Você está na página 1de 13

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/273481855

Religion, Meaning and Purpose, and Mental Health

Article  in  Psychology of Religion and Spirituality · February 2015


DOI: 10.1037/a0037887

CITATIONS READS
24 4,184

5 authors, including:

Kevin J Flannelly Christopher G Ellison


Center for Psychosocial Research University of Texas at San Antonio
223 PUBLICATIONS   4,780 CITATIONS    128 PUBLICATIONS   8,105 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Nava R Silton Katherine R B Jankowski


Marymount Manhattan College 26 PUBLICATIONS   520 CITATIONS   
319 PUBLICATIONS   687 CITATIONS   
SEE PROFILE
SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Nasvhille Stress and Health Study View project

Religion and Sleep View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Nava R Silton on 23 March 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Psychology of Religion and Spirituality © 2014 American Psychological Association
2015, Vol. 7, No. 1, 1–12 1941-1022/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0037887

Religion, Meaning and Purpose, and Mental Health

Kathleen Galek Kevin J. Flannelly


The Spears Research Institute, Healthcare Chaplaincy, New Center for Psychosocial Research, Massapequa, New York
York, New York

Christopher G. Ellison Nava R. Silton


The University of Texas at San Antonio Marymount Manhattan College

Katherine R. B. Jankowski
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Center for Psychosocial Research, Massapequa, New York


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

The present study was specifically designed to examine the associations among religious commitment,
belief in meaning and purpose in life, and psychiatric symptoms among the general public using data
from the 2010 Baylor Religion Survey (BRS). The BRS obtained data from a nationwide sample of 1,714
U.S. adults, 1,450 of which are included in the current analyses. The central hypothesis of the study,
based on identity theory, was that religious commitment would interact with belief in meaning and
purpose in their net associations on psychiatric symptoms: general anxiety, social anxiety, paranoia,
obsession, and compulsion. Specifically, it was hypothesized that believing life lacks meaning and
purpose will have a more pernicious association among highly religious individuals, than it will among
individuals who are less religious. Other hypotheses derived from previous research were also tested. The
results confirm the central hypothesis of the study for 4 of the 5 classes of psychiatric symptoms. The
results are discussed with respect to identity theory, evolutionary threat assessment systems (ETAS)
theory, and the hostile world scenario.

Keywords: beliefs, ETAS theory, identity theory, religion, meaning in life, psychiatric symptoms

Victor Frankl proclaimed, “Man’s search for meaning is the tive psychology do not accord meaning the same status that Frankl
primary force in his life . . .” (Frankl, 1946/1992, p. 105). In so did (Peterson & Park, 2012; Wong, 2012b).
doing, he introduced a novel perspective on human motivation, Frankl claimed the meaning of life was to be found, not in
which challenged the prevailing wisdom of American schools of oneself, but in one’s interaction with the surrounding world. Frankl
psychology at the time: behaviorism and psychoanalysis (McAd- believed that meaning in life could be discovered through three
ams, 2012). Frankl’s ideas also helped to lay the foundation for pathways: “(1) by creating a work or doing a deed; (2) by expe-
positive psychology (Wong, 2011), although researchers in posi- riencing something or encountering someone; and (3) by the
attitude we take toward unavoidable suffering” (Frankl, 1946/
1992, p. 115). Wong and his colleagues have explicated 12 sources
of meaning associated with these three pathways (McDonald,
This article was published Online First November 3, 2014. Wong, & Gingras, 2012; Reker & Wong, 1988; Wong, 1998).
Kathleen Galek, The Spears Research Institute, Healthcare Chaplaincy, Other research has distinguished between the search for meaning
New York, New York; Kevin J. Flannelly, Center for Psychosocial Re- and the presence of meaning, and has explored the associations
search, Massapequa, New York; Christopher G. Ellison, Department of between these two concepts and psychological well-being (Steger,
Sociology, The University of Texas at San Antonio; Nava R. Silton,
Frazier, Oishi, & Kaler, 2006; Steger, Kashdan, Sullivan, &
Department of Psychology, Marymount Manhattan College; Katherine
R. B. Jankowski, Center for Psychosocial Research, Massapequa, New
Lorentz, 2008).
York. According to Frankl (1946/1992), the meaning of one’s life may
The conduct of this research and the preparation of this article for change, but the need for meaning is always present. Baumeister
publication were made possible through the generous support of a grant (1991) believes the search for meaning encompasses four basic
from the John Templeton Foundation to HealthCare Chaplaincy, NY, needs. The first is the need to have a sense of purpose in life, to
NY: ID# 21296, “Spiritual Beliefs as Predictors of Mental Health: A believe that one’s life has purpose. The second is the need for a
Test of ETAS Theory” (Kevin J. Flannelly, PhD, and Kathleen Galek, sense of efficacy, to believe that one is able to meet challenges and
PhD, Co-PIs). The opinions expressed in this publication are those of the
to achieve goals. The third is self-worth, to believe that one is a
authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the John Templeton
Foundation. worthy person with desirable characteristics. The fourth is the need
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Kevin J. for value, to believe that one’s actions are good and justified.
Flannelly, Center for Psychosocial Research, 33 Maple Street, Mass- Reker and Wong’s (1988) definition of personal meaning taps into
apequa, NY 11758. E-mail: kjflannelly@gmail.com the first three of Baumeister’s four needs. To them, personal
1
2 GALEK, FLANNELLY, ELLISON, SILTON, AND JANKOWSKI

meaning is: “coherence and purpose in one’s existence, the pursuit measures of religion among college students, such as intrinsic
and attainment of worthwhile goals, and an accompanying sense of religiosity (Crandall & Rasmussen, 1975; Soderstrom & Wright,
fulfillment” (Reker & Wong, 1988, p. 221). 1977), religious commitment (Soderstrom & Wright, 1977), fre-
Although Frankl thought we lost our ancient instincts over the quency of personal religious practices, and self-ratings of religi-
course of human evolution, some psychologists contend that the osity (Steger & Frazier, 2005). Meaning in life was more closely
need to believe one’s life has meaning is so universal that it may linked to religion than to other factors (e.g., positive mood) among
be an evolutionary adaptation (King & Hicks, 2012; Klinger, 2012; those U.S. college students who report higher levels of religious
Sommer, Baumeister, & Stillman, 2012). For example, Klinger commitment (Hicks & King, 2008). Meaning in life also has been
(2012) suggests that the need for meaning originates from the found to be associated with strength of religiousness in a large,
mammalian brain system involved in goal seeking (i.e., seeking nonprobability sample of adults from the United States and other
food and other necessary resources), which has been extensively English-speaking countries (Peterson & Park, 2012).
studied by Panksepp (1998, 2005). In a similar vein, a theoretical A number of authors have suggested that the relationship be-
paper by Silton, Flannelly, Flannelly, and Galek (2011) proposed tween religion and meaning in life is attributable to the fact that
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

that this same goal-seeking system in the brain underlies the religious commitment fosters a coherent set of goals that provide
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

human needs for meaning and self-actualization (Maslow, 1943, meaning and purpose (Pargament, 1997; Park, 2005; Wong, 1998),
1958). and there is some evidence that this may be so, at least among
According to Klinger, “Because success at these pursuits is college students (Klinger, 2012). One such explicitly religious goal
central to animal survival . . . persistent goal striving . . . consti- concerns life after death. Christianity and other religions put life
tutes an imperative of purpose” (Klinger, 2012, p. 27). Given this experiences in the context of future spiritual salvation, which
imperative, he states that a lack of purpose causes distress. Re- provides a sense of purpose in life (Sommer et al., 2012), and
search on the relationship between psychological distress and lack limited research indicates that personal meaning is associated with
of meaning and purpose in life is discussed below. belief in salvation (Crandall & Rasmussen, 1975).
Although there are many ways through which individuals find
meaning and purpose in life (Wong, 1998, 2012a), religion is
Religion and Meaning
unique in providing meaning to the existential problems that all
Many theorists believe that one of the fundamental functions of individuals encounter (Pargament, 1997; Park, 2005; Wong,
religion is to help individuals fulfill their yearning for meaning and 1998). In addition to explaining problems that are explicitly reli-
purpose in life. Clark, for instance, believed that “religion more gious (e.g., death and suffering), religion provides perspectives
than any other human function satisfies the need for meaning” about other aspects of the world, such as global beliefs about
(Clark, 1958, p. 419). Pargament stated it more simply: “In es- fairness and equity and one’s vulnerability to the vagaries of life
sence, religion offers meaning in life” (Pargament, 1997, p. 49). (Pargament, 1997; Park, 2005; Flannelly, Ellison, Galek, & Silton,
Empirical evidence confirms that there is a positive association 2012).
between religion and the belief there is in meaning and purpose in According to Wong (2010, 2012a), existential meaning encom-
life. Francis and his colleagues, who have studied this relationship passes at least seven personal questions: “Who am I? What should
for years in Britain, have found that various measures of religion I do with my life to make it worthwhile? What can I do to find
are directly related to meaning and purpose. For example, several happiness and life satisfaction? How can I make the right choices
studies by Francis and his associates have reported that frequency in an age of moral ambiguity and conflicting values? Where do I
of church attendance among convenience samples of children and belong and where can I call home? What is the point of living in
adolescents (Francis, 2000; Francis & Burton, 1994; Francis & the face of suffering and death? What happens after death?”
Evans, 1996), as well as college students (Robbins & Francis, (Wong, 2012a, p. xxx). Depending on your point of view, religion
2000) are positively related to believing that one has a purpose in provides answers to all or most of these questions.
life. The same studies also found that the frequency of prayer and Religion has been said to represent the quest for the ultimate
Bible reading (Francis & Burton, 1994; Francis & Evans, 1996) meaning of human existence, and the meaning system provided by
and belief in God (Francis, 2000) are positively related to believing religion is especially helpful for addressing the most severe chal-
one’s life has a purpose. Finally, Francis, Jewell, and Robbins lenges of life, such as death, illness, suffering, and other tragedies
(2010) have reported that intrinsic religiosity has a strong positive (Wong, 1998). Moreover, as alluded to above, religion provides
correlation with having a sense of purpose in life. ultimate goals through which to find meaning and purpose in life
Two related studies have found that beliefs about God are (Park, 2005; Wong, 1998).
related to meaning and purpose in life. The first, which was a study Since people of many different religious faiths rely on their
of Methodist church members in Britain, found that congregants religious beliefs to make sense of the world and to find meaning in
who firmly believed God is involved in their lives more strongly their lives (Pargament, 1997; Park, 2005; Spilka, Hood,
believed there was meaning and purpose in life than congregants Hunsberger, & Gorsuch, 2003), one might wonder whether some
who were less certain of God’s involvement (Jewell, 2010). The religious faiths are better able to promote meaning than others. The
second, which was a U.S. study of Baptist congregants, found very only research, of which we are aware, that has broached this topic
similar results, in that those who believed God was involved in the was a study of U.S. college students belonging to three Christian
world in general, were more likely to believe there was meaning denominations: Catholics, Evangelical Protestants, and non-
and purpose in life (Jackson & Coursey, 1988). Evangelical Protestants (Steger & Frazier, 2005). The study found
Other research in the United States has reported a positive no significant differences in perceived meaning in life among the
relationship between meaning and purpose in life and various three groups of students.
RELIGION, MEANING AND PURPOSE, AND MENTAL HEALTH 3

Meaning and Mental Health a sense of meaning in life or one is searching for meaning in life
(Steger et al., 2006, 2008).
The belief that there is meaning and purpose in life has been These articles report that having a sense of meaning in life,
associated with various aspects of mental health. For instance, which the authors call the “presence” of meaning, was generally
studies of U.S. college students have reported that meaning and associated with good psychological well-being, whereas searching
purpose in life is directly related to positive affect (Hicks, Cicero, for meaning was generally associated with poor psychological
Trent, Burton, & King, 2010; Hicks, Trent, Davis, & King, 2011) well-being (Steger et al., 2006, 2008). For example, presence of
and hope (Mascaro & Rosen, 2005). The positive association meaning had a salubrious association with self-esteem, self-
between meaning in life and hope has also been demonstrated in a efficacy, self-consciousness, depression, neuroticism, and anxiety,
nationally representative sample of American adults (Ryff & whereas searching for meaning had a pernicious associations with
Keyes, 1995). Convenience samples of older adults in Canada and the same six measures of psychological well-being.
New Zealand have likewise found that belief in meaning and
purpose in life is positively associated, respectively, with life
Religion, Meaning, and Mental Health
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

satisfaction (Zika & Chamberlain, 1992), and psychological well-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

being (Fry, 2000). Steger (2012) reviewed research from the A study by Krause (2003) directly connected these two lines of
United States and elsewhere on the association between meaning research in demonstrating that meaning in life that was explicitly
in life and various measures of well-being, all of which confirm the tied to religious faith was positively related to life satisfaction,
salutary association between psychological well-being and the self-esteem, and optimism among a representative sample of older
belief that there is meaning and purpose in life. American adults. The few studies that have examined the interplay
Some of the studies mentioned in the preceding paragraph have between religion and meaning, with respect to their connection to
reported an inverse relationship between meaning and purpose in mental health, have yielded conflicting results.
life and negative affect (Zika & Chamberlain, 1992), including An early study, which employed a convenience sample of
depression (Mascaro & Rosen, 2005). Indeed, Steger (2012) cites women, found significant correlations between affect and meaning
more than a dozen U.S. studies that have reported an inverse and purpose, but not religiousness (Chamberlain & Zika, 1988).
relationship between depression and meaning and purpose in life. Meaning and purpose had salutary associations with both positive
Though most of these studies used convenience samples of college and negative affect. However, a salutary correlation was found
students, this salubrious association has also been observed in a between religiousness and positive and negative affect only after
representative sample of U.S. adults (Krause, 2007a). controlling for meaning and purpose. On the other hand, a recent
Moreover, meaning in life has been found to have an inverse study of U.S. college students reported that religiousness and
relationship with anxiety in undergraduates, patients, and commu- meaning and purpose were both significantly correlated with psy-
nity samples (see review by Steger, 2012). A study of Australian chological well-being (Steger & Frazier, 2005). Moreover, that
adults indicates that belief in meaning and purpose in life has a study found the salubrious association between religion and psy-
salutary association with symptoms of obsessive– compulsive dis- chological well-being was mediated by meaning in life. It is
order, as well (Scannell, Allen, & Burton, 2002). interesting that some research suggests that meaning and purpose
Since the belief that there is meaning in life is inversely related also mediate the effects of role-identity salience on psychological
to psychological distress, it follows that the belief that life lacks well-being (Thoits, 2012).
meaning should be directly related to psychological distress. In- A recent study of American college students reported ethnic
deed, a recent study reported that even daily fluctuations in mean- differences in the associations of religiousness and meaning and
ing are associated with poor psychological well-being (Steger & purpose with a global measure of psychiatric symptomology (Hol-
Kashdan, 2013). And though pure cases of “existential neurosis” mes & Hardin, 2009). When both independent variables were
(i.e., the failure to find meaning in life) are relatively uncommon included in the same regression model, religiousness (but not
among healthy American adults, meaninglessness is fairly com- meaning and purpose) had a significant salutary association with
mon among persons with clinical disorders, such as alcoholism, psychiatric symptoms among African American students, and
posttraumatic stress disorder, obsessive– compulsive disorder, and meaning and purpose (but not religiousness) had a significant
depression, especially among depressed persons who are suicidal salutary association with psychiatric symptoms among Caucasian
(Bergner, 1998). It appears, however, that only three studies have students.
specifically examined the relationship between meaninglessness
and mental health (Harlow, Newcomb, & Bentler, 1986; Kinnier,
Identity Theory and Religious Commitment
Metha, Keim, Okey, 1994; Newcomb & Harlow, 1986). All three
studies reported that the lack of meaning in life was associated According to identity theory, individuals construct personal
with suicidal ideation and substance abuse in convenience samples identities based largely on the roles they occupy in life (Burke,
of American adolescents. 1991; Thoits, 1991). These roles exist in hierarchies, with social
The influence of meaning on psychological health may be more roles in which a person has made a greater temporal, financial, or
complicated than one might conclude from the research findings psychological investment being more central to one’s identity than
reviewed so far, which indicate that meaning in life has a salubri- other roles.
ous association with mental health and lack of meaning has a Religions are inherently social systems through which world-
pernicious association with mental health. The results of two series views, or beliefs systems, are socially constructed and maintained
of studies of American college students suggest that psychological (Berger, 1967). Since religious commitment requires significant
well-being is differentially related to the degree to which one has temporal and psychological investment (Silton, Flannelly, Ellison,
4 GALEK, FLANNELLY, ELLISON, SILTON, AND JANKOWSKI

et al., 2011), the role of being a religious person may be a central nitive psychotherapy. MacLean (1972, 1985) was an early propo-
social role for some individuals. As Park and Edmondson (2012, p. nent of the idea that psychiatric symptoms were rooted in evolu-
150) put it: “Religion is the core of many individuals’ identities, tionarily primitive brain functions related to threats. Marks and
affecting how they understand themselves as religious or spiritual Nesse (Marks & Nesse, 1994; Nesse, 1998) advanced this perspec-
beings . . . as well as determining their social identification with a tive by illustrating how specific psychiatric symptoms reflect
particular religious group.” different kinds of threats of physical harm or threats to social
Identity theory further states that circumstances that threaten relationships. The proposition that psychiatric symptoms are the
central social roles will be more distressing for an individual than product of a brain system that detects and assesses threats is
circumstances that threaten less important roles. There is already steadily gaining acceptance (Eilam, Izhar, & Mort, 2011; Flannelly
evidence that this occurs with respect to threats to one’s central et al., 2007; Hinds et al., 2010; Woody & Szechtman, 2011). In
religious role. For example, Krause reported that doubts about addition to the term ETAS, such neural threat-assessment systems
religious faith have a pernicious association with psychological have been called security motivation systems (Woody & Szecht-
well-being, and that they are more troubling for individuals who man), human alarm systems (Eisenberger, Lieberman, & Williams,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

are more committed to their religious faith than to those who are 2003) and, simply, threat detection systems (Eilam et al., 2011).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

less committed (Krause, 1994, 1998). A later report by Krause and Beck (Beck, 1987; Beck, Emery, & Greenberg, 1985) and
Wulff (2004) yielded similar results with respect to the association Gilbert (1984) introduced the idea that psychiatric symptoms result
between religious doubt and mental health; religious doubt was from distorted beliefs about the dangerousness of the world. These
most troubling for persons who were most committed to their beliefs often are quite general, such as, “Any strange situation
religious faith. Finally, a recent study by Ellison, Fang, Flannelly, should be regarded as dangerous” (Beck et al., 1985, p. 63), or “It
and Steckler (2013) found that doubts about religion, especially is always best to assume the worst” (Beck et al., 1985, p. 63). Beck
doubts about God, had more profound pernicious associations with and his colleagues continue to accumulate evidence that dysfunc-
psychiatric symptoms among individuals who expressed strong tional beliefs are at the root of psychiatric symptomology (Beck &
religious commitment, compared to individuals who expressed Rector, 2002), including depression (Beck, 2008), panic attacks
moderate or no religious commitment. All three sets of findings are (Wenzel, Sharp, Brown, Greenberg, & Beck, 2006), and person-
consistent with the view that individuals who are highly invested
ality disorders (Beck et al., 2001). Cognitive psychotherapy at-
in their roles as religious individuals may be particularly wounded
tempts to alleviate psychiatric symptoms by altering the dysfunc-
by threats to this aspect of their identity. Since meaning and
tional beliefs and underlie them.
purpose in life are so closely tied to religion, we would expect that
ETAS theory incorporates these two theoretical perspectives in
believing life lacks meaning would be more troubling or threaten-
proposing: (a) that certain classes of psychiatric symptoms are the
ing to highly religious persons than to less religious persons,
products of neural assessments about potential threats to individ-
because it undermines their central social role.
uals and their social relationships; (b) that these threat assessments
are moderated by beliefs about the world (Flannelly et al., 2007);
Present Study and therefore, (c) that beliefs about the world moderate psychiatric
symptoms. Threats to individuals involve threats of physical and
A growing body of literature indicates that believing life has
meaning and purpose is associated with both religious commit- psychological harm and threats to social relationships, including
ment and psychological well-being. However, these relationships threats to social status (Price, Gardner, & Erickson, 2004; Price et
may be more complex for religious individuals who may have al., 2007). Threats to social status entail threats to self-concept and
doubts about their religious faith (Krause, Ingersoll-Dayton, Elli- self-esteem.
son, & Wulff, 1999; Krause & Wulff, 2004). With this in mind, the ETAS theory proposes that beliefs about the world influence
current paper explores the relationships among religious commit- psychiatric symptoms by modulating the sensitivity of threat as-
ment, the belief there is meaning and purpose in life, and mental sessment systems or the threshold of what constitutes a threat. In
health. either case, beliefs can inhibit or facilitate threat assessments. In so
doing, religious and secular beliefs about the world can enhance or
ameliorate psychiatric symptoms.
Theoretical Perspective A basic aspect of ETAS theory and related theories of human
People have various beliefs that guide their daily lives: “that the alarm (Eisenberger et al., 2003), security (Woody & Szechtman,
world is reasonably fair, that they are good people, that bad things 2011), and threat (Eilam et al., 2011) systems has been incorpo-
don’t happen to good people, and that God is good and is looking rated into research on meaning and purpose in the form of the
after them and protecting them” (Park, 2005, p. 298). These kinds “hostile world scenario” (Shmotkin & Shrira, 2012; Shrira, Palgi,
of general, or global beliefs, are considered to be an essential Ben-Ezra, & Shmotkin, 2011). The hostile world scenario is said
element of meaning-making (Steger & Park, 2012). Evolutionary “to function as a system of appraisal that scans for any potential
Threat Assessment Systems (ETAS) theory provides a biologically negative condition” (Shmotkin & Shrira, 2012, p. 146). According
plausible, causal mechanism by which such beliefs can directly to Shmotkin and Shrira (2012), the system is adaptive in keeping
affect mental health. individuals vigilant about potential danger, but it can also generate
ETAS theory proposes that beliefs about the world can directly “a continuous sense of survivorship in a disastrous world” (Shmot-
influence psychological well-being, specifically psychiatric symp- kin & Shrira, 2012, p. 146). Shmotkin and Shrira (2012) believe
toms (Flannelly, Koenig, Galek, & Ellison, 2007). The theory is the positive bias of meaning systems counteract the negative
based, in part, on evolutionary or Darwinian psychiatry, and cog- perspective of the hostile world scenario by offering favorable, and
RELIGION, MEANING AND PURPOSE, AND MENTAL HEALTH 5

sometimes unreasonably positive, interpretations of adverse life (Moore & Gee, 2003); paranoia (Fenigstein & Vanable, 1992);
events. obsession (Kaplan, 1994); and compulsion (Kaplan, 1994).
The root question was: “Over the past month, how often have
Hypotheses you . . .” The response options were: Never ⫽ 0; Rarely ⫽ 1;
Sometimes ⫽ 2; Often ⫽ 3; and Very Often ⫽ 4. The three items
The present study was designed, therefore, to test the degree to for each symptom class were: Generalized Anxiety (␣ ⫽ .84)—
which the belief that life lacks meaning and purpose moderates the Felt nervous, anxious or on edge, Unable to stop or control
association of religious commitment on psychiatric symptoms, worrying, Worried too much about different things; Social Anxiety
using data from the 2010 Baylor Religion Survey. The Baylor data
(␣ ⫽ .82)—Feared that you might do something to embarrass
also afford the opportunity to test other hypotheses based on the
yourself in a social situation, Became anxious doing things be-
foregoing review of the literature, using a large nationwide sample
cause people were watching, Endured intense anxiety in social or
of U.S. adults. The following hypotheses were tested. First, reli-
performance situations; Paranoia (␣ ⫽ .77)—Felt like you were
gious commitment has a significant salutary association with psy-
being watched or talked about by others, Felt that it is not safe to
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

chological well-being—specifically, psychiatric symptoms in the


trust anyone, Felt that people were taking advantage of you;
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

present study. Second, the belief that life lacks meaning and
purpose has a significant pernicious association with psychiatric Obsession (␣ ⫽ .76)—Been plagued by thoughts or images that
symptoms. Third, religious commitment has a significant salutary you cannot get out of your mind, Thought too much about things
association with psychiatric symptoms, controlling for beliefs that would not bother other people, Thought too much about
about meaning and purpose. Fourth, the belief that life lacks pointless matters; Compulsion (␣ ⫽ .77)—Felt compelled to per-
meaning and purpose mediates the association of religious com- form certain actions for no justifiable reason, Repeated simple
mitment with psychiatric symptoms. Fifth, religious commitment actions that realistically did not need to be repeated, Been afraid
interacts with the belief that life lacks meaning and purpose in their something terrible would happen if you did not perform certain
net associations on psychiatric symptoms— our central hypothesis. rituals.
Specifically, we hypothesize that believing life lacks meaning and The variable for each class of symptoms consisted of the aver-
purpose will have a more pernicious effect on highly religious age of the three items comprising that class. The five measures
individuals than it will on individuals who are less religious. Sixth, were developed by the authors of the current study specifically for
both religious commitment and the belief that life lacks meaning the 2010 Baylor Religion Survey.
and purpose interact with race on their respective associations with Independent variables. The 2010 Baylor Religion Survey
psychiatric symptoms. In particular, it is hypothesized that the (BRS) contained three questions about meaning and purpose in
salutary association of religious commitment will be significant for life: (a) I do not believe there is any ultimate meaning in life, (b)
African Americans (but not for Caucasians), and that the perni- It is useless to try to discover the purpose of my life, and (c) I
cious association of believing life lacks meaning and purpose will seldom think about the meaning of life. The first two questions
be significant for Caucasians (but not for African Americans). were selected for use in the present study to measure the belief that
life lacks existential meaning (r ⫽ .43; p ⬍ .001, ␣ ⫽ .60);
Method specifically, that participants did not believe that meaning was
present in their lives (Steger et al., 2006). The four response
Participants categories were: Strongly disagree ⫽ 1; Disagree ⫽ 2; Agree ⫽ 3;
Strongly agree ⫽ 4. The third question was not used because its
A national survey was conducted for the Baylor Religion Survey interpretation was considered to be ambiguous. For instance, one
in November 2010 by the Gallup Organization. Random digit might not think about meaning in life because: (a) one has found
dialing was used to contact a sample of 7,000 U.S. adults who were meaning, already; (b) one does not consider meaning to be impor-
asked to participate in a survey. Approximately 2,500 of those who tant; or (c) one does not believe meaning exists.
were contacted were subsequently mailed the survey question- Two of the BRS questions were used to measure religious
naire, of which 1,714 returned the survey. A number of survey commitment (␣ ⫽ .82). The first question, “How religious do you
respondents were eliminated from the final analyses due to missing consider yourself to be?” had four response categories: Not at all
data. Sixty-five of the surveys were excluded due to missing data
religious ⫽ 1; Not too religious ⫽ 2; Somewhat religious ⫽ 3;
on the demographic variables. Seventy-seven surveys were ex-
Very religious ⫽ 4. The second question, “How often do you
cluded because they were missing data on various independent
attend religious services?” had eight category responses ranging
variables, and 119 surveys were excluded due to missing data on
from Never ⫽ 0 to Several times per week ⫽ 8. Responses to each
the dependent variables. The final analyses included responses
question were converted to standardized scores and combined,
from 1,453 participants.
with the average of the two scores forming the variable religious
commitment.
Measures Covariates. The control variables were age, gender, race,
Dependent variables. Five classes of psychiatric symptoms: education, and marital status. Gender was recoded as Female (1 vs.
(general anxiety, social anxiety, paranoia, obsession, and compul- 0), race was coded as Caucasian (1 vs. 0) and African American (1
sion), were measured by three items each. The items were adapted vs. 0), and marital status was recoded as Married (1 vs. 0).
from existing scales in the public domain: general anxiety Education was measured on a 7-point scale from 8th grade or
(Kroenke, Spitzer, Williams, & Lowe, 2009); social anxiety less ⫽ 1 to Postgraduate work/Degree ⫽ 7.
6 GALEK, FLANNELLY, ELLISON, SILTON, AND JANKOWSKI

Analysis Plan Bivariate Correlations


Bivariate and univariate analyses were performed on all of the The intercorrelations among the demographic variables were
variables. A series of Ordinary Least Squares (OLS) regression generally low, but there were a number of notable correlations
analyses were conducted to test the hypotheses. A preliminary between the demographic and independent variables. Age was
regression analysis was conducted to predict the belief that life positively related to religious commitment, r ⫽ .19, p ⬍ .001 as
lacks meaning from religious commitment for use in the mediation well the belief that life lacks meaning and purpose, r ⫽ .11, p ⬍
analyses to test Hypothesis 4 (see below). .001. Females, r ⫽ ⫺.12, p ⬍ .001 and better educated partici-
Model 1 regressed each of the five dependent variables (general pants, r ⫽ ⫺.11, p ⬍ .001 were less likely to hold that belief.
anxiety, social anxiety, paranoia, obsession, and compulsion) on Religious commitment was inversely related to the belief that
the demographic control variables (age, female, Caucasian, Afri- life lacks meaning and purpose, r ⫽ ⫺.35, p ⬍ .001. Religious
can American, education, and married) in five separate analyses. commitment also had small, but significant, negative correlations
Model 2 regressed each psychiatric symptom on religious com- with all five dependent variables: general anxiety, r ⫽ ⫺.10, p ⬍
mitment and the control variables to test Hypothesis 1. Model 3 .001, social anxiety, r ⫽ ⫺.05, p ⬍ .05, paranoia, r ⫽ ⫺10, p ⬍
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

regressed each psychiatric symptom on belief in meaning and .001, obsession, r ⫽ ⫺.08, p ⬍ .01, and compulsion, r ⫽ ⫺.06,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

purpose in life and the control variables to test Hypothesis 2. p ⬍ .05. In contrast, believing that life lacks meaning and purpose
Model 4 regressed each psychiatric symptom on religious com- had small, but significant, positive correlations with four of the
mitment, belief in meaning and purpose in life and the covariates five dependent variables: social anxiety, r ⫽ .07, p ⬍ .05, para-
to test Hypothesis 3. Mediation was analyzed using the Sobel- noia, r ⫽ .08, p ⬍ .05, obsession, r ⫽ .07, p ⬍ .05, and compul-
Goodman test to test Hypothesis 4 (Sobel, 1982). sion, r ⫽ .13, p ⬍ .001.
Model 5 tested Hypothesis 5 that religious commitment will
interact with the belief that life lacks meaning to produce their net OLS Regression Analyses
effects on the dependent variables. Model 5 was identical to Model The regression analysis predicting psychiatric symptoms from
4, except it included the interaction of religious commitment and the six demographic variables (Model 1) found that three of them
meaning and purpose. had significant negative associations with all five classes of symp-
Prior to calculating statistical interactions, the raw scores for toms: age (␤s ⫽ ⫺.070 to ⫺.116), education (␤s ⫽ ⫺.131
belief in meaning and purpose were zero-centered to minimize the to ⫺.241) and being married (␤s ⫽ ⫺.102 to ⫺.216). Being
collinearity of the product terms, as recommended by Aiken and female had a significant positive relationship with general anxiety
West (1991). The scores for religious commitment were already (␤ ⫽ .199) and a significant negative relationship with compulsion
zero-centered because they were standardized scores. The interac- (␤ ⫽ ⫺.066). Being Caucasian also had a significant negative
tion term was created by multiplying the zero-centered scores for relationship with compulsion (␤ ⫽ ⫺.119). No other demographic
religious commitment and belief in meaning and purpose in life. variables were significantly related to the psychiatric symptoms.
Interaction terms were also created for the interaction of Cau- Table 1 presents the results of the four models testing the six
casian with religious commitment, Caucasian with meaning and hypotheses (i.e., Models 2–5). Although religious commitment had
purpose, African American with religious commitment, and Afri- a significant negative association with all of the dependent vari-
can American with meaning and purpose in life. Models 6 –9 ables in the bivariate analyses, it only had a significant negative
separately tested the association of these interactions with each of association with general anxiety (␤ ⫽ ⫺.053) when entered into
the dependent variables. Models 6 –9 were identical to Model 4 Model 2 along with the six demographic variables. These results
except for the inclusion of the interaction terms. provide support for Hypothesis 1 only with respect to general
anxiety. They do not support the overall proposition of Hypothesis
Results
1, that religious commitment has a salutary association with psy-
Descriptive Statistics chiatric symptomology, after controlling for the demographic vari-
ables.
The levels of the five classes of psychiatric symptomology were The belief that life lacks meaning and purpose, which had a
quite low among the survey participants. General anxiety had the significant positive association with four of the five dependent
highest average score, with a mean of 1.16 (SD ⫽ 0.90), which lies variables in the bivariate analyses, exhibited the same patterns of
between rarely and sometimes on the 0 – 4 scale. The average associations in the multivariate regression analyses performed in
scores of the other four classes of psychiatric symptoms fell Model 3. These results provide support for Hypothesis 2, that
between never and rarely: Social Anxiety (M ⫽ 0.67, SD ⫽ 0.75); believing life lacks meaning has a pernicious association with
Paranoia (M ⫽ 0.75, SD ⫽ 0.67); Obsession (M ⫽ 0.95, SD ⫽ psychiatric symptoms (excluding general anxiety).
0.77); Compulsion (M ⫽ 0.35, SD ⫽ 0.59). As found in Model 2, the only significant association that
The average age of participants was nearly 56 years, slightly religious commitment had with any of the dependent variables in
more than half of the respondents were women (53%), and nearly Model 4 was its negative association with general anxiety. There-
two thirds (64%) were married. Most of the sample was Caucasian fore, the results of Model 4 undermine Hypothesis 3, that religious
(86%) or African American (8%). The median level of education commitment will have a salutary association with psychiatric
was “some college” and approximately 39% had earned a college symptoms, controlling for beliefs about meaning in life. Sobel-
or advanced degree. Close to 80% of the sample was Christian, 2% Goodman mediation tests were conducted for all five dependent
were Jewish, and just over 11% said they had no religious affili- variables despite the fact that the results essentially made them
ation. unnecessary. The tests provided no support for Hypothesis 4, that
RELIGION, MEANING AND PURPOSE, AND MENTAL HEALTH 7

Table 1
Estimated Net Effects of Religion Commitment, the Belief That Life Lacks Meaning, and Their Interaction on Psychiatric Symptoms
(Models 2– 4)

General anxiety Social anxiety Paranoia Obsession Compulsion

Model 2
Religious commitment ⫺.053ⴱ (⫺.055) ⫺.014 (⫺.018) ⫺.041 (⫺.049) ⫺.029 (⫺.034) ⫺.019 (⫺.030)
Intercept .722 .711 1.036 .836 .649
Adjusted R2 .047 .038 .089 .041 .044
Model 3
Belief that life lacks meaning and purpose .049 (.037) .072ⴱ (.064) .070ⴱ (.060) .076ⴱ (.066) .100ⴱⴱⴱ (.113)
Intercept .766 .683 .977 .790 .610
Adjusted R2 .046 .042 .094 .044 .055
Model 4
Religious commitment ⫺.046ⴱ (⫺.048) .007 (.009) ⫺.025 (⫺.030) ⫺.009 (⫺.010) .010 (.016)
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Belief that life lacks meaning and purpose .025 (.019) .076ⴱ (.067) .056 (.049) .071ⴱ (.062) .106ⴱⴱⴱ (.120)
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Intercept .766 .527 .900 .663 .393


Adjusted R2 .047 .041 .950 .043 .055
Model 5
Religious commitment ⫺.057ⴱ (⫺.058) .000 (.000) ⫺.032 (⫺.039) ⫺.015 (⫺.018) .008 (.013)
Belief that life lacks meaning and purpose .039 (.029) .086ⴱⴱ (.076) .066ⴱ (.057) .080ⴱ (.069) .109ⴱⴱⴱ (.123)
Religious commitment ⫻ Belief life lacks
meaning .143ⴱⴱⴱ (.104) .098ⴱⴱⴱ (.084) .097ⴱⴱⴱ (.082) .084ⴱⴱ (.071) .033 (.037)
Intercept .809 .550 .922 .683 .401
Adjusted R2 .057 .047 .096 .047 .055
Note. Values are unstandardized parameter estimates and (standardized estimates, ␤s). All the regression models control for age, gender, education,
marital status, Caucasian and African American.

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.

lack of meaning mediates the association between religious com- uals who were low in religious commitment exhibited comparable
mitment and general anxiety or any of the other dependent vari- levels of social anxiety. regardless of their belief in meaning and
ables. purpose in life. As hypothesized, individuals who were high in
The belief that life lacks meaning and purpose exhibited the religious commitment but low in their belief that there is meaning
same patterns of associations with psychiatric symptoms in the and purpose in life had higher levels of social anxiety. Moreover,
interaction model (Model 5) that it did in the main effects models. this group had appreciably higher levels of social anxiety than
Model 5 also found significant interactions of religious commit- religiously committed individuals who had a strong belief in
ment with the belief that life lacks meaning and purpose for meaning and purpose in life. The pattern of the interaction effects
general anxiety, social anxiety, paranoia, and obsession. was nearly identical for obsession (see Figure 2), but differed
The predicted interaction effects (Hypothesis 5), were evident somewhat for paranoia (see Figure 3). General anxiety and com-
for general anxiety, social anxiety, paranoia, and obsession. The pulsion exhibited broadly similar patterns, but in the case of
interaction effect is best illustrated for social anxiety, obsession compulsion, the interaction effect was not statistically significant.
and paranoia, as seen in Figure 1, Figure 2, and Figure 3. Social Models 6 –9 (not shown) provided no support for Hypothesis 6
anxiety showed the most profound effect (see Figure 1). Individ- that race would interact with religious commitment, belief in
meaning and purpose in life, and with their respective associations

0.70 Lack of
Meaning 0.80
Lack of
Social Anxiety

Meaning
0.60 0.75
High +1 SD
Obsession

Mean 0.70 High +1 SD

Low -1 SD Mean
0.50 0.65 Low -1 SD

0.60

0.40 0.55
-1 SD Mean +1 SD -1 SD Mean +1 SD
Religious Commitment Religious Commitment

Figure 1. Interaction effect of religious commitment and the belief that Figure 2. Interaction effect of religious commitment and the belief that
life lacks meaning and purpose on social anxiety. life lacks meaning and purpose on obsession.
8 GALEK, FLANNELLY, ELLISON, SILTON, AND JANKOWSKI

1.05 meaning and purpose in life interacts with religious commitment


Lack of on their net associations with psychiatric symptoms. As hypothe-
Meaning
sized, highly religious individuals who believed their lives lacked
0.95 meaning and purpose experienced more frequent symptoms of
High +1 SD
Paranoia

social anxiety, paranoia, and obsession than individuals who were


Mean less religious or highly religious individuals who believed their
Low -1 SD lives have meaning and purpose. To our knowledge, the present
0.85
study is one of only a few studies specifically designed to analyze
the relationship between lack of meaning in life and mental health
0.75 outcomes. We will return to these findings shortly to place them in
a theoretical perspective.
-1 SD Mean +1 SD
Several theoretical propositions bear on our interpretation of the
Religious Commitment
current results. First, religious beliefs form the heart of all religions
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(Stark & Glock, 1968). Second, religious belief systems are main-
Figure 3. Interaction effect of religious commitment and the belief that
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

life lacks meaning and purpose on paranoia. tained through religious commitment, which is expressed, in part,
by engaging in religious practices, such as attending religious
services. Collectively, religious convictions and practices foster
with the dependent variables. No significant interaction effect was the belief there is meaning and purpose in life.
found for either Caucasians or African Americans. The “hostile world scenario” (Shmotkin & Shrira, 2012; Shrira
et al., 2011) is similar to ETAS theory in that it proposes a
vigilance system that scans for potential threats of harm in the
Discussion
physical and social environment. While this vigilance system is
The present study contributes to our understanding of the rela- adaptive, its focus on adversity in the world can have negative
tionship between religion and meaning and purpose in regard to effects on psychological well-being, according to Shmotkin and
mental health in several ways. The use of a large, nationwide Shrira (2012). Shmotkin and Shrira propose that the negative bias
sample of adults allowed us to test the generality of findings from of the vigilance system can be off-set by the positive bias of
previous studies that employed convenience samples. The results meaning systems, which provide favorable interpretations of ad-
confirm Hypothesis 1 and previous research findings that religious verse life events.
commitment has a salutary association with psychological well- Whereas the hostile world scenario asserts that the focus of the
being, in that religious commitment had a significant inverse vigilance system on adversity can have negative consequences for
association with all five classes of psychiatric symptoms examined psychological well-being, ETAS theory specifically proposes that
in the current study. In addition, the results are consistent with psychiatric symptoms, such as those studied here, are the direct
prior research that meaning and purpose in life have a salutary result of threat assessments by neural circuits in the brain. More-
association with psychological well-being, since lack of meaning over, ETAS theory provides a plausible biological mechanism
and purpose was found to be directly related to psychiatric symp- through which global beliefs about the world, which are central to
tomology in the current study. Specifically, the results provide meaning making, can directly affect psychiatric symptoms.
strong support for Hypothesis 2, that lack of meaning and purpose To the degree that meaning in life counters the negative conse-
has a pernicious association with mental health, in that it had a quences of recognizing the threats posed by the world, lack of
significant positive association with four of the five classes of meaning should exacerbate these negative consequences. Thus, as
psychiatric symptoms. Hypothesis 2 states, the belief that life lacks meaning and purpose
The results do not the support Hypothesis 3 or Hypothesis 4. should have significant pernicious associations with psychiatric
Hypothesis 3 predicted that the salutary association of religious symptoms. Hence, ETAS theory and the hostile world scenario
commitment with psychiatric symptoms would be statistically may account for our findings that lack of meaning is associated
significant after controlling for beliefs about meaning and purpose, with higher levels of psychiatric symptoms.
but this was true for only one of the five classes of symptoms. Religions are inherently social systems (Berger, 1967), and
Hypothesis 4 stated that the association of religious commitment religion provides many people with their core identity or role (Park
with psychiatric symptoms is mediated by beliefs about meaning & Edmondson, 2012). Therefore, based on Identity theory, we
and purpose in life, but this is not the case in our sample. There- hypothesized that religiously committed people who believe life
fore, the present results do not support research on college students lacks meaning and purpose would have higher psychiatric symp-
that found the salubrious association between religion and psycho- toms because it undermines their religious beliefs and, therefore,
logical well-being was mediated by meaning in life (Steger & threatens their social role within the religious community. Such a
Frazier, 2005). The results also undermine research findings on threat should increase certain psychiatric symptoms, according to
college students that religiousness (but not meaning and purpose) ETAS theory. As predicted by Hypothesis 5, individuals who
has a salutary association with psychiatric symptomology among reported the highest levels of religious commitment were the most
African Americans, whereas meaning and purpose (but not reli- troubled, scoring higher on four of the five classes of psychiatric
giousness) has a salutary association with psychiatric symptomol- symptoms.
ogy among Caucasians (Holmes & Hardin, 2009). The belief that life lacks meaning is akin to other forms of
The results provide strong support for the central hypotheses of religious doubt, or spiritual struggles, which have been found to
the current study (Hypothesis 5). Hypothesis 5 stated that belief in have a pernicious association with mental health, especially among
RELIGION, MEANING AND PURPOSE, AND MENTAL HEALTH 9

individuals with strong religious commitment (Ellison et al., 2013; As with any cross-sectional study, it is impossible to ascribe
Krause, 1994, 1998; Krause & Wulff, 2004). However, since causal influences about the directionality of the observed associ-
individuals who have other forms of religious doubt can still derive ations. Hence, it is not possible to say whether religious commit-
meaning and purpose from other sources, the belief that life lacks ment and believing in meaning and purpose in life lead to psycho-
meaning and purpose may have a more profound effect on mental logical well-being, or whether psychological well-being leads to
health than do other forms of struggles and doubt (Krause & religious commitment and the belief that there is meaning and
Ellison, 2009). purpose in life. Moreover, though this study is based on a large
From the perspective of ETAS theory, the belief that life lacks nationwide sample of U.S. adults, since the survey had a relatively
meaning and purpose should facilitate threat assessments that low response rate it may not be entirely representative of the U.S.
cause psychiatric symptoms for a number of reasons. As men- population.
tioned already, belief in meaning and purpose is proposed to Finally, there are two conceptual issues that should be men-
counter the negative consequences of threat assessments that the tioned. First, the operational definition of meaning and purpose
world is a dangerous place to live. This may be because belief in used in the study employed only two questions, whereas Krause
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

meaning and purpose in life encompasses other positive, global (2007a, 2007b) has operationalized meaning in life into four di-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

beliefs about oneself, others, and the world at large. In any case, mensions measured by an array of items. Second, although we
since the consequences of threat assessments are, in many cases, discuss the interpretation of the results from the perspective of
psychiatric symptoms, the lack of belief in meaning and purpose ETAS theory, the study does not directly test ETAS theory and the
contributes to an increase in psychiatric symptoms. According to results to do not directly support ETAS theory.
ETAS theory, lack of meaning would reduce the threshold of what Despite these limitations, we believe the results of this study
constitutes a threat, thereby increasing psychiatric symptomology. make a major contribution to our understanding of the relation-
Religious identity provides a source of self-esteem (Park & ships among religion, belief in meaning and purpose in life, and
Edmondson, 2012), which is one of Baumeister’s (1991) four basic mental health outcomes. In particular, the results point to the
needs for meaning. The belief that life lacks meaning probably importance of examining the extent to which the association be-
undermines one’s identity as a religious person and threatens tween religion and mental health is dependent upon additional
self-esteem. This perceived threat to social status and self-esteem variables. The results further contribute to our understanding of
is reflected in higher levels of both social anxiety and paranoia, how religious beliefs may enhance or ameliorate psychiatric symp-
which according to ETAS theory are increased by neural assess- toms.
ments that elements of the social world pose a threat of harm. Overall, the results of the current study add to a growing body
How does ETAS theory explain the other interaction effects? of research that provide either direct or indirect support for ETAS
Since obsession-compulsion is also thought to be triggered by theory, including studies of the association between psychiatric
social threats (Baxter, 2003; Gilbert, 2001), a perceived threat to symptoms and specific beliefs about God (Ellison et al., 2013;
Flannelly, Galek, Ellison, & Koenig, 2010; Silton, Flannelly,
social status and/or self-esteem could account for the observed
Galek, & Ellison, 2013), life after death (Flannelly, Ellison, Galek,
interaction effects for obsession, as well. Although compulsion
& Koenig, 2008), and doubts about one’s religious faith (Ellison et
exhibited an interaction effect that was generally similar to that of
al., 2013).
obsession, its pattern was not sufficiently pronounced to be statis-
Future research should continue to test the salutary and perni-
tically significant. The nonsignificant finding for compulsion was
cious associations between psychological well-being and religious,
probably due, at least in part, to its very low rate of occurrence
spiritual, and secular beliefs. It would be particularly valuable for
among survey participants.
future research on beliefs about existential meaning to examine the
Gilbert (2001) contends that most psychiatric symptoms, includ-
factors that foster or deter such beliefs.
ing general anxiety, are produced by social threats. Price and other
theorists also think general anxiety can be produced by social
threats (Price et al., 2004). To the degree this is true, this could References
explains why general anxiety is also associated with threats to
social status and self-esteem in the current study. From the per- Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regressions; Testing and
interpreting interactions. London, UK: Sage.
spective of ETAS theory, the belief that life lacks meaning and
Baumeister, R. F. (1991). Meanings of life. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
purpose may facilitate threat assessments about the dangerousness Baxter, L. R. (2003). Basal ganglia systems in ritualistic social displays:
of the future and the world at large, which could increase general Reptiles and humans; function and illness. Physiology & Behavior, 79,
anxiety. Given that feelings of efficacy reflect another dimension 451– 460. doi:10.1016/S0031-9384(03)00164-1
of meaning (Baumeister, 1991), doubts about one’s efficacy to Beck, A. T. (1987). Cognitive models of depression. Journal of Cognitive
deal with challenges to one’s faith may further heighten general Psychotherapy, 1, 5–37.
anxiety. This could help explain why the interaction effect was Beck, A. T. (2008). The evolution of the cognitive model of depression and
strongest for general anxiety. its neurobiological correlates. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 165,
969 –977. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08050721
This study has a number of limitations. For example, Krause and
Beck, A. T., Butler, A. C., Brown, G. K., Dahlsgaard, K. K., Newman,
Wulff (2004) suggest that religious doubts are likely to result from
C. F., & Beck, J. S. (2001). Dysfunctional beliefs discriminate person-
particularly severe events (e.g., serious illness or injury, loss of a ality disorders. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39, 1213–1225. doi:
loved one), which undermine one’s belief that life is meaningful. 10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00099-1
Unfortunately, the survey did not collect information about these Beck, A. T., Emery, G., & Greenberg, R. L. (1985). Anxiety disorders and
types of events. phobias: A cognitive perspective. New York, NY: Basic Books.
10 GALEK, FLANNELLY, ELLISON, SILTON, AND JANKOWSKI

Beck, A. T., & Rector, N. A. (2002). Delusions: A cognitive perspective. Gilbert, P. (1984). Depression: From psychology to brain state. Hillsdale,
Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 16, 455– 468. doi:10.1891/jcop.16 NJ: Erlbaum.
.4.455.52522 Gilbert, P. (2001). Evolutionary approaches to psychopathology: The role
Berger, P. L. (1967). The sacred canopy: Elements of sociological theory. of natural defences. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry,
New York, NY: Doubleday. 35, 17–27. doi:10.1046/j.1440-1614.2001.00856.x
Bergner, R. M. (1998). Therapeutic approaches to problems of meaning- Harlow, L. L., Newcomb, M. D., & Bentler, P. M. (1986). Depression,
lessness. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 52, 72– 87. self-derogation, substance use, and suicide ideation: Lack of purpose in
Burke, P. J. (1991). Identity process and social stress. American Sociolog- life as a mediational factor. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 42, 5–21.
ical Review, 56, 836 – 849. doi:10.2307/2096259 doi:10.1002/1097-4679(198601)42:1⬍5::AID-JCLP2270420102⬎3.0
Chamberlain, K., & Zika, S. (1988). Religiosity, life meaning and wellbe- .CO;2-9
ing: Some relationships in a sample of women. Journal for the Scientific Hicks, J. A., Cicero, D. C., Trent, J., Burton, C. M., & King, L. A. (2010).
Study of Religion, 27, 411– 420. doi:10.2307/1387379 Positive affect, intuition, and feelings of meaning. Journal of Personality
Clark, W. H. (1958). The psychology of religion. New York, NY: Mc- and Social Psychology, 98, 967–979. doi:10.1037/a0019377
Millan. Hicks, J. A., & King, L. A. (2008). Religious commitment and positive
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Crandall, J. E., & Rasmussen, R. D. (1975). Purpose in life as related to mood as information about meaning in life. Journal of Research in
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

specific values. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 31, 483– 485. doi:10.1002/ Personality, 42, 43–57. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2007.04.003
1097-4679(197507)31:3⬍483::AID-JCLP2270310326⬎3.0.CO;2-C Hicks, J. A., Trent, J., Davis, W. E., & King, L. A. (2012). Positive affect,
Eilam, D., Izhar, R., & Mort, J. (2011). Threat detection: Behavioral meaning in life, and future time perspective: An application of socio-
practices in animals and humans. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Re- emotional selectivity theory. Psychology and Aging, 27, 181–189. doi:
views, 35, 999 –1006. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.08.002 10.1037/a0023965.
Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Williams, K. D. (2003). Does Hinds, A. L., Woody, E. Z., Drandic, A., Schmidt, L. A., Van Ameringen,
rejection hurt? An FMRI study of social exclusion. Science, 302, 290 – M., Coroneos, M., & Szechtman, H. (2010). The psychology of potential
292. doi:10.1126/science.1089134 threat: Properties of the security motivation system. Biological Psychol-
Ellison, C. G., Fang, Q., Flannelly, K. J., & Steckler, R. A. (2013). Spiritual ogy, 85, 331–337. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.08.003
struggles and mental health: Exploring the moderating effects of reli- Holmes, J. D., & Hardin, S. I. (2009). Religiosity, meaning in life, and
gious identity. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 23, clinical symptomology: A comparison of African-American and
214 –229. doi:10.1080/10508619.2012.759868 European-American college students. Journal of College Student Psy-
Fenigstein, A., & Vanable, P. A. (1992). Paranoia and self-consciousness. chotherapy, 23, 103–117. doi:10.1080/87568220902743199
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 129 –138. doi: Jackson, L. E., & Coursey, R. D. (1988). The relationship of God control
10.1037/0022-3514.62.1.129 and internal locus of control to intrinsic religious motivation, coping and
Flannelly, K. J., Ellison, C. G., Galek, K., & Koenig, H. G. (2008). Beliefs purpose in life. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 27, 399 –
about life-after-death, psychiatric symptomology and cognitive theories 410. doi:10.2307/1387378
of psychopathology. Journal of Psychology & Theology, 36, 94 –103. Jewell, A. (2010). The importance of purpose in life in an older British
Flannelly, K. J., Ellison, C. G., Galek, K., & Silton, N. R. (2012). Belief in Methodist sample: Pastoral implications. Journal of Religion, Spiritual-
life-after-death, beliefs about the world, and psychiatric symptoms. ity & Aging, 22, 138 –161. doi:10.1080/15528030903321170
Journal of Religion & Health, 51, 651– 662. doi:10.1007/s10943-012- Kaplan, S. L. (1994). A self-rated scale for obsessive-compulsive disorder.
9608-7 Journal of Clinical Psychology, 50, 564 –574. doi:10.1002/1097-
Flannelly, K. J., Galek, K., Ellison, C. G., & Koenig, H. G. (2010). Beliefs 4679(199407)50:4⬍564::AID-JCLP2270500411⬎3.0.CO;2-F
about God, psychiatric symptoms, and evolutionary psychiatry. Journal King, L. A., & Hicks, J. A. (2012). Positive affect and meaning in life. The
of Religion & Health, 49, 246 –261. doi:10.1007/s10943-009-9244-z intersection of hedonism and eudiamonia. In P. T. P. Wong (Ed.), The
Flannelly, K. J., Koenig, H. G., Galek, K., & Ellison, C. G. (2007). Beliefs, human quest for meaning: Theories, research, and application (2nd ed.,
mental health, and evolutionary threat assessment systems in the brain. pp. 125–142). New York, NY: Routledge; Taylor & Francis.
Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 195, 996 –1003. doi:10.1097/ Kinnier, R. T., Metha, A. T., Keim, J. S., & Okey, J. L. (1994). Depression,
NMD.0b013e31815c19b1 meaninglessness, and substance abuse in “normal” and hospitalized
Francis, L. J. (2000). The relationship between bible reading and purpose adolescents. Journal of Alcohol and Drug Education, 39, 101–111.
in life among 13–15-year-olds. Mental Health, Religion & Culture, 3, Klinger, E. (2012). The search for meaning in evolutionary goal-theory
27–36. doi:10.1080/13674670050002072 perspective and its clinical implications. In P. T. P. Wong (Ed.), The
Francis, L. J., & Burton, L. (1994). The influence of personal prayer on human quest for meaning: Theories, research, and application (2nd ed.,
purpose in life among Catholic adolescents. Journal of Beliefs and pp. 23–56). New York, NY: Routledge; Taylor & Francis.
Values, 15, 6 –9. doi:10.1080/1361767940150202 Krause, N. (1994). Stressors in salient social roles and well-being in later
Francis, L. J., & Evans, T. E. (1996). The relationship between personal life. Journal of Gerontology, 49, P137–P148. doi:10.1093/geronj/49.3
prayer and purpose in life among churchgoing and non-churchgoing .P137
twelve-to-fifteen-year-olds in the UK. Religious Education, 91, 8 –21. Krause, N. (1998). Stressors in highly valued roles, religious coping, and
doi:10.1080/0034408960910102 mortality. Psychology and Aging, 13, 242–255. doi:10.1037/0882-7974
Francis, L. J., Jewell, A., & Robbins, M. (2010). The relationship between .13.2.242
religious orientation, personality, and purpose in life among an older Krause, N. (2003). Religious meaning and subjective well-being in late
Methodist sample. Mental Health, Religion & Culture, 13, 777–791. life. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and
doi:10.1080/13674670802360907 Social Sciences, 58, S160 –S170. doi:10.1093/geronb/58.3.S160
Frankl, V. (1992). Man’s search for meaning: An introduction to logo- Krause, N. (2007a). Evaluating the stress-buffering function of meaning in
therapy. Boston, MA: Beacon Press. (Originally published in 1946) life among older people. Journal of Aging and Health, 19, 792– 812.
Fry, P. S. (2000). Religious involvement, spirituality and personal meaning doi:10.1177/0898264307304390
for life: Existential predictors of psychological wellbeing in community- Krause, N. (2007b). Longitudinal study of social support and meaning in
residing and institutional care elders. Aging & Mental Health, 4, 375– life. Psychology and Aging, 22, 456 – 469. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.22.3
387. doi:10.1080/713649965 .456
RELIGION, MEANING AND PURPOSE, AND MENTAL HEALTH 11

Krause, N., & Ellison, C. G. (2009). The doubting process: A longitudinal meaning. In P. T. P. Wong (Ed.), The human quest for meaning:
study of the precipitants and consequences of religious doubt in older Theories, research, and application (2nd ed., pp. 277–296). New York,
adults. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 48, 293–312. doi: NY: Routledge; Taylor & Francis.
10.1111/j.1468-5906.2009.01448.x Price, J. S., Gardner, R., Jr., & Erickson, M. (2004). Can depression,
Krause, N., Ingersoll-Dayton, B., Ellison, C. G., & Wulff, K. M. (1999). anxiety and somatization be understood as appeasement displays? Jour-
Aging, religious doubt, and psychological well-being. The Gerontolo- nal of Affective Disorders, 79, 1–11. doi:10.1016/S0165-
gist, 39, 525–533. doi:10.1093/geront/39.5.525 0327(02)00452-4
Krause, N., & Wulff, K. M. (2004). Religious doubt and health: Exploring Price, J. S., Gardner R., Jr., Wilson, D. R., Sloman, L., Rohde, P., &
the potential dark side of religion. Sociology of Religion, 65, 35–56. Erickson, M. (2007). Territory, rank and mental health: The history of an
doi:10.2307/3712506 idea. Evolutionary Psychology, 5, 531–554.
Kroenke, K., Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B. W., & Lowe, B. (2009). An Reker, G. T., & Wong, P. T. (1988). Aging as an individual process:
ultra-brief screening scale for anxiety and depression: The PHQ-4. Toward a theory of personal meaning. In J. E. Birren & V. L. Bengston
Psychosomatics, 50, 613– 621. (Eds.), Emergent theories of aging (pp. 214 –246). New York, NY:
MacLean, P. D. (1972). Cerebral evolution and emotional processes: New Springer.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

findings on the striatal complex. Annals of the New York Academy of Robbins, M., & Francis, L. J. (2000). Religion, personality, and well-being:
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Sciences, 193, 137–149. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1972.tb27830.x The relationship between church attendance and purpose in life. Journal
MacLean, P. D. (1985). Evolutionary psychiatry and the triune of Research on Christian Education, 9, 223–238. doi:10.1080/
brain. Psychological Medicine, 15, 219 –221. doi:10.1017/ 10656210009484908
S0033291700023485 Ryff, C. D., & Keyes, C. L. M. (1995). The structure of psychological
Marks, I. M., & Nesse, R. M. (1994). Fear and fitness: An evolutionary well-being revisited. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69,
analysis of anxiety disorders. Ethology & Sociobiology, 15, 247–261. 719 –727. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.4.719
doi:10.1016/0162-3095(94)90002-7 Scannell, E. D., Allen, F. C. L., & Burton, J. (2002). Meaning in life and
Mascaro, N., & Rosen, D. H. (2005). Existential meaning’s role in the positive and negative well-being. North American Journal of Psychol-
enhancement of hope and prevention of depressive symptoms. Journal ogy, 4, 93–112.
Shmotkin, D., & Shrira, A. (2012). On the distinction between subjective
of Personality, 73, 985–1014. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00336.x
well-being and meaning in life. In P. T. P. Wong (Ed.), The human quest
Maslow, A. H. (1943). A theory of human motivation. Psychological
for meaning: Theories, research, and application (2nd ed., pp. 143–
Review, 50, 370 –396. doi:10.1037/h0054346
163). New York, NY: Routledge; Taylor & Francis.
Maslow, A. H. (1958). A dynamic theory of human motivation. In C. L.
Shrira, A., Palgi, Y., Ben-Ezra, M., & Shmotkin, D. (2011). How subjec-
Stacey & M. DeMartino (Eds.), Understanding human motivation (pp.
tive well-being and meaning in life interact in the hostile world? The
26 – 47). Cleveland, OH: Howard Allen. doi:10.1037/11305-004
Journal of Positive Psychology, 6, 273–285. doi:10.1080/17439760
McAdams, D. P. (2012). Meaning and personality. In P. T. P. Wong (Ed.),
.2011.577090
The human quest for meaning: Theories, research, and application (2nd
Silton, N. R., Flannelly, K. J., Ellison, C. G., Galek, K., Jacobs, M. R.,
ed., pp. 107–123). New York, NY: Routledge; Taylor & Francis.
Marcum, J. P., & Silton, F. (2011). The association between religious
McDonald, M. J., Wong, P. T. P., & Gingras, D. T. (2012). Meaning-in-life
beliefs and practices and end-of-life fears among members of the Pres-
measures and the development of a brief version of the personal meaning
byterian Church (U.S.A). Review of Religious Research, 53, 357–370.
profile. In P. T. P. Wong (Ed.), The human quest for meaning: Theories,
doi:10.1007/s13644-011-0015-4
research, and application (2nd ed., pp. 357–382). New York, NY:
Silton, N. R., Flannelly, K. J., Galek, K., & Ellison, C. G. (2013). Beliefs
Routledge; Taylor & Francis.
about God and mental health among American adults. Journal of Reli-
Moore, K. A., & Gee, D. L. (2003). The reliability, validity, discriminant gion & Health. Advance online publication. doi:10.1007/s10943-013-
and predictive properties of the social phobia inventory (SoPhi). Anxiety, 9712-3
Stress & Coping, 16, 109 –117. doi:10.1080/1061580021000057068 Silton, N. R., Flannelly, L. T., Flannelly, K. J., & Galek, K. (2011). Toward
Nesse, R. (1998). Emotional disorders in evolutionary perspective. British a theory of holistic needs and the brain. Holistic Nursing Practice, 25,
Journal of Medical Psychology, 71, 397– 415. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8341 258 –265. doi:10.1097/HNP.0b013e31822a0301
.1998.tb01000.x Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in
Newcomb, M. D., & Harlow, L. L. (1986). Life events and substance use structural equation models. In S. Leinhardt (Ed.), Sociological method-
among adolescents: Mediating effects of perceived loss of control and ology (pp. 290 –312). Washington DC: American Sociological Associ-
meaninglessness in life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, ation.
51, 564 –577. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.51.3.564 Soderstrom, D., & Wright, E. W. (1977). Religious orientation and mean-
Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and ing in life. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 33, 65– 68. doi:10.1002/
animal emotions. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. 1097-4679(197701)33:1⫹⬍65::AID-JCLP2270330113⬎3.0.CO;2-0
Panksepp, J. (2005). Affective consciousness: Core emotional feelings in Sommer, K. L., Baumeister, R. F., & Stillman, T. F. (2012). The construc-
animals and humans. Consciousness and Cognition, 14, 30 – 80. doi: tion of meaning from life events: Empirical studies of personal narra-
10.1016/j.concog.2004.10.004 tives. In P. T. P. Wong (Ed.), The human quest for meaning: Theories,
Pargament, K. I. (1997). The psychology of religion and coping: Theory, research, and application (2nd ed., pp. 297–314). New York, NY:
research, practice. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Routledge; Taylor & Francis.
Park, C. L. (2005). Religion and meaning. In R. F. Paloutzian & C. L. Park Spilka, B., Hood, R. W., Hunsberger, B., & Gorsuch, R. (2003). The
(Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of religion and spirituality (pp. psychology of religion: An empirical approach (3rd ed.). New York,
295–314). New York, NY: Guilford Press. NY: Guilford Press.
Park, C. L., & Edmondson, D. (2012). Religion as a source of meaning. In Stark, R., & Glock, C. Y. (1968). American piety. The nature of religious
P. R. Shaver & M. Mikulincer (Eds.), Meaning, mortality, and choice: commitment. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
The social psychology of existential concerns (pp. 145–162). Washing- Steger, M. F. (2012). Experiencing meaning in life: Optimal functioning at
ton, DC: American Psychological Association. the nexus of well-being, psychopathology, and spirituality. In P. T. P.
Peterson, C., & Park, N. (2012). Character strengths and the life of Wong (Ed.), The human quest for meaning: Theories, research, and
12 GALEK, FLANNELLY, ELLISON, SILTON, AND JANKOWSKI

application (2nd ed., pp. 165–182). New York, NY: Routledge; Taylor tory. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 819 – 833. doi:10.1016/j.brat
& Francis. .2005.06.001
Steger, M. F., & Frazier, P. (2005). Meaning in life: One link in the chain Wong, P. T. P. (1998). Spirituality, meaning, and successful aging. In
from religiousness to well-being. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 52, P. T. P. Wong (Ed.), The human quest for meaning: A handbook of
574 –582. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.52.4.574 psychological research and clinical applications (2nd ed., pp. 359 –394).
Steger, M. F., Frazier, P., Oishi, S., & Kaler, M. (2006). The meaning in Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
life questionnaire: Assessing the presence of and search for meaning in Wong, P. T. P. (2010). What is existential positive psychology? Interna-
life. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 53, 80 –93. doi:10.1037/0022- tional Journal of Existential Psychology and Psychotherapy, 3, 1–10.
0167.53.1.80 Wong, P. T. P. (2011). Positive psychology 2.0: Towards a balanced
Steger, M. F., & Kashdan, T. B. (2013). The unbearable lightness of interactive model of the good life. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie
meaning: Well-being and unstable meaning in life. The Journal of Canadienne, 52, 69 – 81. doi:10.1037/a0022511
Positive Psychology, 8, 103–115. doi:10.1080/17439760.2013.771208 Wong, P. T. P. (2012a). Introduction: A roadmap for meaning research and
Steger, M. F., Kashdan, T. B., Sullivan, B. A., & Lorentz, D. (2008). application. In P. T. P. Wong (Ed.), The human quest for meaning:
Understanding the search for meaning in life: Personality, cognitive Theories, research, and application (2nd ed., pp. xxix–xlvi). New York,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

style, and the dynamic between seeking and experiencing meaning. NY: Routledge; Taylor & Francis.
Wong, P. T. P. (2012b). Towards a dual-systems model of what makes life
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Journal of Personality, 76, 199 –228. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2007


.00484.x worth living. In P. T. P. Wong (Ed.) The human quest for meaning:
Steger, M. F., & Park, C. L. (2012). The creation of meaning following Theories, research, and application (2nd ed., pp. 3–22). New York, NY:
trauma: Meaning making and trajectories of distress and recovery. In Routledge; Taylor & Francis.
R. A. McMackin, E. Newman, J. M. Fogler, & T. M. Keane (Eds.), Woody, E. Z., & Szechtman, H. (2011). Adaptation to potential threat: The
Trauma therapy in context: The science and craft of evidence-based evolution, neurobiology, and psychopathology of the security motivation
practice (pp. 171–191). Washington, DC: American Psychological As- system. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 1019 –1033. doi:
sociation. doi:10.1037/13746-008 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.08.003
Thoits, P. A. (1991). On merging identity theory and stress research. Social Zika, S., & Chamberlain, K. (1992). On the relation between meaning in
Psychology Quarterly, 54, 101–112. doi:10.2307/2786929 life and psychological well-being. British Journal of Psychology, 83,
Thoits, P. A. (2012). Role-identity salience, purpose and meaning in life, 133–145. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1992.tb02429.x
and well-being among volunteers. Social Psychology Quarterly, 75,
360 –384. doi:10.1177/0190272512459662 Received January 31, 2012
Wenzel, A., Sharp, I. R., Brown, G. K., Greenberg, R. L., & Beck, A. T. Revision received April 4, 2014
(2006). Dysfunctional beliefs in panic disorder: The Panic Belief Inven- Accepted May 12, 2014 䡲

View publication stats

Você também pode gostar