Você está na página 1de 14

Spanish Journal of Psychology (2013), 16, eX, 1–14.

© Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid


doi:10.1017/sjp.2013.47

Dimensionality of the Inventory of Parent and Peer


Attachment: Evaluation with the Spanish Version

Miriam Gallarin and Itziar Alonso-Arbiol

Universidad del País Vasco UPV/EHU (Spain)

Abstract. Three studies aimed at developing the Spanish version of the Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment (IPPA;
Armsden & Greenberg, 1987) and at analyzing its factor structure are reported. In Study 1, the Spanish translation of
items and their content analysis was carried out via cognitive interviews. In Study 2, the three-factor structure proposed
by authors was tested in a sample of 417 adolescents (270 girls and 147 boys) using confirmatory analysis, and indexes
showed a suboptimal fit. A principal component analysis yielded one-dimensional structures in father, mother and peer
versions, explaining 54.3%, 50.8%, and 50.8% of the variance respectively. On the basis of the factor loadings and of the
item-total correlations, a shortened version of the inventory was created. Convergent validity was tested with measures
of family climate, parenting socialization and self-esteem. In Study 3, the one-dimension structure was confirmed in
a new sample of 604 adolescents (335 girls and 269 boys). Based on all factor analyses and convergent validity indices
obtained, we conclude that the Spanish IPPA questionnaire reliably and validly assesses adolescent attachment using
only a dimension of attachment security.

Received 29 July 2011; Revised 31 January 2012; Accepted 24 May 2012

Keywords: attachment, adolescence, test adaptation, factor structure.

Since Bowlby’s (1969) first formulation of attachment In Spain, there are few valid and reliable assessment
theory in childhood, much research has been conducted tools of the quality of emotional bonds. One of them is
in different developmental stages, and so nowadays it the Parental Bonding Instrument (Parker, Tupling, &
is an unquestionable fact that individuals of all ages Brown, 1979; Spanish version: Ballús-Creus, 1991),
benefit from the development of secure bonds with which assesses the perception of parents’ behavior
other significant ones (Allen, 2008; Mikulincer & Shaver, and attitudes through Care (Affect vs. Rejection) and
2008). Specifically in adolescence, the perception of a Overprotection (Overprotection vs. Stimulation of
secure attachment with parents has been associated Autonomy) dimensions. Although this questionnaire
with higher levels of self-esteem (Gomez & McLaren, enables the assessment of attachment towards parents,
2007), higher emotional and social competence (Laible, it is not applicable for the assessment of the quality of
2007), higher emotion regulation (Allen & Miga, 2010), the bonds developed towards friends/peers. There are
and lower levels of aggressiveness and shyness (Dykas, also some studies in which instruments targeted to assess
Ziv, & Cassidy, 2008). Individuation and identity the attachment towards friends or peers have been used
formulation processes also take place during adoles- (e.g., Sanchez-Queija & Oliva, 2003). Unfortunately, as
cence; adolescents start becoming differentiated relevant and informative the studies have revealed,
from their parents and being closer to their peers these questionnaires have not undergone a thorough
(Shaffer & Kipp, 2007). Quality of both types of rela- psychometric analysis, and neither do they inform
tionships, with parents and with peers, will be relevant about the adaptation process carried out before the
for adolescents’ development and adjustment (Allen, administration. Therefore, no valid and reliable mea-
2008). sure of adolescents’ attachment to parents and peers
are available in Spanish.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to One of the most widely used methods for the assess-
Miriam Gallarin or Itziar Alonso-Arbiol. Faculty of Psychology,
ment of attachment in adolescence is the Inventory
Universidad del País Vasco UPV/EHU. Avenida de Tolosa 70. E-20018
San Sebastian (Spain).
of Parent and Peer Attachment (IPPA), developed by
E-mail: miriam.gallarin@ehu.es or itziar.alonso@ehu.es Armsden and Greenberg (1987). Even though IPPA
This study was supported by a pre-doctoral grant awarded by the adequately incorporates the theoretical underpinning
Gobierno Vasco (BFI06/211) to the first author under the supervision of attachment theory and, therefore, evaluates the
of the second author, and by a grant from the Research Bureau of the
perceived bonds with parents and peers in adolescence
Universidad del País Vasco UPV/EHU (General Funding for Research
Groups, GIU08/09). Parts of this article were presented at the 12th
in a valid way, its factor structure still remains unclear,
Biennial Conference of the European Association for Research on with one-factor (e.g., Günaydin, Selçuk, Sümer, & Uysal,
Adolescence, May 2010, Vilnius, Lithuania. 2005), two-factor (e.g., Johnson, Ketring, & Abshire,
2 M. Gallarin and I. Alonso-Arbiol

2003), and three-factor (e.g., San Martini, Zavattini, & depression and social anxiety (Papini, Roggman, &
Ronconi, 2009) structures having been observed. Thus, Anderson, 1991), and overprotection (Pardo et al., 2006).
in this study we responded to the need to: (a) develop Nevertheless, a doubt cast over the structural fea-
the Spanish version of the IPPA, since there is not a tures of IPPA since the existence of the three factors
single measure for the assessment of the attachment suggested by Armsden and Greenberg (1987) has not
toward parents and peers in adolescence; and (b) eluci- been sufficiently corroborated hitherto. The authors
date whether its factor structure is actually three- themselves acknowledged the need to carry out a fur-
dimensional, as proposed by the authors, or whether ther in-depth study of the structure of the question-
other underlying structures would apply instead. For naire because they observed that, due to the high
that purpose, we first adapted IPPA into Spanish, inter-correlations among the three subscales, the
and afterwards we analyzed the structure of IPPA in independence of these subscales may not be clear
a series of studies with different community samples of (M. Greenberg, personal communication, August 23,
adolescents. 2008). Moreover, the suggestion of taking a composite
of the three factors for the calculation of the total score
Content Validity of IPPA of attachment also helps to call into question the inde-
pendence of these factors.
Based on Bowlby’s (1969) seminal work, Armsden and
Greenberg (1987) suggested that it is possible to eval-
Factorial Structure of IPPA in Different Versions
uate internal working models of attachment figures. It
would be necessary to evaluate the core of the internal- Over the last 20 years, the IPPA has been adapted into
ized experience of attachment. Two elements are men- other cultures in several studies, enabling the suitability
tioned: One, the positive affective/cognitive experience of the three-factor model proposed by Armsden and
deriving from the trust in attachment figures (i.e., in Greenberg (1987) to be examined. For instance, IPPA
their accessibility and response); two, the negative has been adapted into Spanish in a sample of Colombian
affective/cognitive experience deriving from the anger students (Pardo et al., 2006). In that study, the proposed
and loss of hope due to the lack of response or incon- three-factor structure seemed to emerge when an analysis
sistency of responses from attachment figures. Capturing of principal components was conducted. However, the
these elements, Armsden and Greenberg established subscale of alienation had low reliability indexes and
that the quality of perceived attachment towards par- the loadings for each factor are not offered. Moreover,
ents and peers may be inferred with IPPA from the since different possible structures are not compared,
scores of three independent factors referred to as ‘com- nor even examined, it is unclear as to what extent the
munication’, ‘trust’, and ‘alienation’. Although in an observed factors structure is such because it is the only
initial version of the questionnaire, both parents were one analyzed.
assessed together with a total of 28 items, in the revised The underlying structure of IPPA has been also exam-
version of IPPA the authors divided the parents’ scale ined in several studies in Italy. Conducted analyses,
into two: mother version and father version. Then both however, do not enable unequivocal conclusions to be
scales were reduced to 25 items each, with equal drawn. For example, San Martini and colleagues’ (2009)
wording but with the only exception of the parental findings show, on the one hand, that the exploratory
figure. factor analysis conducted in an initial stage suggests a
Since the development of IPPA, the studies conducted one-factor structure for each scale (father, mother, and
to evaluate its construct validity have suggested that peer versions). However, on the other hand, the fit
the questionnaire satisfactorily taps the contents of at- indexes support the three-factor structure proposed by
tachment bonds. Armsden and Greenberg (1987) found the authors of the original American version, although
that IPPA correlated positively with levels of family the alienation factor shows low reliability indexes.
self-concept, support, expressivity and family cohesion, Another study seems to identify one only factor for
and negatively with conflict and control. They also each scale (parents and peers) in a sample of 1,000
observed that IPPA scores could predict self-esteem, Italian adolescents (Baiocco, Laghi, & Paola, 2009).
life satisfaction, depression, anxiety, resentment and Recently, also in Italy, Pace, San Martini, and Zavattini
alienation. In more recent studies, other authors have (2011) have found that the three-factor structure has
also found empirical support for similar relationships. the best fit, although the three dimensions are strongly
IPPA scores have been positively associated with self- interrelated.
esteem (Gomez & McLaren, 2007), expressivity and Vignoli and Mallet (2004) adapted the original version
family cohesion (Gullone & Robinson, 2005), and care of 28 items in a sample of French adolescents. Using an
(Pardo, Pineda, Carrillo, & Castro, 2006); and nega- analysis of principal components, they selected the
tively associated with aggression (Gomez & McLaren), items that loaded highly in their theoretically expected
Dimensionality of Ippa 3

factor in both father and in mother versions. After Study 1


doing this, they tested to see whether the selection of
The aim of this study was to evaluate the content valid-
14 items actually fit the three-factor structure with a
ity of the IPPA questionnaire and its understandability
confirmatory analysis in a different sample.
in the target sample. First, we define the process
Johnson and associates’ (2003) work also deserves a
involving the translation of items. Second, we describe
mention. These authors developed from IPPA a version
the examination of content validity.
to be completed by the parents themselves (referred to
The translation of IPPA into Spanish was carried out
as Revised Inventory of Parent Attachment or R-IPA)
independently by a team made up four people, including
in order to gain a wider, or circular, perspective of rela-
two linguists and two psychologists who are familiar-
tionships between adolescents and their parents. From
ized with research on emotional bonding. This first step
their results, they concluded that the original three-
of the adaptation process was conducted using a
factor factor structure does not fit with data of R-IPA.
back-translation design, and in accordance with the
By using a confirmatory analysis with the IPPA, they
milestones suggested by Balluerka, Gorostiaga, Alonso-
observed that its structure did not fit the three-factor
Arbiol, and Haranburu (2007). Each of the 75 items of
model either. After conducting exploratory factor ana-
the original version —25 items on each scale (mother,
lyses (both with R-IPA and IPPA), the authors concluded
father, and peers) —was translated into Spanish inde-
that the underlying structure is two-dimensional
(i.e., on the one hand, alienation, and on the other hand, a pendently by two people (a psychologist and a linguist).
positive aspect linking both trust and communication). Once both translations had been compared and ana-
Finally, two efforts made to give rise to a shortened lyzed, an agreed version was obtained for each item.
version of the questionnaire may be mentioned. Raja, Stemming from this version, a further two members of
McGee, and Stanton (1992) reduced each scale of IPPA the translation team (a psychologist and a linguist)
(parents and peers) to 12 items. Reliability indexes of independently translated into Spanish the items of the
two factors of this short version —of communication Spanish version back to English and obtained an agreed
and trust— are adequate; those of alienation factors are version of it. Finally, all participants in the process
not. In a more recent study with Turkish students, compared each item of the original version and the
Günaydin and colleagues (2005) could not replicate the inversely adapted English version in order to exam-
three-dimensional structure proposed by Raja and col- ine the possible non-equivalence in meaning, so as
laborators, because the results pointed out a one-factor to make any modifications accordingly in the final
structure of the questionnaire. This one-factor struc- Spanish version later.
ture has also been mentioned as the most optimal in a Cognitive interviews were conducted for the exami-
work with the long IPPA version with a Spanish sam- nation of content analysis of items. With the aim of
ple of Basque-speaking adolescents —geographically evaluating the understanding level of items of the
closer to the samples examined in our study— (Alonso- Spanish adapted version, 24 adolescents of both gen-
Arbiol et al., in press). ders (not using randomization) were drawn from a
In short, the evidence as to which attachment school selected and they answered the 75 items of IPPA.
elements are covered by IPPA is still inconclusive. The After completing the questionnaire, they also answered
three-dimensional structure had to be corroborated some questions in order to analyze the meaning they
before further research in adolescence attachment with had derived from some words and expression in some
this instrument, understood as fitting three attachment items that may be confusing. Some of the questions are
features, might be done. In this work, we conducted as follows: ‘Would you use another word (or words)
several studies with two aims in mind. First and fore- instead of the word “disgustado/a” (distressed) in this
most, we tried to pinpoint the underlying factor struc- sentence? (If so, which one?)’, ‘Could you think of
ture of the IPPA questionnaire. A second aim involved another way in which you would say “tiene en cuenta”
the development of its Spanish version by analyzing (take into account) in this sentence?’, ‘What do you
its psychometric properties. We first adapted the ques- understand by “siento enfado” (I feel angry) in this sen-
tionnaire into Spanish and analyzed content validity of tence?’, or ‘What do you think the sentence “Mi madre
items using cognitive interviews. In a second study, first no me presta mucha atención” (My mother does not pay
we examined the three-dimensional structure of IPPA, too much attention to me) means?’ Participants were
and, since the factor structure was not confirmed, in a also given the opportunity to say that they did not clearly
second phase we used principal component analysis understand the meaning of a word. There were two
yielding a one-dimensional structure. Convergent valid- aims in this phase: (a) to see whether the proposed
ity and internal consistency of the scale were also exam- items of the Spanish version keep the semantic content
ined. Finally, a third study was conducted in order to of the original English version; and (b) to check whether
corroborate the structure observed in the second study. items fit the understanding level of the sample in which
4 M. Gallarin and I. Alonso-Arbiol

they would be used. To ensure that both objectives (true-false), grouped into 10 subscales of 9 items
were met, the students who participated in this pilot each; they tap three main dimensions: Relationships,
phase were at an academic level lower (i.e., 14–15 years Personal Development or Personal Growth, and
old) than the ones who would be included in the sam- Family Structure or Organization. In this study, we
ple of the subsequent empirical phase. only used the Relationships dimension, whose sub-
The first author, a researcher expert in adolescent scales are: cohesion, expressiveness and conflict. Based
attachment, analyzed the responses given by the par- on a previous factor analysis, we decided to unify the
ticipants to the questions designed for clarification of subscales of cohesion and expressiveness, because such
items. The qualitative analysis of the content of these analysis did not show the existence of three factors
answers led to the conclusion that the understanding in our sample. Therefore, a composite was created
level was adequate, and the semantic level of items were for communication and family cohesion (Cohesion/
according to attachment theory postulates. Therefore, Expressiveness) from the total sum of 18 items
all items remained unchanged from the first formula- (e.g., “In my family there is a strong feeling of union”),
tion agreed by members of the translation team. and the score for Conflict was calculated from the total
sum of 9 items (e.g., “In our family we quarrel a lot”).
Study 2 In our study Cronbach’s alphas were acceptable: .80 for
Cohesion/Expressiveness; and .62 for Conflict.
This study has two different phases. The aim of the Parental Bonding Instrument (PBI; Parker et al., 1979;
first phase was to test the three-dimensional structure in its Spanish version, Ballús-Creus, 1991). This self-
proposed initially by authors of the IPPA (Armsden & report instrument assesses the perception children
Greenberg, 1987). Since the three-dimensional struc- have of their parents’ behavior and attitudes to them in
ture was not corroborated, the aims of the second childhood and adolescence. The adolescent is asked
phase were: (a) to analyze the dimensional structure about the recollection s/he has about her/his relation-
of IPPA-S using an exploratory factor analysis; ships with her/his father and her/his mother during
(b) to examine the internal consistency of the possible childhood with 25 Likert-type items ranging from
subscale(s); and (c) to analyze the construct validity 1 ( never) to 5 ( always). Two dimensions derive from
of the questionnaire based on the extracted factor(s). this: Care (Affect vs. Rejection) and Overprotection
The Spanish version described in Study 1 was used for (Overprotection vs. Stimulation of Autonomy). Care
these purposes. includes 12 items related to care, love, closeness, and
perceived attention (e.g. “S/he often smiled at me”),
Method whereas Overprotection refers to the perception of
Participants control related to the lack of fostering individuation
with 13 items (e.g. “S/he tried to make me dependent
The sample consisted of 417 secondary school students on her/him”). In our study Cronbach’s alphas were all
and of first four semesters at university (270 girls and acceptable: .90 and .83 for Care (father and mother
147 boys). Mean age (in years) was 17.9 (SD = 1.64). respectively), and .82 and .76 for Overprotection (father
Although ethnicity was not recorded, not more than and mother respectively).
5% of the sample was presumably different from the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES; Rosenberg, 1989;
mainstream (Caucasian) Spanish group. in its Spanish version, Martin-Albo, Núñez, Navarro, &
Grijalvo, 2007). The RSES is a self-report instrument
Procedure made up of 10 items that assesses self-respect or accep-
tance. The scale rated on a 4-point Likert-type scale
Students filled in the Spanish IPPA (which we will call
ranging from 1 (totally disagree) to 4 (totally agree) in
IPPA-S) in their classrooms during class time. They
which there are 5 positively worded items (e.g. “I feel
were informed about the aim of the study, the way
that I have a number of good qualities”), and 5 nega-
they had to answer the questions, and their free choice
tively worded items (e.g. “All in all, I am inclined to
of taking part in the study, as it is specified in ethical
feel that I am a failure”). The scale gives a total score
regulations of the Spanish Psychologists’ Board (COP).
for self-esteem. In our study Cronbach’s alpha was
good: .80.
Instruments

Family Environmental Scale (FES; Moos & Trichett, Results


1974; in its Spanish version, Moos, Moos, & Trichett,
Phase 1
1984). This self-report instrument assesses the main
socio-environmental features of family. The com- Structural equation modeling was used to test the
plete scale comprises a total of 90 dichotomous items confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) model with AMOS
Dimensionality of Ippa 5

(Arbuckle, 2008). The fit of the model was tested in a Exploratory Factor Analysis and Internal
multiple-group structural equation model, using max- Consistency
imum likelihood estimates, and where girls and boys
For the examination of the general structure of IPPA-S,
were taken as two different groups. An unconstrained
a principal component analysis with oblimin rotation
model in which all parameters were allowed to vary
was conducted. Prior to the analysis, we calculated the
formed the baseline. From here, subsequent analyses
Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) index as well as a sphe-
were made by constraining parameters to being in-
ricity test for each scale (mother, father and peers). All
variant so that the most parsimonious model that still
KMO indexes were .95 which can be considered good.
showed an acceptable fit was chosen.
Bartlett test was statistically significant for all scales:
The sample size of this study was large enough so
χ2(120) = 4093.55 p < .0001, in mother scale; χ2(120) =
as to ensure that the conventional chi-square statistic
4488.89 p < .0001, in father scale; and χ2(120) = 3870.27
would not appear as an optimal good index for the
p < .0001, in peer scale.
model (Schumacker & Lomax, 2004). Instead, other
In order to establish how many factors to extract,
indexes to test the goodness of fit of the model were
two criteria were considered: (a) The sedimentation
used. The relative chi-square is the chi-square fit index
graph with its scree-plot was examined; and (b) the
divided by its degrees of freedom (χ2/df); here values of
semantic and psychological content were taken into
three or less are considered as indications of a good
account in the search for clearly interpretable factors.
fit (Kline, 1998). Based on Hoyle’s (1995) suggestion,
We considered these criteria for three-, two- and one-
adjusted goodness of fit index (AGFI), Tucker-Lewis
factor solutions, and we concluded the one-factor solu-
index (TLI), and comparative fit index (CFI) fit-indexes
tion to be the most optimal. The solution for three
greater than .90 would be considered as indicating a good
factors was not easily interpretable because there were
fit. Values of root-mean-square error of approximation
many cross-loadings with most items loading high not
(RMSEA) lower than .05, and values of root-mean-square
just in one factor, but in two or even three. On the other
(RMR) residual lower than .08 would be acceptable
hand, although the two-factor solution showed good
(Byrne, 2010). None of the fitness indexes were ade-
factor loadings, after evaluating each item in each factor,
quate. Thus, the three-dimensional structure proposed
we realized that the two-factor solution involved
by the authors seems to not adequately fit the data (see
separating positive from negative items, more than
Table 1), in any of the three scales (mother, father and
distinguishing between different attachment-related
peers).
concepts. Therefore, we decided to take the one-factor
as the most optimal solution, also supported by the
Phase 2
scree-plot figure.
This second phase derives from the impossibility of Taking into account that the one-factor structure was
obtaining an optimal fit for the three-dimensional factor the most optimal (see Table 2 to see factor loadings of
structure of the questionnaire. Therefore, we analyzed 25 items in one factor structure), we decided to reduce
the dimensional structure of IPPA-S with an explor- it in order to optimize the questionnaire (in terms of
atory factor analytic strategy, examined the internal applicability and reliability). We considered these
consistency of the subscale(s), and analyzed the con- criteria in order to decide which items will be retained
struct validity of the questionnaire. for a shorter version: (a) items with |.50| or higher
Construct validity was examined calculating Pearson loading in the factor; and (b) items with |.50| or higher
correlations with three different measures tapping scores in the corrected element-total correlation were
constructs related to attachment. Specifically, family kept.
climate, parental bonding and self-esteem variables Thus, some items were eliminated for loading lower
were used for this analysis. Regarding the links of than |.50|: #8 (“Talking over my problems with my
our instrument to family climate, we expected to find: mother/father makes me feel ashamed or foolish”), #9
(a) moderate to high positive correlations with Cohesion (“My mother/father expects too much from me”), and
and Expressiveness dimensions of family climate, #23 (“My mother/father doesn’t understand what I’m
and (b) moderate to high negative correlations with going through these days”). The following items were
Conflict dimension of family climate. As for the rela- dropped for having scores lower than |.50| in the
tionship with parental bonding, we hypothesized corrected element-total correlation: #7 (“My mother/
moderate to high positive correlations between Care father can tell when I’m upset about something”), #10
dimension and IPPA, and moderate to high negative (“I get upset easily around my mother/father”), #11
correlations between Overprotection dimension and (“I get upset a lot more than my mother/father knows
IPPA. Finally, we expected to find positive correlation about”), #14 (“My mother/father has her/his own
with self-esteem. problems, so I don’t bother her/him with mine”), #17
6

Table 1. Fit Indexes for the Three Subscales of IPPA-S in the Three-Dimensional Model
M. Gallarin and I. Alonso-Arbiol

Scales Model no. Model description χ² df χ²/df AGFI TLI RMSEA CFI RMR AIC Nested models Δχ ² Δdf p level

IPPA-M
1 Unconstrained 1276.3 544 2.35 .759 .843 .056 .857 .075 1488.28
2 Measurement weights 1305.8 566 2.31 .765 .847 .056 .856 .085 1473.81 2-1 29.5 22 .001
3 Structural covariances 1317.0 572 2.30 .766 .848 .055 .855 .124 1473.03 3-2 11.2 6 .001
4 Measurement residuals 1342.4 597 2.25 .774 .854 .054 .855 .125 1448.35 4-3 25.4 25 .001
IPPA-F
1 Unconstrained 1289.2 544 2.37 .747 .864 .057 .876 .091 1501.17
2 Measurement weights 1326.8 566 2.34 .751 .866 .057 .874 .104 1494.75 2-1 37.6 22 .001
3 Structural covariances 1342.4 572 2.35 .753 .866 .057 .872 .132 1498.38 3-2 15.6 6 .001
4 Measurement residuals 1382.6 597 2.32 .758 .869 .056 .870 .136 1488.61 4-3 40.2 25 .001
IPPA-P
1 Unconstrained 1142.9 544 2.10 .790 .861 .051 .874 .060 1354.87
2 Measurement weights 1162.4 566 2.05 .795 .867 .050 .875 .065 1330.44 2-1 19.5 22 .001
3 Structural covariances 1172.1 572 2.05 .794 .868 .049 .874 .069 1328.07 3-2 9.7 6 .001
4 Measurement residuals 1306.5 597 2.19 .779 .850 .053 .851 .073 1412.46 4-3 134.4 25 .001

Note: IPPA-M = IPPA Mother version; IPPA-F = IPPA Father version; IPPA-P = IPPA Peer version.
Dimensionality of Ippa 7

Table 2. Factor Loadings of Mother, Father and Peer Scale Items Analysis of Construct Validity
from the Principal Component Analysis
Construct validity of IPPA-S was examined through the
correlations with the Relationships dimension of FES,
Item IPPA-M IPPA-F Item IPPA-P
with PBI, and with RSES, with a distinction being drawn
1 (T) .73 .75 1 (C)* .44 between girls and boys in all cases. All correlations are
2 (T) .74 .78 2 (C) .66 shown in Table 5. As expected, scores of mother and
3 (R/T) −.62 −.64 3 (C) .70 father scales of IPPA-S are highly and positively corre-
4 (T) .68 .71 4 (A)* −.37 lated with the Cohesion/Expressiveness subscale of
5 (C) .62 .61 5 (R/T)* −.54 FES, whereas the size of the correlations are medium
6 (R/C) −.62 −.62 6 (T) .73 and negative with the Conflict subscale. All these correla-
7 (C)* .51 .73 7 (C) .68 tions were higher for girls than for boys. Correlations
8 (A)* −.42 −.49 8 (T) .64 of these FES subscales with the peer version are low,
9 (R/T)* −.19 −.23 9 (A)* .10
and almost non-existent in boys.
10 (A)* −.64 −.68 10 (A)* −.36
On the other hand, and also according to what could
11 (A)* −.52 −.61 11 (A)* −.59
be expected, both the mother and father Care dimen-
12 (T) .72 .76 12 (T) .69
13 (T) .62 .63 13 (T) .69 sion of PBI correlated highly with girls’ and boys’
14 (R/C)* −.55 −.58 14 (T) .68 scores of IPPA-S, whereas Overprotection dimension
15 (C) .73 .78 15 (T) .71 correlated negatively and to a lower extent (correla-
16 (C) .68 .71 16 (C) .67 tions of small size). Moreover, in the specific case of
17 (A)* −.58 −.69 17 (C) .77 father overprotection, only boys’ attachment to father
18 (A)* −.58 −.66 18 (A)* −.52 was considerably related to it, albeit of small size
19 (C) .76 .76 19 (T) .72 again. Lastly, the relationship between IPPA-S scores
20 (T) .81 .82 20 (T) .78 and self-esteem with RSES was observed, although
21 (T) .75 .80 21 (T) .71 correlations were not as high as could be expected.
22 (T) .75 .76 22 (A)* −.34
For this variable, scores are positively, but not to a
23 (A)* −.41 −.40 23 (A)* −.60
major extent, correlated with perceived attachment
24 (C) .79 .78 24 (C) .78
25 (C) .59 .70 25 (C) .68
to mother, father, and peers in girls (somewhat higher
for father); however, in boys, such positive correla-
Note: IPPA-M = IPPA Mother; IPPA-F = IPPA Father; tion can only be observed in relation to perceived
IPPA-P = IPPA Peer. T = Trust subscale in the original American attachment to father, and are even of small magni-
version; C = Communication subscale in the original tude in that case.
American version; A = Alienation subscale in the original
American version. R = Reverse item.*Dropped out items. Study 3

The objective if this last study was to confirm the factor


(“I feel angry with my mother/father”), and #18 structure observed in Study 2. Thus, here we intended
(“I don’t get much attention from my mother/father”). to analyze the goodness of fit of the one-dimensional
Therefore, and taking into account the four criteria model of IPPA-S.
mentioned above, (the same) 16 items in each version
(mother, father, and peer) were selected, which accounted Method
for 50.8%, 54.3%, and 50.8% of the variance in mother,
Participants
father, and peer scales respectively. The specific weights
for each scale of the 16 items retained are given in Table 3 The sample comprised 604 adolescents (335 girls and
(father and mother versions) and Table 4 (peer version). 269 boys), studying in secondary schools or at univer-
Following the deletion of the items mentioned, internal sity in the first four semesters. Mean age (in years) was
consistency values were calculated for the three scales 17.8 (SD = 1.49)1. Although ethnicity was not recorded,
(mother, father and peers). Cronbach alpha coefficients like in the sample used for Study 2 and Study 3, here
were all good and somehow higher than with 25 items: too no more than 5% of the sample was presumably
.88 for mother scale (.77 with 25 items), .91 for father different from the mainstream (Caucasian) Spanish.
scale (.77 with 25 items), and .93 for peer scale (.79 with The procedure for the administration of the question-
25 items). Since the items for the shorter version naire followed here was the same as in Study 2.
have been selected based on the factor analysis, one
would assume that there may be somewhat inflated. 1Due to circumstances such as bereavement or divorce (and therefore,

Therefore, for unbiased estimates, the alpha coefficients because of the lack of relationship with that parent), some adolescents
were again calculated with a new sample in Study 3. decided not to fill in one of the scales: either mother or father.
8 M. Gallarin and I. Alonso-Arbiol

Table 3. Factor Loadings of Mother and Father Scale Items from the Principal Component Analysis

Item IPPA-M IPPA-F

1. Mi madre/padre respeta mis sentimientos (My mother/father respects my feelings) .751 .758
2. Creo que mi madre/padre está haciendo un buen trabajo como madre/padre (I feel my mother/father does .736 .784
a good job as a mother/father)
3. Desearía tener una/un madre/padre diferente (I wish I had a different mother/father) (R) .600 .634
4. Mi madre/padre me acepta tal y como soy (My mother/father accepts me as I am) .697 .723
5. Me gusta saber el punto de vista de mi madre/padre sobre las cosas que me preocupan (I like to get my .607 .638
mother’s/father’s point of view on things I’m concerned about)
6. Creo que no sirve de nada mostrar mis sentimientos delante de mi madre/padre (I feel it’s no use letting .603 .620
my feelings show around my mother/father) (R)
12. Cuando hablamos, mi madre/padre tiene en cuenta mi punto de vista (When we discuss things, my .745 .772
mother/father cares about my point of view)
13. Mi madre/padre se fía de mi buen juicio (My mother/father trusts my judgment) .645 .660
15. Mi madre/padre me ayuda a entenderme mejor (My mother/father helps me to understand .753 .797
myself better)
16. Le cuento a mi madre/padre mis problemas y preocupaciones (I tell my mother/father about my .676 .669
problems and troubles)
19. Mi madre/padre me ayuda a hablar de mis problemas (My mother/father helps me to talk about my .773 .768
difficulties)
20. Mi madre/padre me entiende (My mother/father understands me) .816 .828
21. Cuando estoy enfadado/a por algo, mi madre/padre intenta ser comprensiva/o (When I am angry about .762 .817
something, my mother/father tries to be understanding)
22. Confío en mi madre/padre (I trust my mother/father) .755 .768
24. Puedo contar con mi madre/padre cuando necesito desahogarme (I can count on my mother/father when .806 .794
I need to get something off my chest)
25. Si mi madre/padre sabe que estoy molesto/a por algo, me pregunta por ello (If my mother/father knows .615 .719
something is bothering me, she/he asks me about it)

Note: IPPA-M = IPPA Mother; IPPA-F = IPPA Father. R = Reverse item.

Table 4. Factor Loadings of Peers Scale Items from the Principal Component Analysis

Item IPPA-P

2. Mis amigos/as pueden saber cuándo estoy disgustado/a por algo (My friends can tell when I’m upset about something) .667
3. Cuando hablamos de cosas, mis amigos/as tienen en cuenta mi punto de vista (When we discuss things, my friends care .702
about my point of view)
6. Mis amigos/as me entienden (My friends understand me) .733
7. Mis amigos/as me animan a hablar de mis preocupaciones (My friends encourage me to talk about my difficulties) .706
8. Mis amigos/as me aceptan tal y como soy (My friends accept me as I am) .639
12. Mis amigos/as prestan atención a lo que digo (My friends listen to what I have to say) .675
13. Creo que mis amigos/as son buenos/as amigos/as (I feel my friends are good friends) .680
14. Es bastante fácil hablar con mis amigos/as (My friends are fairly easy to talk to) .684
15. Cuando estoy enfadado/a por algo, mis amigos/as intentan ser comprensivos/as (When I am angry about something, .718
my friends try to be understanding)
16. Mis amigos/as me ayudan a entenderme mejor (My friends help me to understand myself better) .689
17. Mis amigos/as se preocupan por cómo me siento (My friends care about how I am feeling) .768
19. Puedo contar con mis amigos/as cuando necesito desahogarme (I can count on my friends when I need to get .712
something off my chest)
20. Confío en mis amigos/as (I trust my friends) .776
21. Mis amigos/as respetan mis sentimientos (My friends respect my feelings) .717
24. Les puedo contar mis problemas y preocupaciones a mis amigos/as (I can tell my friends about my problems and troubles) .802
25. Si mis amigos/as saben que estoy molesto/a por algo, me preguntan por ello (If my friends know something is .712
bothering me, they ask me about it)

Note: IPPA-P = IPPA Peer.


Dimensionality of Ippa 9

Table 5. Correlations among IPPA-S Three Versions and FES, PBI and RSES Scales

IPPA-M IPPA-F IPPA-P

Scales Girls Boys Total Girls Boys Total Girls Boys Total

Family Climate
FES C/E .600** .462** .561** .548** .463** .521** .284** .121 230**
FES CN −.442** −.319** −.394** −.389** −.266** −.344** −.204** −.164* −.172**
Parental Bonding
Mother PBI-C .703** .769** .723** .360** .266** .330** .262** .208** .236**
Mother PBI-O −.265** −.215** −.250** −.259** −.221** −.239** −.017 −.082 −.056
Father PBI-C .414** .202** .330** .788** .738** .774** .204** .131 .171*
Father PBI-O −.105 −.180* −.239** −.025 −.293** −.101 .057 .001 .044
Self-Esteem
RSES .282** .072 .210** .328** .225** .302** .279** .112 .179**

Note: IPPA-M = IPPA Mother version; IPPA-F = IPPA Father version; IPPA-P = IPPA Peer version; FES C/E = Family Environment
Scale - Cohesion/Expressiveness; FES CN = Family Environment Scale - Conflict; PBI-C = Parental Bonding Instrument - Care;
PBI-O = Parental Bonding Instrument - Overprotection; RSES = Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale.* p < .05; ** p < .01.

Results independent scores may be obtained which are always


referred to in terms of (in)security of attachment per-
Like in Study 2, structural equation modeling was used
ceived by the adolescent and in relation to mother,
here (Arbuckle, 2008) and the fit of the model was
father and peers. IPPA-S shows good indexes of valid-
tested in multiple-group structural equation models
ity and reliability.
(using maximum likelihood estimates), considering girls
Our results point to a one-dimensional structure of
and boys as two different groups. For the evaluation of
IPPA-S. This finding responds to the authors’ demand
the fit of the model, we relied on the same indexes as in
of analyzing the structure of the questionnaire more
Study 2: The relative chi-square (χ2/df), AGFI, TLI, CFI,
in-depth, and it sheds light into the question of how
RMSEA, RMR, and AIC.
many factors could be distinguished, as one, two and
As can be seen in Table 6, all fitness indexes were
three factors had been claimed. On the one hand, our
adequate in all of the three scales (mother, father and
results go in the direction recently pointed out by
peers), and so one-dimensional structure seems to fit
Günaydin and colleagues (2005), by Baiocco and
the data adequately. The measurement weights model
colleagues (2009), and by Alonso-Arbiol and associates
showed the most adequate fit statistics and was the
(in press), who anticipated such one-factor structure in
most parsimonious one for all three versions. This
Turkish, Italian and Basque versions of IPPA. On the
finding indicates that the one-factor structure was
other hand, our findings do not match those of Johnson
common for the two groups, and that equal parameter
and associates (2003), who distinguished two factors
estimates and error variances for males and females
called trust and alienation, nor the ones linked to the
are acceptable. The standardized coefficients for the
Italian adaptation (2009, 2011), which had three factors.
item loadings in each scale are displayed in Figure 1
As for the structure observed by Johnson and col-
(mother and father versions) and Figure 2 (peer version).
leagues (2003), a closer examination of factor loadings
Cronbach alpha coefficients were all good: .87 for mother
of 25 items of IPPA in the two factors suggests that
scale, .88 for father scale, and .93 for peer scale.
other circumstances than just separate constructs may
also explain the underlying structure. In fact, all items
Discussion
with positive meaning (referred to both trust and
This study had the dual aim of: (a) adapting IPPA into communication, as in the original formulation) (i.e. “My
Spanish (IPPA-S), and (b) examining its factor structure. mother respects my feeling” or “I like to get my mother’s
The empirical examination of the structure with both point of view on things I’m concerned about”) fall within
exploratory and confirmatory analyses has revealed a the first factor, whereas the second factor gathers all
one-factor structure of IPPA, refuting a long tradition items capturing a sense of alienation as well as four
of suggested three-dimensionality. We may conclude reversed items referring to trust and communication
that the IPPA-S has 16 items in each of the three scales (i.e. “I get upset easily around my mother” and “I feel it’s no
or versions —mother, father, and peers— grouped into use letting my feelings show around my mother”). Therefore,
one factor. When the three versions are used, three more than distinguishing between trust and alienation,
10

Table 6. Fit Indexes for the Three Subscales of IPPA-S in the One-Dimensional Model
M. Gallarin and I. Alonso-Arbiol

Scales Model no. Model description χ² df χ²/df AGFI TLI RMSEA CFI RMR AIC Nested models Δχ ² Δdf p level

IPPA-M
1 Unconstrained 419.1 174 2.41 .876 .930 .048 .949 .043 615.13
2 Measurement weights 439.9 189 2.33 .881 .934 .046 .948 .057 605.96 2-1 20.8 15 .001
3 Structural residuals 441.2 190 2.32 .881 .934 .046 .948 .068 605.25 3-2 1.3 1 .001
4 Measurement residuals 520.3 223 2.33 .882 .933 .046 .938 .067 618.27 4-3 79.1 33 .001
IPPA-F
1 Unconstrained 372.6 174 2.14 .886 .952 .044 .965 .044 568.59
2 Measurement weights 394.8 189 2.09 .889 .954 .043 .964 .061 560.76 2-1 22.2 15 .001
3 Structural residuals 405.0 190 2.13 .888 .952 .043 .962 .137 569.04 3-2 10.2 1 .001
4 Measurement residuals 466.5 223 2.09 .892 .954 .043 .957 .138 564.54 4-3 61.5 33 .001
IPPA-P
1 Unconstrained 362.2 184 1.97 .895 .954 .040 .965 .029 538.18
2 Measurement weights 380.9 199 1.91 .899 .957 .039 .964 .039 526.93 2-1 15.7 15 .001
3 Structural residuals 383.3 200 1.92 .899 .957 .039 .964 .049 527.31 3-2 2.4 1 .001
4 Measurement residuals 611.9 228 2.68 .861 .921 .053 .924 .062 699.95 4-3 228.6 28 .001

Note: IPPA-M = IPPA Mother; IPPA-F = IPPA Father; IPPA-P = IPPA Peer.
Dimensionality of Ippa 11

Figure 1. Standardized regression coefficients of IPPA


mother and IPPA father scales. IPPA father scale indexes are
in brackets; values without brackets refer to mother scale. All
the parameters are significant at the level p < .001.

Figure 2. Standardized regression coefficients of IPPA peer


one may wonder to what extent this structure does scale. All the parameters are significant at the level p < .001.
not resemble a classical phenomenon of factor analysis
procedures which involve ending up separating items
whose contents are located in two extremes of a con- that the three-factor structure shows the best fit in all
tinuum (Marsch, 1996). three forms (mother, father and peers). However, for
Regarding San Martini and associates’ (2009) obser- all versions of IPPA (mother, father, peer), CFI is not
vation of a three-dimensional structure, neither the over .90, and RMSEA is not under .05. Furthermore, in
exploratory analysis conducted first, nor the confirma- the EFA carried out initially, the one-factor structure
tory factor analysis carried out later seem to support such showed good levels of accounted variance and high
structure unequivocally. First, the sedimentation graph of loadings for almost all the items. Moreover, due the
the exploratory analysis shows a one-dimensional struc- high correlations between the three factors, authors
ture. Second, the goodness-of-fit indexes of the confir- warned about the weak differentiation of the factors,
matory analyses were not sufficiently good for any of and therefore, about the usefulness of the segmenta-
the three options analyzed. Although the indexes for tion of the inventory on a practical level.
the three-factor models are better than the other com- Therefore, so far only Vignoli and Mallet (2004) have
peting ones for all versions (mother, father, peer), GFI been able to more rigorously corroborate the three-
and AGFI are not over .90, and RMSEA is not under factor structure originally proposed. However, a closer
.05. On the other hand, in the recent study also carried inspection of the correlation indices between socializa-
out in Italy (Pace et al., 2011) the authors concluded tion styles and three factors of IPPA reveals that those
12 M. Gallarin and I. Alonso-Arbiol

indexes are very similar for trust and communication. girls scores of the three IPPA scales are correlated to self-
In fact, in that study correlations between these two esteem, in boys only attachment to father is correlated
factors are of .72 for mother scale and of .68 for father with self-esteem. These results go in the same direction
scale, which denotes a high correspondence between as those obtained in previous studies (e.g., Gullone &
them. Furthermore, it seems that authors needed to link Robinson, 2005; Raja et al., 1992), where it seemed that
some errors in order for the three-factor model to obtain peer attachment is more relevant for girls, because
acceptable indexes, because without those covariances girls’ development of identity is more related to the
only the father scale would get close to being acceptable. establishment of intimate relationships than boys’.
For these reasons, this three-dimensional observation Another difference can be found regarding correla-
claimed by Vignoli and Mallet should be interpreted tions between IPPA and PBI. Although taking into
with caution. account the values of the total sample, we obtained
Consequently, although the initial objective of similar results as those reported by Gullone and Robinson
Armsden and Greenberg (1987) was to assess elements (2005) —care positively associated with IPPA, and
of positive experience deriving from closeness and trust overprotection negatively associated—, when we dif-
in attachment figures, as well as negative experience ferentiated between boys and girls, on one hand, and
deriving from the attachment figures’ lack of (or incon- mothers and fathers, on the other hand, the results
sistent) response; it seems that such a distinction is not change. While in girls none of the IPPA scales was sig-
completely defined or captured by respondents’ answers nificantly correlated with father’s overprotection, in boys
to the instrument. In this study, the final 16 items attachment to mother and specially attachment to father
deriving from the adaptation process into Spanish retain were significantly and negatively correlated with
the main feature of attachment, namely, the perceived this subscale. This finding may indicate that in boys
security established in relationships with the most impor- an overprotective father would be associated with a
tant figures. Bowlby (1969) explained how the funda- higher perception of insecurity. A possible explanation
mental function of attachment behavior is to seek and could be that this is so because boys’ development is
maintain proximity with attachment figures. This search more closely linked to individuation and to a higher
for proximity is a device used to regulate emotion distance towards others (Raja et al., 1992); an overprotec-
designed with the aim of protecting the individual tive father does not resemble this identification pattern.
from possible external threats. If these strategies work One limitation of this study is the lack of more spe-
out—i.e., they accomplish their regulatory role— a cific measures for the analysis of convergent validity of
feeling of security in relation to attachment figures is attachment peer scale. Our results have shown consid-
developed, based on which the world and people erable differences between parent two scales and peer
living in it are perceived as secure (see Cassidy & Shaver, scale when these were correlated with the other mea-
2008; and Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007, for reviews). sures. The use of other measures to assess the quality
Therefore, it makes sense that a main underlying dimen- of friendship quality might have resulted in better
sion of attachment security vs. insecurity emerges here. comparison indexes.
On another issue, and as expected, high positive On another issue, the factor structure has been
correlations between IPPA scores and the FES cohesion/ analyzed here with a Spanish version. Therefore, results
expressiveness subscale, PBI care subscale, and self- should be taken with caution before they can be gener-
esteem were observed. Besides, although not sought in alized to include non-Spanish adolescents. For this
the validity analysis, negative correlations with the PBI reason, future research may address the replicability
overprotection subscale, and to less extent with the of the structure of the questionnaire in different cul-
FES conflict subscale, were also found. This last result tures. The examination of factor structures in different
would be in keeping with the observation that adoles- languages/cultures has been indirectly analyzed here.
cent aggressiveness is related to insecure attachment to However, it should be noted that not all the versions
parents (e.g., Gallarin & Alonso-Arbiol, 2012; Gomez & revised here contain the same items, some being shorter
McLaren, 2007). or modified versions with different wording of items.
Moreover, unlike previous studies (Armsdem & A more rigorous procedure involving an examination
Greenberg, 1987; Gullone & Robinson, 2005; Günaydin of structure equivalence of the same (adapted) version
et al., 2005; Pardo et al., 2006), in this work boys and should be conducted before we can confirm or reject that
girls’ scores have been taken into account separately, the dimensionality across language/cultures is com-
which allows for a more detailed analysis of inter-cor- parable (Van de Vijver & Leung, 1997). Based on this
relations between IPPA and the other variables. While analysis and design, we could more confidently study
Günaydin and colleagues found that self-esteem and whether the underlying one-dimensional structure
attachment to father and to mother were associated, of the Spanish IPPA is culture-dependent or reflects
our study allows for a finer observation. Whereas in a more universal pattern.
Dimensionality of Ippa 13

In conclusion, the Spanish IPPA or IPPA-S is a valid A predictive model. Journal of Adolescence, 35, 1601–1610.
and reliable measure for the assessment of the perceived http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2012.07.002
security (or insecurity) of attachment to parents and Gomez R., & McLaren S. (2007). The inter-relations of
mother and father attachment, self-esteem and aggression
peers in adolescence. IPPA-S, therefore, may be a good
during late adolescence. Aggressive Behavior, 33, 160–169.
assessment tool to be confidently used with Spanish
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ab.20181
populations both in applied and research domains. Gullone E., & Robinson K. (2005). The inventory of parent
and peer attachment-revised (IPPA-R) for children:
References A psychometric investigation. Clinical Psychology and
Allen J. (2008). The attachment system in adolescence. Psychotherapy, 12, 67–79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/
In J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of Attachment: cpp.433
Günaydin G., Selçuk E., Sümer N., & Uysal A. (2005).
Theory, research and clinical applications (2nd ed.) (pp. 419–435).
Ebeveyn ve arkadaslara baglanma envanteri kisa formunun
New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
psikometrik acidan degerlendirilmesi [The psychometric
Allen J., & Miga E. (2010). Attachment in adolescence:
evaluation of the short form of inventory of parent and
A move to the level of emotion regulation. Journal of Social
peer attachment]. Türk Psikoloji Yazilari, 8, 13–23.
and Personal Relationships, 27, 181–190. http://dx.doi.
Hoyle R. H. (Ed.), (1995). Structural equation modeling:
org/10.1177/0265407509360898
Concepts, issues, and applications. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Alonso-Arbiol I., Balluerka N., Gorostiaga A., Aritzeta A., &
Johnson L., Ketring S., & Abshire C. (2003). The revised
Gallarin M., & Haranburu M. (in press). Dimensiones del
inventory of parent attachment: Measuring attachment
apego en la adolescencia: Adaptación al Euskera del
in families. Contemporary Family Therapy 25, 333–349.
Inventario de Apego de Progenitores y Pares (IPPA)
http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1024563422543
[Attachment dimensions in adolescence: Basque
Kline R. B. (1998). Principles and practice of Structural Equation
adaptation of the Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment
Modeling. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
(IPPA)]. Estudios de Psicología.
Laible D. (2007). Attachment with parents and peers in late
Armsden G., & Greenberg M. (1987). The inventory of
adolescence: Links with emotional competence and social
parent and peer attachment: Individual differences
behavior. Personality and Individual Differences, 43,
and their relationship to psychological well-being in
1185–1197. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.03.010
adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 16, 427–454.
Marsch H. W. (1996). Positive and negative global self-
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02202939
esteem: A substantively meaningful distinction or
Arbuckle J. L. (2008). AMOS 17 User’s guide. Chicago, IL:
artifactors? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70,
SPSS Inc.
810–819. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.70.4.810
Baiocco R., Laghi F., & Paola M. (2009). La scala per Martin-Albo J., Núñez J., Navarro J., & Grijalvo F. (2007).
l’attaccamento nei confronti dei genitori (IPPA-G) e del grupo dei The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale: Translation and
pari (IPPA-P) in adolescenza: Un contributo alla validazione validation in university students. The Spanish Journal of
italiana. [Attachment Scale to Parents (IPPA-G) and to Peers Psychology, 10, 458–467.
(IPPA-P) in Adolescence: A contribution to the Italian Mikulincer M., & Shaver P. (2007). Attachment in adulthood:
validation]. Psicologia Clinica dello Sviluppo, 13, 355–383. Structure, dynamics, and change. New York, NY: Guilford
Balluerka N., Gorostiaga A., Alonso-Arbiol I., & Press.
Haranburu M. (2007). La adaptación de instrumentos de Mikulincer M., & Shaver P. (2008). Adult attachment and
medida de unas culturas a otras: Una perspectiva práctica affect regulation. In J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook
[Test adaptation to other cultures: A practical approach]. of Attachment. Theory, research and Clinical Applications (2nd ed.)
Psicothema, 19, 124–133. (pp. 503–531). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Ballús-Creus C. (1991). Adaptació del parental bonding Moos R., & Trickett E. (1974). Classroom environment scale
instrument [Adaptation of the parental bonding manual. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologist Press.
instrument]. Barcelona, Spain: Escola Profesional de Moos R., Moos B., & Trickett E. (1984). Escalas de Clima
Psicología Clínica. Social [Social Climate Scales]. Madrid, Spain: TEA.
Bowlby J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. Pace C. S., San Martini P., & Zavattini G. C. (2011). The
New York, NY: Basic Books. factor structure of the inventory of parent and peer
Byrne B. (2010). Structural equation modeling with AMOS: attachment (IPPA): A survey of Italian adolescents.
Basic concepts, applications, and programming (2nd ed.). Personality and Individual Differences, 51, 83–88.
New York, NY: Routledge. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.03.006
Cassidy J., & Shaver P. R. (Eds.). (2008). Handbook of Papini D., Roggman R., & Anderson J. (1991). Early-
attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (2nd ed.). adolescent perceptions of attachment to mother and
New York, NY: Guilford Press. father: A test of the emotional-distancing and buffering
Dykas M., Ziv Y., & Cassidy J. (2008). Attachment and peer hypotheses. Journal of Early Adolescence, 11, 258–275.
relations in adolescence. Attachment & Human Development, http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0272431691112006
10, 123–141. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14616730802113679 Pardo M., Pineda S., Carrillo S., & Castro J. (2006). Análisis
Gallarin M., & Alonso-Arbiol I. (2012). Parenting practices, psicométrico del inventario de apego con padres y pares en una
parental attachment and aggressiveness in adolescence: muestra de adolescents colombianos [Psychometric analysis of
14 M. Gallarin and I. Alonso-Arbiol

the inventory of parent and peer attachment in a sample of Shaffer D., & Kipp K. (2007). Developmental psychology:
Colombian adolescents]. Revista Interamericana de Childhood and adolescence (7th ed.). Belmont, CA:
Psicología, 40, 289–302. Wadsword.
Parker G., Tupling H., & Brown B. (1979). A parental San Martini P., Zavattini G., & Ronconi S. (2009).
bonding instrument. British Journal of Medical Psychology, L’inventario per l’attaccamento ai genitori ed ai pari:
52, 1–10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8341.1979. Un’indagine psicometrica su un campione italiano di adolescenti
tb02487.x [The inventory of parent and peer attachment: A
Raja S., McGee R., & Stanton W. (1992). Perceived psychometric investigation of an Italian sample of
attachments to parents and peers and psychological adolescents]. Giornale Italiano di Psicologia, 36, 199–225.
well-being in adolescence. Journal of Youth and Schumacker R. E., & Lomax R. G. (2004). A beginner’s guide
Adolescence, 21, 471–485. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/ to structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ:
BF01537898 Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Rosenberg M. (1989). Society and the adolescent self-image Van de Vijver F. J. R., & Leung K. (1997). Methods and data
(Rev. Ed.). Middletown, CT: Wesleyan University Press. analysis for cross-cultural research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Sanchez-Queija I., & Oliva A. (2003). Vínculos de apego con Vignoli E., & Mallet P. (2004). Validation of a brief measure
los padres y relaciones con los iguales durante la adolescencia of adolescents’ parent attachment based on Armsden and
[Attachment to parents and peer relationships during Greenberg’s three-dimension model. Revue Européenne de
adolescence]. Revista de Psicología Social, 18, 71–86. Psychologie Apliquée, 54, 251–260. http://dx.doi.org/
http://dx.doi.org/10.1174/02134740360521796 10.1016/j.erap.2004.04.003

Você também pode gostar