Você está na página 1de 7

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: http://www.researchgate.net/publication/257045512

Emotional intelligence (EI), conflict resolution patterns, and


relationship satisfaction: Actor and partner effects revisited

ARTICLE in PERSONALITY AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES · JANUARY 2013


Impact Factor: 1.95 · DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.09.013

CITATIONS READS
7 290

2 AUTHORS, INCLUDING:

Moshe Zeidner
University of Haifa
134 PUBLICATIONS 3,844 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

All in-text references underlined in blue are linked to publications on ResearchGate, Available from: Moshe Zeidner
letting you access and read them immediately. Retrieved on: 13 December 2015
Personality and Individual Differences 54 (2013) 278–283

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

Emotional intelligence (EI), conflict resolution patterns, and relationship


satisfaction: Actor and partner effects revisited
Moshe Zeidner ⇑, Iris Kloda
University of Haifa, Israel

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: This study tested for actor and partner effects in the relationship between ability-based EI and adaptive
Received 21 June 2012 marital relationships and satisfaction employing a standard dyadic design involving 100 young hetero-
Received in revised form 13 September 2012 sexual couples. Participants were assessed on the MSCEIT along with measures of conflict resolution pat-
Accepted 16 September 2012
terns and perceived marital satisfaction. A series of path analyses provided support for actor effects—but
Available online 24 October 2012
not partner effects—in the association of EI, assessed as ability, and marital variables. Overall, the data
provide only partial support for the common claim and some prior research suggesting that EI plays a
Keywords:
pivotal role in marital relationships.
Emotional intelligence
Close personal relationship
Ó 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Marital satisfaction
Conflict resolution pattern

1. Introduction 1.1. Marital conflict and marital satisfaction

In many ways, emotional intelligence (EI) has been heralded as Marital conflict arises because of disagreement or conflict over
a promising new construct in psychological science that is directed personal goals, intentions, values, interests, or behavioral priori-
towards improving the human condition, including close personal ties. Most married couples find themselves engaged in some kind
relationships (Fitness, 2001; Zeidner, Matthews, & Robert, 2009). of conflict throughout their marriage. In young couples, as is the
Popular definitions of EI are varied and inconsistent, with EI case in this study, issues relating to mutual adjustment (managing
assessed in the literature both as a trait (e.g., Pérez, Petrides, & money, relating to in-laws, etc.) bring to the couple new problems
Furnham, 2005) and as ability (Mayer, Caruso, & Salovey, 1999). and this in turn portends exacting demands on their capacities to
Following Salovey and Mayer, we loosely define EI as a set of communicate (Navran, 1967).
inter-related abilities for identifying, understanding, applying, Both cross-sectional and longitudinal data reviewed show that
and regulating emotions. conflictual interactions predict marital satisfaction and stability
Emotional intelligence and related emotional and social compe- (Karney & Bradbury, 1995). The use of mutually constructive dis-
tencies often help convey important information about people’s cussion and direct conflict resolution strategies may enhance inti-
thoughts and intentions, and may help considerably in coordinat- macy among partners (Christensen & Shenk, 1991). By contrast,
ing social encounters, particularly close personal relationships less adaptive strategies (e.g., mutual avoidance of problem discus-
(Lopes et al., 2004). In fact, in emotional transmission, one sions, demand-withdrawal communication) can lead to marital
partner’s competencies and behaviors can increase positive (and unhappiness and dissolution (Noller & Feeney, 2002). Furthermore,
negative!) effects in the other partner (Hatfield, Cacioppo, & perceived relationship quality (or satisfaction) is one of the stron-
Rapson, 1994). Although there are multiple factors determining gest predictors of marriage stability and duration, as well as a ro-
marital satisfaction and quality of personal relationships (Murray, bust predictor of psychological and physical health (Horowitz,
Holmes, & Griffin, 1996), this study focuses on one putatively McLaughlin, & White, 1998).
important predictor of marital relations and satisfaction—EI.
We begin our review of the literature by briefly discussing the 1.2. EI and marital life
link between marital conflict and satisfaction and then move onto
discuss the role of EI in marital life. EI has been claimed to be an important factor contributing to
relationship stability and happiness (Fitness, 2001). In order for
⇑ Corresponding author. Address: University of Haifa, Mt. Carmel, 31905 Haifa, partners to cope successfully with ongoing stresses and emotional
Israel. Tel.: +972 4 8240897; fax: +972 4 8240911. fissures, their skill in identifying, understanding, and regulating
E-mail address: zeidner@research.haifa.ac.il (M. Zeidner). their own emotions and those of their partners becomes critical

0191-8869/$ - see front matter Ó 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.09.013
M. Zeidner, I. Kloda / Personality and Individual Differences 54 (2013) 278–283 279

(Fitness, 2001). Thus, EI has been claimed to help individuals to similarity on personal traits and marital satisfaction (Luo et al.,
maintain positive emotional interactions, communicate and han- 2008).
dle conflicts more effectively, and regulate their emotions better,
thus facilitating solutions during marital conflicts and contribute
2. Method
to marital well-being (Fitness, 2001).
However, the role of EI in shaping close interpersonal relation-
2.1. Participants
ships has received only scant support in the literature, with only a
handful of studies (e.g., Brackett, Rivers, Shiffman, Lerner, &
Participants were 100 newlywed heterosexual couples, married
Salovey, 2006; Fitness, 2001; Schutte et al., 2001) systematically
within one year of assessment, on average (M = 11 months,
examining the association between EI and close personal relation-
SD = 7.23). Participants were solicited from among the wider stu-
ships. Overall, these studies indicated significant EI ‘actor’ effects
dent population and graduates of a major Israeli research univer-
(Kenny, Kashy, & Cook, 2006, pp. 144–184) in predicting marital
sity. Participants ranged in age from 20 to 32 years (M = 25.87,
satisfaction. However, because sound dyadic designs and analyses
SD = 2.71). Husbands were significantly older, on average, than
were not employed in any of the foregoing studies, ‘partner’ effects
their spouses, 27.52 (2.62) > 25.87 (2.72), t (97) = 7.30, p < .001,
could not be tested and the tenability and the generalizability of
with dyadic partners strongly similar in age, r (98) = .67, p < .001.
the reported results remain limited.
Over 92% of the women and 81% of the men had completed at least
In a more recent study, employing a methodologically appropri-
one academic degree (B.A., Engineering, etc.) at the time of
ate dyadic design, Smith, Ciarrochi, and Heaven (2008) examined
assessment.
the relationship between trait self-reports of EI, conflict communi-
cation patterns, and relationship satisfaction in 82 cohabiting het-
erosexual couples. The data showed that those high on EI were also 2.2. Variables and measures
more satisfied with their relationship and handled conflicts more
adaptively, thus indicating tenable ‘actor effects’. However, no sig- 1. Emotional intelligence, as ability, was assessed via the 141 item
nificant ‘partner’ effects were found, suggesting that an individual’s MSCEIT V 2.0 (Mayer, Salovey, & Caruso, 2002). The original
own level of EI has no reliable effect on the partner’s conflict reso- English version was translated into Hebrew by two bilingual
lution patterns or satisfaction with the relationship. psychologists and then back-translated to English by a third
A major concern regarding Smith et al.’s study is that all data bilingual psychologist, to insure correspondence between the
were based on self-report. Specifically, the trait measure of EI used English and Hebrew renditions. This measure was designed to
in their study does not directly tap people’s emotional abilities but measure the following four skills of EI: emotion perception;
rather people’s self-reported beliefs about their emotional abilities. using emotion to facilitate thought; understanding emotion;
Recent research indicates that self-report EI measures are highly and regulation (management) of emotion. All items have either
correlated with established measures of psychological well-being a multiple-choice format or 5-point rating scale format. Items
and personality (Brackett & Mayer, 2003) and may therefore con- were scored via consensus-based scores based on the present
tain a great deal of unwanted variance. Furthermore, the signifi- Israeli sample. The consensus scores reflect the proportion of
cant actor effects in the relationship between EI and marital adults in the sample who endorse each MSCEIT test item. Partic-
variables may be accounted for by common method variance in ipants were given credit for responses to the extent that their
both sets of ratings. answers matched those provided by the sample. The alpha reli-
ability of the total scale which was employed in the various
1.3. Goals and hypotheses analyses was .78 for both males and females. Because the
MSCEIT is a relatively new instrument, which has not been pub-
In view of the shortcomings of prior research, a major goal of lished in Hebrew yet, no national Hebrew norms are presently
this study was to assess actor and partner effect of ability-based available. Details on the validity of the MSCEIT can be found
EI with respect to marital communications and perceived marital in the technical manual (Mayer et al., 2002).
quality (satisfaction). By using an ability-based measure of EI, we 2. Patterns of marital conflict resolution were assessed via a Hebrew
hoped to be able to rule out the possibility of common construct rendition of the 35 item Communication Patterns Questionnaire
variance in the event that actor effects are observed. (CPQ; Christensen & Sullaway, 1984). The CPQ was designed to
Following from established connections between person vari- gauge the extent to which couples employ various types of inter-
ables and perceptions of couples’ conflict communication patterns action strategies when dealing with a relationship problem dur-
(Heaven, Smith, Prabhakar, Abraham, & Mete, 2006), it was antici- ing three phases of conflict. Each partner indicates what typically
pated that EI scores will be positively associated with perceptions occurs in this relationship on a 9-point scale according to which
of constructive conflict resolution patterns and negatively pattern would be very unlikely (1) to very likely (9) to occur
associated with perceptions of demanding and withdrawing and when attempting to deal with the problem. Three subscales were
avoidance and withholding patterns. Furthermore, based on the employed in the analyses: constructive communication patterns
assumption that EI is an enduring resource for adaptive marital rela- (k = 5; alphas = .72, .73 for males and females, respectively);
tionships, along with some prior research, EI was hypothesized to be mutual avoidance (k = 3; alphas = .77, .79, for males and females,
significantly related to the perceived quality of marital satisfaction. respectively); and total demand/withdraw (k = 6; alphas = .76,
Based on the assumption that a defining feature of an intimate rela- .82, for males and females, respectively). The CPQ has shown ade-
tionship is that one partner’s psychological competencies, states, quate reliability and validity as a measure of conflictual interac-
and actions have the capacity to influence those of the other partner tion patterns (Eldridge & Christiansen, 2002).
(Rushbult & van Lange, 1996), significant ‘actor’ as well as ‘partner’ 3. Perceived quality of marital relations (marital satisfaction) was
effects were examined in the nexus of EI and marital variables. assessed via a modified Hebrew adaptation of Norton’s Quality
In addition, we tested for the potential mediating role of conflict of Marriage Index (QMI, Norton, 1983). The QMI is a 6 item scale
resolution patterns in the relationship between EI and perceived asking spouses to rate, on a 9-point scale (1 = strongly disagree,
quality of marriage. Finally, we examined the relationship between 9 = strongly agree), the extent to which they agree with general
EI similarity and satisfaction, as few studies have taken the profile statements about their marriage (e.g., ‘‘We have a good mar-
similarity approach in the examination of the association between riage’’). Both members of each couple indicated how much they
280 M. Zeidner, I. Kloda / Personality and Individual Differences 54 (2013) 278–283

agreed with each statement. Raw scores were rescaled to T We first assessed the degree of homogeneity in perceived qual-
scores (M = 50, SD = 10) – across participants. Satisfactory alpha ity of marital relations among dyadic partners by testing the
scale reliability coefficients were found in this study for both unconditional means model for perceived marital well-being – as
husbands (alpha = .92) and wives (alpha = .97). The QMI has dependent variable. The intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) of
demonstrated high validity relating to satisfaction with mar- RI = .34 attested to a fair bit of nonindependence and clustering
riage in past research (Karney & Bradbury, 1997). of perceived marital well-being within dyads. Over a third of the
variance in marital well being is accounted for by between-dyadic
2.3. Procedure variance, supporting our decision to use analytic procedures that
consider the dependence among dyadic pairs in this study.
Both dyadic partners were present in the lab during assessment. As shown in Table 1, wives scored about .4 standard deviation
After signing of consent forms, each of the participants was seated above their partners on EI, as assessed by total mean MSCEIT scores.
in separate rooms and asked to complete an assessment battery Furthermore, as shown in Table 2, EI was significantly related to
which included personal information and an ability-based measure marital satisfaction mainly among men, but not among women.
of EI (MSCEIT). After a break, participants completed the second However, a test for dependent correlations using Steiger’s (1980)
part of the test battery, (comprised of ways of handling marital formula showed nonsignificant differences (z = 1.13, p = .26) among
conflict and marital quality). The couples were compensated 100 gender group correlations, also, in both husbands and wives, EI was
Israeli shekels (about 25 US dollars) for their participation. positively related to constructive communication and was nega-
tively related to dysfunctional strategies (demand/withdraw,
avoidance), although EI was more consistently correlated with
3. Results interactional variables among men than among women.

3.1. Preliminary analysis


3.2. Similarity analysis
Table 1 presents descriptive statistics for the husbands and wives
on all key variables of interest. Tables 2 and 3 present within- Adapting a couple-centered approach, we conducted an analysis
partner and cross-partner correlations on key variables, respectively. of spousal profile similarity on EI by computing the Pearson

Table 1
Descriptive statistics for key individual level variables in the study, by gender⁄.

Key variables Husbands Wives


M SD M SD ta db
*
MSCEIT total .42 .04 .44 .03 3.77 .38
Conflict resolution
Constructive 7.22 1.08 7.23 1.23 .05 .01
Mutual avoidance 2.85 1.59 2.71 1.59 .75 .07
Demand/withdraw 4.72 1.79 4.04 1.67 3.26* .33
Global marital quality 50.39 8.36 49.61 10.59 .65 .07
a
Calculated as t-tests for dependent measures.
b
d Scores for dependent measures were calculated by dividing the difference score by the standard deviation of the difference score.
*
p < .05.

Table 2
Intercorrelations among key variables in this study, by gender (men above diagonal, women below diagonal).

Key variables 1 2 3 4 5
1. Total MSCEIT == .24* .39** .31** .30**
2. Constructive conflict resolution .14 == .65** .53** .63**
3. Mutual avoidance .14 .62** == .43** .57**
4. Demand/withdraw .06 .58** .50** == .31**
5. Marital well-being .15 .61** .48** .28** ==

Note: Correlations for husbands and wives appear above and below the main diagonal, respectively.
*
p < .05.
**
p < .01.

Table 3
Between-dyad intercorrelations on key variables.

Husband’s a scores
1 2 3 4 5
Wives’ scores
1. EI .07 .05 .13 .05 . 01
2. Constructive conflict resolution . 03 .51** .26** .35** .37**
3. Mutual avoidance .09 .27** .29** .20* .33**
4 Total demand/withdraw .10 .33** .15 .32** .17
5. Perceived quality of marital well-being .03 .26** .14* .23* .32**

Note: Dyadic correlations on identical variables appear on the diagonal.


*
p < .05.
**
p < .01.
M. Zeidner, I. Kloda / Personality and Individual Differences 54 (2013) 278–283 281

product-moment correlations between husband’s and wife’s scores unconstrained, the model showed a reasonable fit, v2 (13) = 20.39,
on the 141 items comprising the MSCEIT, separately by dyad. These p = .09. When actor and partner paths for husbands and wives were
correlations, ranging from .13 to .64 (M = .44, SD = .13) purport- constrained to be equal, the model fit was v2 (15) = 20.97, p = .148,
edly capture the degree of profile similarity between each husband and did not worsen the fit significantly, v2diff ð2Þ ¼ :61 ns. The model
and wife pair on EI (Kenny et al., 2006). Overall, linear as well as fit indices were satisfactory, CFI = .97, NFI = .92, RMSEA = .06. How-
nonlinear attempts to model the relationship between spousal ever, when all actor and partner effects were constrained to be
similarity on EI and marital well-being were unsuccessful. The neg- equal, this yielded a poor fit, 2 (16) = 28.6, p = .03, significantly
ligible correlations observed failed to vindicate the assumption worsening the model, v2diff ð1Þ ¼ 7:63, p < .01. As shown in Fig. 1,
that couples with similar EI profile scores also enjoy greater mari- depicting the empirical model, actor effects were significant (.33
tal satisfaction. and .20 for husband and wife respectively) whereas the partner ef-
fects for both dyad partners (.03) were nonsignificant.
3.3. Modeling actor and partner effects
3.3.2. EI and marital quality
In this section we test the validity of a number of models testing An unconditional path model testing for actor and partner ef-
for actor and partner effects in the relationship between ability- fects in the relation between ability-based EI and marital quality
based EI, on one hand, and conflict resolution patterns and per- (represented as an observed variable) yielded a ‘just identified’
ceived marital satisfaction, on the other. Paths for within-spouse model, with 0 degrees of freedom. When both actor and partner ef-
(actor) effects and cross-spouse (partner) effects were modeled fects, in turn, were constrained to be equal for husbands and wives,
via AMOS 17. As suggested by Kenny et al. (2006), the general the conditional model showed a reasonable fit, 2 (2) = .37, p = .83.
strategy in specifying path models with a dyadic design is that The constrained model yielded acceptable values for all indices,
the model is drawn twice, once for each member of the dyad. We CFI = 1.0, NFI = .99, and RMSEA = 0. However, when all actor and
correlated all exogenous variables across dyad members, and we partner effects were constrained to be equal, this yielded a poor
also correlated the disturbances across dyad members, which al- fit, 2 (3) = 10.75, p = .01, significantly worsening the model,
lows for the nonindependence of the data. v2diff ð1Þ ¼ 9:28, p < .01. The parsimonious empirical model is de-
picted in Fig. 2. Inspection of the path coefficients in Fig. 2 show
3.3.1. EI and conflict resolution strategies that whereas the actor effects for both husbands (p = .28) and wi-
We first tested for actor and partner effects in the relationship ves (p = .19) were significant, the partner effects (.03 and .02
between EI and conflict resolution strategies. EI was represented for husbands and wives, respectively) were nonsignificant.
in the model as an observed variable, whereas conflict resolution
was represented as a latent variable, indexed by three CPQ scales 3.3.3. Mediating effects
(i.e., constructive conflict resolution, total demand/withdraw, and We also tested for the mediating effects of conflict resolution in
mutual avoidance). When paths for actor and partner effects were the relationship between EI and marital quality (EI ? Marital

Fig. 1. Empirical path analysis testing for actor and partner effects in the relationship between ei and conflict resolution strategies, controlling for verbal ability.
282 M. Zeidner, I. Kloda / Personality and Individual Differences 54 (2013) 278–283

Fig. 2. Empirical path analysis testing for actor and partner effects in the relationship between ei and quality of marital relations.

conflict ? Marital Quality), with EI and marital quality represented (2008) employing a self-report trait measure of EI. This lends con-
in the dyadic model as observed variables and marital conflict siderable validity and generalizability for the actor effects of EI in
represented as a latent variable. Only actor effects were included relation to marital conflict. Furthermore, these data showing that
in the model. The fit of the model to the data was not satisfactory, marital partners are moderately to strongly correlated on conflict
2 (31) = 85.55, p < .001, and the fit indices were poor, CFI = .83, resolution strategies are congenial to much of prior research on
NFI = .77, RMSEA = .14. Despite the poor fit of this model, the total this topic (Christensen & Shenk, 1991; Smith et al., 2008).
and indirect paths from EI to marital quality are suggestive of
mediating effects. Thus, when comparing the total effects of EI
on marital quality for husbands (p = .31) and (p = .10) with the 4.2. EI and marital satisfaction
indirect paths (p = .31 for husbands and p = .10 for wives), this
suggests that EI works entirely through conflict resolution in The dyadic analyses reported above supported the hypothesis
impacting marital quality. Furthermore, when the path model stating that EI is significantly related to perceived quality of mari-
was run with a direct path from EI to marital satisfaction, the tal relations. Specifically, significant actor effects-but no partner ef-
model, the direct effects were negligible, further supporting the fects – were found for ability-based EI in its association with
notion of mediation. However, further research is needed to shed marital quality, with dyadic partners higher on EI also rating their
light on this issue. own levels of marital satisfaction more favorably. Thus, these data
are consistent with prior research employing trait measures of EI
4. Discussion reporting significant actor – but no partner – effects in the relation-
ship between EI and marital outcome factors (Brackett, Warner, &
This study investigated the relation of EI to quality of marital Bosco, 2005; Smith et al., 2008).
communication and perceived marital quality employing a stan- A modeling of the relationship between spousal similarity on EI
dard matched-pairs dyadic design. and marital satisfaction, as dependent variable, failed to vindicate
the assumption that couples with similar EI profile scores also en-
4.1. EI and adaptive conflict resolution joy greater marital well-being. Although similarity in personality
has been shown to be correlated moderately to strongly in per-
Under the assumption that EI is an enduring resource for adap- ceived quality of relationship (Neyer &Voigt, 2004), our data, using
tive marital relationships, EI was hypothesized to be significantly an ability-based measure, failed to vindicate this.
related to conflict resolution patterns. Overall, the data supported Dyadic partners who are high on EI may experience less conflict
the first hypothesis, with both correlational and path-analytic and more satisfaction in their marriage for a number of reasons.
analyses of the data showing that EI was positively related to con- First, they may react to and interpret behaviors of their partners
structive conflict resolution strategies and negatively related to in a positive light and be primed to minimize relationship difficul-
dysfunctional strategies. Thus, a person’s EI was positively related ties. In addition, high EI may evoke positive behaviors from their
to a perception of more constructive communication, less demand/ partner that contribute to their own satisfaction. This may also re-
withdrawal pattern, and less avoidance and withholding during flect a rose-colored glass effect of high EI on marital communica-
various phases of conflict resolution. However, no partner effects tion patterns and marital satisfaction.
were found. Overall, these data are consistent with common claims With respect to gender differences, our study was consistent
in the literature that when people can perceive and understand with much of prior research using the MSCEIT indicating signifi-
their partner’s thoughts and feelings, they may also be more be- cant, though modest, gender differences on EI (e.g., Bracket et al.,
nign or positive in their communication patterns, particularly in 2005). Consistent with prior research, only a negligible correlation
times of conflict (Schutte et al., 2001). between dyadic partners on ability-based EI was found (cf. Smith
Also, these data showing that EI was positively associated with et al., 2008). Also, these data did not find significant mean gender
perception of constructive communication patterns and negatively differences in marital satisfaction. These findings are in contrast to
associated with perceptions of dysfunctional conflict communica- some research (Robins, Caspi, & Moffitt, 2000) reporting that men
tion patterns were consistent with those reported by Smith et al. were more satisfied in their relationship outcomes than women.
M. Zeidner, I. Kloda / Personality and Individual Differences 54 (2013) 278–283 283

The current study has a number of limitations. First, this Fitness, J. (2001). Emotional intelligence and intimate relationships. In J. Ciarrochi, J.
P. Forgas, & J. D. Mayer (Eds.), Emotional intelligence in everyday life (pp. 98–112).
research is entirely cross-sectional and therefore it is difficult to
PA: Psychology Press.
untangle cause and effect relationships here. Second, couples Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J., & Rapson, R. (1994). Emotional contagion. New York:
partaking in this research were newly married and dyadic partners Cambridge University Press.
may simply not have been together long enough for ‘partner’ Heaven, P. C. L., Smith, L., Prabhakar, S. M., Abraham, J., & Mete, M. E. (2006).
Personality and conflict communication patterns in cohabiting couples. Journal
effects to manifest themselves. Third, we do not provide data based of Research in Personality, 40, 829–840.
on in situ observations of marital transactions and have relied on Horowitz, A. V., McLaughlin, J., & White, H. R. (1998). How the negative and positive
self-reports of marital transactions and satisfaction. Future aspects of partner relationships affect the mental health of young married
people. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 39, 124–136.
research would profit from gathering such data using observa- Karney, B. R., & Bradbury, T. N. (1995). The longitudinal course of marital quality
tional techniques and micro-analytical procedures. and stability: A review of theory, method, and research. Psychological Bulletin,
Keeping the above constraints in mind, the data provide evi- 118, 3–34.
Karney, B. R., & Bradbury, T. N. (1997). Neuroticism, marital interaction and the
dence in favor of the important role of EI in the capacity of actor trajectory of marital satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72,
effects – but not partner effects. Although marital partners are 1075–1092.
intertwined in a close personal relationship with another person, Kenny, D. A., Kashy, D. A., & Cook, W. L. (2006). Dyadic data analysis. New York:
Guilford Press.
it is their own level of EI that shapes their view of the relationship Lopes, P. N., Brackett, M. A., Nezlek, J., Schutz, A., Sellin, I., & Salovey, P. (2004).
and that drives much of their subjective experience of the relation- Emotional intelligence and social interaction. Personality and Social Psychology
ship. Overall, these findings are consistent with prior studies show- Bulletin, 30, 1018–1034.
Luo, S., Chen, H., Yue, G., Zhang, G., Zhaoyang, R., & Xu, D. (2008). Predicting marital
ing that an individual’s level of relationship is primarily a function
satisfaction from self, partner, and couple characteristics: Is it me, you, or us?
of his or her own personal characteristics (i.e., EI) rather than the Journal of Personality, 76, 1231–1265.
characteristics of the partner (Neyer & Voigt, 2004).This pattern Luo, S., & Klohnen, E. C. (2005). Assortative mating and marital quality in
is also consistent with a review by Luo and Klohnen (2005) report- newlyweds: A couple-centered approach. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 88, 304–326.
ing that while both self and partner effects are substantial, self Mayer, J. D., Caruso, D. R., & Salovey, P. (1999). Emotional intelligence meets
characteristics contribute more to marital communication and sat- traditional standards for an intelligence. Intelligence, 27, 267–298.
isfaction than partner characteristics. Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (2002). Mayer–Salovey–Caruso Emotional
Intelligence Test (MSCEIT) user’s manual. Toronto, Canada: Multi-Health Systems.
In Conclusion when assessing the hypothesis focusing on the Murray, S. L., Holmes, J. C., & Griffin, D. W. (1996). The self-fulfilling nature of
predicted relationship between EI and marital communication pat- positive illusions in romantic relationships: Love is not blind, but prescient.
terns and satisfaction in dyadic couples, the non-significant part- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 1155–1180.
Navran, L. (1967). Communication and adjustment in marriage. Family Process, 6,
ner effects should probably be weighted more heavily than the 173–180.
significant actor effects observed. Indeed, current conceptualiza- Neyer, F. J., & Voigt, D. (2004). Personality and social network effects on romantic
tions suggest that one’s EI should impact mainly on one’s partner’s relationships: A dyadic approach. European Journal of Personality, 18,
279–299.
affective relations rather than one’s own – and this was not found. Noller, P., Feeney, J. A. (2002). Communication, relationship concerns, and
Because the critical effects from a theoretical perspective are part- satisfaction in early marriage. In A. L. Vangelisti, H. T. Reis, M. A. Fitzpatrick
ner rather than actor effects, these data offer only limited and par- (Eds.), Advances in personal relationships. Stability and change in relationships (pp.
129–155). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
tial support for the commonly held claim that EI is a significant
Norton, R. (1983). Measuring marital quality: A critical look at the dependent
predictor of close personal relationships. variable. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 45, 141–151.
Perez, J. C., Petrides, K. V., & Furnham, A. (2005). Measuring trait emotional
References intelligence. In R. Schulze & R. D. Roberts (Eds.), Emotional intelligence. An
international handbook (pp. 181–201). Cambridge, MA: Hogrefe & Huber
Publishers.
Brackett, M. A., & Mayer, J. D. (2003). Convergent, discriminant, and incremental Robins, R. W., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2000). Two personalities, one relationship:
validity of competing measures of emotional intelligence. Personality and Social Both partners’ personality traits shape the quality of their relationship. Journal
Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1147–1158. of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 251–259.
Brackett, M. A., Rivers, S. E., Shiffman, S., Lerner, N., & Salovey, P. (2006). Relating Rushbult, C. E., & van Lange, P. A. M. (1996). Interdependence processes. In E. T.
emotional abilities to social functioning: A comparison of self-report and Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles
performance measures of emotional intelligence. Journal of Personality and (pp. 564–596). New York: Guilford.
Social Psychology, 91, 780–795. Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., Bobik, C., Coston, T. D., Greeson, C., Jedlicka, C., et al.
Brackett, M. A., Warner, R. M., & Bosco, J. S. (2005). Emotional intelligence and (2001). Emotional intelligence and interpersonal relations. Journal of Social
relationship quality among couples. Personal Relationships, 12, 197–212. Psychology, 141, 523–536.
Christensen, A., & Shenk, J. L. (1991). Communication, conflict and psychological Smith, L., Ciarrochi, J., & Heaven, P. C. L. (2008). The stability and change of trait
distance in nondistressed, clinic and divorcing couples. Journal of Consulting and emotional intelligence, conflict communication patterns, and relationship
Clinical Psychology, 59, 458–463. satisfaction: A one-year longitudinal study. Personality and Individual
Christensen, A., & Sullaway, M. (1984). Communication patterns questionnaire. Differences, 45, 738–743.
Unpublished manuscript, University of California, Department of Psychology, Steiger, J. H. (1980). Tests for comparing elements of a correlation matrix.
Los Angeles. Psychological Bulletin, 87, 245–251.
Eldrige, K. A., & Christensen, A. (2002). Demand-withdraw communication during Zeidner, M., Matthews, G., & Robert, R. (2009). What we know about emotional
couple conflict. In P. Noller & J. A. Feeney (Eds.), Understanding marriage: intelligence. How it affects learning, work, relationships, and our mental health.
Developments in the study of couple interaction (pp. 289–322). Cambridge, Cambridge: MIT Press.
England: Cambridge University Press.

Você também pode gostar