal regions may significantly alter the person’s
his ratings was that he now felt he could not capacity for empathy. Moreover, “deep white
drive a car because of his motor difficulties and matter lesions of the frontal lobe, disconnect-
associated visual problems. His mother’s rat- ing frontal limbic pathways, have been asso-
ings on the PCRS-Relative’s form produced a ciated with impaired empathetic capabilities
Disorders Associated with Traumatic Brain Injury 625

and negative social adaptation . . .” (p. 95). This accounted for approximately 11% of the vari-
may help explain why this problem is common ance as noted above. This factor loaded highly
in TBI patients since the pathological changes on irritability, as well as other problems associ-
associated with a loss of empathy are common ated with disinhibition, agitation, etc. In clin-
after severe TBI (Silver et al., 2005). ical practice, many patients with TBI describe
Lawrence et al. (2006) studied patterns of brain themselves as being easily irritable. This irrita-
activation, using fMRI, in twelve healthy volun- bility, at times, is associated with disinhibited
teers who described their and others’ feelings behavior.
during a social perception task. Several areas of Have there been any new insights in under-
the brain were activated during these times. The standing these behaviors? Koponen et al. (2006)
authors, however, suggest that activation of the note in their 30-year follow-up study that 17
anterior cingulate was especially pronounced out of 58 patients (29.3%) studied had definite
during those episodes in which the individu- personality disorders or an organic person-
als seemed to appear to have an empathetic ality syndrome (using DSM-IV classification
response. Whether this activation is secondary system). Many of these patients were described
to the attentional demands of an empathetic as demonstrating disinhibited behavior. These
response or is associated with empathy per se is patients were more likely to have contusions in
unclear. However, broad neurocircuits involving the frontal regions of the brain. This observation
frontal, temporal, parietal, and cingulate gyrus is compatible with earlier reports (Prigatano,
become activated in these tasks. Damage to any 1992). Frontal lobe dysfunction often produces
of these areas may result in a lack of empathy or a disinhibited state in some patients. This dis-
a reduction of empathy for TBI patients. inhibited state seems to involve an inability to
control one’s emotional reactions, particularly
angry feelings, and therefore the individual may
Irritability and Disinhibited Behavior
be described as “more irritable.”
After TBI
Research on children and adults with hypo-
It has long been recognized that irritability is a thalamic hamartomas may provide, however,
common sequelae of TBI (van Zomeren, 1981). A new insights into the etiology of irritability in
recent study by Deb and Burns (2007) reported TBI patients (Prigatano, 2007). Patients with
that 37% of TBI participants ages 18–65 (n = this rare congenital anomaly often have refrac-
120) endorsed problems with irritability. This tory epilepsy. A recent report suggests that when
is consistent with the existing literature, which epilepsy is stopped via surgical intervention,
suggests that about one-third of patients with these patients may be less irritable. They are
mild to moderate injuries experience increased likely to be described by their family members
irritability (Alderman, 2003). as showing a reduction in their rage reactions.
Early efforts to relate irritability to mea- The mechanism underlying this change, how-
sures of severity of TBI or location of TBI have ever, remains unclear. Irritability after TBI may
repeatedly been unsuccessful (van Zomeren & be related to damage to deep brain structures
van den Burg, 1985). Ratings of irritability, rat- that influence behavior in complicated ways.
ings of forgetfulness, and ratings of tiring easily Davidson et al. (2003) summarized several
around people, however, load on a single factor findings that are relevant to the topic of irritabil-
(Hinkley & Corrigan, 1990). The failure to find ity and disinhibition after TBI. The orbital fron-
a relationship between severity and location of tal cortex plays an important role in inhibitory
brain injury and yet observations that irritabil- control. Ventral medial lesions may negatively
ity correlates essentially with failures in coping affect the ability of the person to use feelings to
in everyday life suggest that irritability may be guide decision making. This can lead to a vari-
a reactionary problem. This obviously requires ety of behavioral abnormalities, from excessive
further research before firm conclusions can be gambling to failure to be sensitive to others’
made. feelings (i.e., empathy). Damage to the dorso-
In their factor analytic studies, Jorge and lateral prefrontal cortex, particularly on the
Starkstein (2005) identified a third factor that left side of the brain, may predispose a person
626 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

to depression because of a fundamental distur- are frequently related to preinjury family func-
bance in approach behavior. Lesions to the ante- tion, social class, and the presence of preinjury
rior cingulate may disturb the basic connection oppositional-defiant disorder symptoms.
between attention and feelings, which again can There is a high incidence of premorbid psy-
provide a wide variety of cognitive and behav- chiatric disorders in children that may not be
ioral disorders. reported by their parents but become obvi-
ous when structured psychiatric interviews are
conducted (see Bloom et al., 2001; Max et al.,
Psychiatric and Behavioral Problems in
2001). The family environment appears to sub-
Children with TBI
stantially contribute to the behavioral problems
Rutter and colleagues (Rutter et al., 1983) have of these children (Shaffer et al., 1975; Taylor
demonstrated that TBI in children can result et al., 2001), but the role of brain damage is clearly
in considerable psychiatric and behavioral dif- present. How these two interact becomes a chal-
ficulties. Brown et al. (1981) demonstrated that lenging clinical question in individual cases.
“new psychiatric disorders” may dramatically
appear during the first year following TBI.
Socially Inappropriate Comments
Shaffer et al. (1975) recognized that an adverse
after TBI
family environment may greatly contribute to
these disorders. The final behavioral disorder to be discussed
Over the last 20 years, a number of important is that of socially inappropriate comments or
and well-designed studies have documented behavior following TBI. Such behavioral difficul-
and supported these earlier observations. Some ties are seen in both children (Rutter et al., 1983)
of the more seminal findings from these stud- and adults (Prigatano, 1999). The problem is not
ies include the following observations. Children just one of disinhibition. The TBI child or adult
who present with behavioral problems (perhaps may make comments that reflect a lack of appre-
not surprisingly) have much more difficulty ciation of what are the “do’s” and the “don’ts” of
succeeding in school (Hawley, 2004). TBI chil- different social situations. A child may comment
dren with behavioral problems typically per- on how fat a fellow child is without realizing that
form at lower levels on measures of intelligence the comment would offend that child. A young
(Hawley, 2004; Max et al., 1997, 2000). Children adult male may comment in public how beauti-
described as showing personality change after ful a young woman’s legs are without knowing
TBI have consistently been shown to have her or recognizing that the comments would
severe injuries, as judged by a combination of embarrass or offend her. Middle-aged men or
GCS scores and neuroimaging findings (Max women with severe TBI may tell a spouse that
et al., 2001). Interestingly, however, the num- they want a divorce followed by the comment,
ber of contusions versus diff use injuries to the “have a nice day.” The last comment is not said
brain have not specifically identified those chil- sarcastically. It is simply disjointed and reveals
dren who were described as having personality that the patient fails to cognitively understand
changes. and affectively experience what the words mean.
The psychiatric disorders frequently reported Not uncommonly, the problem of socially inap-
in children include attentional deficit disorder, propriate comments is associated with signif-
obsessive-compulsive disorder, oppositional- icant bilateral frontal lobe dysfunction with
defiant disorder, anxiety, and depression. Bloom associated impairment of self-awareness, par-
et al. (2001) suggested that attentional deficit ticularly awareness of one’s problems with plan-
disorders and depressive disorders were, in fact, ning and social interaction (Prigatano, 1999).
most common. Max (2004) has emphasized,
however, that oppositional-defiant disorders
Molecular Biology, Genes, and
are frequently observed in these children, par-
Psychiatric Disorders After TBI
ticularly after mild injury. Prevalence rates are
not reported. Max and colleagues (1998) report Since the genetic underpinnings of various dis-
that oppositional-defiant disorder symptoms eases are starting to be unraveled (e.g., Corder
Disorders Associated with Traumatic Brain Injury 627

et al., 1993; Fazekas et al., 2001), it is natural axonal injury, the symptoms may appear polar
that the methodologies of molecular biology be opposite. These individuals may not experi-
applied to the study of psychiatric disorders after ence any anxiety. In fact, they may appear to be
TBI (McAllister & Summerall, 2003). Several unaware of their actual limitations. However,
studies have examined how a person’s genetic they can become easily angry and frequently
makeup could be related to various markers of show memory and processing-speed difficulties.
recovery after TBI (e.g., Chiang et al., 2003; Diaz- Compatible with Goldstein’s (1952) observa-
Arrastia et al., 2003; Sorbi et al., 1995; Teasdale tions, they have a loss of the “abstract attitude.”
et al., 1997). Most of these studies have analyzed When they fail in day-to-day life activities, they
the relationship between the Apolipoprotein E4 may become overwhelmed by intense anxiety
allele (APOE ε4) and outcome after brain trauma and depression (i.e., show the catastrophic reac-
(Diaz-Arrastia & Baxter, 2006). Poorer neuro- tion). These individuals, however, often do not
psychological outcome after TBI has also been ruminate over how bad they are or how unim-
associated with presence of APOE ε4 (Liberman portant or meaningless their life is. Rather, they
et al., 2002; Teasdale et al., 2000), positive find- have repeated experiences of failure and do not
ings being reported for attention and facial know why they fail. Unlike patients with major
recognition (Sundstrom et al., 2004), memory depressive disorder they are likely to minimize
(Crawford et al., 2002), and executive function- their description of cognitive limitations. Their
ing (Ariza et al., 2006). Also, the development depressive affect, therefore, appears quite differ-
of posttraumatic dementia has been related to ent from what is seen in major depressive disor-
APOE ε4 (Koponen et al., 2004). der. After TBI, the individual may simply not be
Other authors, however, were not able to find able to cope with their environment, and anxiety
an influence of APOE ε4 on cognitive outcome and depression reflect their failure in coping.
after TBI (Chamelian et al., 2004; Millar et al., Patients with moderate TBI, particularly
2003). For example, Nathoo et al. (2003) stud- those with focal brain damage, provide the
ied 110 African patients and could not find an most varied subjective reports of what they
association between APOE ε4 presence and TBI experience. When lesions involve the mesial
outcome. left temporal lobe, language and memory func-
tions are often affected, and patients may report
worrying that they are “going crazy” (Prigatano
Phenomenological Experiences of & Smith, 2000). Their ability to communicate
Persons with Traumatic Brain Injury with others and remember what has been said
Due to the wide variety of disturbances in brain has been altered, and they may not have an easy
function that can be induced by TBI, it is not explanation for this change in functional capac-
possible to classify the varied phenomenologi- ity that they perceive.
cal experiences of patients who suffer such inju- Patients with focal lesions to the right hemi-
ries. Yet there are common descriptions that sphere, particularly the parietal lobes, however,
have been repeated by many patients. Following may have a curious lack of awareness of their
a mild TBI, patients often sense that their brain difficulties with a euphoric quality to their
function has been altered. They may experience affect. This is not true happiness, but rather a
headache and nausea. They are easily fatigued. childlike giddiness over life’s events. Few clini-
They note that any kind of physical activity cians or theorists, however, have been able to
may exacerbate a somatic complaint (such as a capture exactly what these phenomenological
headache). These individuals are frightened, experiences are, but Goldstein’s work comes the
and anxiety is often a part of their symptom closest (Goldstein, 1952). While it is important
picture. As they progressively improve, their to apply the methods of science to understand
anxiety diminishes, but many do not forget the the emotional, motivational, and behavioral
horrifying experience of not having one’s brain problems associated with TBI, it is equally
function normally. important to understand their phenomenolog-
With severe TBI, in which frontal tempo- ical experience in order to engage them in the
ral contusions are common as well as diffuse rehabilitation process (Prigatano, 1999).
628 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

Summary, Conclusions, and American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic


Reflections and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.).
Washington, DC: Author.
The study of the neuropsychiatric, psychiat- Andreasen, N. C. (2001). Brave new brain: Conquering
ric, and behavioral problems of children and mental illness in the era of the genome. New York:
adults after TBI has produced a complicated Oxford University Press.
literature. The growing use of neuroimaging Arciniegas, D. B., Harris, S. N., & Brousseau, K.
studies often report a relationship between M. (2003). Psychosis following traumatic brain
neuroimaging fi ndings and depression during injury. International Review of Psychiatry, 15(4),
328–340.
the acute-phase posttrauma, but not during the
Ariza, M., Pueyo, R., Matarín, Mdel M., Junqué, C.,
postacute phase. Premorbid factors seem to play
Mataró, M., Clemente, I., et al. (2006). Influence
a predominant role in postacute depression. of APOE polymorphism on cognitive and behav-
The absence of anxiety, especially in children, ioural outcome in moderate and severe traumatic
appears to relate to the severity of frontal lobe brain injury. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery,
damage. There may be a link between the devel- and Psychiatry, 77(10), 1191–1193.
opment of paranoid ideation and residual signs Bigler, E. D. (2005). Structural Imaging. In J. M.
of anosognosia. Irritability, once thought to be Silver, T. W. McAllister, and S. C. Yudofsky
primarily an indirect effect of brain trauma, (Eds.), Textbook of traumatic brain injury (pp.
may ultimately be found to be related to distur- 79–105). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric
bances in deep brain structures. Socially inap- Publishing, Inc.
Bloom, D. R., Levin, H. S., Ewing-Cobbs, L.,
propriate comments are common after frontal
Saunders, A. E., Song, J., Fletcher, J. M., et al.
lobe lesions.
(2001). Lifetime and novel psychiatric disorders
The potential contribution of genetic factors after pediatric traumatic brain injury. Journal of
to the recovery process after TBI and specifically the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
the neuropsychological sequelae is beginning Psychiatry, 40(5), 572–579.
to be appreciated. The data presently available, Brown, G., Chadwick, O., Shaffer, D., Rutter, M.,
however, are sparse and do not specifically & Traub, M. (1981). A prospective study of chil-
address neuropsychiatric problems after TBI. dren with head injuries: III. Psychiatric sequelae.
The more biological approach to studying the Psychological Medicine, 11(1), 63–78.
emotional and motivational disturbances after Chamelian, L., Reis, M., & Feinstein, A. (2004). Six-
TBI has produced many interesting findings. month recovery from mild to moderate traumatic
brain injury: The role of APOE-epsilon4 allele. Brain:
However, the translation of these findings into
A Journal of Neurology, 127(Pt 12), 2621–2628.
more effective rehabilitation strategies has yet
Chiang, M. F., Chang, J. G., & Hu, C. J. (2003).
to occur. Failure to separate a direct (i.e., neu- Association between apolipoprotein E genotype
ropsychiatric, behavioral) disturbance from an and outcome of traumatic brain injury. Acta
indirect one (e.g., reactionary anxiety) has con- Neurochirurgica, 145(8), 649–654.
tinued to hamper progress in the field. Such a Corder, E. H., Saunders, A. M., Strittmatter, W. J.,
description may help clinicians focus on dif- Schmechel, D. E., Gaskell, P. C., Small, G. W., et al.
ferent strategies for treating these disturbances (1993). Gene dose of apolipoprotein E type 4 allele
(Prigatano, 1999). Direct neuropsychiatric and the risk of Alzheimer’s disease in late onset
disturbances may be especially responsive to families. Science, 261(5123), 921–923.
pharmacological therapies, while the indirect Crawford, F. C., Vanderploeg, R. D., Freeman, M. J.,
Singh, S., Waisman, M., Michaels, L., et al. (2002).
psychiatric and behavioral problems may well
APOE genotype influences acquisition and recall
benefit from a combination of psychotherapy
following traumatic brain injury. Neurology, 58(7),
and pharmacological interventions. 1115–1118.
Davidson, R. J., Pizzagalli, D., Nitschke, J. B., &
References Kalin, N. H. (2003). Parsing the subcomponents
of emotion and disorders of emotion: Perspectives
Alderman, N. (2003). Contemporary approaches to from affective neurosciences. In R. J. Davidson, K.
the management of irritability and aggression fol- R. Scherer, and H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook
lowing TBI. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 13, of Affective Sciences (pp. 8–24). New York: Oxford
211–240. University Press.
Disorders Associated with Traumatic Brain Injury 629

Deb, S., & Burns, J. (2007). Neuropsychiatric con- Hawley, C. A., Ward, A. B., Magnay, A. R., & Long, J.
sequences of traumatic brain injury: A compar- (2003). Parental stress and burden following trau-
ison between two age groups. Brain Injury, 21, matic brain injury amongst children and adoles-
301–307. cents. Brain Injury, 17(1), 1–23.
Deb, S., Lyons, I., Koutzoukis, C., Ali, I., & McCarthy, Hillbom, E. (1960). After-effects of brain-injuries.
G. (1999). Rate of psychiatric illness 1 year after Acta Psychiatrica et Neurologica Scandinavica,
traumatic brain injury. The American Journal of S142, 1–195.
Psychiatry, 156(3), 374–378. Hinkeldey, N. S., & Corrigan, J. D. (1990). The struc-
DeLuca, J., & Cicerone, K. D. (1991). Confabulation ture of head-injured patients’ neurobehavioural
following aneurism of the anterior communicat- complaints: A preliminary study. Brain Injury,
ing artery. Cortex, 27, 417–423. 4(2), 115–133.
Diaz-Arrastia, R., & Baxter, V. K. (2006). Genetic Hiott, D. W., & Labbate, L. (2002). Anxiety disor-
factors in outcome after traumatic brain injury: ders associated with traumatic brain injuries.
What the human genome project can teach us NeuroRehabilitation, 17(4), 345–355.
about brain trauma. The Journal of Head Trauma Jorge, R. E. (2005). Neuropsychiatric consequences of
Rehabilitation, 21(4), 361–374. traumatic brain injury: A review of recent fi ndings.
Diaz-Arrastia, R., Gong, Y., Fair, S., Scott, K. D., Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 18(3), 289–299.
Garcia, M. C., Carlile, M. C., et al. (2003). Increased Jorge, R. E., Acion, L., Starkstein, S. E., & Magnotta,
risk of late posttraumatic seizures associated with V. (2007). Hippocampal volume and mood dis-
inheritance of APOE epsilon4 allele. Archives of orders after traumatic brain injury. Biological
Neurology, 60(6), 818–822. Psychiatry, 62(4), 332–338.
Dikmen, S. S., Bombardier, C. H., Machamer, J. E., Jorge, R. E., Robinson, R. G., Arndt, S. V., Starkstein,
Fann, J. R., & Temkin, N. R. (2004). Natural his- S. E., Forrester, A. W., & Geisler, F. (1993).
tory of depression in traumatic brain injury. Depression following traumatic brain injury:
Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, A 1 year longitudinal study. Journal of Affective
85(9), 1457–1464. Disorders, 27(4), 233–243.
Eslinger, P. J., Parkinson, K., & Shamay, S. G. (2002). Jorge, R. E., Robinson, R. G., Moser, D., Tateno, A.,
Empathy and social-emotional factors in recovery Crespo-Facorro, B., & Arndt, S. (2004). Major
from stroke. Current Opinion in Neurology, 15(1), depression following traumatic brain injury.
91–97. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61(1), 42–50.
Fann, J. R., Burington, B., Leonetti, A., Jaffe, K., Jorge, R. E., & Starkstein, S. E. (2005). Pathophysiologic
Katon, W. J., & Thompson, R. S. (2004). Psychiatric aspects of major depression following trau-
illness following traumatic brain injury in an adult matic brain injury. The Journal of Head Trauma
health maintenance organization population. Rehabilitation, 20(6), 475–487.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 61(1), 53–61. Klonoff, P. S., & Lage, G. A. (1991). Narcissistic injury
Fazekas, F., Strasser-Fuchs, S., Kollegger, H., Berger, after traumatic brain injury. Journal of Head
T., Kristoferitsch, W., Schmidt, H., et al. (2001). Trauma Rehabilitation, 6, 11–21.
Apolipoprotein E epsilon 4 is associated with Koponen, S., Taiminen, T., Kairisto, V., Portin, R.,
rapid progression of multiple sclerosis. Neurology, Isoniemi, H., Hinkka, S., et al. (2004). APOE-
57(5), 853–857. epsilon4 predicts dementia but not other psy-
Fedoroff, J. P., Starkstein, S. E., Forrester, A. W., chiatric disorders after traumatic brain injury.
Geisler, F. H., Jorge, R. E., Arndt, S. V., et al. (1992). Neurology, 63(4), 749–750.
Depression in patients with acute traumatic brain Koponen, S., Taiminen, T., Kurki, T., Portin, R.,
injury. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 149(7), Isoniemi, H., Himanen, L., et al. (2006). MRI
918–923. findings and Axis I and II psychiatric disorders
Fujii, D., Ahmed, I., & Hishinuma, E. (2004). A neu- after traumatic brain injury: A 30-year retrospec-
ropsychological comparison of psychotic disorder tive follow-up study. Psychiatry Research, 146(3),
following traumatic brain injury, traumatic brain 263–270.
injury without psychotic disorder, and schizophre- Koponen, S., Taiminen, T., Portin, R., Himanen, L.,
nia. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Isoniemi, H., Heinonen, H., et al. (2002). Axis I
Neurosciences, 16(3), 306–314. and II psychiatric disorders after traumatic brain
Goldstein, K. (1952). The effect of brain damage on injury: A 30-year follow-up study. The American
the personality. Psychiatry, 15(3), 245–260. Journal of Psychiatry, 159(8), 1315–1321.
Hawley, C. A. (2004). Behaviour and school per- Lawrence, E. J., Shaw, P., Giampietro, V. P.,
formance after brain injury. Brain Injury, 18(7), Surguladze, S., Brammer, M. J., & David, A. S.
645–659. (2006). The role of ‘shared representations’ in
630 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

social perception and empathy: An fMRI study. Meyer, A. (1904). The anatomical facts and clinical
NeuroImage, 29(4), 1173–1184. varieties of traumatic insanity. American Journal
Levin, H. S., McCauley, S. R., Josic, C. P., Boake, of Insanity, 60, 373–441.
C., Brown, S. A., Goodman, H. S., et al. (2005). Millar, K., Nicoll, J. A., Thornhill, S., Murray, G. D., &
Predicting depression following mild traumatic Teasdale, G. M. (2003). Long term neuropsycholog-
brain injury. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(5), ical outcome after head injury: Relation to APOE
523–528. genotype. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and
Liberman, J. N., Stewart, W. F., Wesnes, K., & Psychiatry, 74(8), 1047–1052.
Troncoso, J. (2002). Apolipoprotein E epsilon 4 Mizrahi, R., Starkstein, S. E., Jorge, R., & Robinson,
and short-term recovery from predominantly R. G. (2006). Phenomenology and clinical cor-
mild brain injury. Neurology, 58(7), 1038–1044. relates of delusions in Alzheimer disease. The
Lishman, W. A. (1968). Brain damage in relation to American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 14(7),
psychiatric disability after head injury. The British 573–581.
Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Nathoo, N., Chetry, R., van Dellen, J. R., Connolly,
Science, 114(509), 373–410. C., & Naidoo, R. (2003). Apolipoprotein E poly-
Lishman, W. A. (1978). Organic psychiatry: The morphism and outcome after closed traumatic
psychological consequences of cerebral disorder. brain injury: Influence of ethnic and regional
Boston: Blackwell Scientific Publications. differences. Journal of Neurosurgery, 98(2),
Luria, A. R. (1948/1963). Restoration of function after 302–306.
brain trauma (in Russian). Moscow: Academy of Prigatano, G. P. (1988). Anosognosia, delusions,
Medical Science; London: Pergamon. and altered self-awareness after brain injury. BNI
Max, J. E. (2004). Psychiatric sequelae of trau- Quarterly, 4(3), 40–48.
matic brain injury in children and adole- Prigatano, G. P. (1992). Personality disturbances
scents. American Academy of Physical Medicine associated with traumatic brain injury. Journal
and Rehabilitation, Course Handouts, www. of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60(3),
aapmr.org. 360–368.
Max, J. E., Castillo, C. S., Bokura, H., Robin, D. Prigatano, G. P. (1999). Principles of neuropsycholog-
A., Lindgren, S. D., Smith, W. L., et al. (1998). ical rehabilitation. New York: Oxford University
Oppositional defiant disorder symptomatology Press.
after traumatic brain injury: A prospective study. Prigatano, G. P. (2005). Therapy for emotional and
The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disorders, 186, motivational disorders. In W. M. High, A. M.
325–332. Sander, M. A. Struchen, and K. A. Hart (Eds.),
Max, J. E., Koele, S. L., Castillo, C. C., Lindgren, S. D., Rehabilitation for traumatic brain injury (pp. 118–
Arndt, S., Bokura, H., et al. (2000). Personality change 130). New York: Oxford University Press.
disorder in children and adolescents following trau- Prigatano, G. P. (2007). Cognitive and behavioral
matic brain injury. Journal of the International dysfunction in children with a hypothalamic
hamartoma syndrome. Seminars in Pediatric
Neuropsychological Society, 6(3), 279–289.
Neurology, 14(2), 65–72.
Max, J. E., Robertson, B. A., & Lansing, A. E. (2001).
Prigatano, G. P., Fordyce, D. J., Zeiner, H. K.,
The phenomenology of personality change due
Roueche, J. R., Pepping, M., & Wood, B. C. (1986).
to traumatic brain injury in children and adoles-
Neuropsychological rehabilitation after brain
cents. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical
injury. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University
Neurosciences, 13(2), 161–170.
Press.
Max, J. E., Robin, D. A., Lindgren, S. D., Smith,
Prigatano, G. P., Gray, J. A., & Gale, S. D. (2007).
W. L., Sato, Y., Mattheis, P. J., et al. (1997).
Individual case analysis of processing speed dif-
Traumatic brain injury in children and adoles-
ficulties in children with and without traumatic
cents: Psychiatric disorders at two years. Journal
brain injury. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 22,
of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
603–619.
Psychiatry, 36(9), 1278–1285.
Prigatano, G. P., & Smith, K. (2000). The role of
McAllister, T. W., & Ferrell, R. B. (2002). Evaluation
psychotherapy in a neurological institute. BNI
and treatment of psychosis after traumatic brain
Quarterly, 16(3), 7–11.
injury. NeuroRehabilitation, 17(4), 357–368.
Prigatano, G. P., & Summers, J. (1997). Depression in
McAllister, T. W., & Summerall, L. (2003). Genetic
patients with brain dysfunction. In M. Robertson
polymorphisms in the expression and treatment
and C. Katona (Eds.), Depression and physical ill-
of neuropsychiatric disorders. Current Psychiatric
ness (pp. 341–357). Sussex, England: John Wiley
Reports, 5(5), 400–409.
and Sons, Ltd.
Disorders Associated with Traumatic Brain Injury 631

Ruff, R. M., & Richards, P. M. (2003). Neuro- Teasdale, G. M., Nicoll, J. A., Murray, G., & Fiddes,
psychological assessment and management of M. (1997). Association of apolipoprotein E poly-
patients with persistent postconcussional disorders. morphism with outcome after head injury. Lancet,
In G. P. Prigatano and N. H. Pliskin (Eds.), Clinical 350(9084), 1069–1071.
neuropsychology and cost outcome research: A begin- Teasdale, T. W., Jorgensen, O. S., Ripa, C., Nielsen,
ning (pp. 61–81). New York: Psychology Press. A. S., & Christensen, A.-L. (2000). Apolipoprotein
Rutter, M., Chadwick, O., & Shaffer, D. (1983). Head E and subjective symptomatology following
injury. In M. Rutter (Ed.), Developmental neuropsy- brain injury rehabilitation. Neuropsychological
chiatry (pp. 83–111). New York: The Guilford Press. Rehabilitation, 10, 151–166.
Schilder, P. (1934). Psychic disturbances after head Thomsen, I. V. (1984). Late outcome of very severe
injuries. American Journal of Psychiatry, 91, blunt head trauma: A 10–15 year second fol-
155–188. low-up. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and
Shaffer, D., Chadwick, O., & Rutter, M. (1975). Psychiatry, 47(3), 260–268.
Psychiatric outcome of localized head injury in chil- van Zomeren, A. H. (1981). Reaction time and atten-
dren. Ciba Foundation Symposium, 34, 191–213. tion after closed head injury. Lisse: Swets and
Silver, J. M., Kramer, R., Greenwald, S., & Weissman, Zeitlinger.
M. (2001). The association between head injuries van Zomeren, A. H., & van den Burg, W. (1985).
and psychiatric disorders: Findings from the New Residual complaints of patients two years
Haven NIMH Epidemiologic Catchment Area after severe head injury. Journal of Neurology,
Study. Brain Injury, 15(11), 935–945. Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 48(1), 21–28.
Silver, J. M., McAllister, T. W., & Yudofsky, S. Vasa, R. A., Grados, M., Slomine, B., Herskovits,
C. (Eds.). (2005). Textbook of traumatic brain E. H., Thompson, R. E., Salorio, C., et al. (2004).
injury. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Neuroimaging correlates of anxiety after pediat-
Publishing, Inc. ric traumatic brain injury. Biological Psychiatry,
Sorbi, S., Nacmias, B., Piacentini, S., Repice, A., 55(3), 208–216.
Latorraca, S., Forleo, P., et al. (1995). ApoE as a Winstanley, J., Simpson, G., Tate, R., & Myles, B.
prognostic factor for post-traumatic coma. Nature (2006). Early indicators and contributors to psy-
Medicine, 1(9), 852. chological distress in relatives during rehabili-
Sundstrom, A., Marklund, P., Nilsson, L. G., Cruts, tation following severe traumatic brain injury:
M., Adolfsson, R., Van Broeckhoven, C., et al. Findings from the Brain Injury Outcomes Study.
(2004). APOE influences on neuropsychological The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 21(6),
function after mild head injury: Within-person 453–466.
comparisons. Neurology, 62(11), 1963–1966. Wood, R. L. L., & Williams, C. (2008). Inability
Tateno, A., Jorge, R. E., & Robinson, R. G. (2004). to empathize following traumatic brain injury.
Pathological laughing and crying following trau- Journal of the International Neuropsychological
matic brain injury. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry Society, 14, 289–296.
and Clinical Neurosciences, 16(4), 426–434. Yeates, K. O, Taylor, H. G., Drotar, D., Wade, S. L.,
Taylor, H. G., Yeates, K. O., Wade, S. L., Drotar, D., Klein, S., Stancin, T., et al. (1997). Preinjury fam-
Stancin, T., & Burant, C. (2001). Bidirectional child- ily environment as a determinant of recovery from
family influences on outcomes of traumatic brain traumatic brain injuries in school-age children.
injury in children. Journal of the International Journal of the International Neuropsychological
Neuropsychological Society, 7(6), 755–767. Society, 3, 617–630.
Teasdale, G., & Jennett, B. (1974). Assessment of Zhang, Q., & Sachdev, P. S. (2003). Psychotic disor-
coma and impaired consciousness. A practical der and traumatic brain injury. Current Psychiatry
scale. Lancet, 2(7872), 81–84. Reports, 5(3), 197–201.
26

Neuropsychology in Relation to Everyday


Functioning
Erin E. Morgan and Robert K. Heaton

identification, and another important shift that


Overview: The
is still under way is the need to provide an under-
Neuropsychological Approach to
standing of the relationship of basic abilities to
Predicting Everyday Functioning
adequacy of everyday functioning (Chelune &
The field of clinical neuropsychology has his- Moehle, 1986). Whereas “basic ability” refers
torically been concerned with detecting and to an individual’s skill or talent, “functioning”
characterizing the nature of cerebral disorders, refers to enacting the ability in the environ-
which has resulted in a well-established role for ment (Goldstein, 1996). Evaluation of ability
neuropsychologists in the diagnosis of brain lends itself to assessment in the laboratory with
pathology. Although increasing use of advanced standardized tests, but referral questions have
neuroimaging techniques has somewhat reduced changed to require “prescriptive statements” in
the need for neuropsychological test data as an which inferences are made from an individual’s
ancillary diagnostic tool, the diagnostic role pattern of strengths and deficits to predict his/
of the neuropsychologist remains valuable in her ability to carry out everyday activities in the
cases where neuroimaging is inconclusive (e.g., real world (Chelune & Moehle, 1986). For exam-
after brain trauma or in suspected cases of early ple, the major clinical questions often concern
dementia). Furthermore, the field has evolved the impact of the brain insult or disorder on the
from its early diagnostic focus, and, arguably, patient’s ability to live independently, his/her
today the most important contribution of neu- prospects for employment and rehabilitation,
ropsychology is the identification and delinea- and identification of a need for environmental
tion of cognitive and behavioral consequences of support (Heaton & Pendleton, 1981).
cerebral pathology. An accurate understanding Initially, questions about the functional
of such consequences often is as important as impact of cerebral disorders were addressed by
an accurate diagnosis (Chelune & Moehle, 1986; considering performance on neuropsychologi-
Heaton & Pendleton, 1981). As focus has shifted cal tests that were developed for primarily diag-
from organic brain integrity to brain–behavior nostic purposes (Chelune & Moehle, 1986). An
relationships, standardized assessments have important question that arises from this prac-
been developed to successfully measure rela- tice is whether the same test procedures that
tionships between brain dysfunction and its were used for lesion identification and localiza-
cognitive and behavioral outcomes in every- tion can also be used for making predictions of
day life (Chaytor & Schmitter-Edgecomb, 2003; patient’s capabilities and limitations in carry-
Chelune & Moehle, 1986). The defining fea- ing out everyday tasks and activities (Heaton &
ture of this shift was the focus on assessment Pendleton, 1981). Accordingly, concerns about
of ability as a primary goal, rather than lesion the ecological validity of neuropsychological

632
Neuropsychology in Relation to Everyday Functioning 633

tests have been raised—namely, the degree to accurate understanding of the cognitive factors
which basic abilities assessed in controlled labo- that influence everyday functioning.
ratory settings relate to real-world performance
(Franzen & Wilhelm, 1996; Tupper & Cicerone, Methodological Considerations for
1990). Many methods of defining such real-
Predicting Everyday Functioning
world functional outcomes have been utilized,
including self-report, proxy/caregiver reports,
Defining Predictors
and clinician ratings. However, these subjective,
report-based measures have limitations that On the basis of a neuropsychological approach to
may obscure relationships between basic abil- prediction of everyday functioning, if a patient
ities and adequacy of functioning in everyday demonstrates impairment of a test involving
life. In order to address these limitations, recent a particular ability, he/she is assumed to be
work has focused on development of objective at risk of experiencing difficulty in everyday
tests of functional capacity, or a person’s level tasks and activities in which the same ability is
of competence with specific everyday activi- required (Heaton & Pendleton, 1981). Although
ties. These tests, which are performance-based seemingly straightforward, linking cognition
instruments that simulate real-world activities, to everyday functioning is complicated by the
are intended to bridge the gap between the lab- need to understand which abilities are essential
oratory and the environment. Performance- for successful performance of which everyday
based tests of functional capacity have more tasks, and under what circumstances. In fact,
face validity compared to traditional neuropsy- although the process of “functional analysis” of
chological tests with regard to real-world rel- complex everyday activities has been previously
evance, and therefore address some concerns described (Goldstein, 1996), to date there is no
regarding ecological validity. A review of the well-established account of the specific abilities
types of everyday functioning measures that are that are required for various everyday tasks,
available, as well as their strengths, limitations, such as managing money and keeping appoint-
and indications, appears later in this chapter. ments. Also, specific demands for these classes
Despite the limited face validity of traditional of activities vary considerably from person to
neuropsychological tests for this purpose, ongo- person; for example, complexity of “financial
ing research has provided considerable evidence management” can vary considerably with the
regarding the predictive validity of the neuro- number, types, and predictability of income
psychological approach for estimating success sources and expenses, and the associated bud-
in important aspects of everyday functioning. geting and accounting that is required. In the
This research has major clinical applications absence of a firmly established understanding
for predicting whether an individual’s brain of specific abilities required by specific daily
disorder will affect his/her functional abili- functions, and how they relate to our standard
ties, such as returning to work or driving a car. neuropsychological tests, considerable clinical
This is especially true in the context of foren- judgment is still required.
sic neuropsychology, in which a neuropsychol- Many early studies focused solely on the
ogist’s predictions can directly influence the relationship between general intelligence (IQ)
legal outcomes in a patient’s case, such as dis- and various aspects of functioning. This work
ability claims or compensation awarded (Long clearly demonstrated that IQ as a sole pre-
& Collins, 1997). Accurate prediction of the dictor does have some validity with respect to
impact of ability deficits on functioning is also many aspects of functioning. However, con-
necessary to inform rehabilitation profession- sideration of more specific cognitive abilities
als and family members regarding likely areas assessed within a comprehensive battery not
of impaired everyday functioning and poten- only provides more information regarding the
tial for developing compensatory strategies. nature of a patient’s deficits but also allows for
Although not reviewed within this chapter, the potential to utilize the patient’s strengths to
neuropsychological rehabilitation is an impor- compensate for impairments. One widely used
tant clinical application that is based upon an “fi xed battery” (a standard, comprehensive set
634 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

of tests administered to all patients regardless operationally defined in such a way that allows
of suspected diagnosis or impairment) is an for measurement and empirical study. In exam-
expanded Halstead–Reitan Battery (HRB), such ining everyday functioning, behavior in the
as that described by Heaton and Taylor (2004). natural environment is the criterion, and this
Another approach is to focus on the six ability introduces numerous challenges to finding
domains that have been identified through con- outcome measures that are reliable, compara-
firmatory factor analysis of the Wechsler Adult ble, and meaningful across different patients
Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III; Psychological (Heaton & Pendleton, 1981). The most obvious
Corporation, 1997) and Wechsler Memory challenge is the fact that the “environment” is
Scale-III (WMS-III; Psychological Corporation, quite variable across patients, and the factors
1997) test batteries, including verbal compre- that determine functional success are complex
hension, perceptual organization, working and vary with the demands of an individual’s
memory, auditory learning/memory, visual environment. Numerous ability deficits, alone
learning/memory, and speed of information or in various combinations, can result in failure
processing (Tulsky & Price, 2003). in performing a previously successful activity.
Despite a persisting lack of evidence linking Similar deficits, alone or in combination, could
specific abilities with specific everyday tasks, have a very different impact on different peo-
findings in the literature generally reveal that ple, making it difficult to make global predic-
neuropsychological tests do measure abilities tions about functional status (Farias et al., 2003;
that are relevant for everyday functioning. For Heaton & Pendleton, 1981).
example, performances on various sets of neu- Data collected through direct observation
ropsychological tests have been shown to be of individuals in their natural environments
predictive of academic success, current and arguably would be the most accurate measure
future employment status, requirements of jobs of everyday functioning, because there would
at which patients can succeed, successful per- be no need for inferences from basic abilities to
formance of activities necessary for indepen- functional performance. However, this requires
dent living, medication management, driving considerable time, effort, and cost, and is there-
ability, and social and community adjustment. fore not feasible in most cases. Also, direct
Given the complexity of these everyday activ- observation of everyday functioning can be
ities and the likelihood that different patterns intrusive and can actually change the phenom-
of ability deficits could similarly interfere with ena being observed; for example, the presence
their successful performance, it may be that of a clinician observing a patient’s vocational
multifactorial summary measures will always behavior in a job setting can influence what the
provide the best predictions. However, more patient does, what his/her coworkers and super-
information is still needed regarding the basic visors do, or both.
abilities that should be represented in these After direct observation, another straightfor-
summary measures, as well as the most efficient ward method of gathering information about
ways to assess them, and the severity of impair- an individual’s level of functioning in activities
ment that is needed to interfere with the out- of daily living (ADL) is to ask for his/her own
come of interest. In addition, a consideration of assessment, or self-report. In addition to its con-
the motivational abilities that may not be well venience, most would agree that the individu-
represented in such summary ability (or deficit) al’s own perception of functioning is central to
scores will be needed to plan rehabilitation and/ quality of life and is therefore quite important
or compensation strategies. (Awad et al., 1997). Numerous self-report mea-
sures have been developed and used with a wide
variety of patient populations. However, many
Defining Outcomes
factors that can reduce the accuracy of self-re-
Everyday functioning is a broad concept port have been well documented, including level
encompassing numerous activities and behav- of insight, psychopathology, and situational fac-
iors. Also, in studies examining everyday tors (Williams, 1994). With regard to a patient’s
functioning outcomes, the criterion must be level of insight, the cognitive impairment that
Neuropsychology in Relation to Everyday Functioning 635

the person is experiencing may lead the indi- level of functioning (e.g., Patterson et al., 2001).
vidual to underreport deficits and functional Individuals with a close relationship to the patient
problems (e.g., Cahn-Weiner et al., 2003). The may be biased by their feelings for the patient or
validity of self-report measures is especially may only observe the patient in settings in which
questionable in demented and severely men- the problems with functioning are less likely to
tally ill populations, in which a patient’s level occur, such as the home (Heaton & Pendleton,
of insight is likely to be most compromised 1981). Loewenstein et al. (2001) found that care-
(Farias et al., 2003; Patterson et al., 2001). givers of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) patients fre-
Psychopathology, especially depression, very quently failed to predict their relatives’ difficulties
often distorts the cognitive and emotional expe- on fairly simple ADLs (e.g., telling time, letter
riences upon which judgments about everyday preparation, identifying and counting currency,
functioning are based. Even individuals who do making change, and using eating utensils). Other
not meet criteria for Major Depressive Disorder studies have similarly reported that caregivers
are greatly influenced by their depressive mood tend to overestimate the functional abilities of
symptoms when completing self-report mea- the patients in their care (e.g., Doble et al., 1999).
sures. This effect has been demonstrated in var- On the other hand, recent contrary evidence
ious populations, including individuals with has been reported by Davis et al. (2006), who
HIV infection (Cysique et al., 2007; Heaton found that caregivers in their study were gener-
et al., 2004; Rourke et al., 1999) and normal ally accurate in reporting the functional abili-
aging (Kliegel & Zimprich, 2005). ties of the AD patients in their care and were not
Another illustration of the effect of psychopa- heavily influenced by their own characteristics
thology on self-report is a finding from a diverse (e.g., level of depressive symptoms) or the quality
group of individuals referred for clinical evalu- of their relationship with the patients. However,
ation, in which complaints of cognitive difficul- the authors also noted that most of the caregiv-
ties were more related to their emotional status ers in their study were spouses with high levels
than to objective measures of their cognitive of education, and minorities were underrepre-
abilities (Heaton et al., 1978). Patient self-report sented in their sample. Additionally, caregivers
was measured with the Patient’s Assessment of and family members may be disproportionately
Own Functioning (PAOFI; Chelune et al., 1986), affected by particular patient deficits. For exam-
a questionnaire containing 32 items on which ple, one study reported that caregiver ratings
patients rate their difficulties with everyday func- of memory were more influenced by language
tioning in four domains: general cognitive abili- problems such as word finding deficits than by
ties, memory, language and communication, and actual memory impairment (Cahn-Weiner et al.,
sensorimotor skills. Multiple regression analyses 2003). A more recent informant-based question-
revealed that patient self-report was more related naire, the Dementia Severity Scale (DSS; Harvey
to results from the Minnesota Multiphasic et al., 2005), has been developed as a brief, simple
Personality Inventory (MMPI; Hathaway & tool for gathering meaningful clinical informa-
McKinley, 1951) than to those from an extended tion from caregivers. The sound psychometric
HRB. In fact, the MMPI results accounted for properties of the questionnaire suggest that this
42.8% of the variance in cognitive complaints, instrument may be useful in research as well as
as compared to only 6.6% for neuropsychological clinical settings (Harvey et al., 2005). Collateral
test scores. Additionally, situational factors such reports are frequently used in conjunction with
as secondary gain could influence the accuracy self-report in an effort to assess reliability of
of a patient’s reported problems. patient reports, but in many cases there may be
Collateral/proxy reports from caregivers, little agreement between the two sets of judg-
spouses, or family members are often assumed ments of the patient’s functioning. In such cases
to be more accurate than patient self-report. it can be difficult to determine the causes of the
However, not all patients will have someone to disagreements and the relative accuracy of the
provide the report, and for those patients who two reports.
do have someone available, that person may not Functional ratings by clinicians based on
be entirely appropriate to report on the patient’s observation of a patient during an interview in
636 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

a hospital, clinician’s office, or research setting has been studied extensively. Much of the work
have also been used to measure everyday func- that has been done in this area has focused on
tioning. However, these ratings may not directly the relationship between results of intelligence
relate to the individual’s functioning in his/ tests and academic performance indices such as
her everyday environment. Clinicians’ interac- years of education completed and grades. Several
tion with patients is often brief and limited in reviews of the relevant literature are available,
scope, and clinicians typically do not have the such as those by Fishman and Pasanella (1960),
opportunity to observe the everyday behaviors Lavin (1965), Matarazzo (1972), and Heaton and
of interest. In fact, their ratings are most often Pendleton (1981).
based on or greatly influenced by the reports of Generally, the literature indicates that the
the patient and/or family, and as a result clini- relationship between IQ and academic suc-
cian ratings often are not independent or neces- cess is robust (e.g., Heaton & Pendleton, 1981).
sarily more objective. Matarazzo’s review (1972) demonstrated that the
Molar outcomes, such as employed versus approximate correlation between intelligence and
unemployed, are commonly utilized functional academic success (i.e., grades) was r = 0.50 when
outcome measures that generally show a robust averaged across various studies and populations.
association with neuropsychological indices Despite the consistent relationship between IQ
(e.g., Heaton et al., 2004). Results from a battery and academic success, IQ alone is not sufficient
of tests, often calculated as a summary score such for explaining academic outcomes, especially
as an average impairment rating or global defi- in clinical populations. Heaton and Pendleton
cit score (GDS; e.g., Heaton et al., 2004), appear (1981) advocated the use of a comprehensive neu-
to be the best at predicting such molar outcomes ropsychological battery with adult neurological
(employed versus unemployed; independent patient populations, noting that deficits associ-
versus dependent living status) because a com- ated with brain disorders may impact academic
bination of abilities is responsible for both the performance regardless of pre- and postmorbid
predictor measure and outcome classification IQ. Matarazzo also noted that, with only 25% of
(Franzen & Wilhelm, 1996). As such, discrete the variance in academic success explained for
categories of functional outcome often reflect a neuropsychologically normal individuals, other
wide range of actual functioning. Even the seem- cognitive and noncognitive factors must play
ingly straightforward categorical variable of important roles. On the basis of the findings of
employment status (employed vs. unemployed) his review, Matarazzo suggested that a mini-
can vary greatly, depending on the nature of mum IQ threshold for completion of successive
the job requirements and whether employment educational degrees (i.e., high school diploma,
is defined full-time, part-time, or intermittent Bachelor’s degree, graduate school) might exist,
(Guilmette & Kastner, 1996; Heaton et al., 1978). and that once that threshold has been met, non-
intellective factors (e.g., motivation) may be more
Neuropsychological Predictors influential in determining academic success in
of Everyday Functioning neuropsychologically normal persons. However,
other reports have indicated that accounting for
Neuropsychological test results have typically
nonintellective variables in addition to intellec-
been examined as predictors of everyday func-
tive factors only modestly improved predictive
tioning in three major classes of functional
accuracy (for reviews see Fishman & Pasanella,
outcomes: academic achievement, independent
1960; Lavin, 1965).
living, and vocational functioning. Overall,
studies have demonstrated that neuropsycho-
logical test results are significantly related to Independent Living
success in all of these areas.
The functions that have been conceptualized
as ADLs vary in the literature, depending on
Academic Achievement
the nature and purpose of the study. Some
The relationship between cognitive test perfor- commonalities in the context of brain injury
mance and the ability to succeed academically include self-care skills, ambulation, mobility,
Neuropsychology in Relation to Everyday Functioning 637

vision, health and safety, and communication. significant IADL dependence (e.g., Heaton
In general, ratings (such as those assigned in et al., 2004).
rehabilitation settings) and structured instru- Several recent studies have linked more spe-
ments used to assess basic ADLs have been cific neuropsychological abilities to IADL per-
shown to be moderately related to neuropsy- formance. For example, IADLs have been shown
chological test results. For example, WAIS to be strongly related to executive functioning in
subtest scores have been related to both cur- normal aging (Bell-McGinty et al., 2002; Cahn-
rent self-care skills as well as future self-care Weiner et al., 2000), AD (e.g., Boyle et al., 2003;
performance in patients who suffered cere- Cahn-Weiner et al., 2003), vascular dementia
brovascular accidents (Lehmann et al., 1975). (e.g., Boyle et al., 2004; Jefferson et al., 2006),
Neuropsychological test results also have been and schizophrenia (e.g., Green, 1996). A study
related to prognosis for independent living of HIV-infected individuals demonstrated that
in both psychiatric and neurological patients deficits in learning, abstraction/executive func-
(Lehmann et al., 1975; Rioch & Lubin, 1959). tion, and attention/working memory domains
More recent evidence has linked memory dys- accounted for a considerable proportion of the
function to declines in ADLs in patients with variance in predicting IADL failures and func-
AD (e.g., Drachman et al., 1990) and vascular tional dependence, overall (as indicated by fail-
dementia (e.g., Jefferson et al., 2006). ure on a performance-based functional battery;
Given that basic ADLs are relatively simple, Heaton et al., 2004). Also, a more recent study
overlearned skills that tend to have a signifi- of HIV-infected individuals revealed that a new
cant motor component (such as grooming and verbal fluency measure (action fluency) by itself
mobility), they may be more associated with was significantly predictive of IADL depen-
physical illness than cognitive dysfunction. dence (Woods et al., 2006).
Accordingly, there is evidence to suggest that
neuropsychological test results may be most Vocational Functioning
useful for estimating patients’ ability to carry
out complex tasks that draw more heavily upon One of the most common and important neu-
cognitive abilities (e.g., money management) ropsychological referral questions involving
than other, more routine and motor-based daily prediction of functioning concerns the patient’s
activities, such as bathing (McCue et al., 1990; ability to maintain employment and perform
Richardson et al., 1995). Thus, researchers have work functions. Much research has been con-
also focused on instrumental activities of daily ducted to investigate the relationship between
living (IADLs), which are more complex every- neuropsychological data and vocational func-
day tasks that have been surveyed with a mod- tioning, and generally the literature reveals that
ified version of the Lawton and Brody (1969) neuropsychological predictions of employment
scale. A modified version of this scale consists status and job performance are quite good.
of 13 items, which require the individual to Early studies in this area revealed that in
separately rate his/her current level of indepen- the healthy adults IQ was related to type of job
dence and highest previous level of indepen- obtained, mean income level, ratings of occupa-
dence in the following functional categories: tional prestige, and job performance (reviews
financial management, home repair, medica- by Heaton & Pendleton, 1981; Matarazzo, 1972).
tion management, laundry, transportation, gro- Research conducted with more comprehensive
cery shopping, comprehension of reading/TV neuropsychological test batteries has revealed
materials, shopping, housekeeping (cleaning), that these tests can discriminate between
cooking, bathing, dressing, and telephone use. employed and unemployed individuals having
The total score indicates the number of activi- either neurologic and psychiatric disorders,
ties for which the individual currently requires strongly suggesting that current employment
increased assistance. Increased dependence in status is dependent upon the level of neuropsy-
two or more areas, determined from the dis- chological functioning. In the first prospective
tribution found in a normal sample, has been study of the relationship between neuropsy-
proposed as a cut score in defining clinically chological functioning and employment status,
638 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

Newnan et al. (1978) examined the ability of of specific occupations. Many types of abilities
an expanded HRB and MMPI to predict future are required for most jobs, and variable patterns
employment in patients with stable neurologic of deficits may significantly interfere. Also, pat-
conditions who were followed for six months. terns of deficits seen with brain disorders are
The results of that study revealed that the extremely variable, if not unique, even within
Average Impairment Rating from the HRB was groups of patients with a common etiology (e.g.,
the best single predictor of employment status, traumatic brain injury, multiple sclerosis, HIV
and that the MMPI showed less predictive abil- disease). It is not possible to research the effects of
ity with regard to future employment than it a single deficit in isolation (which is rarely seen)
had to current employment status in a previous or every possible pattern of deficits. Therefore,
study (Heaton et al., 1978). Similarly, employed it is likely that there will always be a substantial
HIV-infected individuals who eventually experi- role for “clinical judgment” in these decisions.
enced work disability demonstrated significantly Still, research can be directed at determining the
poorer performance at baseline on a variety of accuracy of such decisions and the patient, job,
neuropsychological tests (Albert et al., 1999). and environmental factors that contribute to
More recently, a study of HIV-infected individu- success or failure.
als examined numerous predictors of return to In general, the research so far suggests that,
work and revealed that the California Verbal as the number and severity of deficits increase,
Learning Test (CVLT; Delis et al., 1987), a mea- the likelihood of obtaining and holding com-
sure of learning and memory, was a significant petitive employment decreases. For example,
and robust predictor of becoming employed, the Average Impairment Rating from the HRB
even when effects of IQ and medical measures appears to serve as a good single predictor of
of disease severity were controlled (van Gorp employment status (Newnan et al., 1978), but
et al., 2007). Other studies demonstrating the may be improved upon by adding measures of
significant associations between neuropsycho- episodic memory and other abilities underrep-
logical test data and employment status and job resented by the core HRB (Heaton et al., 1978).
performance have been conducted in various Further, evidence in the literature suggests that
populations, such as traumatic brain injury (Rao although neuropsychological variables do have
& Kilgore, 1992), multiple sclerosis (Pierson & predictive power in discriminating employed
Griffith, 2006; Rao et al., 1991), and schizophre- and unemployed individuals, these data in iso-
nia (Heaton et al., 1994). lation are often not adequate predictors and may
It is important to note that neuropsycholo- be improved upon by also considering personal-
gists are often asked to judge not only whether a ity variables (reviewed in Guilmette & Kastner,
person can work in general, but specifically what 1996; Heaton & Pendleton, 1981).
kind of work that individual is capable of doing.
Although findings in the literature generally sup-
General Conclusions and Limitations
port a relationship between neuropsychological
of the Neuropsychological Approach
test results and global vocational potential, the
differing cognitive demands of specific occupa- Taken together, studies of academic achieve-
tions should be considered when basing predic- ment, independent living (ADLs and IADLs),
tions of vocational functioning on an individual’s and vocational functioning demonstrate that
pattern of neuropsychological strengths and def- neuropsychological test results generally relate
icits (Chelune & Moehle, 1986). The high level of to adequacy of everyday functioning and can be
cognitive demands of certain vocations, such as useful in the prediction of success or failure in
those experienced by corporate executives, attor- these important everyday activities. However,
neys, scientists, and physicians, are more likely use of neuropsychological data to predict func-
to result in significant changes in job perfor- tional outcomes is an iterative process resting
mance in response to cognitive decline than in on the assumption that demonstration of an
vocations with fewer cognitive demands. There impaired ability in the laboratory indicates diffi-
are challenges to conducting research that would culty or failure with functions that rely upon that
guide decisions regarding specific requirements ability in the natural environment (Heaton &
Neuropsychology in Relation to Everyday Functioning 639

Pendleton, 1981). Furthermore, research has that negative symptoms were related only to
still not fully delineated the cognitive abilities social problem solving and positive psychotic
that are necessary for success in specific every- symptoms were not related to any functional
day tasks (Farias et al., 2003), and the reported outcome measure, providing additional support
correlations between test scores and func- for the important independent role of cognitive
tional outcomes generally do not exceed 40% abilities in functional outcomes (Green, 1996).
of explained variance (see Goldstein, 1996). The The use of neuropsychological test results
relatively modest amount of variance accounted to predict everyday functioning has several
for by neuropsychological tests alone indicates limitations. Importantly, as stated above, the
that other variables, such as demographics, demands of home and work environments vary
health status, level of experience with the activ- widely, and therefore the types and degrees
ities in question, and psychiatric and environ- of impairment that will significantly impact
mental factors, may be highly important when performance of daily activities will also vary
predicting everyday functioning. with the situation. This is true of more general
Several trends have been revealed in the lit- aspects of functioning, such as ability to work
erature. Generally, the neuropsychological indi- or live independently, as well as functioning in
ces that are best for making global predictions, specific activities, since there is also variation
such as employment status, are those that are the in the demands of the same class of activities
most sensitive to brain dysfunction. These are (e.g., driving, financial management, or writ-
typically summary scores such as the Average ing tasks) for different people. Additionally, an
Impairment Rating from the HRB or the GDS individual who shows no impairment in rou-
from the expanded HRB and other test batter- tine aspects of everyday functioning may nev-
ies (e.g., Carey et al., 2004; Heaton et al., 2004). ertheless be unable to adequately respond to
Another trend that has previously been noted is less common, high-demand, or emergency-type
that complex cognitive tests can better predict situations (e.g., for airline pilots or physicians),
functioning in complex everyday lives than can which could put the individual or others at sig-
simpler tests of specific abilities (Goldstein, 1996; nificant risk. Such situations in the home or
McCue et al., 1990; Richardson et al., 1995). One on the job, though infrequent, may be critical
multifactorial ability domain that likely relates for the safety of the patient or others. It is also
to functioning across patient groups is executive important to consider that an individual’s previ-
functioning. The relationship between executive ous experience with a particular task or activity
functioning and everyday functioning has been may influence the degree to which an acquired
reported frequently in both patient and healthy cognitive deficit affects success in future task
populations, leading some authors to suggest performance. Compensation for impaired abil-
that this is an especially important area to focus ities or functioning may also be possible, but
future research on the predictors of function- this will vary depending on an individual’s
ing (Cahn-Weiner et al., 2003; Guilmette & strengths and environmental demands. In light
Kastner, 1996). Recent research has also indi- of these limitations, the relationship between
cated that learning and memory also have par- performance on specific laboratory measures
ticularly important roles in daily functioning of cognitive abilities and functioning in the
(Heaton et al., 2004; van Gorp et al., 2007). In natural environment still needs to be clarified
a review of studies investigating the neurocog- with further research, but neuropsychological
nitive deficits associated with functional dis- predictors may always rely heavily on clinical
ability in patients with schizophrenia, verbal judgment.
episodic memory was revealed to be associated
with all types of functional outcome studied
Direct Assessment of Everyday
(Green, 1996). Whether this relates specifically
Functioning: Performance-Based
to the verbal nature of the tests in these stud-
Measures
ies is unclear, because visual episodic memory
typically has not been assessed in the relevant As noted above, individual differences in
studies. Interestingly, the review also indicated cognitive deficits, preserved strengths, past
640 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

experiences, and specific environmental similar to the demands of the skills required
demands present significant challenges for the to perform that activity in the natural envi-
measurement and prediction of ability to per- ronment (Chaytor & Schmitter-Edgecombe,
form everyday tasks. Rather than abandoning 2003; Franzen & Wilhelm, 1996) and avoids or
standardized assessment, however, a poten- minimizes possible effects of factors other than
tial solution to this problem is to develop new patient ability (e.g., intereference or assistance
assessment instruments that provide bet- by other people in the natural environment).
ter insight into an individual’s functioning Performance-based tests have been developed
(Franzen & Wilhelm, 1996). Another impetus to measure what a person is capable of doing in
in the development of new assessment tools is a way that resembles the tasks that the person is
the above-mentioned question of the ecologi- required to complete in everyday life.
cal validity of neuropsychological tests. In the In comparison with standard neuropsycho-
context of neuropsychological assessment, eco- logical tests, performance-based tests of every-
logical validity generally refers to the degree to day activities provide a more direct assessment
which controlled laboratory assessments mir- of an individual’s functional capacity. Rather
ror requirements of everyday life. With regard than measuring cognitive abilities in the
to the neuropsychological approach to the abstract and generalizing from deficits in those
prediction of everyday functioning, ecological cognitive abilities to an estimation of how an
validity is particularly important because the individual would be able to perform on a given
purpose of the evaluation is to predict behav- everyday task, the individual demonstrates
iors that occur in the natural environment (i.e., what he or she is capable of doing by engaging
“extra-test behavior;” Franzen & Wilhelm, in an actual everyday task in the laboratory.
1996). As neuropsychologists are increasingly Performance-based everyday functioning tests
presented with referral questions that require attempt to objectively measure skills that are
predictions of everyday task performance, an required to function independently in the natu-
understanding of the empirical relationship ral environment, such as performing household
between existing neuropsychological tests and chores, cooking, and management of finances
measures of everyday functioning is important. and medications (Heaton et al., 2004; Patterson
This approach to ecological validity, known as et al., 2001). The greater face validity of these
veridicality, involves statistically relating neu- performance-based everyday functioning mea-
ropsychological test findings to various func- sures makes the results of the tests more readily
tional outcome measures, and is the method interpretable as predictors of functional out-
utilized in the research that has been summa- comes. Furthermore, the direct and objective
rized in this chapter thus far. In this approach, a assessment of functional abilities and the lack of
deficit in a particular cognitive ability or a sum- reliance on the individual’s or caregiver’s insight
mary score is first identified with neuropsycho- into the patient’s functioning are compelling
logical testing, and then is related to a selected advantages to the use of performance-based
functional outcome measure. Although this tests results as functional outcome measures.
process has demonstrated that existing tests do Scores on standardized versions of everyday
have some utility in the prediction of real-world tasks may also tend to have good psychometric
functioning, the limited variance accounted properties, which would enhance their ability to
for by the neuropsychological test data in func- detect changes in functional abilities related to
tional outcomes and the limited face validity of disease progression or treatment (Heaton et al.,
neuropsychological tests for the measurement 2004).
of everyday functioning have been addressed As is the case with neuropsychological pre-
by a newer line of research that takes a different dictors, there are potential limitations for
approach to ecological validity, known as verisi- drawing conclusions from performance-based
militude. With this approach, functional capac- measures. An important limitation is based
ity (i.e., ability to perform an everyday task) is on the distinction between what a person is
directly and objectively assessed. This improves capable of doing and what the person actually
the degree to which the demands of the test are does in the real world. What an individual is
Neuropsychology in Relation to Everyday Functioning 641

capable of doing is only one piece of the puzzle, the CCS subscales were significantly correlated
because numerous other factors influence what with Full-Scale IQ (r = .72, p < .01), Verbal IQ
the individual will actually do in everyday life, (r = .77, p < .01), and Performance IQ (r = .55,
including motivation and environment-specific p < .01). The pattern of correlations with WAIS-R
factors such as opportunity to carry out certain subtests was not interpreted, but stronger cor-
activities, finances, social support, secondary relations (moderately to highly correlated) were
gain, and the like. Additionally, the fact that shown between the CSS subscales and WAIS-R
functional capacity is being measured in a con- verbal subtests (Searight et al., 1989). The CCS
trived environment may raise questions about has also been shown to be moderately asso-
the validity of conclusions drawn from these ciated with neuropsychological functioning
assessments (Patterson et al., 2001). The fact as measured by the HRB (Dunn et al., 1990).
that performance-based tests are conducted in Also the Occupational Therapy Evaluation of
a controlled setting may result in an overesti- Performance and Support (OTEPS) is a standard-
mation of a individual’s real-world function- ized performance-based measure containing sev-
ing because of the lack of distractions in the eral functional domains (i.e., hygiene/self-care,
laboratory that are typically found in the real safety, medication administration, cooking/
world. Also, in his/her natural environment, nutrition, money management, and community
an individual may be forced to manage sev- access/utilization). In samples of older adults
eral activities simultaneously or for extended referred for neurological testing, OTEPS scores
periods of time, whereas the testing situation have been significantly related to performance
usually requires that the individual focus only on the Dementia Rating Scale (DRS; Mattis,
on a single task and does not last long enough 1988) (Nadler et al., 1993), and to visuospatial
to demonstrate the effects of fatigue. On the functioning (Richardson et al., 1995).
other hand, an individual’s ability to perform Loewenstein and colleagues (1989) devel-
in everyday life could be underestimated at oped a performance-based measure, the Direct
times because in the laboratory he/she is not Assessment of Functional Status (DAFS), for
able to use compensatory strategies that may the purpose of assessing functional abilities in
aid functional performance in the real world older adults with suspected functional impair-
(Franzen & Wilhelm, 1996). The verisimiltude ments (e.g., due to various dementing condi-
approach is not considered to be as rigorous as tions). As functional impairment is an essential
the veridicality approach because the similarity criterion for the diagnosis of dementia accord-
of a testing situation is not empirically com- ing to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual-IV
pared to the real-life situation it is intended to (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association,
represent (Chaytor & Schmitter-Edgecombe, 1994), a clear picture of functional deficits is
2003). Despite these limitations, performance- particularly important in this population. High
based measures have demonstrated promise in interrater and test–retest reliability have been
the prediction of everyday functioning. reported for the DAFS, as well as convergent and
Earlier work examined the relationships discriminant validity (reviewed in Loewenstein
between neuropsychological test results and et al., 2001). The DAFS consists of several tasks
performance-based measures that had previ- that simulate higher- and lower-order func-
ously been developed for specific populations tional activities, including reading a clock, pre-
or in other disciplines. For example, Searight paring a letter for mailing, identifying currency,
and colleagues (1989) investigated the rela- counting currency, writing a check, balancing a
tionship between performance on the WAIS-R checkbook, making change for a purchase, and
and results from a modified version of the eating skills. The time orientation task involves
Community Competence Scale (CCS; Loeb, reading a series of clock settings, and for the
1984) in older adults with suspected dementia. communication task the patient prepares a let-
The CCS combines structured interview with ter for mailing. Loewenstein and colleagues
performance-based measures, and was orig- (2001) compared caregiver reports of AD
inally designed to determine competence in patients’ functional abilities with the patients’
elderly individuals. Their study revealed that performance on the DAFS, demonstrating that
642 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

caregivers overestimated the abilities of those a shopping list for a pudding recipe given to the
who were functionally impaired on the DAFS. individual by the examiner, and then selecting
The results of another study of AD patients items necessary to prepare the pudding from an
indicated that performance-based tests, includ- array of items in a mock grocery store. Not all
ing the DAFS, were more valid and reliable than items needed to prepare the pudding are pro-
caregiver reports (Farias et al., 2003). vided in the mock store, and the individual is
The DAFS has also been validated for dem- asked to write down those items that he/she
onstrating functional consequences of cogni- would still need to purchase. Points are given
tive impairment in schizophrenic populations. for correct items written on the shopping list.
Findings have generally indicated that global For the Communication category, individuals
indices of cognitive functioning were the best demonstrate their ability to use a telephone in
predictors of functional capacity (e.g., Green, particular situations (e.g., in an emergency) and
1996; Harvey, et al., 1997; Klapow et al., 1997). respond to a medical appointment confirmation
For example, in one study the global DRS score letter. The Finances category involves counting
was the best predictor of performance on the change and paying bills. The Transportation
DAFS, even after controlling for potentially category evaluates the individual’s ability to
confounding demographic and illness variables determine a travel route to a predetermined
(Patterson et al., 1998). Also, in a study of older destination using public transportation, also
patients with schizophrenia, general cognitive accounting for the cost of the trip, transfers, and
functioning (based on a battery of tests) was the other information. The Planning Recreational
strongest predictor of DAFS performance, and Activities category involves role-playing two
specific cognitive domains were inconsistent in outings to geographically relevant locations,
predicting DAFS subscales (Evans et al., 2003). with a description of what he/she would take
Although these studies support the importance on the trip, the type of clothes that would be
of considering cognitive impairment in predict- worn on the trip, and how he/she would travel
ing functional performance, there are some to the destination. The points assigned for per-
limitations for the use of the DAFS in this pop- formance in each category are used to calculate
ulation, including ceiling effects on subscales five subscale scores, which are summed to cre-
that are more relevant in AD populations (e.g., ate a total score (range 0–100).
time orientation; see McKibbin et al., 2004). As predicted, Patterson and colleagues (2001)
Patterson and colleagues (2001) developed found that their sample of 50 older patients
a performance-based measure to be used spe- with schizophrenia and schizoaffective dis-
cifically in middle-aged to elderly, commu- orders performed worse on the UPSA than 20
nity-dwelling schizophrenic patients with demographically similar (in terms of gender
schizophrenia. The UCSD Performance-Based and education) normal controls. In examin-
Skills Assessment (UPSA) consists of tasks ing concurrent validity, the authors found that
that were designed to mimic situations that the UPSA scores significantly correlated with
older individuals living in the community fre- another performance-based measure (DAFS)
quently encounter. Based on information gath- but not with a self-report measure of well-being
ered from the literature and a variety of clinical that is frequently used in older schizophrenic
specialties (e.g., occupational therapy, social patients. The authors also reported the results
work), the test was designed to measure skills of a hierarchical multiple regression analysis,
in five categories, including Household Chores, which revealed that UPSA scores were statis-
Communication, Finances, Transportation, tically predicted by severity of global cogni-
and Planning Recreational Activities. For each tive impairment, as measured by Mini-Mental
skill category, individuals role-play activities Status Examination (MMSE; Folstein et al.,
that are described by the examiner, who assigns 1975) scores, and negative symptoms, but not
points for correctly completing various aspects by positive psychotic symptoms.
of the task. For example, the Household Chores In the discussion of their results, the authors
category focuses on cooking and shopping drew several conclusions regarding perfor-
skills. The five-minute task involves preparing mance-based measures, and specifically with
Neuropsychology in Relation to Everyday Functioning 643

regard to their test. They stressed that although schizophrenia and assessed the relationship
self-report can provide valuable information, between neuropsychological performance,
the data are not specific enough regarding func- functional capacity (as measured by the UPSA),
tioning in areas that determine whether a person and real-world functional skills performance
can live independently. Therefore, the authors (as measured by a caregiver report). Their
concluded that performance-based tests such results provide support for previous reports
as the UPSA have advantages over self-report as in the literature regarding the significant cor-
outcome measures, including the quality, spec- relations between neuropsychological perfor-
ificity, and reliability of the data obtained. Also, mance and real-world functional outcomes, but
they stated that their findings argue for use of demonstrated that when functional capacity is
the UPSA in schizophrenic and schizoaffective also considered, neuropsychological test scores
populations, because the test was tailored to the inconsistently provide additional predictive
specific everyday functional problems experi- power in determining real-world functional
enced by these patients. performance. The authors concluded that func-
Numerous studies have been conducted with tional capacity was the most consistent predictor
the UPSA as a measure of functional capacity. of real-world functioning, and also noted that
For example, Twamley and colleagues (2002) additional variance is accounted for by negative
investigated the relationship between cognitive and affective symptoms (e.g., depression; Bowie
functioning (measured with the DRS and a bat- et al., 2006).
tery of standardized neuropsychological tests) Heaton and colleagues (2004) noted the
and the UPSA in older outpatients with psy- absence of a sensitive, comprehensive, perfor-
chosis. They reported that all of the cognitive mance-based functional battery for use with
abilities examined were significantly related to other types of disorders, which present with rel-
UPSA total scores and, similarly, the UPSA sub- atively mild neuropsychological impairments.
scale scores were significantly related to most As in the case of the use of the DAFS in schizo-
cognitive variables. The lack of specific domain phrenic patients, if performance-based tests
relationships was interpreted as evidence for that were developed for use with more severely
the multifactorial nature of the UPSA tasks impaired individuals are used with patients
(Twamley et al., 2002). However, a more recent whose cognitive impairment is milder, inac-
study has examined the relationships among curate predictions about everyday functioning
neuropsychological performance and indices could result. The functional battery used in this
of two types of functional capacity in schizo- study included the Financial Skills and Shopping
phrenic patients: social skills were assessed measures from the DAFS, supplemented with
with the Social Skills Performance Assessment a newly developed Advanced Finances task
(SSPA; involving role plays of social problem that requires individuals to role-play paying
situations) and the everyday living skills were bills and managing a checkbook. The greatest
assessed with the UPSA (McClure et al., 2007). level of difficulty in this task involves asking
The two types of functional capacity measures the individual to pay as much on his/her bills
were associated with different cognitive abilities, as possible, while retaining a certain balance
such that social competence was associated with in the checkbook for emergencies. Medication
working memory, episodic memory, and verbal management ability was measured with a mod-
fluency performance, whereas everyday living ified version of a previously published test
skills were related to processing speed, episodic (Medication Management Test; MMT; Albert
memory, and executive functioning. However, et al., 1999). In a pill-dispensing portion of the
the authors reported that the findings provided test, individuals’ ability to dispense a day’s dos-
only modest evidence of specific relationships age and follow a scripted prescription regimen
between functional domains and specific cog- is assessed. Realistic pill bottles with standard-
nitive deficits (McClure et al., 2007). ized instructions are utilized in this component
To further elucidate the relationship of func- of the test. In another portion of this test, indi-
tional capacity to real-world functioning, Bowie viduals are instructed to transfer the correct
and colleagues (2006) studied outpatients with number of pills from the bottles to a medication
644 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

organizer that holds supply of medications for their study, can be used to complement neuro-
one week. Another portion of the test is the psychological testing for determining the pres-
“medication inference” component, in which ence of syndromic neurocognitive conditions
individuals respond to questions that can be in HIV-infected individuals. The authors noted,
answered using information provided on the however, that besides disease-related impair-
pill bottles and a medication insert. The mod- ments of functional capacity, there are other rea-
ified Medication Management Test (MMT-R) sons why individuals may fail IADLs in the real
only included those items from the original test world, namely, depression, substance use disor-
that have been found to be the most reliable and ders, and motivational factors. In this study, a
valid in the population being studied, which measure of depressed mood significantly con-
consisted of HIV-infected individuals. For the tributed to the prediction of both subjective and
Cooking task, examinees are required to follow objective measures of everyday functioning.
recipes and coordinate preparation of a meal. A Spanish-language version of the functional
Individuals are given three recipes, and the battery also has been developed at the same
recipes call for measuring, stirring, and wrap- research center, and ongoing research is eval-
ping. The most challenging aspect of this task uating the cultural relevance of the measures
is determining the order and timing of cooking and examining the relationship between neuro-
the items, which ensures that they are completed psychological test results and performance on
at the same time. The authors of this study also functional measures in HIV-infected Spanish
administered a comprehensive neuropsycholog- speakers (Mindt et al., 2003). Like English
ical battery, a self-report measure of functioning speakers in the Heaton et al. (2004) study, neu-
outside of the laboratory (PAOFI; Chelune et al., ropsychologically impaired Spanish speakers
1986), and a modified version of the Lawton performed significantly worse on the functional
and Brody (1969) IADL scale, which has been measures than did the neuropsychologically
described above. The authors also collected normal Spanish speakers (Mindt et al., 2003).
objective information regarding vocational Also, performance on both the neuropsycholog-
functioning, including a multimodal, criterion- ical battery and the functional assessment were
referenced, standardized battery of vocational related to indicators of everyday functioning,
tasks that provided information regarding 13 including employment status. Mindt and her
job abilities as defined by the US Department colleagues (2003) concluded that the Spanish
of Labor. All of the various functional measures version of the functional assessment, with only
were summarized as a functional deficit score minor modifications from the original version,
(FDS), in order to reflect overall impairment is a valid assessment tool for evaluating every-
on the functional battery. Certain cognitive day functioning in monolingual Spanish speak-
domains were found to account for most of the ers in the U.S.
unique variance in predictions of IADL failures Given that there are numerous tasks encom-
and failures on the functional battery, namely, passing global everyday functioning, it may
learning, abstraction/executive function, and be useful to measure functional capacity spe-
attention/working memory. When included cifically for tasks that are particularly relevant
in a model alone, neuropsychological test per- for the individual given his/her environmental
formance predicted employment status, but it needs and safety, or for answering the refer-
was no longer a significant predictor in multi- ral question at hand. Targeted measurements
variate prediction models with functional mea- of functional capacity could also be useful
sures included. The FDS was a unique predictor for directly examining level of functioning in
of each type of outcome measuring everyday an area likely to be at risk given the nature of
functioning, the PAOFI, IADL dependence, impairment. Examples of specific functional
and employment status, even when neuropsy- evaluations include employment, medication
chological testing and HIV disease stage were management, and driving.
included in the models. Heaton and colleagues Objective, criterion-referenced instruments
(2004) suggested that performance-based mea- that evaluate an individual’s skill level in impor-
sures, such as the functional battery used in tant areas related to vocational functioning
Neuropsychology in Relation to Everyday Functioning 645

include the MESA SF2 and COMPASS Programs delay (1 hour between presentation of the pre-
(Valpar International Corporation, 1986, scription regimen and testing), and shorter
1992). These programs comprise computer- duration of the test. Importantly, the scoring
ized subtests (Vocabulary, Reading, Spelling, procedures indicate over- and undermedication
Mathematics, Language Development (Editing), (Patterson et al., 2002). The authors reported
Problem Solving, Short-term Visual Memory, that older adults with schizophrenia performed
Shape Discrimination, Size Discrimination, significantly worse on the MMAA than a nor-
and Placing and Tracking) and noncomputer- mal comparison group, demonstrating with
ized mechanical tests (Alignment and Driving, an objective measure that the patient group
Machine Tending, Wiring). Raw scores cor- had greater difficulty managing medications.
respond to ability levels, which are referenced Interestingly, the most common error made by
to the US Department of Labor Dictionary of the schizophrenic patients was taking fewer pills
Occupational Titles (DOT; US Department of than prescribed. This finding illustrates how
Labor, 1991). The jobs included in the DOT are tests of functional capacity may also aid in the
assigned profiles that indicate specific levels of development of intervention strategies for med-
abilities needed to perform the job. For the MESA ication management, as the process is observed
SF2 and COMPASS programs, each item relates firsthand.
to an ability level, and the number of errors com- Automobile driving is another impor-
mitted by an individual within an ability level tant real-world activity that has been assessed
determines the highest level achieved. The out- with standardized, performance-based tests.
come scores from these programs correspond In addition to studies of driving abilities in
to the number and types of jobs the examinee is AD (Herrmann et al., 2006; Hunt et al., 1993;
considered capable of performing. Perryman & Fitten, 1994), driving ability has
Medication management is an important also been examined in the context of Parkinson’s
everyday function for numerous types of patient disease (Grace et al., 2005) and HIV infection
populations. Successful management of medica- (Marcotte et al., 1999, 2006). Recently, a study
tions is particularly challenging and relevant for of HIV-infected individuals and uninfected
HIV-infected persons, due to the complicated controls assessed performance on neuropsy-
regimens and serious consequences of nonad- chological tests and the Useful Field of View
herence. Hinkin and colleagues (2002) exam- (UFOV), a performance-based, computerized
ined the influence of cognitive impairment on test of visual attention (Marcotte et al., 2006).
adherence to HIV medication regimens, finding Poor performance on the UFOV was related to
that deficits in executive functioning, attention, higher accident rates in the past year, and the
and memory were associated with poor adher- highest-risk individuals were neuropsycho-
ence, even when accounting for age and his- logically impaired, and had poor UFOV per-
tory or psychiatric and neurological disorders. formance. The findings indicate that general
However, no performance-based test such as the cognitive status and visual attention specifi-
MMT was included in this study, and addition cally are important factors in determining risk
of such a measure to neuropsychological testing for impaired driving (Marcotte et al., 2006).
may provide more powerful predictions about Performance-based measures of driving, such
the individual’s ability (capacity) to adhere. For as driving simulators, provide information that
example, the Medication Management Ability might not be captured on a self-reported driv-
Assessment (MMAA; Patterson et al., 2002) ing history, such as how an individual would
is a modification of the MMT (Albert et al., respond to unusual, high-risk situations (e.g.,
1999) that was designed to simulate a medica- when another driver cuts in front of his/her
tion regimen similar to one that would likely be vehicle unexpectedly).
prescribed to an older individual. The new task
involves a role play and differs from the MMT
Conclusions
with regard to the types of actions required of
the participant (e.g., handing the pills to the Estimation of everyday functioning is an
tester at a certain time), incorporation of a important role of neuropsychologists today.
646 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

Such predictions have considerable implica- pattern of strengths as well as weaknesses. This
tions for the livelihood, lifestyle, and safety information can be important and useful in
of the individuals under evaluation and their assisting the individual to compensate for his/
families. As the field of neuropsychology has her cognitive deficits and could therefore be
increasingly focused on estimation of real- used in targeting cognitive rehabilitation and
world functional abilities, neuropsychologists interventions.
have utilized traditional measures of cognitive Although use of demographically-corrected
abilities that reveal the individual’s pattern of norms in neuropsychological test interpreta-
cognitive strengths and weaknesses. With the tion is important for determining the nature
neuropsychological approach to the predic- and severity of acquired impairments (due to
tion of everyday functioning, an assumption disease or injury), the clinician should care-
is made that if an individual demonstrates an fully consider whether corrected or uncorrected
impairment of an ability in the laboratory, that scores are best for predicting performance in
individual will be at risk for functional impair- particular everyday tasks and activities. This is
ment in real-world tasks that draw upon that because uncorrected scores (e.g., scaled scores
ability. Considerable evidence has supported in the normative system of Heaton & Taylor,
that assumption by showing the association of 2004) reflect absolute level of abilities more
neuropsychological test results with a variety of directly, compared to the general adult popu-
functional outcomes. lation. Thus, if the everyday activity to be pre-
In addition to the wealth of reported findings, dicted is one that almost all adults in the normal
there are several advantages for using standard population can perform, uncorrected scores
neuropsychological tests to make predictive would be preferable. Even then, however, the
statements regarding everyday functioning. clinician may first want to establish the likeli-
Importantly, many neuropsychological tests hood that a loss of ability has occurred (using
have demonstrated good psychometric prop- corrected scores) and then consider whether
erties. In addition to favorable reliability and any such loss is severe enough to interfere in
validity, normative standards have been devel- relevant everyday tasks. On the other hand, if
oped for many of these tests. The use of norma- the everyday task is sufficiently demanding that
tive standards in interpretation of test results only a small proportion of the adult popula-
involves comparing an individual’s test score tion would be expected to perform it adequately
to a predicted score that would be expected for (e.g., pilot, physician, corporate executive), at
a neurocognitively normal person with simi- least education-adjusted norms are likely to be
lar demographic characteristics (e.g., age, years helpful. For example, if a physician has suffered
of education, ethnicity, and sex). This process a head injury or is suspected of having early
reduces the likelihood that misclassification dementia, his/her test performances probably
of neurocognitive impairment will occur due should be compared to normal expectations
to demographic factors, and can help to eluci- for a person with a high education level (e.g., 20
date whether or not a cognitive deficit is due years for a doctorate). Again, considerable clin-
to acquired brain dysfunction. In addition, ical judgment is required in this interpretation
test–retest reliability has been examined for process.
most neuropsychological tests. When evaluat- Given the abundance of valuable information
ing an individual over time in order to deter- that is provided by a comprehensive evaluation,
mine whether his/her cognitive performance administration of a full battery is recommended
is improving or declining (or remaining sta- whenever a prediction of everyday functioning
ble), it is important that the clinician under- is to be made. As such, a standard neuropsy-
stands what a meaningful discrepancy in scores chological battery could be administered, such
from one testing session to the next would be. as the widely used expanded HRB. The neuro-
Accordingly, norms for change are available for psychologist can also select additional specific
many neuropsychological tests. Notably, a com- tests that tap those abilities that are most rele-
prehensive neuropsychological battery provides vant to the referral question to supplement the
extensive information regarding the individual’s traditional battery.
Neuropsychology in Relation to Everyday Functioning 647

Although performance-based tests of every- testing and performance-based measures. For


day functional abilities are more face valid, and example, Heaton and colleagues (2004) dem-
significant associations have been demonstrated onstrated in an HIV-infected sample that if an
between functional capacity (as measured by individual performed in the normal range on
performance-based functional tests) and neu- both a neuropsychological battery and a FDS
ropsychological tests, functional capacity tests but was still not working, depressive symptoms
should not be administered in isolation. That is, could be interfering with employment. In addi-
we suggest they can best be used to supplement, tion to depression, other noncognitive factors
rather than replace, a comprehensive neuro- that can affect everyday functioning include
psychological examination. Results of per- substance use disorders, motivation, and envi-
formance-based functional tests may provide ronment-specific factors.
additional information about what real-world In sum, the neuropsychological approach to
activities an individual has difficulty perform- the prediction of everyday functioning provides
ing, but they do not provide information about valuable information about individuals’ ability
how the functional impairment relates to a to carry out a variety of everyday tasks in the
brain disorder. Further, performance-based real world. The system of estimating everyday
functional tests do not provide sufficient insight functioning on the basis of neuropsychological
into the individual’s preserved strengths, which test performance is well-supported by evidence
is important information for compensation and in the literature. This approach can be aug-
targeting intervention in cognitive rehabilita- mented by more recently developed measures
tion settings. of functional capacity, and these performance-
In addition to the benefits of having the based tests can help to elucidate the nature of an
wealth of information that a comprehensive individual’s functional disability.
neuropsychological evaluation provides, this
approach is also quite efficient in predicting
a range of everyday activities. Given that the
References
number of potential everyday tasks of interest
is vast, there would not be time to develop and Albert, S. M., Weber, C. M., & Todak, G. (1999). An
administer functional assessments of all such observed performance test of medication manage-
activities. However, performance-based tests ment ability in HIV: Relation to neuropsycholog-
do provide a window into how an individual’s ical status and medication adherence outcomes.
AIDS and Behavior, 3, 121–128.
cognitive deficits may manifest in specific real-
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic
world situations of interest. This could be espe- and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.).
cially useful for particularly important everyday Washington, DC: Author.
activities that have implication for health and Awad, A. G., Voruganti, L. N., & Heslegrave, R. J.
safety, such as medication management and (1997). A conceptual model of quality of life in
driving. Additionally, results of performance- schizophrenia: Description and preliminary
based tests could be useful for evaluating the clinical validation. Quality of Life Research: An
efficacy of cognitive rehabilitation in a more International Journal of Quality of Life Aspects of
objective manner. As such, performance-based Treatment, Care and Rehabilitation, 6, 21–26.
tests can be used to supplement a neuropsycho- Bell-McGinty, S., Podell, K., Franzen, M., Baird,
logical battery as needed to address the referral A. D., & Williams, M. J. (2002). Standard mea-
sures of executive function in predicting instru-
question at hand. A review of performance-
mental activities of daily living in older adults.
based measures by Moore et al. (2007) provided International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 17,
recommendations for selecting performance- 828–834.
based measures according to population and Bowie, C. R., Reichenberg, A., Patterson, T. L., Heaton,
specific domain of interest. R. K., & Harvey, P. D. (2006). Determinants of
Consideration of noncognitive factors real-world functional performance in schizophre-
appears to be especially important when real- nia subjects: Correlations with cognition, func-
world functioning is worse than expected on tional capacity, and symptoms. American Journal
the basis of the results of neuropsychological of Psychiatry, 163, 418–425.
648 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

Boyle, P. A., Malloy, P. F., & Salloway, S. (2003). Alzheimer’s disease: Comparison of family infor-
Executive dysfunction and apathy predict func- mants’ ratings and performance-based assess-
tional impairment in Alzheimer disease. American ment findings. International Psychogeriatrics, 11,
Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 11, 214–221. 399–409.
Boyle, P. A., Paul, R. H., Moser, D. J., & Cohen, R. Drachman, D. A., O’Donnell, B. F., Lew, R. A., &
A. (2004). Executive impairments predict func- Swearer, J. M. (1990). The prognosis in Alzheimer’s
tional declines in vascular dementia. Clinical disease: “How far” rather than “how fast” best
Neuropsychology, 18, 75–82. predicts the course. Archives of Neurology, 47,
Cahn-Weiner, D. A., Malloy, P. F., Boyle, P. A., 851–856.
Marran, M., & Salloway, S. (2000). Prediction of Dunn, E. J., Searight, H. R., Grisso, T., Margolis,
functional status from neuropsychological tests in R. B., & Gibbons, J. L. (1990). The relation of the
community-dwelling elderly individuals. Clinical Halstead--Reitan Neuropsychological Battery to
Neuropsychology, 14, 187–195. functional daily living skills in geriatric patients.
Cahn-Weiner, D. A., Ready, R. E., & Malloy, P. F. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 5, 103–117.
(2003). Neuropsychological predictors of every- Evans, J. D., Heaton, R. K., Paulsen, J. S., Palmer, B.
day memory and everyday functioning in patients W., Patterson, T., & Jeste, D. V. (2003). The rela-
with mild Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Geriatric tionship of neuropsychological abilities to spe-
Psychiatry and Neurology, 16(2), 84–89. cific domains of functional capacity in older
Carey, C. L, Woods, S. P., Gonzalez, R., Conover, E., schizophrenia patients. Biological Psychiatry, 53,
Marcotte, T. D., Grant, I., et al. (2004). Predictive 422–430.
validity of global deficit scores in detecting Farias, S. T., Harrell, E., Neumann, C., & Houtz,
neuropsychological impairment in HIV infec- A. (2003). The relationship between neuropsy-
tion. Journal of Clinical and Experimental chological performance and daily functioning in
Neuropsychology, 26, 307–319. individuals with Alzheimer’s disease: Ecological
Chaytor, N., & Schmitter-Edgecombe, M. (2003). The validity of neuropsychological tests. Archives of
ecological validity of neuropsychological tests: A Clinical Neuropsychology, 18, 655–672.
review of the literature on everyday cognitive Fishman, J. A., & Pasanella, A. K. (1960). College
skills. Neuropsychology Review, 13, 181–197. admission-selection studies. Reviews of Education
Chelune, G., Heaton, R., & Lehman, R. (1986). Research, 30, 298–310.
Neuropsychological and personality correlates Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R.
of patient’s complaints of disability. In R. Tarter (1975). Mini-mental state: A practical method for
and G. Goldstein (Eds.), Advances in Clinical grading the cognitive state of patients for the clini-
Neuropsychology (pp. 95–126). New York: New cian. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189–198.
York Plenum Press. Franzen, M. D., & Wilhelm, K. L. (1996). Conceptual
Chelune, G. J., & Moehle, K. A. (1986). foundations of ecological validity in neuropsy-
Neuropsychological assessment and everyday chological assessment. In R. J. Sbordone and C.
functioning. In D. Wedding, A. MacNeill Horton, J. Long (Eds.), Ecological validity of neuropsycho-
and J. Webster (Eds.), The Neuropsychology logical testing (pp. 91–112). Boca Raton, FL: St.
Handbook: Behavioral and Clinical Perspectives Lucie.
(pp. 489–525). New York: Springer. Goldstein, G. (1996). Functional considerations in
Cysique, L. A., Deutsch, R., Atkinson, J. H., Young, neuropsychology In R. J. Sbordone and C. J. Long
C., Marcotte, T. D., Dawson, L., et al. (2007). (Eds.), Ecological validity of neuropsychological
Incident major depression does not affect neuro- testing (pp. 75–89). Delray Beach, FL: GR Press/St
psychological functioning in HIV-infected men. Lucie Press.
Journal of the International Neuropsychological Grace, J., Amick, M. M., D’ Abreu, A., Festa, E.
Society, 12, 1–11. K., Heindel, W. C., & Ott, B. R. (2005). Neuro-
Davis, B. A., Martin-Cook, K., Hynan, L. S., & Weiner, psychological deficits associated with driving per-
M. F. (2006). Caregivers’ perceptions of demen- formance in Parkinson’s and Alzheimer’s disease.
tia patients’ functional ability. American Journal Journal of the International Neuropsychological
of Alzheimer’s Disease and Other Dementias, 21, Society, 11, 766–775.
85–91. Green, M. F. (1996). What are the functional conse-
Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B. A. quences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophre-
(1987). California verbal learning test. San Antonio, nia? American Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 321–330.
TX: The Psychological Corporation. Guilmette, T. J., & Kastner, M. P. (1996). The predic-
Doble, S. E., Fisk, J. D., & Rockwood, K. (1999). tion of vocational functioning from neuropsycho-
Assessing the ADL functioning of persons with logical data. In R. J. Sbordone and C. J. Long (Eds.),
Neuropsychology in Relation to Everyday Functioning 649

Ecological validity of neuropsychological tests (pp. Jefferson, A. L., Cahn-Weiner, D., Boyle, P., Paul,
387–411). Florida: GR Press/St. Lucie Press. R. H., Moser, D. J., Gordon, N., et al. (2006).
Harvey, P. D., Davidson, M., Mueser, K. T., Parrella, Cognitive predictors of functional decline in vas-
M., White, L., & Powick, P. (1997). Social-Adaptive cular dementia. International Journal of Geriatric
Functioning Evaluation (SAFE): A rating scale Psychiatry, 21, 752–754.
for geriatric psychiatric patients. Schizophrenia Klapow, J. C., Evans, J., Patterson, T. L., Heaton, R.
Bulletin, 23(1), 131–145. K., Koch, W. L., & Jeste, D. V. (1997). Direct assess-
Harvey, P. D., Moriarity, P. J., Kleinman, L., Coyne, ment of functional status in older patients with
K., Sadowsky, C. H., Chen, M., et al. (2005). The schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry,
validation of a caregiver assessment of dementia: 154, 1022–1024.
The Dementia Severity Scale. Alzheimer Disease Kliegel, M., & Zimprich, D. (2005). Predictors of
and Associated Disorders, 19, 186–194. cognitive complaints in older adults: A mixture
Hathaway, S. R., & McKinley, J. C. (1951). Minnesota regression approach. European Journal of Ageing,
multiphasic personality inventory; man- 2, 13–23.
ual (Revised). San Antonio, TX:Psychological Lavin, D. E. (1965). The prediction of academic per-
Corporation. formance. Oxford, England: Russel Sage Found.
Heaton, R. K., Chelune, G. J., & Lehman, R. A. W. Lawton, M. P., & Brody, E. M. (1969). Assessment
(1978). Using neuropsychological and personality of older people: Self-maintaining and instru-
tests to assess the likelihood of patient employ- mental activities of daily living. Gerontologist, 9,
ment. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 166, 179–186.
408–416. Lehmann, J. F., DeLateur, B. J., Fowler, R. S. Jr.,
Heaton, R. K., Marcotte, T. D., Mindt, M. R., Sadek, Warren, C. G., Arnhold, R., Schertzer, G., et al.
J., Moore, D. J., & Bentley, H. (2004). The impact (1975). Stroke rehabilitation: Outcome and
of HIV-associated neuropsychological impair- prediction. Archives of Physical and Medical
ment on everyday functioning. Journal of the Rehabilitation, 56, 383–389.
International Neuropsychological Society, 10, Loeb, P. A. (1984). Validity of the Community
317–331. Competence Scale with the elderly. Dissertation
Heaton, R. K., Paulsen, J. S., McAdams, L. A., Abstracts International, 45, 1919.
Kuck, J., Zisook, S., Braff, D., et al. (1994). Loewenstein, D. A., Amigo, E., & Duara, R. (1989).
Neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenics: A new scale for the assessment of functional sta-
Relationship to age, chronicity, and dementia. tus in Alzheimer’s disease and related disorders.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 469–476. Journals of Gerontology, 4, 114–121.
Heaton, R. K., & Pendleton, M. G. (1981). Use of Loewenstein, D. A., Arguelles, S., Bravo, M.,
neuropsychological tests to predict adult patients’ Freeman, R. Q., Arguelles, T., & Acevedo, A.
everyday functioning. Journal of Consulting and (2001). Caregivers’ judgments of the functional
Clinical Psychology, 49, 807–821. abilities of the Alzheimer’s disease patient: A com-
Heaton, R. K., & Taylor, M. (2004). Revised compre- parison of proxy reports and objective measures.
hensive norms for an Expanded Halstead-Reitan Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 2,
Battery: Demographically adjusted neurpsycho- P78–P84.
logical norms for African American and Caucasian Long, C. J., & Collins, L. F. (1997). Ecological validity
adults. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment and forensic neuropsychological assessment. In R.
Resources, Inc. J. McCaff rey, A. D. Williams, J. M. Fisher, and L.
Herrmann, N., Rapoport, M. J., Sambrook, R., C. Laing (Eds.), The practice of forensic neuropsy-
Hébert, R., McCracken, P., Robillard, A.,et al. chology: Meeting challenges in the courtroom (pp.
(2006). Predictors of driving cessation in mild-to- 153–164). New York: Plenum Press.
moderate dementia. Canadian Medical Association Marcotte, T. D., Heaton, R. K., Wolfson, T., Taylor, M.
Journal, 175, 591–595. J., Alhassoon, O., & Arfaa, K. (1999). The impact
Hinkin, C. H., Castellon, S. A., Durvasula, R. S., of HIV-related neuropsychological dysfunction
Hardy, D. J., Lam, M. N., Mason, K. I., et al. (2002). on driving behavior. Journal of the International
Medication adherence among HIV + adults: Neuropsychological Society, 5, 579–592.
Effects of cognitive dysfunction and regimen com- Marcotte, T. D., Lazzaretto, D., Cobb, J. S., Roberts,
plexity. Neurology, 59, 1944–1950. E., Woods, S. P., & Letendre, S. (2006). Visual
Hunt, L., Morris, J. C., Edwards, D., & Wilson, B. attention deficits are associated with driving
(1993). Driving performance in persons with mild accidents in cognitively-impaired HIV-infected
senile dementia of the Alzheimer type. Journal of individuals. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
the American Geriatrics Society, 41, 747–753. Neuropsychology, 28, 13–28.
650 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

Matarazzo, J. D. (1972). Wechsler’s measurement and disease. In J. L. Fitten (Ed.), Facts and research
appraisal of adult intelligence (5th ed). Oxford, in gerontology: Dementia and cognitive impair
England: Williams and Wilkins. ments (pp. 91–102). New York: Springer
Mattis, S. (1988). Dementia rating scale professional Publishing.
manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Pierson, S. H., & Griffith, N. (2006). Treatment
Resources. of cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis.
McClure, M. M., Bowie, C. R., Patterson, T. L., Behavioral Neurology, 17, 53–67.
Heaton, R. K., Weaver, C., Anderson, H., et al. Psychological Corporation. (1997). WAIS-III and
(2007). Correlations of functional capacity and WMS-III technical manual. San Antonio, TX:
neuropsychological performance in older patients Author.
with schizophrenia: Evidence for specificity of rela- Rao, N., & Kilgore, K. M. (1992). Predicting return
tionships? Schizophrenia Research, 89, 330–338. to work in traumatic brain injury using assess-
McCue, M., Rogers, J. C., & Goldstein, G. (1990). ment scales. Archives of Physical Medicine and
Relationships between neuropsychological and Rehabilitation, 73, 911–916.
functional assessment in elderly neuropsychiatric Rao, S. M., Leo, G. J., Ellington, L., & Nauertz, T.
patients. Rehabilitation Psychology, 35, 91–99. (1991). Cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclero-
McKibbin, C. L., Brekke, J. S., Sires, D., Jeste, D. V., & sis: II. Impact on employment and social function-
Patterson, T. L. (2004). Direct assessment of func- ing. Neurology, 41, 692–696.
tional abilities: Relevance to persons with schizo- Richardson, E. D., Nadler, J. D., & Malloy, P. F.
phrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 72, 53–67. (1995). Neuropsychologic prediction of perfor-
Mindt, M. R., Cherner, M., Marcotte, T. D., Moore, mance measures on daily living skills in geriatric
D. J., Bentley, H., Esquivel, M. M., et al. (2003). The patients. Neuropsychology, 9, 565–572.
functional impact of HIV-associated neuropsycho- Rioch, M. J., & Lubin, A. (1959). Prognosis of social
logical impairment in Spanish-speaking adults: adjustment for mental hospital patients under psy-
a pilot study. Journal of Clinical Experimental chotherapy. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 23,
Neuropsychology, 25, 1, 122–132. 313–318.
Moore, D. J., Palmer, B. W., Patterson, T. L., & Jeste, Rourke, S. B., Halman, M. H., & Bassel, C. (1999).
D. V. (2007). A review of performance-based Neurocognitive complaints in HIV infection
measures of functional living skills. Journal of and their relationship to depressive symptoms
Psychiatric Research, 41, 97–118. and neuropsychological functioning. Journal of
Nadler, J. D., Richardson, E. D., Malloy, P. F., Marran, Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21,
M. E., & Brinson, M. E. H. (1993). The ability of the 737–756.
Dementia Rating Scale to predict everyday func- Searight, H. R., Dunn, E. J., Grisso, J. T., & Margolis,
tioning. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 8, R. B. (1989). Relation of cognitive functioning
449–460. to daily living skills in a geriatric population.
Newnan, O. S., Heaton, R. K., & Lehman, R. A. W. Psychological Reports, 64, 399–404.
(1978). Neuropsychological and MMPI correlates Tulsky, D. S., & Price, L. R. (2003). The joint WAIS-
of patients’ future employment characteristics. III and WMS-III factor structure: Development
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 46, 635–642. and cross-validation of a six-factor model of cog-
Patterson, T. L., Goldman, S., McKibbin, C. L., Hughs, nitive functioning. Psychological Assessment, 15,
T., & Jeste, D. V. (2001). UCSD performance based 149–162.
skills assessment: Development of a new measure Tupper, D., & Cicerone, K. (1990). Introduction to
of everyday functioning for severely mentally ill the neuropsychology of everyday life. In D. Tupper
adults. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 27, 235–245. and K. Cicerone (Eds.), The neuropsychology of
Patterson, T. L., Klapow, J. C., Eastham, J., Heaton, R. everyday life: Assessment and basic competencies
K., Evans, J. D., Koch, W. L., et al. (1998). Correlates (pp. 3–18). Boston: Kluwer Academic.
of functional status in older patients with schizo- Twamley, E. W., Doshi, R. R., Nayak, G. V., Palmer,
phrenia. Psychiatry Research, 80, 41–52. B. W., Golshan, S., Heaton, R. K., et al. (2002).
Patterson, T. L., Lacro, J., & McKibbin, C. L. (2002). Generalized cognitive impairments, ability to per-
Medication management ability assessment: form everyday tasks, and level of independence in
Results from a performance-based measure in community living situations of older patients with
older outpatients with schizophrenia. Journal of psychosis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159,
Clinical Psychopharmacology, 22, 11–19. 2013–2020.
Perryman, K. M., & Fitten, L. J. (1994). Impact of U.S. Department of Labor. (1991). Dictionary of
attentional deficits on driving performance of occupational titles (4th ed). Washington, DC: U.S.
the elderly and individuals with mild Alzheimer’s Government Printing Office.
Neuropsychology in Relation to Everyday Functioning 651

Valpar International Corporation. (1986). Micro- Williams, B. (1994). Patient satisfaction: A valid con-
computer Evaluation and Screening Assessment cept. Social Science and Medicine, 38, 509–516.
(MESA) Short Form 2. Tuscon, AZ: Author. Woods, S. P., Morgan, E. E., Dawson, M., Scott, J.
Valpar International Corporation. (1992). C., Grant, I., & the HNRC Group. (2006). Action
Computerized Assessment (COMPASS). Tucson, (verb) fluency predicts dependence in instrumen-
AZ: Author. tal activities of daily living in persons infected
Van Gorp, W. G., Rabkin, J. G., Ferrando, S. J., with HIV-1. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Mintz, J., Ryan, E., Borkowski, T., et al. (2007). Neuropsychology, 28, 1030–1042.
Neuropsychiatric predictors of return to work
in HIV/AIDS. Journal of the International
Neuropsychological Society, 13, 80–89.
27

Neuropsychological Performance and the


Assessment of Driving Behavior
Thomas D. Marcotte and J. Cobb Scott

There continues to be growing interest in under- weather, faulty equipment). A significant body
standing the relationship between neuropsy- of research has focused on the relationship
chological functioning and the ability to carry between driver behaviors and safety errors in
out everyday activities. One such activity, driv- healthy individuals, as well as interventions
ing an automobile, is an important, complex, at both the individual and system (e.g., road
and potentially dangerous task that is not only design, air bags) levels for injury and crash pre-
associated with mobility but also often with an vention, helping to clarify some of the compo-
individual’s sense of identity and quality of life nents that give rise to unsafe (and safe) driving.
(Fonda et al., 2001; Owsley, 1997). Automobile At the same time that progress is being made
driving is the primary form of transportation in in various aspects of driver safety, new tech-
many areas, due in part to the paucity of pub- nologies abound that compete with the driver’s
lic transportation infrastructure (Collia et al., focus of attention. Although cell phones are the
2003). However, the prominence of driving most ubiquitous, personal music players (e.g.,
comes at a cost, as an average of 6 million auto- mp3 players, iPods) and global positioning sys-
mobile crashes occur every year in the United tems (for navigation), among other infotain-
States, with approximately 40,000 of these fatal ment devices, are increasingly finding their way
and 2 million resulting in injuries (National into the driver’s activities. And, unfortunately,
Highway Traffic Safety Administration, 2006). as developers improve designs so that users
Worldwide, the World Health Organization require less time to interact with a device (e.g.,
estimates that 1.2 million persons are killed fewer menu levels), overall risk might increase as
in driving crashes each year, with 50 million users become more inclined to use the product
injured. Injuries due to traffic crashes are pre- while driving—known as the usability paradox
dicted to be third among factors contributing to (Lee & Strayer, 2004).
the global disease and injury burden by 2020, While such factors are essential to consider
just behind ischemic heart disease and unipolar in discussions of driving safety with cognitively
major depression (World Health Organization, normal individuals, impairments in cognitive
2004). and motor functioning can drastically increase
While many driving crashes are thought the probability of safety errors and risky deci-
to be preventable, discerning how to prevent sions during driving. A variety of neurologi-
crashes presents a complex challenge. Myriad cal (e.g., Alzheimer’s disease, multiple sclerosis
factors can lead to automobile crashes, includ- [MS], HIV), medical (e.g., arthritis), and psy-
ing inattention, risky decisions or maneuvers, chiatric disorders (e.g., antisocial personality
slowed reactions, poor hazard perception, as disorder), as well as use of licit (e.g., alcohol,
well as external factors (e.g., other drivers, benzodiazepines) and illicit (e.g., marijuana)

652
Neuropsychological Performance and the Assessment of Driving Behavior 653

drugs, can influence driving capacity. Thus, a significant need for continued development of
recent investigations have attempted to deter- valid, and practical, methods of identifying at-
mine the most effective assessment methods for risk individuals.
detecting driving deficits and to delineate the Despite the responsibilities frequently placed
driving impairments that might be expected in upon them, clinicians rarely receive formal
various clinical populations. training in methods for determining driver
Although not reviewed in detail here, infor- safety and may be unaware of the potential
mation regarding the specific cognitive abili- problems that certain disorders may present for
ties affected by certain diseases and disorders driving (Valcour et al., 2002). This chapter will
may also lead to important interventions to therefore focus on the literature regarding the
improve driving behavior. In acknowledgment relationship between neurocognitive function-
of the dearth of transportation alternatives ing and driving abilities, particularly in neuro-
that are available in many countries, there has logic disorders. In order to put this relationship
been a strong emphasis recently on the devel- in context, the first section of the chapter will
opment of remediation methods in order to present a general model of driving behavior that
maximize that period over which individu- addresses the possible levels at which driving
als can drive safely (National Highway Traffic abilities could break down, as well as a sampling
Safety Administration, 1999; Stephens et al., of the various factors that might contribute to
2005; Wang & Carr, 2004). Such methods may impaired driving. The following section will
include driver remediation (e.g., retraining), review clinic-based performance measures,
vehicle modifications, technological advances such as neuropsychological tests, that are often
(e.g., impact warning systems), or changes in used to predict driving performance in clinical
roadway design (Dickerson et al., 2007). Recent populations. In order to facilitate a critical read-
studies have shown driver remediation efforts ing of the literature, we will discuss approaches
(Ball et al., 2007; Marottoli et al., 2007; Mazer to driving assessments and their relative assets
et al., 2003) and vehicle modifications (Kramer and weaknesses. And lastly we will briefly
et al., 2007; Ponsford et al., 2008) to be bene- review some common neuropsychiatric disor-
ficial for certain populations, including those ders and their impact on driving performance.
affected by normal aging and individuals recov-
ering from traumatic brain injuries (TBIs) or
A General Model of Driving
cerebrovascular accidents.
Behavior
Given the potential risks to patients and oth-
ers, it would be understandable to have a bias Driving is one of the more complex activities
toward overidentifying individuals as likely to of daily living, requiring intact attention, per-
be impaired on the road. Yet a number of stud- ception, tracking, choice reactions, sequen-
ies of neurologic patients have found that a diag- tial movements, judgment, and planning. It is
nosis alone is not necessarily sufficient cause somewhat remarkable, then, that most individ-
for a person to lose his or her driver’s license uals relatively quickly achieve driving compe-
(Dubinsky et al., 2000; Molnar et al., 2006). For tence (Ogden & Moskowitz, 2004) and are able
example, Hunt et al. (1993) found that some to maneuver a one-ton vehicle at high speeds
individuals with mild Alzheimer’s disease con- down 10-foot lanes while surrounded by vehi-
tinued to have adequate driving skills. Similarly, cles moving at different velocities/directions,
in our research examining HIV infection, only unpredictable pedestrians, and countless other
a subset of those persons with HIV-associated hazards.
cognitive impairment demonstrated a signifi- A number of models of driving behavior have
cant decline in driving performance (Marcotte been proposed over the years (Ranney, 1994),
et al., 2004b). Since the loss of one’s license can emphasizing factors ranging from motivation
hamper independent living and driving ces- (e.g., risk avoidance, Fuller, 1984) to informa-
sation has been associated with an increase in tion processing (e.g., automaticity, Shiff rin &
depressive symptoms in older drivers (Fonda Schneider, 1977). For this chapter, in order to
et al., 2001; Marottoli et al., 1997), there remains provide a broad overview of the factors that
654 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

might affect driving performance, some of these place—minutes in the case of the strategic level,
more focused models have been combined into a seconds for the tactical level, and milliseconds
broad model of driving behavior (Figure 27–1). at the operational level. Decisions made at the
It is acknowledged that this is a much simpli- higher levels can affect the work load at lower
fied model and that driving is a dynamic and levels (e.g., the strategic decision to drive dur-
complicated process. For example, consider the ing rush hour can impact the workload at the
behaviors/abilities involved in simply changing tactical and operational level), and capacities
traffic lanes. One must determine when it is safe at the lower levels may inform decisions made
to glance away from the roadway, track speed at higher levels (slowed reaction time may lead
and steering while looking at the side mirror individuals to drive more slowly). Levels overlap
and out the driver’s window, press the turn during the driving task. For example, maneu-
signal, determine the speed and positioning of vering at the tactical level involves simultaneous
any vehicles adjacent to the driver, accelerate actions at the operational level.
if needed, initiate fine movement of the steer- To the left of the Michon model in Figure 27–1,
ing wheel, be ready to respond if a car is in the we list a number of factors that can affect driv-
blind spot, insure that lead cars do not decel- ing behavior. Individuals start with certain pre-
erate during the maneuver, and complete the morbid cognitive abilities that may affect driving
lane change. In addition to the many cognitive performance. Some of these are listed in Table
factors involved in such a simple lane change 27–1 later in this chapter. Most people have suf-
(judgment, perception, attentional shifts, pro- ficient cognitive capacity to successfully accom-
cessing speed, visual scanning, fine motor plish the tasks that human factors investigators
skills, reaction time, etc.), other variables could consider essential for safe driving. These include
affect the success of the lane change, such as having adequate situation awareness (“the per-
physical limitations, environmental influences ception of elements in the environment within a
(e.g., weather, traffic flow), and vehicle design. volume of time and space, the comprehension of
However, since the focus of this chapter is pri- their meaning, and the projection of their status
marily on factors internal to the individual, we in the near future”; see Endsley, 1995), sufficient
do not address these latter issues here. hazard perception (recognizing and predict-
On the right side of Figure 27–1 we show ing potentially hazardous situations, including
the hierarchical control structure model pro- assessing the magnitude of the hazard), and the
posed by Michon (1985). This is one of the ability to make critical driving decisions, such
more commonly referenced models in the driv- as gap acceptance (a driver’s determination as
ing literature, and includes strategic, tactical to whether a gap between cars for a turn is safe
(maneuvering), and operational levels. At the or unsafe, based upon traffic flow, vehicle accel-
strategic level, decisions are made regarding eration capacity, etc.). Most of these factors, of
planning for the drive. Such decisions include course, involve risk assessment and decision
assessing weather conditions, determining making.
whether the individual feels competent to go Other factors, including personality (e.g., sen-
on the road, choosing a route, deciding whether sation seeking, which can also lead to behav-
to take breaks while driving, and anticipating iors that put individuals at risk for CNS insult)
the steps needed to complete the drive. At the and driving experience, have been shown to be
tactical level, individuals make decisions about strong predictors of driving behaviors and can
maneuvering the vehicle, including speed, fol- influence or even supersede other factors in
lowing distance, lane choice, whether to turn determining driving performance. Unsafe (or
on the headlights, as well as whether one should safe) driving behavior may be influenced by an
be involved in secondary tasks, such as cell individual’s motivation for the behavior at the
phone use. At the operational level, the driver time, such as speeding to avoid being late to an
directly maneuvers the vehicle, using the steer- appointment, opting to eat while on the road,
ing wheel, pedals, and so on. One distinguish- or driving unsafely based on other, overriding
ing aspect of the different levels is the amount needs. In addition, familiarity with a route may
of time pressure under which each level takes affect one’s driving safety, often in unexpected
Metacognition
CNS insult
Automobile driving
Strategic
Psychoactive Neuropsychological Route selection, deciding to
Personality go on the road
substances impairment

Tactical
Speed/lane selection, passing,
Premorbid Cognitive Abilities Compensatory strategies
early hazard detection,
secondary tasks
Motivation Visual functioning

Experience Familiarity with route Physical conditions Operational


Steering control, breaking,
accelerating

Figure 27–1. General model of driving behavior (incorporating the model from Michon, 1985).
655
656 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

ways, as increased cognitive and attentional is associated with risky driving behaviors such
demands are placed on the driver attempting to as moving traffic violations, driving under the
navigate unfamiliar routes. Medically, problems influence, and crashes (Hilakivi et al., 1989;
with visual function (i.e., impaired vision) and Paaver et al., 2006). Sensation seeking, defined
physical conditions (e.g., neuropathy, spasticity) as enjoying and seeking out exciting, novel,
can also impact driving ability. and risky activities (Zuckerman et al., 1972), is
Within this larger context, specific neuropsy- moderately correlated with impulsivity (Hur &
chological abilities that are needed to perceive Bouchard, 1997), and has been shown to predict
and attend to conditions, interpret the situa- risky driving behaviors such as speeding, driv-
tion, make judgments, implement the planned ing under the influence, and reckless driving
actions, and monitor feedback (Rizzo & (Dahlen et al., 2005). Sensation seeking driv-
Kellison, in press) might be altered by numerous ers may have higher thresholds for accepted
conditions (CNS insult), such as normal aging, risk or overestimate their driving skills in
medical disease, and neurologic disorders, relation to the perceived level of risk (Jonah,
which may reach the level of neuropsychological 1997). Importantly, both sensation seeking and
impairment. Cognition can also be affected by impulsivity have strong associations with driv-
use of psychoactive substances. CNS insult and ing outcomes even when accounting for other
prolonged use of psychoactive substances may cognitive and health-related factors (Arthur
effect changes in personality, such as increased et al., 1991), although this relationship may be
impulsivity. Adaptation to these conditions can affected by age (Schwebel et al., 2007) or gender
be affected by an individual’s metacognition, or (Oltedal & Rundmo, 2006). Anger and hostil-
self-awareness of one’s own cognitive and driv- ity are also constructs that have been associated
ing abilities. If individuals are aware of their with risky driving (Deffenbacher et al., 2001;
deficits in driving skills, they may utilize com- Schwebel et al., 2006).
pensatory strategies (e.g., only driving in certain
situations) in an attempt to reduce their driving
Experience
risk. However, neurocognitive disorders such as
Alzheimer’s disease can also reduce awareness It has been hypothesized that novice drivers
of deficits, potentially leading to underestimates operate primarily via controlled processing,
of driving risk. Below, we briefly discuss these characterized by slow, effortful processing of
factors and their relationship to different driv- information, while experienced drivers use
ing behaviors. quick, effortless (i.e., automatic) processes to
guide behavior (Summala, 1996), and presum-
ably have more resources to dedicate to atten-
Factors Impacting Driving
tional needs. Novice drivers have elevated crash
Performance
rates compared to more experienced drivers
(Crundall et al., 2003), even when accounting for
Personality
age differences (Mayhew et al., 2003). It has been
Personality characteristics have consistently suggested that experienced drivers may develop
been associated with risky driving in younger a larger perceptual field of view (Crundall et al.,
and middle-aged adults (Arthur et al., 1991; 1999), a quicker response to hazardous stim-
Dahlen et al., 2005; Tillmann & Hobbs, 1949), uli (Wallis & Horswill, 2007), enhanced situa-
as well as older samples (Owsley et al., 2003; tional awareness (Gugerty, 1997), or improved
Schwebel et al., 2007; cf. Strahan et al., 1997). ability to more fully scan the field of vision
These personality characteristics are found in (Underwood, 2007). As noted by van Zomeren
nonneurologic disorders, but may be accen- et al. (1987), the efficient driver is not the one
tuated with cerebral dysfunction, especially who reacts quickly to a hazardous situation, but
involving the frontal lobes, and by certain the one who avoids it in the first place. Extensive
drugs of abuse, such as methamphetamine experience with driving could also potentially
(Semple et al., 2005; Simons et al., 2005). A create a “reserve,” in which the experienced
number of studies have shown that impulsivity driver could suffer greater brain dysfunction
Neuropsychological Performance and the Assessment of Driving Behavior 657

yet remain a reasonably safe driver for a longer purely passive responders (Ranney, 1994), and
period of time. This possibility requires empir- one should always keep in mind that drivers are
ical confirmation in direct studies of driving often establishing their priorities, such as speed-
but is indirectly supported by research pointing ing to get to a movie on time, deciding to inter-
to preserved cognitive function in dementia in act with friends in the car rather than focus on
individuals with high levels of cognitive reserve the driving task, or driving in poor weather to
(e.g., Stern, 2006). get badly needed medications at the pharmacy.

Familiarity Visual Functioning


With experience, driving behavior in famil- Safe automobile driving requires effective pro-
iar situations may become more automated. It cessing of a multitude of sensory and percep-
is assumed that individuals drive more safely tual information. Although adequate auditory
on familiar routes, since the drivers do not use and somatosensory functioning is important
resources to orient themselves to their surround- to driving, it may be possible to compensate for
ings. However, the automaticity of experienced such deficits. The processing of visual cues, how-
drivers may bring its own hazards. Drivers may ever, is critical as it provides essential informa-
be more likely to be involved in fatal crashes on tion about movement detection, environmental
roads on which they frequently travel on (Blatt structure, depth perception, optic flow, and the
& Furman, 1998) and may take more risks and time before collision with other objects. Despite
thus make more driving errors in familiar ver- the obvious importance of vision in driving
sus unfamiliar locations (Rosenbloom et al., behavior, variable success has been achieved
2007). Experienced drivers may also not respond in using traditional visual tests in predicting
appropriately in potentially risky situations in driving performance. Although widely used,
familiar locales, especially those with ambigu- tests of visual acuity and visual field loss have
ous feedback (Duncan et al., 1991). Therefore, been weakly associated with driving behavior,
further research is needed to examine the except in cases of ocular injury, such as cata-
potentially complex relationship between route racts, glaucoma, macular degeneration, or ret-
or setting familiarity and driving safety, espe- inal disorders (Owsley & McGwin, 1999; Szlyk
cially in individuals with questionable driving et al., 1995), or with hemi- or quandrantano-
skills. psia that may occur with strokes, trauma, or
tumors (Rizzo & Kellison, 2004). Contrast sen-
sitivity deficits, on the other hand, have shown
Motivation
stronger associations and deserve further study
Crashes may occasionally come down to the (Marottoli et al., 1998; Owsley & McGwin,
individual’s motivation at the time. Driving 1999). Some important aspects of visual cogni-
models focusing on motivation begin with the tion that may affect driving performance, such
notion that “driving is self-paced and drivers as motion and object perception, have not been
select the amount of risk they are willing to tol- studied extensively and are rarely measured in
erate in any given situation” (Ranney, 1994). One standard clinical visual assessments (Rizzo &
interesting hypothesis in the Risk Homeostasis Kellison, 2004).
model (Wilde, 1982) is that in order to main-
tain a constant level of risk, drivers will adjust
Physical Conditions
for improvements in safety by driving less
cautiously (e.g., some individuals drive faster Numerous physical conditions, such as hemipa-
because of the safety provided by airbags). Some resis from stroke (Ponsford et al., 2008), spastic-
models have been criticized for their primary ity associated with MS (Marcotte et al., 2008),
focus on risk, and it is pointed out that motiva- and peripheral neuropathy, among many others,
tion is multifactorial. Regardless, the emphasis can affect driving performance, although there
on motivation is of interest since it emphasizes are studies suggesting that in many cases in
that drivers are active decision makers, and not which the individual is able to operate a vehicle,
658 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

cognitive functioning remains central to the attention, and reduced judgment, among others.
evaluation of driving safety (Marshall et al., Although “driving under the influence” is typ-
2005). A variety of adaptations have been useful ically based upon blood alcohol concentrations
in improving driving performance in individu- (BACs) of .05 to 1.0 (50–100 mg/dL), reduced
als with physical limitations (Haslegrave, 1991; performance is seen at even lower blood alco-
Lawton et al., 2008). hol levels (Moskowitz & Robinson, 1988), and
there is no clear threshold wherein one transi-
tions from unimpaired to impaired (Ogden &
Psychoactive Substances
Moskowitz, 2004). Epidemiologically, crash
Any substance that alters cognition, licit or rates increase as legally accepted BAC levels
illicit, can impact driving performance. A thor- increase (Fell & Voas, 2006). There is minimal
ough review of the effects of such substances on information regarding the impact of alcohol on
neuropsychological functioning and driving is driving abilities in neurologic patients, although
beyond the scope of this chapter. However, we it is likely that alcohol’s effects are exacerbated in
will briefly note that prescription medications these individuals. Given the extensive research
for neuropsychiatric conditions can negatively on alcohol and driving, performance at differ-
affect driving. For many medications, the great- ent BAC levels has been used as a benchmark
est impact on driving is during the early stages for comparing the effect of other substances on
of use, until tolerance develops. Although some driving (Ramaekers, 2003). Although this offers
medications reduce driving ability, it is possible a useful comparison regarding the relevant effect
that some individuals are better drivers when sizes of these substances, readers should keep in
compared to their untreated condition, although mind the outcomes being assessed. For exam-
this has rarely been examined. Benzodiazepines ple, while the inability to maintain lane position
can affect visual perception, processing speed, is an important (and measurable) complication
coordination, reaction time, memory and atten- of alcohol use, other factors (e.g., impaired judg-
tion (Kelly et al., 2004). Epidemiologic studies ment) are also likely critical in determining an
suggest increased crash risks are associated intoxicated individual’s crash risk.
with benzodiazepine use (Thomas, 1998), and Marijuana, or Cannabis sativa, can acutely
on-road and simulator studies indicate that result in reduced learning, attention, process-
benzodiazepines reduce steering skills, deci- ing speed, and psychomotor abilities, depend-
sion making, braking, and reaction time (Bocca ing upon the dose. Driving studies involving
et al., 1999; Moskowitz & Smiley, 1982; van Laar cannabis suggest delayed reactions, poor lane
et al., 2001; Verster et al., 2002). Of the anti- tracking (Ramaekers et al., 2004), and poor
depressants, tricyclic antidepressants have the judgment of speed and distance. Peak psycho-
greatest effects on driving abilities (Rapoport & active effects occur approximately 10–20 min-
Banina, 2007), again primarily during the first utes after smoking, with acute impairment
1–2 weeks of treatment (Ramaekers, 2003), of driving usually subsiding after 3–4 hours
while the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (Grotenhermen et al., 2007). Low doses of can-
(SSRIs) appear to be less impairing (Ramaekers, nabis result in moderate driving impairment,
2003; Wingen et al., 2005). There is no research but this is dramatically increased when alcohol
regarding the direct effects of antipsychotics on is also consumed (Ramaekers et al., 2004). Since
driving performance, although studies of driv- delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC), the pri-
ing-related psychomotor and cognitive abilities mary psychoactive component of cannabis, can
suggest that the atypical neuroleptics may affect be detected for days or weeks after intake, there
driving performance less than conventional has been no clear relationship between urine
neuroleptics (Kagerer et al., 2003). or blood levels and cognition. With respect to
Nonprescription substances also significantly driving laws there is no per se biological limit
impact driving skills. Alcohol, of course, is for the allowable concentration of THC in blood
the most prevalent drug used recreationally. or urine, although preliminary efforts are under
Alcohol affects driving in the areas of steering way (Grotenhermen et al., 2007). Interestingly,
control, maintenance of lane position, divided in contrast to alcohol, wherein drivers may have
Neuropsychological Performance and the Assessment of Driving Behavior 659

a false sense of self-confidence, subjects ingest- on driving performance, although sedation and
ing cannabis tend to drive more slowly than impairment are the most common side effects
when not under the influence, most likely due of these substances. Long-term maintenance
to their awareness of impairment and increased with methadone does not appear to be associ-
cautiousness (Grotenhermen 2007; Hindrik ated with increased crash risk, after the initial
et al., 1999). This may not be effective when treatment (Lenne et al., 2003).
under time pressure and confronted with unan-
ticipated hazards. Although controlled studies
Neuropsychological Functioning
suggest driving impairments with cannabis,
the relationship between real-world on-road Driving requires the coordination of a broad
driving performance and cannabis use remains range of cognitive and motor skills, as drivers
equivocal. must constantly (1) perceive and attend to stim-
Small doses of stimulants can improve per- uli; (2) interpret the situation; (3) decide what,
formance on some cognitive tasks, particularly if any, action is needed based on prior experi-
in overcoming fatigue on psychomotor tasks, ence and perceptual cues; (4) formulate a plan of
but it is clear that elevated or long-term doses of action in coordination with working memory of
drugs such as cocaine and methamphetamine the situational factors involved; (5) execute the
can result in significant cognitive impairments plan by coordinating muscle movements; (6)
(Jovanovski et al., 2005; Scott et al., 2007). process feedback from sensory and perceptual
Withdrawal may also result in neuropsychiat- input; and (7) determine whether further action
ric impairments. However, there is little direct is needed. Usually, these actions are done under
research regarding stimulant abuse and driving time pressure. One can surmise that a number
ability. Field reports of individuals under the of neuropsychological processes are involved
influence of stimulants note speeding, poor lane in this sequence of actions. However, the pre-
maintenance, agitation, and risk taking (Logan, cise contribution of various neurocognitive
1996). In general, low doses of these substances processes to driving behavior remains murky.
might improve performance in persons who are In part, this is due to the fact that the study of
fatigued, but they do not seem to enhance per- such processes is often dependent on research
formance in normal individuals, nor improve examining the association of cognitive deficits
functioning on complex tasks (e.g., requiring with driving behavior, as these correlations are
divided attention) such as driving. Of note, most generally weak in unimpaired individuals. In
laboratory studies have used low doses (e.g., of addition, driving is an overlearned repertoire
cocaine), and it is anticipated that at higher of behaviors in which processes often operate
doses chronic use and withdrawal would result in tandem without much directive action, and
in impaired performance on driving tasks. drivers may be able to overcome some deficits
Methamphetamine, in particular, may be asso- with additional attentional resources or effort-
ciated with increased risk taking and impulsiv- ful processing. Individuals with intact higher-
ity (Gonzalez et al., 2007; Semple et al., 2005). level abilities to direct and organize (i.e., exec-
MDMA (3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine; utive functioning) may be able to overcome
Ecstasy) is a popular party drug that may affect deficits in lower-level processes (i.e., psychomo-
attention, perception and memory (Cami et al., tor slowing). Lastly, driving is a dynamic and
2000). One simulator study found modest integrative process, and significant inference
effects of MDMA on vehicle control and greater must take place to project performance from
risk taking (Brookhuis et al., 2003). An on-road single, focused measures administered within
study found that certain aspects of driving were structured, quiet clinic settings to the rich, and
improved (e.g., maintenance of lane position) occasionally random, environment of the real
with MDMA. whereas others were negatively world.
impacted (e.g., overshooting a lead car’s speed Partially due to the complexity of these fac-
decelerations) (Ramaekers et al., 2006). tors, investigations of the relationship between
There is very little research examining the neuropsychological tests and driving have
effects of opiates such as heroin and methadone historically met with mixed results, despite a
660 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

history of using such tests to predict driving Thus, no consensus yet exists regarding
competence. Early studies examined the pre- which neuropsychological measures best iden-
diction of driving competence in populations tify high-risk drivers. This might occur because
with brain injury or neurologic disease and neuropsychological tests primarily assess
had inconsistent results, often dependent upon impairment at the strategic or tactical level of
the particular clinical population studied, with driving (Schanke & Sundet, 2000). More likely,
predictor (i.e., tests) and criterion variables (i.e., it seems that a broad consensus on the relation-
driving tests or driving history) varying widely ship is difficult to attain, in part due to varying
by study (Galski et al., 1990; O’Neill et al., 1992; participant populations, divergent test batteries
Sivak et al., 1981; van Zomeren et al., 1987). In and sample sizes, and different gold standards
recent years, researchers have utilized more regarding what constitutes driving impair-
standardized road tests and multimodal assess- ment (Molnar et al., 2006; Reger et al., 2004;
ment approaches, such as combinations of Withaar et al., 2000). Nonetheless, the gen-
on-road, simulator, medical, and neuropsycho- eral conclusions are that cognitively impaired
logical evaluations. In some cases, such efforts individuals as a group perform significantly
have led to increased reliability and validity worse than controls on driving measures, and
of criterion variables, although results remain increased levels of cognitive impairment are
variable. found in groups involved in traffic crashes
Neuropsychological tests have been predic- (Withaar et al., 2000). The weakness of corre-
tive of on-road (Fitten et al., 1995; Hunt et al., lations between cognitive test results and on-
1993; Odenheimer et al., 1994) and driving road driving performance makes it difficult to
simulator performance (Marcotte et al., 1999; determine from neuropsychological tests alone
Rebok et al., 1995; Rizzo et al., 1997; Szlyk et al., whether cognitively impaired subjects are safe
2002) in several studies, while others have found or unsafe drivers. But since no single approach
poor relationships between the two (Bieliauskas to evaluating driving safety can possibly assess
et al., 1998; Fox et al., 1997). Even in the con- all abilities involved in such a complex activity,
text of dementia, associations between neuro- neuropsychological tests remain a reasonable
psychological tests and driving outcomes are modality for evaluating aspects of a patient’s
inconsistent, despite using tests hypothesized fitness to drive, as part of a multidisciplinary
to be particularly sensitive to driving-related assessment.
skills, such as Trails B or the Clock-Drawing Table 27–1 presents a summary of hypoth-
Test (Molnar et al., 2006; Reger et al., 2004). esized relationships between general neuro-
Moreover, the practical utility of most studies psychological domains and driving behaviors,
is limited, as few studies report cutoff scores for as structured according to Michon’s model
impairment (Molnar et al., 2006). (1985). Although deficits in most domains could

Table 27–1. Neurocognitive Abilities Hypothesized To Be


Involved at Different Levels of the Driving Task
Strategic Tactical Operational
Attention ƒ λ λ
Visuospatial abilities ƒ λ λ
Executive functioning ƒ λ λ
Working memory λ
Learning/memory ƒ λ
Procedural memory/ λ λ
knowledge
Processing speed λ λ
Psychomotor speed λ λ
Language λ
Motor λ
Neuropsychological Performance and the Assessment of Driving Behavior 661

potentially impact driving at each level, we have with an unobtrusive network of sensors and
identified the domains that we suspect are most video cameras (Neale et al., 2005). Data were
important at that level. In the following sections recorded from 241 drivers over the course of 1
we briefly review the findings regarding these year, who drove a total of over 2 million miles.
domains and driving performance. Despite obtaining drivers with diverse driving
As in any evaluation, clinicians should always histories, 82 crashes (ranging from injury acci-
attend to the examinee’s level of alertness, since dents to minimal property damage) and 761
reduced levels, due to fatigue, sleep disorders, near-crashes were recorded during the study
and use of prescription and illicit substances, period. The cause of 78% of the crashes and
among other causes, can significantly impact 65% of the near-crashes was characterized upon
both driving and neuropsychological test review as “driver inattention,” which included
performance. secondary task engagement, inattention to the
forward roadway, eye-glances away from the
roadway, and lapses of attention due to fatigue.
Attention
Many of the distractions drawing the attention
Attention is often cited as a critical neurocog- of the drivers from the critical aspects of the
nitive domain in driving. Not only is intact driving tasks were related to operating wire-
attention a precondition for almost every neu- less devices and interacting with passengers.
rocognitive process (Lezak, 2004), but lapses in (Which, of course, also draws into question
attention are one of the most cited reasons for executive functioning [e.g., judgment] in this
crashes in a number of epidemiological reports, subset of normal individuals.)
ranging from 15% to 40% of all accidents (e.g., Although lapses of attention are common in
Stutts et al., 2001). Attention is a multidimen- cognitively normal drivers, a number of stud-
sional construct, and a number of attentional ies have also shown that impairments in atten-
processes are essential for safe driving. Selective tion have significant implications for driving
attention is needed to be able to filter out the safety across diverse populations (e.g., Galski
irrelevant details in a driving environment and et al., 1997; Owsley et al., 1998; Ponsford et al.,
focus on more important elements. The ability 2008; Reger et al., 2004). While basic atten-
to divide one’s attention effectively is also crucial tion (e.g., attention span) has shown weak pre-
in order to deal with complex situations, such as dictive value, complex attentional processes
driving with distraction from passengers (Rizzo (e.g., divided attention), especially under sig-
& Kellison, in press), and some consider the nificant time pressure, have been observed to
inability to divide attention while under time have strong relationships with driving out-
pressure to be one key factor in impaired driv- comes in neurologic samples (Brouwer, 2002;
ing (Brouwer et al., 2002). Sustained attention Lengenfelder et al., 2002; Uc et al., 2006c).
is needed to continually direct one’s cognitive Sustained attention may also have particu-
resources to the pertinent aspects of the driving lar relevance for disorders with lapses of con-
environment and to ensure that the individual sciousness (e.g., seizure disorders) and those
is prepared to react with any unexpected occur- that result in excessive fatigue (Brouwer, 2002).
rences (Brouwer, 2002), since in many instances Parasuraman and Nestor (1991) have proposed
visual search in driving is bottom-up—drivers that measures of attention may be most useful in
waiting to notice a conspicuous target (Cole & predicting driving safety in aging populations,
Hughes, 1990; Ranney, 1994). Drivers may also as older populations are more likely to have
schedule “attentional checks,” in which they attentional deficits, and the driving safety of
increase attentional resources at key moments younger drivers is more likely to be affected by
(e.g., when deviating from a familiar route) use of substances, personality factors, or risky
(Ranney, 1994). decision making. More specifically, Duchek
The U.S. National Highway Traffic Safety and colleagues (1997; Duchek et al., 1998) have
Administration funded a novel observational argued that selective visual attention may be a
study, the “100-Car Naturalistic Driving Study,” particularly critical skill to assess in dementia
in which 100 cars in Virginia were instrumented patients.
662 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

The Useful Field of View (UFOV; Ball & tasks (e.g., Rey–Osterrith Complex Figure) are
Roenker, 1998; Ball et al., 1988) is a sensitive indi- commonly used and have shown modest results,
cator of driving impairment. UFOV is a comput- depending on the disorder under study (Amick
erized measure that assesses both divided and et al., 2007; Grace et al., 2005; Lundberg et al.,
selective attention by measuring the amount of 1998; Schanke & Sundet, 2000), as have visuo-
time it takes an individual to accurately acquire perceptual measures such as the Benton Revised
both central and peripheral visual information Retention Test (Hunt et al., 1993) and Benton
without head or eye movements. These abilities Visual Form Discrimination Test (Galski et al.,
are evaluated over extremely brief and focused 1992). Road sign identification may be an ecolog-
time points, and therefore not a typical para- ically valid test of visuospatial abilities (Lincoln
digm wherein one needs to attend to multiple & Radford, 2008; Uc et al., 2005). Clock drawing
tasks over a period of time. In aging groups, poor (with modified scoring) has also been proposed
UFOV performance has been correlated with as a sensitive test for driving outcomes (Freund
higher rates of past (Ball et al., 1993) and future et al., 2005), although this awaits further study.
automobile crashes (Owsley et al., 1998) and has
been associated with poor performance during
Executive Functioning
on-road driving evaluations (Duchek et al., 1998;
Myers et al., 2000). UFOV is also significantly Executive functions have been extensively dis-
reduced in many patient populations, includ- cussed, though not necessarily evaluated, as
ing persons with TBI (Fisk et al., 2002a), MS being important to driving ability, as they com-
(Schultheis et al., 2001), stroke (Fisk et al., 2002b; prise abilities theoretically essential for driving,
Mazer et al., 2003), HIV (Marcotte et al., 2006), including multitasking, planning, organization,
and mild Alzheimer’s disease (Duchek et al., decision making, mental flexibility, problem
1998). UFOV is commercially available, easy to solving, and judgment. For example, prudent
administer on a PC, and has been shown to pro- decision making is constantly required regard-
vide incremental validity to standard laboratory ing safe distances (e.g., “gap acceptance”) and
tests, and thus may offer diagnostic utility to cli- potential hazards, or, simply, whether condi-
nicians and researchers (Marcotte et al., 2006). tions are acceptable to drive. In addition, mental
As with all neuropsychological measures flexibility is critical for the executive control of
administered within the clinician’s office, a attentional set-shifting, as when drivers glance
potential confound is that brain-injured individ- between the rearview mirror and the roadway.
uals may be highly motivated and able to muster Although executive functions are subserved by
cognitive resources, and perform better within distributed neural networks, research generally
the structured office environment yet have dif- supports their strong reliance upon the frontal
ficulties over prolonged time periods and in cortex and frontostriatal systems (e.g., Stuss &
the unstructured driving environment (British Alexander, 2007). As such, they can be affected
Psychological Society, 2001; Lezak, 1995). by a variety of neurologic and psychiatric disor-
ders, as well as substance use.
Executive functions have been predictive of
Visuospatial Abilities
driving skills in cognitively normal (e.g., Avolio
Driving depends not only on intact vision but et al., 1985) and neurological populations (e.g.,
also the ability to interpret and integrate the Daigneault et al., 2002; Marcotte et al., 2004b;
information received. Most neuropsychological van Zomeren et al., 1987; Whelihan et al., 2005).
research shows these abilities to be one of the A generally consistent finding is that switching
most robust predictors of driving outcomes. In trials of psychomotor sequencing tasks (e.g.,
fact, visuoperceptual/constructional tasks were Trails B) show moderate associations with driv-
shown to have the strongest correlations with ing outcomes (Ball et al., 2006; Grace et al., 2005;
driving ability among all neuropsychological cf. Molnar et al., 2006; Szlyk et al., 2002). Other
domains in a meta-analysis of dementia (Reger tasks of problem solving and set-shifting have
et al., 2004). Visuoconstructional tasks such as shown less impressive findings. However, get-
the WAIS-III Block Design and Figure Copy ting lost, an integration of visuoperception with
Neuropsychological Performance and the Assessment of Driving Behavior 663

executive functioning, is often presumed to be impairment (Hunt et al., 1993; Odenheimer


an early sign of trouble with driving (Marottoli, et al., 1994; Rizzo et al., 1997). However, the
1997), although few studies examine this hypoth- associations in normal aging and other disorders
esis. Studies using maze navigation tasks or city (e.g., TBI, Tamietto et al., 2006) have been more
navigation simulations have shown some prom- modest in comparison to other neurocognitive
ise in predicting fitness to drive (Marcotte et al., domains, such as attention (Anstey et al., 2005;
2004b; Ott et al., 2003, 2008a), but as of now most McKnight & McKnight, 1999). Although epi-
tasks have limited psychometric information. sodic memory (i.e., memory for specific events
Executive functioning deficits can potentially and information) may not appear to be the most
affect driving performance over very short time face valid neuropsychological ability for driving,
periods. For example, disinhibition may result it involves the integration of numerous cogni-
in generalized risky driving, but may also lead tive processes that may themselves be involved
to difficulties in overriding an initiated action in driving (British Psychological Society, 2001).
(e.g., stopping at a red light once the individual Alternatively, given that memory tests are some
has already started the process of going through of the more sensitive measures to overall cog-
the yellow light). nitive impairment, the relationship between
Despite these findings, executive functions memory abilities and driving skills may be
have not been examined as extensively as some partially due to memory serving as a proxy for
other neurocognitive domains in the prediction general level of cognitive impairment.
of driving ability. Many components of execu-
tive functioning are difficult to measure in the
Procedural Memory/Knowledge
clinic, but it is an area warranting further study
(Daigneault et al., 2002). Many aspects of the driving task are overlearned
and, with time, implemented without conscious
attention. As such, driving a car is often cited as
Working Memory
an exemplar of procedural memory, or a type of
In order to drive safely, individuals need to “nondeclarative memory for skills that are not
briefly store information in working memory verbalized or consciously inspected” (Loring,
about their surroundings and the current task 1999). Procedural learning and memory is most
for future use. Although much of driving may closely linked to functioning of the basal gan-
be routine, working memory becomes espe- glia or striatum (Squire, 1993). Previous studies
cially important in complex, dynamic situations have demonstrated relatively preserved proce-
(Guerrier et al., 1999). Despite this clear rele- dural learning in select disorders (Alzheimer’s
vance to driving ability, few investigations have disease, Korsakoff ’s syndrome), with impair-
examined the contributions of working mem- ments found in disorders of frontostriatal
ory to driving outcomes, although some studies systems (Parkinson’s disease, Huntington’s
have found predictive value with multifactorial disease). However, few studies have examined
tasks that assess working memory among other procedural memory and driving. One relevant
functions (e.g., Trails B, Grace et al., 2005). study of two individuals with amnesia, but gen-
Working memory deficits may also contribute erally preserved cognition in other domains,
to change blindness, or the inability to detect found intact driving abilities, consistent with
noticeable changes in a visual scene with a brief their performance on two procedural learning
visual disruption (e.g., blinks, saccades), poten- tasks (Anderson et al., 2007), supporting the
tially influencing the ability to detect important notion that for some driving tasks procedural
variations in traffic (Rizzo & Kellison, 2004). knowledge might be a more important compo-
nent than declarative memory.
Learning/Memory
Processing Speed
There is evidence that learning and memory
performance are associated with driving, espe- Swift processing of information is also theoret-
cially in individuals with more severe cognitive ically critical for driving, although few studies
664 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

have specifically investigated the contribution may predominantly involve more gross motor
of this neurocognitive ability to driving behav- skills. In addition, many driving-related motor
ior. For example, drivers must not only react skills become overlearned with experience,
to stimuli in the roadway but also effectively and may be more robust to declines in abili-
process road signs and the pattern of traffic ties. The mixed nature of fi ndings, even in
around them. Speed of processing may be an diseases with significant psychomotor impair-
especially critical skill in diseases in which bra- ments (e.g., Parkinson’s disease), reflects these
dyphrenia is a primary symptom, or in disor- complications. Nonetheless, it may be benefi-
ders that affect the integrity of white matter. To cial to carry out screening for a broad range
this end, a number of studies have shown that of motor abilities, dependent on the disorder,
processing-speed deficits are strongly associ- as deficits in psychomotor functioning have
ated with impairments in driving-related abil- proven useful as predictors of driving safety
ities in Parkinson’s disease (Stolwyk et al., 2006; in certain studies (even in the absence of gross
Uc et al., 2006b; Worringham et al., 2006), HIV motor abnormalities), including those exam-
infection (Marcotte et al., 1999), and stroke ining Parkinson’s disease (Worringham et al.,
(Galski et al., 1993; Sundet et al., 1995), although 2006), HIV infection (Marcotte et al., 1999),
these findings are by no means universal (Grace and aging (Lundberg et al., 1998). In addition,
et al., 2005). although associations between impairment
There is also evidence that older individuals and driving are inconsistent, one study found
may have insight regarding declines in speed of that a training program focused on improving
information processing, as it has been shown to speed of movement, coordination, and flex-
be predictive of driving cessation in this pop- ibility among older adults improved driving-
ulation (Edwards et al., 2008), although this related outcomes (Marottoli et al., 2007).
finding awaits further study. Interestingly, More sophisticated and potentially sensitive
recent data suggest that processing-speed defi- motor assessments (e.g., motor programming,
cits may be partially remediable, and, although velocity scaling, forced steadiness, motor dis-
the evidence is still preliminary, improvement inhibition) may be useful in demarcating the
in driving-related outcomes has been shown contributions of motor abnormalities to driv-
with processing-speed training in older adults ing behaviors.
(Roenker et al., 2003).
In general, simple reaction time measures
have not been very sensitive predictors of driv- Language
ing impairments, although choice reaction time In general, measures of language and ver-
measures have been more useful. As noted ear- bal functioning have yielded mixed results,
lier, while good reaction times may help one get depending upon the population under study,
out of a dangerous situation, the good driver and severity of impairment. For example, Hunt
avoids those situations in the first place (e.g., et al. (1993) found a modest correlation between
consider that adolescents have fast reaction the Boston Naming Test and driving perfor-
times but high accident rates). mance. However, such measures are not consid-
ered the most sensitive to driving performance
Psychomotor Speed across disorders.
Psychomotor functioning has been inves-
tigated as a potential explanatory factor in
Motor
driving studies, but it has rarely been a focus
except in disorders with prominent motor Measures of pure motor functioning (e.g., finger
symptoms (Marcotte et al., 2008). Th is may tapping) are usually of limited utility in predict-
be due to the fact that neurocognitive tests ing driving competence, although they may be
often examine psychomotor functioning at of some benefit in assessing neurologic disor-
the level of fi ne motor coordination (e.g., ders that have significant motor impairments
Grooved Pegboard Test), whereas driving (Grace et al., 2005; Radford et al., 2004).
Neuropsychological Performance and the Assessment of Driving Behavior 665

is little justification for the selection of these


Mental Status/Screening Tests
tests or for the recommended cutoffs (Molnar
The use of cognitive screening tests, such as et al., 2006).
the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE)
and Mattis Organic Mental Status Syndrome
Metacognition
Examination (MOMSSE), to screen for driving
impairments has the same limitations found Metacognition, or the awareness of one’s own
when using such measures for other types cognitive (and driving) abilities and processes,
of evaluations: poor sensitivity at the milder can significantly affect driving outcomes. For
ranges of impairment and a lack of clear deci- example, if an individual is not aware of a
sion points for those who score in the borderline visuoperceptual deficit, he/she may choose to
range. Such measures are typically most useful continue driving or drive less cautiously than
in mid-to-late stages of dementia (Reger et al., if cognizant of the deficit. On the other hand,
2004). In addition, most screening measures fail if aware of deficits, he/she may cease driving or
to assess abilities believed to be important for compensate for deficits through various strat-
driving (e.g., executive functioning, impulsiv- egies (see below). Studies of the relationships
ity). While a meta-analysis showed that mental between clinical neuropsychological measures,
status measures had a moderate association with metacognition, and driver behavior are still
road test scores when dementia and control par- in their infancy, although a few studies have
ticipants were included, this relationship disap- shown that awareness of cognitive deficits in
peared when only those with dementia were TBI is a fairly accurate predictor of driving out-
included (Reger et al., 2004). Thus, screening comes (Coleman et al., 2002; Galski et al., 1990).
measures should be used with caution in deter- However, emerging evidence also indicates that
mining whether individuals are at risk for driv- impaired drivers with various disorders often
ing impairments (Lundberg et al., 1997). overestimate their driving abilities (Freund
A few professional groups have provided et al., 2005; Lundqvist & Alinder, 2007; Marcotte
guidelines regarding determining when a et al., 2004b; Wild & Cotrell, 2003), although
patient might be an at-risk driver. For exam- this may improve somewhat with training (Eby
ple, the American Medical Association (Wang et al., 2003). Clinicians should also be aware
et al., 2003) recommends looking for “red flags” that certain disorders have a higher prevalence
in the patient’s history (e.g., medical conditions of anosognosia (lack of awareness of impair-
or medications that may impair driving skills) ments), including Alzheimer’s disease, stroke,
and reviewing driving history. If problems are and Korsakoff ’s amnesia (e.g., Cosentino &
suspected, they recommend formally assess- Stern, 2005; Orfei et al., 2007).
ing vision, cognition, and motor functioning
and provide guidelines and cutoff scores that
Compensatory Strategies
indicate potential problems. Aspects of vision
that should be evaluated include visual acuity Awareness of one’s deficits or limitations may
(assessed via Snellen chart) and visual fields lead to compensatory strategies on the part of
(assessed via confrontation testing). For cog- the driver, such as driving only at certain times
nition, the Trail-Making Test Part B and the (e.g., avoiding night driving) or on particular
Clock-Drawing Test (with Freund Clock Scoring routes. Studies have shown differential use of
for Driving Competency) are recommended, compensatory strategies between cognitively
while the Rapid Pace Walk and Manual Tests impaired and healthy comparison groups (Fisk
of Range of Motion and Motor Strength are et al., 2002b; Foley et al., 2000; Rebok et al.,
recommended for the assessment of motor 1995; Stutts, 1998; Trobe et al., 1996), indicat-
functioning. Although such guidance is a pos- ing that some individuals are aware of their
itive step, the authors acknowledge that other cognitive limitations and adjust accordingly.
driving-related abilities are missed by this brief Yet this relationship seems to be quite complex.
evaluation, such as contrast sensitivity, memory, Individuals with more severe (Trobe et al., 1996;
attention, and coordination. In addition, there Valcour et al., 2002) or more prolonged (Gilley
666 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

et al., 1991) cognitive impairment may reduce et al., 2003). Moreover, many assessments are
their driving involvement more than those with designed to ensure drivers know and apply cer-
less severity or a shorter duration of impair- tain rules and are generally safe, not to detect
ment. Individuals with medical conditions (e.g., specific declines in functioning. In addition,
Hakamies-Blomqvist & Siren, 2003), physical the results of many driving assessments are
performance difficulties (e.g., Edwards et al., often determined by a subjective global assess-
2008), poorer self-rated health (e.g., Anstey ment (e.g., “fit” or “unfit” to drive) that may not
et al., 2006), or vision problems (e.g., Freeman encompass all problem areas. Even when driv-
et al., 2005) may also cease driving at higher lev- ing assessments are standardized and compre-
els. This relationship may be mediated or moder- hensive, they are often not validated against
ated by other factors, such as gender, availability criterion variables of interest, such as crash
of public transportation, or availability of other involvement or records of violations. Studies
drivers in the household (Freund & Szinovacz, that do validate against driving history may
2002). A common strategy, of course, is driv- have insufficient statistical power, as crashes
ing more slowly, although this in itself may be and violations are relatively rare events and are
indicative of impaired driving (Withaar et al., often underreported (Fox et al., 1998). Reliable,
2000). Another compensatory strategy is driv- criterion-based cutoffs for failure on many mea-
ing with a “copilot,” or another passenger in sures are also lacking, leading to imprecise con-
the car, who assists with driving-related tasks. cepts of an “unsafe performance.” Furthermore,
This method has been proposed as a potentially if a study does find significant differences or an
helpful method for reducing crash risk (Bedard increased risk ratio between clinical and healthy
et al., 1998), although it may be less beneficial comparison samples, the degree to which indi-
in challenging traffic situations (Shua-Haim & viduals in the real world are at risk is often not
Gross, 1996) or in navigating unfamiliar routes clear.
(Vrkljan & Polgar, 2007). Nonetheless, many On the other hand, significant advances in
cognitively impaired individuals continue to driving assessments have taken place in the
drive (Valcour et al., 2002), and some com- last two decades. There has been an increased
pensatory strategies, such as avoiding highway focus on standardizing on-road evaluations and
driving, may actually increase crash risk (Baker establishing their reliability and validity, pos-
et al., 2003). For example, unlike highway driv- sibly leading to replicable studies (Fox et al.,
ing, local driving involves intersections that 1998). In addition, researchers have begun to
place even greater emphasis on intact peripheral focus on the clinical utility of driving assess-
vision and attention. Evidence is also lacking as ments by providing classification statistics such
to whether compensatory strategies are actually as sensitivity and specificity (e.g., Schultheis
effective in reducing the risk of future crashes et al., 2003). There is also increasing apprecia-
(Ball et al., 1998). tion of the limitations of various assessment of
driving safety, leading to innovative assessment
methodologies. For example, driving simula-
Assessment of Driving Capacity—
tors are increasingly affordable, with enhanced
What Measures Constitute
technology, graphics, and realism.
“Outcomes” in Driving Studies?
Nonetheless, it can be confusing to read the
Establishing efficient and accurate assessments ever-growing literature on driver safety in var-
of driving impairment poses a significant chal- ious medical and neurologic conditions. When
lenge. There is currently no clear, accepted evaluating such research, the reader should
outcome variable that captures the concept of always keep in mind (1) how the outcome is
“impaired driving skills” (Yale et al., 2003), and measured, (2) whether there is any criterion
the best method for assessment remains debated variable with which to correlate the outcome,
(Marcotte & Scott, 2004). Although on-road and (3) how much inference is being made from
evaluations are often used as a gold standard, the results. For example, is performance on a
even by most government testing agencies, they driving simulator assumed to relate to “real-
are not universally accepted as such (Brookhuis world” driving? If neuropsychological tests are
Neuropsychological Performance and the Assessment of Driving Behavior 667

administered, are they correlated with a small decisions, which in turn may hamper detec-
portion of driving behavior? Would the impact tion of planning and problem-solving deficits
be different if participants were evaluated within (i.e., impairments in executive functions). It has
a rich driving environment, which includes thus been recommended that on-road evalua-
ongoing distractions and requires multitasking tions include a significant “free-drive” period,
and vigilance over long periods of time? in which the examinee drives without direc-
There are a variety of methodologies for tion from the examiner (British Psychological
assessing driving abilities in research and clin- Society, 2001). It is also important to design
ical settings, each with its own strengths and driving tasks that place demands on executive
limitations. These include evaluating abilities functions, attention, visual perception, and
based upon an on-road drive (open and closed memory, in order to more effectively cover the
course), reviewing recent driving history, and broad extent of abilities needed for safe driving
assessing performance on a laboratory-based in the real world (Uc et al., 2004). In general,
driving simulator. due to time and resource constraints, on-road
evaluations conducted by governmental agen-
cies are usually less challenging, and therefore
Behind the Wheel Evaluations
likely to be less sensitive, than the longer and
On-Road Driving Evaluations. On-road eval- more complex evaluations typically conducted
uations are most often considered the gold by driving rehabilitation specialists.
standard for identifying impairments in driv- Objective ratings of on-road driving can also
ing abilities, as they provide the greatest face be complicated by the fact that the primary
validity, with true sensory feedback and qua- examiner often needs to worry about safety
si-real-world situations (Withaar et al., 2000). issues (e.g., grabbing the steering wheel during
Participants typically drive a standardized route an unsafe maneuver). In part because of this
and are scored by driving rehabilitation special- complication, many on-road evaluations yield a
ists on tasks such as scanning the environment, clinical impression of driving ability, with lim-
maintaining safe distances, and avoiding dan- ited objective scoring, rather than using extensive
gerous or risky maneuvers. Studies that have scoring schemas. When feasible, a second exam-
utilized structured evaluations through clin- iner can offset this limitation. Since these tests
ical ratings have achieved adequate reliability entail real-world unpredictability, individuals
(Akinwuntan et al., 2005a; Hunt et al., 1997b; being assessed on a similar route can encounter
Marcotte et al., 2004b; Odenheimer et al., 1994). disparate scenarios. In addition, unanticipated
In addition, there has been a movement toward cueing can take place. For example, Hunt
developing empirically based and standardized et al. (1997a) found that some mildly impaired
on-road evaluations for various populations patients with Alzheimer’s disease slowed at an
(e.g., Fox et al., 1998), and some researchers unregulated intersection, while others did not.
have achieved success in establishing such eval- In reviewing their data, they noticed that those
uations (e.g., Akinwuntan et al., 2005a). participants who appropriately slowed were
Despite the clear face validity of on-road tests, mimicking a car that happened to be in front of
questions still remain regarding their ecologi- them at the time. As with all testing, although
cal and external validity. Few studies have data undergoing evaluation may result in increased
regarding their criterion validity with relation anxiety, it can also lead examinees to maintain
to future crashes or violations. Participants are their concentration at a higher level than during
usually presented with step-by-step directions, routine daily driving. Inadvertent cueing may
which do not emulate decisions made under also occur when the examiner unintentionally
real-world conditions, where one must respond responds to dangerous situations. Despite these
to novel or emergency conditions (e.g., a pedes- limitations, and the fact that such evaluations
trian appearing in the roadway). Furthermore, engender risk and require significant resources,
evaluations may not be sensitive to certain this methodology provides important insights
deficits. For example, directions given dur- into how individuals may perform under rea-
ing the evaluation may minimize navigational sonably controlled, real-life situations.
668 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

In addition to these clinically oriented evalu- it has been argued that this may exaggerate the
ations, some investigators (primarily examining risk for low-mileage groups, since high-mileage
pharmacologic effects) have focused on distinct groups often accumulate their miles on less
driving parameters such as the standard devia- congested and simpler roadways (freeways or
tion of lateral position (a quantitative measure multilane highways) (Janke, 1991).
of the degree to which individuals adjust lane Despite the benefits that a review of driving
position while driving) and time to adaptation history offers, crashes and violations are rare
to a lead car’s changes in speed on public road- events, restricting the range of outcome vari-
ways (Kuypers et al., 2006; Ramaekers, 2003). ables. This may limit statistical power and lead
to an inadequate number of validation studies.
“Closed-Course” On-Road Driving Evaluations. Review of driving history also does not directly
“Closed-course” evaluations provide the same inform us of how an individual would perform
sensory feedback as normal on-road evaluations under unusual (but not rare) situations, such as
but offer an additional level of situational con- a pedestrian running into the roadway. In addi-
trol. These evaluations typically measure basic tion, other unidentified factors may affect crash
skills, such as the standard deviation of lateral rates (a crash can be caused by another driver,
position, reaction time in braking, or maneu- leading to overestimates of driver impairment,
vering through cones or other objects (Galski or avoided because of the defensive behavior
et al., 1990, 1992; Sivak et al., 1981). Most are of other drivers, leading to underestimates of
conducted without interaction with other vehi- driver impairment). Another difficulty con-
cles, ensuring a level of control that in-traffic cerns the availability of appropriate crash sta-
evaluations cannot provide. However, this level tistics. State motor vehicle records are often
of control may preclude evaluation of critical difficult to obtain and tend to reflect underre-
skills that drivers need in the real world, such porting of crashes, since most individuals do
as decision making or maneuvering in traffic, not report minor crashes (one study found that
which may primarily limit the assessment to only 40% of crashes were reported (Insurance
the operational level of Michon’s driving model Research Council, 1991)). Self-reports of crashes
(1985) and may not evaluate critical skills such and violations are complicated by possible
as decision making or maneuvering in traffic. biases, including the influence of social desir-
Furthermore, it is often not practical to find a ability (i.e., the desire to look unimpaired to the
location to conduct these evaluations. Given researcher) and the inaccuracy of normal mem-
these limitations, “closed-course” evaluations ory, much less the memory impairments seen in
have been recommended only as a precursor to neurologic populations. To this end, self-reports
actual on-road assessment (Fox et al., 1998). of crashes and violations have shown generally
modest association with official driving records
(Arthur et al., 2005; McGwin et al., 1998).
Crash History
In order to understand how individuals actu-
Driving Simulators
ally behave in their usual driving environment,
investigators often examine a driver’s crash his- Driving simulators overcome some of the limi-
tory. This methodology provides a fair degree tations of other approaches and allow research-
of ecological and external validity by measur- ers to examine driving behavior under more
ing behavior over an extended period of time in controlled and standardized conditions, and
a truly real-world setting, thus avoiding some often in greater detail. Whereas most research
limitations of laboratory-based assessments has focused on using neuropsychological mea-
(i.e., only providing a snapshot of the person’s sures to predict real-world driving (i.e., verid-
functioning in a contrived environment). In icality, or the degree to which performance on
addition, for determining risk this extended laboratory tests relates to performance in the
sampling also has the advantage of enabling real world), the use of simulators represents an
adjustment for an individual’s overall amount approach that emphasizes verisimilitude, or the
of driving exposure (i.e., miles driven), although degree to which the demands of the measure
Neuropsychological Performance and the Assessment of Driving Behavior 669

reflect the demands imparted by the real-world 1999; Rizzo et al., 2001). The replication of such
task (Franzen & Wilhelm, 1996; Spooner & situations are important, because even though
Pachana, 2006). Simulator performance has much of routine driving involves overlearned
been considered both an outcome, indicative of behaviors, there are also times when drivers
driving abilities (Liguori et al., 1998; Marcotte must take quick, decisive action, or anticipate
et al., 1999; Rizzo et al., 2001; Schultheis et al., risks and adjust their driving accordingly. This
2001; Weiler et al., 2000; Zesiewicz et al., 2002) could be considered analogous to piloting an
as well as a useful predictor of on-road per- aircraft, in which the vast majority of activ-
formance (Galski et al., 1997; Lee et al., 2003; ities are entirely routine, but during a crisis it
Marcotte et al., 2004b) and future crashes (e.g., is imperative that the pilot/driver be cogni-
Lew et al., 2005). tively intact across many domains. Since these
Simulations range from as simplistic as hav- responses require higher level and integrated
ing the participant press a pedal when a specified skills such as attention, spatial processing, pro-
target appears on the screen to full motion sys- cessing speed, and executive functioning (self-
tems that provide horizontal and longitudinal monitoring, judgment), abilities that cannot
travel and nearly 360 degrees of rotation, thus always be assessed during a directed on-road
providing the driver with realistic acceleration, drive, simulators provide an effective and safe
braking, and steering cues (e.g., the University method of evaluation.
of Iowa/National Highway Traffic Safety Another key advantage of simulators is the
Administration’s National Advanced Driving ability to perform detailed investigation of
Simulator; Simulator III at the Swedish National driving behavior, and its correlates, under con-
Road and Transport Research Institute). In trolled conditions. This includes analyses of
between these two extremes are high-fidelity, specific driving maneuvers in potential crash
low-cost simulators that are modifiable and situations (Rizzo et al., 2001), application of eye
provide a feasible option for researchers wish- movement analyses to infer attentional alloca-
ing to measure driving behavior. Hardware tion (Mapstone et al., 2001), and facilitation of
for most current simulations include a steer- high-resolution magnetic resonance imaging
ing wheel (proprietary or off-the-shelf joystick studies, such as fMRI (Walter et al., 2001), that
style), brake/accelerator pedals, and anywhere might provide insights into the brain regions
from a single computer monitor to full field of involved in driving. Preliminary studies also
view projection systems. suggest that training on a simulator may gener-
Numerous outcome variables can be exam- alize to on-road performance in a rehabilitation
ined in simulations, such as average speed, setting (Akinwuntan et al., 2005b). In addi-
speed variability, number of crashes, number tion, performance on simulations may provide
of near misses, and reaction time, among oth- additional, independent information above and
ers. Methodologies utilized in on-road evalua- beyond neuropsychological testing in predict-
tions, such as tracking the standard deviation ing fitness to drive (Marcotte et al., 2004b).
of lateral position and car-following behavior, There remain limitations to simulator tech-
have also been adapted for simulations (Lenne nology. Despite tremendous advances in graph-
et al., 2003; Marcotte et al., 2008; Moller et al., ics and processing capabilities, simulators still
2002; Weiler et al., 2000), although the degree do not fully recreate the multisensory driving
to which on-road and simulation performance experience (i.e., three-dimensional environ-
on these measures are interchangeable remains ment, sounds, feel of the roadway, number of
controversial (Blana & Golias, 2002). typical cars in the road). Interestingly, despite
A principal advantage of simulator technol- the attention it receives, graphics quality is not
ogy is the ability to place participants into crash necessarily the most salient feature regarding
avoidance situations (e.g., pedestrians walk- whether participants “buy in” to the experi-
ing into the roadway), wherein they must take ence. Many factors affect how “real” the sim-
evasive action, as well as novel conditions (e.g., ulation feels to the participant, including lag
fog, mountain driving) that are not reproduc- time between driver input and car response,
ible during on-road evaluations (Marcotte et al., screen refresh rates, and the behavior of other
670 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

simulated vehicles. For example, in our early Administration, 2001, 2004). Slight declines in
studies, despite using a single-monitor desktop driving performance have been shown to occur
model with very rudimentary graphics, research with normal aging (Duchek et al., 2003; Wood,
participants routinely peered over their shoul- 2002), but there is by no means consensus
ders to ensure it was safe to change lanes. regarding the extent or even occurrence of such
Though participants may take the testing seri- declines (Ball & Owsley, 2003), and while these
ously and strive to avoid crashes, they are still may result in reduced driving performance, they
aware that a simulator crash will not cause prop- do not necessarily put the older person into the
erty damage or bodily injury, and thus they may “impaired driver” category. It has been argued
not drive as cautiously as they might in the real that it is not necessarily age itself but, rather, the
world. And, although much information can be specific changes that often accompany aging
garnered from computer simulations, they do that are risk factors for increased crashes (Fain,
not negate the necessity for behavioral observa- 2003; Fitten, 2003). As such, older age groups are
tion to fully evaluate the participant’s behavior potentially at greater risk of driving impairment
at the time of a crash. Lastly, simulation sickness due to increased medication use, functional
is a constant concern. As realism increases and injuries, and medical conditions that affect driv-
participants sense motion but remain seated on ing ability, and subgroups with these conditions
a fi xed platform, there is a greater likelihood of may increase the average risk for the entire age
nausea. This is particularly true for older indi- group (Ball et al., 1998). Declines in one’s UFOV
viduals, especially females. While interventions have been associated with past (Ball et al., 1993)
such as keeping the room cooled and ventilated and future (Owsley et al., 1998) accidents in
help, they do not entirely eliminate simulator older adults. In a review of the aging and driving
sickness. And, unfortunately, common medica- literature, Anstey et al. (2005) identified declines
tions for motion sickness are of limited use since in measures of attention, visuoperception, com-
they may themselves affect driving ability. plex reaction time, and executive functioning as
being associated with driving performance, and
emphasized the importance of self-monitoring
The Impact of Neurologic,
in adjusting for cognitive, visual, and physical
Psychiatric, and Medical
deficits.
Conditions on Driving Ability
Below we briefly review findings regarding driv-
Neurologic Disease
ing and select neuropsychiatric disorders. The
usual caveat applies here: the impairments dis- Alzheimer’s Disease. Driving abilities decline
cussed are at the group level and do not neces- in individuals with Alzheimer’s disease, even
sarily apply to individual patients. In addition, with mild severity (i.e., Clinical Dementia
findings vary, depending upon the methodology Rating—CDR ≤ 1). This has been found via
being used to assess driving status (real-world informant reports (Dubinsky et al., 1992),
crashes, simple versus complex simulations, review of crash records, simulator assessments
on-road assessments). (Rizzo et al., 1997, 2001), and on-road tests
(Fitten et al., 1995; Hunt et al., 1993), although
the results have not been entirely consistent
Normal Aging
(Bieliauskas et al., 1998; Trobe et al., 1996).
Older driver safety is an increasing public health Driving impairment typically increases signif-
concern. For example, although older drivers icantly over time and tracks with disease sever-
in the United States have the lowest crash rate ity, although there is variability (Duchek et al.,
per licensed driver of all age groups, their crash 2003). Proposed predictors of driving perfor-
rate per million miles driven is higher than mance in Alzheimer’s disease have included
most other age groups (with the exception of the measures of processing speed (e.g., Trail-Making
16- to 20-year-old age group); even more alarm- Test, Part A), attentional switching (e.g., Trail-
ing, their fatality rate per crash is higher than any Making Test, Part B), language (e.g., Boston
other age group (National Highway Traffic Safety Naming Test), and higher-order visuoperceptual
Neuropsychological Performance and the Assessment of Driving Behavior 671

function (e.g., Rey–O Complex Figure Copy). 2006). There remains uncertainty regarding
These have been fairly consistent predictors of the degree to which driving performance does
on-road performance, even after adjusting for (Zesiewicz et al., 2002), or does not (Wood et al.,
disease status in some cases (Grace et al., 2005; 2005), relate to common measures of disease
Hunt et al., 1993; Uc et al., 2004, 2006a). Duchek progress (i.e., Haehn and Yahr scale and Unified
and colleagues (1998) theorized that measures Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale [UPDRS]). In
of selective visual attention (visual search errors addition to motor and cognitive impairments,
and UFOV) may be particularly important, and sleep abnormalities, such as “sleep attacks,”
this has been corroborated (Uc et al., 2004). which may occur when patients are fatigued,
Memory impairment may also be important on appear to put patients at increased risk for
driving tasks that make demands upon retro- accidents (Adler & Thorpy, 2005; Hobson et al.,
spective memory (Uc et al., 2004). Neither care- 2002; Meindorfner et al., 2005), may be exacer-
giver perception nor patient self-assessment is a bated by the use of dopamine agonists (Avorn
consistently good predictor of on-road perfor- et al., 2005).
mance or future crashes in this patient group
(Hunt et al., 1993; Wild & Cotrell, 2003). Some Multiple Sclerosis. Cognitive dysfunction
consensus meetings have generated specific rec- occurs in 30%–70% of MS patients, at least at
ommendations regarding dementia and driv- some point during their lifetime (Rao et al.,
ing. For example, an international workgroup 1991). Memory, attention, processing speed,
(Lundberg et al., 1997) recommended that and executive functioning may be affected,
individuals with moderate to severe dementia with intellectual functioning and language
should stop driving, those with mild demen- skills typically preserved (Bobholz & Rao,
tia and functional deterioration should have a 2003). From small epidemiologic studies, MS
specialized driving assessment, and individu- patients have been found to have higher crash
als with mild dementia and stable functioning rates (Lings, 2002; Schultheis et al., 2002) and,
should have periodic follow-ups. However, as in the laboratory, worse performance on simu-
with many such guidelines, these recommen- lators (Kotterba et al., 2003; Marcotte et al.,
dations did not include guidance regarding the 2008) than controls. Neuromotor symptoms
tools for reaching these classifications. (weakness, sensory disturbance, coordination
problems, spasticity), as well as MS-related
Parkinson’s Disease. Although motor dys- neurocognitive disturbance may affect driving
function is most commonly associated with ability (Schultheis et al., 2001), and MS patients
Parkinson’s disease, it can also result in declines have greater difficulty on the UFOV (Schultheis
in executive functioning and attention, as well et al., 2001). Unfortunately, there is limited
as visual functioning. Numerous studies have information relating specific cognitive abilities
concluded that driving is impaired in patients to driving performance in MS patients. A small
with Parkinson’s disease, whether assessed via on-road study of individuals with MS (Lincoln
simulator (Madeley et al., 1990), on-road driv- & Radford, 2008) found measures of attention/
ing (Heikkila & Kallanranta, 2005; Uc et al., concentration (Dot Cancellation), a Road Sign
2006c), or accident history (Dubinsky et al., Recognition task, Design Learning, and an
1991). Driving impairments in patients with information-processing task (Adult Memory
Parkinson’s disease may be exacerbated by and Information-Processing Task B) to be rea-
increased task complexity, although there is sig- sonably predictive of on-road failures. A sim-
nificant variability among patients (Uc et al., ulator study assessing the ability to maintain
2006c). Difficulties in set-shift ing and cogni- lane position while attending to a secondary
tive flexibility (e.g., Trail-Making Test, Brixton task found moderate (r = ~.5) correlations with
test), processing speed, and attention (including the Trail-Making Test, Digit Symbol, and the
visual attention, with the UFOV) have been rea- Hopkins Verbal Learning Test (Marcotte et al.,
sonably consistent predictors of performance 2008). Many studies excluded individuals with
in this population (Amick et al., 2007; Stolwyk significant spasticity in order to focus on cog-
et al., 2006; Uc et al., 2006c; Worringham et al., nitive issues. However, spasticity, which affects
672 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

40%–70% of patients, may also result in specific as the Glasgow Coma Scale score, length of coma,
driving impairments, such as poor speed main- or posttraumatic amnesia, have been predictive
tenance, and it may be possible to differentiate of driving ability (e.g., Korteling & Kaptein,
the contribution of cognition and spasticity to 1996), although some studies have failed to find
impairments in specific driving skills (Marcotte such associations (e.g., Sivak et al., 1981). While
et al., 2008). In addition, clinicians should it might be possible to compensate for impair-
remain cognizant of the possible role that visual ments in speed of processing by reducing speed
dysfunction has on driving performance. or using other methods for decreasing time pres-
sure (Schmidt et al., 1996), in some cases patients
Epilepsy. There is limited data regarding direct fail to make these adjustments, resulting in
assessment of driving in individuals with epi- prolonged glances away from the roadway and
lepsy. Although some jurisdictions require suggesting limited self-awareness. No specific
health-care providers to report persons with pattern of neuropsychological deficits predicts
epilepsy who have seizures and want to drive, driving safety or risk in TBI patients, although
a consensus statement (“Consensus statements, neuropsychological tests are generally useful
sample statutory provisions, and model regula- when assessing driving fitness in this population.
tions regarding driver licensing and epilepsy. As with other disorders, clinicians should assess
American Academy of Neurology, American a broad range of cognitive abilities, including
Epilepsy Society, and Epilepsy Foundation of executive functions, psychomotor skills, visu-
America,” 1994) was not supportive of manda- ospatial ability, processing speed, divided and
tory reporting. There is general agreement that sustained attention, and working memory, as all
individuals with uncontrolled seizures should have shown predictive power in previous studies
not drive, and determination regarding permis- (e.g., Coleman et al., 2002; Korteling & Kaptein,
sion to drive is in part related to the seizure-free 1996). In particular, executive functioning may
interval, usually recommended to be between 3 be useful in predicting driving performance
and 12 months (Drazkowski, 2007). In addition, (Coleman et al., 2002), and perhaps the UFOV
antiepileptic medications can result in visual (Novack et al., 2006). In contrast to Alzheimer’s
problems, fatigue, and tremors (French et al., disease, patient and caregiver insight into defi-
2004), and may affect driving ability. cits in TBI patients may be valuable predictors of
driving risk (Coleman et al., 2002; Galski et al.,
Traumatic Brain Injury. Although TBIs often 1990; Rapport et al., 1998). Premorbid factors
result in impairments that might affect driving may also play a prominent role in the prediction
(e.g., cognitive, perceptual, behavioral), approx- of driving ability after brain injury. Pietrapiana
imately 40%–60% of patients who survive a TBI and colleagues (2005) found that premorbid
resume driving. In making this decision, patients risky behavioral characteristics and pre-injury
often overvalue recovery in physical function driving crashes and violations were critical pre-
and minimize the effects of sustained cognitive dictors of crashes over the follow-up period,
and emotional problems (Leon-Carrion et al., although retrospective questionnaires were used
2005). Many return to driving without spe- to probe informants regarding participants’ pre-
cific evaluations or medical advice (Fisk et al., morbid characteristics. Importantly, studies
1998; Pietrapiana et al., 2005). The predictors have begun to emerge showing that a number
of driving fitness are therefore critical in this of TBI patients who undergo intensive, multi-
population, due to these high rates of driving disciplinary rehabilitation programs can safely
postinjury and the possibility of recovery with return to driving (Leon-Carrion et al., 2005;
rehabilitation (see below). Unfortunately, studies Schultheis et al., 2002), although longitudinal
examining driving fitness in patients recovering studies of the effectiveness of such programs are
from TBI have had inconsistent findings due to badly needed.
the heterogeneous nature of the brain injuries,
the broad range of severity, and the differences Stroke. Driving abilities can be significantly
in time since injury (Tamietto et al., 2006). In diminished by a stroke, with more signifi-
many studies, indicators of injury severity, such cant cognitive and motor symptoms leading to
Neuropsychological Performance and the Assessment of Driving Behavior 673

worse outcomes (e.g., Schanke & Sundet, 2000). driving impairments due to the underlying con-
Marshall and colleagues (2007) conducted a dition versus the medications aimed at treating
review of the predictors of driving ability fol- the disorder.
lowing stroke. Although methodological qual- An on-road study (Wingen et al., 2006) of indi-
ity was wanting in most studies, consistent viduals with depression and receiving SSRI or
predictors of on-road driving assessments in SNRI medications found that, compared to con-
poststroke patients were visual field and acu- trols, the treated depressed group had a higher
ity testing, Trail-Making Test Parts A and B, standard deviation of lateral position (weaving)
Rey–O Complex Figure Copy, UFOV, road sign and were also slower to adapt to speed changes
and hazard recognition, and reaction time mea- in a lead car. Given that prior research suggests
sures. Older age, the presence of aphasia, and that SSRIs do not impair driving after acute
right-sided lesions were also predictive of worse repeated doses in healthy controls (Ramaekers,
outcomes, while motor function, coordination, 2003), the authors concluded that the driving
and emotional symptoms were judged to need difficulties were most likely the result of resid-
further investigation. There is some evidence ual depression rather than the medications.
that simulator-based training may enhance Little research exists with respect to driving
recovery of driving abilities (Akinwuntan performance and schizophrenia. A study of 83
et al., 2005a), although these findings await older outpatients with schizophrenia (Palmer
replication. et al., 2002) found that 43% were currently
driving. Using a desktop driving simulation (St
HIV/AIDS. Cognitive impairment is a com- Germain et al., 2005), drivers with schizophre-
mon sequela of HIV infection (Heaton et al., nia tended to have more line crossings, colli-
1995). Despite dramatic reductions in mortality sions, and drove slower compared to controls.
and morbidity with the advent of increasingly The disorder with perhaps the most extensive
effective medications, there are indications literature regarding driving performance is
that HIV-related neurologic disorders are still attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).
prevalent and remain a significant clinical con- ADHD includes symptoms of poor sustained
cern (Antinori et al., 2007; Sacktor et al., 2002). attention, distractibility, impaired impulse con-
Neuropsychological impairments short of trol, and hyperactivity (American Psychiatric
severe dementia may result in impaired driving, Association, 1994). Simulator (Biederman et al.,
as assessed via simulator and on-road meth- 2007) and epidemiological (Barkley et al., 2002;
odologies (Marcotte et al., 1999, 2004b), with Fried et al., 2006) research indicates that persons
impairments in executive functioning, atten- with ADHD have impaired driving abilities.
tion/working memory, and motor abilities being Individuals with ADHD are more likely to have
most predictive of driving dysfunction. Poor traffic citations (Jerome et al., 2006) and higher
visual attention (assessed on the UFOV) is asso- accident rates (Barkley et al., 1993; Cox et al.,
ciated with increased crashes, particularly in 2000b). This appears to be especially true for
the context of other neuropsychological impair- younger individuals with ADHD. Medication
ments (Marcotte et al., 2006). Importantly, neu- improves driving performance on simulators
ropsychological tests and simulator evaluations (Barkley et al., 2005; Cox et al., 2000b) and on
may provide unique, independent data regard- the road (Cox et al., 2004; Verster et al., 2008).
ing the abilities required for intact driving See Barkley and Cox (2007) for a review of the
(Marcotte et al., 2004b), and thus a multimodal driving and ADHD literature.
assessment approach appears most promising
in this population.
Medical Disease
It is beyond the scope of this chapter to review
Psychiatric Disorders
literature on medical disease and driving
There is a dearth of research directly assessing impairment. However, clinicians should be
driving ability in various psychiatric conditions, cognizant that many conditions can affect driv-
and it is often difficult to differentiate between ing performance, and these may be amplified
674 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

by neuropsychological impairments. These Retesting


include conditions such as cataracts (Owsley
If an evaluation suggests that a patient is capable
et al., 1999), mild hepatic encephalopathy (Bajaj
of continuing to drive, clinicians are confronted
et al., 2008; Wein et al., 2004), diabetes (Cox
with the question of whether the patient should
et al., 2000a, 2002), coronary artery bypass
be reevaluated at a later time and, if so, which
grafting (CABG) (Ahlgren et al., 2003), sleep
methodology should be used and over what
apnea (Pizza et al., 2008; Vorona & Ware, 2002)
time interval should the person be reassessed.
and receiving radiation treatment (Yuen et al.,
In the case of AD, the American Academy
2008).
of Neurology (Dubinsky et al., 2000) recom-
mends that patients with a CDR severity of 1
Other Assessment Issues or greater discontinue driving completely while
the patients and family of those with a rating
Assessing Driving History
of 0.5 be informed that the individual is at risk
It is helpful to gather information about driv- and should be evaluated by a qualified driving
ing history and current driving behavior from examiner and reassessed every 6 months. Since
both the driver and a collateral source, if possi- up to 30% of individuals with a CDR of 0.5 pro-
ble. Information regarding frequency and dis- gress to a score of 1.0 within 1 year (Morris &
tance, length of driving episodes, type of roads Cummings, 2005), this follow-up period gener-
commonly driven, driving scenarios that are ally appears appropriate. Although few studies
avoided, and the use of “copilots” may prove have investigated driving performance longitu-
valuable in understanding a person’s driving dinally, in individuals with mild AD, Duchek
experience. As noted earlier, although useful, et al. (2003), Fox et al. (1997), and Ott et al.
for a variety of reasons (poor memory or aware- (2008b) have shown that driving performance
ness, biased reporting), self and other reports can significantly decline from safe to unsafe in
may at times not be completely reliable. Table only a few years. The longitudinal driving pro-
27–2 provides a list of questions that might be fi les of other conditions await further study.
helpful in eliciting possible driving difficulties.
Sample questionnaires for gathering informa-
Informed Consent and Limits
tion on driving behaviors include the Driving
to Confidentiality
Habits Questionnaire (Ball et al., 1998) and the
Dula Dangerous Driving Index (Willemsen As in any clinical evaluation, it is important to
et al., 2008). properly inform the client regarding any limits

Table 27–2. Questions for Assessing Potential Driving Problems


1. Has the driver received any traffic tickets or warnings in the past year or two?
2. Has the driver been in any accidents in the last year or two, even “fender benders”?
3. Does the driver hit curbs or other objects such as the fence or garage more often then he/she used to?
4. Has the driver had more “near misses” or “close calls” lately?
5. Does the driver miss turns or respond slowly to signals or road signs while driving?
6. Does the driver ever get lost?
7. Does the driver have more difficulty when directions need to be followed?
8. Do friends or family members express worry about the person’s driving?
9. Do friends or family members ever avoid riding in a car with the driver?
10. Does the driver worry or feel nervous about driving at night, making left turns into oncoming traf-
fic, or driving in unfamiliar places?
11. Does the driver ever describe how pedestrians or other cars “appear out of nowhere”?
12. Does the driver respond slower to unexpected situations?
13. Do other drivers honk at the person when he/she is behind the wheel?
14. Does the driver seem distracted or have a hard time concentrating while driving?
15. Has the driver lost some confidence in his/her driving ability?
(Source: Adapted from Eby et al. (2000) and Wang et al. (2003).
Neuropsychological Performance and the Assessment of Driving Behavior 675

of confidentiality with respect to the reporting recommended that, in part, the neuropsycho-
of driving-related concerns, and it is recom- logical evaluation focus on the types of impair-
mended that clinicians keep up to date on their ments likely to be encountered based upon the
local reporting requirements. presenting problem (i.e., differential diagnoses).
While the choice of tests will vary depending on
the suspected cause of driving impairment, tests
Reporting Requirements
of visuospatial functioning, psychomotor func-
Laws in the U.S. vary by state as to whether cli- tioning, processing speed, attention, and execu-
nicians are required to report certain medical, tive functioning should ideally be administered.
psychiatric, and neurological diagnoses to the If available in the community and within the
Department of Motor Vehicles (DMV), health resources of the patient, an on-road evaluation
departments, or similar entities. Although most by a driving rehabilitation specialist may yield
states have regulations regarding impaired driv- specific areas of driving deficits (a listing of
ers, many of the policies are vague, and await qualified individuals can be found at the Web
clarification via future legal cases. In some site for the American Occupational Therapy
states, reporting is required (often with immu- Association (www.aota.org)). Unfortunately,
nity from liability), while in others reporting while failing an on-road test may clearly indi-
is either “permitted” or “encouraged.” On the cate impaired driving, passing the test does not
basis of court precedent, physicians and psy- guarantee driving competency.
chologists may be held liable for not adequately In making recommendations, clinicians might
evaluating driving ability should a crash occur consider compensatory strategies, which usu-
involving dangerous driving on the part of ally entail either reduction in miles driven (risk
a patient (Pettis, 1992), although valid argu- exposure) or the avoidance of certain driving
ments have been made against this standard situations. Since the loss of driving privileges can
(e.g., Pettis & Gutheil, 1993). It is critical that significantly affect mobility and independence,
clinicians be conversant with their state’s laws clinicians should ideally be ready to discuss alter-
regarding reporting procedures. The American nate transportation options, as well as ways to cope
Medical Association provides a useful list of with the psychosocial consequences (Windsor &
the reporting procedures for each state (Wang Anstey, 2006). In addition, it is important that,
et al., 2003) (available at http://www.ama-assn. when feasible, the patient’s family be included in
org/ama1/pub/upload/mm/433/chapter8.pdf), the process, as they may potentially provide sup-
although clinicians should request the most port with respect to environmental structure and
current information directly from their state’s transportation alternatives (Hopewell, 2002). It
Department of Motor Vehicles or Medical should be noted that in most jurisdictions the
Advisory Board. neuropsychologist will not be responsible for
determining whether individuals should keep
their drivers license, but the clinician may need
Conclusion
to make his/her best determination as to whether
Driving safety is a significant public health con- the person should be referred for evaluation by
cern, and this is reflected in the growing number the state licensing agency.
of research articles in the medical and psycho- Emerging technologies, such as minimally
logical literature. While it is clear that fitness to intrusive eye tracking systems, which measure
drive decreases as the breadth and depth of neu- gaze patterns and allocation of visual attention
ropsychological impairment increases, unfor- while driving, may in the future be able to help
tunately there is still no single cognitive test (or better clarify driving-related cognitive processes
battery of tests) that is clearly predictive of fitness as they occur and perhaps differentiate between
to drive within specific neurologic conditions, unsafe and safe drivers (Sodhi et al., 2002;
let alone across disorders (British Psychological Underwood et al., 2003). Th is is also true of in-
Society, 2001; Withaar et al., 2000). car systems that track automobile movement
Driving competence can be reduced by a and have instrumentation to potentially pro-
variety of cognitive impairments, and it is vide real-time information on driver behavior,
676 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

strategy, and even decision making. The field American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic
has been hampered, in part, by the fact that most and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders—
investigators use methods and scenarios devel- Fourth Edition: DSM-IV. Washington, DC:
oped within their own labs, and few assessment American Psychiatric Association.
Amick, M. M., Grace, J., & Ott, B. R. (2007). Visual
methods relating to driving have been widely
and cognitive predictors of driving safety in
dispersed for general use. This limits our ability
Parkinson’s disease patients. Archives of Clinical
to translate findings across diseases and within Neuropsychology, 22(8), 957–967.
comparable cohorts. In addition, basic psycho- Anderson, S. W., Rizzo, M., Skaar, N., Stierman,
metric data such as test–retest reliabilities have L., Cavaco, S., Dawson, J., et al. (2007). Amnesia
been lacking, with a few exceptions (Hunt et al., and driving. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
1997b; Marcotte et al., 2004a, 2004b; O’Hanlon Neuropsychology, 29(1), 1–12.
et al., 1986; van Laar et al., 1995; Vermeeren & Anstey, K. J., Windsor, T. D., Luszcz, M. A., &
O’Hanlon, 1998). Although in their infancy, Andrews, G. R. (2006). Predicting driving ces-
there are efforts under way to develop stan- sation over 5 years in older adults: Psychological
dardized simulations that can be administered well-being and cognitive competence are stron-
ger predictors than physical health. Journal of the
across laboratories and patient populations. In
American Geriatrics Society, 54(1), 121–126.
addition, new studies using brain imaging tech-
Anstey, K. J., Wood, J., Lord, S., & Walker, J. G.
nologies may also yield insights into the neural (2005). Cognitive, sensory and physical factors
substrates of driving behaviors. enabling driving safety in older adults. Clinical
Determining driving ability in individuals is Psychology Review, 25(1), 45–65.
one of the most difficult issues facing clinicians, Antinori, A., Arendt, G., Becker, J. T., Brew, B. J.,
and, although there are promising new develop- Byrd, D. A., Cherner, M., et al. (2007). Updated
ments, predicting real-world competency from research nosology for HIV-associated neurocog-
behavior in the clinic or lab will likely remain a nitive disorders. Neurology, 69(18), 1789–1799.
challenge for many years to come. Arthur, W., Barrett, G. V., & Alexander, R. A. (1991).
Prediction of Vehicular Accident Involvement: A
Meta-Analysis. Human Performance, 4, 89–105.
Acknowledgments Arthur, W., Jr., Bell, S. T., Edwards, B. D., Day, E. A.,
Tubre, T. C., & Tubre, A. H. (2005). Convergence of
The authors would like to thank Wade Allen self-report and archival crash involvement data: A
and Abiodun Akinwuntan for their comments two-year longitudinal follow-up. Human Factors,
on aspects of this chapter. We would also like to 47(2), 303–313.
thank Rachel Meyer for her help with the litera- Avolio, B. J., Kroeck, K. G., & Panek, P. E. (1985).
ture review and formatting of the document. Individual-differences in information-processing
ability as a predictor of motor-vehicle accidents.
Human Factors, 27(5), 577–587.
Avorn, J., Schneeweiss, S., Sudarsky, L. R., Benner,
References
J., Kiyota, Y., Levin, R., et al. (2005). Sudden
Adler, C. H., & Thorpy, M. J. (2005). Sleep issues in uncontrollable somnolence and medication use
Parkinson’s disease. Neurology, 64(12 Suppl 3), in Parkinson disease. Archives of Neurology, 62(8),
S12–S20. 1242–1248.
Ahlgren, E., Lundqvist, A., Nordlund, A., Aren, C., Bajaj, J. S., Hafeezullah, M., Hoff mann, R. G., Varma,
& Rutberg, H. (2003). Neurocognitive impair- R. R., Franco, J., Binion, D. G., et al. (2008).
ment and driving performance after coronary Navigation skill impairment: Another dimen-
artery bypass surgery. European Journal of Cardio- sion of the driving difficulties in minimal hepatic
Thoracic Surgery, 23(3), 334–340. encephalopathy. Hepatology, 47(2), 596–604.
Akinwuntan, A. E., De Weerdt, W., Feys, H., Baten, Baker, T. K., Falb, T., Voas, R., & Lacey, J. (2003).
G., Arno, P., & Kiekens, C. (2005a). The validity Older women drivers: Fatal crashes in good condi-
of a road test after stroke. Archives of Physical tions. Journal of Safety Research, 34(4), 399–405.
Medicine and Rehabilitation, 86(3), 421–426. Ball, K., Edwards, J. D., & Ross, L. A. (2007). The
Akinwuntan, A. E., De Weerdt, W., Feys, H., Pauwels, impact of speed of processing training on cognitive
J., Baten, G., Arno, P., et al. (2005b). Effect of sim- and everyday functions. Journals of Gerontology.
ulator training on driving after stroke: A random- Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences,
ized controlled trial. Neurology, 65(6), 843–850. 62 Spec No 1, 19–31.
Neuropsychological Performance and the Assessment of Driving Behavior 677

Ball, K., & Owsley, C. (2003). Driving competence: Bieliauskas, L. A., Roper, B. R., Trobe, J., Green,
It’s not a matter of age. Journal of the American P., & Lacy, M. (1998). Cognitive measures, driv-
Geriatrics Society, 51(10), 1499–1501. ing safety, and Alzheimer’s disease. Clinical
Ball, K., Owsley, C., Sloane, M. E., Roenker, D. L., Neuropsychologist, 12(2), 206–212.
& Bruni, J. R. (1993). Visual attention problems Blana, E., & Golias, J. (2002). Differences between
as a predictor of vehicle crashes in older drivers. vehicle lateral displacement on the road and in
Investigational Ophthalmology and Visual Science, a fi xed-base simulator. Human Factors, 44(2),
34(11), 3110–3123. 303–313.
Ball, K., Owsley, C., Stalvey, B., Roenker, D. L., Sloane, Blatt, J., & Furman, S. M. (1998). Residence loca-
M. E., & Graves, M. (1998). Driving avoidance and tion of drivers involved in fatal crashes. Accident
functional impairment in older drivers. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 30(6), 705–711.
Analysis and Prevention, 30(3), 313–322. Bobholz, J. A., & Rao, S. M. (2003). Cognitive dys-
Ball, K., & Roenker, D. (1998). Useful field of view. function in multiple sclerosis: A review of recent
San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation. developments. Current Opinion in Neurology,
Ball, K. K., Beard, B. L., Roenker, D. L., Miller, R. 16(3), 283–288.
L., & Griggs, D. S. (1988). Age and visual search: Bocca, M. L., Le Doze, F., Etard, O., Pottier, M.,
Expanding the useful field of view. Journal of the L’Hoste, J., & Denise, P. (1999). Residual effect of
Optical Society of America A. Optics and Image zolpidem 10 mg and zopiclone 7.5 mg versus fluni-
Science, 5(12), 2210–2219. trazepam 1 mg and placebo on driving perfor-
Ball, K. K., Roenker, D. L., Wadley, V. G., Edwards, mance and ocular saccades. Psychopharmacology,
J. D., Roth, D. L., McGwin, G., Jr., et al. (2006). 143(4), 373–379.
Can high-risk older drivers be identified through British Psychological Society. (2001). Fitness to drive
performance-based measures in a Department of and cognition: A document of the multi-disciplin-
Motor Vehicles setting? Journal of the American ary working party on acquired neuropsychological
Geriatrics Society, 54(1), 77–84. deficits and fitness to drive 1999. Leicester, UK: The
Barkley, R. A., & Cox, D. (2007). A review of driving British Psychological Society.
risks and impairments associated with attention- Brookhuis, K. A., De Waard, D., & Fairclough, S. H.
deficit/hyperactivity disorder and the effects of (2003). Criteria for driver impairment. Ergonomics,
stimulant medication on driving performance. 46(5), 433–445.
Journal of Safety Research, 38(1), 113–128. Brouwer, W. H. (2002). Attention and driving: A
Barkley, R. A., Guevremont, D. C., Anastopoulos, A. cognitive neuropsychological approach. In M.
D., DuPaul, G. J., & Shelton, T. L. (1993). Driving- Leclercq and P. Zimmerman (Eds.), Applied
related risks and outcomes of attention deficit neuropsychology of attention: Theory, diagno-
hyperactivity disorder in adolescents and young sis and rehabilitation (pp. 230–254). Hove, UK:
adults: A 3- to 5-year follow-up survey. Pediatrics, Psychology Press.
92(2), 212–218. Brouwer, W. H., Withaar, F. K., Tant, M. L., & van
Barkley, R. A., Murphy, K. R., Dupaul, G. I., & Bush, Zomeren, A. H. (2002). Attention and driving
T. (2002). Driving in young adults with attention in traumatic brain injury: A question of cop-
deficit hyperactivity disorder: Knowledge, perfor- ing with time-pressure. Journal of Head Trauma
mance, adverse outcomes, and the role of exec- Rehabilitation, 17(1), 1–15.
utive functioning. Journal of the International Cami, J., Farre, M., Mas, M., Roset, P. N., Poudevida,
Neuropsychological Society, 8(5), 655–672. S., Mas, A., et al. (2000). Human pharmacol-
Barkley, R. A., Murphy, K. R., O’Connell, T., & ogy of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine
Connor, D. F. (2005). Effects of two doses of meth- (“ecstasy”): Psychomotor performance and subjec-
ylphenidate on simulator driving performance in tive effects. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology,
adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disor- 20(4), 455–466.
der. Journal of Safety Research, 36(2), 121–131. Cole, B. L., & Hughes, P. K. (1990). Drivers don’t
Bedard, M., Molloy, D. W., & Lever, J. A. (1998). search: They just notice. In D. Brogan (Ed.), Visual
Factors associated with motor vehicle crashes search: Proceedings of the First International
in cognitively impaired older adults. Alzheimer Conference on Visual Search. University of
Disease and Associated Disorders, 12(3), 135–139. Durham, England: Taylor and Francis.
Biederman, J., Fried, R., Monuteaux, M. C., Reimer, Coleman, R. D., Rapport, L. J., Ergh, T. C., Hanks, R.
B., Coughlin, J. F., Surman, C. B., et al. (2007). A A., Ricker, J. H., & Millis, S. R. (2002). Predictors
laboratory driving simulation for assessment of of driving outcome after traumatic brain injury.
driving behavior in adults with ADHD: A con- Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation,
trolled study. Annals of General Psychiatry, 6, 4. 83(10), 1415–1422.
678 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

Collia, D. V., Sharp, J., & Giesbrecht, L. (2003). The expression inventory. Psychological Reports, 89(3),
2001 national household travel survey: A look into 535–540.
the travel patterns of older Americans. Journal of Dickerson, A. E., Molnar, L. J., Eby, D. W., Adler,
Safety Research, 34(4), 461–470. G., Bedard, M., Berg-Weger, M., et al. (2007).
Consensus statements, sample statutory provisions, Transportation and aging: A research agenda
and model regulations regarding driver licensing for advancing safe mobility. Gerontologist, 47(5),
and epilepsy. American Academy of Neurology, 578–590.
American Epilepsy Society, and Epilepsy Drazkowski, J. (2007). An overview of epilepsy and
Foundation of America. (1994). Epilepsia, 35(3), driving. Epilepsia, 48(Suppl 9), 10–12.
696–705. Dubinsky, R. M., Gray, C., Husted, D., Busenbark, K.,
Cosentino, S., & Stern, Y. (2005). Metacognitive the- Vetere-Overfield, B., Wiltfong, D., et al. (1991). Driving
ory and assessment in dementia: Do we recognize in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology, 41(4), 517–520.
our areas of weakness? Journal of the International Dubinsky, R. M., Stein, A. C., & Lyons, K. (2000).
Neuropsychological Society, 11(7), 910–919. Practice parameter: Risk of driving and
Cox, D. J., Gonder-Frederick, L. A., Kovatchev, B. P., Alzheimer’s disease (an evidence-based review):
& Clarke, W. L. (2002). The metabolic demands of Report of the quality standards subcommittee of
driving for drivers with type 1 diabetes mellitus. the American Academy of Neurology. Neurology,
Diabetes/Metabolism Research and Reviews, 18(5), 54(12), 2205–2211.
381–385. Dubinsky, R. M., Williamson, R. N., Gray, C. S., & Glatt,
Cox, D. J., Gonder-Frederick, L. A., Kovatchev, B. P., S. L. (1992). Driving in Alzheimer’s disease. Journal
Julian, D. M., & Clarke, W. L. (2000a). Progressive of the American Geriatrics Society, 40, 1112–1116.
hypoglycemia’s impact on driving simulation per- Duchek, J. M., Carr, D. B., Hunt, L., Roe, C. M.,
formance. Occurrence, awareness and correction. Xiong, C., Shah, K., et al. (2003). Longitudinal
Diabetes Care, 23(2), 163–170. driving performance in early-stage dementia
Cox, D. J., Humphrey, J. W., Merkel, R. L., Penberthy, of the Alzheimer type. Journal of the American
J. K., & Kovatchev, B. (2004). Controlled-release Geriatrics Society, 51(10), 1342–1347.
methylphenidate improves attention during on- Duchek, J. M., Hunt, L., Ball, K., Buckles, V., &
road driving by adolescents with attention-deficit/ Morris, J. C. (1997). The role of selective atten-
hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American tion in driving and dementia of the Alzheimer
Board of Family Practice, 17(4), 235–239. type. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders,
Cox, D. J., Merkel, R. L., Kovatchev, B., & Seward, R. 11(Suppl 1), 48–56.
(2000b). Effect of stimulant medication on driving Duchek, J. M., Hunt, L., Ball, K., Buckles, V., & Morris,
performance of young adults with attention-defi- J. C. (1998). Attention and driving performance in
cit hyperactivity disorder: A preliminary double- Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Gerontology, 53(2),
blind placebo controlled trial. Journal of Nervous P130–P141.
and Mental Disease, 188(4), 230–234. Duncan, J., Williams, P., & Brown, I. (1991).
Crundall, D., Chapman, P., Phelps, N., & Underwood, Components of driving skill—experience does not
G. (2003). Eye movements and hazard perception mean expertise. Ergonomics, 34(7), 919–937.
in police pursuit and emergency response driving. Eby, D. W., Molnar, L. J., & Shope, J. T. (2000).
Journal of Experimental Psychology. Applied, 9(3), Driving decisions Workbook (Report UMTRI-
163–174. 2000-14). Ann Abor: University of Michigan,
Crundall, D., Underwood, G., & Chapman, P. (1999). Transportation Research Institute.
Driving experience and the functional field of Eby, D. W., Molnar, L. J., Shope, J. T., Vivoda, J. M.,
view. Perception, 28(9), 1075–1087. & Fordyce, T. A. (2003). Improving older driver
Dahlen, E. R., Martin, R. C., Ragan, K., & Kuhlman, knowledge and self-awareness through self-assess-
M. M. (2005). Driving anger, sensation seeking, ment: The driving decisions workbook. Journal of
impulsiveness, and boredom proneness in the pre- Safety Research, 34(4), 371–381.
diction of unsafe driving. Accident Analysis and Edwards, J. D., Ross, L. A., Ackerman, M. L., Small, B.
Prevention, 37(2), 341–348. J., Ball, K. K., Bradley, S., et al. (2008). Longitudinal
Daigneault, G., Joly, P., & Frigon, J. Y. (2002). predictors of driving cessation among older
Executive functions in the evaluation of acci- adults from the ACTIVE clinical trial. Journals of
dent risk of older drivers. Journal of Clinical and Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and
Experimental Neuropsychology, 24(2), 221–238. Social Sciences, 63(1), P6–P12.
Deffenbacher, J. L., Lynch, R. S., Deffenbacher, D. Endsley, M. R. (1995). Toward a theory of situation
M., & Oetting, E. R. (2001). Further evidence awareness in dynamic systems. Human Factors,
of reliability and validity for the driving anger 37(1), 32–64.
Neuropsychological Performance and the Assessment of Driving Behavior 679

Fain, M. J. (2003). Should older drivers have to Freeman, E. E., Munoz, B., Turano, K. A., & West, S.
prove that they are able to drive? Archives of K. (2005). Measures of visual function and time to
Internal Medicine, 163(18), 2126–2128; discus- driving cessation in older adults. Optometry and
sion 2132. Vision Science, 82(8), 765–773.
Fell, J. C., & Voas, R. B. (2006). The effectiveness French, J. A., Kanner, A. M., Bautista, J., Abou-
of reducing illegal blood alcohol concentration Khalil, B., Browne, T., Harden, C. L., et al. (2004).
(BAC) limits for driving: Evidence for lowering Efficacy and tolerability of the new antiepilep-
the limit to .05 BAC. Journal of Safety Research, tic drugs I: Treatment of new onset epilepsy:
37(3), 233–243. Report of the Therapeutics and Technology
Fisk, G. D., Novack, T., Mennemeier, M., & Roenker, Assessment Subcommittee and Quality Standards
D. (2002a). Useful field of view after traumatic brain Subcommittee of the American Academy of
injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, Neurology and the American Epilepsy Society.
17(1), 16–25. Neurology, 62(8), 1252–1260.
Fisk, G. D., Owsley, C., & Mennemeier, M. (2002b). Freund, B., Colgrove, L. A., Burke, B. L., & McLeod,
Vision, attention, and self-reported driving behav- R. (2005). Self-rated driving performance among
iors in community-dwelling stroke survivors. elderly drivers referred for driving evaluation.
Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Accident Analysis and Prevention, 37(4), 613–618.
83(4), 469–477. Freund, B., Gravenstein, S., Ferris, R., Burke, B. L.,
Fisk, G. D., Schneider, J. J., & Novack, T. A. (1998). & Shaheen, E. (2005). Drawing clocks and driving
Driving following traumatic brain injury: cars. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 20(3),
Prevalence, exposure, advice and evaluations. 240–244.
Brain Injury, 12(8), 683–695. Freund, B., & Szinovacz, M. (2002). Effects of cogni-
Fitten, L. J. (2003). Driver screening for older adults. tion on driving involvement among the oldest old:
Archives of Internal Medicine, 163(18), 2129–2131; Variations by gender and alternative transporta-
discussion 2131. tion opportunities. Gerontologist, 42(5), 621–633.
Fitten, L. J., Perryman, K. M., Wilkinson, C. J., Little, Fried, R., Petty, C. R., Surman, C. B., Reimer,
R. J., Burns, M. M., Pachana, N., et al. (1995). B., Aleardi, M., Martin, J. M., et al. (2006).
Alzheimer and vascular dementias and driving. Characterizing impaired driving in adults with
A prospective road and laboratory study. Journal attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A con-
of the American Medical Association, 273(17), trolled study. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 67(4),
1360–1365. 567–574.
Foley, D. J., Masaki, K. H., Ross, G. W., & White, L. R. Fuller, R. (1984). A conceptualization of driving
(2000). Driving cessation in older men with inci- behaviour as threat avoidance. Ergonomics, 27(11),
dent dementia. Journal of the American Geriatrics 1139–1155.
Society, 48(8), 928–930. Galski, T., Bruno, R. L., & Ehle, H. T. (1992). Driving
Fonda, S. J., Wallace, R. B., & Herzog, A. R. (2001). after cerebral hemorrhage: A model with impli-
Changes in driving patterns and worsening cations for evaluation. The American Journal of
depressive symptoms among older adults. Journals Occupational Therapy, 46, 324–332.
of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Galski, T., Bruno, R. L., & Ehle, H. T. (1993).
Social Sciences, 56(6), S343–S351. Prediction of behind-the-wheel driving perfor-
Fox, G. K., Bowden, S. C., Bashford, G. M., & Smith, mance in patients with cerebral brain damage: A
D. S. (1997). Alzheimer’s Disease and driving: discriminant function analysis. American Journal
Prediction and assessment of driving perfor- of Occupational Therapy, 47(5), 391–396.
mance. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, Galski, T., Ehle, H. T., & Bruno, R. L. (1990). An
45, 949–953. assessment of measures to predict the outcome of
Fox, G. K., Bowden, S. C., & Smith, D. S. (1998). driving evaluations in patients with cerebral dam-
On-road assessment of driving competence after age. American Journal of Occupational Therapy,
brain impairment: Review of current practice and 44(8), 709–713.
recommendations for a standardized examination. Galski, T., Ehle, H. T., & Williams, J. B. (1997).
Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Off-road driving evaluations for persons with
79(10), 1288–1296. cerebral injury: A factor analytic study of pre-
Franzen, M. D., & Wilhelm, K. L. (1996). Conceptual driver and simulator testing. American Journal of
foundations of ecological validity in neuropsy- Occupational Therapy, 51(5), 352–359.
chological assessment. In R. J. S. C. J. Long (Ed.), Gilley, D. W., Wilson, R. S., Bennett, D. A., Stebbins,
Ecological validity of neuropsychological testing G. T., Bernard, B. A., Whalen, M. E., et al.
(pp. 91–112). Boca Raton, FL: St. Lucie Press. (1991). Cessation of driving and unsafe motor
680 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

vehicle operation by dementia patients. Archives of (Report No. DOT HS 808 939). Washington DC:
Internal Medicine, 151(5), 941–946. National Highway Traffic Safety Administration;
Gonzalez, R., Bechara, A., & Martin, E. M. U.S. Department of Transportation.
(2007). Executive functions among individu- Hobson, D. E., Lang, A. E., Martin, W. R., Razmy, A.,
als with methamphetamine or alcohol as drugs Rivest, J., & Fleming, J. (2002). Excessive daytime
of choice: Preliminary observations. Journal sleepiness and sudden-onset sleep in Parkinson
of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, disease: A survey by the Canadian Movement
29(2), 155–159. Disorders Group. Journal of the American Medical
Grace, J., Amick, M. M., D’Abreu, A., Festa, Association, 287(4), 455–463.
E. K., Heindel, W. C., & Ott, B. R. (2005). Hopewell, C. A. (2002). Driving assessment issues
Neuropsychological deficits associated with for practicing clinicians. Journal of Head Trauma
driving performance in Parkinson’s and Rehabilitation, 17(1), 48–61.
Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of the International Hunt, L., Morris, J. C., Edwards, D., & Wilson, B. S.
Neuropsychological Society, 11(6), 766–775. (1993). Driving performance in persons with mild
Grotenhermen, F. (2007). The toxicology of can- senile dementia of the Alzheimer type. Journal of
nabis and cannabis prohibition. Chemistry and the American Geriatrics Society, 41, 747–753.
Biodiversity, 4(8), 1744–1769. Hunt, L. A., Murphy, C. F., Carr, D., Duchek, J. M.,
Grotenhermen, F., Leson, G., Berghaus, G., Buckles, V., & Morris, J. C. (1997a). Environmental
Drummer, O. H., Kruger, H. P., Longo, M., et al. cueing may effect performance on a road test for
(2007). Developing limits for driving under can- drivers with dementia of the Alzheimer type.
nabis. Addiction, 102(12), 1910–1917. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders,
Guerrier, J. H., Manivannan, P., & Nair, S. N. (1999). 11(Suppl 1), 13–16.
The role of working memory, field dependence, Hunt, L. A., Murphy, C. F., Carr, D., Duchek, J. M.,
visual search, and reaction time in the left turn Buckles, V., & Morris, J. C. (1997b). Reliability of
performance of older female drivers. Applied the Washington University Road Test. A perfor-
Ergonomics, 30(2), 109–119. mance-based assessment for drivers with demen-
Gugerty, L. J. (1997). Situation awareness dur- tia of the Alzheimer type. Archives of Neurology,
ing driving: Explicit and implicit knowledge in 54(6), 707–712.
dynamic spatial memory. Journal of Experimental Hur, Y. M., & Bouchard, T. J., Jr. (1997). The genetic
Psychology-Applied, 3(1), 42–66. correlation between impulsivity and sensation
Hakamies-Blomqvist, L., & Siren, A. (2003). seeking traits. Behavior Genetics, 27(5), 455–463.
Deconstructing a gender difference: Driving Insurance Research Council. (1991). Adequacy of motor
cessation and personal driving history of older vehicle records in evaluating driver performance.
women. Journal of Safety Research, 34(4), Oak Brook, IL: Insurance Research Council.
383–388. Janke, M. K. (1991). Accidents, mileage, and the exag-
Haslegrave, C. M. (1991). Driving for handicapped geration of risk. Accident Analysis and Prevention,
people. International Disability Studies, 13(4), 23(2–3), 183–188.
111–120. Jerome, L., Segal, A., & Habinski, L. (2006). What
Heaton, R. K., Grant, I., Butters, N., White, D. we know about ADHD and driving risk: A liter-
A., Kirson, D., Atkinson, J. H., et al. (1995). The ature review, meta-analysis and critique. Journal
HNRC 500--neuropsychology of HIV infection of the Canadian Academy of Child and Adolescent
at different disease stages. HIV Neurobehavioral Psychiatry, 15(3), 105–125.
Research Center. Journal of the International Jonah, B. A. (1997). Sensation seeking and risky
Neuropsychological Society, 1(3), 231–251. driving: A review and synthesis of the literature.
Heikkila, V. M., & Kallanranta, T. (2005). Evaluation Accident Analysis and Prevention, 29(5), 651–665.
of the driving ability in disabled persons: A practi- Jovanovski, D., Erb, S., & Zakzanis, K. K. (2005).
tioners’ view. Disability and Rehabilitation, 27(17), Neurocognitive deficits in cocaine users: A quanti-
1029–1036. tative review of the evidence. Journal of Clinical and
Hilakivi, I., Veilahti, J., Asplund, P., Sinivuo, J., Experimental Neuropsychology, 27(2), 189–204.
Laitinen, L., & Koskenvuo, K. (1989). A sixteen- Kagerer, S., Winter, C., Moller, H. J., & Soyka, M.
factor personality test for predicting automo- (2003). Effects of haloperidol and atypical neuro-
bile driving accidents of young drivers. Accident leptics on psychomotor performance and driving
Analysis and Prevention, 21(5), 413–418. ability in schizophrenic patients. Results from an
Hindrik, W., Robbe, J., & O’Hanlon, J. F. (1999). experimental study. Neuropsychobiology, 47(4),
Marijuana, alcohol, and actual driving performance. 212–218.
Neuropsychological Performance and the Assessment of Driving Behavior 681

Kelly, E., Darke, S., & Ross, J. (2004). A review of Lezak, M. D. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment
drug use and driving: Epidemiology, impairment, (3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
risk factors and risk perceptions. Drug and Alcohol Lezak, M. D. (2004). Neuropsychological assessment
Review, 23(3), 319–344. (4th ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
Korteling, J. E., & Kaptein, N. A. (1996). Liguori, A., Gatto, C. P., & Robinson, J. H. (1998).
Neuropsychological driving fitness tests for Effects of marijuana on equilibrium, psychomotor
brain-damaged subjects. Archives of Physical performance, and simulated driving. Behavioural
Medicine and Rehabilitation, 77(2), 138–146. Pharmacology, 9(7), 599–609.
Kotterba, S., Orth, M., Eren, E., Fangerau, T., & Lincoln, N. B., & Radford, K. A. (2008). Cognitive
Sindern, E. (2003). Assessment of driving perfor- abilities as predictors of safety to drive in people
mance in patients with relapsing-remitting mul- with multiple sclerosis. Multiple Sclerosis, 14(1),
tiple sclerosis by a driving simulator. European 123–128.
Neurology, 50(3), 160–164. Lings, S. (2002). Driving accident frequency
Kramer, A. F., Cassavaugh, N., Horrey, W. J., Becic, increased in patients with multiple sclerosis. Acta
E., & Mayhugh, J. L. (2007). Influence of age and Neurologica Scandinavica, 105(3), 169–173.
proximity warning devices on collision avoid- Logan, B. K. (1996). Methamphetamine and driving
ance in simulated driving. Human Factors, 49(5), impairment. Journal of Forensic Sciences, 41(3),
935–949. 457–464.
Kuypers, K. P., Samyn, N., & Ramaekers, J. G. (2006). Loring, D. W. (1999). INS Dictionary of
MDMA and alcohol effects, combined and alone, Neuropsychology. New York: Oxford University
on objective and subjective measures of actual Press.
driving performance and psychomotor function. Lundberg, C., Hakamies-Blomqvist, L., Almkvist,
Psychopharmacology, 187(4), 467–475. O., & Johansson, K. (1998). Impairments of some
Lawton, C., Cook, S., May, A., Clemo, K., & Brown, cognitive functions are common in crash-involved
S. (2008). Postural support strategies of disabled older drivers. Accident Analysis and Prevention,
drivers and the effectiveness of postural support 30(3), 371–377.
aids. Applied Ergonomics, 39(1), 47–55. Lundberg, C., Johansson, K., Ball, K., Bjerre, B.,
Lee, H. C., Cameron, D., & Lee, A. H. (2003). Blomqvist, C., Braekhus, A., et al. (1997). Dementia
Assessing the driving performance of older and driving: An attempt at consensus. Alzheimer
adult drivers: On-road versus simulated driving. Disease and Associated Disorders, 11(1), 28–37.
Accident Analysis and Prevention, 35(5), 797–803. Lundqvist, A., & Alinder, J. (2007). Driving after
Lee, J. D., & Strayer, D. L. (2004). Preface to the spe- brain injury: Self-awareness and coping at the
cial section on driver distraction. Human Factors, tactical level of control. Brain Injury, 21(11),
46(4), 583–586. 1109–1117.
Lengenfelder, J., Schultheis, M. T., Al-Shihabi, Madeley, P., Hulley, J. L., Wildgust, H., & Mindham,
T., Mourant, R., & DeLuca, J. (2002). Divided R. H. (1990). Parkinson’s disease and driving
attention and driving: A pilot study using vir- ability. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and
tual reality technology. Journal of Head Trauma Psychiatry, 53(7), 580–582.
Rehabilitation, 17(1), 26–37. Mapstone, M., Rosler, A., Hays, A., Gitelman, D. R., &
Lenne, M. G., Dietze, P., Rumbold, G. R., Redman, Weintraub, S. (2001). Dynamic allocation of atten-
J. R., & Triggs, T. J. (2003). The effects of the opi- tion in aging and Alzheimer disease: Uncoupling
oid pharmacotherapies methadone, LAAM and of the eye and mind. Archives of Neurology, 58(9),
buprenorphine, alone and in combination with 1443–1447.
alcohol, on simulated driving. Drug and Alcohol Marcotte, T. D., Corey-Bloom, J., Rosenthal, T. J.,
Dependence, 72(3), 271–278. Roberts, E., Lampinen, S., & Allen., R. W. (2008).
Leon-Carrion, J., Dominguez-Morales, M. R., & The contribution of cognitive deficits and spastic-
Martin, J. M. (2005). Driving with cognitive defi- ity to driving performance in Multiple Sclerosi.
cits: Neurorehabilitation and legal measures are Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation,
needed for driving again after severe traumatic 89(9), 1753–1758.
brain injury. Brain Injury, 19(3), 213–219. Marcotte, T. D., Heaton, R. K., Wolfson, T., Taylor,
Lew, H. L., Poole, J. H., Lee, E. H., Jaffe, D. L., Huang, M. J., Alhassoon, O., Arfaa, K., et al. (1999). The
H. C., & Brodd, E. (2005). Predictive validity of impact of HIV-related neuropsychological dys-
driving-simulator assessments following trau- function on driving behavior. Journal of the
matic brain injury: A preliminary study. Brain International Neuropsychological Society, 5(7),
Injury, 19(3), 177–188. 579–592.
682 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

Marcotte, T. D., Lazzaretto, D., Scott, J. C., Roberts, Mazer, B. L., Sofer, S., Korner-Bitensky, N., Gelinas,
E., Woods, S. P., Letendre, S., et al. (2006). Visual I., Hanley, J., & Wood-Dauphinee, S. (2003).
attention deficits are associated with driving Effectiveness of a visual attention retraining
accidents in cognitively-impaired HIV-infected program on the driving performance of clients
individuals. Journal of Clinical and Experimental with stroke. Archives of Physical Medicine and
Neuropsychology, 28, 13–28. Rehabilitation, 84(4), 541–550.
Marcotte, T. D., & Scott, J. C. (2004). The assessment McGwin, G., Jr., Owsley, C., & Ball, K. (1998).
of driving abilities. Advances in Transportation Identifying crash involvement among older driv-
Studies: An International Journal, 79–90. ers: Agreement between self-report and state
Marcotte, T. D., Scott, J. C., Lazaretto, D., & Rosenthal, records. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 30(6),
T. J. (2004a). Long-term stability of standard 781–791.
deviation of lateral position in neurocognitively McKnight, A. J., & McKnight, A. S. (1999).
normal and impaired individuals. Advances in Multivariate analysis of age-related driver ability
Transportation Studies: An International Journal, and performance deficits. Accident Analysis and
December, 57–65. Prevention, 31(5), 445.
Marcotte, T. D., Wolfson, T., Rosenthal, T. J., Heaton, Meindorfner, C., Korner, Y., Moller, J. C., Stiasny-
R. K., Gonzalez, R., Ellis, R. J., et al. (2004b). A Kolster, K., Oertel, W. H., & Kruger, H. P. (2005).
multimodal assessment of driving performance in Driving in Parkinson’s disease: Mobility, acci-
HIV infection. Neurology, 63(8), 1417–1422. dents, and sudden onset of sleep at the wheel.
Marottoli, R. A. (1997). Crashes: Outcome of choice Movement Disorders, 20(7), 832–842.
in assessing driver safety? Alzheimer Disease and Michon, J. (1985). A critical review of driver behav-
Associated Disorders, 11(Suppl 1), 28–30. ior models: What do we know, what should we do?
Marottoli, R. A., Mendes de Leon, C. F., Glass, T. A., In L. Evans and R. Schwing (Eds.), Human behav-
Williams, C. S., Cooney, L. M., Jr., Berkman, L. ior and traffic safety (pp. 485–524). New York:
F., et al. (1997). Driving cessation and increased Plenum.
depressive symptoms: Prospective evidence from Moller, J. C., Stiasny, K., Hargutt, V., Cassel, W.,
the New Haven EPESE. Established Populations Tietze, H., Peter, J. H., et al. (2002). Evaluation
for Epidemiologic Studies of the Elderly. Journal of sleep and driving performance in six patients
of the American Geriatrics Society, 45(2), 202–206. with Parkinson’s disease reporting sudden onset
Marottoli, R. A., Ness, P. H., Araujo, K. L., Iannone, of sleep under dopaminergic medication: A pilot
L. P., Acampora, D., Charpentier, P., et al. (2007). study. Movement Disorders, 17(3), 474–481.
A randomized trial of an education program to Molnar, F. J., Patel, A., Marshall, S. C., Man-Son-
enhance older driver performance. Journals of Hing, M., & Wilson, K. G. (2006). Clinical util-
Gerontology. Series A, Biological Sciences and ity of office-based cognitive predictors of fitness
Medical Sciences, 62(10), 1113–1119. to drive in persons with dementia: A systematic
Marottoli, R. A., Richardson, E. D., Stowe, M. H., review. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society,
Miller, E. G., Brass, L. M., Cooney, L. M., Jr., et al. 54(12), 1809–1824.
(1998). Development of a test battery to iden- Morris, J. C., & Cummings, J. (2005). Mild cogni-
tify older drivers at risk for self-reported adverse tive impairment (MCI) represents early-stage
driving events. Journal of the American Geriatrics Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Alzheimer’s
Society, 46(5), 562–568. Disease, 7(3), 235–239; discussion 255–262.
Marshall, S., Man-Son-Hing, M., Molnar, F., Hunt, Moskowitz, H., & Robinson, C. (1988). Effects of low
L., & Finestone, H. (2005). An exploratory study dose alcohol on driving related skills. A review of
on the predictive elements of passing on-the-road the evidence. (Technical Report DOT FT 807 280).
tests for disabled persons. Traffic Injury Prevention, Springfield, VA: National Highway Traffic Safety
6(3), 235–239. Administration.
Marshall, S. C., Molnar, F., Man-Son-Hing, M., Blair, Moskowitz, H., & Smiley, A. (1982). Effects of chron-
R., Brosseau, L., Finestone, H. M., et al. (2007). ically administered buspirone and diazepam on
Predictors of driving ability following stroke: A driving-related skills performance. Journal of
systematic review. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation, Clinical Psychiatry, 43(12 Pt 2), 45–55.
14(1), 98–114. Myers, R. S., Ball, K. K., Kalina, T. D., Roth, D. L.,
Mayhew, D. R., Simpson, H. M., & Pak, A. (2003). & Goode, K. T. (2000). Relation of useful field of
Changes in collision rates among novice driv- view and other screening tests to on-road driving
ers during the first months of driving. Accident performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 91(1),
Analysis and Prevention, 35(5), 683–691. 279–290.
Neuropsychological Performance and the Assessment of Driving Behavior 683

National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. Ott, B. R., Festa, E. K., Amick, M. M., Grace, J., Davis,
(1999). Safe mobility for older people. Washington, J. D., & Heindel, W. C. (2008a). Computerized maze
DC.: U.S. Department of Transportation. navigation and on-road performance by drivers
Publication No. HS 808 853. with dementia. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and
National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. Neurology, 21(1), 18–25.
(2001). Traffic safety facts 2000: Older population. Ott, B. R., Heindel, W. C., Papandonatos, G. D., Festa,
Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Transportation. E. K., Davis, J. D., Daiello, L. A., et al. (2008b). A
Publication No. HS 809 328. longitudinal study of drivers with Alzheimer dis-
National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. ease. Neurology, 70(14), 1171–1178.
(2004). Traffic safety facts 2002: A compilation of Ott, B. R., Heindel, W. C., Whelihan, W. M., Caron,
motor vehicle crash data from the fatality analy- M. D., Piatt, A. L., & DiCarlo, M. A. (2003). Maze
sis reporting system and the general estimates test performance and reported driving ability in
system. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of early dementia. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and
Transportation. Publication No. HS 808 620. Neurology, 16(3), 151–155.
National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. Owsley. (1997). Clinical and research issues on older
(2006). Fatality analysis reporting system general drivers: Future directions. Alzheimer Disease and
estimates system: 2005 data summary. Washington, Associated Disorders, 11(Suppl 1), 3–7.
DC: U.S. Department of Transportation. Owsley, C., Ball, K., McGwin, G., Jr., Sloane, M. E.,
Publication No. HS 810 632. Roenker, D. L., White, M., et al. (1998). Visual
Neale, V. L., Dingus, T. A., Klauer, S. G., Sudweeks, processing impairment and risk of motor vehicle
J., & Goodman, M. (2005). An overview of the 100- crash among older adults. Journal of the American
car naturalistic study and findings (No. 05–0400). Medical Association, 279(14), 1083–1088.
Washington, DC: National Highway Traffic Safety Owsley, C., & McGwin, G., Jr. (1999). Vision impair-
Administration. ment and driving. Survey of Ophthalmology, 43(6),
Novack, T. A., Banos, J. H., Alderson, A. L., 535–550.
Schneider, J. J., Weed, W., Blankenship, J., et al. Owsley, C., McGwin, G., Jr., & McNeal, S. F. (2003).
(2006). UFOV performance and driving ability Impact of impulsiveness, venturesomeness, and
following traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, empathy on driving by older adults. Journal of
20(5), 455–461. Safety Research, 34(4), 353–359.
O’Hanlon, J. F., Brookhuis, K. A., Louwerens, J. W., Owsley, C., Stalvey, B., Wells, J., & Sloane, M. E. (1999).
& Volkerts, E. R. (1986). Performance testing as Older drivers and cataract: Driving habits and crash
part of drug registration. In J. F. O’Hanlon and J. risk. Journals of Gerontology. Series A, Biological
J. de Gier (Eds.), Drugs and driving (pp. 311–330). Sciences and Medical Sciences, 54(4), M203–M211.
London: Taylor and Francis. Paaver, M., Eensoo, D., Pulver, A., & Harro, J.
O’Neill, D., Neubauer, K., Boyle, M., Gerrard, J., (2006). Adaptive and maladaptive impulsivity,
Surmon, D., & Wilcock, G. K. (1992). Dementia platelet monoamine oxidase (MAO) activity and
and driving. Journal of the Royal Society of risk-admitting in different types of risky drivers.
Medicine, 85(4), 199–202. Psychopharmacology, 186(1), 32–40.
Odenheimer, G. L., Beaudet, M., Jette, A. M., Palmer, B. W., Heaton, R. K., Gladsjo, J. A., Evans,
Albert, M. S., Grande, L., & Minaker, K. L. (1994). J. D., Patterson, T. L., Golshan, S., et al. (2002).
Performance-based driving evaluation of the Heterogeneity in functional status among older
elderly driver: Safety, reliability, and validity. outpatients with schizophrenia: Employment his-
Journal of Gerontology, 49(4), M153–M159. tory, living situation, and driving. Schizophrenia
Ogden, E. J., & Moskowitz, H. (2004). Effects of alco- Research, 55(3), 205–215.
hol and other drugs on driver performance. Traffic Parasuraman, R., & Nestor, P. G. (1991). Attention
Injury Prevention, 5(3), 185–198. and driving skills in aging and Alzheimer’s dis-
Oltedal, S., & Rundmo, T. (2006). The effects of per- ease. Human Factors, 33(5), 539–557.
sonality and gender on risky driving behaviour Pettis, R. W. (1992). Tarasoff and the dangerous
and accident involvement. Safety Science, 44(7), driver: A look at the driving cases. Bulletin of the
621–628. American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law,
Orfei, M. D., Robinson, R. G., Prigatano, G. P., 20(4), 427–437.
Starkstein, S., Rusch, N., Bria, P., et al. (2007). Pettis, R. W., & Gutheil, T. G. (1993). Misapplication
Anosognosia for hemiplegia after stroke is a mul- of the Tarasoff duty to driving cases: A call for
tifaceted phenomenon: A systematic review of the a reframing of theory. Bulletin of the American
literature. Brain, 130(Pt 12), 3075–3090. Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 21(3), 263–275.
684 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

Pietrapiana, P., Tamietto, M., Torrini, G., Mezzanato, Rizzo, M., & Kellison, I. L. (in press). The brain on
T., Rago, R., & Perino, C. (2005). Role of premor- the road. In T. D. Marcotte and I. Grant (Eds.),
bid factors in predicting safe return to driving The neuropsychology of Everyday functioning. New
after severe TBI. Brain Injury, 19(3), 197–211. York: Guilford.
Pizza, F., Contardi, S., Ferlisi, M., Mondini, S., & Rizzo, M., McGehee, D. V., Dawson, J. D., & Anderson,
Cirignotta, F. (2008). Daytime driving simulation S. N. (2001). Simulated car crashes at intersections
performance and sleepiness in obstructive sleep in drivers with Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer
apnoea patients. Accident Analysis and Prevention, Disease and Associated Disorders, 15(1), 10–20.
40(2), 602–609. Rizzo, M., Reinach, S., McGehee, D., & Dawson, J.
Ponsford, A. S., Viitanen, M., Lundberg, C., & (1997). Simulated car crashes and crash predic-
Johansson, K. (2008). Assessment of driving after tors in drivers with Alzheimer disease. Archives of
stroke-A pluridisciplinary task. Accident Analysis Neurology, 54(5), 545–551.
and Prevention, 40(2), 452–460. Roenker, D. L., Cissell, G. M., Ball, K. K., Wadley, V.
Radford, K., Lincoln, N., & Lennox, G. (2004). The G., & Edwards, J. D. (2003). Speed-of-processing
effects of cognitive abilities on driving in peo- and driving simulator training result in improved
ple with Parkinson’s disease. Disability and driving performance. Human Factors, 45(2),
Rehabilitation, 26(2), 65–70. 218–233.
Ramaekers, J. G. (2003). Antidepressants and driver Rosenbloom, T., Perlman, A., & Shahar, A. (2007).
impairment: Empirical evidence from a standard Women drivers’ behavior in well-known versus
on-the-road test. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, less familiar locations. Journal of Safety Research,
64(1), 20–29. 38(3), 283–288.
Ramaekers, J. G., Berghaus, G., van Laar, M., & Sacktor, N., McDermott, M. P., Marder, K., Schifitto,
Drummer, O. H. (2004). Dose related risk of G., Selnes, O. A., McArthur, J. C., et al. (2002).
motor vehicle crashes after cannabis use. Drug and HIV-associated cognitive impairment before and
Alcohol Dependence, 73(2), 109–119. after the advent of combination therapy. Journal of
Ramaekers, J. G., Kuypers, K. P., & Samyn, N. (2006). Neurovirology, 8(2), 136–142.
Stimulant effects of 3,4-methylenedioxymetham- Schanke, A. K., & Sundet, K. (2000). Comprehensive
phetamine (MDMA) 75 mg and methylphenidate driving assessment: Neuropsychological testing
20 mg on actual driving during intoxication and and on-road evaluation of brain injured patients.
withdrawal. Addiction, 101(11), 1614–1621. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 41(2),
Ranney, T. A. (1994). Models of driving behavior: A 113–121.
review of their evolution. Accident Analysis and Schmidt, I. W., Brouwer, W. H., Vanier, M., & Kemp,
Prevention, 26(6), 733–750. F. (1996). Flexible adaptation to changing task
Rao, S. M., Leo, G. J., Bernardin, L., & Unverzagt, F. demands in severe closed head injury patients: A
(1991). Cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis. driving simulator study. Applied Neuropsychology,
I. Frequency, patterns, and prediction. Neurology, 3(3–4), 155–165.
41(5), 685–691. Schultheis, M. T., Garay, E., & DeLuca, J. (2001). The
Rapoport, M. J., & Banina, M. C. (2007). Impact of influence of cognitive impairment on driving per-
psychotropic medications on simulated driving: A formance in multiple sclerosis. Neurology, 56(8),
critical review. CNS Drugs, 21(6), 503–519. 1089–1094.
Rapport, L. J., Hanks, R. A., Millis, S. R., & Schultheis, M. T., Garay, E., Millis, S. R., & Deluca,
Deshpande, S. A. (1998). Executive functioning J. (2002). Motor vehicle crashes and violations
and predictors of falls in the rehabilitation setting. among drivers with multiple sclerosis. Archives
Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 83(8),
79(6), 629–633. 1175–1178.
Rebok, G. W., Bylsma, F. W., Keyl, P. M., Brandt, J., Schultheis, M. T., Hillary, F., & Chute, D. L. (2003).
& Folstein, S. E. (1995). Automobile driving in The neurocognitive driving test: Applying tech-
Huntington’s disease. Movement Disorders, 10(6), nology to the assessment of driving ability follow-
778–787. ing brain injury. Rehabilitation Psychology, 48(4),
Reger, M. A., Welsh, R. K., Watson, G. S., Cholerton, 275–280.
B., Baker, L. D., & Craft, S. (2004). The relation- Schultheis, M. T., Matheis, R. J., Nead, R., & DeLuca,
ship between neuropsychological functioning J. (2002). Driving behaviors following brain injury:
and driving ability in dementia: A meta-analysis. Self-report and motor vehicle records. Journal of
Neuropsychology, 18(1), 85–93. Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 17(1), 38–47.
Rizzo, M., & Kellison, I. L. (2004). Eyes, brains, and Schwebel, D. C., Ball, K. K., Severson, J., Barton, B. K.,
autos. Archives of Ophthalmology, 122(4), 641–647. Rizzo, M., & Viamonte, S. M. (2007). Individual
Neuropsychological Performance and the Assessment of Driving Behavior 685

difference factors in risky driving among older Stern, Y. (2006). Cognitive reserve and Alzheimer dis-
adults. Journal of Safety Research, 38(5), 501–509. ease. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders,
Schwebel, D. C., Severson, J., Ball, K. K., & Rizzo, M. 20(2), 112–117.
(2006). Individual difference factors in risky driv- Stolwyk, R. J., Charlton, J. L., Triggs, T. J., Iansek, R.,
ing: The roles of anger/hostility, conscientious- & Bradshaw, J. L. (2006). Neuropsychological func-
ness, and sensation-seeking. Accident Analysis tion and driving ability in people with Parkinson’s
and Prevention, 38(4), 801–810. disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Scott, J. C., Woods, S. P., Matt, G. E., Meyer, R. Neuropsychology, 28(6), 898–913.
A., Heaton, R. K., Atkinson, J. H., et al. (2007). Strahan, R. F., Mercier, C. R., Mercier, J. M., &
Neurocognitive effects of methamphetamine: A O’Boyle, M. W. (1997). Personality structure of
critical review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychology elderly drivers. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 85(2),
Review, 17(3), 275–297. 747–755.
Semple, S. J., Zians, J., Grant, I., & Patterson, T. L. Stuss, D. T., & Alexander, M. P. (2007). Is there a dys-
(2005). Impulsivity and methamphetamine use. executive syndrome? Philosophical Transactions
Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 29(2), of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological
85–93. Sciences, 362(1481), 901–915.
Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled Stutts, J. C. (1998). Do older drivers with visual and
and automatic human information-processing 2. cognitive impairments drive less? Journal of the
Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a gen- American Geriatrics Society, 46(7), 854–861.
eral theory. Psychological Review, 84(2), 127–190. Stutts, J. C., Reinfurt, D. W., & Rodgman, E. A.
Shua-Haim, J. R., & Gross, J. S. (1996). The “co- (2001). The role of driver distraction in crashes:
pilot” driver syndrome. Journal of the American An analysis of 1995–1999 Crashworthiness Data
Geriatrics Society, 44(7), 815–817. System Data. Annual Proceedings/Association for
Simons, J. S., Oliver, M. N., Gaher, R. M., Ebel, the Advancement of Automotive Medicine, 45,
G., & Brummels, P. (2005). Methamphetamine 287–301.
and alcohol abuse and dependence symptoms: Summala, H. (1996). Accident risk and driver behav-
Associations with affect lability and impulsivity in iour. Safety Science, 22(1–3), 103–117.
a rural treatment population. Addictive Behaviors, Sundet, K., Goffeng, L., & Hofft, E. (1995). To drive
30(7), 1370–1381. or not to drive: Neuropsychological assess-
Sivak, M., Olson, P. L., Kewman, D. G., Won, H., ment for driver’s license among stroke patients.
& Henson, D. L. (1981). Driving and percep- Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 36(1), 47–58.
tual/cognitive skills: Behavioral consequences of Szlyk, J. P., Myers, L., Zhang, Y., Wetzel, L., &
brain damage. Archives of Physical Medicine and Shapiro, R. (2002). Development and assessment
Rehabilitation, 62(10), 476–483. of a neuropsychological battery to aid in predict-
Sodhi, M., Reimer, B., & Llamazares, I. (2002). Glance ing driving performance. Journal of Rehabilitation
analysis of driver eye movements to evaluate dis- Research and Development, 39(4), 483–496.
traction. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, Szlyk, J. P., Seiple, W., & Viana, M. (1995). Relative
and Computers, 34(4), 529–538. effects of age and compromised vision on driving
Spooner, D. M., & Pachana, N. A. (2006). Ecological performance. Human Factors, 37(2), 430–436.
validity in neuropsychological assessment: A case Tamietto, M., Torrini, G., Adenzato, M., Pietrapiana,
for greater consideration in research with neuro- P., Rago, R., & Perino, C. (2006). To drive or not to
logically intact populations. Archives of Clinical drive (after TBI)? A review of the literature and its
Neuropsychology, 21(4), 327–337. implications for rehabilitation and future research.
Squire, L. R. (1993). Neuropsychology of memory (2nd NeuroRehabilitation, 21(1), 81–92.
ed.). New York: The Guilford Press. Thomas, R. E. (1998). Benzodiazepine use and motor
St Germain, S. A., Kurtz, M. M., Pearlson, G. D., vehicle accidents. Systematic review of reported
& Astur, R. S. (2005). Driving simulator perfor- association. Canadian Family Physician, 44,
mance in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 799–808.
74(1), 121–122. Tillmann, W. A., & Hobbs, G. E. (1949). The acci-
Stephens, B., McCarthy, D. P., Marsiske, M., dent-prone automobile driver; a study of the psy-
Classen, S., Shechtman, O., & Mann, W. C. (2005). chiatric and social background. American Journal
International older driver consensus conference of Psychiatry, 106(5), 321–331.
on assessment, remediation, and counseling for Trobe, J. D., Waller, P. F., Cook-Flannagan, C. A.,
transportation alternatives: Summary and recom- Teshima, S. M., & Bieliauskas, L. A. (1996). Crashes
mendations. Physical and Occupational Therapy in and violations among drivers with Alzheimer dis-
Geriatrics, 23(2/3), 103–121. ease. Archives of Neurology, 53(5), 411–416.
686 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

Uc, E. Y., Rizzo, M., Anderson, S. W., Shi, Q., & Verster, J. C., Bekker, E. M., de Roos, M., Minova,
Dawson, J. D. (2004). Driver route-following and A., Eijken, E. J., Kooij, J. J., et al. (2008).
safety errors in early Alzheimer disease. Neurology, Methylphenidate significantly improves driv-
63(5), 832–837. ing performance of adults with attention-
Uc, E. Y., Rizzo, M., Anderson, S. W., Shi, Q., & deficit hyperactivity disorder: A randomized
Dawson, J. D. (2005). Driver landmark and traf- crossover trial. Journal of Psychopharmacology,
fic sign identification in early Alzheimer’s disease. 22(3), 230–237.
Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, Verster, J. C., Volkerts, E. R., & Verbaten, M. N.
76(6), 764–768. (2002). Effects of alprazolam on driving ability,
Uc, E. Y., Rizzo, M., Anderson, S. W., Shi, Q., & memory functioning and psychomotor perfor-
Dawson, J. D. (2006a). Unsafe rear-end collision mance: A randomized, placebo-controlled study.
avoidance in Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of the Neuropsychopharmacology, 27(2), 260–269.
Neurological Sciences, 251(1–2), 35–43. Vorona, R. D., & Ware, J. C. (2002). Sleep disordered
Uc, E. Y., Rizzo, M., Anderson, S. W., Sparks, breathing and driving risk. Current Opinion in
J., Rodnitzky, R. L., & Dawson, J. D. (2006b). Pulmonary Medicine, 8(6), 506–510.
Impaired visual search in drivers with Parkinson’s Vrkljan, B. H., & Polgar, J. M. (2007). Driving, nav-
disease. Annals of Neurology, 60(4), 407–413. igation, and vehicular technology: Experiences
Uc, E. Y., Rizzo, M., Anderson, S. W., Sparks, J. D., of older drivers and their co-pilots. Traffic Injury
Rodnitzky, R. L., & Dawson, J. D. (2006c). Driving Prevention, 8(4), 403–410.
with distraction in Parkinson disease. Neurology, Wallis, T. S., & Horswill, M. S. (2007). Using fuzzy
67(10), 1774–1780. signal detection theory to determine why experi-
Underwood, G. (2007). Visual attention and the tran- enced and trained drivers respond faster than nov-
sition from novice to advanced driver. Ergonomics, ices in a hazard perception test. Accident Analysis
50(8), 1235–1249. and Prevention, 39(6), 1177–1185.
Underwood, G., Chapman, P., Brocklehurst, N., Walter, H., Vetter, S. C., Grothe, J., Wunderlich,
Underwood, J., & Crundall, D. (2003). Visual A. P., Hahn, S., & Spitzer, M. (2001). The neu-
attention while driving: Sequences of eye fi xa- ral correlates of driving. Neuroreport, 12(8),
tions made by experienced and novice drivers. 1763–1767.
Ergonomics, 46(6), 629–646. Wang, C. C., & Carr, D. B. (2004). Older driver safety:
Valcour, V. G., Masaki, K. H., & Blanchette, P. L. A report from the older drivers project. Journal of
(2002). Self-reported driving, cognitive status, the American Geriatrics Society, 52(1), 143–149.
and physician awareness of cognitive impairment. Wang, C. C., Kosinski, C. J., Schwartzberg, J. G.,
Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 50(7), & Shanklin, A. V. (2003). Physician’s guide
1265–1267. to assessing and counseling older drivers.
van Laar, M., Volkerts, E., & Verbaten, M. (2001). Washington, DC: National Highway Traffic
Subchronic effects of the GABA-agonist lora- Safety Administration.
zepam and the 5-HT2A/2C antagonist ritan- Weiler, J. M., Bloomfield, J. R., Woodworth, G. G.,
serin on driving performance, slow wave sleep Grant, A. R., Layton, T. A., Brown, T. L., et al. (2000).
and daytime sleepiness in healthy volunteers. Effects of fexofenadine, diphenhydramine, and
Psychopharmacology, 154(2), 189–197. alcohol on driving performance: A randomized pla-
van Laar, M. W., van Willigenburg, A. P., & Volkerts, cebo-controlled trial in the Iowa Driving Simulator.
E. R. (1995). Acute and subchronic effects of nefa- Annals of Internal Medicine, 132, 354–363.
zodone and imipramine on highway driving, cog- Wein, C., Koch, H., Popp, B., Oehler, G., & Schauder,
nitive functions, and daytime sleepiness in healthy P. (2004). Minimal hepatic encephalopathy impairs
adult and elderly subjects. Journal of Clinical fitness to drive. Hepatology, 39(3), 739–745.
Psychopharmacology, 15(1), 30–40. Whelihan, W. M., DiCarlo, M. A., & Paul, R. H.
van Zomeren, A. H., Brouwer, W. H., & Minderhoud, (2005). The relationship of neuropsychologi-
J. M. (1987). Acquired brain damage and driv- cal functioning to driving competence in older
ing: A review. Archives of Physical Medicine and persons with early cognitive decline. Archives of
Rehabilitation, 68(10), 697–705. Clinical Neuropsychology, 20(2), 217–228.
Vermeeren, A., & O’Hanlon, J. F. (1998). Wild, K., & Cotrell, V. (2003). Identifying driving
Fexofenadine’s effects, alone and with alcohol, impairment in Alzheimer disease: A comparison
on actual driving and psychomotor performance. of self and observer reports versus driving evalua-
Journal of Allergy and Clinical Immunology, 101(3), tion. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders,
306–311. 17(1), 27–34.
Neuropsychological Performance and the Assessment of Driving Behavior 687

Wilde, G. J. S. (1982). The theory of risk hoeostasis: Wood, J. M., Worringham, C., Kerr, G., Mallon,
Implications for safety and health. Risk Analysis, K., & Silburn, P. (2005). Quantitative assessment
2, 209–225. of driving performance in Parkinson’s disease.
Willemsen, J., Dula, C. S., Declercq, F., & Verhaeghe, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry,
P. (2008). The Dula Dangerous Driving Index: 76(2), 176–180.
An investigation of reliability and validity across World Health Organization. (2004). World report
cultures. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 40(2), on road traffic injury prevention. Geneva: World
798–806. Health Organization.
Windsor, T. D., & Anstey, K. J. (2006). Interventions Worringham, C. J., Wood, J. M., Kerr, G. K., &
to reduce the adverse psychosocial impact of driv- Silburn, P. A. (2006). Predictors of driving assess-
ing cessation on older adults. Clinical Interventions ment outcome in Parkinson’s disease. Movement
in Aging, 1(3), 205–211. Disorders, 21(2), 230–235.
Wingen, M., Bothmer, J., Langer, S., & Ramaekers, Yale, S. H., Hansotia, P., Knapp, D., & Ehrfurth, J.
J. G. (2005). Actual driving performance and psy- (2003). Neurologic conditions: Assessing medical
chomotor function in healthy subjects after acute fitness to drive. Clinical Medicine and Research,
and subchronic treatment with escitalopram, mir- 1(3), 177–188.
tazapine, and placebo: A crossover trial. Journal of Yuen, H. K., Sharma, A. K., Logan, W. C., Gillespie,
Clinical Psychiatry, 66(4), 436–443. M. B., Day, T. A., & Brooks, J. O. (2008). Radiation
Wingen, M., Ramaekers, J. G., & Schmitt, J. A. dose, driving performance, and cognitive func-
(2006). Driving impairment in depressed patients tion in patients with head and neck cancer.
receiving long-term antidepressant treatment. Radiotherapy and Oncology, 87(2), 304–307.
Psychopharmacology, 188(1), 84–91. Zesiewicz, T. A., Cimino, C. R., Malek, A. R., Gardner,
Withaar, F. K., Brouwer, W. H., & van Zomeren, A. N., Leaverton, P. L., Dunne, P. B., et al. (2002).
H. (2000). Fitness to drive in older drivers with Driving safety in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology,
cognitive impairment. Journal of the International 59(11), 1787–1788.
Neuropsychological Society, 6(4), 480–490. Zuckerman, M., Bone, R. N., Neary, R., Mangelsdorff,
Wood, J. M. (2002). Age and visual impairment D., & Brustman, B. (1972). What is the sensation
decrease driving performance as measured on seeker? Personality trait and experience correlates of
a closed-road circuit. Human Factors, 44(3), the Sensation-Seeking Scales. Journal of Consulting
482–494. and Clinical Psychology, 39(2), 308–321.
28

Neuropsychological Function and Adherence


to Medical Treatments
Steven A. Castellon, Charles H. Hinkin, Matthew J. Wright, and
Terry R. Barclay

A recurrent theme throughout this text is that We have chosen to focus on three specific pop-
neuropsychological assessment plays an impor- ulations, arguably those that have been best
tant role in the characterization of various dis- studied in regard to adherence and cognition.
eases and disorders. The methods to reliably We will examine findings in both normal and
measure cognitive functioning and the sig- pathological aging and also in HIV/AIDS. The
nature neuropsychological deficits associated chapter then introduces important theoreti-
with various disorders have been the traditional cal models that have been developed to explain
focus of most neuropsychology textbooks, adherence behavior, including theories such
this book included. More recently, however, as as the Health Beliefs Model and Social Action
described in previous chapters, neuropsycholo- Theory. Finally, we conclude with a brief review
gists and their colleagues are striving to bring a and discussion of interventions designed to
better empirical and clinical understanding of improve treatment adherence in patients with
the impact of neuropsychological compromise cognitive compromise.
on various aspects of “real-world” functioning,
including driving, employment status, self-
The Impact and Importance
care, and money management. In this chapter,
of Adherence
we review another important potential “real-
world” consequence of neurocognitive impair- Former United States Surgeon General C.
ment, its impact upon medication adherence. Everett Koop reminded us, “Drugs don’t work
In the following sections we present an over- if patients don’t take them,” and the clinical and
view of the current knowledge regarding the scientific community has spent a lot of energy,
complex nature of adherence to medication reg- time, and money attempting to figure out why
imens, with a specific focus on neurocognitive some patients do not take their medications.
factors that are associated with adherence. We For many medications to be fully beneficial,
start by discussing the importance of adherence they require the individual taking them to
in disease control/management and the stag- closely follow a prescribed regimen, and suc-
gering costs of nonadherence, and review issues cessful adherence is typically associated with
related to adherence measurement. The major- a variety of positive outcomes. Unfortunately,
ity of the chapter specifically explores the effect poor or suboptimal compliance to medication
of neurocognitive compromise on medication regimens is surprisingly common, economi-
adherence, including the potential impact of cally staggering, and associated with greatly
comorbidities such as substance abuse and/or increased morbidity and mortality, including
psychiatric disorder that can interact with cog- declines in overall health and increased risk of
nitive dysfunction to affect adherence behavior. hospitalization as well as mortality (Callahan &

688
Adherence to Medical Treatments 689

Wolinsky, 1995; Col et al., 1990; Ganguli et al., adherence is particularly important, as many
2002; Gilmer et al., 2004; Hepke et al., 2004; patients will suffer symptomatic relapse and will
Huybrechts et al., 2005; Sokol et al., 2005; Vik then require hospitalization secondary to poor
et al., 2006). adherence. For example, the annual health-care
How common is poor adherence? It certainly costs associated with schizophrenia have been
depends on the specific disease, condition, or estimated at 32.5 billion dollars, the majority
population being studied, the length of time of which have been attributed to the direct or
that the given group is studied, and the methods indirect effects of medication nonadherence
used to study them. It also depends, of course, (Thieda et al., 2003). Many patients, especially
on the definition of adherence that is used, but, if the elderly living on fi xed incomes, will take
adherence is defined as successfully taking one’s less medication than prescribed in an attempt
expected medications at their expected times, to lower or avoid costs, ironically putting them-
some easily supported generalizations can be selves at much higher risk for higher costs sec-
made. The literature suggests that adherence to ondary to acute illness and/or hospitalization
medication regimens is, at best, moderate and (Briesacher et al., 2007).
almost always declines over time in chronic dis- Not only are adverse outcomes associated
eases (Benner et al., 2002; Dunbar-Jacob et al., with nonadherence, but several positive out-
2000; Osterberg & Blaschke, 2005). In fact, in comes are seen among the medically adherent.
spite of association between poor adherence Adequate medication adherence can prevent,
and adverse outcome, rates of compliance are reduce, or modify the effects of many chronic
lower than 50% in most studies (for review, see illnesses and is generally associated with
Dunbar-Jacob et al., 2000; Haynes et al., 2003). improved health outcomes (Gray et al., 2001;
Adherence rates are often highest in clinical tri- Horwitz et al., 1990). For example, it has been
als due to the increased attention and scrutiny demonstrated that adherence to a consistent
that study participants receive and the more antihypertensive therapy is associated with a
stringent selection criteria driving many trials, 35%–40% lower incidence of stroke, a 20%–25%
but even in clinical trial settings for chronic dis- reduction in myocardial infarction, and a more
eases, adherence rates only rarely exceed 75% than 50% reduction in heart failure (Neal et al.,
(Osterberg & Blaschke, 2005). Rates of adher- 2000). A recent review by Sokol and colleagues
ence are typically higher among patients with (2005) found that among four of the leading
acute conditions compared to those with chronic drivers of health-care costs (diabetes, hyper-
conditions, and medication adherence rates tend cholesterolemia, high blood pressure, and con-
to drop rapidly after the first 6 months of treat- gestive heart failure), hospitalization rates were
ment (Benner et al., 2002; Haynes et al., 1996; significantly lower for patients with high levels
Jackevicius et al., 2002). As we discuss in detail of medication adherence and that, for all condi-
below, when adherence is assessed by means of tions, a net economic gain—that is, higher costs
patient self-report, rates tend to be higher, some- for medication but lower overall costs, includ-
times markedly so, than when adherence is ing hospitalization or acute, emergency care—is
measured using more objective indicators, such associated with good adherence as well (Sokol
as pill counts or electronic monitoring (Arnsten et al., 2005). A review of insurance company
et al., 2001; Levine et al., 2005). patients undergoing diabetes treatment found
The financial costs of poor adherence are dif- higher “sense of disease control” and higher
ficult to accurately estimate but, even according overall “sense of well-being” among those who
to conservative estimates, impressive. A recent were more adherent—not surprisingly, these
review in JAMA suggested that between one- patients were also using fewer additional medi-
third and two-thirds of all medically oriented cal care services (Hepke et al., 2004).
hospital admissions were due, at least in part, Further testimony to the overall salience of
to poor medication adherence, with an esti- medication adherence is the fairly dramatic
mated annual cost approaching 100 billion dol- proliferation of adherence-related research in
lars (Osterberg & Blaschke, 2005). Among the the last several years, including studies designed
severely mentally ill, medication maintenance/ to better understand how to improve treatment
690 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

adherence, or at least modify risk of nonadher- medication costs, degree of family or social sup-
ence. In fact, the World Health Organization port, patients’ beliefs and attitudes regarding
recently published an evidence-based guide health, and level of health literacy have all been
for clinicians and others to improve strate- shown to impact adherence behavior. Of course,
gies of medication adherence (World Health a combination of these factors may interact to
Organization, 2003), recognizing the magni- impact adherence, and the same patient may
tude of the deleterious effects of nonadherence. show adherence difficulties for different reasons
Given the importance of adherence, it is not sur- across time (e.g., when first started taking blood
prising that there is a rich literature associated pressure medications, forgetfulness secondary
with trying to better understand its predictors. to stroke, then, later in the disease course, felt
the medication was too expensive and stopped
refi lling it) or across different medications at
Defining and Measuring
the same point in time (e.g., patients do not take
Adherence
their antidepressant medication because they
Adherence can be defined broadly as the extent feel it causes side effects, while they consistently
to which a person’s behavior conforms to the skip their blood pressure medication because
advice or directives given them by their physi- they do not believe it is really needed).
cian (Bruer, 1982). As it relates more specifically
to pill-taking behavior, adherence is broadly
Adherence Measurement
defined as the degree to which patients take
Methodologies
medications as prescribed by their health-care
providers (Osterberg & Blaschke, 2005). An There are a number of techniques that have
implied aspect of medication adherence is that been used to measure medication adherence, all
medications are taken accurately, with the proper of which are characterized by unique strengths
route of administration, in the correct dosage, and weaknesses, with no single measurement
at the appropriate time, and in accordance with gold standard as yet. These can be divided into
any special instructions (Gould et al., 1999). techniques that are more objective, such as
Although there are several other types of adher- plasma drug levels and electronic measuring
ence, including faithfully attending health-care devices, and those that are more subjective,
appointments, implementing lifestyle changes such as patient self-report or clinician ratings.
(e.g., exercise, smoking cessation), and making Table 28–1 lists several of the methods for mea-
dietary changes, the main focus of most clini- suring adherence, which we briefly discuss
cal and research effort regarding adherence has below.
been with pill-taking behavior.
Clearly, determining the causes of poor med-
Subjective Methods
ication adherence and using that knowledge
to structure effective interventions is critically Self-Report. Self-report is perhaps the most
needed. What the existing literature suggests commonly used technique for gathering adher-
so far is that treatment of nonadherence is a ence data. Reliance on patient self-report has
complex, multidetermined problem. Studies several decided strengths, most notably its neg-
have found poor medication adherence to be ligible cost and ease of data collection. However,
associated with a host of factors, including there are several substantial limitations to this
cognitive function, substance use, psychiatric approach. Foremost, multiple studies have
disturbance, regimen complexity, medication shown that many patients may overstate their
efficacy, the occurrence of side effects, and the actual adherence rates. For example, studies of
chronicity of the disease/illness/condition being HIV-infected adults have revealed that patient
treated. Also, human factors (e.g., packaging self-report, relative to electronic monitoring
and labeling of medication bottles, grade level techniques, tends to be accurate among patients
at which health-related materials are written), who candidly admit to poor adherence but may
physicians’ interaction and communication overestimate actual adherence rates by approxi-
style, patients’ financial resources in relation to mately 10%–20% among a large subset of patients
Adherence to Medical Treatments 691

Table 28–1. Methods of Assessing Medication Adherence


Objective measures Analog measures Subjective measures
Blood Levels Laboratory Performances Self-Report
Pro: Plasma drug levels can be Pro: Assesses the ability to Pro: Simple collection (e.g., ver-
used as objective markers of self-administer and man- bal report, questionnaire
medication adherence. age fictitious medications endorsements).
Con: Only useful for recently (e.g., sorting, planning for Con: Biased toward overestimating
taken medications; not useful trips). rates of adherence.
for ascertaining adherence Con: The relationship Con: Degree of bias often varies as a
rates unless frequent blood between laboratory and function of participant characteris-
draws are completed. objective measures is not tics (e.g., personality, motives).
well studied.
Pill Counts Con: Laboratory tasks may Clinician Ratings
Pro: Straightforward approach not simulate participant’s Pro: Clinical rating scales regarding a
given that initial pill count, true medication regimen participant’s acceptance of and level
number of pills to be-taken, demands. of participation in their medication
and the number of pills regimen are easily obtained.
remaining after the study Virtual Reality Performances Con: Participants often portray their
period. Pro: Virtual environment attitudes toward medication adher-
Con: Participants can easily adjust can simulate real-world ence as being more favorable than
their pill counts to appear more settings in which medica- they actually are and clinical ratings
adherent. This can be reduced tion management can be tend to overestimate adherence
by conducting unannounced assessed. rates.
pill counts. Con: The association between
virtual reality and
Electronic Monitoring [Medication real-world measures are
Event Monitoring System unclear.
(MEMS)] Con: Some older participants
Pro: MEMS utilizes pill bottle may have difficulty adapt-
caps with a microchip that ing to the virtual reality
records date, time, and dura- environment.
tion of pill bottle opening.
Con: The MEMS cap pill bottle
is bulky, which can result in
participants’ removing extra
doses from the bottle for later
use (“pocket dosing”).
Con: MEMS cap use precludes the
use of pillbox organizers.

Pharmacy Refill Records


Pro: Cost-effective alternative
method for the indirect mea-
surement of adherence.
Con: Requires centralized phar-
macy records (e.g., Veterans
Administration Medical
Centers computerized records).
Con: Assumes that participants
refi lling their medications in a
timely manner are adherent.

who claim perfect or near-perfect adherence pill count include its minimal cost and ease of
(Arnsten et al., 2001; Levine et al., 2005). data collection. The technique itself is relatively
straightforward. If one knows how many pills a
Pill Counts. Pill counts are another technique patient initially possessed and how many pills
that have been used to measure adherence they should have ingested in the intervening
rates. Similar to self-report, the benefits of a time period, it is easy to calculate the number of
692 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

pills that should remain at the end of the study automatically records the date, time, and dura-
period. Excess doses are therefore considered to tion of pill bottle opening. Although electronic
reflect doses not taken as prescribed. For exam- monitoring devices are not without their own
ple, consider a patient on a regimen of 3 pills/day limitations, a number of studies have demon-
who begins with 100 pills and returns to clinic strated their incremental validity relative to pill
30 days later. If 10 pills remain, that would be counts or self-reports, which frequently overesti-
interpreted as perfect adherence (100 – (30 × 3) mate adherence rates. Drawbacks of this method
= 10). While easy for the researcher/clinician to include the bulky nature of the MEMS cap bottle,
calculate, a decided drawback is that this is easy which precludes inconspicuous transportation
for patients to calculate as well. Accordingly, of one’s medications. This can lead to behavior
prior to their return to clinic, patients may called “pocket-dosing” in which patients remove
remove extra doses from their pill bottle and an extra dose from their pill bottle to consume
thus appear more adherent than is actually the at a later point in time rather than carry their
case. pill bottle with them. Also, in the past, the use of
David Bangsberg and his colleagues at UCSF MEMS devices has precluded use of daily/weekly
(Bangsberg et al., 2001) have introduced an inno- pill organizers, although technological advances
vative approach to overcome this limitation. They are now emerging that will overcome this limita-
conduct “unannounced pill counts” and appear at tion in the future.
participants’ residences without warning to con-
duct pill counts. They have found this approach Pharmacy Refill Records. Pharmacy refi ll
to correlate well with biologic outcomes (e.g., HIV records have been shown to be a cost-effective
viral load). While this methodology works well means of gathering indirect data regarding
in a dense urban community, it would be exces- medication adherence. This technique is pred-
sively cumbersome for use in sparsely populated icated on the assumption that if patients are
rural settings or in a sprawling metropolis with- refi lling their medication prescriptions in a
out public transportation such as Los Angeles. timely fashion, then they are more likely to be
To overcome such geographical difficulties, taking their medication as prescribed as com-
Kalichman and colleagues (2007) have adapted pared to individuals who are tardy in refilling
this technique and have attempted unannounced their prescriptions. This approach works best in
pill counts conducted by telephone; they have settings where pharmacy records are central-
found this method to correlate highly with unan- ized and can be easily attained (e.g., in Veterans
nounced in-person pill counts. Administration Medical Centers).

Biologic Markers. Blood levels provide precise


quantification of adherence for medications with
Objective Methods
a long half-life. For example, blood tests are an
Electronic Measuring Devices. The above refer- excellent means for ascertaining lithium levels
enced limitations associated with self-report may and, by extension, whether patients with bipolar
be particularly pronounced when dealing with disorder are indeed taking their lithium carbon-
individuals with neurocognitive impairment. ate as prescribed. In contrast, blood tests are not
Patients with dementia frequently encounter as useful for evaluating adherence rates for med-
difficulty recalling whether they took their pre- ications that are rapidly metabolized. In such
scribed medication. This is especially problematic cases blood levels can detect whether patients
when self-reported adherence rates are assessed have recently taken their medications but cannot
over lengthier time periods. For this reason, the assist in determining whether patients typically
utilization of electronic monitoring devices (e.g., take their medication as prescribed.
Medication Event Monitoring System [MEMS],
Aprex Corp, Union City, California) may pro-
vide a more accurate measure of actual adher- Laboratory-Based Analog Measures
ence rates. MEMS caps employ a computer chip In addition to assessing actual medication
that is embedded in the top of a pill bottle that adherence, several investigators have developed
Adherence to Medical Treatments 693

laboratory-based measures thought to be reflec- et al., 2003; Patterson et al., 2002). Interestingly,
tive of one’s ability to adhere to medical recom- the MMAA was recently studied in relation-
mendations. Coincidently, two separate groups ship to a virtual reality (VR) task designed to
of investigators (Albert et al., 1999; Gurland simulate the medication-taking environment of
et al., 1994) independently developed analog schizophrenic participants (Baker et al., 2006).
measures of medication management skills by Like the MMAA, the experimental VR task cor-
the same name—The Medication Management related with memory and executive function-
Test (MMT). Gurland et al.’s version (MMT- ing, but it also showed significant relationship
Gurland) was initially developed to assess the with sustained attention.
ability of older adults to self-administer medica-
tions, while Albert et al.’s edition (MMT-Albert)
Impact of Cognitive Function on
was created to assess medication management
Adherence in Select Populations
skills among HIV-infected individuals. Both
tests entail sorting, organizing, and making
inferences about fictitious medications (e.g., Normal Aging
when a prescription would need to be refi lled). Among elderly individuals, medication adher-
The MMT-Albert is more in-depth and requires ence is particularly salient as older persons con-
15–25 minutes to administer; the MMT-Gurland sume a disproportionate amount of prescription
takes approximately 5 minutes to administer. medications. In North America, individuals
The MMT-Gurland has been shown to be asso- over the age of 65 make up approximately 15%
ciated with cognitive decline in older adults of the population but consume nearly 35%–40%
(Fulmer & Gurland, 1997; Gurland et al., 1994). of all prescription medications (Vik et al., 2006).
The MMT-Albert has been associated with cog- Elderly patients are at much greater likelihood of
nitive deficits in HIV-infected individuals, spe- suffering from chronic disease that will require
cifically difficulties in memory, executive, and some degree of adherence to one or more drugs
motor functioning (Albert et al., 1999, 2003). (Gurwitz et al., 2003; Vik et al., 2004; Williams
The MMT-Albert has been further revised by & Kim, 2005). In fact, it has been estimated
Patterson et al. (2002; Medication Management that upward of 70% of all ambulatory, nonin-
Ability Assessment; MMAA) and Heaton et al. stitutionalized elderly persons take some form
(2004; Medication Management Test-Revised; of medication, and often multiple medications.
MMT-R). Heaton et al.’s adaptation included Unfortunately, estimates of suboptimal medi-
reordering test items by ascending order of dif- cation adherence among this group of elderly,
ficulty, rewording some test items and the mock noninstitutionalized patients approach (or even
medication insert, as well as reducing the num- slightly exceed) 50% in many studies (Barat
ber of fictitious medications (from 5 to 3) and et al., 2001; Dolder et al., 2003; Gray et al., 2001;
inference items (from 15 to 7). The MMT-R Salzman, 1995). As stated above, the risk for
requires approximately 10 minutes to adminis- adherence failure is accentuated in individuals
ter and has been shown to correlate with neuro- suffering from chronic diseases, and therefore
psychological deficits in executive function and antihypertensive medications, lipid-lowering
memory in HIV-infected individuals (Heaton drugs, and antiarthritic medications are among
et al., 2004). To better characterize the possi- the most common victims of nonadherence
ble medication management problems faced by in elderly individuals (Bennet et al., 2002;
individuals suffering from schizophrenia, the Chapman et, al., 2005).
MMAA was modified from the MMT-Albert to Well known to neuro- and geropsycholo-
better mimic interactions between patients and gists are the cognitive changes that can accom-
prescribing physicians (Patterson et al., 2002). pany the aging process. Normal aging often
It also requires examinees to demonstrate how results in some degree of slowing of process-
they would self-administer medications after a ing speed, difficulty with more complex or
1-hour delay. Performance on the MMAA has divided attention, and a mild decline in work-
been associated with memory and executive ing memory and executive functions (Bennett
abilities of schizophrenic participants (Jeste et al., 2002; Rabbitt & Lowe, 2000). While there
694 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

is considerable individual variability among patient’s self-report of medication adherence


elderly individuals across all of these cognitive (Mackin & Arean, 2007).
domains, many adults over the age of 65 will Deficits in comprehension have been associ-
show these characteristic cognitive changes. ated with suboptimal adherence to medication
There is also often some degree of decline in instructions, including those written on the
the efficiency of information retrieval, espe- label of the pill bottle, pamphlets accompanying
cially for more recently learned information, the medications, and those verbally given by
which can lead to difficulty recalling names and the patient’s health-care provider (Diehl et al.,
places. Changes in episodic memory retrieval 1995; Kendrick & Bayne, 1982). For example,
functions often occur with relative sparing of Kendrick and Bayne (1982) reported that older
procedural memory and basic memory storage adults had difficulty translating instructions
ability (Fleischman & Gabrieli, 1998). Similarly, seemingly as straightforward as “Take every
older adults often show less efficient prospec- 6 hours” into a specific medication plan, and
tive memory than their younger counterparts Hurd and Butkovich (1986) found that a major-
(Einstein & McDaniel, 1990). Some of these ity of older individuals made errors when inter-
cognitive changes likely contribute to the diffi- preting prescription labels. Similarly, Morrell
culties with adherence seen in some older adults et al. (1989, 1990) found that about 25% of the
(Einstein et al., 1998; Raz, 2000). information in a medication plan was misun-
In fact, one of the most frequently reported derstood by older adults when they were pre-
causes for medication nonadherence among the sented with an array of prescription labels and
elderly is “forgetfulness” (Brainin, 2001; Leirer asked to develop a medication schedule based
et al., 1988). Col and colleagues (1990) reported on the instructions. Others have demonstrated
that poor memory recall had a stronger relation- that older adults have particular difficulty with
ship to treatment nonadherence than did any comprehension of text when inferences are
other of a host of predictors (odds ratio = 7.1) in required (Cohen, 1981).
a nondemented elderly sample. Poor medication In addition to age-related decrements in
adherence may be influenced by two different memory and comprehension abilities, declines
types of memory deficit, difficulties with ret- in sensorimotor function, attention, and visuo-
rospective and/or prospective memory. Morell perceptual ability have been shown to be asso-
et al. (1990) suggested that patients employ ciated with adherence to medication regimens
retrospective memory to remember the cor- (Conn et al., 1994; Isaac & Tamblyn, 1993; Stilley
rect way to take their medication and must use et al., 2004). Age-related declines in visual func-
prospective memory to do so at the right time. tioning have been well documented and include
Morrell and colleagues (1990) reported multiple reduced visual acuity, diminished perception of
challenges to successful adherence among older peripheral targets, and poorer color discrimi-
individuals, relative to their younger counter- nation (Kline & Scialfa, 1997). Deterioration
parts. First, older persons showed less efficient in perceptual acuity may interfere with the
recall of drug instructions than did younger patient’s ability to discriminate basic informa-
controls. Second, while both young and older tion about the medication (s) they are being
adults have more difficulty recalling medication asked to adhere to, including such factors as
regimens as they became more complex, often pill tablet color and shape (Hurd & Butkovich,
the older patients are more likely to be on more 1986). In some cases, impaired motor function
complex medication regimens. A recent study may be related to problems with opening med-
of predictors of adherence in elderly primary ication bottles and effectively cutting/scoring
care clinic patients showed that memory scores pills (Isaac & Tamblyn, 1993). With regard to
on a cognitive screening scale were predictive of attentional abilities, Zacks and Hasher (1997)
missed appointments (Mackin & Arean, 2007). reported that older adults are less efficient in
It is interesting to note that in this study, while their ability to both direct and focus their atten-
lower memory scores predicted more frequent tion as well as to inhibit attention to irrelevant
missed appointments, there was no relationship information. Similarly, older adults have been
found between lower memory scores and the found to be highly susceptible to both internal
Adherence to Medical Treatments 695

and external distraction over delays (Rekkas, increased social isolation likely impacts adher-
2006). Such deficits may be particularly prob- ence, and there is some evidence to suggest that
lematic when older individuals are faced with the overall health literacy among the elderly
the administration of multiple medications and is poorer than that seen in nonelderly popu-
complex drug regimens. Indeed, noncompli- lations (Council on Scientific Affairs, 1999).
ance has been shown to increase dramatically Health literacy will surely impact compre-
among the elderly in relation to the number hension of instructions provided by the treat-
of drugs prescribed (Fernandez et al., 2006; ment team (written and oral) and adherence to
Wandless & Davie, 1987). For example, dosage medical instructions (Baker et al., 1998). As a
errors in one study increased 15-fold among strong example of the impact of health literacy,
older patients when the number of drugs pre- a large study of Medicare patients from a man-
scribed was increased from 1 to 4 (Parkin et al., aged care plan encompassing four cities showed
1986). Similarly, noncompliance was found to be that a third of their English-speaking and more
3.6 times more prevalent among elderly patients than one-half of Spanish-speaking respondents
using two or more pharmacies to fi ll their pre- had inadequate to marginal health literacy
scriptions than among those using only one (Gazmararian et al., 1999). In this sample, only
(Col et al., 1990). 12% of respondents understood the correct tim-
Medication adherence also involves work- ing of dosing medications, and only 16% under-
ing memory and other components of executive stood how to take a medication on an empty
functioning. Working memory, the capacity to stomach (Gazmararian et al., 1999).
process, manipulate, and temporarily store new
or recently accessed information, has also been
Dementia
found to decline with age (Craik & Jennings,
1992; Park et al., 1996) and therefore negatively In general, there have been fewer investigations
impact medication-taking behaviors. In the of medication adherence behavior in patients
context of medication management, individuals with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) or frank
must keep the intention to take their medicines dementia syndromes, although more recently
in working memory while doing other things some relevant studies are starting to emerge
and must further rely upon these functions to (Cotrell et al., 2006). A recent large-scale study
integrate and develop a medication plan for fol- of medication adherence in elderly individu-
lowing multiple drug regimens simultaneously. als from 11 European countries found that
Planning and the ability to monitor the suc- the presence of cognitive dysfunction, as mea-
cess of that plan and modify the plan if needed sured by fairly broad screening instruments,
are functions that are dependent upon intact was associated with increased risk for nonad-
executive functioning. Arguably the cognitive herence (Cooper et al., 2005). It is impossible
domain most implicated in successful adher- to determine the contribution of various types
ence to medication regimen is executive func- of cognitive dysfunction (e.g., memory versus
tioning (Ownby, 2006), and Insel and colleagues executive dysfunction versus visuospatial defi-
have shown that to be the case in the elderly as cits) as the screening measure used produces
well (Insel et al., 2006). only an “impairment” score and specifies only
Of course, other noncognitive factors neg- the degree, not the specific nature of the impair-
atively impact adherence among the elderly, ment detected. These authors noted that the vast
some of which may interact with cognitive majority of those who scored poorly on the cog-
compromise. Financial factors may be an espe- nitive screening measure—in a range suggestive
cially salient concern among the elderly, many of possible dementia—were not formally diag-
of whom are living on reduced and/or fi xed nosed with a cognitive disorder at the time of
income. Many older patients note that they can- the assessment (Cooper et al., 2005). Tierney
not afford to buy one or more of the prescribed and her colleagues (2007) recently reported
medications (Piette & Heisler, 2004) and, con- results from a fairly large (n = 160) conve-
sequently, skip or take partial doses of vari- nience sample of persons over 65 living alone,
ous medications. In some elderly populations, focusing on the relationship between self-care
696 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

and neuropsychological functioning. They persons with executive dysfunction may fail
reported that among those with low scores on to organize their schedule in a manner neces-
the Dementia Rating Scale (DRS), three specific sary to accommodate medication taking. On
neurocognitive tasks, measuring memory, exec- the other hand, such individuals may persev-
utive functioning, and conceptualization, were erate on medication taking and unintention-
associated with increased risk of having expe- ally overdose. Moreover, executive deficits may
rienced some instance of self-harm or neglect, contribute to faulty reasoning that medication
including medication failures. Indeed, patients adherence is not necessary or that alternative
with dementing illnesses such as Alzheimer’s doses or regimens are acceptable.
disease typically have difficulty not only Additional studies have demonstrated that,
remembering which medications they are tak- in at least some cases, the functional loss associ-
ing but also the reason for their use, as a result ated with executive impairments may be behav-
of disruptions in short-term memory, judg- iorally mediated by apathy. Indeed, apathy has
ment, insight, and, not infrequently, depression been associated with executive impairments in
(Ayalon et al., 2006; Mackin & Arean, 2007). patients with various forms of cognitive impair-
The importance of executive functioning, ment, including dementia (Castellon et al., 2000;
as alluded to above, cannot be overstated as it Royall et al., 2000). For example, Boyle et al.
relates to adherence. Executive abilities include (2003) demonstrated that executive impairment
the ability to plan, initiate, monitor, and inhibit and apathy scores contributed to 44% of the var-
complex, goal-directed behavior. Several stud- iance in instrumental activities of daily living
ies have found patients with executive func- in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Given that
tioning deficits are more likely to resist care executive functions are involved in behaviors
and are less likely to comply with medication associated with motivation, disruption of the
regimens (Allen et al., 2003; Hinkin et al., 2002; neural circuitry maintaining these higher-order
Schillerstrom et al., 2005; Stewart et al., 1999). processes may lead to apathy and subsequent
In one study, executive impairment explained functional impairment, resistance to care, and
28% of the variance in the performance of activ- impaired decision-making capacity.
ities of daily living in patients with Alzheimer’s
disease (Boyle et al., 2003).
HIV/AIDS
Several aspects/types of memory require
intact executive functions for efficient opera- Our group has engaged in several studies
tion, and some of these tasks may be particularly designed to identify factors that are associated
important for medication adherence. Taking with medication adherence, with a particular
medicines consistently involves developing and emphasis on neurocognitive factors. We pre-
implementing a plan to adhere, remembering sent an overview of the primary findings from
to adhere (which typically requires both time- these studies to illustrate how neurocognitive
based [e.g., take medications at 5:00 p.m.] and dysfunction can adversely affect medication
event-based prospective remembering [e.g., adherence in a disease that requires meticulous
take medications with food]), and remember- adherence to an often demanding and unfor-
ing whether the medicine was taken as desired giving regimen over extended periods of time.
(described as source monitoring). Patients with We should mention here that other groups have
executive functioning deficits typically show studied neurocognitive predictors of adherence
problems with both prospective memory and in HIV/AIDS, either in isolation or in combi-
source monitoring. nation with other predictors (e.g., Avants et al.,
Impaired executive functions may contribute 2001; Solomon & Halkitis, 2007; Wagner, 2002;
to poor medication adherence in a number of Waldrop-Valverde et al., 2006). While some
ways. For example, individuals with deficits in have found similar cognitive predictors (e.g.,
such higher-order abilities may fail to take their Avants et al., 2001; Solomon & Halkitis, 2007),
medications because they cannot maintain the others have reported somewhat different find-
cognitive representation related to the need for ings (see Waldrop-Valverde et al., 2006)—per-
medication in the face of other events. Similarly, haps a function of demographic differences seen
Adherence to Medical Treatments 697

among the different samples studied and differ- 100 1 or 2 doses/day


ences in testing battery depth and coverage. 3 doses/day
In an early study conducted by our group, 90
we found that medication adherence rates are

Percent adherence
adversely affected by HIV-associated neurocog- 80
nitive dysfunction (Hinkin et al., 2002). In this
study we administered a battery of neuropsy- 70
chological tests to a cohort of 137 HIV-infected
60
adults and collected adherence data using an
electronic monitoring device (MEMS cap). The
50
mean adherence rate across all 137 participants
was 80%. Only 34% (46/137) of participants 40
were classified as good adherers (≥95% of doses Normal Impaired
taken as scheduled), whereas fully two-thirds of Global cognitive status
subjects showed suboptimal adherence, often
dramatically so. We found that those who were Figure 28–1. Relationship between cognitive sta-
classified as cognitively compromised (global tus, regimen complexity, and medication adherence
T-score less than 40) had a mean adherence rate among HIV-infected adults.
of only 70% while neuropsychologically normal
subjects’ mean adherence rate was 82%. Post
hoc analyses revealed that deficits in executive HIV infection. The impact of age-related neu-
function and higher-order attentional process- rocognitive dysfunction on medication adher-
ing were the primary deficits underlying this ence can also be readily seen. Below we present
finding. Surprisingly, in this study memory data showing the interplay of poor adherence,
dysfunction was not related to poor adherence. advancing age, and neurocognitive dysfunc-
While the cognitively compromised partici- tion (Hinkin et al., 2004). After grouping sub-
pants demonstrated poorer adherence in gen- jects based on age (using age 50 as a cut point)
eral, this was particularly pronounced when and adherence rates (≥90% adherence = good
they had to adhere to a more complex medi- adherence; <90% = poor adherence), we found
cation regimen. As can be seen in Figure 28–1, that older subjects who were poor adherers per-
cognitively compromised subjects who were formed disproportionately poorly on neuropsy-
prescribed complex dosage regimens (defined chological testing (See Figure 28–2). Additive
in that study as a t.i.d schedule) demonstrated effects of other frequently co-occurring risk
the greatest degree of difficulty with adherence. factors such as Hepatitis C infection, drug
Complex regimens were not nearly as problem- use, and psychiatric disturbance can also be
atic for the neurocognitively intact subjects. expected to similarly interact with neurocogni-
The interplay between person-specific fac- tive dysfunction to adversely affect activities of
tors and medication-specific factors is of critical daily function.
importance. Complex regimens, complicated We recently conducted another series of anal-
labeling, poorly designed dispensing devices, yses in which we explored the possible moder-
and so on may all adversely affect medication ating effects of age on relationships between
adherence. However, our data suggests that neurocognitive functioning, health beliefs, and
patients with cognitive and/or physical limita- medication adherence in a sample of 431 HIV-
tions may be at particular risk. With advancing infected participants (Ettenhofer et al., in sub-
age, motor and sensory limitations, for exam- mission). Adherence to antiretroviral medication
ple, loss of visual acuity, may affect the ability was tracked prospectively for approximately 30
to read the fine print on medication inserts, and days using the MEMS caps electronic monitor-
peripheral motor dysfunction can lead to diffi- ing system and self-report. Structural equation
culty opening pill bottles. Our group as well as modeling (SEM) was used to evaluate models
others have also found that advancing age can of the variable relationships, with estimates
potentiate the adverse neurocognitive effects of obtained separately for younger individuals (age
698 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

80
Good Adherence (>95%)
70 Poor Adherence (<95%)

Percentage of subjects
60
50
40
30
20
10
0
Older Young

Figure 28–2. Medication adherence in younger (<50 years) and older (≥50 years) HIV-infected adults.

< 50; n = 352) and older individuals (age ≥ Psychiatric Status


50; n = 79). As can be seen in Figure 28–3 neu-
rocognitive functioning was significantly related In many neuromedical populations that show
to adherence for older participants (r = .38, cognitive compromise there is also often
p < .05), but not for younger participants some degree of neuropsychiatric dysfunction
(r = .09, p > .05). Executive functioning, the (Castellon et al., 2001). As an example, in both
cognitive domain that had the strongest loading stroke and several neurodegenerative disorders,
for both younger and older participants appears depression can be seen as both a direct effect of
to account for much of this relationship. These the disease or disorder (e.g., impact upon neu-
findings suggest that neurocognitive function- rochemical or neurophysiological function) or
ing is a potent predictor of medication adherence may emerge as an indirect effect of the disease,
for HIV-infected individuals age 50 and over, such as reaction to the loss of function and dis-
but not for individuals under age 50. Therefore, ability. Below, we review the impact of psychi-
age should be considered carefully in research atric disturbance on medication adherence in
and clinical interventions related to medication both idiopathic primary psychiatric diseases
adherence in HIV. (e.g., bipolar disorder, schizophrenia) and then
Finally, we recently explored the impact of touch upon its impact in populations where
illicit drug use on medication adherence rates neuropsychiatric features are likely to accom-
among HIV-infected subjects (Hinkin et al., pany cognitive dysfunction (e.g., stroke, HIV/
2007). Among a different cohort of 150 HIV+ AIDS, neurodegenerative disease).
subjects, 102 of whom tested urine toxicology Studies have reported nonadherence rates
positive for illicit drug use, we found that drug- among psychiatric patients ranging from 26%
using participants were nearly four times more (Drake et al., 1989) to as high as 73% (Razali &
likely to be classified as poor adherers com- Yahya, 1995), often depending on the method
pared to subjects who tested drug negative. This used to measure medication adherence. This is
effect was especially marked among stimulant particularly problematic given the importance of
users who were seven times more likely to fail adherence in preventing disease expression (e.g.,
to adequately adhere to their medication regi- symptoms, signs). In depressive disorders, the
men. Recent work coming out of the San Diego 1-year relapse rates are as high as 80% in patients
HIV Neurobehavioral Research Center shows not taking antidepressants as compared to 30%
that methamphetamine-dependent HIV+ for those who adhere, yet some studies have
adults showed increased rates of both episodic shown that nearly 2 in 3 patients discontinue
and prospective memory deficits (Carey et al., their medications within 3 months of begin-
2006; Woods et al., 2005), which might partially ning treatment (Myers & Braithwaite, 1992).
explain the greater degree of adherence failure in Treatment nonadherence is a common reason for
this particular subset of HIV-infected persons. psychiatric rehospitalization as well. A study by
Adherence to Medical Treatments 699

Cognition and Medication Adherence among Younger HIV+ Participants

e1 e2 e3 e4 e5 e6 e7 e8 e9

Speed Mem. Attent. Exec. Fluency Motor MOS Miss MEMS

.69 .46 .64 .83 .43 .43 .66 –.66 .57

Cognition Adherence

.07
(ns)
Cognition and Medication Adherence among Olderr HIV+ Participants

e1 e2 e3 e4 e5 e6 e7 e8 e9

Speed Mem. Attent. Exec. Fluency Motor MOS Miss MEMS

.72 .50 .47 .93 .34 .58 .46 –.12 .65

Cognition Adherence

.56

Figure 28–3. Both of these diagrams are from the same multigroup model, 2(48) = 61.67, p > .05, RMSEA
= .03, NFI = .93, CFI = .98.

Keck and colleagues found that 60% of patients poorer medication compliance, to varying
hospitalized with acute mania failed to adhere to degrees, in studies using diverse measures and
their medication regimen in the month prior to methodologies (Carney et al., 1995; Edinger et al.,
hospitalization (Keck et al., 1996). 1994; Sensky et al., 1996; Shapiro et al., 1995).
Psychiatric disturbances, including depres- For example, a small meta-analysis (12 articles)
sion and anxiety, have been associated with found that depressed patients were 3 times more
700 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

likely to be noncompliant with medication and focused on the severely mentally ill, including
behavioral treatment regimens than their non- schizophrenia and other psychotic disorders.
depressed counterparts (DiMatteo et al., 2000). Despite the overwhelming evidence that psy-
Patients with coronary artery disease who also chiatric medication is effective in the treat-
had a diagnosis of major depression adhered to ment of their disease, many patients do not
their cardiac medication regimen less than 45% take their medications (Dencker & Liberman,
of the time during a brief adherence monitor- 1995; Hale, 1995), and antipsychotic nonadher-
ing phase, while nondepressed CAD patients ence is therefore a major barrier to the effective
had 70% adherence (Carney et al., 1995). Wang pharmacological treatment of these individuals
and colleagues (2002) demonstrated significant (Dolder et al., 2004). Several studies have shown
effects of depressive symptoms on antihyperten- that approximately one-third of patients with
sive medication adherence and Ciechanowski schizophrenia are fully compliant with medi-
and colleagues (2000) found that depressive cations, one-third are partially compliant, and
symptom severity was associated with poorer one-third of patients are entirely noncompli-
diet and medication regimen adherence among ant (Buchanan, 1992; Fleischhacker et al., 1994;
diabetics. Mood disturbance may interact with Weiden et al., 1995). Moreover, 55% of people
health beliefs and other variables, as described with schizophrenia who do not take antipsy-
below. One study found that health locus of chotic medication will relapse over the course of
control and social support were each negatively a year compared to only 14% of those who com-
impacted by prominent depressive symptoms ply with their medication regimen (Stephenson
and, together, these had an adverse impact on et al., 1993). Not surprisingly, multiple investiga-
adherence to diet and other health behaviors tions have demonstrated that poor medication
among patients on chronic hemodialysis (Sensky adherence among patients with schizophrenia
et al., 1996). Finally, neuropsychiatric dysfunc- is associated with a variety of poor health out-
tion, including apathy, depression, and hostility, comes, including increased rehospitalization,
has been related to decreased adherence among repeated emergency room visits, worsening of
the elderly as well (Carney et al., 1995). symptoms, and even homelessness (Marder,
Recent studies have revealed that cognitive 1998; Moore et al., 2000; Olfson et al., 2000;
deficits associated with psychiatric illness may Weiden & Olfson, 1995).
play a role in adherence behaviors. For example, Several reviews of the literature on medication
there is evidence that cognitive deficit in some adherence in schizophrenia (Fenton et al., 1997;
patients with bipolar disorder is enduring and Kampman & Lehtinen, 1999) have identified con-
may represent a trait-like variable. Deficits in sistent predictors of poor adherence in this pop-
learning and memory (Cavanagh et al., 2002) ulation, including more severe psychopathology,
and executive functioning (Goldberg et al., 1993; comorbid substance abuse, presence of medica-
Morice, 1990) have been found consistently, tion side effects, depressive symptoms, absence
even during the euthymic phase of bipolar ill- of social support from family or friends, practical
ness. Such impairments may have detrimental barriers, such as inability to afford medications,
effects on patients’ ability to remember dos- lack of insight, and neurocognitive dysfunc-
ing instructions and to appropriately plan and tion. Although there is substantial heterogeneity
organize a medication-taking regimen. Also, in among such individuals with regard to the level
some diseases, patients with apathy, depression, and pattern of cognitive impairment, some of the
and other neuropsychiatric symptoms/syn- most commonly impaired abilities in schizophre-
dromes are more likely to show neurocognitive nia include attention, working memory, verbal
deficits, especially executive dysfunction (e.g., and nonverbal learning, executive functions, and
Castellon et al., 2000). These patients may be some psychomotor abilities (Heaton & Drexler,
at particularly high risk for adherence failure, 1987; Heinrichs & Zakzanis, 1998; Schwartz
with the “double whammy” of neurocognitive et al., 1996; see also Harvey & Keefe, this volume).
and neuropsychiatric issues. A major barrier to adherence in this population
Medication adherence has received increas- is related to a lack of insight or inability to under-
ing attention among researchers and clinicians stand one’s disorder and the need for treatment
Adherence to Medical Treatments 701

(Lacro et al., 2002). Diminished insight, cou- compliance describes a kind of cost–benefit
pled with other cognitive deficits, may decrease analysis in which individuals weigh the treat-
patients’ ability to adhere to their treatment regi- ment’s effectiveness against potential negative
mens (Green, 1996; Green et al., 2000). consequences of compliance, such as disrup-
tion of daily activities and adverse side effects.
HBM theory predicts that those individuals
Psychosocial Models of Adherence
who perceive themselves to be potentially vul-
While the main focus of the current chapter nerable to the illness, perceive the consequences
is the relationship between cognitive function of the illness as severe, and are convinced of the
and medication adherence, it is important to efficacy of the proposed treatment regimen are
realize that adherence behavior is a complex more likely to adhere, especially if they perceive
and dynamic process that can be determined relatively few “costs” associated with adherence
by multiple factors (Remien et al., 2003). Many (Budd et al., 1996; Smith et al., 1987).
theories have set forth a combination of inter- In addition to these four dimensions, the
nal (e.g., attitudes, health beliefs, motivation) or HBM also postulates that diverse demographic,
external influences (e.g., environmental, social) psychosocial, and psychological variables may
to explain various health-related behaviors, affect individuals’ perceptions and thereby indi-
including medication adherence. A detailed rectly influence health-related behaviors. The
review of the various theoretical models of model also states that an individual needs a
adherence or of all the many adherence-related prompt (e.g., a reminder either of the threat of
constructs is well beyond the scope of this illness or the action that must be taken against
chapter. However, we briefly discuss a few of the it) before they will engage in health-related
more important theoretical contributions to the behaviors (Weinstein, 1988). These “cues to
adherence literature. action” may be internal (e.g., recognition of pro-
dromal symptoms) or external (e.g., comments
made by significant others). The HBM has been
Health Beliefs Model
shown to explain variation in medical regimen
The Health Beliefs Model (HBM) was pro- adherence behavior in patients with a variety
posed to predict a wide variety of health-related of diseases and disorders, including HIV/AIDS
behaviors (Rosenstock, 1974) and has been a (Barclay et al., 2007), hypertension (Mendoza
focus of numerous studies of medication adher- et al., 2006), diabetes (Harris et al., 1982), heart
ence (Janz & Becker, 1984). The HBM posits that disease (Mirotznik et al., 1995), epilepsy (Green
health behaviors, including medication adher- & Simons-Morton, 1988), mild cognitive impair-
ence, depend mainly on the desire to avoid ment (Ownby et al., 2006), and psychiatric dis-
illness and the belief that certain actions will orders including depression and schizophrenia
prevent or alleviate disease. The model consists (Adams & Scott, 2000; Cohen et al., 2000). One’s
of a number of dimensions, including perceived health beliefs are dynamic over time and typi-
susceptibility to illness, perceived illness severity, cally will involve the development, modification,
perceived benefits of treatment, and perceived and monitoring of health-related cognitions.
barriers to treatment compliance. Susceptibility Individual cognitive capacity may influence the
refers to an individual’s belief that he or she is at accuracy of one’s beliefs, the ability to incorpo-
risk of contracting an illness or disease or even rate new information, and the ability to con-
the patient’s vulnerability to illness in general. sistently determine and monitor cause–effect
Perceived illness severity relates to the patient’s relationships. It stands to reason that acquired
belief in the seriousness of contracting an illness cognitive deficits might mediate the relationship
and/or of leaving it untreated, including possi- between HBM and adherence behavior.
ble social, emotional, and functional conse-
quences. Perceived benefits of treatment refer to
Social Action Theory
beliefs in the efficacy of any actions in reducing
disease threat and the impact of living disease Social Action Theory (SAT; Ewart, 1991)
free. Finally, perceived barriers to treatment addresses the interactions between internal
702 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

and external factors in the drive toward action negatively related to medication adherence.
or behavior change. SAT holds that contextual Results from this meta-analysis would suggest
factors such as one’s physical environment and that the mere presence of others in the patient’s
social and cultural group membership, in com- environment may not always provide a positive
bination with various biological factors, will impact on adherence—rather it is the amount
interact to modulate mood and arousal, which of perceived and real support provided by those
impact self-regulatory processes (such as prob- others, whether they be marital partners or
lem solving, motivation, and interactive social friends, that is most predictive of adherence
factors) that, in turn, lead to action states such success.
as initiating and adhering to one’s medication Recent studies of adherence to highly active
regimen. A large, multisite study of HIV+ indi- antiretroviral therapy (HAART) in HIV+ indi-
viduals taking complex antiretroviral therapy viduals have attempted to delineate the nature of
(ART) medications revealed that contextual the relationships between social support, affect,
factors of African American heritage, number and medication adherence with structural equa-
of daily doses, symptom difficulty, involve- tion modeling. One such study (Simoni et al.,
ment in a primary relationship, a history of 2006), showed that social support was positively
drug use, and a history of homelessness were associated with spirituality and negatively asso-
predictive of poor (<90%) adherence (Johnson ciated with negative affect (e.g., depression),
et al., 2003). Additionally, poor adherers also which, in turn, predicted adherence self-efficacy
reported higher rates of emotional distress. in HIV+ individuals. Adherence self-efficacy
Self-regulatory factors associated with poor was predictive of self-reported adherence, which
adherence were low adherence self-efficacy, dif- in turn predicted viral load though the model
ficulty fitting medication schedule into daily only accounted for 8% of the total variance in
routines, problems managing medication side ART adherence. A more compelling study, also
effects, fatigue related to medication adherence, utilizing structural equation modeling, demon-
and disbelief in the efficacy of the prescribed strated that the relationship between medication
ART regimen. This study is consistent with SAT adherence, social support, and affect was medi-
and supports an association between medica- ated by avoidant-coping strategies reported by
tion adherence and both contextual and self- HIV-infected individuals (Weaver et al., 2005).
regulatory factors. More specifically, 20% of the variance in med-
ication adherence (measured via MEMS caps)
was predicted by negative affect and poor social
Social Support
support as moderated by avoidant coping. The
Several studies have shown the importance of same model also accounted for 44% of the vari-
social support in medication adherence (Berk ance in viral load.
et al., 2004; DiMatteo, 2004; Simoni et al., 2006; Clearly one’s health beliefs and attitudes, sense
Weaver et al., 2005). The construct of social sup- of self-efficacy, treatment expectancies, and
port can be further parsed into structural and degree of social support influence adherence.
functional factors. Structural factors include the Also, sociodemographic and contextual factors
patient’s marital status, living arrangement, and (e.g., homelessness, ethnic minority status, low
density of their social network. Variables such income, active substance abuse) play a part in
as emotional support, family cohesion, and the medication adherence. Particularly relevant to
amount of practical support reflect functional the current chapter is that there is yet very little
factors. A meta-analysis by DiMatteo (2004) known about the interaction between some of
found that several aspects of functional sup- these factors and the presence of neurocogni-
port were highly associated with better adher- tive deficits on health behaviors. Beyond their
ence, including the amount of practical and direct effect on adherence, cognitive deficits
emotional support and family cohesiveness. may moderate or mediate the influence of pre-
Structural support factors, including marital dictors such as treatment expectancies, health
status and living arrangement were only mod- beliefs, autonomous health-care management,
estly related to adherence. Family conflict was coping styles, and social support. Indeed, recent
Adherence to Medical Treatments 703

data collected from individuals suffering from problems being addressed (see Wilson, 2000
schizophrenia has shown that difficulties in and Wilson et al., 2001). For the most part, cog-
sustained attention, verbal memory, and exec- nitive rehabilitation or intervention techniques
utive functioning correlate with their attitudes to improve medication management in popula-
and beliefs about medications (Kim et al., 2006; tions with neuropsychological deficits have not
Maeda et al., 2006). been well studied to date.
On the other hand, compensatory interven-
tion strategies and technologies have received
Medication Adherence
considerable empirical scrutiny. Studies have
Interventions
explored the efficacy of external aids such as
Medication adherence problems are fairly wide- pillboxes and pill bottle alarms (Mackowiak
spread among patients with cognitive deficits, et al., 1994; Park et al., 1992), phone/voice mail/
including the elderly and/or those with chronic pager cues (Leirer et al., 1991), and organiza-
diseases. Interventions aimed at improving tional charts (Park et al., 1992). Haynes and
adherence are obviously needed in patients with colleagues have reviewed the medication adher-
cognitive deficits. Strategies used to increase ence intervention research (Haynes et al., 1996,
adherence in these populations include reha- 2000, 2003) and have consistently observed
bilitation techniques that are aimed at restor- that successful interventions are typically mul-
ing lost function(s) or at improving the patient’s tifactorial, involving some combination of
current cognitive limitations, or can involve improving the convenience of care, educating
teaching compensatory strategies or providing the person and/or family, and cues or remind-
assistive devices to mediate cognitive limitations. ers to take medication. Also, encouragement of
Examples of the latter include pillboxes, medica- self-monitoring and increasing patient support
tion charts, or personal data assistants (PDA) or through counseling, family therapy, and/or
voicemail reminder services (see Table 28–2). supervision, have also been shown to be effec-
Of course, the improved detection and treat- tive in some instances. However, perhaps the
ment of cognitive deficits may have a positive most notable finding from their review of this
impact on medication compliance if the cogni- body of research is that even the effective inter-
tive limitations were indeed impacting adher- ventions have not always led to strong, sustained
ence behavior. In patients with an acquired outcomes. For the most part, interventions tar-
brain injury (e.g., traumatic brain injury, stroke geting adherence have typically demonstrated
patients), the natural recovery of function that small to medium effect sizes (in the d ≤ 0.25
typically occurs in many of these disorders may range), indicating that significant and lasting
lead to better adherence as cognitive deficits behavioral changes with such strategies may be
improve. By extension, those patients in the difficult (Haynes et al., 2003).
early recovery stages of acquired brain injury However, some studies have found reason
may be particularly vulnerable to adherence to be optimistic about a wide variety of com-
problems, precisely at the time when adherence pensatory strategies in improving adherence.
might be most important. Family or caregiver Park and colleagues (1992) found that older
support may be particularly crucial at this time, adults who used both an organizer and a chart
especially ensuring that medications are taken designed to minimize cognitive effort in tak-
as prescribed and routines are developed for pill ing medications made significantly fewer errors
taking. There are many cognitive rehabilitation in their medication regimens than did control
techniques that have been set forth to address subjects (nearly a 10-fold decrease, relative to
problems in executive functioning (see Evans, controls—18.3% for controls versus 1.8% for the
2003 or Levine et al., 2000), which, as discussed intervention group). Many other studies have
above, are among the most robust predictors of also demonstrated the efficacy of charts, orga-
adherence problems. Similarly, memory prob- nizers, and written instructions to augment
lems have been addressed by rehabilitation spe- the verbal instruction of physicians (Coe et al.,
cialists with mixed success, in part dependent 1984; Lamy et al., 1992). Similarly, color-coded
upon the nature and severity of the memory pill bottles matched with a weekly pillbox have
704 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

Table 28–2. Obstacles to Medication Adherence


Neurocognitive Psychological Demographic/social Human factors
Attention Deficits Psychiatric Disturbance Age Complex Medication
Learning & Memory Substance Abuse/ In general, youn- Regimen
Deficits Dependence ger adults tend to This tends to have a larger
Executive Deficits Stimulant use is particu- demonstrate poorer impact on older adults
Prospective Memory larly deleterious. adherence than older and cognitively compro-
Deficits Acute substance use adults. mised patients.
effects can also impair Poor Social Support Poorly Worded Labeling
neurocognitive Financial Barriers Poor Visual Acuity
functioning. Motor Incoordination
Counterproductive
Health Beliefs
e.g., external locus of
control, belief that
disease is less severe
than it is, medications
not effective, external
health locus of control.
Poor Doctor-Patient
Relationship
Interventions to Improve Medication Adherence
Organizational Aides Psychiatric screening and Psychoeducation Simplification of medication
Pillboxes treatment regarding age effects regimen
Medication schedules/ Psychoeducation & and the impor- Reduction in the number of
calendars Health Psychology tance of medication medications
Mnemonic strategies interventions regarding adherence Simplification of daily
(e.g., grouping and illness Family interventions dosing requirements;
studying medica- Clinician training, and/or social work avoid tid regimens
tion names by daily interviewing, and interventions regard- Language-simplified
does requirements) communication skills ing medication medication labels
External remind- regimen support Large print, brail, or audio
ers Programmed Medication scholar- medication labels
alarms (wrist ships, community Easy-to-open pill bottle lids,
watch; personal resource use, social pill bottle openers
data assistant) work intervention
Remotely controlled
reminders (pager,
phone)

been demonstrated to enhance compliance in for older adults and those with vision impair-
some patients (Martin & Mead, 1982). Finally, ments (Drummond et al., 2004; Vanderplas
Leirer et al. (1991) found a fourfold reduction & Vanderplas, 1980). Instructions may also
in episodes of poor adherence when voice mail be improved by adding icons or cartoons that
reminders were used to cue elderly subjects highlight important information (Wickens,
about their medication regimen. 1992) as such images tend to be more explicit
For individuals with vision problems, increas- than text, reducing the need for inferential rea-
ing contrast in written instructions avoiding soning (Larkin & Simon, 1987).
subtle distinctions among colors, and mini- Well-designed medication instructions that
mizing the need to discriminate fine detail can reduce cognitive demands and motivate the
help improve adherence. Similarly, studies on patient may also work to improve adherence
the effects of aging suggest that larger font sizes (Park et al., 1994). Such instructions may be
(i.e., 12 or 14 points), conventional font styles, even more effective if, in addition to explain-
and use of unjustified text are more appropriate ing how to take medication, they present
Adherence to Medical Treatments 705

information that targets incorrect beliefs about DA13799 awarded to Dr. Hinkin). Additional
the illness or the drug. For example, Carter support was provided by the VA Merit Review
et al. (1986) found that reminder messages for program to Drs. Hinkin and Castellon. Drs.
flu vaccinations improved clinic attendance Wright and Barclay received support from an
significantly when they contained information NIMH training grant (T32 MH19535). We
that addressed incorrect beliefs about the side would like to thank Amanda Gooding and
effects and risks of the immunizations. Michelle S. Kim for assistance with manuscript
Others have noted that there are a number of preparation.
interventions that health-care providers could
perform more regularly to potentially increase
References
medication adherence in their patients. These
include explaining why particular medications Adams, J., & Scott, J. (2000). Predicting medi-
are being prescribed and what outcome is typ- cal adherence in severe mental disorders. Acta
ically expected, including common side effects. Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 101, 119–124.
When physicians try to anticipate common bar- Albert, S. M., Flater, S. R., Clouse, R., Todak, G.,
Stern, Y., & Marder, K. (2003). Medication man-
riers to adherence such as economic limitations,
agement skill in HIV: I. Evidence for adaptation
cognitive impairment, and negative side effects of medication management strategies in people
and discuss these potential problems prospec- with cognitive impairment. II. Evidence for a per-
tively, this can be effective as well (Bultman & vasive lay model of medication efficacy. AIDS and
Svarstad, 2000). Providing information about Behavior, 7(3), 329–338.
health conditions and medications both ver- Albert, S. M., Weber, C. M., Todak, G., Polanco,
bally and in writing and using an explanatory C., Clouse, R., McElhiney, M., et al. (1999). An
level consistent with the patient’s education and observed performance test of medication manage-
health-care sophistication may improve adher- ment ability in HIV: Relation to neuropsycholog-
ence by improving the patient’s knowledge about ical status and medication adherence outcomes.
what they are expected to do (Brown & Segal, AIDS and Behavior, 3, 121–128.
Allen, S. C., Jain, M., Ragab, S., & Malik, N. (2003).
1996). If cognitive limitations might be expected
Acquisition and short-term retention of inhaler
to interfere with the patient’s own ability to techniques require intact executive function in
adhere, then working with the patient’s poten- elderly subjects. Age and Ageing, 32, 299–302.
tial caregivers to help them understand many Arnsten, J. H., Demas, P. A., Farzadegan, H., Grant, R.
of these aforementioned issues has been shown W., Gouretvitch, M. N., Chang, C. J., et al. (2001).
to be effective as well (Ownby et al., 2006). The Antiretroviral therapy adherence and viral sup-
simple intervention of asking the patient to pression in HIV-infected drug users: Comparison
explain (e.g., to their doctor or nurse) their own of self-report and electronic monitoring. Clinical
understanding of the consequences of not tak- Infectious Diseases, 33 (8), 1417–1423.
ing medications or how they might cope with Avants, S. K., Margolin, A., Warburton, L. A.,
adverse side effects can serve the dual-pronged Hawkins, K. A., & Shi, J. (2001). Predictors of non-
adherence to HIV-related medication regimens
purpose of helping health-care providers detect
during methadone stabilization. American Journal
subtle cognitive problems (e.g., memory, com- on Addictions/American Academy of Psychiatrists
prehension, language, etc.) or clarify simple in Alcoholism and Addictions, 10, 69–78.
misunderstandings that are not necessarily due Ayalon, L., Ancoli-Israel, S., Stepnowsky, C., Marler,
to cognitive compromise. Finally, attempting M., Palmer, B.W., Liu, L., et al. (2006). Adherence
to enlist patients’ families to assist in providing to continuous positive airway pressure treatment
additional support or supervision of medica- in patients with Alzheimer’s disease and obstruc-
tion taking, has been shown to be effective in tive sleep apnea. American Journal of Geriatric
improving adherence (DiMatteo, 2004). Psychiatry, 14, 176–180.
Baker, E. K., Kurtz, M. M., & Astur, R. S. (2006).
Virtual reality assessment of medication
Acknowledgments compliance in patients with schizophrenia.
CyberPsychology and Behavior.Special Issue:
This chapter includes data gathered as part of Virtual and physical toys: Open-ended features for
two NIH-funded studies (R01 MH58552 and R01 non-formal learning, 9, 224–229.
706 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

Baker, D. W., Parker, R. M., Williams, M. V., & Clark, Buchanan, A. (1992). A two year prospective study of
W. S. (1998). Health literacy and the risk of hospital treatment compliance in patient with schizophre-
admission. Journal of General Internal Medicine, nia. Psychological Medicine, 22, 787–797.
13, 791–798. Budd, R. J., Hughes, I. C., & Smith, J. A. (1996).
Bangsberg, D. R., Hecht, F. M., Charlebois, E. D., Health beliefs and compliance with antipsychotic
Chesney, M., & Moss, A. (2001). Comparing objec- medication. British Journal of Clinical Psychology,
tive measures of adherence to HIV antiretroviral 35(3), 393–397.
therapy: Electronic medication monitors and Bultman, D. C., & Svarstad, B. L. (2000). Effects of
unannounced pill counts. AIDS and Behavior, 5, physician communication style on client medi-
275–281. cation beliefs and adherence with antidepressant
Barat, I., Andreasen, F., & Damsgaard, E. M. (2001). treatment. Patient Education and Counseling, 40,
Drug therapy in the elderly: What doctors believe 173–185.
and patients actually do. British Journal of Clinical Callahan, C. M., & Wolinsky, F. D. (1995).
Pharmacology, 51, 615–622. Hospitalization for major depression among
Barclay, T. R., Hinkin, C. H., Mason, K. I., Reinhard, older Americans. Journal of Gerontology, Series
M. J., Marion, S. D., Levine, A. J., et al. (2007). Age- A, Biological Science and Medical Science, 50A,
associated predictors of medication adherence in M196–M202.
HIV+ adults: Health beliefs, self-efficacy, and neu- Carey, C. L., Woods, S. P., Rippeth, J. D., Heaton, R.
rocognitive status. Health Psychology, 26, 40–49. K., Grant, I., & HIV Neurobehavioral Research
Benner, J. S., Glynn, R. J., Mogun, H., Neumann, Center (HNRC) Group. (2006). Prospective mem-
P. J., Weinstein, M. C., & Avorn, J. (2002). Long- ory in HIV-1 infection. Journal of Clinical and
term persistence in use of statin therapy in Experimental Neuropsychology, 28, 536–548.
elderly patients. Journal of the American Medical Carney, R. M., Freedland, K. E., Eisen, S. A., Rich,
Association, 288, 455–461. M. W., & Jaffe, A. S. (1995). Major depression and
Bennett, D. A., Wilson, R. S., Schneider, J. A., Evans, medication adherence in elderly patients with cor-
D. A., Beckett, L. A., Aggarwal, N. T., et al. (2002). onary artery disease. Health Psychology, 14, 88–90.
Natural history of mild cognitive impairment in Carter, W. B., Beach, L. R., & Inui, T. S. (1986). The flu
older persons. Neurology, 23, 198–205. shot study: Using multiattribute utility theory to
Berg, J. S., Dischler, J., Wagner, D. J., Raia, J. J., & design a vaccination intervention. Organizational
Palmer-Shevlin, N. (1993). Medication compliance: Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 38,
A healthcare problem. Annals of Pharmacotherapy, 378–391.
27(Suppl 9), S1–S24. Castellon, S. A., Hinkin, C. H., & Satz, P. (2001).
Berk, M., Berk, L., & Castle, D. (2004). A collabora- Behavioral disorders associated with central ner-
tive approach to the treatment alliance in bipolar vous system dysfunction. In P. B. Sutker and H.
disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 6, 504–518. E. Adams (Eds.), Comprehensive Handbook of
Boyle, P. A., Malloy, P. F., Salloway, S., Cahn- Psychopathology (3rd ed., pp. 813–839). New York:
Weiner, D. A., Cohen, R., & Cummings, J. L. Plenum Press.
(2003). Executive dysfunction and apathy pre- Castellon, S. A., Hinkin, C. H., & Myers, H. F. (2000).
dict functional impairment in Alzheimer’s dis- Neuropsychiatric disturbance is associated with
ease. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 11, executive dysfunction in HIV-1 infection. Journal
214–221. of the International Neuropsychological Society 6,
Brainin, J. (2001). The role of memory strategies in 336–347.
medication adherence among the elderly. Home Cavanagh, J. T., Van Beck, M., Muir, W., &
Health Care Services Quarterly, 20, 1–16. Blackwood, D. H. R. (2002). Case-control study of
Briesacher, B. A., Gurwitz, J. H., & Soumerai, S. B. the neurocognitive function in euthymic patients
(2007). Patients at-risk for cost-related medication with bipolar disorder: An association with mania.
nonadherence: A review of the literature. Journal British Journal of Psychiatry, 180, 320–326.
of General Internal Medicine, 22, 864–871. Chapman, R. H., Benner, J. S., Petrilla, A. Tierce, J.
Brown, C. M., & Segal, R. (1996). The effects of health C., Collins, S. R., Battleman, D. S., et al. (2005).
and treatment perceptions on the use of prescribed Predictors of adherence with antihypertensive
medication and home remedies among African and lipid-lowering therapy. Archives of Internal
American and White American hypertensives. Medicine, 165, 1147–1152.
Social Science & Medicine, 43, 903–917. Ciechanowski, P. S., Katon, W. J., & Russo, J. E.
Bruer, J. T. (1982). Methodological rigor and cita- (2000). Depression and diabetes: Impact of depres-
tion frequency in patient compliance literature. sive symptoms on adherence, function, and costs.
American Journal of Public Health, 72, 297–303. Archives of Internal Medicine, 160, 3278–3285.
Adherence to Medical Treatments 707

Coe, R. M., Prendergast, C. G., & Psathas, G. (1984). Dolder, C. R., Lacro, J. P., & Jeste, D. V. (2003).
Strategies for obtaining compliance with medica- Adherence to antipsychotic and non-psychiatric
tion regimens. Journal of the American Geriatrics medications in middle-aged and older patients
Society, 32, 589–594. with psychotic disorders. Psychosomatic Medicine,
Cohen, G. (1981). Inferential reasoning in old age. 65, 156–162.
Cognition, 9, 59–72. Drake, R. E., Osher, F. C., & Wallach, M. A. (1989).
Cohen, N. L., Parikh, S. V., & Kennedy, S. H. (2000). Alcohol use and abuse in schizophrenia: A pro-
Medication compliance in mood disorders: spective community study. Journal of Nervous and
Relevance of the Health Belief Model and other Mental Disorders, 177, 408–414.
determinants. Primary Care Psychiatry, 6, 101–110. Drummond, S. R., Drummond, R. S., & Dutton, G. N.
Col, N., Fanale, J. E., & Kronholm, P. (1990). The (2004). Visual acuity and the ability of the visually
role of medication noncompliance and adverse impaired to read medication instructions. British
drug reactions in hospitalizations of the elderly. Journal of Ophthalmology, 88(12), 1541–1542.
Archives of Internal Medicine, 150, 841–845. Dunbar-Jacob, J., Erlen, J. A., Schlenk, E. A., Ryan,
Conn, V., Taylor, S., & Miller, R. (1994). Cognitive C. M., Sereika, S. M., & Doswell, W. M. (2000).
impairment and medication compliance. Journal Adherence in chronic disease. Annual Review of
of Gerontological Nursing, 12, 41–47. Nursing Research, 18, 48–90.
Cooper, B., Carpenter, I., Katona, C., Schroll, M., Edinger, J. D., Carwile, S., Miller, P., & Hope, V.
Wagner, C., Fialova, D., et al. (2005). The AdHOC (1994). Psychological status, syndromatic mea-
study of older adults’ adherence to medication sures, and compliance with nasal CPAP therapy
in 11 countries. American Journal of Geriatric for sleep apnea. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 78,
Psychiatry, 13, 1067–1076. 1116–1118.
Cotrell, V., Wild, K., & Bader, T. (2006). Medication Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (1990). Normal
management and adherence in cognitively aging and prospective memory. Journal of
impaired older adults. Journal of Gerontological Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
Social Work, 47, 32–46. Cognition, 16, 717–726.
Craik, F. I. M., & Jennings, J. M. (1992). Human mem- Einstein, G. O., McDaniel, M. A., Smith, R., & Shaw,
ory. In F. I. M. Craik and T. A. Salthouse (Eds.), P. (1998). Habitual prospective memory and aging:
The Handbook of Aging and Cognition. Hillsdale, Remembering instructions and forgetting actions.
NJ (pp. 51–110): Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Psychological Science, 9, 284–288.
Council on Scientific Affairs (1999). Health liter- Evans, J. J. (2003). Rehabilitation of executive defi-
acy report. Ad hoc committee on Health Literacy cits. In B. A. Wilson (Ed), Neuropsychological
for the Council of Scientific Affairs, American Rehabilitation: Theory and Practice (pp. 53–70).
Medical Association. JAMA, 281, 552–557. Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets and Zeitlinger.
Dencker, S. J., & Liberman, R. P. (1995). From Ewart, C. K. (1991). Social action theory for pub-
compliance to collaboration in the treat- lic health psychology. American Psychologist, 46,
ment of schizophrenia. International Clinical 931–946.
Psychopharmacology, 9(Suppl5), 75–78. Fenton, W. S., Blyler, C. R., & Heinssen, R. K.
Diehl, M., Willis, S. L., & Schaie, W. (1995). Everyday (1997). Determinants of mediation compliance
problem solving in older adults: Observational in schizophrenia: Empirical and clinical findings.
assessment and cognitive correlates. Psychology Schizophrenia Bulletin, 23, 637–651.
and Aging, 10(3), 478–491. Fernandez, L. C., Baron, F. B., Vazquez, D. B.,
Dimatteo, M. R. (2004). Social support and patient Martinez, G. T., Urendes, J. J., & Pujol de la Llave,
adherence to medical treatment: A meta-analysis. E. (2006). Medication errors and non-compliance
Health Psychology, 23, 207–218. in polymedicated elderly patients. Farmacia
Dimatteo, M. R., Lepper, H. S., & Croghan, T. W. Hospitalaria, 30, 280–283.
(2000). Depression is a risk factor for noncompli- Fleischhacker, W. W., Meise, U., Gunther, V., &
ance with medical treatment: Meta-analysis of the Kurz, M. (1994). Compliance with antipsychotic
effects of anxiety and depression on patient adher- drug treatment: Influence of side effects. Acta
ence. Archives of Internal Medicine, 160, 2101–2107. Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 89, 11–15.
Dolder, C. R., Lacro, J. P., Warren, K. A., Golshan, S., Fleischman, D. A., & Gabrieli, J. D. (1998). Repetition
Perkins, D. O., & Jeste, D. V. (2004). Brief evaluation priming in normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease:
of medication influences and beliefs: Development A review of findings and theories. Psychology of
and testing of a brief scale for medication adher- Aging, 13, 88–119.
ence. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, Fulmer, T., & Gurland, B. (1997). Evaluating the care-
24(4), 404–409. giver’s intervention in the elder’s task performance:
708 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

Capacity versus actual behavior. International Harris, R., Skyler, J. S., Linn, M. W., Pollack, L., &
Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 12, 920–925. Tewksbury, D. (1982). Relationship between the
Ganguli, M., Dodge, H. H., & Mulsant, B. H. (2002). health belief model and compliance as a basis for
Rates and predictors of mortality in an aging, rural, intervention in diabetes mellitus. Pediatric and
community-based cohort: The role of depression. Adolescent Endocrinology, 10, 123–132.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 1046–1052. Haynes, R. B., Montague, P., Oliver, T., Mc Kibbon,
Gazmararian, J. A., Baker, D. W., Williams, M. K. A., Brouwers, M. C., & Keneni, R. (2000).
V., Parker, R. M., Scott, T. L., Green, D. C. et al. Interventions for helping patients to follow pre-
(1999). Health literacy among medicare enrollees scriptions for medications. Cochrane Database of
in a managed care organization. Journal of the Systematic Reviews, 2, CD000011.
American Medical Association, 281, 545–551. Haynes, R. B., McDonald, H., Garg, A. X., &
Gilmer, T. P., Dolder, C. R., Lacro, J. P, Folsom, D. Montague, P. (2003). Helping patients to fol-
P., Lindamer, L., Garcia, P., & Jeste, D. V. (2004). low prescribed treatment: Clinical applications.
Adherence to treatment with antipsychotic medi- Journal of the American Medical Association, 288,
cation and healthcare costs among Medicaid ben- 2880–2883.
eficiaries with schizophrenia. American Journal of Haynes, R. B., McKibbon, K. A., & Kanani, R. (1996).
Psychiatry, 161, 692–699. Systematic review of randomized trials of inter-
Goldberg, T. E., Gold, J. M., Greeberg, R., Griffi n, ventions to assist patients to follow prescriptions
S., Schulz, S. C., Pickar, D., Kleinman, J. E., & for medications. Lancet, 348, 383–386.
Weinberger, D. R. (1993). Contrasts between Heaton, R. K., & Drexler, M. (1987). Clinical neu-
patients with affective disorders and patients ropsychological findings in schizophrenia and
with schizophrenia on neuropsychological test aging. In N. E. Miller and G. D. Cohen (Eds.),
battery. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, Schizophrenia and Aging. . (pp. 145–161). New
1355–1362. York: Guilford Press.
Gould, O. N., McDonald-Miszczak, L., & Gregory, Heaton, R. K., Marcotte, T. D., Rivera-Mindt, M.,
J. (1999). Prediction accuracy and medication Sadek, J., Moore, D. J., & the HIV Neurobehavioral
instructions: Will you remember tomorrow? Aging, Research Center Group. (2004). The impact of HIV-
Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 6, 141–154. associated neuropsychological impairment on
Gray, S. L., Mahoney, J. E., & Blough, D. K. (2001). everyday functioning. Journal of the International
Medication adherence in elderly patients receiving Neuropsychological Society, 10, 317–331.
home health services following hospital discharge. Heinrichs, R. W., & Zakzanis, K. K. (1998).
Annals of Pharmacotherapy, 35, 539–545. Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: A quan-
Green, L. W., & Simons-Morton, D. G. (1988). Denial, titative review of the evidence. Neuropsychology,
delay and disappointment: Discovering and over- 12, 426–445.
coming the causes of drug errors and missed Hepke, K.L., Martus, M.T., & Share, D.A. (2004).
appointments. Epilepsy Research, 1, 7–21. Costs and utilization associated with pharmaceu-
Green, M. F. (1996). What are the functional conse- tical adherence in a diabetic population. American
quences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophre- Journal of Managed Care, 10, 144–151.
nia? American Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 321–330. Hinkin, C. H., Barclay, T. R., Castellon, S. A., Levine,
Green, M. F., Kern, R. S., Braff, D. L., & Mintz, J. A. J., Durvasula, R. S., Marion, S. D., et al. (2007).
(2000). Neurocognitive deficits and functional Drug use and medication adherence among
outcome in schizophrenia: Are we measuring the HIV-1 infected individuals. AIDS and Behavior,
“right stuff ”? Schizophrenia Bulletin, 26, 119–136. 11, 185–194.
Gurland, B. J., Cross, P., Chen, J., & Wilder, D. E. Hinkin, C. H., Castellon, S. A., Durvasula, R. S.,
(1994). A new performance test of adaptive cog- Hardy, D. J., Lam, M. N., Mason, K. I., et al. (2002).
nitive functioning: The medication management Medication adherence among HIV+ adults: Effects
(MM) test. International Journal of Geriatric of cognitive dysfunction and regimen complexity.
Psychiatry, 9, 875–885. Neurology, 59, 1944–1950.
Gurwitz, J. J., Field, T., Harrold, L., Rothschild, J., Hinkin, C. H., Hardy, D. J., Mason, K. I., Castellon,
Debellis, K., Seger, A., et al. (2003). Incidence and S. A., Durvasula, R. S., Lam, M. N., et al. (2004).
preventability of adverse drug events among older Medication adherence in HIV-infected adults:
persons in the ambulatory setting. Journal of the Effect of patient age, cognitive status, and sub-
American Medical Association, 289, 141–164. stance abuse. AIDS, 18, S19–S25.
Hale, A. S. (1995). Atypical antipsychotic and com- Horwitz, R. I., Viscoli, C. M., Berkman, L.,
pliance in schizophrenia. Nordic Journal of Donaldson, R. M., Horwitz, S. M., Murray, C.
Psychiatry, 49(Suppl 35), 31–39. J., et al. (1990). Treatment adherence and risk of
Adherence to Medical Treatments 709

death after a myocardial infarction. Lancet, 336, patients with mania. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry,
542–545. 57, 292–297.
Hurd, P. D., & Butkovich, S. L. (1986). Compliance Kendrick, R., & Bayne, J. (1982). Compliance with pre-
problems and the older patient: Assessing func- scribed medication by elderly patients. Canadian
tional limitations. Drug Intelligence and Clinical Medical Association Journal, 127, 961–962.
Pharmacy, 20, 228–231. Kim, S., Shin, I., Kim, J., Yang, S., Shin, H., & Yoon, J.
Huybrechts, K. F., Ishak, K. J., & Caro, J. J. (2005). (2006). Association between attitude toward medi-
Assessment of compliance with osteoporo- cation and neurocognitive function in schizophre-
sis treatment and its consequences in a man- nia. Clinical Neuropharmacology, 29, 197–205.
aged care population. Bone, Epub, December Kline, D. W., & Scialfa, C. T. (1997). Sensory and per-
1. http://www.sciencedirect.com/science?_ ceptual functioning: Basic research and human
ob=ArticleURL&_udi=B6T4Y-4HR72JN-2&_ factors implications. In A. D. Fisk and W. A. Rogers
user=4423&_rdoc=1&_fmt=&_orig=search&_ (Eds.), Handbook of Human Factors and the Older
s o r t=d & v i e w =c & _ a c c t= C 0 0 0 0 59 6 0 5 & _ Adult (pp. 27–54). New York: Academic Press.
version=1&_urlVersion=0&_userid=4423&md5 Lacro, J. P., Dunn, L. B., Dolder, C. R., Leckband, S.
=1cbce099a9602b01efee460b1d1a1751 G., & Jeste, D. V. (2002). Prevalence of and risk fac-
Insel, K., Morrow, D., Brewer, B., & Figueredo, A. tors for medication nonadherence in patients with
(2006). Executive function, working memory, schizophrenia: A comprehensive review of recent
and medication adherence among older adults. literature. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 63(10),
Journals of Gerontology: Psychological and Social 892–909.
Sciences, 61, P102–P107. Lamy, P. P, Salzman, C., & Nevis-Olesen, J. (1992).
Isaac, L. M., & Tamblyn, R. M. (1993). Compliance and Drug prescribing patterns, risks, and compliance
cognitive function: A methodological approach guidelines. In C. Salzman (Ed.), Clinical Geriatric
to measuring unintentional errors in medication Psychopharmacology (2nd ed., pp. 15–37).
compliance in the elderly. The Gerontologist, 33, Baltimore, MD: Williams and Wilkins.
772–781 Larkin, J. H., & Simon, H. A. (1987). Why a dia-
Jackevicius, C. A., Mamdani, M., & Tu, J. V. (2002). gram is (sometimes) worth ten thousand words.
Adherence with statin therapy in elderly patients Cognitive Science, 11, 65–99.
with and without acute coronary syndromes. Leirer, V. O., Morrow, D. G., Pariante, G. M., & Sheikh,
Journal of the American Medical Association, 288, J. L. (1988). Elders’ nonadherence, its assessment,
462–467. and computer assisted instruction for medication
Janz, N. K., & Becker, M. H. (1984). The Health Belief recall training. Journal of the American Geriatric
Model: a decade later. Health Education Quarterly, Society, 36, 877–884.
11, 1–47. Leirer, V. O., Morrow, D. G., Tanke, E. D., & Pariante,
Jeste, S. D., Patterson, T. L., Palmer, B. W., Dolder, C. G. M. (1991). Elders’ nonadherence: Its assess-
R., Goldman, S., & Jeste, D. V. (2003). Cognitive ment and medication reminding by voice mail.
predictors of medication adherence among mid- Gerontologist, 31, 514–520.
dle-aged and older outpatients with schizophre- Levine, A. J., Hinkin, C. H., Castellon, S. A., Mason,
nia. Schizophrenia Research, 63, 49–58. K. I., Lam, M. N., Perkins, A., et al. (2005).
Johnson, M. O., Catz, S. L., Remien, R. H., Rotheram- Variations in patterns of highly active antiret-
Borus, M. J., Morin, S. F., Charlebois, E., et al. roviral therapy (HAART) adherence. AIDS and
(2003). Theory-guided, empirically supported ave- Behavior, 9, 355–362.
nues for intervention on HIV medication nonad- Levine, B., Robertson, I. H., Clare, L., Carter, G.,
herence: Findings from the healthy living project. Hong, J., Wilson, B. A., et al. (2000). Rehabilitation
AIDS Patient Care and STDs, 17, 645–656. of executive functioning: an experimental-clinical
Kalichman, S. C., Amaral, C. M., Stearns, H., White, validation of Goal Management Training. Journal
D., Flanagan, J., Pope, H., et al. (2007). Adherence of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6,
to antiretroviral therapy assessed by unannounced 299–312.
pill counts conducted by telephone. Journal of Mackin, R. S., & Arean, P. A. (2007). Cognitive and
General Internal Medicine, 22, 1003–1106. psychiatric predictors of medical treatment adher-
Kampman, O., & Lehtinen, K. (1999). Compliance ence among older adults in primary care clinics.
in psychosis. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 100, International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 22,
167–175. 55–60.
Keck, P., McElroy, B., Strakowski, S., Stanton, S. P., Mackowiak, E. D., O’Connor, T. W., Jr., Thomason,
Kizer, D., Balistreri, T., et al. (1996). Factors asso- M., Nighswander, R., Smith, M., Vogenberg,
ciated with pharmacologic non-compliance in A., et al. (1994). Compliance devices preferred
710 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

by elderly patients. American Pharmacy, NS34, Osterberg, L., & Blaschke, T. (2005). Adherence to
47–52. medication. New England Journal of Medicine,
Maeda, K., Kasai, K., Watanabe, A., Henomatsu, K., 353, 487–497.
Rogers, M. A., & Kato, N. (2006). Effect of subjec- Ownby, R. (2006). Medication adherence and cog-
tive reasoning and neurocognition on medica- nition: Medical, personal and economic fac-
tion adherence for persons with schizophrenia. tors influence level of adherence in older adults.
Psychiatric Services, 57, 1203–1205. Geriatrics, 61, 30–35.
Marder, S. R. (1998). Facilitating compliance with Ownby, R. L., Hertzog, C., Crocco, E., & Duara, R.
antipsychotic medication. Journal of Clinical (2006). Factors related to medication adherence in
Psychiatry, 59, 21–25. memory disorder clinic patients. Aging & Mental
Martin, D. C., & Mead, K. (1982). Reducing medica- Health, 10, 378–385.
tion errors in a geriatric population. Journal of the Park, D. C., Morrell, R. W., Frieske, D., & Kincaid, D.
American Geriatrics Society, 4, 258–260. (1992). Medication adherence behaviors in older
McDonnell, P. J., & Jacobs, M. R. (2002). Hospital adults: Effects of external cognitive supports.
admissions resulting from preventable adverse Psychology and Aging, 7, 252–256.
drug reactions. Annals of Pharmacotherapy, 36, Park, D. C., Smith, A. D., Lautenschlager, G., Earles,
1331–1336. J., Frieske, D., Zwahr, M., et al. (1996). Mediators
Mendoza, P. S., Munoz, P. M., Merino, E. J., & Barriga, of long-term memory performance across the life
O. A. (2006). Determinant factors of therapeu- span. Psychology and Aging, 11, 621–637.
tic compliance in elderly hypertensive patients. Park, D. C., Willis, S. L., Morrow, D., Diehl, M., &
Revista Medica de Chile, 134, 65–71. Gaines, C. L. (1994). Cognitive function and med-
Mirotznik, J., Feldman, L., & Stein, R. (1995). The ication usage in older adults. Journal of Applied
health belief model and adherence with a com- Gerontology, 13, 39–57.
munity center-based, supervised coronary heart Parkin, D. M., Henney, C. R., Quirk, J., & Crooks,
disease exercise program. Journal of Community J. (1986). Deviation fro prescribed drug treatment
Health, 20, 233–247. after discharge from hospital. British Medical
Moore, A., Sellwood, W., & Stirling, J. (2000). Journal, 2, 686–688.
Compliance and psychological reactance in Patterson, T. L., Lacro, J., McKibbin, C. L., Moscona,
schizophrenia. British Psychological Society, 39, S., Hughs, T., & Jeste, D. V. (2002). Medication
287–295. management ability assessment: Results from
Morice, R. (1990). Cognitive flexibility and pre- a performance-based measure in older outpa-
frontal dysfunction in schizophrenia and mania. tients with schizophrenia. Journal of Clinical
British Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 50–54. Psychopharmacology, 22, 11–19.
Morrell, R. W., Park, D. C., & Poon, L. W. (1989). Piette, J. D., & Heisler, M. (2004). Problems due to
Quality of instructions on prescription drug medication costs among VA and non-VA patients
labels: Effects on memory and comprehension in with chronic illnesses. American Journal of
young and old adults. Gerontologist, 29, 345–354. Managed Care, 10, 861–868.
Morrell, R. W., Park, D. C., & Poon, L. W. (1990). Rabbitt, P., & Lowe, C. (2000). Patterns of cognitive
Effects of labeling techniques on memory and aging. Psychology Research, 63, 308–316.
comprehension of prescription information in Raz, N. B. (2000). Aging of the brain and its impact
young and old adults. Journal of Gerontology: on cognitive performance: Integration of struc-
Psychological Sciences, 45, P166–P172. tural and functional findings. In F. I. M. Craik
Myers E.D, & Braithwaite A (1992). Out-patient com- and T. A. Salthouse (Eds.), The Handbook of Aging
pliance with antidepressant medication. British and Cognition (2nd ed. pp. 1–90). Mahwah, NJ:
Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 83–86. Erlbaum.
Neal, B., MacMahon, S., & Chapman, N. (2000). Razali, M. S., & Yahya, H. (1995). Compliance with
Effects of ACE inhibitors, calcium antagonists, treatment in schizophrenia: A drug intervention
and other blood-pressure-lowering drugs: Results program in a developing country. Acta Psychiatrica
of prospectively designed overviews of random- Scandinavica, 91, 331–335.
ized trials. Blood Pressure Lowering Treatment Rekkas, P. V. (2006). Interference resolution in
Trialists’ Collaboration. Lancet, 356, 1955–1964. the elderly: Evidence suggestive of differences
Olfson, M., Mechanic, D., Hansell, S., Boyer, C. A., in strategy on measures of prepotent inhibi-
Walkup, J., & Weiden, P. J. (2000). Predicting med- tion and dual task processing. Neuropsychology,
ication noncompliance after hospital discharge Development, and Cognition, Section B, Aging,
among patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 13(3–4),
Services, 51, 216–222. 341–365.
Adherence to Medical Treatments 711

Remien, R. H., Hirky, A. E., Johnson, M. O., Stewart, J. T., Gonzalez-Perez, E, Zhu, Y., & Robinson,
Weinhardt, L. S., Whittier, D., & Minh Le, G. B. E. (1999). Cognitive predictors of resistive-
(2003). Adherence to medication treatment: A ness in dementia patients. American Journal of
qualitative study of facilitators and barriers among Geriatric Psychiatry, 7, 259–263.
a diverse sample of HIV+ men and women in four Stilley, C. S., Sereika, S., Muldoon, M., Ryan, C. M.,
U.S. cities. AIDS and Behavior, 7, 61–72. & Dunbar-Jacob, J. (2004). Psychological and cog-
Rosenstock, I. M. (1974). Historical origins of the nitive function: predictors of adherence with cho-
health belief model. Health Education Monographs, lesterol lowering treatment. Annals of Behavioral
2, 1–8. Medicine, 27, 117–124.
Royall, D. R., Chiodo, L. K., & Polk, M. J. (2000). Thieda, P., Beard, S., Richter, A., & Kane, J. (2003).
Correlates of disability among elderly retirees An economic review of compliance with medica-
with “subclinical” cognitive impairment. Journal tion therapy in the treatment of schizophrenia.
of Gerontology, Series A, Biological Sciences, 55, Psychiatric Services, 54, 508–516.
M541–M546. Tierney, M. C., Snow, G., Charles, J., Moineddin, R.,
Salzman, C. (1995). Medication compliance in the & Kiss, A. (2007). Neuropsychological predictors
elderly. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 56(Suppl 1), of self-neglect in cognitively impaired older peo-
18–23. ple who live alone. American Journal of Geriatric
Schillerstrom, J. E., Horton, M. S., & Royall, D. R. Psychiatry, 15, 140–148.
(2005). The impact of medical illness on executive Vanderplas, J. M., & Vanderplas, J. H. (1980). Some fac-
function. Psychosomatics, 46, 508–516. tors affecting legibility of printed materials for older
Schwartz, B. L., Rosse, R. B., Veazey, C., & Deutsch, S. adults. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 50, 923–932.
I. (1996). Impaired motor skill learning in schizo- Vik, S. A., Maxwell, C. J., & Hogan, D. B. (2004).
phrenia: Implications for corticostriatal dysfunc- Measurement, correlates, and health outcomes of
tion. Biological Psychiatry, 39, 241–248. medication adherence among seniors. Annals of
Sensky, T., Leger, C., & Gilmour, S. (1996). Pharmacotherapy, 38, 303–312.
Psychosocial and cognitive factors associated with Vik, S. A., Hogan, D. B., Patten, S. B., Johnson, J.
adherence to dietary and fluid restriction regimens A., Romonko-Slack, L., & Maxwell, C. J. (2006).
by people on chronic hemodialysis. Psychotherapy Medication nonadherence and subsequent risk of
and Psychosomatics, 65, 36–42. hospitalization and mortality among older adults.
Shapiro, P. A., Williams, D., Foray, A., Gelman, I., Drugs and Aging, 23, 345–356.
Wukich, N., & Sciacca, R. (1995). Psychosocial Wandless, I., & Davie, J. W. (1987). Can drug compli-
evaluation and prediction of compliance prob- ance in the elderly be improved? British Medical
lems and morbidity after heart transplantation. Journal, 1, 359–361.
Transplantation, 60, 1462–1466. Wagner, G. J. (2002). Predictors of antiretroviral
Simoni, J. M., Frick, P. A., & Huang, B. (2006). A lon- adherence as measured by self-report, electronic
gitudinal evaluation of a social support model of monitoring, and medication diaries. AIDS Patient
medication adherence among HIV-positive men Care and STDs, 16, 599–608.
and women on antiretroviral therapy. Health Waldrop-Valverde, D., Ownby, R. L., Wilkie, F.
Psychology, 25, 74–81. L., Mack, A., Kumar, M., & Metsch, L. (2006).
Smith, N. A., Ley, P., Seale, J. P., & Shaw, J. (1987). Neurocognitive aspects of medication adherence
Health beliefs, satisfaction and compliance. in HIV-positive injecting drug users. AIDS and
Patient Education and Counseling, 10, 279–286. Behavior, 10, 287–297.
Sokol, M. C., McGuigan, K. A., Verbrugge, R. R., & Wang, P. S., Bohn, R. L., Knight, E., Glynn, R. J.,
Epstein, R. S. (2005). Impact of medication adher- Mogun, H., & Avorn, J. (2002). Noncompliance
ence on hospitalization risk and healthcare costs. with antihypertensive medications: The impact
Medical Care, 43, 521–530. of depressive symptoms and psychosocial factors.
Solomon, T. M., & Halkitis, P. N. (2007). Cognitive Journal of General Internal Medicine, 17, 504–511.
executive functioning in relation to HIV medi- Weaver, K. E., Llabre, M. M., Durán, R. E., Antoni,
cation adherence among gay, bisexual, and other M. H., Ironson, G., Penedo, F. J., et al. (2005). A
men who have sex with men. AIDS and Behavior, stress and coping model of medication adherence
Epub, July 17, 2007. http://www.springerlink.com/ and viral load in HIV-positive men and women
content/k121387545155030/fulltext.html on highly active antiretroviral therapy (HAART).
Stephenson, B. J., Rowe, B. H., Haynes, B., Macharia, Health Psychology, 24, 385–392.
W. M., & Leon, G. (1993). Is this patient taking the Weiden, P. J., & Olfson, M. (1995). Cost of relapse
treatment as prescribed? Journal of the American in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 21,
Medical Association, 269, 2779–2781. 419–429.
712 Psychosocial Consequences of Neuropsychological Impairment

Weiden, P. J., Shaw, E., & Mann, J. (1995). problems by means of a paging system: A random-
Antipsychotic therapy: Patient preferences and ized control crossover study. Journal of Neurology,
compliance. Current Approaches in Psychosis, 4, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 70, 477–482.
1–7. Woods, S. P., Rippeth, J. D., Conover, E., Gongvatana,
Weinstein, N. D. (1988). The precaution adoption A., Gonzalez, R., Carey, C., et al. (2005). Deficient
process. Health Psychology, 7, 355–386. strategic control of verbal encoding and retrieval
Wickens, C. D. (1992). Engineering psychology and in individuals with methamphetamine depen-
human performance (2nd ed.). New York: Harper dence. Neuropsychology, 19, 35–43.
Collins. World Health Organization. (2003).Adherence to
Williams, B. R., & Kim, J. (2005). Medication use and long-term therapies: evidence for action. Available
prescribing considerations for elderly patients. at: http://www.who.int/chronic_conditions/adher-
Dental Clinics of North America, 49, 411–427. encereport/en/. (accessed July 3, 2007)
Wilson, B. A. (2000). Compensating for cognitive Zacks, R., & Hasher, L. (1997). Cognitive gerontol-
deficits following brain injury. Neuropsychological ogy and attentional inhibition: A reply to Burke
Review, 10, 233–243. and McDowd. The Journals of Gerontology, Series
Wilson, B. A., Emslie, H. C., Quirk, K., & Evans, J. B, Psychological Science and Social Sciences, 52,
(2001). Reducing everyday memory and planning P274–P283.
Author Index

Aarsland, D., 177, 202, 206, 209, 210 Allen, S. C., 696 Arendt, G., 367
Abbott, M. W., 428, 432 Allen, Van, M., 68 Arendt, T., 413
Abelson, J. F., 243 Allport, A., 254 Argyelan, M., 532
Abi-Saab, D., 461 Almeida, O. P., 107, 112 Ariza, M., 611, 627
Abrams, D. I., 380 Almkvist, O., 318 Armstrong, C., 293
Abrams, R., 563 Aloia, M. S., 341, 342 Arnett, P. A., 291, 292, 293, 294, 295,
Abramson, L. S., 66 Alper, K., 274 296
Acker, C., 402, 407 Alsobrook, J. P., 251 Arnold, B. R., 143
Acker, W., 429 Alterman, A. I., 415, 418, 419, 420, Arnold, S. E., 508
Adams, H. P., 323 423, 424, 429, 432 Arnsten, J. H., 374, 689, 691
Adams, J., 701 Alvarez, P., 359, 581 Aron, A. R., 226
Adams, K. M., 84, 85, 408, 411, 415, Alzheimer, A., 116, 117, 163 Aronoff, J. M., 167
416, 417, 428, 429 Amaducci, L. A., 612 Arria, A. M., 421, 429
Adler, C. H., 207, 671 Amato, M. P., 284 Arthur, W., 656, 668
Adolphs, R., 230 Amick, M., 176, 662, 671 Artmann, H., 430
Agamanolis, J., 561 Amieva, H., 577 Asam, U., 246
Agar, E., 431 Anand, A., 530, 533 Ashendorf, L., 536
Agranovich, A. V., 149 Anastasi, A., 128, 129, 130, 142 Astrup, J., 308
Ahles, T. A., 537 Ancoli-Israel, S., 340, 343 Atkinson, J. H., 373, 379
Ahlgren, E., 674 Anderson, D. W., 280 Attix, D. K., 79
Airaksinen, E., 379 Anderson, N. A., 142 Audenaert, K., 533
Akinwuntan, A. E., 669, 667, 673 Anderson, R. J., 356, 357, 537 Auriel, E., 87
Akshoomoff, N. A., 411 Anderson, R. P., 403 Austin, M.-P., 524, 526, 527, 539
Al Faqih, S. S., 251 Anderson, S. W., 69, 512, 663 Avants, S. K., 380, 696
Albers, G. W., 309, 310 Andrade, A. S., 383 Avila, J., 212
Albert, M. L., 29, 164 Andreasen, N., 165, 619 Avolio, B. J., 662
Albert, M. S., 129, 140, 161, 166, 167, Andresen, J. M., 224 Avorn, J., 671
171, 172, 174, 424, 564, 581 Andrew, S. E., 224 Awad, A. G., 634
Albert, S. M., 638, 643, 645, 693 Andrews, P., 461 Awad, C. A., 316
Albin, R. L., 162, 200, 201, 245 Angelini, G., 426 Awad, I. A., 316
Albright, T. D., 169 Annett, M., 242 Awad, N., 355, 356
Alchanatis, M., 345 Anstey, K. J., 663, 666, 670, 675 Axelrad, D. A., 501
Alderman, N., 36, 625 Anthenelli, R. M., 426 Ayalon, L., 696
Alegret, M., 203 Anthony, J. C., 107 Ayers, J., 424, 428, 429
Aleman, A., 559 Antinori, A., 367, 368, 369, 382, 384, Aylward, E. H., 231, 366
Alexander, D. M., 162 673 Azrin, R. L., 460
Alexander, G. E., 164, 228 Antonelli Incalzi, R., 337, 338, 342
Alexander, G. M., 243 Apter, A., 247 Babcock, D., 274
Alexander, M. P., 54, 75, 662 Araki, Y., 356 Bachman, D. L., 161
Alexander, S., 423 Arbit, J., 571 Bachoud-Levi, A. C., 230
Alexopoulos, G. S., 357, 527, 537, 543 Archibald, S. L., 285, 290, 366, 420 Backman, L., 166, 171, 172, 564
Alinder, J., 665 Arciniegas, D. B., 623 Baddeley, A., 35, 175, 176, 289, 370,
Alkadhi, H., 322 Ardila, A., 142, 149, 460 569, 564, 577
Allain, A. N., 425 Ardouin, C., 203, 207 Baer, L., 248
Allen, M. H., 118 Arean, P. A., 694, 696 Baghurst, P. A., 492, 496

713
714 Author Index

Bagos, P. G., 283 Beauvois, M. F., 31, 33 Blackburn, H. L., 66


Bajaj, J. S., 674 Bechara, A., 72, 229, 370, 461, 465 Blacker, D., 171, 564
Bak, J. S., 140 Beck, A. T., 72 Blana, E., 669
Baker, D. W., 695 Becker, J. T., 369, 404, 414 Blansjaar, B. A., 413, 414
Baker, E. K., 693 Becker, M. H., 701 Blaschke, T., 689, 690
Baker, E. L., 486, 490 Bedard, M., 341, 342, 666 Blass, J. P., 413
Baker, E. L., Jr., 480, 483 Beebe, D., 341, 343, 344 Blatt, J., 657
Baker, R. P., 320 Beevers, C. G., 542 Bleecker, M. L., 488, 490
Baker, T. K., 666 Begleiter, H., 408, 409, 417, 419 Blekher, T., 231
Bakker, F. C., 311 Behrmann, M., 33 Blessed, G., 111
Bakshi, R., 281, 296 Belanger, H. G., 601, 606 Bloch, M. H., 244, 245, 246
Baldereschi, M., 200 Bell, V., 38 Block, D. S., 376
Baldo, J. V., 564 Belle, S. H., 109, 115 Block, R. I., 458
Baldwin, C. M., 345 Bellebaum, C., 324 Bloom, D. R., 626
Bale, R. N., 357 Bellinger, A. M., 482 Bloom, F. E., 419
Ball, K., 72, 653, 662, 670, 674 Bellinger, D., 482, 484, 491, 492, 496 Bloss, C. S., 162
Ballard, C., 177, 178, 208, 309, Bell-McGinty, S., 637 Blostein, P. A., 116
311, 315 Bellodi, L., 248 Blumbergs, P. C., 409
Ballinger, W. E., 410 Benarroch, E. E., 213 Blumer, D., 611
Bambakidis, N. C., 319 Benbadis, S. R., 274 Blumstein, S. E., 44
Bamford, K. A., 224 Bench, C. J., 532 Blusewicz, M. J., 292, 417
Banaschewski, T., 245 Bender, L. A., 43 Boake, C., 610
Band, G. P., 159 Benedict, R. H., 72, 232, 283, 285, Bobholz, J. A., 671
Bangsberg, D. R., 692 295, 296, 370 Bocca, M. L., 658
Banina, M. C., 658 Benner, J. S., 689 Bohlhalter, S., 244
Banks, W. A., 359 Bennet, T. L., 17 Bohnen, N. I., 202
Barat, I., 693 Bennett, D., 312, 693 Boland, R., 537
Barber, J., 602 Benson, D. F., 51, 160, 162, 531, 534, Bolla, K. I., 458, 460
Barber, R., 178 581 Boller, F., 73
Barbizet, J., 560 Benton, A. L., 45, 66, 306 Bombardier, C. H., 597
Barch, D. M., 356, 533 Benton, R., 143 Boncinelli, M., 107
Barclay, T. R., 701 Berent, S., 213 Bond, L., 249
Barker, M. J., 462, 463 Berg, D., 296 Bond, M., 606
Barker-Collo, S., 312 Berg, E. A., 36, 45 Bondi, M. W., 162, 163, 167, 170, 171,
Barkley, G. L., 308 Bergendal, D., 284 205, 228
Barkley, R. A., 673 Berger, J. R., 367 Bonifati, V., 201
Barnes, D. E., 161 Berger, M., 53 Boone, K. B., 143, 145
Barnes, J., 204 Berglund, M., 408, 410, 411, Borenstein, A. R., 162
Baron-Cohen, S., 241, 250, 255 428, 429 Boring, E. G., 19
Barr, A. M., 463 Bergman, H., 405, 406, 407, 462 Borkowska, A., 539
Barr, C. L., 243 Berk, M., 702 Bornovalova, M. A., 461, 469
Barr, W. B., 61 Berkovic, S. F., 268 Bornstein, B. A., 255
Barrash, J., 72 Berman, S. M., 420 Bornstein, R. A., 253, 254, 256, 257,
Barron, J. H., 411 Bermel, R. A., 281 373, 527, 579
Barry, J. J., 274 Bernard, B. A., 251 Borod, J. C., 146
Bartok, J. A., 370 Berry, J. W., 142 Borson, S., 107, 108, 537
Bartsch, A. J., 402, 412, 430 Berry, M., 340 Bos, E. H., 538
Bartzokis, G., 159, 459 Bertera, J. H., 403 Boswell, E. B., 101
Baskin, D. G., 359 Berthier, M. L., 250 Boswell, J., 242, 251
Basso, M. R., 230, 373, 527 Bertolucci, P. H., 107, 112 Botez, M. I., 213
Bates, M., 232, 416, 418 Bettermann, K., 563 Botez-Marquard, T., 215
Bauer, L. O., 408, 460 Bhalla, R. K., 530 Botteron, K., 530
Baumann, M. H., 466 Bhattachary, S., 467 Botwinick, J., 129, 131
Baune, B. T., 535 Biederman, I., 405 Bouchard, T. J., Jr., 656
Baxter, D. M., 33 Biederman, J., 673 Bouhuys, A. L., 531
Baxter, V. K., 611, 627 Bieliauskas, L. A., 525, 531, 534, Boustani, M., 112, 118
Bayles, K. A., 160, 167, 174, 204 660, 670 Bowden, S. C., 413, 414, 416, 428, 429
Bayley, P. J., 166, 560, 584 Biessels, G. J., 355, 357, 358, 359 Bowen, M., 536
Bayne, J., 694 Bigler, E. D., 313, 622 Bowie, C. R., 511, 513, 516, 643
Beal, M. F., 482 Bilder, R. M., 507 Bowler, J. V., 312, 313, 314, 315,
Bean, K. L., 428, 429 Binder, L. M., 72, 606 316, 317
Bearden, C. E., 539 Binet, A., 3 Bowles, N. L., 167
Beats, B. C., 526 Binetti, G., 179, 180 Boyle, P. A., 637, 696
Beatty, P. A., 291 Birren, J. E., 131, 140 Bozeat, S., 178
Beatty, W. W., 166, 174, 281, 284, 285, Birzele, H. J., 462 Braak, E., 163, 165, 166, 171, 564
286, 287, 290, 291, 292, 293, Bissessur, S., 210 Braak, H., 163, 165, 201, 564
294, 403, 414, 460 Black, F. W., 559 Bradley, B. P., 531
Author Index 715

Braggio, J. T., 419 Bulmash, E. L., 525, 526 Carney, R. M., 699, 700
Brainin, J., 694 Bultman, D. C., 705 Carpenter, M. B., 59
Braithwaite, A., 698 Burchiel, K. J., 203 Carr, D. B., 653
Braithwaite, R. S., 379 Burd, L., 243, 250 Carroll, B. J., 527
Brand, A. N., 527 Burger, G. K., 72 Carroll, L. J., 606
Brands, A. M. A., 353, 354, 356, 357, Burger, M. C., 417 Carroll, M., 290, 291, 292
358, 360 Burgess, P. W., 36 Carter, A. S., 247, 250
Brandt, J., 112, 174, 176, 223, 224, Burk, K., 213 Carter, W. B., 705
225, 226, 227, 228, 229, 230, Burns, A., 224 Caruso, K., 421
231, 232, 403, 417, 428, 566, Burns, J., 625 Casau, N. C., 382
573, 586 Burt, D. B., 524, 527, 534 Casoni, F., 295
Brassington, J. C., 283 Buschke, H., 94, 108, 166, 291, 569, Caspari, D., 408
Braver, T. S., 356 573, 575 Cass, W. A., 464
Bravin, J. H., 294 Busto, U. E., 461 Cassidy, J. D., 606
Breen, E. K., 204 Butkovich, S. L., 694 Castellon, S. A., 696, 698, 700
Breitbart, W., 373 Butler, T., 244 Cattell, R. B., 43, 137, 138
Breivogel, C. S., 457 Butters, M. A., 542 Cavanagh, J. T., 700
Breteler, M. M., 199, 200, 317, 318, Butters, N., 45, 48, 60, 165, 166, 167, Cavanna, A. E., 242, 246, 248, 250,
320, 325 174, 175, 176, 223, 224, 226, 251, 254, 255
Brew, B. J., 377, 382 227, 228, 402, 403, 412, 413, Cercy, S. P., 177
Brewer, C., 405 417, 428, 559, 562, 569, 578, Cermak, L. S., 48, 60, 166, 292, 413,
Brewer, W. J., 508 570, 579, 584 428, 429, 559, 562, 578
Briesacher, B. A., 689 Byas-Smith, M. G., 542 Cernich, A., 601
Bright, P., 584 Bylsma, F. W., 176, 177, 223, 226, 227 Cerrone, P., 538
Brodaty, H., 108, 118 Byrd, D. A., 148, 149 Chabriat, H., 358
Broderick, P. A., 357 Chace, P. M., 573
Brody, A., 531, 532, 533, 543 Cadaveira, F., 409 Chamelian, L., 611, 627
Brody, E. M., 637, 644 Cader, S., 295, 296, Chan, A. S., 167, 175, 183, 226
Brody, M. C., 541 Cadet, J. L., 464 Chana, G., 381
Broe, G. A., 612 Caetano, S. C., 529, 530 Chandler, B. C., 410
Brookhuis, K. A., 659, 666 Cahn, D. A., 203 Chandra, V., 612
Brooks, D. J., 202 Cahn-Weiner, D. A., 635, 637, 639 Chandrasekhar, S. S., 374
Brooks, D. N., 565, 569 Caine, D., 344 Chaney, E. F., 428, 429
Brooks, J. O., 541 Caine, E. D., 175, 176, 242, 291, 402, Chang, L., 367, 381, 465, 466
Brooks, N., 423 403, 404, 523, 528, 530, 534, Channon, S., 526
Brookshire, B. L., 254, 256, 257 535, 536, 539 Chanraud, S., 407
Brouwer, W. H., 225, 661 Cala, L. A., 405 Chapman, J., 509, 559
Brouwers, P., 174, 176 Calderon, J., 177, 178, 208 Chapman, L. J., 509
Brower, K. J., 432 Caligiuri, M. P., 465 Chapman, R. H., 693
Brown, C. M., 705 Callahan, C. M., 688–689 Chapman, S. L., 469, 452
Brown, G., 84, 85, 311, 325, 626 Callicott, J. H., 511, 512 Chase, T. N., 244
Brown, M., 317 Cami, J., 466, 659 Chatterjee, A., 224
Brown, R. D., 320 Cammermeyer, M., 114 Chayakulkeeree, M., 377
Brown, R. F., 283 Campbell, J. J., 317 Chaytor, N., 632, 640, 641
Brown, R. G., 204, 205, 228, 524, 526, Campbell, M. C., 229 Chelune, G., 579, 632, 635, 638, 644
527 Campbell, R., 31 Chen, A., 488
Brown, R. T., 142 Campbell, S., 529 Chen, P., 171
Brown, S. A., 412, 426 Campbell, W. G., 424 Chen, W., 379
Bruce, J. M., 285, 295 Campbell, Z., 467 Cherner, M., 381, 382, 464
Bruer, J. T., 690 Campodonico, J. R., 231 Chertkow, H., 167
Brun, A., 165, 317 Campos-Rodriguez, F., 342 Cherubini, A., 312
Brust, J. C., 313 Canellopoulou, M., 294 Chesnut, R. M., 607
Bruun, R., 250 Canfield, R. L., 496 Chiang, M. F., 611, 627
Bryson, G., 512 Canli, T., 532 Chick, J. D., 407
Bub, D., 167 Cannestra, A. F., 321, 322 Chikama, M., 230
Buchanan, A., 700 Cannon, T. D., 512 Childers, S. R., 457
Buchanan, R. W., 518 Capitani, E., 166 Chiodo, L. M., 494
Buchtel, H. A., 271 Caplan, R., 274 Cho, Y. H., 581
Buckner, R. L., 159 Caramazza, A., 26, 31 Chochinov, H. H. M., 537
Buckwalter, J. G., 112 Cardenas, V. A., 407, 430 Choi, B. H., 480, 497
Budd, R. J., 701 Carey, C. L., 369, 371, 373, 381, Choi, J. H., 320
Budka, H., 366 639, 698 Chorbov, V. M., 529
Budman, C., 250 Carey, K. B., 416 Christiansen, A. L., 45
Buffery, A. W., 131 Carey, R., 316 Chu, N. S., 409
Buffett-Jerrott, S. E., 462, 541 Carlen, P. L., 405, 430 Chuang, H. Y., 488, 490
Buitenhuys, C., 465 Carlesimo, G. A., 166, 566, 559 Chui, H. C., 109, 115, 164, 313, 314,
Buklina, S. B., 320 Carlin, A. S., 426, 458, 470 315, 317
716 Author Index

Churchill, J. D., 161 Cornell, D. G., 539 Daumann, J., 466, 467
Ciarmiello, A., 231 Corrigan, J. D., 597, 602, 625 David, A. S., 223, 509
Cicerone, K., 383, 623, 633 Corsellis, J. A. N., 162 Davidshofer, C. O., 105
Ciechanowski, P. S., 537, 700 Coryell, W., 530 Davidson, M., 508, 511, 513
Ciesla, J. A., 378 Coscia, J. M., 493 Davidson, C., 464
Cifelli, A., 295, 296 Cosentino, S. A., 314, 318, 665 Davidson, P. W., 498, 499
Cinque, P., 378 Cosi, V., 409 Davidson, R., 533, 625
Cipolotti, L., 33 Costa, L., 67, 408 Davie, J. W., 695
Claes, S., 224 Cotrell, V., 665, 671, 695 Davis, B. A., 635
Claiborn, J. M., 428, 429 Cotter, V., 107 Davis, J. D., 227
Clark, D. C., 426 Coughlan, A. K., 31, 35 Davis, K. L., 509
Clarke, E., 306 Courville, C. B., 409 Davis, P. H., 323
Clarke, J., 428, 429 Coutin-Churchman, P., 408 Davis, S. J., 366
Claus, J. J., 204 Covinsky, K. E., 537 Davison, L. A., 42, 60, 309
Clegg, F., 35 Cowan, N., 571 de Bie, R. M., 203
Clifford, D. B., 381 Cox, D. J., 673, 674 de Boo, G. M., 231
Clifton, J. C., 497 Craik, F. I., 159, 695 de Jong, R. N., 360
Climent, E., 431 Crawford, F. C., 611, 627 de la Monte, S. M., 410
Cloninger, C. R., 420 Crawford, J. R., 526 De Lacoste, M., 169
Cluydts, R., 343 Crawford, S., 256 de Lau, L. M., 199, 200
Clydesdale, S., 181 Creed, F. H., 296 de Leeuw, F. E., 309, 317, 354
Codori, A. M., 232 Crews, F. T., 408, 412, 431 De Nil, L. F., 254
Coe, R. M., 703 Cristiani, S. A., 380 de Obaldia, R., 428, 429
Coffey, B. J., 245, 246 Critchley, M., 54 De Sonneville, L. M. J., 285, 289,
Coffey, C., 317, 530 Croft, R. J., 458, 467 290, 294
Coffey, S. F., 465 Cronbach, L. J., 42 Dean, R. S., 5, 204
Cohen, A. I., 294 Cronholm, B., 573 Deary, I. J., 352
Cohen, B. D., 66, 572 Cronin-Golomb, A., 176 Deb, S., 625, 627
Cohen, G., 694 Cronqvist, S., 321 Debes, F., 498, 500, 501
Cohen, H. L., 419, 419 Crook, T. H., 84, 85, 94, 95 DeCarli, C., 316, 317
Cohen, J., 284, 353 Crosson, B., 325, 566 Defer, G. L., 203
Cohen, N., 568, 569, 701 Crovitz, H. F., 34, 566, 583 Deffenbacher, J. L., 656
Cohen, R. M., 536 Croxford, J. L., 380 DeFilippis, N. A., 72
Cohen-Zion, M., 342 Crum, R. M., 107, 139 DeFronzo, R. A., 359
Col, N., 689, 694, 695 Crundall, D., 656 DeGutis, J. M., 59
Cole, B. L., 661 Crutch, S. J., 32 Deif, A., 458
Cole, M., 29, 413 Cuellar, I., 143 Dejgaard, A., 354
Coleman, R. D., 665, 672 Cuetos, R., 172 DeJong, C. A., 425
Coles, A., 280, 281, 283 Cullen, B., 107, 118 Delaney, R. C., 572
Collerton, D., 208 Cullum, C. M., 142 Delbeuck, X., 169
Collette, F., 175, 176 Cummings, J. L., 103, 160, 162, D’Elia, L., 140
Collia, D. V., 652 201, 202, 206, 207, 212, 248, Delis, D. C., 43, 44, 45, 55, 56, 72,
Collie, A., 92, 171 313, 524, 525, 528, 531, 534, 165, 166, 174, 206, 224, 227,
Collins, L. F., 633 543, 674 292, 370, 371, 526, 574, 569,
Collins, S. J., 212 Curran, H. V., 462 573, 638
Coltheart, M., 52 Curtis, D., 242 Delisi, L., 509
Comi, G., 295 Curyto, K. J., 102 Della Sala, S., 169
Comings, B. G., 250, 253 Cushman, L. A., 312 Deloire, M. S., 293
Comings, D. E., 247, 250, 253, 241 Cutler, R. G., 378 DeLuca, J., 70, 290, 291, 324, 325,
Como, P. G., 253, 254 Cutting, J. C., 427 566, 623
Compston, A., 280, 281, 283 Cynn, V. E., 403 Demakis, G. J., 101, 118, 203
Confavreux, C., 280, 281 Cysique, L. A., 92, 117, 372, 378, Demaree, H. A., 289, 290, 296
Conn, V., 694 382, 635, den Heijer, T., 356
Connor, D. J., 177, 178, 208 Dencker, S. J., 700
Conry, J., 421 Dahlen, E. R., 656 Deng, H., 243
Cools, R., 205, 207 Daigneault, G., 662, 663 Denney, D. R., 285, 290, 293
Cooper, B., 695 Dal Pan, G. J., 366 Derouesne, J., 33
Cooper, J. A., 204, 206 Dally, S., 408 Desgranges, B., 169
Copersino, M. L., 118 Dalton, C. M., 295 Desmond, D. W., 181, 312, 537
Corbett, D., 410 Dalton, E. J., 282 Desmond, J. E., 408
Corbett, J., 246, 252 Damasio, A. R., 67, 229, 175, 325, 372 D’Esposito, M., 169, 296, 289
Cordato, N. J., 212 Damasio, H., 67, 465 Devanand, D. P., 564
Corder, E. H., 162, 378, 626–627 Daniele, A., 203 Devane, W. A., 457
Cordier, S., 500, 502 Daniels, J. L., 498, 500 Devasenapathy, A., 315, 316
Corey-Bloom, J., 169 Danion, J. M., 531, 541 Devonshire, V. A., 283
Corkin, S., 532, 569, 560, 611 Danos, P., 468 deWall, C., 578, 580
Cormak, F., 177 Darke, S., 456, 469 Di, S. V., 460
Cornblatt, B., 508, 524 Daum, I., 584 Di Figlia, M., 164
Author Index 717

Di Sclafani, V., 460 Eapen, V., 241, 242, 245, 247, 248, 250 Everett, M., 404
Diamond, A., 203, 291, 325 Eastman, C. J., 359 Ewart, C. K., 701
Diaz, F., 318 Eastwood, M., 536
Diaz-Arrastia, R., 611, 627 Ebers, G. C., 283 Fabian, M. S., 402, 404, 410, 415, 416,
Dichter, M. A., 267 Ebmeier, K. P., 525 428, 429
Dickerson, A. E., 653 Eby, D. W., 665 Factor, S. A., 207
Dickinson, D., 212, 510, 511 Eckardt, M. J., 398, 402, 417, 421, 428, Fagnoni, F. F., 382
Diehl, M., 319, 694 429, 432 Fain, M. J., 670
Dierckx, E., 544 Edinger, J. D., 699 Faletti, M. G., 92
Dietrich, K. N., 484, 493, 496 Edwards, J. D., 664, 666 Fals-Stewart, W., 115, 432
DiFranza, J. R., 424 Edwards, K. L., 415, 425, 429 Fann, J. R., 621
Dikmen, S., 423, 597, 602, 603, 604, Egan, V. G., 373 Fanselow, M. S., 581
604, 605, 606, 607, 608, 609, Egede, L. E., 537 Farah, M. J., 26, 31
610, 619, 621, Egeland, J., 526, 535, 536, 538 Farias, S. T., 634, 635, 639, 642
Dimatteo, M. R., 537, 700, 702, 705 Ehlers, C. L., 408, 419 Farinpour, R., 370
Dingfelder, S. F., 149 Ehrenreich, H., 458 Farmer, R. H., 428, 429
Dobbs, A. R., 292 Einstein, G. O., 694 Farr, S. P., 402
Dobkins, K. R., 169 Eisenstein, N., 114 Farre, M., 466
Doble, S. E., 635 Eisner, M. D., 537 Farrin, L., 525
Doctor, J. N., 608, 609 Elderkin-Thompson, V., 357, 525, 535 Fava, G. A., 538
Dodd, M. L., 207 Elfgren, C., 180 Fazekas, F., 627
Dodrill, C. B., 274 Elger, C. E., 274 Fearnley, J. M., 202
Doehring, D. G., 11 Elias, M., 101 Fedio, P., 167, 224, 226
Dolan, R. J., 532 Elkin, I., 535 Fedoroff, J. P., 620
Dolder, C. R., 693, 700 Ellenberg, L., 428, 429 Fehm, H. L., 360
Dong, M. J., 162 Elliott, A., 378 Feigin, A., 231
Doniger, G. M., 87 Elliott, R., 524, 527, 533 Feigin, V., 307, 308, 312
Doninger, N. A., 102, 114, 116 Elliott, W. A., 428, 429, 432 Fein, G., 370, 428
Donovan, D. M., 403, 432 Ellis, C. E., 200 Feinstein, A., 102, 281, 282, 295,
Donovan, N. J., 309, 312, 325 Ellis, R. J., 366, 376, 377, 378, 411 296, 523
Doody, R. S., 318 Elmer, L., 202, 210 Feldman, M. J., 45
Dorn, L. D., 538 Elstein, D., 87 Feldman, R. G., 480, 482, 484, 485,
Dorsey, E. R., 199 Elstner, K., 249, 251 496, 497
Doty, R. L., 72 Elwood, R. W., 86 Fell, J. C., 658
Douglas, N. J., 340 Emery, C., 339, 340 Fenton, W. S., 700
Douglas, S. D., 383 Emery, V. B., 317 Ferenczi, S., 242
Drachman, D. A., 571, 637 Emmerson, R. Y., 409, 427 Ferguson, S. C., 354, 358
Drake, A. I., 411, 430 Emre, M., 204, 207, 208, 209 Fergusson, D. M., 493
Drake, R. E., 698 Ende, G., 430 Ferman, T. J., 178
Drane, D. L., 114, 116, 117 Endsley, M. R., 654 Fernandez, L. C., 695
Draper, R. J., 427 Engel, D. C., 269 Fernandez-Duque, D., 293, 294
Drasgow, J., 45 Engelhart, C. I., 116 Ferrando, S., 382
Drayer, B. P., 316 Engleman, H., 341 Ferraro, F. R., 142
Drazkowski, J., 672 Epstein, J., 539 Ferrell, R. B., 623
Drejer, K., 418 Erbas, B., 408 Ferreri, F., 207
Drevets, W. C., 527 Erenberg, G., 242, 245, 250 Ferrier, I. N., 539
Drevets, W. C., 530, 532 Erickson, K. I., 161 Ferrier, I. N., 540
Drexler, M., 700 Erickson, R. C., 128, 140 Ferris, S., 96
Driver-Dunckley, E., 207 Erkinjuntti, T., 103, 313, 314, Festa, E., 169
Drummond, S. R., 704 315, 317 Festa, J. R., 320, 321
D’Souza, D. C., 457 Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L., 508 Feuerlein, W., 405, 424
Dubinsky, R. M., 653, 670, 671, 674 Ernhart, C. B., 493 Feuerstein, C., 341, 342
Dubois, B., 160, 172 Ernst, T., 464 Fields, S., 469
Duchek, J. M., 661, 662, 670, 674 Errico, A. L., 427 Fieschi, C., 308
Dujardin, K., 203, 206 Ersche, K. D., 468, 469 Figlewicz, D. P., 359
Duke, L. M., 176 Ervin, C. S., 421 Filippi, M., 295
Dumont, G. J., 466 Esiri, M. M., 382 Fillmore, M. T., 460, 461
Dunbar-Jacob, J., 689 Eslinger, P. J., 67, 107, 112, 165, Filoteo, J. V., 205, 226, 229
Duncan, J., 169, 542, 661 175, 624 Finkelstein, E., 598
Dunn, E. J., 641 Espino, D. V., 146 Finlayson, M. A., 128, 131
Dupont, R. M., 408 Essman, W. B., 563 Finn, P. R., 408
Durand, D., 410 Etnier, J., 338, 340 Fischer, J. S., 290
Durvasula, R. S., 379, 380 Ettinger, A., 274 Fischman, M. W., 459
Dustman, R. E., 339 Evans, D. A., 564 Fishbein, D. H., 469
Duvoisin, R., 207 Evans, J. D., 515, 642 Fisher, C. M., 308, 323
Duyao, M. P., 224 Evans, J. J., 703 Fisher, R. S., 267
Dwolatzky, T., 85 Evans, M. E., 36 Fishman, J. A., 636
Dworetzky, B. A., 274 Everall, I. P., 383 Fisk, G. D., 662, 672
718 Author Index

Fisk, J. D., 108, 285, 290 Fromm-Auch, D., 131 Gifford, D. R., 103
Fitten, L. J., 645, 660, 670 Fu, C. H. Y., 533 Giladi, N., 202
Fitzhugh, L. C., 11, 401, 411, 428 Fuhrer, R., 111 Gilbert, S. G., 501
Fitzsimmons, B. F., 322 Fujii, D., 623 Gilboa-Schechtman, E., 532
Fladby, T., 114 Fujishima, M., 309 Gilchrist, A. C., 296
Flannery, B., 402 Fukuda, M., 203 Gillberg, C., 241, 247
Fleischhacker, W. W., 700 Fuld, P. A., 109, 166, 171, 291, 569, Gillen, R., 418
Fleischman, D. A., 175, 694 573, 575 Gilles de la Tourette, G., 241, 245,
Fleminger, S., 613 Fuller, R. K., 416 246, 250
Fletcher-Jansen, E., 142 Fullerton, J., 469 Gilley, D. W., 665–666
Flicker, C., 166 Fulmer, T., 693 Gillin, J. C., 424
Flynn, J. R., 129 Funkiewiez, A., 203 Gilman, S., 212, 213, 408
Fogarty-Mack, P., 319 Furlan, A. J., 320 Gilmer, T. P., 689
Fogel, B. S., 110 Furman, S. M., 657 Gilroy, J., 306
Fogel, M. L., 68 Ginsberg, M. D., 308
Foley, D. J., 665 Gabrieli, J. D., 159, 175, 228, 694 Giovannetti-Carew, T. G., 318
Foliart, R. H., 295 Gaede, P., 352 Giovannoni, G. O., 207
Folstein, M. F., 53, 107, 109, 139, 146, Gaffney, G. R., 242 Gispen, W. H., 357, 358
164, 174, 223, 224, 642 Gainotti, G., 31 Gladsjo, J. A., 148, 510, 514
Fonda, S. J., 652, 653 Galasko, D., 177, 178 Glass, M., 457
Foong, J., 292, 293, 294, 295 Gallant, J. E., 376 Glenn, S. W., 402, 425, 432, 433
Ford, H., 281 Galloway, P. H., 178 Glosser, G., 60, 72, 403
Forder, E., 56 Gallucci, M., 407 Gnanalingham, K. K., 177
Forget, H., 538 Galski, T., 660, 661, 664, 665, 668, Goetz, C. G., 242, 256
Forman, S. D., 468 669, 672 Goetz, M. B., 382
Foroud, T., 231 Galton, F., 583 Goggin, K. J., 383
Forsberg, L. K., 428, 433 Galvan, F. H., 379 Gold, A. E., 357
Förstl, H., 180 Gambini, O., 512 Gold, G., 314
Forton, D. M., 381, 423 Gamma, A., 466 Gold, J. J., 513, 561, 562
Fossati, P., 524, 526, 527, 532, 539 Gan, X., 380 Goldapple, K., 527
Foster, J. K., 169 Gange, J. J., 291 Goldberg, T. E., 378, 508, 700
Foulkes, M. A., 607 Ganguli, M., 102, 107, 689 Goldblum, M., 166
Fox, G. K., 660, 666, 667, 668, 674 Ganzini, L., 537 Golden, C. J., 42, 60, 72
Fox, H. C., 467 Gaoni, Y., 457 Golden, G. S., 246, 252, 257
Franceschi, M., 405 Garcia, J. H., 317 Golding, C. V., 231
Frangou, S., 539 Garcia-Albea, J. E., 146 Golding, E., 38
Frank, S., 253 Garde, E., 356 Goldman, M. S., 402, 428, 432, 433
Frankel, M., 248 Gardner, M. K., 428 Goldman, R. S., 428
Franklin, T. R., 459 Garland, M. A., 418 Goldman-Rakic, P. S., 510
Franzen, M. D., 633, 636, 640, Garrett, K. D., 314, 315, 316, 317, 318 Goldstein, F. C., 292, 312
641, 669 Garruto, R. M., 482 Goldstein, G., 401, 402, 412, 425, 426,
Fraser, H. F., 469 Gasquoine, P., 143 632, 633, 639
Fraser, L. M., 563 Gates, J., 274 Goldstein, K., 618, 622, 627
Fraser, R., 603 Gaudino, E. A., 290, 294 Goldstein, R. Z., 456, 459, 461
Frasure-Smith, N., 537 Gauthier, S., 102 Goldstein, S. G., 401, 402
Fratiglioni, L., 161 Gautier, J. C., 323 Golias, J., 669
Fratiglioni, L., 312 Gawin, F. H., 460 Golob, E. J., 169
Frazer, D., 324 Gazdzinski, S., 430, 431 Golomb, J., 538
Freedman, M., 167, 169 Gazmararian, J. A., 695 Gomaa, A., 495
Freedman, M., 55 Ge, Y., 280, 281 Gomez, R. G., 538
Freeman, E. E., 666 Gebhardt, C. A., 407 Gomez-Tortosa, E., 164, 177, 226
Freeman, R. D., 247, 250, 251 Gechter, G. L., 428, 429 Gongvatana, A., 368, 371
Freeman, W. R., 374 Geckle, M. O., 355, 360 Gontkovsky, S. T., 285
Freides, D., 79 Geier, S. A., 374 Gonzalez, E., 378
French, J. A., 672 Geisinger, K. F., 142 Gonzalez, R., 378, 458, 464, 465, 659
French, L. R., 612 George, C. F., 340 Gonzalez-Scarano, F., 366, 377
Freund, B., 662, 665, 666 George, D. T., 35 Goodglass, H., 43, 45, 72, 111, 115,
Freund, G., 410 George, M. S., 248, 532 198, 322, 355
Frey, K. A., 245 Georgiou, N., 223, 255 Goodwin, D. W., 419
Fried, P., 458 Gerhand, S., 34 Gorenstein, C., 462, 541
Fried, R., 673 Gerra, G., 468 Gorenstein, E. E., 425
Friedman, G., 611 Gerstley, L. J., 419 Gorham, D. R., 45
Friedman, H., 287 Geschwind, N., 43 Gorno-Tempini, M., 532
Friedman, J. I., 508, 517, 518 Geser, F., 208 Gotlib, I. H., 531
Frier, B. M., 358 Ghaffar, O., 523 Gould, O. N., 690
Frisoni, G. B., 179 Ghika, J., 203 Gourdie, A., 242, 251
Frodl, T., 529 Ghoneim, M. M., 458 Gouzoulis-Mayfrank, E., 458,
Froehlich, T. E., 111 Gibson, G. E., 413 466, 467
Author Index 719

Gozal, D., 341 Guridi, J., 203 Havrankova, J., 359


Grace, J., 645, 662, 663, 664, 671 Gurland, B. J., 118, 693 Hawley, C. A., 618, 626
Grady, C. L., 159, 167, 169 Gurling, H., 469 Haxby, J. V., 167, 169
Grady, M., 45 Gurwitz, J. J., 693 Hayden, M. R., 223
Graf, C. J., 320 Gusella, J. F., 224 Hayes, C. J., 229
Graf, P., 569, 584, 585 Guskiewicz, K. M., 162 Haynes, R. B., 689, 703
Grafman, J., 291, 294, 601 Gutheil, T. G., 675 Haythornthwaite, J. A., 541, 542
Graham, N. L., 164, 181 Guthrie, A., 428, 429, 432 He, J., 342
Grande, L., 55 Gutierrez, S., 469 Heald, A. H., 538
Grandjean, P., 498, 500, 502 Guttman, C. R., 169 Heaton, R. K., 17, 42, 60, 72, 127, 128,
Granholm, E., 412, 508 130, 131, 132, 136, 138, 144,
Grant, B. F., 422 Haaland, K. Y., 140 147, 150, 291, 294, 339, 370,
Grant, D. A., 45 Haase, A. T., 366 373, 375, 376, 378, 379, 427,
Grant, I., 290, 291, 295, 337, 367, 368, Hachinski, V., 306, 307, 309, 313, 314, 511, 514, 632, 633, 634, 635,
370, 378, 379, 380, 398, 400, 315, 316, 317, 326 636, 637, 638, 639, 640, 643,
402, 403, 411, 415, 417, 423, Hadjivassiliou, M., 324 644, 646, 647, 673, 693, 700
424, 426, 427, 428, 429, 458, Haeberle, K. C., 56 Hedden, T., 159
461, 469 Haefely, W. E., 462 Hefter, H., 223
Grant, M. M., 524, 525, 526, 527 Haenninen, H., 480, 485, 486 Heikkila, V. M., 671
Grant-Webster, K. S., 501 Hagberg, B., 180 Heilbrun, A. B., 66
Grasset, J., 246 Hahn-Barma, V., 231 Heilman, K. M., 59, 572
Graves, A. B., 612 Hakamies-Blomqvist, L., 666 Heindel, W. C., 175, 228, 584
Gray, J. A., 131 Halkitis, P. N., 696 Heinrichs, R. W., 295, 511,
Gray, J. M., 231 Hall, J. G., 418 514, 700
Gray, S. L., 689, 693 Hall, W., 456 Heisler, M., 695
Graybiel, A. M., 228 Hallett, M., 203 Helkala, E. L., 358
Greco, J. B., 376 Halliday, G. M., 210 Hellawell, D. J., 324
Green, A. R., 466 Halpern, J. H., 467 Helmes, E., 86
Green, J. E., 379 Halstead, W. C., 3, 43, 45, 72 Helmstaedter, C., 270
Green, L. W., 701 Hamann, C., 53 Hemmings, S. M., 248
Green, M. F., 514, 515, 637, 639, Hamil, W. L., 458 Henderson, R., 207
642, 701 Hamilton, J. M., 178, 179, 224 Hendler, N., 462
Green, R. C., 251 Hammar, A., 526 Hennenlotter, A., 230
Greenberg, G. D., 342 Hamsher, K., 67 Henry, J. D., 174, 226, 284, 285,
Greene, J. D. W., 166 Han, S. D., 162 286, 526
Greene, R. L., 140, 428, 429 Hanes, K. R., 175, 176 Hepke, K. L., 689
Greene, T., 493 Hannay, H. J., 76, 283, 285 Herman, D. O., 130
Greenhill, L., 252 Hannon, R., 416 Hermann, B. P., 270
Greenwood, P. M., 159 Hansen, L., 164, 177 Hernandez-Lopez, C., 466
Gregg, E. W., 357 Hansen, O. N., 492 Herridge, M. S., 345
Gregory, C. A. S., 180 Harding, A. J., 210 Herrmann, N., 645
Gregson, R. A., 428, 432 Hardy, D. J., 369, 370 Hershey, L. A., 318
Greicius, M. D., 533 Hardy, J., 201 Hess, R. M., 323
Greve, K. W., 18 Harik, L., 541 Hesselbrock, M. N., 425
Griffin, W. C., 380 Harlow, J., 529 Hesselbrock, V. M., 418, 419
Griffith, N., 297, 638 Harper, C. G., 409, 410, 412 Hester, R., 428, 429, 461
Griffiths, S. Y., 290, 292 Harper, P. S., 223 Heun, R., 118
Grigoletto, F., 115 Harrington, D. L., 371 Heyman, A., 178, 612
Grober, C., 171 Harris, G. J., 231 Hickie, I., 530
Grober, E., 573, 575 Harris, J. G., 143 Hierons, R., 307
Gronwall, D., 606 Harris, R., 701 Higgins, S. T., 459
Gross, C. R., 320 Hartlage, S., 525 Higginson, C. I., 283, 294
Gross, J. S., 666 Harvan, J. R., 107 Hightower, J. M., 497
Grossman, M., 180, 204, 208, 294 Harvey, P. D., 508, 510, 511, 513, 514, Hightower, M. G., 403
Grotenhermen, F., 457, 658, 659 515, 516, 517, 635, 642 Higuchi, M., 177
Grove, E., 56 Harvey, P. O., 525, 526, 527, 533 Hilakivi, I., 656
Gualtieri, C. T., 524, 525, 542 Hasher, L., 525, 527, 694 Hildebrandt, H., 294, 295,
Guerra, D., 469 Hashimoto, H., 320 309, 403
Guerrera, M. P., 424 Hashimoto, M., 178 Hill, M. D., 311
Guerrier, J. H., 663 Hasking, P., 468 Hill, R. D., 94
Gugerty, L. J., 656 Haslegrave, C. M., 658 Hill, S. Y., 416, 469
Guidi, M., 178 Hassing, L. B., 359 Hillbom, E., 623
Guilmette, T. J., 636, 638, 639 Hassler, O., 323 Hillbom, M., 423, 429
Guinon, G., 246 Hathaway, S. R., 72, 635 Hillert, A. E., 283
Gunning-Dixon, F. M., 169, Hatsukami, D. K., 459 Hillis, A. E., 26, 31, 324
317, 356 Haug, J. O., 405 Hilsabeck, R. C., 423
Gunstad, J., 84 Haug, T., 324 Hilsted, J., 358
Gur, R. C., 408, 531, 542 Haughton, H., 428, 429 Hindrik, W., 659
720 Author Index

Hinkeldey, N. S., 625 Hunt, L., 645, 653, 660, 662, 663, 664, Jarvenpaa, T., 118
Hinkin, C. H., 369, 370, 375, 376, 382, 667, 670, 671, 676 Jarvik, J. G., 366
383, 645, 696, 697, 698 Hunt, W. E., 323 Jarvik, M. E., 559, 573
Hinton, S. C., 231 Huntington, G., 224 Jasinski, D. R., 427
Hiott, D. W., 622 Hur, Y. M., 656 Jastak, J. F., 45
Hirtz, D., 200 Hurd, P. D., 694 Jastak, S. R., 45
Hiscock, C. K., 374 Hurley, S., 86 Jefferson, A. L., 637
Hiscock, M., 374, 421 Hurst, R. W., 320, 321 Jelic, V., 169
Hitch, G. J., 370 Hurtig, H. I., 210 Jellinger, K. A., 162, 210
Hjalmarsen, A., 339 Hutter, B. O., 325 Jenkins, R. L., 410
Ho, A. K., 226, 229, 230 Huybrechts, K. F., 689 Jennekens-Schinkel, A., 291
Ho, W. Z., 383 Hyde, T. M., 251 Jennett, B., 566, 600, 602, 620, 606
Hobbs, G. E., 656 Hyman, B. T., 169, 564 Jennings, J. M., 695
Hoblyn, J. C., 541 Jensen, G. B., 410
Hobson, D. E., 671 Iidaka, T., 532 Jernigan, T. J., 169
Hochanadel, G., 61 Illarioshkin, S. N., 224 Jernigan, T. L., 367, 381, 405, 406,
Hochla, N. A. N., 417 Incagnoli, T., 252, 253 407, 414
Hodak, J. A., 316 Ince, P. G., 164, 165 Jerome, L., 673
Hodges, J. R., 31, 37, 164, 166, 167, Ineichen, B., 160 Jeste, S. D., 693
174, 176, 180, 226, 227, 230, Inglis, J., 411 Jeyaratnam, J., 486
563, 581 Insel, K., 695 Jin, Y. P., 309
Hodgins, D. C., 424 Inzitari, D., 315 Johanson, A., 180
Hodgson, T. L., 205 Irle, E., 324 Johanson, C. E., 464, 465, 466
Hodkinson, H. M., 108 Irvine, S. H., 142 Johnson, B., 459, 464
Hoekstra, P. J., 243, 245 Irwin, M., 422, 423, 429 Johnson, D. K., 178
Hof, P. R., 169 Isaac, C. L., 562 Johnson, J. L., 418
Hoff, A. L., 507 Isaac, L. M., 694 Johnson, M. O., 702
Hoffman, J. J., 425 Isbell, H., 469 Johnson, S. K., 292, 537
Hoffman, W. F., 464, 465, 466 Ishibashi, M., 424 Johnston, D. C. C., 311
Hoffstein, V., 342 Ishikawa, Y., 408 Johnston, R. B., 281, 282, 283
Hofmeister, C., 320 Itard, J. M. G., 241, 245 Jolles, J., 356
Hogan, M. J., 169 Ito, M., 320 Jonah, B. A., 656
Hogstedt, C., 486 Iudicello, J. E., 372 Jones, B. M., 401, 403, 405, 410, 415,
Hollows, S. E., 35 Iverson, G. L., 117 416, 417
Holm, L., 423, 429 Ivnik, R. J., 95, 140, 141, 142 Jones, M. K., 415, 575
Holmes, A. J., 533 Ivory, S. J., 204 Jones, R. D., 72
Hom, J., 75, 308, 309 Iwama, T., 319, 321 Jordan, B. D., 162
Honer, W. G., 296 Jorge, R., 612
Hook, J. N., 538 Jack, C. R., 169 Jorge, R. E., 619, 620, 621, 622,
Hooper, H. E., 45, 72 Jackevicius, C. A., 689 623, 625
Hopewell, C. A., 675 Jacklin, C. N., 131 Jorm, A. F., 111, 160, 357, 531
Hopkins, R. O., 345 Jackson, M., 404, 405, 424 Josiassen, R. C., 176, 224
Horn, J. L., 43, 138 Jacobs, D., 166 Jost, W. H., 201
Hornsey, H., 241, 247, 249 Jacobs, D. H., 229 Jovanovski, D., 460, 659
Horswill, M. S., 657 Jacobs, D. M., 142, 171 Joy, J. E., 281, 282, 283
Horvath, T. B., 399 Jacobs, I. G., 459 Joy, S., 47, 49
Horwitz, R. I., 689 Jacobs, J. W., 109, 115 Joyce, E. M., 403, 413
House, A., 311 Jacobs, M., 274 Joynt, N. J., 68
Houston, W. S., 173 Jacobson, A. M., 352 Justus, A., 408
Howard, D., 32 Jacobson, M. W., 172, 173
Howden, R., 307 Jacobson, R. R., 406, 412, 414, 430 Kadesjo, B., 241
Howieson, D. B., 171 Jacoby, J. H., 357 Kagerer, S., 658
Hozumi, A., 205 Jaffe, C., 464 Kahn, R. L., 109
Hu, H., 484, 494 Jaffe, D., 87 Kahneman, D., 356
Huang, J., 323 Jager, T., 563 Kail, R., 285
Hubacek, J. A., 162 Jagusch, W., 358 Kalbe, E., 117
Huber, S. J., 295 Jahanshahi, M., 203 Kalechstein, A. D., 371, 464, 465,
Huff, F. J., 165, 167 Jain, N. B., 498 466, 467
Hugdahl, K., 533 James, M., 29, 227 Kales, H. C., 306
Hughes, A. J., 213 James, W., 571 Kalichman, S. C., 702
Hughes, J., 468 Janke, M. K., 668 Kalin, N., 532, 533
Hughes, P. K., 661 Jankovic, J., 199, 203, 212, 245 Kallanranta, T., 671
Huijbregts, S. C., 294 Janowsky, J. S., 413, 564, 565, 573, Kalmijn, S., 358
Hulicka, I. M., 140 575, 576, 579 Kampf-Sherf, O., 542
Hultsch, D. F., 161 Janssen, R. S., 376 Kampman, O., 700
Hunt, A. L., 318 Janulewicz, P. A., 481, 501 Kandel, E. R., 567
Hunt, E., 44 Janvin, C. C., 208 Kane, R. L., 86, 88, 253
Hunt, H. F., 20 Janz, N. K., 701 Kanner, A., 274
Author Index 721

Kantarci, O., 280, 281, 283 Kish, G. B., 428, 429 Kurtzke, J. F., 283
Kaplan, E., 28, 42, 43, 44, 45, 46, 51, Kitada, T., 201 Kurylo, D. D., 169
52, 53, 54, 55, 61, 62, 72, 111, Kivisaari, R., 468 Kushner, H. L., 242
115, 322 Kjeldsen, M. J., 268 Kuypers, K. P., 466, 668
Kaptein, N. A., 672 Kjellstrom, T., 498, 499 Kwak, C., 245
Kapur, N., 84, 85 Klapow, J. C., 642
Karasievich, G. O., 428, 429 Klawans, H., 256 Labbate, L., 622
Kartsounis, L. D., 31 Klein, D., 534 Labouvie-Vief, G., 140
Karzmark, P., 61, 116 Kleinknecht, R. A., 402 Lackner, J. R., 560
Kaseda, Y., 409 Kliegel, M., 635 Lackovic, Z., 357
Kassell, N. F., 306, 307, 323, 324 Klimo, P., 319 Lacritz, L. H., 203
Kastner, M. P., 636, 638, 639 Kline, D. W., 694 Lacro, J. P., 701
Kaszniak, A. W., 160, 165, 167, 169, Klisz, D. K., 417 Lafosse, J. M., 181, 313
176, 228 Klonoff, H., 140 LaFrance, W. C., 274
Katai, S., 204 Klonoff, P. S., 618 Lage, G. A., 618
Katon, W., 537 Klunk, W. E., 210 Lai, B. C., 200
Katzman, R., 108, 160, 161, 162, 170, Kløve, H., 45 Laiacona, M., 325
171, 564, 612 Knauper, B., 533 Lakmache, Y., 169
Kaufman, A., 130, 536 Knell, E. R., 247 Lal, B. K., 101
Kaufman, N., 536 Knight, H., 342 Lamar, M., 181
Kawabata, K., 210 Knopman, D. S., 118, 160, 165, 166, Lamberty, G. J., 531, 534
Kawas, C. H., 160, 161, 162, 171 223, 228 Lambon Ralph, M. A., 177
Kay, G. G., 85, 86, 87, 88, 89, 95, 96 Knott, V., 169 Lamers, C. T. J., 466
Keck, P., 699 Knowlton, B. J., 228 Lamp, R. E., 145
Keedwell, P., 539 Knutson, B., 539 Lampley-Dallas, V. T., 117
Keefe, R. S. E., 510, 513, 514, 517 Kodituwakku, P. W., 421 Lamy, P. P., 703
Keegan, B. M., 280 Koenig, P., 172 Landro, N. I., 296, 525, 537
Keen-Kim, D., 243 Koivisto, T., 325 Lane, D. M., 285
Keenan, S. P., 342 Kokmen, E., 108 Lanfranchi, P., 344
Kegeles, L. S., 532 Kolb, B., 528 Lang, C. J., 227
Kelder, W., 378 Kondo, K., 612 Langdon, D. W., 27, 32
Kelleher, L. M., 458 Kopelman, M. D., 166, 561, 564, 567, Lange, K. L., 171
Kellison, I. L., 656, 657, 661, 663 569, 580, 581, 583, 584, 589 Lange, K. W., 167, 175, 176
Kelly, E., 658 Koponen, S., 621, 624, 625, 627 Lange, R. T., 423
Kelman, D. H., 248 Kordas, K., 495 Langenecker, S. A., 525, 526, 531, 533,
Keltikangas-Jarvinen, L., 162 Korin, H., 469 542, 543
Kendler, K. S., 417 Koroshetz, W. J., 228 Langford, D., 367
Kendrick, R., 694 Korostil, M., 296 Langlois, J. A., 597
Kenealy, P. M., 293 Korteling, J. E., 672 Lanphear, B. P., 495, 496
Kennedy, M., 140 Kosaka, K., 164 Lanz, M., 295
Kennedy, S. E., 532 Kostandov, E. A., 411 Lanzi, G., 241
Kensinger, E. A., 532 Kotterba, S., 671 Larisch, R., 532
Kerbeshian, J., 250 Kozora, E., 338, 340 Larkin, J. H., 704
Kerr, B. B., 541 Kramer, A. F., 159, 161, 653 Larrabee, G. J., 84, 85, 94, 95
Kertesz, A., 165, 178, 181 Kramer, J. H., 227, 292, 404, 417 Laska, A. C., 537
Kessler, H. R., 294 Kramer, M., 160 Lau, J. T., 241
Keswani, S., 374 Kraybill, M. L., 178 Lauterbach, E. C., 206, 207, 212
Ketter, T. A., 533 Kreiter, K. T., 325 Laux, L. F., 295
Khalifa, N., 241, 243, 247, 251 Kremen, W. S., 514 Lavin, D. E., 636
Khavari, K. A., 406 Kreutzer, J. S., 611 Lavoie, M. E., 256
Kidd, D., 280 Kribbs, N. B., 342 Law, W. A., 370
Kieburtz, K., 224 Krieg, E. F., Jr., 489 Lawrence, A. D., 175, 176, 207, 226,
Kiernan, R. J., 110, 116 Kril, J. J., 409, 410 227, 228, 229, 231, 372
Kilander, L., 316 Krishnan, K. R., 530 Lawrence, E. J., 625
Kilgore, K. M., 638 Kritchevsky, M., 563, 566, 567 Lawrence, J., 102
Kim, J., 511, 693 Krohn, E. J., 145 Laws, K. R., 32
Kim, J. J., 581 Krop, H., 337, 339 Lawson, J. S., 411
Kim, J. S., 228 Krystal, J. H., 468 Lawton, C., 658
Kim, S., 703 Kubu, C. S., 203 Lawton, M. P., 637, 644
Kimura, D., 72 Kudoh, A., 541 Layden, T. A., 428, 429
King, D. A., 523, 528, 530, 535, 536 Kujala, P., 285 Lazar, R. M., 311, 319, 321, 322
King, J. T., 115 Kukull, W. A., 112 Lazeron, R. H. C., 289, 295
King, M. A., 410 Kumar, A., 317, 534 Leber, W. R., 401, 402, 403, 404, 405,
Kinnear-Wilson, S. A., 248 Kumar, P., 487 411, 417, 428, 432
Kinsbourne, M., 32 Kumari, V., 533 Leckliter, I. N., 129, 131, 140
Kirby, K. N., 461, 469 Kuo, M. F., 241 Leckman, J. F., 242, 243, 245, 246, 248
Kirkwood, S. C., 231 Kurl, S., 321 Ledbetter, M., 86
Kirwan, C. B., 584 Kurlan, R., 241, 242, 246, 250 Lee, A., 529
722 Author Index

Lee, C., 243 Locascio, J. J., 165, 167 Malec, J. F., 141
Lee, G. P., 142, 530 Lockwood, K. A., 357 Malhi, G. S., 540
Lee, H. C., 669 Loeb, P. A., 641 Malik, G. M., 321
Lee, J. D., 652 Loewenstein, D. A., 635, 641 Malitz, S., 563
Lee, S. -H., 536 Logan, B. K., 659 Malloy, P. F., 118, 423, 425, 429
Leedom, L., 357 Loiselle, C. R., 243 Manly, J. J., 142, 145, 148, 149, 375
Leentjens, A. F., 206 Lombardi, W. J., 203 Mann, K., 406, 430
Lees, A., 202, 242, 243 London, E. D., 465, 468 Manning, A., 427
Lefleche, G., 167 Long, C. J., 633 Manning, L., 31
Lehericy, S., 322 Long, J. A., 428, 429 Manns, J. R., 569
Lehmann, J. F., 637 Longstreth, W. T., 317, 356 Manolio, T. A., 356
Lehtinen, K., 700 Looi, J. C. L., 314, 317 Manschot, S. M., 355, 356, 358, 359,
Leirer, V. O., 694, 703, 704 Lopez, J. F., 529 360
Leitner, Y., 87 Lopez, O. L., 372 Manzel, K., 72
Lemiere, J., 231 Lopez, S. R., 142, 145 Maor, Y., 296
Lencz, T., 507 Lorentz, W. J., 103, 118 Mapou, R. L., 379
Lengenfelder, J., 290, 296, 661 Loring, D. W., 319, 320, 663 Mapstone, M., 669
Lenne, M. G., 659, 669 Louko, A. M., 325 Marchesi, C., 428, 430
Lenz, L. P., 226 Lourenco, R. A., 107, 112 Marcos, T., 525
Leon-Carrion, J., 672 Lovera, J., 294 Marcotte, T. D., 116, 370, 375, 645,
Leonard, H. L., 243 Lowe, C., 693 653, 657, 660, 662, 663, 664,
Lerer, B., 563 Lowe, L. P., 357 665, 666, 667, 669, 671, 672,
Leri, F., 456 Lozano, A. M., 203 673, 676
Lerner, A., 244 Lu, C., 207 Marder, K., 201
Lesperance, F., 537 Lubin, A., 637 Marder, S. R., 700
Letendre, S. L., 381, 382, 383, 423 Lublin, F. D., 282 Margolin, A., 381
Leufkens, T. R., 541 Lucchinetti, C. F., 280 Markela-Lerenc, J., 526
Levin, B. E., 205, 208, 211 Lucchini, R., 487 Markowitsch, H., 562
Levin, H., 73, 565, 566, 600, 606, 621 Lucki, I., 462 Marks, J. L., 359
Levine, A. J., 380, 689, 691 Ludlow, C., 253, 256 Marmarou, A., 607
Levine, B., 45, 703 Lundberg, C., 662, 664, 665, 671 Marottoli, R. A., 653, 657, 663, 664
Levine, S. R., 308, 459 Lundqvist, A., 665 Marrie, R. A., 283
Levinson, T., 428, 429 Lunetta, M., 354 Marsden, C. D., 204, 205
Levy, D. A., 569, 586 Luo, F., 243 Marsh, N. V., 283
Lew, H. L., 669 Luria, A. R., 293, 622 Marshall, G. A., 320
Lewis, C. V., 404, 424 Luukinen, H., 612 Marshall, L. F., 607
Lewohl, J. M., 410 Lye, T. C., 613 Marshall, S., 658, 673
Lezak, M. D., 36, 42, 48, 70, 92, 101, Lynch, S. G., 294 Martin, A., 166, 167, 372
118, 128, 140, 285, 293, 576, Lyons, M. J., 459 Martin, D. C., 704
577, 579, 580, 586, 661, 662 Lyvers, M., 468, 469 Martin, E. M., 369, 370, 371, 375, 381
Lhermitte, F., 31 Løberg, T., 403, 411, 425, 426, 427 Martin, P. R., 398, 399, 402, 412
Liberman, J. N., 627 Martin, R., 274
Liberman, R. P., 700 Ma, X., 420 Martinez-Aran, A., 539
Libon, D. J., 55, 180, 313, 317, 318 Maas, A. I., 607 Martin-Garcia, J., 366, 377
Lichtenberg, P. A., 312 Macaulay, C., 117 Martino, D., 251
Lichtenberger, E. O., 284 Macciocchi, S. N., 432, 606 Martinot, M. L. P., 532
Lichtman, S. W., 611 Maccoby, E. E., 131 Martin-Soelch, C., 468
Lieberman, J., 509 Machamer, J., 610, 611 Martone, M., 227, 228
Liesker, J., 338 Machiyama, Y., 241 Maruff, P., 85, 171, 370
Liguori, A., 669 Macia, F., 213 Masliah, E., 169
Lilis, R., 480, 486 MacKenzie, D. M., 107, 113 Massman, P. J., 61, 166, 203, 204, 206
Limousin, P., 203 Mackin, R. S., 694, 696 Mast, H., 319, 320, 321
Lin, H., 243 MacKinnon, D., 583 Mastaglia, F. L., 405
Lincoln, N. B., 662, 671 Mackowiak, E. D., 703 Masterman, T., 283
Lindau, M., 179 MacNiell, S. E., 312 Masur, D. M., 315
Linden, J. D., 428, 429 MacQueen, G., 529 Matarazzo, J. D., 130, 131, 636, 637
Lindenmayer, J. P., 517 MacVane, J., 415 Matarazzo, J. E., 129, 131, 140
Lineweaver, T. T., 176 Maddock, R. J., 532 Mate-Kole, C. C., 317
Lings, S., 671 Maddox, W. T., 205 Mathew, R. J., 408
Linn, R. T., 171 Madeley, P., 671 Mathews, C. A., 251
Liolitsa, D., 355, 358 Maeda, K., 703 Mathuranath, P. S., 179
Liotti, M., 532 Magni, E., 107 Matsuda, L. A., 457
Lippa, C. F., 178, 208, 209, 210 Mahalick, D. M., 320, 322 Matsuda, S., 320, 321
Lipton, A. M., 180 Mahant, N., 224, 230 Matsui, H., 210
Lishman, W. A., 412, 414, 618, 623 Mahler, M. S., 242 Matsumoto, I., 409, 412
Littell, C., 536 Mahoney, R., 117 Matsuo, K., 533
Litvan, I., 203, 212, 289, 290 Mair, W. G. P., 562 Matthews, C. G., 85, 96
Lobnig, B. M., 354 Maisto, S. A., 416 Matthews, P. M., 295, 296
Author Index 723

Mattila, P. M., 210 Mendoza, P. S., 701 Molina, J. A., 207, 424
Mattis, S., 72, 110, 224, 586, 641 Menefee, F. L., 66 Moll, G. H., 247
Mattson, S. N., 420, 421 Meng, K., 118 Moller, J. C., 669
Mauri, M., 538 Mercer, P. W., 406 Molnar, F. J., 653, 660, 662, 665
Max, J. E., 618, 619, 626 Merdes, A. R., 177 Mondadori, C. R. A., 162
Max, W., 597–598 Merelli, E., 295 Moniz, E., 529
Mayberg, H. S., 527, 531, 532, 543 Merlin, M. D., 455 Monsch, A. U., 167, 174
Mayes, A., 559, 562 Mervaala, E., 529 Monson, N., 293, 294
Mayeux, R., 112, 162, 206, 207, 224, Messinis, L., 458 Montagne, B., 229
376, 378, 612, 613 Mesulam, M. M., 165, 180 Montalban, X., 282
Mayhew, D. R., 656 Meyer, A., 107, 307, 308, 618 Monteiro de Almeida, S., 377–378
Mazer, B. L., 653, 662 Meyer, J. S., 306, 313, 315 Monterosso, J. R., 465
McAllister, T. W., 623, 627 Meyerhoff, D. J., 379 Montgomery, M. A., 248, 249, 251
McArdle, N., 342 Michel, D., 572 Montoya, A., 174, 227
McArthur, J. C., 368, 377 Michon, J., 654 Moore, A., 700
McCall, R. B., 148 Michopoulos, I., 539 Moore, D., 115, 379, 497, 647
McCann, U. D., 467 Mickes, L., 172 Moore, R. D., 382
McCardle, K., 467 Miech, R. A., 161 Morgan, E. E., 373
McCarthy, R. A., 26, 29, 31, 33, 34, Miglioni, M., 410 Morgan, M., 467
37, 581 Miguel, E. C., 248 Morgan, S., 603
McCarty, S. M., 140 Miguez, M. J., 379 Mori, E., 177
McClure, M. M., 516, 643 Mihalka, L., 308 Moriarty, J., 244, 255
McCormick, W. F., 320 Mikhael, M. A., 469 Morice, R., 700
McCrady, B. S., 432 Mikhailova, E., 531 Mormont, E., 208
McCue, M., 637, 639 Milak, M. S., 532 Morphew, J. A., 246
McDaniel, J. S., 537 Milberg, W., 44, 45, 55, 47, 53, 78 Morrell, R. W., 694
McDaniel, M. A., 694 Milberger, S., 249 Morris, J. C., 166, 674
McDonald, W. I., 281, 282 Milders, M., 229 Morris, M., 232
McEwen, B. S., 538 Miles, W. R., 352 Morris, R., 46, 85
McGee, C. L., 420 Millar, K., 611, 627 Morrison, C. E., 203
McGurk, S. R., 516, 517 Miller, A. E., 281, 282 Morrison, D. F., 131
McGwin, G., 668 Miller, B. L., 178, 180 Morrison, J. H., 169
McGwin, G., Jr., 657 Miller, J. R., 85, 94 Mortimer, J. A., 161, 162, 613
McHenry, L. C., 241, 242 Miller, M., 86 Moscovitch, M., 166, 563, 586
McHugh, G. S., 607 Miller, R., 431 Moses-Kolko, E. L., 532
McHugh, P. R., 164, 223 Miller, W. R., 401, 402, 425, Moskowitz, H., 653, 658
McIntyre, B. M., 428, 429 426, 524 Moss, H. B., 412
McIntyre, N. J., 107, 112, 116 Millikin, C. P., 372 Moss, M. B., 174
McKee, R. D., 562 Milner, B., 34, 559, 560, 561, 571, 572, Mossello, E., 107
McKeith, I., 177, 202, 208, 210 576, 579 Mossner, R., 206
McKenna, P., 28, 31, 32, 33, 34, 38 Minden, S. L., 291 Moszczynska, A., 464, 465
McKhann, G., 101, 160, 314 Mindt, M. R., 644 Mueller, J., 107, 113, 116
McKibbin, C. L., 642 Mink, J. W., 244 Mula, M., 248, 249
McKinley, J. C., 72, 635 Minoshima, S., 177 Muldoon, S. B., 488, 490
McKnight, A. J., 663 Mintzer, M. Z., 469 Munn, M. A., 529
McKnight, A. S., 663 Mioshi, E., 109 Munn, N. D., 149
McLachlan, J. F. C., 428, 429 Mirotznik, J., 701 Munoz, D. G., 317
McLean, A., 602 Mirsen, T. R., 318 Munoz-Moreno, J. A., 382
McLellan, A. T., 419 Mirza, S. A., 377 Munro, C., 227
McLoughlin, D. M., 531 Mishkin, M., 94 Munshi, M., 55
McMahon, W. M., 242 Mitrushina, M., 139, 140, 577 Murata, K., 501
McMichael, A. J., 492, 496 Mittenberg, W., 84, 374 Murden, R. A., 107
McMullen, P. A., 410, 431 Miyake, A., 229, 526 Murji, S., 370
McWilliams, L. A., 537 Miyao, S., 318 Murkin, J. M., 101
Mead, K., 704 Miyazato, Y., 409 Murphy, J., 319
Mechoulam, R., 457 Mizrahi, R., 624 Murphy, K. R., 105
Meco, G., 213 Mocco, J., 324 Murray, G. D., 607
Mega, M. S., 525 Moehle, K. A., 632, 638 Murray, L. L., 226
Meguro, K., 315 Mogg, K., 531 Murray, T. J., 241, 242, 280
Mehta, K. M., 612 Mohamed, S., 507 Musen, G., 354, 358
Meier, M. J., 5 Mohns, L., 461 Mushkudiani, N. A., 607
Meindorfner, C., 671 Mohr, D., 281, 283, 295, 296 Muslimovic, D., 206
Meissen, G., 224 Mohr, E., 89, 167, 204, 226, 227 Muuronen, A., 428, 429, 430
Melgaard, B., 408 Mohr, J. H., 320 Myers, E. D., 698
Mell, L. K., 243 Mohr, J. P., 320, 323 Myers, G. J., 497, 499
Mendelsohn, A. L., 494 Mohs, R. C., 464 Myers, R. H., 224, 483
Mendez, M. F., 177, 178, 180, 201, Mohsen, A. H., 381 Myers, R. S., 662
202, 207, 212 Molgat, C. V., 537 Müller, S. V., 225
724 Author Index

Naarding, P., 313 Ochsner, K., 532 Pandina, R. J., 418


Nabors, N. A., 116 O’Connor, M., 559 Pandovani, A., 167
Nadel, L., 166 O’Connor, S., 419 Pandya, D. N., 227
Nadler, J. D., 641 Oded, Y., 566 Pantoni, L., 317
Naegele, B., 341, 342 Odenheimer, G. L., 660, 663, 667 Papagno, C., 325
Nagai, M., 530 O’Donnell, J. P., 17 Papke, K., 324
Naismith, R. T., 283 Oehlert, M. E., 116 Paque, L., 28
Naismith, S. L., 526, 527 Oestreicher, J. M., 17 Paradiso, S., 533
Nakase-Richardson, R., 610 Ogden, E. J., 653, 658 Parasuraman, R., 167, 661
Nance, M. A., 224 Ogden, J. A., 324 Parati, E. A., 213
Nandi, D., 531 Ogura, C., 409 Parisi, J. E., 280
Nanson, J. L., 421 O’Hanlon, J. F., 676 Park, D. C., 356, 694, 695, 703, 704
Nasreddine, Z. S., 117 Ohayon, M. M., 342 Park, H. L., 159
Nath, A., 380 Ojemann, G. A., 572 Parker, D. A., 415
Nath, U., 211 Okada, G., 533 Parker, E. S., 415, 416
Nathan, P. W., 566 O’Leary, M. R., 411, 428, 429, 432 Parker, J. B., 402
Nathoo, N., 611, 627 Olesen, P. J., 373 Parkin, A. J., 414
Neal, B., 689 Olfson, M., 700 Parkin, D. M., 695
Neale, V. L., 661 Olley, A., 539 Parkinson, A., 486
Neary, D., 165, 180 Olmedo, E. L., 142 Parmenter, B. A., 289, 292, 295
Nebes, R., 167, 526 Oltedal, S., 656 Parrott, A. C., 466, 467
Needleman, H. L., 485, 491, 496 Olzak, M., 203 Parry, R., 32, 33
Nehl, C., 224 Ondra, S. L., 320 Parsons, O. A., 128, 129, 131, 398,
Neisser, U., 43, 44 Ongradi, J., 380 401, 402, 403, 404, 405, 409,
Nell, V., 149 Ongur, D., 530 410, 411, 415, 416, 417, 418,
Nelson, H. E., 28 Ökvist, A., 432 420, 427, 428, 429, 432, 433
Nemetz, P. N., 612 O’Malley, S., 460, 470 Parsons, T. D., 203
Nemiah, J. C., 567 O’Neill, D., 660 Parvizi, J., 296
Nestor, P. G., 661 O’Neill, J., 430 Pasanella, A. K., 636
Neundorfer, B., 405, 424 O’Quinn, A., 253 Pasquier, F., 179
Neundorfer, M. M., 383 Orfei, M. D., 665 Paterniti, S., 534
Newcombe, F., 565, 611 Ornstein, T. J., 469 Patten, S. B., 537
Newman, M. C., 169 Orozco, S., 143 Patterson, B. W., 403, 409
Newnan, O. S., 638 Orpwood, L., 33 Patterson, K., 32, 167
Newton, T. H., 321 Orr, J., 401, 402 Patterson, T. L., 635, 640, 641, 642,
Nicholas, M., 167 Orth, M., 249 645, 693
Nichols, D. E., 466 Osato, S. S., 116 Pau, C. W., 381
Nickelsen, T., 95 Osborn, A. G., 307 Paul, R. H., 293
Nicolas, J. M., 408 Oscar-Berman, M., 169, 409, 411, 563 Paul, S. L., 308
Nielsen-Bohlman, L., 371 Osmon, D. C., 116 Pauls, D. L., 243, 247, 248, 251
Nilsson, B., 307 Osterberg, K., 487 Paulsen, J. S., 176, 223, 224, 228, 229,
Nilsson, F. M., 527 Osterberg, L., 689, 690 231, 232, 511
Nishioka, H., 320 Osterreith, P., 45, 57, 58 Paulus, M. P., 465
Nissen, M. J., 228 Osterrieth, P. A., 576 Paxton, J. L., 167, 168
Nixon, K., 431 Ostrosky-Solis, F., 107, 113 Payami, H., 201
Nixon, S. J., 402, 403, 404, 420, 427 O’Sullivan, M., 159 Payne, R., 541
Noble, E. P., 409, 415, 416, 418 Ott, B. R., 663, 674 Peinemann, A., 176
Nocentini, U., 284, 288 Ottosson, J. O., 573 Pelli, D. G., 72
Noël, X., 403 Owen, A. M., 90, 527 Penner, I. K., 297, 295, 297
Noe, E., 177 Owen, M., 206 Pentland, B., 324
Noel, X., 403 Ownby, R., 695, 701, 705 Pentney, R. J., 410
Nolan, K. A., 313 Owsley, C., 652, 656, 657, 661, 662, Perez, J. L., 382
Nomoto, F., 241 670, 674 Perez-Garcia, M., 469
Noonberg, A., 417 Ozakbas, S., 289 Perfect, J. R., 377
Nordberg, M., 489, 490 Ozdemir, F., 312 Perret, G., 320
Norman, M. A., 144 Ozkaragoz, T. Z., 418 Perrett, L., 405
Nornes, H., 321 Ozonoff, S., 255 Perrine, K., 203
Northoff, G., 533 Perros, P., 354
Norton, L. E., 167 Paaver, M., 656 Perry, E. K., 209
Noseworthy, J. H., 280, 281, 283 Pachana, N. A., 179, 180, 669 Perry, R. H., 164
Novack, T. A., 672 Packer, L. E., 246 Perry, R. J., 167, 176
Nussbaum, P. D., 534 Paelecke-Habermann, Y., 524, Perry, W., 43
Nussbaum, R. L., 200 526, 535 Perryman, K. M., 645
Page, R. D., 410, 428, 429 Persad, C., 525
O’Brien, C., 213, 462 Pahwa, R., 204 Persad, S., 531
O’Brien, J., 102, 529, 530 Pakkenberg, B., 410 Pert, C. B., 468
O’Brien, P., 319 Palmer, B. W., 511, 514, 673 Perugi, G., 249
Obwegeser, A. A., 203 Palmer, P., 51 Pessin, M. S., 309
Author Index 725

Petersen, R. C., 102, 107, 118, Price, C. C., 181 Reitan, R. M., 5, 6, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12, 13,
171, 564 Price, J. C., 358 16, 17, 19, 20, 21, 22, 42, 60, 128,
Peterson, B. S., 243, 244 Price, L. J., 129 129, 132, 309, 403
Peterson, J. B., 418 Price, L. R., 634 Rekkas, P. V., 695
Peterson, P. K., 380 Prigatano, G. P., 128, 337, 338, 403, Rektor, I., 200
Petry, N. M., 461, 469 411, 618, 620, 621, 622, 623, Remien, R. H., 701
Pettis, R. W., 675 624, 625, 626, 627, 628 Rempel-Clower, N., 561
Petursson, H., 462 Pryse-Phillips, W., 283 Rendell, P. G., 294
Pezawas, L., 468, 529 Puente, A. E., 149 Reneman, L., 467
Pfeffer, A. Z., 469 Pujol, J., 296 Rennard, S. I., 337
Pfefferbaum, A., 159, 169, 379, 406, Pukay-Martin, N. D., 375 Reske-Nielsen, E., 352, 360
407, 408, 409, 417, 430 Purcell, R., 535 Rettig, G. M., 203
Pfeiffer, E., 109 Putman, S. H., 70 Reuber, M., 274
Phan, K. L., 532 Reuter, T. A., 207
Phillips, D. E., 431 Quaid, K. A., 232 Rey, A., 45, 54, 55, 57, 58, 72, 87, 142,
Phillips, M., 533 Quraishi, S., 539 536, 573, 576
Phillips, N. A., 317 Reynolds, C. R., 130, 131, 142, 144
Piatt, A. L., 372 Raaymakers, T. W., 323 Reynolds, E. H., 461
Pick, A., 165 Rabbitt, P., 693 Reynolds, N. C., 231
Pick, T. P., 307 Radford, K., 662, 664, 671 Rhodes, S. S., 427
Piercy, M., 25 Radkowski, M., 381 Ribot, T., 581
Pierson, S. H., 297, 638 Raghavan, R., 432 Rice, D. P., 456
Pietrapiana, P., 672 Raichle, M. E., 527 Rich, J. B., 226
Pietrzak, R. H., 92 Ramaekers, J. G., 466, 658, 659, Richards, P. M., 622
Piette, J. D., 695 668, 673 Richardson, E. D., 423, 637, 639, 641
Pihl, R. O., 418 Ramirez, M., 107 Richardson, J. T., 294
Pillon, B., 176, 208, 213, 227 Randolph, C., 72, 110, 226, 252, 256, Rickards, H., 253
Pitman, R. K., 251 257, 292, 294 Rickels, K., 462
Pizza, F., 674 Ranen, N. G., 224 Riddoch, M. J., 26
Pizzagalli, D. A., 530, 543 Ranga, U., 387 Ridenour, T. A., 204
Plant, M., 421 Ranganathan, M., 457 Riley, E. P., 420
Plassman, B. L., 612, 613 Rangaswamy, M., 408 Rimel, R. W., 602
Plessen, K. J., 244 Ranney, T. A., 653, 657, 661 Rinker II, J. R., 280
Pohjasvaara, T., 317 Rao, N., 638 Rio, J., 282
Polgar, J. M., 666 Rao, S. M., 280, 283, 284, 285, 288, Rioch, M. J., 637
Polich, J., 419, 420 289, 290, 291, 292, 293, 295, Riordan, H. J., 203
Polivy, J., 531 318, 638, 671 Rippeth, J. D., 381, 464, 465, 466
Polymeropoulos, M. H., 201 Rapoport, M. J., 658 Riso, L. P., 535
Pomara, N., 366 Rapport, L. J., 672 Ristic, M., 431
Ponsford, A. S., 653, 657, 661 Rascovsky, K., 180, 181 Ritchie, K., 111
Ponsford, J., 606 Raskin, S. A., 204, 205 Ritsner, M. S., 87
Pont’on, M. O., 144 Rasmussen, T., 75 Riva, D., 143
Poon, L. W., 140, 142, 694 Rasmusson, D. X., 612 Rizzo, A., 96
Poon, W. S., 116 Raulin, M. L., 457 Rizzo, M., 656, 657, 660, 661, 663,
Pope, H. G., 457, 458 Raven, J. C., 43, 45 669, 670
Poreh, A. M., 55 Raven, P., 30 Rizzo, R., 243, 247
Porjesz, B., 408, 409, 417, 419 Ravnkilde, B., 525, 526, 527, 532 Robbins, T. W., 403, 413
Porter, R. J., 524, 525, 526, 527 Raymond, P., 86 Roberts, J. E., 378
Porteus, S. D., 45, 142 Raz, N., 159, 169, 284, 286, 287, 290, Robertson, G. J., 72
Portnoff, L. A., 417 291, 292, 294, 296, 316, 317, Robertson, I. H., 312
Poser, C. M., 282 318, 356, 694 Robertson, K., 369, 382
Postman, L., 291, 292 Razali, M. S., 698 Robertson, L. C., 56
Postuma, R. B., 207 Razani, J., 374 Robertson, M. M., 242, 244, 243, 245,
Potegal, M., 228 Reading, S. A. J., 231 246, 247, 248, 249, 250, 251, 255
Pournajafi-Nazarloo, H., 529 Rebok, G. W., 660, 665 Robinson, C., 658
Powderly, W. G., 382 Redekop, G., 320 Robinson, J. E., 460, 461
Powell, D. H., 45, 53, 54, 85, 86 Reed, B. R., 182 Robinson, L. J., 540
Powell, H., 467 Reed, L. J., 577, 578, 584 Rocca, M. A., 295
Powell, M. R., 172 Reed, R., 402, 420, 428, 429 Roccaforte, W. H., 115
Power, C., 373 Reeves, D. L., 85, 92 Rockwood, K., 312, 313, 314, 315,
Power, K. G., 462 Reger, M., 369, 372, 376, 377, 380, 316, 317
Prakash, R. S., 284, 285, 286, 287, 660, 661, 662, 665 Rodgers, J., 458, 467
289, 293 Regeur, L., 242 Roehrich, L., 428, 433
Prange, M. E., 432 Regier, D. A., 461 Roenker, D. L., 72, 662, 664
Pratt, R. T. C., 37 Reichard, P., 352, 354 Rogers, M. A., 524, 539
Prendergast, V., 114 Reichenberg, A., 508, 514 Rogers, R. D., 229, 469
Prescott, C. A., 417 Reingold, S. C., 282 Rogers, R. L., 408
Prestele, S., 357 Reinstein, D. Z., 246 Rohling, M. L., 527, 536
726 Author Index

Rohrer, D., 174, 226 Ryan, L. T., 283 Schlaepfer, T. E., 531
Rojas, D. C., 17 Ryback, R. S., 415, 428, 429 Schmeck, R. R., 56
Rolf, J. E., 418 Rybakowski, J. K., 539 Schmidt, I. W., 672
Rolls, E., 528, 543 Ryberg, S., 160 Schmidt, R., 317, 356, 359
Roman, G. C., 164, 312, 313 Schmitter-Edgecombe, M., 632,
Roman, M. J., 226 Sachdev, P. S., 314, 317, 623 640, 641
Romani, A., 409 Sachon, C., 352, 357 Schmolck, H., 560
Romero, A., 145 Sachs, H., 408 Schneider, W., 653
Ron, M. A., 405, 430 Sachs, W., 54 Schofield, P. W., 612
Root, H. F., 352 Sackeim, H., 524, 526, 559, 563 Schottenbauer, M. A., 407
Roper, B. L., 114, 115, 116 Sacktor, N., 368, 382, 383, 387, 673 Schretlen, D., 225, 539
Ropper, A. H., 307, 308 Sadavoy, J., 534, 541 Schrimsher, G. W., 114, 116, 117
Rorschach, H., 43 Sadek, J. R., 174, 371, 581 Schubert, D. S., 295
Rosario-Campos, M. C., 248 Sadovnick, A. D., 280, 283, 295 Schuckit, M. A., 400, 401, 408, 418,
Rosas, H. D., 231 Sagar, H., 166, 202 419, 426
Rose, E. J., 525 Saghafi, R., 535 Schuerholz, L. J., 252, 253, 254, 255
Rose, J. S., 468 Sahakian, B. J., 90 Schulte, T., 379
Roselli, M., 460 Sahgal, A., 177, 178 Schultheis, M. T., 662, 666, 669,
Rosen, V. M., 172 St Germain, S. A., 673 671, 672
Rosenberg, N. K., 231 Saint-Cyr, J. A., 199, 203, 227, 584 Schultz, R. T., 257
Rosenblatt, A., 224 Sakol, M. S., 462 Schulz, D., 283
Rosenbloom, T., 657 Salat, D. H., 169 Schwab, K., 601
Rosenfield, D. B., 320 Saletu-Zyhlarz, G. M., 408 Schwartz, B. L., 700
Rosenheck, R., 516 Salik, Y., 101 Schwartz, B. S., 487, 488, 490
Rosenqvist, U., 352 Salmon, D. P., 163, 166, 167, 168, 173, Schwebel, D. C., 656
Rosenstock, I. M., 701 175, 177, 178, 224, 412, 413, Schweiger, A., 87
Rosiers, M. D., 308 561, 564, 576 Schweinsburg, B. C., 412, 425, 430
Ross, B., 605 Salmon, G., 249 Scialfa, C. T., 694
Rosselli, M., 107, 142, 146 Salo, R., 465 Scogin, F., 527
Roth, M., 110 Salorio, C. F., 341 Scott, J. C., 371, 380, 659, 666, 701
Roth, T., 424 Salter, K., 309 Scoville, W. B., 559, 561
Rothenhausler, H. B., 345 Salthouse, T. A., 159, 356, 369 Searight, H. R., 641
Rotheram-Fuller, E., 469 Salzman, C., 462, 693 Segal, R., 705
Rothlind, J. C., 224, 229 Samii, A., 199, 200 Seidman, L. J., 508
Rouleau, I., 55, 176 Samson, Y., 408 Seinela, A., 292
Rounsaville, B. J., 469 Sander, A. M., 427 Selby, M. J., 460
Rourke, S. B., 378, 398, 403, 407, 428, Sanders, H. I., 34, 581 Sellers, E. M., 461
429, 430, 635 Sandor, P., 246, 247, 250 Selnes, O. A., 381
Rovaris, M., 295 Santangelo, S. L., 243 Seltzer, B., 581
Roy, M., 357 Sapolsky, R., 529, 538 Semple, S. J., 656, 659
Roy-Byrne, P. P., 536 Sartori, R., 101 Senin, U., 312
Royall, D. R., 55, 696 Sasaki, T., 306, 307 Sensky, T., 699, 700
Royer, F. L., 56 Sateia, M. J., 341 Serper, M. R., 118
Rubin, E. H., 171 Sattler, J. M., 144 Serretti, A., 162
Rubinsztein, J. S., 539 Satz, P., 360 Sevigny, J. J., 376, 377
Ruble, D. C., 469 Saunders, A. M., 162 Sevigny, M., 525
Ruchinskas, R. A., 101, 102, 114, Sawada, H., 210 Shafer, P. W., 307
116, 117 Sax, D. S., 224 Shaffer, D., 626
Ruchkin, D. S., 289 Saykin, A. J., 507, 511 Shah, P., 529, 543
Rude, S. S., 531 Scarone, A., 512 Shallice, T., 27, 31, 32, 33, 36, 51, 72,
Ruff, R. M., 423, 622 Scarrabelotti, M., 292 138, 254, 255
Ruiz Perez, I., 374 Schacter, D. L., 34, 169, 175, 559, 563, Shapiro, A., 242, 247, 252, 253, 255
Rund, B. R., 508 566, 569, 580 Shapiro, P. A., 699
Rundhaugen, L., 274 Schaeffer, K. W., 403, 404, 416, Sharp, J. R., 428, 429
Rundmo, T., 656 420, 427 Sharpe, D., 284, 286, 295, 296
Ruo, B., 537 Schafer, J., 432 Shaw, T. G., 408
Russell, E. W., 42, 43, 128, 132, 403, Schafer, K., 426 Shear, P. K., 430
404, 411 Schaie, K. W., 171 Shekhar, A., 530
Russell, M., 421 Schanke, A. K., 660, 662, 673 Sheline, Y., 525, 529, 532, 533, 535
Russell, W. R., 566, 600 Schau, E. J., 410, 428, 429 Shelly, C., 401, 412, 425, 426
Rutschman, O. T., 283 Schaub, L. H., 410, 428, 429 Shelton, M. D., 427
Rutter, M., 626 Scheltens, P., 317 Shelton, P. A., 223
Rutterford, N. A., 611 Schievink, W. I., 323 Shen, X. M., 494
Ryan, C. M., 352, 354, 355, 358, 359, Schiffman, H., 583 Sher, K. J., 418
360, 402, 403, 416, 417, 428 Schifitto, G., 383 Sherer, M., 404, 610
Ryan, E. L., 381 Schilder, P., 618 Shiba, M., 207
Ryan, J. J., 404, 424 Schillerstrom, J. E., 696 Shiffrin, R. M., 653
Author Index 727

Shimamura, A. P., 414, 563, 564, 569, Solomon, P. R., 108 Strauss, A. A., 3
573, 577, 578, 584, 585, 586 Solomon, T. M., 696 Strauss, E., 140, 285, 293
Shimizu, S., 210 Solowij, N., 457, 458 Strauss, M. E., 70, 224
Shimomura, T., 177, 178 Somers, V. K., 344 Streissguth, A. P., 421
Shipley, W. C., 45 Sonino, N., 538 Strickland, T. L., 107, 113, 114,
Shores, E. A., 613 Sorbi, S., 627 117, 459
Shorr, J. S., 57 Sosin, D. M., 597 Stringer, A. Y., 428
Shrager, Y., 559 Soukup, V. M., 203 Strittmatter, W. J., 162
Shua-Haim, J. R., 666 South, M. B., 55 Stromgren, L. S., 559
Shulman, K. I., 102, 118 Sowell, E. R., 420 Strong, C. A., 150
Shuttleworth-Edwards, A., 143 Spampinato, U., 210 Stroop, J. R., 36, 45
Siegle, G. J., 532, 543 Sparadeo, F. R., 402 Strub, R. L., 559
Siemers, E. R., 207 Specka, M., 469 Stuss, D. T., 36, 51, 338, 662
Siesjo, B., 308 Speedie, L. J., 229, 572 Stutts, J. C., 661, 665
Silberstein, J. A., 402, 403, 411 Spencer, T., 247 Suhr, J. A., 84, 537
Silver, J. M., 619, 621, 623, 625 Sperling, R. A., 281, 290, 292, 295 Sukhodolsky, D. G., 247, 250
Sim, M., 246 Spetzler, R. F., 316 Sullivan, E. V., 402, 403, 404, 407, 408,
Sim, T., 464 Spiegel, D., 537 412, 426
Simon, E. J., 468 Spooner, D. M., 669 Sullivan, P., 612
Simon, E. S., 87 Spreen, O., 67, 139, 140 Sumanti, M., 536
Simon, H. A., 704 Sprengelmeyer, R., 175 Summala, H., 656
Simon, S. L., 465, 466 Squire, L. R., 166, 175, 292, 359, 559, Summerall, L., 627
Simon, T., 3 560, 561, 562, 567, 568, 569, Summers, J., 621
Simoni, J. M., 702 572, 573, 577, 578, 580, 581, Sundet, K., 312, 660, 662, 664, 673
Simons, J. S., 656 582, 583, 584, 585, 586, 663 Sundstrom, A., 611, 627
Simons-Morton, D. G., 701 Stabell, K. E., 321 Sutherland, R., 253, 254, 256
Simpson, D. M., 373 Stacy, M., 199, 201, 212 Sutker, P. B., 425
Simpson, S. W., 530 Staffen, W., 295, 296 Svarstad, B. L., 705
Singer, H. S., 244, 245 Stanhope, N., 562, 564 Swan, G. E., 316
Singer, R., 320 Stapf, C., 319, 320 Swanson, R. A., 66
Singh, K. K., 378 Starkman, M. N., 538 Swedo, S. E., 243
Sinha, R., 426 Starkstein, S. E., 180, 612, 620, Sweeney, J. A., 539
Siren, A., 666 623, 625 Sweet, J. J., 374
Sivak, M., 660, 668, 672 Stavitsky, K., 178 Sweet, R. A., 534
Sjogren, P., 542 Stebbins, G. T., 203, 204 Szinovacz, M., 666
Ska, B., 167 Steffens, D. C., 530 Szlyk, J. P., 657, 660, 662
Skeel, R. L., 204 Stein, M., 207, 208, 379
Skinner, A. E., 427 Steinvorth, S., 320, 321 Tabori-Kraft, J., 247
Slater, P. C., 560, 583 Stephens, B., 653 Takeuchi, K., 250
Slick, D. J., 374 Stephens, R. J., 247, 250 Tales, A., 169
Sloka, S., 283 Stephens, S., 317 Talland, G. A., 45, 52
Small, B. J., 166, 171, 172 Stephenson, B. J., 700 Tamblyn, R. M., 694
Smeding, H. M., 203 Stern, E., 244 Tamietto, M., 663, 672
Smiley, A., 340, 658 Stern, M., 324 Tanaka, K., 320, 321
Smith, A., 600, 695 Stern, R., 57, 373 Tangalos, E. G., 107, 113, 116
Smith, C. D., 317 Stern, Y., 138, 161, 360, 657, 665 Tanner, C. M., 201
Smith, D. E., 432 Sternberg, D. E., 559, 573 Tanner-Smith, E. E., 466
Smith, G. E., 171 Sternberg, R. J., 44 Tapert, S. F., 412
Smith, G. P., 72 Sternberg, S., 285, 289 Tarter, R. E., 101, 115, 402, 403, 405,
Smith, J. W., 428, 429 Stevens, A., 169 411, 415, 416, 418, 419, 421,
Smith, K. A., 532, 537, 627 Stevens, L., 416, 417 422, 423, 424, 425, 429
Smith, K. J., 421 Stewart, J. T., 696 Tasman, A., 419
Smith, M. L., 308 Stewart, R., 274, 355, 358 Tata, P. R., 462
Smith, N., 566, 701 Stewart, S. H., 462, 541 Tate, R. L., 607
Smyer, M. A., 160 Stiles, K. M., 492 Tatemichi, T. K., 311
Snaphaan, L., 309 Stilley, C. S., 694 Tateno, A., 619
Snowden, J. S., 165, 180, 230, 232 Stitzer, M. L., 469 Taylor, A. E., 199, 584
Snowling, M., 33 Stockmeier, C. A., 529 Taylor, A. M., 30, 32
Snyder, S. H., 468 Stokes, L., 494 Taylor, B. P., 542
Sobin, C., 524, 526 Stolwyk, R. J., 664 Taylor, E., 254
Sobow, T., 523, 530 Stone, C. P., 413 Taylor, G. M., 428, 432
Sodhi, M., 675 Storandt, M., 165, 168, 171, 172 Taylor, H. G., 626
Sokol, M. C., 689 Stout, J. C., 176, 226, 229, 367 Taylor, J. L., 88
Sokoloff, L., 308 Strachan, M. W., 355, 358 Taylor, M., 130, 646
Soliveri, P., 212, 213 Strahan, R. F., 656 Taylor, R., 294
Solomon, A. C., 231, 232 Strang, J., 469 Teasdale, G., 600, 602, 611, 627
Solomon, D. A., 423, 429 Strassels, S. A., 337 Teasdale, T. W., 627
728 Author Index

Tellez-Rojo, M. M., 495 Tuffiash, E., 324 Vianna, E. P. M., 74


Temkin, N., 602 Tulsky, D. S., 129, 634 Victor, M., 307, 308, 414, 561, 562
Temkin, O., 269 Tulving, E., 569 Videbech, P., 357, 527, 529, 532
Templer, D. I., 428 Tupler, L. A., 318 Vighetto, A., 413
ten Bensel, R., 421 Tupper, D., 633 Vigil, O., 381
Teng, E. L., 109, 115 Turchan-Cholewo, J., 383 Vignola, A., 462
Terenius, L., 468 Turetsky, B. I., 511 Vik, S. A., 689, 693
Terry, R. D., 564 Turjman, F., 320 Villardita, C., 167
Testa, D., 213 Turk, D. C., 541 Vincent, K. R., 145
Teuber, H. L., 576, 579, 601 Turner, J., 403 Vinkers, D. J., 534
Thacker, E. L., 283 Twamley, E. W., 172, 515, 643 Vitiello, M. V., 424
Thal, L. J., 171 Twycross, R. G., 542 Vitkovitch, M., 293
Tham, W., 311 Voas, R. B., 658
Thibert, A. L., 250 Uc, E. Y., 661, 662, 664, 667, 671 Volkmann, J., 203
Thieda, P., 689 Uitti, R. J., 203 Volkow, N. D., 408, 456, 459, 460, 465,
Thomas, K., 533 Uldall, K. K., 373 466, 541
Thomas, R. E., 56, 658 UNAIDS 383 Vollenweider, F., 466
Thomas-Anterion, C., 179, 180 Underwood, B. J., 291, 292 von Giesen, H. J., 381
Thompson, L. L., 139 Underwood, G., 656, 675 von Knorring, A. L., 241, 243, 246,
Thompson, P. M., 367, 464 Unkenstein, A. E., 428, 429 247, 251
Thompson, R., 253 Vonsattel, J. P., 164
Thomsen, A. F., 537 Vadnal, V. L., 536 Vorona, R. D., 674
Thomsen, I. V., 623 Vainio, A., 542 Vrbaski, S. R., 431
Thornton, A. E., 284, 286, 287, 290, Vakil, E., 565, 566 Vrkljan, B. H., 666
291, 292, 294, 296 Valciukas, J. A., 480, 486 Vukusic, S., 280, 281
Thorpy, M. J., 207, 671 Valcour, V., 377, 378, 382, 653, Vythilingam, M., 529
Thurman, D. J., 597 665, 666
Thurstone, L. L., 45 Van Beek, J. G., 607 Wada, J., 75
Tierney, M. C., 695 Van den Berg, E., 101, 358, 360 Wadsworth, E. J., 541
Tillmann, W. A., 656 van den Burg, W., 291, 625 Wagner, G., 533, 696
Tippett, L. J., 169 van Dijk, E. J., 359 Wagner, M. T., 312
Tiraboschi, P., 165, 178, 202, 209 van Dijk, J. G., 413 Wahler, H. J., 66
Tisserand, D. J., 356 van Duijn, C. M., 161 Wahlin, T. B. R., 231, 232
Tivis, L. J., 427 van Gelder, J. M., 323 Waldhorn, R. E., 342
Tombaugh, T. N., 37, 72, 107, 112, Van Gorp, W. G., 107, 112, 113, 114, Waldrop-Valverde, D., 696
113, 116 116, 373, 375, 460, 566, 638, 639 Waldstein, S. R., 316
Tomer, A., 95 van Harten, B., 117, 355, 356, Walker, A. E., 611
Tomer, R., 205 358, 359 Walker, R. D., 432
Tomoeda, C. K., 165, 167, 174 van Laar, M., 658, 676 Walker, Z., 178, 208
Tong, S., 493, 496 Van Oostrom, J. C. H., 231 Walkup, J. T., 242
Tonne, U., 462 Van Praag, H. M., 161, 528 Wallace, J. J., 114, 116
Toole, J. F., 307, 308 Van Thiel, D. H., 402 Wallace, M. A., 31
Torner, J. C., 323 van Zomeren, A. H., 225, 625, 656, Wallin, M. T., 294, 296
Torrent, C., 540 660, 662 Wallis, T. S., 657
Torrey, E. F., 514 Vanacore, N., 212 Walter, H., 669
Torvik, A., 409, 413 Vanderplas, J. H., 704 Waltz, J. A., 167
Touradji, P., 143 Vanderplas, J. M., 704 Wancata, J., 160
Townend, E., 537 Vanderploeg, R. D., 566, 601 Wandless, I., 695
Tozzi, V., 368, 374, 377, 382 Vanhanen, M., 358 Wang, C. C., 653, 665, 675
Tracy, J. I., 416, 514 Vanyukov, M. M., 457 Wang, G. J., 380, 408, 466
Tranel, D., 67, 229, 372 Varma, A. R., 178 Wang, H. S., 241
Treisman, G., 378 Varma, V. K., 458 Wang, H. X., 161
Tremlett, H. L., 283 Vasa, R. A., 622 Wang, P. S., 700
Trepanier, L. L., 203, 374, 378 Vates, G. E., 323 Wang, X., 379
Triandis, H. C., 142 Vecchierini, M. F., 342 Ward, C. D., 581
Trimble, M. R., 461 Vega, A. Jr., 129, 131 Ward, J., 176, 230, 674
Trivedi, S., 432 Velligan, D. I., 517 Wareing, M., 467
Trobe, J. D., 665, 670 Veras, R. P., 107, 112 Warlow, C. P., 563
Troster, A. I., 166, 174, 204, Verbaten, M. N., 467 Warrington, E. K., 26, 27, 28, 29, 30,
572, 576 Verdejo-Garcia, A. J., 459, 461, 469 31, 32, 33, 34, 35, 36, 37, 72, 227,
Troyer, A. K., 226 Verfaellie, M., 559 577, 581, 584
Trupin, E. W., 458 Verkes, R. J., 466, 467 Wasserman, G. A., 494
Tschanz, J. T., 171 Vermeer, S. E., 354, 356 Watkins, L. H., 229
Tse, W., 369 Vermeeren, A., 676 Watkins, P. C., 531
Tuck, R. R., 404, 405, 424 Verster, J. C., 658, 673 Watson, J. D. G., 344
Tucker, G., 523 Verstraeten, E., 341, 342, 343, 344 Watson, S., 538
Tucker, J. S., 379 Vert, C., 274 Watters, M. R., 377
Author Index 729

Waugh, M., 416 Wik, G., 408 Xu, G., 113


Weaver, K. E., 702 Wild, K., 665, 671
Weaver, T. E., 343 Wild, S., 351 Yahya, H., 698
Wechsler, D., 28, 43, 45, 46, 51, 52, 70, Wilde, E. A., 423 Yakimo, M., 469
86, 87, 129, 138, 142, 145, 322, Wilde, G. J. S., 657 Yale, S. H., 666
413, 570, 572, 576, 578, 579 Wiley, C. A., 366 Yang, V., 254
Weder, N. D., 165 Wilhelm, K. L., 633, 636, 640, Yaryura-Tobias, J. A., 248
Weickert, T. W., 509, 513 641, 669 Yates, A., 25, 37
Weiden, P. J., 700 Wilhelmsen, K. C., 165 Yeates, K. O., 620
Weigl, E., 36 Wilk, C. M., 514, 515, 517 Yesavage, J. A., 72
Weiland-Fiedler, P., 524, 525, 527 Wilkinson, D. A., 405, 430 Yeterian, E. H., 227
Weiler, J. M., 669 Wilkinson, G. S., 72 Yeudall, L. T., 129, 131
Wein, C., 674 Wilkus, R. J., 274 Ylikoski, A., 354
Weinberger, D. R., 378 Willemsen, J., 674 Ylikoski, R., 169
Weiner, R. D., 572 Williams, B., 634, 693 Yohannes, A. M., 537
Weingartner, H., 536 Williams, C., 427, 624 Yohman, J. R., 403, 404, 428, 429
Weinger, K., 352 Williams, D. B., 612 Yokoyama, K., 487
Weinshenker, D., 280, 281, 283 Williams, D. H., 600 Yordanova, J., 247
Weinstein, N. D., 701 Williams, G. C., 162 York, J. L., 405
Weinstein, S., 601 Williams, T. M., 352 York, M. K., 203
Weir, B., 323 Willingham, D. B., 228 Young, A. H., 538
Weiser, M., 508 Willis, T., 307 Young, D. A., 467
Weiskrantz, L., 35, 584 Wilson, B. A., 36, 578, 580, 703 Young, E. A., 538
Weisskopf, M. G., 484, 489, 490 Wilson, J. M., 464 Young, T., 340
Welch, K. M. A., 308 Wilson, J. T., 606 Youngjohn, J. R., 94, 95, 139
Welford, A. T., 61 Wilson, R. S., 61, 161, 166, 534 Yousem, D. M., 354
Welsh, K., 112, 115, 165, 166, 573 Wilson, W. H., 408 Yuen, H. K., 674
Wentzel, C., 315 Windsor, T. D., 675 Yurgelun-Todd, D., 458
Wenz, F., 320, 321 Wingen, M., 658, 673
Werner, H., 3, 43 Winocur, G., 581 Zacks, R. T., 525, 527, 694
Wert, R. C., 457 Winokur, G., 249 Zacny, J. P., 469, 541, 542
Wesnes, K. A., 85, 89 Winstanley, J., 618 Zakzanis, K. K., 224, 284, 285,
Wessels, A. M., 354, 355, 358 Winterer, G. A., 408 286, 290, 294, 467, 511,
West, R. L., 94, 95, 159 Wiseman, M. B., 370 514, 700
Westervelt, H. J., 374 Wishart, H., 284, 286, 295, 296 Zandi, P. P., 161
Wetter, S., 173 Withaar, F. K., 660, 666, 667, 675 Zangwill, O. L., 563
Wetterling, T., 314, 317 Witjes-Ane, M. N., 231 Zatz, L. M., 405
Whalen, J., 532 Wixted, J. T., 569 Zec, R. F., 566
Wheeler, L., 8, 10 Wolf, H., 317 Zeidman, S. M., 321
Wheeler, M. A., 564 Wolfson, D., 5, 6, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12, 13, Zemke, D., 308
Whelihan, W. M., 662 16, 17, 19, 20, 21, 22, 129, 132 Zesiewicz, T. A., 669, 671
Whipple, S. C., 409, 419 Wolinsky, F. D., 688–689 Zgaljardic, D. J., 199, 204,
Whishaw, I., 528 Wong, C. K., 241 205, 206
White, C. L., 169 Wong, T. Y., 358 Zhang, L., 380
White, J., 531 Wood, J. M., 670, 671 Zhang, M., 161
White, R. F., 482, 483, 484, 485, 490, Wood, R. L., 611, 624 Zhang, Q., 623
494, 497 Woods, D. W., 245 Zhao, W. O., 359
Whitehouse, P. J., 163 Woods, S. P., 204, 371, 372, 374, 376, Zheng, J., 387
Whiteley, A. M., 27 465, 637, 698 Zheng, R. Y., 241
Whiteside, D. M., 116 Woodward, N. D., 517 Zhu, Y., 247
Whitfield, R. M., 380 Woody, G. E., 380 Zimmerman, M. E., 343
Whittington, C. J., 204 Workman-Daniels, K. L., 418 Zimprich, D., 635
Wiart, L., 537 Worringham, C. J., 664 Zink, M. C., 383
Wickens, C. D., 704 Wredling, R., 352, 357 Zipursky, R. B., 430
Wickens, D. D., 291 Wright, C. I., 532 Zivadinov, R., 283, 284, 295
Wicklund, A. H., 183 Wright, R. O., 162 Zola-Morgan, S., 166, 561, 562,
Wiebe-Velazquez, S., 306, 307 Wright, S. L., 525 568, 583
Wig, N. N., 458 Wrightson, P., 606 Zorzon, M., 295, 296
Wigg, N. R., 492, 496 Wu, C. M., 311 Zubieta, J., 532, 539, 540
Wiggins, S., 232 Wutoh, A. K., 382 Zuckerman, M., 656
Subject Index

Abbreviated Mental Test abstraction, reasoning and frontal lobe dysfunction model,
(AMT), 108 problem-solving skills, 411–412
absence seizures, 269 402–403 health consequences, 401
abstraction abilities, of cerebral age and neuropsychological impact of head injury on,
cortex, 11–12 performance, 417–420 423–424
Acculturation Rating Scale alcohol abuse, 399 impact of hypoxemia, 424
for Mexican Americans alcohol dependence, 398, 399 impact of nutrition, 424
(ARSMA), 143 alcohol-induced persisting impact of sleep disturbances,
acetylcholine, 163 amnestic disorder, 399 424
Ackerly, Spafford, 67 attention and concentration influence of concurrent
acquired dyslexias, 33 skills, 402 neuromedical risk factors in
Addenbrooke’s Cognitive and binge drinking, 400 alcoholics, 421–426
Examination-Revised blackouts, 424 influence of motivation and
(ACE-R), 109, 117 clinical course, 400–401 expectancies, 427
Affective Go–No Go Test, 91 clinical implications and influence of pre-abuse
African Americans and Tests of treatment outcome, 432–433 neuropsychiatric risk factors,
Intelligence, 144–145 cognitive disorders, 399, 400 420–421
age-associated memory impair- complex perceptual–motor intellectual performance,
ment, 95 deficits, 403 401–402
decline rate of, 131 and cortical atrophy, 405 learning and memory deficits,
in fluid intelligence, 138 CT studies, 405–406 403–404
in neuropsychological detoxified alcoholics, 401–402 neurobehavioral and
assessment, 129–130, diagnosis, 398–400 neuroimaging fi ndings
133–136, 147 diff use or generalized associated with, 401–410
on the Verbal Comprehension dysfunction model, 412 neurobehavioral recovery in,
factor, 129 drinking indices and 428–429
age-related “disconnection” neuropsychological neuroimaging reversibility,
syndrome, 159 performance, 415–417 429–432
agrammatism, 46 DSM-IV criteria for, 398 neuropathology in, 409–410
akinesia, 200 educational influences, 426–427 personality disorders, 425–426
alcoholic Korsakoff syndrome, 413 effects of hepatic dysfunction pneumoencephalographic
alcohol-induced persist- on, 421–423 studies, 405
ing amnestic disorder. electrophysiological fi ndings, relation neuroimaging
See Wernicke–Korsakoff 408–409 and neuropsychological
Syndrome etiology of neuropsychological variables, 407
alcoholism, neurobehavioral deficits, 414–427 right hemisphere dysfunction
correlates of family history and, 420 model, 410–411

730
Subject Index 731

role of cerebral blood flow anterograde and retrograde autonomic neuropathy, 351
(CBF), 408 amnesia, 561 Average Impairment Index, 309
seizures, 424–425 assessment, 568–577
simple motor skills, 404–405 causes, 562–568 Babinski reflexes, 242
social-related problems, 401 characteristics of, 560–561 Balloon Analogue Risk
symptoms, 401 declarative memory, 570 Task, 461
time course associated diencephalic amnesia, 563–564 barbiturates, 461
with, 400 frontal lobe memory basal forebrain degeneration. See
Wernicke–Korsakoff Syndrome, dysfunction, 565 Alzheimer’s disease
412–414 functional amnesia, 567–568 base rate, 106
withdrawal syndrome, 400 medial temporal lobe amnesia, Beck Depression Inventory, 426
alertness, 9 562–563 Behavioral Assessment of the
Alzheimer’s disease (AD), 71, 95, memory processes, 570–571 Dysexecutive Syndrome
142, 355 neuropsychological assessment battery, 36
abnormal serial position effect, of anterograde amnesia, behavioral deficits, associated with
in the episodic memory, 166 573–581 HD, 164
and aging, 169–170 neuropsychological assessment Bender-Gestalt Test, 43
aspects of attention, 167, 175 of memory dysfunction, benign epilepsy with
deficit in parietotemporal blood 571–573 centrotemporal spikes
flow, 161 neuropsychological assessment (BECTS), 269
deficits in “executive” of retrograde amnesia, benign occipital epilepsy, 269
functions, 167 581–587 Benton, Arthur, 66. See also
deficits in visuospatial abilities, transient global amnesia, 564 Iowa-Benton (I-B) school,
167–168 traumatic brain injury (TBI), of neuropsychological
dementia syndrome of, 163 566–567 assessment
diagnosis, 168–169 amputations, 351 Benton Neuropsychology
encoding ability, 166 amyloid precursor protein Laboratory. See also
episodic memory gene, 162 Iowa-Benton (I-B) school,
impairment, 166 Anton’s syndrome, 29 of neuropsychological
and hypoxia, 338 anxiety, 164 assessment
intrusion errors, 166 aphasia, 27, 73 divisions, 67
neuropsychological detection, Aphasia Screening Exam, 92, emeritus status, 67
170–173 136, 271 report writing, 78–79
pathology, 164 Aphasia Screening Test (AST). research programs and
performance on the parallel See Reitan-Indiana Aphasia sponsorships, 67
search task, 177 Screening Test (AST) staff, 77
priming tasks, 175 apolipoprotein E (APOE) gene, 162 technicians, 77–78
retrograde amnesia, 166 apraxia, 43, 127 training model, 76–77, 77
semantic memory deficit, arteriovenous malformation Benton Visual Retention Test
166–167, 177–178 (AVMs) of brain, 319–320 (BVRT), 92, 139,
and TBI, 612 Articulatory Working Memory 543, 577–578
verbal fluency, 180 Test, 89 benzodiazepines, 311
visuospatial processing deficits, assessment, philosophy of, 67–68 neuropsychological impact,
176, 178 Asymptomatic Neurocognitive 461–463
vs dementia with Lewy bodies, Impairment (ANI), 367 binge drinking, 400
176–178 atomoxetine, 517 biological intelligence, concept
vs frontotemporal dementia, atonic seizures, 269 of, 3
178–181 attentional dyslexia, 32 blackouts, 424
vs Huntington’s disease, 173–176 attention-deficit hyperactivity Blessed Dementia Rating Scale,
vs vascular dementia, 181–182, disorder (ADHD), 246–247 111, 118
312–313 attentiveness, 9 Blessed Information-
working memory deficits, Autobiographical Memory Memory-Concentration Test
175–176 Interview, 585 (BIMC), 109
amnesic syndrome, 34–35 Automated Neuropsychological Blessed Orientation-
Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and, Assessment Metrics (ANAM), Memory-Concentration Test
564–565 92–93 (BOMC), 108
732 Subject Index

Block Design test, 180, 181, 230, Brain and Intelligence: cerebrovascular anomalies,
271, 543 A Quantitative Study of the 319–322
blood clots, 307 Frontal Lobes (Halstead), 4 distribution of cerebral vessels
blood flow and cerebrovasculature brain arteries, 306 and their collaterals, 306–307
connections, 307 brain arteriovenous ischemic stroke, 307–312
blood oxygenation level dependent malformations, 319–320 vascular cognitive impairment,
(BOLD) signal, 367 brain atrophy, 281 314–316
Boston Diagnostic Aphasia brain damage, detection vascular dementia, 312–314
Examination (BDAE), 43, 146 tests for, 3 Wepfer’s distinction with
manual, 55 intelligence tests, 3 occlusive disease, 306
Spanish version, 146 language deficits and, 3 white matter lesion pathology,
Boston Naming Test, 146, 271 brain lesion and quality of the 317–319
Boston Parietal Lobe Battery, 55 patients’ responses, 43 Charcot joints, 351
Boston Process Approach, to Brief Test of Attention, childhood methylmercury
neuropsychological 225, 230 exposure, 499–500
assessment Brief Visuospatial Memory Chlamydia pneumoniae, 283
analysis of Rorschach Test-Revised (BVMT-R), 271 cholestokinin, 202
responses, 43 Broca’s aphasia, 27 cholinesterase inhibitors, 517
aphasia examination, 43 Broca’s lesions, 73 chorea, 164
benefits, 60–61 Chronic Multiple Tics (CMT), 243
California Verbal Learning Test California Verbal Learning Test chronic obstructive pulmonary
(CVLT), 54 (CVLT), 54, 168, 224, 575 disease
Clock Drawing Test, 54–55 Cambridge Cognitive abnormalities on brain CT and
Delis–Kaplan Executive Examination-Revised EEG, 338
Function System (DKEFS), 55 (CAMCOG-R), 110, 118 causes, 337
development, 42–44 Cambridge Gambling Test, 91 chronic hypoxemia in, 337
early approaches, 44–46 Cambridge Neuropsychology depressive symptoms, 339
effect of education, 61 Test Automated Battery effect of exercise, 340
effects of aging, 61 (CANTAB), 90–91 neuropsychological functioning
featural vs contextual priority, Camden Memory Test battery, in, 337–338
56–59 35, 37 posttreatment
hemispatial priority, 59–60 Cancellation Test, 177 neuropsychological
new generation of cannabinoid receptor (CB1), 457 functioning in, 338–340
neuropsychological tests, cannabis, neuropsychological Circle of Willis, 306
54–55 impact, 457–459 Classification Tree Analysis, 168
overview, 42 carbamazepine, 276, 277 Clinical Antipsychotic Trials of
principles of “complexity” and catechol-O-methlytransferase Intervention Effectiveness
“fluidity,” 43 (COMT) inhibitor, 202 (CATIE) project, 517
procedure, 44–52 categorical organization, of Clock Drawing Test (CDT), 54–55,
psychopathology, 61 semantic knowledge 111, 115, 118, 180, 181
quantified process approach, 55 systems, 31 Clock-in-a-Box task, 55
sample of tests in, 45 category-specific impairment, 32 CLOX Text, 55
screening instruments, 53–54 Category Test, 6, 12, 92, 131, 138, cocaine, neuropsychological
sensory motor handicaps, 61 271, 401 impact, 459–461
use in localizing lesions, characteristics, 12 cocaine hydrochloride, 459
56–62 memory test, 12–13 Cognistat, 110t, 114–115, 116–117
Wechsler Adult Intelligence purpose, 12 cognition, benefits of exercise and
Scale-Revised (WAIS-R), C clade virus, 387 cardiorespiratory
46–52 central dyslexias, 33 fitness, 161
Wechsler Memory Scale, cerebral aneurysm, 323–325 Cognitive Abilities Screening
52–53 cerebrovascular anomalies, Instrument (CASI), 109
and Wechsler scales, 43 319–322 Cognitive Capacity Screening
Boston/Rochester cerebrovascular disease (CVD) Exam (CCSE), 109, 116–117
Neuropsychological brain arteriovenous Cognitive Drug Research (CDR)
Screening Test, 53 malformations, 319–320 Battery, 89–90, 90
bradykinesia, 177, 200 cerebral aneurysm, 323–325 Cognitive Estimates Test, 36
Subject Index 733

cognitive flexibility, 159 computed tomography (CT) Alzheimer’s disease (AD), 163
Cognitive Rehabilitation scans, 69 clinical and neuropathologic
Laboratory, 79 of GTS, 244 features, 162–165
cognitive screening methods computerized cognitive testing clinical profi les, 101
benefits of diagnosis of advantages, 84–85 cognitive changes, 160
neuropsychiatric conditions, computerized batteries for definition, 160
101–102 assessing memory and related dementia with Lewy bodies
brief focused screening cognitive abilities, 85–96 (DLB), 164–165
instruments, 118 concentration, 9 diagnosis of, 160
Cognistat (Neurobehavioral concept formation, test of. See epidemiological evidences, 162
Cognitive Status Category Test and family history, 161–162
Examination, NCSE), 116–117 Conceptual Level Analogy frontotemporal (FTD), 165
of dementia, 101–102 Test, 403 gender as risk factor, 161
descriptions of tests, 108–112 conduction aphasia, 27 genetics, 162
detection of cognitive constructional apraxia, 26–27 and head injury, 162
impairment, 102 Contention Scheduling System Huntington’s disease (HD), 164
effect of cutoff scores, 105–106 (CSS), 255 influence of physical
general instruments, 117–118 Controlled Oral Word Association activity, 161
of geriatric patients, 102 Test (COWAT), 230, 271 low education and low
ideal, 102–104 copropraxia, 256 occupational attainment as
informant-based screening Core Assessment Program for risk factor, 161
instruments, 118 Surgical Interventional ther- and mental activity, 161
limitations, 103 apies in Parkinson’s disease prevalence, 160
in medical settings, 101–104 (CAPSIT-PD), 203 risk factors, 160–162
Mini-Mental State Examination Core Battery tests, of I-B school, vascular, 163–164
(MMSE) and related 70–71, 70t dementia lacking distinctive
measures, 107–116 Corsi Block Test, 255 histopathology, 165
reliability, 104 cortical blindness, 29 Dementia Rating Scale, 168
and review bias, 104 cortical choline acetyltransferase dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB),
review of psychometric (ChAT), 165 164–165
properties of tests, 104–107 Cortical Vision Screening Test histopathologic abnormalities
significance of screening, (CORVIST), 29 in the limbic system and
101–102 Critchley, Macdonald, 67 neocortex in, 165
and spectrum bias, 103–104 Crovitz Test, 584 vs Alzheimer’s disease, 176–178
strengths, 103 crystallized intelligence, 137 demographic characteristics,
telephone screening, 112 Cube Analysis test, 30 in neuropsyhological
trade-off between sensitivity cumulative decline, in cognitive assessment
and specificity, 106 functioning, 163 age, 129–130, 133–136, 147
usefulness, 106 age- and education-corrected
validity, 104–107 Damasio, Antonio, 67 norms, 138–139
and workup (verification) Damasio, Hanna, 67 education, 130–131, 133–136,
bias, 104 decision theory, 105 147–148
CogScreen deep brain stimulation (DBS), 203 effects on Verbal
correlation with specialized deep white matter lesions Comprehension factor, 129
neuropsychological tasks, 88 (DWML) vs periventricular ethnicity, 142–144, 149
focus of, 87 (PVL) WML, 318 extended version of
strength, 89 delayed recall, 53, 165 HRB, 132
tasks in, 87–88 Delayed Reward Discounting gender differences, 131–132, 136,
time to complete, 88 task, 461 148–149
validity, 88 Delis–Kaplan Executive Function Mayo Older Americans
versions, 88 System (DKEFS), 55 Normative Studies Project
CogState tests delta-9- tetrahydrocannabinol, 457 (MOANS), 141–146
construct validity, 92 dementia, neurophsychology of normative data, 127–129
test–retest reliabilities, 92 acetylcholine level and, 163 norms for elderly, 140–141
tests, 91, 91 age as risk factor, 160–161 processing speed men vs
complementary syndrome, 34 age of occurrence, 160 women, 131–132
734 Subject Index

demographic characteristics, Developmental Test of DOD Performance Assessment


in neuropsyhological Visual-Motor Integration, 230 Batteries (PABs), 93
assessment (cont.) diabetes, brain dysfunction and Doors and People Test, 35
purpose of demographically cognitive impairment in dopadecarboxylase inhibitor
corrected norms, 146–150 acute effects, 352 carbidopa, 202
questions related to norms, and aging, 359–360 driving behavior,
146–150 classification and prevalence, neuropsychological
rate of age-related cognitive 350–351 performance
decline, 131 cognitive performance in adults experience and, 656–657
results of study, 133–138 with Type 1diabetes, 352–353 factors influencing, 652–659
specificity vs sensitivity, 130, cognitive performance in older familiarity and, 657
136–137 people with Type 1diabetes, general model of, 653–656
subjects and methods, 132–133 353–354 impact of neurologic,
DemTect, 117 cognitive performance in Type 2 psychiatric, and medical
Denver Developmental Screening diabetes, 355 conditions, 670–673
Test (DDST), 498 complications and treatment issues with assessment, 673–675
depression and depression-related goals, 351–352 motivation and, 657
cognitive difficulties, 164 diagnosis of, 351 neuropsychological functioning,
age of onset, 534–535 and fasting plasma glucose 659–666
among cerebrovascular accident (FPG) levels in the blood, 351 personality and, 656
(CVAs) patients, 537–538 implications for clinical physical conditions and,
anticholinergics effects, 541–542 practice, 360–361 657–658
attention in MDD, 525 medical nutrition therapy, 351 psychoactive substances and,
benzodiazepines and, 542 MRI findings, 354–356 658–659
bipolar, 540–541 pattern of cognitive visual fucntioning and, 657
cognitive dysfunction related, impairment, 356 drug abuse, neuropsychological
534–537 prevalence of psychiatric consequences of
cognitive functions in mood disorders, 356–357 benzodiazepines, 461–463
disorders, 526–527 risk factors and possible cannabis, 457–459
cognitive profi le, 524–527 pathophysiological cocaine, 459–461
comorbid condition in medical mechanisms, 357–360 MDMA (3,4-methylenedioxy
illnesses, 538–539 role of atherosclerotic risk methamphetamine), 466–468
Cushing’s disease and, 539–540 factors, 358–359 methamphetamine (MA),
effort, motivation, and abulia, role of hyperglycemia in 463–466
536–537 pathogenesis of cognitive neurobehavioral
functional imaging studies of impairments, 358 consequences, 455
MDD, 531–534 role of insulin, 359 opiates, 468–470
and GTS, 249–250 role of pancreatic islets, 350 dysarthria, 46, 164, 281
hypercortisolemia in, 539 diabetes mellitus, 350 dysexecutive syndrome, 28, 36
medication effects, 541 diabetic encephalopathy, 352, Dysexecutive Syndrome
melancholic depression, 540 359–360 battery, 36
memory functions in mood Diagnostic Confidence dysgraphic patients, 33
disorders, 527 Index, 242 dyslexia, 28
neuroanatomy of mood diencephalic amnesia, 563–564 with dysgraphia, 32
disorders, 527–528 differential diagnosis, in without dysgraphia, 32
opiates and, 542–543 neuropsychiatric assessment, dysprosody, 46
psychomotor speed in mood 36–37
disorders, 525–526 Digits Span forwards test, 37 echopraxia, 256
severity, 535–536 Digits Span test, 271 ecstacy. See MDMA
structural imaging studies of Digit Symbol Coding, 28 (3,4-methylenedioxy
MDD, 528–531 Direct Assessment of Functional methamphetamine)
subtypes of mood disorders, Status (DAFS), 641–642 educational factors, in
537–543 directed and divided attention, 159 neuropsyhological
treatment, 543–544 Dissimulation Index, 19 assessment, 130–131, 133–136,
Deterioration Cognitive Observee dissociation, notion of, 19, 27 147–148
(DECO), 111t DJ-1, 201 electroencephalography (EEG), 69
Subject Index 735

entacapone, 202 Facial Recognition Test (FRT), 73 association with stressful life
epilepsy fact-like recall ability, 34 events, 243
brain abnormalities Famous Faces Test, 582–584 attention and executive
causing, 267 feedback and neuropsychological functions, 254–256
characteristics of seizures, examination, 69–70 clinical characteristics,
275, 278 fetal alcohol syndrome, 420 245–246
childhood, 275–276 Finger localization, 11 definition, 241–242
classification of, 269–270 Finger Tapping–Preferred Hand, 6 depression and, 249–250
defined, 267 Finger Tapping Test, 92, 136 dopamine receptor D2 gene
diagnosis, 268 finger-tip number writing polymorphisms, 243
generalized seizure, 269 perception, 11 epidemiology, 241–242
genetics of, 267–268 First–Last Names Test, 94 etiology, 242–244
idiopathic or cryptogenic, 269 flexible batteries, 15, 44 etiopathogenesis of, 243
lateralization and fluency tests, 36 evidence of GABHS
localization, 270 fluid intellectual functions, 43 infections, 243
neuropsychological profi le of focal seizures, 269 family psychopathology,
patients, 270 Follow-Up Neuropsychological 250–251, 251
neuropsychometric testing, Tests, 72 gender differences, 246
270–276 frontal functions, 159 intelligence, 252–253
nonepileptic seizures, 273–274 frontal-temporal dementia, 71 language skills, 253–254
partial seizures, 269 frontostriatal loops, 164 long-term outcome of, 245
pharmacological treatment, frontotemporal dementia memory, 256
276–277 (FTD), 165 neuroimaging and
pregnancy and genetic vs Alzheimer’s disease, 178–181 pathophysiology, 244–245
aspects, 276 functional deficit score (FDS), 644 neurology, 242
prevalence and incidence, 268 neuropsychiatric disorders,
semiology and EEG gabapentin, 277 250–252
findings, 269 gait disturbance, 164 neuropsychology, 252, 257
social aspects of, 268–269 Gambling Decision Making OCD and, 247–249
stigma and loss of independence task, 176 personality disorders, 250
due to, 268–269 gamma amino butyric acid psycholinguistic studies
surgical intervention, 272–273 (GABA), 202 of, 253
episodic memory, 31 Garcin, Raymond, 67 rage and explosive
impairment, 166 generalized tonic-clonic seizures outbursts, 250
epoch assessments, 74 (GTCs), 269 role of basal ganglia, 244–245
ERSET, 270 General Neuropsychological visuomotor skills, 256–257
Erythroxylon coca, 459 Deficit Scale (GNDS), 6 Glasgow Coma Scale, 600, 609
Escala de Inteligencia Wechsler General Practitioner Assessment Global Deficit Score (GDS), 514
para Adultos (EIWA), 145 of Cognition, 118 glucocorticoids, 351
ethnicity effects, impact General Practitioner Assessment glutamate decarboxylase, 202
in neuropsyhological of Cognition (GPCOG), 108 Go-NoGo Task, 255
assessment, 142–144, 149 Geriatric Evaluation of Mental graded-difficulty irregular-word
event memory, neuropsychological Status (the GEMS), 53 reading tests, 33
assessment, 33–35 gestational diabetes mellitus graded-difficulty memory tests, 37
executive dysfunction (GDM), 350 Graded Naming Test, 91
in MA-abusing populations, 465 Gilles de la Tourette Syndrome grapheme–phoneme
in MDD, 534–537 (GTS) correspondences, 33
in schizophrenia, 507–509 ADHD and, 246–247 Greenacre, Phyllis, 67
exercise and cardiorespiratory anxiety disorders, 250 Grip Strength and Finger
fitness, benefits on assessment, 242 Oscillation, 271
cognitions, 161 associated psychiatric Grooved Pegboard Test, 92,
experimental psychology, disorders, 246 256–257, 271
modern, 43–44 associated psychopathology and Groton Maze Learning Test, 92
extrinsic alertness, 225 comorbidities, 246
association with SLITRK1 Halstead, Ward C, 3–4
facial numbness, 281 gene, 243 Halstead Category Test, 309
736 Subject Index

Halstead Impairment Index, 6, Heschel’s gyrus seizures, 269 host biomarkers, 377–378
309, 401 High Sensitivity Cognitive Screen impact of normal aging on, 382
Halstead–Reitan (HSCS), 110t individuals with substance
Neuropsychological Hiscock Digit Memory Test dependence, 374
Inventory, 271 (HDMT), 374 intellectual functioning, 373
Halstead–Reitan HIV Dementia Scale, 387 language abilities, 372
Neuropsychological Test HIV-related dementia, 71 and marijuana-related
Battery (HRB), 401 Hopkins Motor and Vocal Tic disorders, 380
assessment of visual–spatial Severity Scale, 242 medication adherence, 375–376
functions, 10 Hopkins Verbal Learning mental status, 373
association between age Test, 118 and methamphetamine-related
and, 129 Hopkins Verbal Learning Test- disorders, 380–381
association between education Revised (HVLT-R), 230, 232 mortality, 376
and, 131 human immunodeficiency motor and psychomotor
background and development virus (HIV) abilities, 368–369
of, 3 and alcohol-related disorders, neurobiology of, 366–367
clinical inferences regarding 379–380 neuropsychological profi le of,
individuals, 13–16 antiretroviral therapies, 367–374
and clinical issues in 382–383 nomenclature for HIV-
neuropsychology, 16–22 and anxiety disorders, 378–379 associated neurocognitive
complementary associated neural injury, 367 disorders, 367
approaches, 7–8 associated neurocognitive non-antiretroviral treatments,
correct classifications, 13 disorders, 383–387 386–387
degree of concurrence, 14 attentional deficits, 369 nondeclarative memory, 371
early observations of brain- biomarkers of, 376–378 older adults living with, 381–382
damaged persons, 3 and bipolar disorders, 379 and opioid-related
error rate, 14 CD4 lymphocyte counts, 377 disorders, 381
extended version of, 132 Center for Disease Control prevalence, 368
legal validity, 14–16 and Prevention (CDC) published guidelines for
neuropsychological tests in, 7 staging, 376 classifying confounds,
process in the development CNS opportunistic 384–386
of, 4–5 infections, 377 regional distribution of
psychological testing and cocaine-related abnormalities, 366
procedures, 3 disorders, 380 risky behaviors, 375
screening tests for determining cognitive rehabilitation, 383 RNA, 377
comprehensive tests, 20–22 cognitive screening, 373 semantic memory, 371
sensitivity of tests, study, 5–7 co-infected with Hepatitis C sensory-perceptual functions,
sex differences and, 136 virus (HCV), 381 373–374
tests for sensory perceptual comorbidities of, 378–382 Social Security disability
examination, 11 course of neurocognitive benefits, 374
test validity of, 5–7 diseases, 368 spatial cognition, 372–373
theoretical model and content and depression, 378 structural abnormalities, 367
of, 8–13 diff usion tensor images, 368 suboptimal efforts, 374
Halstead’s brain-damaged range, driving, 375 and substance-related disorders,
on the Impairment Index, 129 effects in frontal cortex and 379–381
Halstead Tactual Performance striatum, 366–367 treatment, 382–383
Test, 43 employment status, 375 working memory deficits, 369
Hamilton Depression Rating episodic memory, 369–370 Huntington’s disease (HD), 164,
Scores, 426 everyday consequences 200, 318
Hand Dynamometer, 92, 136 of neuropsychological attention deficits, 225–226
Hayling and Brixton Tests, 36 impairment in, 374–376 dementia of, 224–230
Heimburger, Robert F., 4 executive function deficits, 369 emotional processing, 229–230
hemorrhage, related to AVMs, 320 genetic factors, 378 executive control, 229
Henmon–Nelson Test of health-related quality of life, hereditary nature of, 224
Intelligence, 20 374–375 juvenile onset, 224
Subject Index 737

language deficits, 226 Informant Questionnaire on behavioral effects, 308–309


letter-cued word generation Cognitive Decline, 118 causes, 307
tasks, 226 Informant Questionnaire on cerebral, 308
level of impairment, 225 Cognitive Decline in the cerebral blood flow (CBF), 308
memory deficits, 227–228 Elderly (IQCODE), 111 diminished molecular
motor learning and other forms insulin injections, 352 energy, 308
of nondeclarative memory, intelligence tests, for assessing epidemiology, 307–308
228–229 cerebral damage, 3 Fluid-attenuated inversion
neuropathology of, 223 International League Against recovery (FLAIR) magnetic
neuropsychological profi le Epilepsy, 269 resonance (MR) images, 307
of, 224 International Mission on neurological symptoms, 307
onset of disease, 224 Prognosis and Analysis of outcome of stroke, 311–312
overview, 223–224 Clinical Trials in Traumatic pathophysiology, 308
presymptomatic, 231–232 Brain Injury (IMPACT), 600 protocols, 310
restriction fragment length intoxication effects, of transient ischemic attack (TIA),
polymorphism (RFLP) linked cannabies, 457 310–311
to, 224 intracarotid amobarbital ischemic vascular dementia, 164
sequence of DNA bases, 224 procedure (IAP), 271
spatial cognition deficits, Iowa-Benton (I-B) school, Judgment of Line Orientation
226–227 of neuropsychological (JLO) test, 74
studies, 230–232 assessment juvenile myoclonic epilepsy, 269
symptom triad, 223–224 carbon monoxide poisoning, 75
vs Alzheimer’s disease, 173–176 Cognitive Rehabilitation Kaplan’s methods. See Boston
5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid, 202 Laboratory, 79 Process Approach, to
hypercortisolemic mood disorders, competence, 75 neuropsychological
539–540 core battery of tests, 70, 70–71 assessment
hyperglycemia, 350, 352 current practice, 68–70 Knox Cube Test, 255
hypertension, 352 distinguish conditions of Kokmen Short Test of Mental
hypofrontality, 512 psychiatric diseases, 74 Status (STMS), 108
hypoglycemia, 352, 354 early developments, 66
hypoxia feedback sessions, 76 lamotrigine, 277
and acute respiratory distress identifying developmental lead exposure
syndrome (ARDS), 345–346 learning disorders, 75 adult, 485–490
and chronic obstructive indications for assessment, assessment of, 484
pulmonary disease, 336–340 74–76 availability, 483–484
definition, 336 influence of pain, 75–76 blood concentration, 484
medical syndromes associated interpretation of assessment, 76 childhood, 490–496
with, 336–346 interpretation of developmental stage and, 484
and obstructive sleep apnea neuropsychological data, 71 forms of inorganic lead, 484
(OSA), 340–345 legal issues, 74–75 inorganic lead poisoning, 485
hyrolase L1 (UCHl1), 201 “localizing value” of Benton’s occupational and environmental
tests, 73 exposure, 484, 486–489
Immediate Post-Concussion mild cognitive impairment, 75 left cerebral hemisphere functions,
Assessment and Cognitive and neuroanatomy, 71–74 10, 26, 30, 32, 56
Testing (ImPACT), 117 and Neuropsychology Lennox-Gastaut syndrome, 269
immunoglobin G [IgG] index, 281 Laboratory, 67 letter-by-letter reading, 32
ImPACT Test Battery, 93–94 obstructive sleep apnea, 76 levetiracetam, 276, 277
impairment and cerebral philosophy of assessment, 67–68 Levine Hypothesis Testing, 403
functioning, 12 report writing, 78–79 levodopa, 202
implicit memory, 35 staff and technicians, 77–78 Lewy bodies, 164–165
Implicit Memory Test, 35 training model, 76–77 Lewy body dementia, 164–165,
Incidental Memory Test, 94 Wada testing, 75 200, 202, 208–209
independent performance of Iowa Gambling Task, 461 histopathologic abnormalities
instrumental activities of irritability, 164 in the limbic system and
daily living (IADLs), 367 ischemic stroke neocortex in, 165
738 Subject Index

Lewy body dementia (cont.) identifying and quantifying modality-specific memory


vs Alzheimer’s disease, 176–178 subclinical effects of, deficits, 34
Lewy body pathology, 210 482–483 Modified Mini-Mental Status
Lhermitte’s sign, 281 issues in behavioral toxicology, Examination (3MS), 109
limited-capacity system, 33 481–482 monoamine oxidase (MAO-B)
lipoprotein metabolism known, 481 inhibitor, 202, 383
abnormality, 352 lead, 481, 483–496 Montreal Cognitive Assessment
loss of associated movements, 200 likelihood of confounding, 483 (MoCA), 117
loss of bowel or bladder mercury, 481, 496–502 MOVES Scale, 242
control, 281 thallium, 481 multi-infarct dementia (MID), 164
LRRK2, 201 methadone maintenance therapy Multiple Ability Self-Report
(MMT), 469 Questionnaire (MASQ), 271
MA-associated neurotoxicity, 465 methamphetamine (MA), multiple sclerosis (MS)
magnetic resonance imaging neuropsychological impact, autoimmune response in, 280
(MRI) scans, 69 463–466 axonal loss, 280
of GTS, 244 methionine-enkephalin, 202 benign, 282
of MS, 281 methylmercury poisoning, and brain atrophy, 281
malingering, studies of, 18–19 impacts, 497–498 clinically defi nite, 282
masked facies, 177, 199–200 methylphenidate, 386 cortical signs, 281
matched graded-difficulty proper Miale-Holsopple Sentence definite, 282
nouns naming test, 32 Completion, 271 diagnosis, 281–282
Matching-to-Sample Visual Search MicroCog, 54 disease course, 282
Test, 90 administration, 86 distribution of plaques, 280
Mattis Dementia Rating Scale advantage, 86 emotional functioning, 295–296
(MDRS), 110, 118, 543 correlation with WMS, 86 estimates of cognitive
memory subscale, 180, 181 development, 85 impairment, 288
Mayo Older Americans Normative test reliability, 86 etiology and risk factors, 283
Studies Project (MOANS) and Visual Memory Span, 86 executive functions and
African Americans and Tests of weaknesses, 86 metacognition, 293–294
Intelligence, 144–145 MicroCog Attention/Mental gender differences, 283
contributions, 141–142 Control Index, 86 implicit and procedural
ethnicity in Neuropsychological mild cognitive impairment, 75 memory, 292
Test Performance, 142–144 MindStreams information processing,
extension of, 142 advantages, 87 285–289
goal, 141 correlation with WMS-III, 87 intelligence, 284
norms, 141 Global Assessment Battery, 87 laboratory supported defi nite,
norms for Spanish-speaking procedure, 86–87 282
Adults, 145–146 test reliability, 87 long-term memory, 290–292
subject sample, 141 tests in, 86–87 meta-analysis reviews, 286–287
medial temporal lobe amnesia, test validity, 87 neurocognitive dysfunction in,
562–563 Verbal Memory score, 87 283–294
memory impairment, 560 versions, 87 neuropathology, 280–281
Memory Impairment Screen Mini-Cog, 108 preferential damages, 280
(MIS), 108, 118 Mini-Mental State Examination primary-progressive, 282
Mendelian PARK loci, 201 (MMSE), 53, 107, 109, 112, probable, 282
Mental Status Questionnaire 113, 115–116, 139, 180, prognosis, 282–283
(MSQ), 109 224–225, 311, 490, 642 relapsing-remitting, 282
mercury exposure, 496–502 Minnesota Multiphasic Personality remote memory, 292–293
metal neurotoxicity Inventory (MMPI), 425–426 risk factors associated with,
arsenic, 481 Minnesota Multiphasic Personality 294–295
clinical manifestations of, 482 Inventory-2 (MMPI-2), 271 role of T lymphocytes, 280
dose–effect relationships, 482 minocycline, 383 short-term and working
encephalopathy, 481 Minor Cognitive-Motor Disorder memory, 289–290
gene–environment (MCMD), 368 symptoms, 281
interactions, 483 Misplaced Objects Test, 94 verbal abilities, 284–285
Subject Index 739

visuoperceptual and reasoning and behavior, 35–36 oral glucose tolerance test
constructional abilities, 285 research methods, 27 (OGTT), 351
multiple system atrophy, 212 semantic system, 30–33 oxcarbamazepine, 277
autonomic, 213 theoretical orientation, 26–27 OxyContin, 468–469
cerebellar, 213 visual functions, 29–30
multiple system atrophy- neuropsychological assessment, Paired-Associates Learning
parkinsonism (MSA-P), Benton’s comments, 67–68 Test, 92
212–213 Neuropsychological Deficit pallidotomy, 203
myoclonic seizures, 269 Scale, 8 paramimia, 43
neuropsychological tests, 4–5, 7 paraphasias, 46
N-acetyl aspartate NeuroTrax central computer, 86 The Parietal Lobes (Critchley,
(NAA), 425, 465 nicotine patch treatment of MacDonald), 55
Name–Face Association Test, Age-Associated Memory parkin (PARK2), 201
94–95 Impairment, test of, 93 parkinsonism, 177, 199
names/common nouns NINDS–Canadian Stroke Network Parkinson-plus syndromes, 211
dissociation, 32 Neuropsychological Test Parkinson’s disease (PD), 74, 318
Narrative Recall measures, 94 Protocols, 310 annual incidence rates, 200
National Adult Reading Test N-methyl-Daspartic acid (NMDA) anxiety, 206–207
(NART), 28 antagonists, 386 attentional skills, 204
National Hospital Interview non-PD Parkinsonian syndromes, in Blacks and Asians, 200
Schedule, 242 199 cholinergic deficits, 202, 209
Negative Predictive Value, 106 norepinephrine, 202 clinical characteristics, 200–201
neglect dyslexia, 32 normative data, in cognitive abilities, 205
nephropathy, leading to renal neuropsyhological cognitive changes associated
failure, 351 assessment, 127–129 with “on–off ” periods, 206
neural tissue, of penumbral norms for elderly, in community-based studies, 200
region, 308 neuropsyhological comprehension of complex
Neurobehavioral Cognitive Status assessment, 140–141 commands and grammar, 204
Examination (NCSE). See deep brain stimulation
Cognistat Object Decision Test of the (DBS), 203
neurological deficits, AVM, 320 VOSP, 32 deficit of free recall, 204
neuro-ophthalmic abnormalities, obsessive-compulsive behaviors definition, 199
200–201 (OCB) and/or disorder dementia, 207–210
neuropsychological approach, (OCD), 244, 247–249 depression, 206
to predicting everyday obstructive sleep apnea (OSA) differential diagnosis, 210–213
functioning cognitions in, 341 disease onset, 199
academic achievement, 636 learning and memory, 341 dopaminergic deficit, 209
defining outcomes, 634–636 neurocognitive testing, 341 early vs late, 205–206
direct assessment, 639–645 posttreatment epidemiology, 200
independent living, 636–637 neuropsychological genetics, 201
limitations, 638–639 functioning in, 342–343 in industrialized countries, 200
methodological considerations, potential mechanisms f or language skills, 204
633–634 neurobehavioral dysfunction and loss of dopaminergic
overview, 632–633 in, 343–345 projections, 201–202
vocational functioning, 637–638 psychomotor performance, 342 monoaminergic deficit, 209
neuropsychological assessment, Occupational Therapy Evaluation multiple system atrophy, 212
analytical approach of Performance and Support multiple system atrophy-
approach to differential (OTEPS), 641 autonomic, 213
diagnosis, 36–37 oculomotor dysfunction, 164 multiple system
clinical testing of cognitive olivoponto-cerebellar atrophy atrophy-cerebellar, 213
functions, 28–29 (OPCA), 200, 213 multiple system
elimination of cognitive “on–off ” phenomenon in atrophy-parkinsonism
operations, 36 PD, 206 (MSA-P), 212–213
event memory, 33–35 opiates, neuropsychological multiple system
reading disorders, 32–33 impact, 468–470 degenerations, 200
740 Subject Index

Parkinson’s disease (PD) (cont.) progressive supranuclear palsy, Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure
muscular rigidity, 200 211–212 Test, 57, 58, 256, 543, 577
neuro-ophthalmic promipaxole, 202 Rhythm Test, 6, 9–10
abnormalities, 200–201 Psychologix Computer-Simulated right cerebral hemisphere
neuropsychiatric features, Everyday Memory Test functions, 10, 30
206–208 Battery, 94–96 rigidity, 177, 200
neuropsychological aspects of, Public Events Test, 582 rivastigmine, 202
203–205 Pyramids and Palm Trees Rivermead Behavioral Memory
occurrence of tremor, 199 Test, 32 Test (RBMT), 35, 581
Parkinson-plus syndromes, 211 Rogers Decision-Making Task, 461
pathological gambling, 207 Quantified Neurological ropinerole, 202
pathology, 201–202 Examination (QNE), 226 Rorschach responses, analysis
pharmacological treatment, 202 of, 43
primary or idiopathic, 199 rapid forgetting, 165 Rorschach Test, 43
progressive supranuclear palsy, Rapid Visual Information-
211–212 Processing test, 90–91 satellite tests, 44
psychosis, 207 rasageline (N-propargyl- schizophrenia, 32, 61
recognition memory in, 204 R-aminoindan) mesylate, 202 amnesia, 511–512
secondary parkinsonism, 199 Raven Colored Progressive assessment strategies in,
sleep disorders, 207 Matrices, 28, 145, 178, 403 516–517
surgical interventions, 203 Raven Standard Progressive cognitive remediation
treatment for dementias in, 210 Matrices, 43 interventions, 516
vascular form of, 200 Reaction Time Test, 94–95 confirmatory modeling
visuospatial skills, 204–205 reading disorders, 32–33 procedures, 510
volitional eye movements, 201 reasoning and behavior, cortical dementia, 511
vs Alzheimer’s disease, 209–211 neuropsychological course of cognitive
PARK8 (LRRK2) locus, 201 assessment, 35–36 impairments, 507–509
Pattern Recognition Memory Receiver Operating Characteristic diagnostic criteria for, 507
Test, 90 (ROC), 168 differential performance
Pediatric Autoimmune Recognition Memory Test (RMT), deficits, 509
Neuropsychiatric Disorders 35, 578 factor analysis of, 510
Associated with Streptococcal Recognition of Faces—Signal frontostriatal conditions,
infections (PANDAS), 243 Detection, 94 510–511
Perceptual Organizational Regard’s Five Point Test, 55 functional relevance of
Index, 28 Reitan, Ralph, 4 neuropsychological (NP)
peripheral dyslexias, 32 Reitan–Indiana Aphasia Screening impairments, 515–516
peripheral neuropathy, with risk of Test (AST), 7, 10 Israeli draft board
foot ulcers, 351 Reitan–Kløve Sensory-Perceptual assessments, 508
phenobarbital, 277 Test, 7 later, 508
phenytoin, 277 Reitan–Wolfson model of neuroimaging techniques, 509
Pick’s disease, 71, 164–165 neuropsychological prevalence of
Picture Completion, difficulty functioning, graphic neuropsychological (NP)
function, 28 representation of, 9 impairments, 514––515
PINK1, 201 Reitan–Wolfson theory, of profi le of cognitive
polychlorinated biphenyls neuropsychological impairments, 509–512, 513
(PCBs), 498 functioning, 11 severity of impairments,
Poser classification system, 281 Repeatable Battery for 512–514
Positive Predictive Value, 106 the Assessment of treatments, 517–518
positron emission tomography Neuropsychological Status verbal memory and olfactory
(PET), 69 (RBANS), 110, 118 functions, 507–508
posttraumatic amnesia (PTA), 600 retinopathy, with potential loss of Schonell Reading Test, 28
priority of processing, in Boston vision, 351 Seashore Rhythm Test, 92, 131, 271
Process Approach, 56–60 retrograde amnesia, 166 sedatives, abused. See
progressive aphasia, 165 Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test Benzodiazepine
progressive dementia (RAVLT), 54–55, 87, 92, 142, selection ratio, defi ned, 106
syndromes, 71 271, 574 Selective Reminding Test, 94, 576
Subject Index 741

selective serotonin reuptake abuse, neuropsychological assessment of World War II


inhibitor (SSRI), 517–518 consequences of soldiers, 618
selegiline, 202, 383 substance P, 383 baseline information, 601
self-monitoring performance, 159 Supervisory Attentional System challenges in determination
Self-Rating Questionnaire, 271 (SAS), 255 of, 601
semantic dementia, 165 supraspan word list test, 85 consequences of, 597
semantic system, Symbol Copy, 49 costs, 597–598
neuropsychological Symbol-Digits Modalities Test depressive mood, 620–622
assessment, 30–33 (SDMT), 285 genetic factors (APOE4) and
Semantic/Verbal Memory α-synuclein, 201 outcome, 611
construct, 91 hospitalization rates for, 597
sensitivity, defi ned, 105 Tactile Form Recognition Test, incidence rates, 597
Sensory-Perceptual Exam, 92 11, 92 indices of severity of brain
serotonin, 202 Tactual Performance Test (TPT), injury, 599–600
serotonin reuptake inhibitors, 383 6, 11, 92, 136, 401 irritability and disinhibited
Seven-Minute Screen (7MS), Telephone Dialing Test, 94 behavior, 625–626
108, 117 Telephone Interview for limitation of choice of control
sex differences, impact in Cognitive Status-Modified groups, 601–602
neuropsychological (TICS-m), 115 loss of empathy, 624–625
assessment, 131–132, 136, Telephone Interview for Cognitive methodological problems, 620
148–149 Status (TICS), 112 molecular biology, genes, and
Shipley Institute of Living Scale Television Test, 584 psychiatric disorders after,
Abstracting Test, 403 Test of Language Development 626–627
Short and Sweet Screening (TOLD), 498 neuropsychiatric
Instrument (SASSI), 109, 115 Test of Memory Malingering, aspects of, 619
Short Portable Mental Status 18, 37 neuropsychological outcomes,
Questionnaire (SPMSQ), 109, Tests in CogState Academic 602–606
117–118 Battery, 91 nonavailability of sample
short-term memory, 33 Tests of cognitive flexibility, 403 cases, 602
Shy Drager syndrome, 200 theory of cerebral outcome following, 598–600
spasticity, 281 specialization, 26 paranoid ideation, 623–627
spatial imaging, analysis of, 30 Theory of Mind, 36 phenomenological
spatial processing domain, 85 third-order development, 30 experiences, 627
Spatial Relations Assessment, 92 tiagabin, 277 psychiatric and behavioral
Spatial Span test, 90 Time and Change Test (T&C), problems in children, 626
Spatial Working Memory Test, 92 111, 118 psychosocial/functional status
specificity, defined, 105 Timed psychomotor tests, 230 outcomes, 606–611
Speech-Sounds Perception Test Token Test, 68 psychotic thoughts, 623–627
(SSPT), 6, 9, 20, 92, 131 tonic seizures, 269 severity of injury, 599–600
spinocerebellar ataxia (SCA), 213 topiramate, 276–277, 277 socially inappropriate
standardization samples, 129 Tourette’s syndrome, 242 comments, 626
Stanford Binet battery, 30 Tourette’s syndrome videotaped treatment adherence
status epilepticus, 270 scale, 242 cognitive function impacts,
Stockings of Cambridge Test of Tower of Hanoi puzzle, 175 693–701
spatial planning, 90 Tower of London Test, 90, 176, 229 defining and measuring, 690
striatonigral degeneration, 200 Trail Making Test, 36, 55, 73, 92, impact and importance,
Stroop Color-Word Test, 232 118, 131, 168, 230, 257, 271, 688–690
Stroop interference and Initiation/ 285, 309, 401, 465 management of methodologies,
Perseveration scores, 543–544 components of, 13 690–693
Stroop Test, 36, 176, 230 method, 13 medication, 703–705
Structured Clinical Interview Tranel, Daniel, 67 psychosocial models,
for DSM-III-R Personality transient global amnesia, 564 701–703
Disorders II (SCID-II), 250 traumatic brain injury (TBI), tremor, 200–201
substance abuse, 17–18 type 1 and 2 diabetes. See
neuropsychological aging with, 611–613 Diabetes, brain dysfunction
consequences of. See Drug anxiety after, 622–623 and cognitive impairment in
742 Subject Index

UCSD Performance-Based Skills arithmetic, 543 Wechsler Memory Scale-Third


Assessment (UPSA), 642 arithmetic subtest, 47–48 Edition (WMS-III), 48
unusual-view photograph test, 30 block design subtest, 50 Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS), 8,
U.S. Department of Labor comprehension subtest, 47 43, 46, 579–581
Dictionary of Occupational Digit Span subtest, 48, 177 Boston revision of, 52–53
Titles, 645 digit symbol subtest, Logical Memory subtest, 53
49–50, 50 and MicroCog, 86
valproic acid, 276, 277 errors, 49 normative data for, 140
vascular cognitive impairment Information Multiple-choice recall, 57, 59
(VCI), 309, 314–317 subtest, 47, 47 visual reproduction, 53
vascular dementia, 163–164 information subtest, 46–47 Wechsler Memory Scale
clinicopathological modifications, 46, 51 (WMS-III), 129
classification, 313 multiple-choice format for the age effects, 129
diagnostic considerations, 313 Vocabulary subtest, 49 education effects, 130
epidemiology, 312 object assembly subtest, 51–52 sex differences on, 131
problems with the DSM-IV picture arrangement subtest, Wechsler Scales, 6, 17, 20
vascular dementia diagnosis, 50–51 Wechsler Test of Adult Reading
314–315 picture completion (WTAR), 29
vs Alzheimer’s disease, 181–182 subtest, 50 Weigl Sorting Test, 36
VCI without dementia (VaCIND), sentence arrangement Wernicke–Korsakoff Syndrome,
315–317 subtest, 51 412–414
Verbal and Nonverbal Selective similarities subtest, 48 Wernicke’s aphasia, 27
Reminding Test, 256 span of apprehension, 48 Wernicke’s lesions, 73
Verbal and Performance Scales, 28 Spatial Span subtest, 48 white matter lesion pathology,
verbal fluency, 159 verbal subtests, 641 317–319
Verbal Recognition Memory Vocabulary subtest, 48–49 Wilson’s disease, 200, 242
test, 91 Wechsler Adult Intelligence Wisconsin Card Sorting Test
vicodin, 468 Scale (WAIS), 10, 28, 43, 70, (WCST), 36, 73, 176, 203, 205,
visual disorientation, 29, 37 129, 145 224, 254, 403, 460
visual evoked potentials age effects, 129 Word Finding Test, 403
(VEP), 281 education effects, 130 Word Fluency Test, 254
Visual Object and Space sex differences on, 131 word-form or spelling
Perception (VOSP) battery, standardization sample, 131 dyslexia, 32
29, 227 Verbal subscales, 285 Word List-Learning
Visual Paired Associates Test, 53 Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Tasks, 574
visual perceptual analysis, 29–30 (WAIS)-III, 271 Word Memory Test, 37
Visual Reproduction Wechsler Intelligence Scale for working memory, 159
Test, 168, 577 Children-Revised (WISC-R), Working Memory
Visual Retention Test, 68 145, 181, 254 Index, 28
visual–spatial abilities, 10 Wechsler Intelligence Scale
for Children (WISC-III), 51 x-ray fluoroscopy (XRF), 484
Wada Test, 271–272 Arithmetic and Block Designs
Washington Psychosocial Seizure subtests of, 68 Yale Global Tic Severity
Inventory, 271 Visual Reproduction Scale, 242
Wechsler Adult Intelligence subtest, 57 YES/NO recognition trial, 54
Scale-Revised (WAIS-R), 36, Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised
43, 224, 230, 490 (WMS-R), 142, 224, 374 zonisamide, 277, 277

Você também pode gostar