Você está na página 1de 5

Auditory Comprehension Problems in Adult Aphasic

Individuals
Cynthia M. Shewan

Whether an aphasic patient understands what is said auditory and linguistic message components (Figure 1).
appears to be influenced by many factors in addition to This applies to aphasic patients functioning in everyday
the message itself, among them: how the material is pres- situations and in the clinical setting.
ented, what the situational context is, the nature of the
response required, the individual's cognitive functioning,
his/her motivation, and the location and extent of the
cerebral lesion_ Of course, the individual engaged in the
comprehension task is also important. The nature of PSYCHOLOGICAL PRESENTATION
auditory comprehension problems in adult aphasic indi- VARIABLES VARIABLES
viduals is indeed complex, but we are making headway_
To paraphrase Shakespeare, To understand or not to
understand is a complex question.
Researchers now generally agree that all aphasic
patients demonstrate auditory comprehension problems_
Of less consensus is whether all comprehension prob- Figure 1. Dimensions influencing auditory compre-
lems are similar in type. hension of a linguistic message.(reprinted with
permission).
Whenever we communicate with an aphasic patient,
during testing or in conversation, some of the time we talk Processing Auditory Stimuli:
and the patient listens. These interactions require that the
aphasic person understand our linguistic messages. Perceptual Data
Whether that understanding takes place is influenced by Auditory perceptual deficits do not appear to be the
many factors operating in the situation, including auditory major contributing factors to aphasic patients' inability to
perception, auditory comprehension, and factors which understand auditory messages. The auditory perceptual
influence auditory comprehension. problems may involve discrimination, recognition, and/or
Presumably, the ultimate goal of processing mes- temporal ordering, using verbal or nonverbal material,
sages is to comprehend them, that is, to assign approp- and may be found among several aphasic types. How-
riate meaning to them. Take the message "Gotcha". ever, the disociation betweeen perceptual problems and
What does it mean? Interpretation depends upon linguistic comprehension problems in many aphasic patients pre-
aspects, context, and extralinguistic variables. Said by a cludes using perceptual problems as the explanatory
parent to a young child, "Gotcha" represents "I've got cause for comprehension deficits. Recognizing a "towel
you, you're not going to fall". Used in the context of rack" as a "motorcycle", as one of our aphasic clients did,
playing a game of tag, or changed slightly to "Caughtcha", probably represented a semantic confusion that did not
it means "Tag, you're it". Said by an adult during conver- involve a failure of perceptual processing.
sation, it might mean "I understand what you said", or, in
another context with a different intonation pattern, it Processing Auditory Language:
might mean "You fell into the trap I set for you". Uttered Understanding the Linguistic Input
during a game of cowboys and rustlers, "Gotcha" repres- Comprehension of spoken language requires indi-
ents "Bang, bang, you're dead". The point of this example vidual processing of the linguistic components· phonol-
is to illustrate that whether a person understands or fails ogy, semantics, and syntax - as well as interactive pro-
to understand depends on many dimensions of the com- cessing among them.
munication situation as well as the processing of the

Cynthia M. Shewan, Ph.D.


Phonology
University of Western Ontario Comprehension requires the ability to perceive
speech and to identify phonemes with some accuracy,
Address Correspondence To:
Cynthia M. Shewan, PhD. but the suprasegmental aspects of phonology are also
Department of Communicative Disorders important. Perception of stress as a phonemic cue
University of Western Ontario appears to be maintained in aphasic adults (Blumstein &
Elborn College Goodglass, 1972), allowing them for example to distin-
London, Ontario N6G IHl guish "record" from "record". Stress is an important
factor in aphasic patients' performance on receptive lan-

Human Communication Canada/Communication Humaine Canada, Vol. 9, No. 5, 1985 151


guage tasks and exaggerating stress can assist compre- increase in task difficulty generally stems from the greater
hension (Kellar, 1978; Pashek & Brookshire, 1980). amount of material to process and the necessity to relate
Although the cues provided by intonation contours the material together.
may be redundant for normal adults, they appear to be Although most research has investigated aspects of
necessary for aphasic patients with severe comprehen- denotative meaning, impairment of connotative aspects
sion problems (Boiler & Green, 1972; Green & Boiler, parallels that for denotative meaning (Gardner & Denes,
1974). When presented with questions, statements, or 1973). Comprehension of metaphorical meaning in both
commands, aphasics made few correct responses. How- single sentences and paragraphs, by contrast, is main-
ever, their many appropriate responses, which matched tained in aphasic patients (Stachowiak et al., 1977;
the type of grammatical structure, suggested that they Winner & Gardner, 1977).
were obtaining relevant information from the intonation
contour of the message. In summary, it appears that a variety of phonological,
semantic, and syntactic variables influence aphasic
Lexical Processing patients' auditory comprehension.
Part of the aphasic patient's difficulties in under-
standing language stems from impairment in the lexicon, Table 1
which Schuell, Jenkins and Jimenez·Pabon (1964) des- Hierarchy of Difficulty for Sentence Types from Easy to
cribed as a "reduced availability of vocabulary". Fre- Difficult
quency of occurrence of lexical items and their semantic
category membership are known to influence comprehen-
sion (Goodglass et al., 1966; Shewan & Canter, 1971). SENTENCE TYPE
The research data for the effects of grammatical form EASY Simple Active Affirmative Declarative
class on comprehension, however, have been less con- e.g., The dog is chasing the cat.
sistent, and more controlled study is needed before firm NEGATIVE
conclusions are drawn (Goodglass, G leason & Hyde, 1970; e.g., The dog is not chasing the cat.
Parisi & Pizzamiglio, 1970; Shewan, 1976; Smith, 1974).
Passive
Aphasics' performance on auditory identification A. Nonreversible
tasks may also represent an impairment in semantic e.g., The ball is being caught by the dog.
representation or semantic knowledge. Aphasic patients B. Reversible
have been shown to have difficulty grouping lexical items e.g., The cat is being caught by the dog.
on the basis of semantic features (Zurif et al., 1974).
Furthermore, their semantic category boundaries were Nega tiue· Passive
found to be different from those of normal adults in terms e.g., The cat is not being chased by the dog.
of scope and hierarchical organization (L'hermitte, Cent er· Embedded
Derouesne, & Lecours, 1971; Derouesne & Lacours, A. Nonreversible
1972). Therefore, lexical impairment may involve not only e.g., The cat that the dog is chasing
a less accessible vocabulary but also disturbed semantic is meowing.
representation.
DIFFICULT B. Reversible
Semantic and Syntactic Processing e.g., The cat that the dog is chasing
Work in semantic and syntactic processing has indi- is black.
cated that, as the grammatical complexity of a sentence C. Improbable
increases, there is a corresponding decrease in compre· e.g., The boy that the dog is patting
hension accuracy. A typical difficulty hierarchy is shown is fat.
in Table 1. Not only is the sentence type itself an impor·
tant contributor to comprehension, but there are also
differences within types. Reversible sentences are more Other Dimensions Influencing Auditory
difficult than nonreversible ones (Lasky, Weidner, & Comprehension
Johnson, 1976; Caramazza & Zurif, 1978). Sentences Other dimensions to consider are context, manner
which follow an order of mention strategy are easier to of presentation, form of response, psychology, and extra-
understand than those which do not (Ansell & Flowers, linguistic. Both linguistic and situational context variables
1980). Whether sentences are true or false also influences can influence comprehension. A supportive linguistic
comprehension (Brookshire & Nicholas, 1980; Just, context usually aids comprehension (Gardner, Albert, &
Davis, & Carpenter, 1977). Increasing the length of sent- Weintraub, 1975; Wailer & Darley, 1978) by creating a set
ences mayor may not increase comprehension difficul- for the aphasic person (e.g., structuring all questions in a
ties, depending upon other variables (Shewan, 1979). "yes-no" format rather than random presentation of sev-
Comprehension of a series of sentences, such as a eral varieties), by reducing the number of likely alterna-
paragraph or text, is often more impaired than isolated tives, or by providing cues about what is to follow.
sentence comprehension in adult aphasic patients. This Although linguistic context is generally used by speech-

152 Human Communication Canada/Communication Humaine Canada, Vo!. 9, No. 5. 1985


language pathologists and family members to aid com- tant variables to consider. Most recent research in apha-
prehension, context in the form of competing messages sia has recognized the necessity to avoid verbal
or indirectly worded messages can reduce comprehen- responses and to employ gestural responses, such as
sion (Green & Boiler, 1974). pointing or manipulation of objects, when measuring aud-
Situational context may also positively influence itory comprehension (Shewan, 1979; Goodglass &
comprehension performance by adding redundancy to Kaplan, 1972; 1983). Also the number of choices in a
the linguistic message. This facilitates linguistic process- response array can affect the difficulty of the comprehen-
fng by biasing the subject toward using one strategy sion task and an aphasic adult's chance score. With only
rather than another or by limiting the number of possibili- two choices, the chance score is 50% which is reduced to
ties in a situation. For example, the patient does not need 25% when four choices are provided. The types of
to comprehend the entire question "Would you like response choices also influence task difficulty_ Selection
orange juice?" since this is more likely to occur than of "ball" from the array of "ball, house" is much easier
"Would you like orange socks?" in the context of a break- than from "ball, bat, wheel, doll". The former contains
only an unrelated decoy while the latter contains a seman-
fast situation.
tic association, a perceptual similarity, and a phonetic
Research has also shown that aphasic individuals similarity.
may perform differently depending on the situation in
which they are observed. The fact that aphasic patients The scoring system employed in an auditory com·
are likely to perform better in the clinical situation with a prehension task can determine the amount and quality of
speech-language pathologist that in other environments information obtained. While accuracy scores are impor-
may explain discrepant reports by various health profes- tant, multidimensional scoring systems provide more
sionals about aphasics' auditory comprehension (Egolf & information. The 16-point PICA scale measuring accu-
Chester, 1977; Helmick, Watamori, & Palmer, 1976). racy, responsiveness, completeness, efficiency, and
Moreover, a speaker's intended meaning does not always promptness (Porch, 1967) is a good example. This kind of
correspond to the literal meaning of the words used. system is not without its drawbacks, however, as outlined
Aphasic individuals' difficulty comprehending indirect by Silverman (1974).
requests, such as "Must you tap that pencil?", demon-
strates the importance of context in determining an The psychological dimension can be divided into
appropriate interpretation. motivation, intelligence, and memory variables. As a
speaker interacting with an aphasic patient, one can
How messages are presented to the aphasic patient motivate better performance. With positive instructions,
can influence understanding. Timing aspects of verbal Stoicheff (1960) found that aphasic patients performed
messages are important to aphasic patients' comprehen· better; with negative expectations, they performed
sion. Expanding the speech signal by inserting pauses has poorer. The patient's own intrinsic motivation is also
a positive effect on comprehension, either on the accu- important, but it is a difficult variable to quantify empiri-
racy or on the speed of response (Liles & Brookshire, cally. Clinical experience, however, repeatedly tells us
1975; Sheehan, Aseltine, & Edwards, 1973). Longer that motivated patients generally do better in treatment
pauses appear to be more facilitory. Aphasic individuals than un motivated ones.
may use different strategies during pause time (Salvatore,
1975). Expanding both speech and pause intervals has Although aphasia does not cause intellectual
not generally facilitated comprehension, although com· impairment, it is important to consider an aphasic
pressing the signal in this way is detrimental to it (Berg- patient's premorbid intellectual level when selecting goals
man et al., 1977). Supplementing the primary auditory and materials for rehabilitation. To state the obvious, it is
input modality with visual information (gesture, written inappropriate to use materials that exceed a patient's
language) can assist comprehension (Gardiner & Brook- premorbid intellectual level. Whether a sentence is
shire, 1972; Yorkston, Marshall, & Butler, 1977; York- understood also depends, to some extent, on the con-
ston, Beukelman, & Waugh, 1979). straints it imposes on memory. Auditory memory factors
Presentation aspects, such as repetition of the mes- play a role in an aphasic patient's ability to decode, retain,
sage, frequently improve aphasic patients' performance and recall verbal material. However, specifying and quan-
(Brookshire, 1978; Schuell, Jenkins, & Jimenez-Pabon, tifying exactly how memory influences understanding is
1964). Live voice presentation of auditory material is difficult, especially since measures of memory have
superior to tape recorded presentation (Boiler et aI., frequently used unrelated items. Numbers are unrelated
1979). The sequence of events presented may also be in an auditory digit span task, and may be processed
important because presentation of difficult commands, differently from related items, words in sentences.
interspersed among easy ones, has been shown to inter-
fere with aphasics' performance on commands previously Many aphasic patients have more difficulty accu-
determined to be easy (Brookshire, 1976). rately comprehending nonredundant material with highly
concentrated information content than sentences of
The response required from an aphasic person can equivalent length in which the information content is dis-
affect performance on an auditory comprehension test. tributed throughout (DeRenzi & Vignolo, 1962). Part of
Response modality, choices, and scoring are some impor- the difficulty may arise from "chunking" or "clustering"

Human Communication Canada/Communication Humaine Canada, Vot 9, No. 5, 1985 153


problems that reduce the amount of information an DeRenzi, E. & Vignolo, L.A. (1962). The Token Test: A sensi·
aphasic patient is able to process (TiIlman & Gerstman, tive test to detect receptive disturbances in aphasics. Brain, 85,
1977). 665·678.
High emotional content of material has been found to Derouesne, J. & Lecours, AR (1972). Two tests for the study
facilitate auditory comprehension (Boiler et al., 1979). of semantic deficits in aphasia. International Journal 0/ Mental
However, comprehension of affectively toned or melodi- Health, 1, 14·24.
cally intoned sentences, which are neutral in semantic Egolf, D.8. & Chester, S.L. (1977). A comparison of aphasics'
content, was not superior to comprehension of normally verbal performance in the language clinic with their verbal per·
toned sentences (Wallace, Canter, & Shewan, 1980). formance in other program areas of a comprehensive rehabilita·
Also, familiar content can facilitate auditory comprehen- tion center. Rehabilitation Literature, 38, 9·11, 32.
sion. Severely impaired aphasic patients have been Gardiner, B.J. & Brookshire, RH. (1972), Effects of unisensory
shown to be able to follow familiar conversational topics, and multisensory presentation of stimuli upon naming by
but became confused and misunderstood less familiar aphasic subjects. Language & Speech 15, 324·357.
topics (Stachowiak et al., 1977). Interest and familiarity
probably interact with listener motivation and directly Gardner, H., Albert, M.L., Weintraub, S. (1975). Comprehend·
ing a word: The influence of speed and redundancy on auditory
affect it. comprehension in aphasia. Cortex, 11, 155·162.
Summary Gardner, H. & Denes, G. (1973). Connotative judgements by
aphasic patients on a pictorial adaptation of the semantic differ·
Comprehension problems in the aphasic adult gen- ential. Cortex, 9, 183·196.
erally encompass understanding the meaning of the lingu-
istic message; that is, they involve semantic and syntactic Goodglass, H., Gleason, J.B., & Hyde, M.R (1970). Some
dimensions of auditory language comprehension in aphasia.
processing. Explanations of these problems on the basis
Journal 0/ Speech & Hearing Research, 13, 595·606.
of auditory perceptual disturbances are no longer widely
accepted. Although perception may be involved in some Goodglass, H. & Kaplan, E. (1972). The assessment 0/ aphasia
cases, it is not the underlying explanatory factor in most. and related disorders. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger.
Goodglass, H. & Kaplan, E. (1983). The assessment 0/ aphasia
References and related disorders (2nd Ed.). Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger.
Ansell, B.J. & Flowers, C.R (1980). Apasic adults' use 0/ struc- Goodglass, H., Klein, 8., Carey, P. W., & Jones, K.J. (1966).
tura/linguistic cues/or analysing sentences. Paper presented at Specific semantic word categories in aphasia. Cortex, 2, 74-89.
the annual convention of the American Speech-Language-
Hearing Association, Detroit. Green, E. & Boiler, F. (1974). Features of auditory comprehen·
sion in severely impaired aphasics. Cortex, 10, 133·145.
Bergman, M., Fiselon, J., Tze'elon, R, Mendelson, L., &
Schechter, L (1977). The effects of message speed on auditory Helmick, J.W., Watamori, T.S. & Palmer, J.R. (1976). Spouses'
comprehension in patients with cerebral cranial injury. Scan- understanding of the communication disabilities of aphasic
dinavian Journal 0/ Rehabilitative Medicine, 9, 169-191. patients. Journal 0/ Speech & Hearing Disorders, 41,238·243.

Blumstein, SE & Goodglass, H. (1972). The perception of Just, M.A., Davis, G.A, & Carpenter, P.A (1977). A compari·
stress as a semantic cue in aphasia. Journal 0/ Speech & Hear- son of aphasic and normal adults in a sentence verification task.
ing Research, 15, 800-806. Cortex, 13,402·423.
Boiler, F., Cole, M., Vrtunski, P.b., Patterson, M., & Kim, Y. Kellar, L.A (1978). Stress and syntax in aphasia. Paper pres·
(1976). Paralinguistic aspects of auditory comprehension in ented at the Academy of Aphasia, Chicago.
aphasia. Brain & Language, 7, 164-174.
Lasky, E., Weidner, W.E., & Johnson, J.P. (1976). Influence of
Boiler, E, & Green, E. (1972). Comprehension in severe apha- linguistic complexity, rate of presentation and interphrase
sia. Cortex, 8, 382-394. pause time on auditory·verbal comprehension of adult aphasic
patients. Brain & Language, 3, 386·395.
Brookshire, RH. (1976). Effects of task difficulty on sentence
comprehension performance of aphasic subjects. Journal 0/ Lhermitte, F., Derouesne, J., and Lecours, A.R (1971). Contri·
Communication Disorders, 9, 167-173. bution a I'etude des troubles semantiques dans I'aphasie. Revue
Neuro{ogique, 125,81·101.
Brookshire, RH. (1978). Auditory comprehension and aphasia.
In D.F. Johns (Ed.), Clinical management 0/ neurogenic com· Liles, 8.Z. & Brookshire, R H. (1975). The effects of of pause
municative disorders. Boston: Little, Brown and Company. time on auditory comprehension of aphasic subjects. Journal 0/
Communication Disorders, 8, 221·235.
Brookshire, RH. & Nicholas, L.E. (1980). Verification of active
and passive sentences by aphasic and nonaphasic subjects. Parisi, P. & Pizzamiglio, L (1970). Syntactic comprehension in
Journal 0/ Speech & Hearing Research, 23, 878·893. aphasia. Cortex, 6, 204·215.
Caramazza, A. & Zurif, E.8. (1978). Comprehension of corn· Pashek, G.V. & Brookshire, RH. (1980). Effects of rate of
plex sentences in children and aphasics: A test of the regression speech and linguistic stress on auditory paragraph comprehen·
hypothesis. In A. Caramazza & E.B. Zurif (Eds.), Language sion of aphasic individuals (Abstract). In RH. Brookshire (Ed.),
acquisition and language breakdown. Baltimore: The John Clinical aphasiology conference proceedings. Minneapolis:
Hopkins University Press. BRK Publishers.

154 Human Communication Canada/Communication Humaine Canada, Vol. 9, No. 5. 1985


Porch, B.E. (1967). Porch index of communicative ability. Palo Stoicheff, M.L. (1960). Motivating instructions and language
Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press. performance of dysphasic subjects. Journal of Speech & Hear·
ing Research, 3, 75·85.
Salvatore, AP. (1975). The effects of pause duration on sent·
ence comprehension by aphasic individuals. Paper presented at Tillman, D., & Gerstman, L.J. (1977). Clustering by aphasics in
the annual convention of the American Speech and Hearing free recall. Brain & Language, 4, 355·364.
Association, Washington.
Wallace. G.L., Canter, G.J., & Shewan, C.M. (1980). The effect
Schuell, H., Jenkins, J.J., & Jimenez·Pabon, E. (1964). Aphasia of affectively·toned and melodically·intoned speech on the aud·
in adults. New York: Harper & Row. itory comprehension of aphasic patients. Paper presented at
the annual convention of the American Speech· Language-
Sheehan, J.G., Aseltine, So. & Edwards, AE. (1973). Aphasic Hearing Association, Detroit.
comprehension of time spacing. Journal of Speech & Hearing
Research, 16, 650·657. Wailer, M.R. & Darley, F.L. (1978). The influence of context on
the auditory comprehension of paragraphs by aphasic subjects.
Shewan, C.M. (1976). Error patterns in auditory comprehen· Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 21, 732·745.
sion of adult aphasics. Cortex, 12. 325·336.
Winner. E. & Gardner, H. (1977). The comprehension of meta·
She wan, C.M. (1979). Auditory comprehension test for senten· phor in brain-damaged patients. Brain, lOO, 717·729.
ces. Chicago: Biolinguistics Clinical Institutes.
Yorkston, K.M. Beukelman. D.R. & Waugh. P.F. (1979). A
Shewan, CM. & Canter, G.J. (1971). Effects of vocabulary, comparison of verbal. pantomime, and combined instruction
syntax, and sentence length on auditory comprehension of modes with severely aphasic individuals. In R.H. Brookshire
aphasic patients. Cortex, 7, 209·226. (Ed.). Clinical aphasio/ogy conference proceedings. Minneapo·
Silverman, F.H. (1974). The Porch Index of Communicative lis: BRK Publishers.
Ability (PICA): A psychometric problem and its solution. Jour· Yorkston, K.M., Marshall, R.C. & Butler, M.R. (1977). Effects
nal of Speech & Hearing Disorders, 39, 225·226. on aphasics' auditory comprehension of visually and non·
Smith, M.D. (1974). On the understanding of some relational visually cued material. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 44,647·655.
words in aphasia. Neuropsychologia, 12,377·384. Zurif, E.B., Caramazza, A, Meyerson, R., & Galvin, J. (1974).
Stachowiak, F.J., Huber, W., Poeck. K., & Kerschensteiner, M. Semantic feature representations for normal and aphasic lan·
(1977). Text comprehension in aphasia. Brain & Language, 4, guage. Brain & Language, 1, 167·187.
177-195.

Human Cammunication Canada/Communication Humaine Canada. Vo!. 9, No. 5, 1985 155

Você também pode gostar