Você está na página 1de 16

J Contemp Psychother (2009) 39:171–186

DOI 10.1007/s10879-008-9087-z

ORIGINAL PAPER

Clients’ Experiences of Disengaged Moments in Psychotherapy:


A Grounded Theory Analysis
Ze’ev Frankel Æ Heidi M. Levitt

Published online: 16 September 2008


 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2008

Abstract The study offers an empirically derived model the frequent, manifold and unavoidable sensations of
of clients’ experiences of disengaged moments, defined as pain and unpleasure the removal and avoidance of
moments when clients withdraw, distance, or lessen their which is enjoined by the pleasure principle, in the
intensity of involvement with therapy-relevant material or exercise of its unrestricted domination. (Freud 1930/
relationships. Interpersonal Process Recall (IPR) interviews 1959)
were conducted with nine psychotherapy clients of thera-
Emotional processing is stressed as an important component
pists with varied orientations and experience. The
of the therapeutic change process by all of the three major
interviews were transcribed verbatim and subjected to
psychotherapy schools—psychodynamic (Freud 1959/
qualitative analysis using grounded theory analysis (Glaser
1914; Fromm-Reichmann 1950; Gabbard 2004; Messer
and Strauss, The discovery of grounded theory: Strategies
and Wolitzky 1997; Vakoch and Strupp 2000), humanistic
for qualitative research, 1967) methodology. The results
(Greenberg and Pascual-Leone 1995; Greenberg et al. 1998;
suggested that clients use disengagement for protection
Rogers 1951), and cognitive-behavioral (Foa and Kozak
from pain, strategic retreat, relationship management, self-
1986; Rachman 1980)—and has been supported by empir-
through-other discovery, and self-betterment. The core
ical findings as well (Elliott et al. 2004; Hoehn-Saric 1974;
category across all of the experiences of disengagement
Kamphuis and Telch 2000; Pos et al. 2003; Rauch and Foa
relates to communicating difficulties and experimenting
2006). Nonetheless, the opposite phenomenon of emotional
within the therapy relationship toward self-change. Rec-
disengagement, although not an infrequent event in psy-
ommendations are offered for research and practice.
chotherapy, has received little attention. The present study
aims to contribute to this area of psychotherapy process
Keywords Disengagement  Resistance  Psychotherapy 
research, by investigating clients’ experiences of disen-
Qualitative research  Grounded theory
gaged moments, defined as moments when clients
withdraw, distance, or lessen their intensity of involvement
with therapy-relevant material or relationships.
Introduction

Client Disengagement in Psychotherapy


A further incentive to a disengagement of the ego
from the general mass of sensations…is provided by
Four research areas in psychotherapy specifically have
emphasized the importance of disengagement. Research on
Z. Frankel (&) client resistance, storytelling, secrecy, and silences have
The University of Memphis, Memphis, TN 38152, USA each described points in therapy during which processes of
e-mail: zfrankel@memphis.edu disengagement occur. These occurrences are by no means
mutually exclusive; in fact they can overlap a great deal.
H. M. Levitt
Department of Psychology, The University of Memphis, However, each area of research has been explored indi-
202 Psychology Building, Memphis, TN 38152, USA vidually in the psychotherapy literature.

123
172 J Contemp Psychother (2009) 39:171–186

Resistance a significant and moderate relationship between alliance


and outcome (Orlinsky et al. 2004). More specifically,
Reviews of psychotherapy outcome research consistently however, the findings raise significant questions about what
have found clients’ resistance in therapy, as opposed to therapists are to do when clients emotionally disengage
collaboration or compliance, to be negatively associated from the exploration, experiencing, and experimenting that
with psychotherapy outcome (Beutler et al. 2004; Beutler so often comprise the work of therapy.
et al. 2002; Clarkin and Levy 2004; Orlinsky et al. 2004).
In psychotherapy process research the findings have been Storytelling and Reporting
less clear cut, however. For instance, Patterson and Forg-
atch (1985) found that resistance measured in family A narrative view of human experience in psychology has
behavior therapy sessions was significantly and positively become more popular over the past 20 years, in which
related to counselors’ ‘‘teaching’’ and ‘‘confronting’’ people are thought to generate stories of their lives and
behaviors, while it was significantly negatively related to their surroundings in their search for coherence and
counselors’ ‘‘supportive’’ and ‘‘facilitative’’ behaviors. meaning (Angus et al. 1999; McAdams 1997; Monk et al.
Chamberlain et al. (1984) found that resistance occurred 1997; Neimeyer 2000; Polkinghorne 1988). In addition to
significantly more during the middle phase (‘‘teaching’’) of these potentially productive functions of narrative, psy-
family behavior therapy, among premature terminators, chotherapy researchers and clinicians also have identified
and among those clients referred by external agencies. styles of storying that can obstruct the processes of psy-
Bischoff (1997) assessed the relationship between client chotherapy. For example, Hailparn and Hailparn (1999)
resistance and therapist directiveness and found a signifi- described ‘‘reporting’’ as a powerful defense against dis-
cant but small positive relationship between therapist closing or addressing information that would be
directiveness and client resistance, replicating to some experienced as fear inducing or shameful. This avoidance
degree the earlier findings of Patterson and colleagues is accomplished by talking at length about those issues the
(1985; Chamberlain et al. 1984), but also found that this client feels safer to verbalize during the therapy session.
relationship was moderated by clients’ levels of reactance They accordingly warn that such incidents can get in the
and satisfaction. Clients with low reported satisfaction way of therapeutic gains and recommend keen observation
consistently exhibited resistance in the face of therapist and subsequent intervention to return the clients to the
directiveness, regardless of their level of reactance. Among substantive issues of therapy.
clients with higher levels of reported satisfaction, those Similarly, Rennie (1994) conducted a qualitative anal-
clients with higher levels of reactance were more likely ysis of storytelling and found that clients can utilize
than lower reactance clients to exhibit resistance when storytelling to avoid personal discomfort or uneasiness. He
therapists were directive. found that some clients told stories as a way to delay
In addition, Rice (1999) assessed the relationship of talking about a disturbing subject. Other clients used sto-
client distress, client–therapist alliance, and client aware- rytelling as a way to evade the feelings associated with an
ness of self and other, differentiating five objects of upsetting topic. Storytelling and reporting, then, has been
resistance. She found that lower other-awareness was studied as narrative means of self construction and defini-
related significantly to resistance to therapist; lower self- tion as well as strategic ways of disengaging from
awareness was related significantly to resistance to recol- emotionally charged issues.
lect material; client–therapist alliance was negatively
related to resistance to expressing painful affect, while the Secrecy
alliance subscale of task alliance was negatively related to
resistance to change; and distress was unrelated to any Secrecy had been theorized to correlate negatively with
form of resistance. Weller (2003) explored the relationship therapeutic gains as well. For instance, the Preoccupation
between resistance and early client psychotherapy dropout Model of Secrecy (Lane and Wegner 1995) predicts that
and found that clients who dropped out early had both engaging in thought suppression in order to keep a secret
higher proportions of resistance behaviors (48.2% vs. will lead paradoxically to greater intrusion of the very
34.4%) and more intense manifestations of resistance. thought one is trying to suppress. Although Finkenaeur and
Although these findings are not readily consolidated, Rime (1998) found no direct effect of emotional secrecy on
they seem to broadly suggest that increasing the overall subjective well being; they found an indirect effect via the
therapeutic alliance will help to reduce client resistances of negative impact of secrecy on physical health.
all sorts, both toward the therapist and toward themselves. In contrast, Kelly (1998) found that keeping secrets was
This general finding is consistent with one of the most actually a predictor of positive outcomes among outpa-
robust findings in psychotherapy outcome research to date: tients at a community hospital. She explains that clients

123
J Contemp Psychother (2009) 39:171–186 173

come to see themselves in a more favorable light by that poor outcome client-centered therapy dyads did indeed
withholding negative personal information and helping exhibit significantly more disengaged silences than good
their therapists to view them favorably. Such positive outcome dyads while the good outcome dyads exhibited
outcome findings have not gone without debate. Hill et al. more productive silences than the poor outcome dyads.
(2000), upon reviewing findings on non-disclosure in These findings demonstrated the utility of differentiating
psychotherapy, concluded that clients do not withhold a types of silence, and underscored the salience of emotional
great deal of information and that the relationship between disengagement in psychotherapy.
such withholding and outcome is not clear. In addition, Common to the research of resistance, secrecy, story-
Farber et al. (2006) found that while clients may struggle telling, and silence in psychotherapy are the recent findings
somewhat with disclosure, they consider it to be a valuable that differentiate multiple forms of each. No longer can we
component of psychotherapy. conceive of these events in psychotherapy as homogeneous
Indeed, Kelly et al. (2001) found that revealing secrets occurrences, but rather as complex multifaceted phenom-
can in fact be beneficial, under certain conditions. Their ena that may be beneficial when prompted by some
study was designed to ascertain what differentiates revela- motivations but detrimental when motivated by others. For
tions of secrets that lead to positive and to negative feelings this reason it is important that the exploration of disen-
in a non-clinical sample. They found that after subjects gaged moments be undertaken to deepen our understanding
anonymously revealed their secrets in writing while trying of the multiple processes and constructions involved, to
to gain new insight they reported feeling better than those identify therapy events associated with disengagement, and
simply attempting catharsis by letting the secret out. Fur- to develop better strategies for handling their occurrences
ther, they found that not coming to terms with the secret was in therapy. While past research suggests a negative corre-
associated with bad feelings about the secret. Together, lation between disengaged moments and psychotherapy
these findings suggest that secrecy may be a heterogeneous outcome, this study aims to develop a theory of their inner
variable that impacts change depending on both its mode of workings to guide the development of testable hypotheses
expression and context, and direct our attention to both the and intervention strategies. The qualitative approach uti-
helpful and harmful potential of disengagement. lized allows for rigorous inquiry into subjective experience
and facilitates the development of empirically-grounded
Silence theory.

Within the last decade researchers have begun to investi-


gate the effects of silence within psychotherapy (e.g., Hill Method
et al. 2003; Ladany et al. 2004; Levitt 2001a, b; 2002).
These empirical findings together with the theoretical lit- Participants
erature (e.g. Greenson 1961; Trad 1993; Zeligs 1961)
suggest that while some silences may alert therapists and Clients and therapists were recruited within three different
researchers to processes potentially hindering progress in settings—community psychologists and psychiatrists, a
psychotherapy, other silences may signal engagement in university psychological services center, and a university
change and growth. counseling center. Participant diversity is a strength in this
This heterogeneous understanding of silences was fur- qualitative design as it can allow for the study of variation
thered by Levitt (1998, 2001a, b, 2002; H.M. Levitt and Z. of experiences and the development of a more encom-
Frankel, Unpublished manuscript), who conducted a passing theory. As a result, diversity was sought in
grounded theory analysis (Glaser and Strauss 1967) of therapists’ gender, experience, training background, set-
clients’ experiences of silent processes in session, and ting, and therapeutic orientation and in clients’ gender, age,
systematically identified seven different types of silences. and length of therapy. Descriptions of therapist and client
The silent moments were conceptualized heuristically as participants are presented in Table 1.
falling within three categories: Productive Silences, Neu-
tral Silences, and Obstructive Silences. The Obstructive Therapists
Silences category was comprised of two types of silences:
disengaged and interactional. Disengaged silence was Eight therapists were recruited by word of mouth for this
associated with avoiding emotion or withdrawing. Inter- study, one of whom worked with two clients in the study.
actional silence entailed considering communication with They included two licensed clinical psychologists, one
the therapist or safeguarding the alliance. Frankel and licensed counseling psychologist, one board certified psy-
colleagues (Frankel 2004; Frankel et al. 2006), upon testing chiatrist, and four advanced doctoral students in clinical
the hypotheses laid out in Levitt’s silence typology, found psychology meeting clinical practicum requirements.

123
174 J Contemp Psychother (2009) 39:171–186

Table 1 Description of participants and their Therapists


# Gender Age Range Ethnicity Length Therapist Primary orientation

1 Female 50–60 Caucasian 2 Years 4th-Year student, male* Constructivist


2 Female 40–50 Caucasian 1.5 Years 4th-Year student, male* Constructivist
3 Female 50–60 Caucasian 6 Months 5th-Year student, female Cognitive behavioral
4 Male 30–40 Caucasian 2 Years 4th-Year student, male Cognitive behavioral
5 Female 30–40 Caucasian 2 Years 3rd-Year student, male Process-experiential
6 Female 20–30 Caucasian 3 Months Licensed Ph.D., female Feminist
7 Female 20–30 Caucasian 6 Months Licensed Ph.D., female Process-experiential
8 Male 50–60 Caucasian 2 Years Licensed Ph.D., female Cognitive behavioral
9 Female 30–40 Caucasian 4 Years Licensed M.D., male Unified therapy
* The same therapist worked with two clients

Five theoretical orientations of psychotherapy were rep- Disorder. Interviewees were paid $20 to compensate them
resented by the therapists: Cognitive Behavioral Therapy; for their time and participation.
Constructivist Therapy; Feminist Therapy; Process-Expe-
riential Therapy; and Unified Therapy. Cognitive behavioral Researchers
therapy (Beck 1975) works to dispel cognitions and
behavioral contingencies that are understood to lead to and The interviews were conducted, transcribed, and analyzed
maintain distress by the use of Socratic questioning and by the first author, who received training in qualitative
homework assignments. Constructivist psychotherapy interviewing and analysis as well as ethics training about
(Mahoney 2003) integrates techniques from various psy- interviewing by the second author. The interviews and
chotherapy schools within a constructivist philosophy analyses were reviewed and discussed with the second
with the goal of enhancing meaning making and reducing author until consensus regarding the findings was reached.
suffering. Feminist therapy (Brown 1994) is a feminist-
theory-based integrative therapy that focuses on the ineq- Procedures
uitable societal influences on personal meaning making.
Process-experiential therapy (Greenberg et al. 1993) is a Interpersonal Process Recall Interviews
contemporary emotion-focused therapy with humanist
underpinnings, which combines client-centered relationship IPR interviews entailed the interviewees reviewing a
conditions and experiential exercises in a process-directive recording of a psychotherapy session to cue their cognitive
manner toward understanding and changing emotional and affective memory, pausing the recording at moments
schemas. Unified therapy was developed specifically for that they identified as disengagement, and exploring those
patients with personality disorders by David Allen (1993), occurrences therein with the interviewer (Kagan 1975).
and integrates elements of psychodynamic, family systems, The reflective distance inherent in the process of describing
and cognitive behavioral therapies. their experiences in the session, and the supportive envi-
ronment of the interview acted to reduce defensiveness
Interviewees about their experiences. This method has been found
effective in stimulating the recollection of the clients’
Interviewees were recruited through therapists. Intervie- experiences during the phenomenon of interest (Elliott
wees admissible for this study were 18 year-old and older 1986; Levitt 2002; Rennie 1994), and has been empirically
clients in ongoing outpatient psychotherapies who did not demonstrated to result in experiences and descriptions
exhibit any psychotic symptoms and were not judged to be highly correlated with those experienced in-session (Gott-
at high risk for suicide or violence by their therapists, nor man and Levenson 1985). Video recordings and/or audio
in the first two or last two scheduled sessions of therapy. recordings were used for the review, as dictated by each
The sample reflected some diversity, including clients with setting.
a range of ages from 20s to 60s, professionals and non- To minimize influencing the recorded therapy session,
professionals, in short-term and long-term psychotherapies, interviewees were not told prior to the interview the exact
who described themselves variously as being depressed, focus of the interview, only that it would explore their
anxious, having interpersonal difficulties, suffering with experiences of the session. The interview therefore began
fibromyalgia, and suffering with Borderline Personality with an orientation to the concept of disengagement,

123
J Contemp Psychother (2009) 39:171–186 175

described broadly as lessening involvement in therapy minimize any biasing effect they might have on the inter-
discussion or withholding from the therapist. The goal of viewing and analytic process, in order to keep the
this method was to explore what the client actually was conceptualization of the phenomenon closely tied to the
experiencing in the recorded session rather than how they data and to keep a careful record of the procedures and
felt as they reviewed it. For this reason, interviewees were theoretical developments of the analysis.
directed not to reinterpret the session but to describe what
took place in the moment under investigation (Rennie Credibility Checks
1992).
The interviews were semi-structured, with questions Consistent with the aim of grounded theory, to generate
directed to clarify clients’ experiences preceding, during, rigorous theory grounded in the phenomenon of interest,
and following disengaged moments. While the interviewer three types of credibility checks were conducted. (1) At the
had general statements and questions prepared to guide the end of each interview interviewees were asked about the
IPR process (see Appendix A for interview questions), information gathered to assess whether the data collection
questions were modified and added as needed to fully was complete and representative. (2) Consensus with the
explore clients’ experiences. Questions were designed to be second author was sought, as consensus has been noted to
open-ended and as non-leading as possible. strengthen the credibility of research findings, demon-
strating agreement regarding the interpretation of data from
Data Analysis multiple analysts (Hill et al. 2005). (3) After the model of
disengagement was completed participants were contacted
Grounded theory analysis (Glaser and Strauss 1967) was and asked to review a summary of the results of the study
chosen as the method of analysis for this study in an and to provide feedback via a questionnaire. The ques-
attempt to generate rigorous and empirically-based theory tionnaire summarized the findings and asked participants to
based on the data derived from the interviews. The version rate how well each cluster and the core category matched
of grounded theory used in this analysis was based upon their experiences on a 7-point Likert scale, ranging from 1
that suggested by Rennie et al. (1988). (‘‘does not fit at all’’) to 7 (‘‘fits very much’’), as well as the
The transcribed data collected in the interviews was degree to which the overall summary matched and
divided into meaning units (MUs). MUs are segments of contradicted (1 = ‘‘does not contradict at all’’; 7 = ‘‘con-
texts that each contain one main idea (Giorgi 1970). In the tradicts very much’’) their experiences. Eight participants
initial stages of the analysis, the MUs were labeled in a provided feedback on the summary of the disengagement
manner that remained very close to the language used by model via the feedback questionnaire. The one remaining
the participants. The MUs were compared with each other participant had moved and could not be located. Two
and organized according to their similarities, creating participants left one question each unanswered (one left the
descriptive categories. MUs could be assigned to more than overall summary questions unanswered and the other left
one category, as dictated by the meanings contained the cluster 5 question unanswered). Participants indicated
therein. Once the initial descriptive categories were cre- the overall summary matched their experiences of disen-
ated, a more abstract conceptualization of the phenomenon gagement to a high degree (M = 6.42). Participants
was pursued by grouping the descriptive categories into similarly indicated that the overall summary contradicted
higher order categories based on their commonalities. This their experiences to a low degree (M = 1.42).
process was repeated and, in this way, a hierarchy of cat-
egories was developed. At the top of the hierarchy one core
category was formed that represented the central interpre- Results
tation drawn from the analysis.
The process of collecting data continued until the cate- The nine in-depth interviews yielded 241 single-spaced
gories became ‘‘saturated,’’ that is new descriptive pages of transcripts, resulting in 833 meaning units related
categories did not appear to be present in additional to clients’ experiences of disengagement in psychotherapy.
interviews. This analysis became saturated at the sixth To differentiate the levels of the higher order categories the
interview—meaning that the last three interviews did not following lexicon will be used, the three top levels of
add any new categories and that the analysis appeared to be which are depicted in the hierarchical presentation of the
comprehensive. model in Fig. 1. The level above the fundamental meaning
A detailed record of the researcher’s intuitions, suspi- units included ‘‘subcategories,’’ which were followed by
cions, feelings and thoughts about the interviews and ‘‘categories’’ of common experiences, above which were
analysis was maintained throughout the study. By noting central ‘‘clusters.’’ The core category, representing the
these assumptions via memoing, an attempt was made to overarching principle of the model, was referred to as the

123
176 J Contemp Psychother (2009) 39:171–186

Fig. 1 Hierarchical
presentation of the three top Disengagement as
levels of the model of client Communicative experimentation toward self-change:
disengagement and the number Reconfiguring self-advancement and self-protection (9)
of participants contributing to
each category
A reflex averting A deliberate Selective disclosure of A retrospective testing A conscious disruption
attention from sensitive disconnection from personal experience in of how the therapist of self-impeding
topics in order to emotions to allow full order to establish and responds to avoidance patterns toward the
protect oneself from focus on rationally maintain a safe and vulnerability (9) goal of sustained
potential pain (9) developing strategies therapeutic self-esteem,
toward resolving relationship, in which effectiveness,
problems (9) the client feels accepted and hopefulness (9)
and trusting of the
therapy process (9)

Keeping it pleasant: Moderating distress Assessment of client Observable Avoiding the dark
Avoiding toward continued acceptability and disruption and point of no return:
uncomfortable exploration of self-determination redirection: Preventing
feelings (8) sensitive within the Struggling to depression and
experiences: therapeutic remain composed in worsening life
Constructive affect relationship: Am I the presence of the functioning (6)
regulation (8) really OK just as I therapist (9)
am? (6)

Rapidly shifting Retention of reason: Facilitating Reflecting lack of Maintaining belief


behavior, speech, I’m going to lose it communication and self-confidence and in self: Improving
and mood: I can't if I don’t stop this understanding with insufficient personal self-image and self-
stay here one second (9) therapist: Do you resilience: I can’t esteem (9)
longer (7) get me? (6) handle this (9)

Unconsciously Maintaining clarity Responding to Maintaining self in Maintaining belief


preventing loss of and realistic therapist when little familiar form: I am in the world:
control: I'm not perspective: understanding, this and nothing else Invoking hope for
sure how it happens, Promoting support, and safety (8) better future (6)
like a reflex just objectivity towards conveyed: If you
shutting it down (8) increased insight (6) can't handle this I
won't talk about it
(9)
Recruitment of Forging a new way
available resources: of being: Let's try
Maximizing this another way (6)
therapeutic
potential (8)

‘‘core category.’’ The 5-level hierarchy of disengagement Likert scale where 1 = ‘‘does not fit at all’’ to 7 = ‘‘fits
experiences decreased in the number of units or categories very much’’). The three categories of experience that
at each level; from meaning units (N = 833), to subcate- contributed to this first cluster are described below.
gories (N = 48), to categories (N = 17), to clusters
(N = 5), which are all crowned by the core category Disengagement as Keeping it Pleasant: Avoiding
(N = 1). The five central clusters with their constituent Uncomfortable Feelings
categories will first be presented, followed by the core
category. The participant, from whom illustrative quotes Eight participants described disengaging at times by
originated, will be indicated by a number parenthetically steering the conversation toward positive elements of
following each quote (i.e., P#). experience when something uncomfortable or threatening
seemed to be looming in the horizon of the conversation.
Cluster 1. Disengagement as a Reflex, Averting
It’s just like, sometimes [at a choice point about the
Attention from Sensitive Topics in order to Protect
depth of discussion] you can like sense that things are
Oneself from Potential Pain
an issue, but you just, they’re not an issue yet because
you haven’t really like felt the bad feelings that you
All nine participants contributed to the first cluster, which
think you’re going to feel if you think about them.
included three categories of experience. Participants
(P7)
reported experiencing disengagement as a reprieve from
anticipated discomfort that seemed to happen automati- Similarly, they reported disengaging to forestall discussion
cally, without their conscious control. Eight participants of experiences that they anticipated could result in being
provided feedback on this cluster via the questionnaire and criticized or invalidated by the therapist. For example, two
indicated a high level of agreement (M = 6, on a 7-point participants described this experience very pointedly:

123
J Contemp Psychother (2009) 39:171–186 177

‘‘He’s gonna be like ‘She’s in denial!’ [laughs vigorously]’’ Cluster 2. Disengagement as a Deliberate
(P5), and ‘‘I don’t want to get into a confrontation [with Disconnection from Troublesome Emotions to Allow
therapist]’’ (P9). Thus clients reported using disengage- Full Focus on Rationally Developing Strategies
ment to avoid discomfort arising both intrapersonally and and Improved Functioning
interpersonally.
The second cluster was endorsed by all nine participants.
Disengagement Achieved by Rapidly Shifting Behavior, The participants reported withdrawal from intense emotion
Speech, and Mood: I Can’t Stay Here One Second Longer in order to better equip themselves for overcoming the
associated difficulties. Eight participants provided feed-
Seven participants described disengaging by switching back on this cluster via the questionnaire and indicated a
their focus to alternate activities in order to escape dis- moderate-high degree of agreement (M = 5.9, on a 7-point
comfort. Six of these participants disengaged by changing Likert scale where 1 = ‘‘does not fit at all’’ to 7 = ‘‘fits
the topic of discussion. very much’’). The four categories of experience that con-
tributed to this cluster are described below.
When I’m trying to keep that mask up, um, what
happens is that my story is very disjointed, I will
Disengagement as Moderating Distress Toward Continued
jump from situation to situation, I will talk about so
Exploration of Sensitive Experiences: Constructive Affect
and so did such and such to me, I’ll play the victim
Regulation
role. And then I’ll bring in story after story as to how
I’m the victim, and I tend to stay there. (P2)
Eight participants described disengagement as a process
Six interviewees described disengaging by reducing their that allowed them to continue to broach therapeutic but
verbal and non-verbal expressiveness, for example via a threatening topics in this category.
sudden lapse of memory.
I’m trying to like switch it from like an internal thing
I’m sure she’s [family member] made derogatory to like an external kind of situation…I’m switching it
comments, you know, I just couldn’t remember them. from like ‘How do I feel about something?’ to like
Like it’s kind of hard to focus, and…when someone ‘Why did I do this?’ Like it feels more comfortable, I
[therapist] asks you something like that…all of a think, to talk about [the reasons]. (P7)
sudden I get like [disengaged]…it overwhelms again.
In this way participants described using disengagement to
(P6)
continue therapy-relevant discussions without threatening
Thus participants described disengaging by quickly chang- their mood.
ing the focus of their activity to evade discomfort.
Disengagement as Retention of Reason: I’m Going
to Lose it if I Don’t Stop This
Disengagement as Unconsciously Preventing Loss of
Control: I’m Not Sure How it Happens, Like A Reflex
Nine participants described disengaging at times during
Just Shutting it Down
session in an effort to secure their rational capacities.
Within these, six clients discussed disengaging to prevent
Eight participants described disengagement as an uncon-
being emotionally overwhelmed during the session, as one
scious protection against loss of control.
client described succinctly. ‘‘It helps me to keep talking
I’ve dug so deep that I lose my self. I think that to [about other things to prevent] just totally disintegrating as
lose would mean total loss of control, being purely far as emotionally’’ (P3). Similarly, seven interviewees
emotional, getting out of control….That self-protec- described disengaging to prevent losing control of them-
tive mechanism just turns on and it’s just a hum selves or to maintain emotional balance in the session.
upstairs. Nobody’s home. And it happens quick-
I’m not to the point where I can talk about that
ly….It’s almost like somebody’s tripped the breaker
[traumatic] situation so I back up when [therapist]
and I’ve got to go down and turn the breaker back on.
asks me stuff like that—‘wait ta-ta-ta-ta-ta!’—
(P4)
because I don’t want to get too anxious because then
Thus despite considerable determination to work through I’ll really never get where I’m going. Because if he
their difficulties, some participants reported experiencing [therapist] gave me an answer… [to the question]
disengagement as a kind of circuit-breaker that trips when ‘why, what’s the relevance?’…that seemed rational
the specter of loss of control looms. or seemed to make sense then, you know, then I

123
178 J Contemp Psychother (2009) 39:171–186

would uh consider uhh you know attempting to find Cluster 3. Disengagement as the Selective Disclosure
out at a later point, but as I see it I don’t need to know of Personal Experience with the Therapist in Order
that. (P9) to Establish and Maintain a Safe Therapeutic
Relationship, in which One Feels Accepted and
Thus, participants used disengagement to prevent becom-
Trusting of the Therapy Process
ing so distraught that their ability to function in therapy
was disrupted.
The third cluster of disengagement was contributed to by
all nine participants. Participants described disengagement
Disengagement as Maintaining Clarity and Realistic
as related to their gradually sharing experiences with the
Perspective: Promoting Objectivity Towards Increased
therapist in order to develop a mutual relationship of
Insight
respect and fondness. Eight participants provided feedback
on this cluster via the questionnaire and indicated a mod-
Six participants described disengagement as sometimes
erate-high level of agreement (M = 5.125, on a 7-point
directed toward providing a broader understanding of their
Likert scale where 1 = ‘‘does not fit at all’’ to 7 = ‘‘fits
experiences by taking some distance from it. They dis-
very much’’). The three categories that contributed to this
cussed disengaging to facilitate their ability to problem-
cluster are described below.
solve, which three interviewees explicitly described as a
conscious process. ‘‘I [disengaged from my feelings to]
Disengagement as Assessment of Client Acceptability
really try to focus in on what his questions are and what
and Self-Determination within the Therapeutic
he’s after, um, so that I can try to get there’’ (P5). Simi-
Relationship: Am I Really OK Just as I am?
larly, three participants described disengaging to provide
themselves with objective perspective.
Six clients described disengaging at times to assess the
But what it [disengagement] did was it had me…go extent to which therapists would respect their autonomy.
back and look at that insecurity…and amplify my They described disengaging from therapeutic exploration
description of it…so I can look at it more if I pull to test their therapists’ trustworthiness.
back and come at it from a different angle. (P4)
It [the disengagement] was ‘I have to keep up the
In these ways participants described using disengagement mask.’ And it was my way of testing to see ‘Am I
to remain objective and discerning toward mastering their unconditionally accepted?’ It was a process of
difficulties. establishing ‘How safe am I?’ and ‘Is this really a
sacred space?’, ‘Can I trust, how much can I trust this
Disengagement as Recruitment of Available Resources: person?’ And in making that judgment that I can trust
Maximizing Therapeutic Potential this person the flip side of that coin was ‘I can accept
what this [opinion] says if they [therapist] decide I
Eight participants described disengaging at times in order am not normal,’ you know, or ‘my response is not
to rally the necessary resources (e.g., time, focus, support, normal,’ I can hear that from them now. (P2)
stability) to best utilize their therapy experiences. For
Thus participants described disengaging in therapy to
instance, seven clients discussed disengaging to allow time
figure out their role in therapy and their relationship with
for discussion of important topics. Four participants dis-
their therapists.
cussed disengaging in order to garner additional needed
strength to work with difficult material, as illustrated by
Disengagement as Facilitating Communication
one interviewee’s experience.
and Understanding with Therapist: Do you Get me?
I have a need a lot of times when therapy gets really
tense sometimes to step out and get a drink of water Six participants described disengaging in order to make
and it’s not always just, partly it’s because I talk a lot sure their therapists were on the same page with them. Of
and I lose my voice, also it seems to me also like these, four interviewees discussed disengaging in order to
there’s this need when you dig real deep to…kind of provide their therapists with details to facilitate commu-
draw back and…reassure yourself that everything’s nication and understanding. Three participants also
OK. (P4) described disengaging due to their confusion regarding the
therapeutic method being used.
In these ways participants described disengaging to collect
needed resources to better benefit from their therapeutic I’ll [disengage as I] actually want to say [to the
work. therapist] ‘‘why do I need to know that, why do I need

123
J Contemp Psychother (2009) 39:171–186 179

that information? I think I can do this without, I don’t Cluster 4. Disengagement as Allowing a Retrospective
need to know all the gory details, why are you asking Discovery of How the Therapist Responds to One’s
me to get this information?’’—that’s what I feel, you Avoidance and Vulnerability
know. I just, I would just like for him to stop or say,
umm ‘I’m sorry, I guess you’re not ready to talk The fourth cluster was contributed to by all nine partici-
about that yet.’ (P9) pants and included three categories of experience. Eight
participants provided feedback on this cluster via the
As this example reflects, disengagement can serve many
questionnaire and indicated a moderate-high degree of
functions described in this category and the previous one
agreement (M = 5.125, on a 7-point Likert scale where
simultaneously. By pulling back from the discussion with a
1 = ‘‘does not fit at all’’ to 7 = ‘‘fits very much’’). The
vague ‘‘uh huh’’ the client expressed not only her lack of
fourth cluster centers on the expression of difficulties in the
understanding, but also her lack of personal confidence and
presence of an observant other. Participants presumed their
felt power in the relationship, and her bewilderment as to the
disengagements to be apparent to therapists and thereby
therapist’s understanding of her and the therapeutic process.
providing clients with an opportunity to learn how thera-
pists respond to their difficulties. The three categories that
Disengagement as Responding to Therapist when Little contributed to this cluster are described below.
Understanding, Support, and Safety Conveyed: If You
Can’t Handle this I Won’t Talk About it Disengagement as Observable Disruption and Redirection:
Struggling to Remain Composed in the Presence of the
Nine participants described disengagement as stemming in Therapist
part from their understanding that the therapist was not able
or willing to respond to their experience in the way they Meaning units from nine participants were gathered toge-
wanted. Participants described disengaging less under a ther in this category. Eight of these participants reported
number of circumstances: When their therapist demon- that their disengagements were exhibited by becoming
strated empathy (identified in eight participant’s involved in alternate activities.
interviews); when their therapist assisted them in discuss-
I think there is probably, obviously! (laughs) some
ing therapeutically salient experiences (nine participants);
level of anxiety that is there, uh, you know, because
when their therapist provided sufficient structure for their
my stomach just feels about like this (pushes stomach
discussion of important material (three participants); when
in)…the fingers [braiding one over the other] are a
they felt relaxed and at ease in the session (seven partici-
little more revealing [of the anxiety]. (P5)
pants); and when their therapist allowed them to work at
their own pace (seven participants). One client, for exam- Nonetheless, six participants also described their disengage-
ple, exemplified multiple circumstances in one description. ments as displayed in their reduced expressiveness and body
language, as succinctly described by one interviewee. ‘‘I
Umm, I remember her [therapist] being…concerned…
would have to say the less body language I gave out, maybe
and wanting to know more about that [the ‘pushing
the more intense [in struggling to not feel painful emotions] I
on’]. You know, wanting to know what that was about,
was’’ (P2). Thus these disengagements took on a commu-
and…I didn’t call it depression…at that time I was
nicative function as clients expected that their therapists
calling it ‘pushing on’ or something, but yeah…she
could readily observe such disengagements and understand
pursued it…and got me to think about what…was
what underlies them: ‘‘I think maybe there is some, some
causing it. I think she said ‘was it something I was not
feedback that I probably read from Therapist…if she asks
doing or something I was doing?’ and umm I think it
uhh probes deeper into a into a line of questions…I feel like
was something I was not doing, not taking care of
maybe she’s on to something’’ (P8). Clients therefore
myself, but she kind of already knew that. (P8)
closely attended to their therapists’ responses to disengage-
As this example richly reflects, disengagement from express- ments in an effort to better themselves.
ing the full extent of his experience served as a test of the
therapist’s understanding. If the therapist had not demon- Disengagement as Reflecting Lack of Self-Confidence
strated that understanding—as well as the facility to pursue and Insufficient Personal Resilience: I Can’t Handle this
the topic without the client feeling pressured or unsup-
ported—the topic would have been dropped. Regardless of Nine participants described disengaging at times due to self-
the individual concerns, participants typically reported that doubt and feeling inadequate, which they presumed was
understanding, support, and safety were fundamental noticeable to their therapists. Eight of the clients discussed
requirements for them to engage fully in therapy. their disengagements as decreasing over time in therapy,

123
180 J Contemp Psychother (2009) 39:171–186

while three experienced it as decreasing immediately once patterns, clients enacted their difficulties in therapy. Such
they learned it was not an effective strategy in the long run. observable disengagements were seen by clients to provide
These processes are reflected in clients’ reports that disen- therapists with opportunities to view first hand what clients
gagement generally was related to feeling negatively about are struggling with, and thereby provided clients with an
themselves, the environment, and the future. opportunity to learn from their therapists’ responses.
Through this process clients could discover new ways of
I think self worth has a lot to do with what I say and
understanding themselves and responding to themselves
why I say it, and if I disengage in some things, one of
through the therapists’ examples.
the things that I’m thinking is umm ‘Is it really that
important to talk about and like burden this person
Cluster 5. Disengagement as a Conscious Disruption of
with this right now?’ (P6)
Self-Impeding Patterns toward the Goal of Sustained
Similarly, six interviewees described their disengagements Self-Esteem, Effectiveness, and Hopefulness
decreasing as they gained self-compassion and more ways
to self-soothe, while four participants discussed disengage- The fifth cluster was contributed to by all nine participants
ment as decreasing as they learned feared outcome is and included four categories. Seven participants provided
usually worse than actual outcome. These two processes feedback on this cluster via the questionnaire and indicated
were illustrated articulately by one interviewee. a moderate-high level of agreement (M = 5.714, on a 7-
point Likert scale where 1 = ‘‘does not fit at all’’ to
I have learned [from the therapist] how to sort of
7 = ‘‘fits very much’’). This cluster centers on the inten-
soothe, self-soothe, and I can make myself feel better.
tional use of disengagement to implement new knowledge
Umm so I’m not afraid as much. Now sometimes
and skills toward improved functioning outside of therapy.
when I do, when I’m disengaging that’s the old part
The four categories that contributed to this cluster are
of me trying to steer clear of the discomfort, but I’ve
described below.
learned ways to cope that help me, that I realize that
I’m not going to be a mess when I leave my therapy,
Disengagement as Avoiding the Dark Point of No Return:
it’s going to be OK. (P9)
Preventing Depression and Worsening Life Functioning
Thus, clients utilized disengagement in therapy to both
protect themselves from noxious experiences and to recruit Six participants described disengaging sometimes in order
new knowledge from therapists toward better handling to avert the return of depression or other limiting conditions.
them. They all explicitly described disengagement as a way to
prevent loss of ability to function outside of session, while
Disengagement as Maintaining Self in Familiar Form: I am five interviewees discussed disengagement also as a way to
this and Nothing Else avoid becoming depressed. One participant exemplified
both of these processes as he described deliberating the
Eight participants described disengaging at times in order benefit of addressing painful issues in therapy versus the
to prevent the loss of their known world. Seven of these emotional, temporal, and financial costs that could result.
interviewees discussed disengagement as a way to prevent
I don’t want to damage myself by going there [to the
encountering material contrary to previous accounts.
pain] and want to self-destruct… it would be
I don’t, I’m maybe afraid of what he’s going to say is destructive…it would be maybe, not that I wouldn’t be
going to freak me out, you know, the answer to that able to, but, it would be harmful and umm it would be
[question] might be disturb, might disturb me and I’d time, it would be like crushing a plant or something,
rather not think about that. Of course, you know, you know, it would take time to regrow… I’d be uhh
that’s the hard part and therapy is not easy and I know withdrawing and have a uhh loss of confidence. (P8)
that but I’m not just ready to go into, I don’t know if I
Thus participants described using disengagement to limit
ever want to know that [about her family]. (P9)
the potential costs outside of the session of addressing their
Five participants also described disengagement as a way to pain in session.
continue a previous pattern of experience. ‘‘I think in
general whenever I’ve revealed myself and talked about Disengagement as Maintaining Belief in Self: Improving
something that I’m, you know, touchy about, emotional Self-Image and Self-Esteem
about, uhh there’s certainly ample examples of people not
responding the way that I thought they would’’ (P4). Thus, Nine participants described utilizing disengagement toward
by using disengagement to continue their previous life increased morale. In contrast to the category in the third

123
J Contemp Psychother (2009) 39:171–186 181

cluster in which clients disengaged to promote an improved again, hearing things like this [self-criticism], then I
image to their therapist, this category stresses clients’ start that [revised] tape instead of letting the nega-
disengagements toward improving their self-perception. tivity go on and then I substitute that tape for the
Eight clients described disengagement as a way to negativity and I can come to a workable under-
manage self-presentation toward increasing their self- standing. (P2)
esteem. Through a process of disengaging from their less
In these ways participants described using disengagement
prized aspects clients described working to create an
at times to end self-impeding habits and turn over a new
improved self-presentation that was more in line with how
leaf.
they would like to be seen. Similarly, eight participants
described disengagement as a way to prevent being criti-
Core category: Disengagement as Communicative
cized or invalidated and to increase self-worth. ‘‘What if I
Experimentation Toward Self-Change: Reconfiguring
reveal something and my therapist shows any sign of shock
Self-Advancement and Self-Protection
or um reacts in any way um that could be in any way
construed as negative?’’ (P2). Thus participants described
The overarching theme of clients’ experiences of disen-
disengagement as a way to increase their self-worth and
gagement was based upon commonalities among the five
self-assurance within the therapeutic relationship.
central clusters. Eight participants provided feedback via
the questionnaire on the core category and indicated a high
Disengagement as Maintaining Belief in the World:
level of agreement with it (M = 6.375, on a 7-point Likert
Invoking Hope for Better Future
scale where 1 = ‘‘does not fit at all’’ to 7 = ‘‘fits very
much’’). Participants experienced considerable awareness
Six participants described disengaging at times in order to
of their disengagement in psychotherapy, as illustrated in
feel more hopeful about their prospects. They described
clusters 2, 3, 4, and 5—sometimes observing disengage-
disengagement as a way to maintain a positive perspective,
ment ‘‘in the moment,’’ while it occurred, and sometimes
working to sustain a vision of the world that was neither
only noticing it after the fact. In both cases, clients expe-
paralyzing nor Pollyannaish.
rienced the disengagements as generally beneficial. The
I guess at this point I’m just trying to say that the disengagements could be beneficial by allowing some
[long overdue] check came, get over it, you know, I distance and protecting clients from something harmful.
can’t just live in total paranoia and let them [her They could also be beneficial by directing clients to con-
worries] take over my brain and my emotions, and sider how to best negotiate the therapy experience and
just try to think positive that they [her debtors] are moving them toward something more productive. These
going to continue to do the right thing. (P1) disengagements allowed clients to be either self-protecting
or self-advancing in a way that felt useful to them.
Similarly, two interviewees explicitly described disengage-
However, disengagement also was experienced as a
ment as a way to integrate their negative experiences with
hindrance at points. As illustrated in cluster 1, within the
their positive experiences, linking past negative experi-
context of psychotherapy—which clients viewed as a place
ences to positive experiences to remain more hopeful. In
to be completely open, disengagement sometimes made
these ways, participants described disengaging from
them unable to share or discuss thoughts, feelings, and
demoralizing perspectives in order to develop a more
experiences; or unable to act in a way they wish they could
affirming interpretation of the past toward a more encour-
act. In addition, clients sometimes found that the disen-
aging vision of the future.
gagement was involuntary and difficult to curtail at will.
Clients struggled to find ways to be both self-advancing and
Disengagement as Forging a New Way of Being: Let’s Try
self-protective at the same time, but their self-protective
This Another Way
responses could impede their progress in therapy when it
kept them at bay from important aspects of their experience.
Six participants described disengaging at times with an aim
As illustrated in cluster 4, clients sometimes expected
toward self-betterment. They discussed disengagement as a
their therapists were noticing their disengagements during
way to facilitate their problem solving, while two also
sessions and that the therapists were thereby gaining a
described disengagement as a way to discontinue past
deeper understanding and appreciation of their struggle to
disruptive ways of self-relating. These two processes were
be simultaneously self-protective and self-advancing. Cli-
exemplified in one client’s description.
ents then would look to their therapists’ reactions and
When I’m listening to myself, when I’m having this responses to their disengagements as guideposts toward
discussion or I catch myself um doing the critic thing how to manage this predicament.

123
182 J Contemp Psychother (2009) 39:171–186

As clients became comfortable with this striving to experiences of disengagement and a descriptive categori-
better guide their progress while still protecting them- zation to assist therapists in their moment-by-moment
selves, they would more readily notice when their assessment of what may be transpiring during identified
disengagement was becoming a hindrance and remedy it. periods of disengagement. Consistent with the combined
As illustrated in clusters 2 and 5, clients could remedy findings from the literatures on storytelling, silence, and
hindering disengagements (e.g., becoming self-critical, or secrecy in psychotherapy (e.g., Beutler et al. 2004; Rennie
preventing self-assertion) by learning to recognize and 1994; Levitt 2001b; Hill et al. 2000), the present findings
tolerate the fears that previously had them disengaging partially support a differentiated view of disengagement,
involuntarily and stuck. They began not only to learn to be which includes both ostensibly productive and obstructive
self-advancing about less-threatening topics, but also to forms of disengagement in relation to clients’ therapy
learn how to confront, explore and make progress in the goals. Disengagement experiences described in Cluster 1
face of more intense or painful concerns. In this manner, tended to be automatic and to run counter to clients’ pre-
disengagement helped clients to become increasingly dominant therapy goals, while disengagement experiences
confident about their abilities to continue difficult explo- found in the remaining four clusters were utilized pre-
ration in psychotherapy. dominantly to address problems, manage therapeutic
relationships, learn about oneself, and interrupt self-
impeding patterns (for further discussion see Frankel
Discussion 2007). While the present study identified many productive
processes in disengagement, additional research is needed
The participants’ reported experiences during disengaged to enumerate the relative frequencies of productive and
moments in psychotherapy offer a rare view of the internal obstructive disengagements, explore their relative impact,
workings of disengagement from clients’ perspectives. The and to relate these types of disengagement to the processes
rigorous in-depth analysis of these experiences has led to a described in those literatures. However, despite consider-
comprehensive model of disengagement to further psy- able effort to find them, no clear markers for differentiating
chotherapy practice and research. However, in interpreting between types of disengagement were found in the present
the findings a number of the study’s limitations are worth study, as disengagement behaviors were seen to be simi-
bearing in mind. While attempts were made to recruit a larly employed for both productive and obstructive
diverse sample, there are aspects of the sample that limit processes (e.g., finger braiding to both distract from dis-
generalizability. Specifically, as the participants of this comfort and to signal difficulty to therapist).
study all identified as Caucasian, and were functioning That said, a number of behaviors were consistently
sufficiently to receive treatment in outpatient settings, reported to be related to disengagement. As reflected in two
caution is warranted when generalizing these findings to subcategory titles, clients indicated that abrupt shifts in
other populations. behavior, speech, and mood were often related to disen-
Nonetheless, a number of factors can strengthen confi- gagements. Such behaviors included obvious and frank
dence in these findings. The large number of gestures such as leaving the therapy room to ostensibly
disengagement experiences (47) present in the interviews, drink water; hand-wringing or other fidgeting; pronounced
the comprehensive grounded theory analysis, the saturation looking away or avoidance of eye contact; and ‘‘turtling,’’
of the model, the participants’ agreement with the findings, which involved a hunched posture in a form of physical
and its meaningfulness to readers, all suggest that the recoil. Other more subtle disengagement-related behaviors
phenomenon was well-studied and accurately captured included sheepishly voiced I-don’t-knows; rapid-fire ask-
clients’ experiences. Another notable strength of the pres- ing of questions; bracing and stiffening of body; long self-
ent study is its inclusion of clients from long- as well as filtering pauses; changes in topic or tenor of discussion;
short-term psychotherapies (range of length three and deep breathing. Although these behaviors were neither
months—four years), in contrast to most research on dis- systematically analyzed to differentiate between productive
engagement that has tended to focus on psychotherapies of and obstructive aspects of disengagement nor reported to
24 sessions or less (e.g., Frankel et al. 2006). do so by participants, upon informal observation, those
behaviors that occurred more abruptly generally appeared
Implications for Research more likely to be obstructive.
These disengagement-related behaviors are further sup-
As Engle and Arkowitz (2006) recently underscored, in the ported by their high degree of consistency with those
past 100 years of psychotherapy research resistance inter- arrived at with a different, task analytic, research method-
vention has received only scarce empirical attention. The ology by Safran and Muran (2000) for identifying what they
present findings offer a coherent understanding of clients’ refer to as ‘‘withdrawal alliance ruptures’’: denial, minimal

123
J Contemp Psychother (2009) 39:171–186 183

response, shifting the topic, intellectualization, storytelling, explained variance in psychotherapy outcome (meta-anal-
and talking about other (p. 142). Although their behavioral yses find effect sizes ranging from 0.22 to 0.29). If we
markers concur with those reported in the present study understand disengagement to entail experimentation and
their model does not differentiate between positive aspects non-verbal communication around core concerns then the
and negative aspects of disengagement, instead regarding importance of the relationship in resolving those concerns
all disengagement as an alliance rupture. Thus, given the is clear, as is the influence of therapists’ responses to dis-
consistently variegated nature of disengagement reported engagements on psychotherapy outcome.
by participants in the present study, future research might This proposed model of disengagement therefore argues
focus on specific markers useful in distinguishing produc- for a transposition of previous understandings regarding the
tive and obstructive aspects of disengaged moments. nature of intervention. Safran and Muran (2000) have
Such research might begin with an in-depth exploration emphasized that intervention is a ‘‘relational act’’ (p. 41),
of differentially effective high-disengagement psychother- communicating therapists’ attitudes and defining the tenor
apies, to discover patterns of client disengagement and of therapeutic relationships. Of course the way techniques
therapist responses associated with good and poor out- are introduced and used within the relationship do carry
come. The use of psychotherapy coding systems might help important implications, however, it may be more helpful to
in this effort. For instance, the Pausing Inventory Coding stress in regard to disengagement that relational acts are
System (H.M. Levitt and Z. Frankel, Unpublished manu- interventions. That is to say that the way in which thera-
script), can help researchers to identify two types of pists are in the relationship—how they respond on a
disengagement processes through verbal and para-verbal session-to-session, minute-by-minute basis to their clients’
markers (e.g., disengagements that are more geared to difficulties and challenges—comprises the bulk of the
avoid emotion and those that are more geared to safeguard intervention; because those responses, that behavior, is
the alliance). In addition research might focus on experi- what provides answers to the core of clients’ disengage-
enced therapists to learn how they conceptualize and ment experiences, the communicative experimentation
navigate choice points related to disengagement in the toward self-change.
moment. To what extent therapists are actually aware of Thus the core category of the present model of disen-
their clients’ disengagements has yet to be studied. In gagement, referred to in shorthand as ‘‘communicative
preliminary investigations of therapists’ experiences asso- experimentation,’’ suggests an intervention strategy of
ciated with three of the current study’s therapy sessions, contextually sensitive ‘‘mindful responsiveness’’ to meet
therapists’ reports of therapy events were strikingly dis- the needs of clients’ enacted issues. Specifically, therapist
parate from clients’ reports, raising the possibility that mindful responsiveness may be benefited by orienting to
therapists may be less attuned to clients’ disengagements two broad themes of clients’ enacted issues common
than clients imagine. Findings from such studies then could among clients’ reported experiences of both productive and
be integrated to construct a training manual that could be obstructive forms of disengagement within the therapeutic
evaluated in controlled experimental research. Given the relationship: (1) Disengagement is experienced as a unified
centrality of disengagement experiences in the change movement aimed toward resolving difficulties, either
process, learning how to better respond to them would directly by assembling additional personal resources or
greatly benefit psychotherapy training, practice, and indirectly by strengthening the therapeutic relationship;
research. Toward those aims, the paper now briefly turns to and (2) disengagement is experienced as a recruitment of
clinical implications of the present findings. therapist feedback—communicating both clients’ fears and
hopes—regarding either clients’ difficulties or regarding
Relational Acts as Interventions in Disengagement possible client changes.

The present findings of clients’ experiences of disengage- The Disengagements are Friendly
ment provide a phenomenological context within which
existing literature on disengagement phenomena can be Despite considerable debate (cf. Leahy 2001; Messer 2002;
better understood and applied clinically. Central to the Engle and Arkowitz 2006), resistance phenomena in psy-
present model of client disengagement is a sense of rec- chotherapy continue to be viewed pejoratively and,
onfiguring the dance of self-advancement and self- ultimately, destructively: ‘‘The conflict, therefore, is a
protection. Such a model of disengagement provides a conflict between the patient’s healthy wish to change and
theoretical context for one of the most robust findings in his unhealthy resistance to change’’ (Stark 1994, p. 3).
psychotherapy research (Orlinsky et al. 2004)—regardless Indeed, even in the client-centered tradition, although the
of the therapeutic technique or school, the therapeutic blame is placed squarely at the feet of therapists, resistance
alliance reliably accounts for a substantial proportion of is nonetheless viewed disapprovingly, as ‘‘it [resistance]

123
184 J Contemp Psychother (2009) 39:171–186

grows primarily out of poor techniques of handling the we’re going to go through the whole tape—and I’ll
client’s expression of his problems and feelings’’ (Rogers want to hear more about it later—could you give me a
1951, p. 151). In the present study clients described pro- brief description now?
ductive processes in disengagement, that suggest quite to 5. I’ll give you the remote control and you can watch the
the contrary of both these perspectives, that more similar to tape, stopping at such disengaged moments.
Mahoney’s (2003, p. 172) ‘‘compassionate’’ views of
resistance, disengagement is not only a natural part of the
Questions about Each Disengaged Moment
change process, but can facilitate it.
Nonetheless, the findings do not recommend a laissez
1. What were you experiencing in this disengaged
faire attitude toward disengagement. Rather, just the
moment?
opposite, they suggest that in response to disengage-
2. How did these feelings come up for you at this point?
ments—as Fromm-Reichmann reportedly proclaimed over
What was going on before?
50 years ago—‘‘patients need an experience, not an
3. What was it that prompted you to disengage?
explanation’’ (quoted in Strupp and Binder 1984, p. 139).
4. Was there anything else going on for you during this
Specifically, the findings direct our attention to clients’
moment?
enacted communications and to the multiple meanings
5. What do you think was going on for the therapist at
behind them. They direct our attention to the agentic,
this point?
aware, and invested person of the client. They direct our
6. How would you have liked the therapist to respond to
attention to the responses of therapists, to which clients
you here? How would that have made you feel through
closely attend and interpret on many levels. And, perhaps
the rest of the session?
most important of all, they direct our attention to the very
heart of the therapeutic encounter, the relationship.
Questions and Statements to be Made at the End of the
Acknowledgement We would like to acknowledge and express our Interview
gratitude to Dr. Robert A. Neimeyer, Dr. Sharon G. Horne, Dr. Sam
B. Morgan, Dr. Karina Koerner, and Joseph Currier for their help with
this study. 1. We’realmostfinishedwiththeinterview.Iwouldliketoask
yousomequestionsaboutourinterviewtodaybecauseIwill
beinterviewingsomemorepeopleandIwouldliketomake
Appendix A: Disengagement Interview Questions sure that I am asking the best questions and using the best
interviewingstylethatIcan.
Statements Made Preceding Each Interview 2. What did these moments mean for you? Do you see a
trend in your disengagement? Do you have a theory
1. I am meeting with you today to learn about your about disengagement after viewing your session tape?
experience in the therapy session. 3. Which disengaged moment felt the most important to
2. If at any part during the interview you are uncomfort- you?
able with a certain question or topic please let me 4. Is there anything I haven’t asked that feels like it
know and we can move to another topic, or if you need would help me understand your experience of
we can terminate the interview altogether. disengagement?
3. I am particularly interested in moments where you 5. Is there anything you have learned from this interview
might lessen your involvement in the session, remove process?
yourself from the topic that was being discussed, or 6. Is there anything you would want to share with your
hold back things that you are thinking or feeling. For therapist?
instance, when you feel yourself detach from the 7. Do you have any questions or comments, about
therapy conversation or pull back from what’s being anything?
discussed or feel less involved in the therapy process. I 8. How do you feel about our meeting today?
call these occurrences disengaged moments. I am 9. Let me remind you that I’ll be asking for your
particularly interested in what is happening for you in feedback once the findings of this study are compiled.
the session rather than your reactions now, but if you I’ll be interested in hearing what you think about the
share a new reaction, please let me know that it is not study’s findings. This is actually really important for
what you were feeling then. the study, as part of the assessment of my findings is
4. Before we start are there any moments like this that whether they are coherent and fitting with your
stand out for you within your last session? Because experience. So even if you have nothing to add to

123
J Contemp Psychother (2009) 39:171–186 185

the findings it is important to me that you return the Frankel, Z., Levitt, H. M., Murray, D. M., Greenberg, L. S., & Angus,
feedback form. L. (2006). Assessing psychotherapy silences: An empirically
derived categorization system and sampling strategy. Psycho-
therapy Research, 16, 627–638.
Freud, S. (1959). Remembering, repeating, and working through. In J.
Strachey (Ed. & Trans.), The standard edition of the complete
References psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 12, pp. 147–156).
London: Hogarth Press. (Original work published in 1914).
Allen, D. M. (1993). Unified therapy. In G. Stricker & J. Gold (Eds.), Freud, S. (1959). Civilization and its discontents. In J. Strachey (Ed.
Comprehensive handbook of psychotherapy integration (pp. & Trans.), The standard edition of the complete psychological
125–137). New York: Plenum. works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 21, pp. 59–145). London: Hogarth
Angus, L., Levitt, H., & Hardtke, K. (1999). Narrative processes and Press. (Original work published in 1930).
psychotherapeutic change: An integrative approach to psycho- Fromm-Reichmann, F. (1950). Principles of intensive psychotherapy.
therapy research and practice. Journal of Clinical Psychology, Chicago: University Press.
55, 1255–1270. Gabbard, G. O. (2004). Long-term psychodynamic psychotherapy: A
Beck, A. T. (1975). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. basic text. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.
Madison, CT: International Universities Press. Giorgi, A. (1970). Psychology as a human science: A phenomeno-
Beutler, L. E., Malik, M., Alimohamed, S., Harwood, T. M., Talebi, logical approach. New York: Harper & Row.
H., Noble, S., & Wong, E. (2004). Therapist variables. In M. J. Glaser, B. G., & Strauss, A. L. (1967). The discovery of grounded
Lambert (Ed.), Bergin and Garfield’s handbook of psychother- theory: Strategies for qualitative research. New York: Aldine de
apy and behavior change (5th ed., pp. 227–306). New York: Gruyter.
Wiley. Gottman, J. M., & Levenson, J. W. (1985). A valid procedure for
Beutler, L. E., Moleiro, C. M., & Talebi, H. (2002). Resistance in obtaining self-report of affect in marital interaction. Journal of
psychotherapy: What conclusions are supported by research? Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53(2), 151–160.
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 58, 207–217. Greenberg, L. S., & Pascual-Leone, P. (1995). A dialectical
Bischoff, M. M. (1997). Predictors of client resistance in the constructivist approach to experiential change. In R. A.
counseling interaction. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Uni- Neimeyer & M. J. Mahoney (Eds.), Constructivism in psycho-
versity of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, IL. therapy (pp. 169–191). Washington: American Psychological
Brown, L. S. (1994). Subversive dialogues: Theory in feminist Association.
therapy. New York: Basic Books. Greenberg, L. S., & Pavio, S. C. (1997). Working with emotions in
Chamberlain, P., Patterson, G. R., Reid, J. B., Kavanagh, K., & psychotherapy. New York: Guilford Press.
Forgatch, M. S. (1984). Observation of client resistance. Greenberg, L. S., Rice, L. N., & Elliott, R. (1993). Facilitating
Behavior Therapy, 15, 144–155. emotional change: The moment-by-moment process. New York:
Clarkin, J. F., & Levy, K. H. (2004). The influence of client variables Guilford Press.
on psychotherapy. In M. J. Lambert (Ed.), Bergin and Garfield’s Greenberg, L., Watson, J., & Lietaer, G (Eds.). (1998). Handbook of
handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (5th ed., pp. experiential therapy . New York: Guilford Press.
194–226). New York: Wiley. Greenson, R. R. (1961). On the silence and sounds of the analytic
Elliott, R. (1986). Interpersonal Process Recall (IPR) as a psycho- hour. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 6,
therapy process research method. In L.S. Greenberg & W. M. 79–84.
Pinsoff (Eds.), The psychotherapeutic process: A research Hailparn, D. F., & Hailparn, M. (1999). Reporting as resistance:
handbook (pp. 503–527). New York: Guilford Press. Strategies for overcoming reporting. Journal of Contemporary
Elliott, R., Fischer, C. T., & Rennie, D. L. (1999). Evolving Psychotherapy, 29, 155–165.
guidelines for publication of qualitative research studies in Hill, C. E., Gelso, C. J., & Mohr J. J. (2000). Client concealment and
psychology and related fields. British Journal of Clinical self-presentation in therapy: Comment on Kelly (2000). Psy-
Psychology, 38, 215–229. chological Bulletin, 126, 495–500.
Elliott, R., Greenberg, L. S., & Lietaer, G. (2004). Research on Hill, C. E., Knox, S., Thompson, B. J., Williams, E. N., Hess, S. A., &
experiential psychotherapies. In M. J. Lambert (Ed.), Bergin and Ladany, N. (2005). Consensual qualitative research: An update.
Garfield’s handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (5th Journal of Counseling Psychology, 52, 196–205.
ed., pp. 493–539). New York: John Wiley. Hill, C. E., Thompson, B. J., & Ladany, N. (2003). Therapist use of
Engle, D. E., & Arkowitz, H. (2006). Ambivalence in psychotherapy: silence in therapy: A survey. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 59,
Facilitating readiness to change. New York: Guilford Press. 513–524.
Farber, B. A., Berano, K. C., & Capobianco, J. A. (2006). A temporal Hoehn-Saric, R. (1974). Attitude change and attribution of arousal in
model of patient disclosure in psychotherapy. Psychotherapy psychotherapy. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 159,
Research, 16, 463–469. 234–243.
Finkanaeur, C., & Rime, D. (1998). Keeping emotional memories Kagan, N. (1975). Interpersonal process recall: A method of
secret: Health and subjective well-being when emotions are not influencing human interaction. Houston, TX: University of
shared. Journal of Health Psychology, 3, 47–58. Houston.
Foa, E. B., & Kozak, M. J. (1986). Emotional processing of fear: Kamphuis, J. H., & Telch, M. J. (2000). Effects of distraction and
Exposure to corrective information. Psychological Bulletin, 99, guided threat reappraisal on fear reduction during exposure-
20–35. based treatments for specific fears. Behaviour Research and
Frankel, Z. (2004). Which silences are golden? Giving pause to Therapy, 38(12), 1163–1181.
pauses in psychotherapy. Unpublished master’s thesis, The Kelly, A. E. (1998). Clients’ secret keeping in outpatient therapy.
University of Memphis, Memphis, TN. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 45(1), 50–57.
Frankel, Z. (2007). Clients’ experiences of disengagement: A Kelly, A. E., Klusas, J. A., von-Weiss, R. T., & Kenny, C. (2001).
grounded theory analysis. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, What is it about revealing secrets that is beneficial? Personality
The University of Memphis, Memphis, TN. and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(6), 651–665.

123
186 J Contemp Psychother (2009) 39:171–186

Ladany, N., Hill, C. E., Thompson, B. J., & O’Brien, K. M. (2004). behavior modifier. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol-
Therapist perspectives on using silence in therapy: A qualitative ogy, 53, 846–851.
study. Counselling and Psychotherapy Research, 4, 80–89. Polkinghorne, D. E. (1988). Narrative knowing and human sciences.
Lane, J. D., & Wegner, D. M. (1995). The cognitive consequences of Albany: State University of New York.
secrecy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 237– Pos, A. E., Greenberg, L. S., Goldman, R. N., & Korman, L. M.
253. (2003). Emotional processing during experiential treatment of
Leahy, R. L. (2001). Overcoming resistance in cognitive therapy. depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71,
New York: Guilford Press. 1007–1016.
Levitt, H. M. (1998). Silence in psychotherapy: The meaning and Rachman, S. (1980). Emotional processing. Behaviour Research &
function of pauses. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, York Therapy, 18, 51–60.
University, Toronto, ON. Rauch, S., & Foa, E. (2006). Emotional processing theory and
Levitt, H. M. (2001a). Clients’ experiences of obstructive silence: exposure therapy for PTSD. Journal of Contemporary Psycho-
Integrating conscious reports and analytic theories. Journal of therapy, 36, 61–65.
Contemporary Psychotherapy, 31, 295–309. Rennie, D. L. (1992). Qualitative analysis of the client’s experience of
Levitt, H. M. (2001b). The sounds of silence in psychotherapy: psychotherapy: The unfolding of reflexivity. In S. G. Toukmanian
Clients’ experiences of pausing. Psychotherapy Research, 11, & D. L. Rennie (Eds.), Psychotherapy process research: Para-
295–309. digmatic and narrative approaches. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Levitt, H. M. (2002). The unsaid in the psychotherapy narrative: Rennie, D. L. (1994). Storytelling in psychotherapy: The client’s
Voicing the unvoiced. Counseling Psychology Quarterly, 15, subjective experience. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Prac-
333–350. tice, Training, 31, 234–243.
Mahoney, M. J. (2003). Constructive psychotherapy: A practical Rennie, D.L., Phillips, J.R., & Quartaro, G.K. (1988). Grounded
guide. New York: Guilford Press. theory: A promising approach to conceptualization in psychol-
McAdams, D. (1997). The stories we live by: Personal myths and the ogy? Canadian Psychology, 29, 139–149.
making of the self. New York: Guilford Press. Rice, J. L. (1999). Does awareness of relationship patterns predict
Messer, S. B. (2002). A psychodynamic perspective on resistance in intensity of resistance? Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Texas
psychotherapy: Vive la résistance. Journal of Clinical Psychol- A & M University, College Station, TX.
ogy: In Session, 58, 157–163. Rogers, C. R. (1951). Client-centered therapy: Its current practice,
Messer, S. B., & Wolitzky, D. L. (1997). The traditional psychoan- implementation, and theory. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin.
alytic approach to case formulation. In T. D. Eells (Ed.), Safran, J. D., & Muran, J. C. (2000). Negotiating the therapeutic
Handbook of psychotherapy case formulation (pp. 26–57). New alliance: A relational treatment guide. New York: Guilford
York: Guilford Press. Press.
Monk, G., Winslade, J., Crocket, K., & Epston, D. (1997). Narrative Strupp, H. H., & Binder, J. L. (1984). Psychotherapy in a new key: A
therapy in practice: The archeology of hope. San Francisco: guide to time-limited dynamic psychotherapy. New York: Basic
Jossey-Bass. Books.
Neimeyer, R. A. (2000). Narrative disruptions in the construction of Trad, P. V. (1993). Silence: The resounding experience. American
the self. In J. D. Rasking & R. A. Neimeyer (Eds.), Construc- Journal of Psychotherapy, 47, 167–170.
tions of disorder: Meaning-making frameworks for Vakoch, D. A., & Strupp, H. H. (2000). Psychodynamic approaches to
psychotherapy (pp. 207–242). Washington, DC: American psychotherapy: Philosophical and theoretical foundations of
Psychological Association. effective practice. In C. R. Snyder & R. E. Ingram (Eds.),
Orlinsky, D. E., Rønnestäd, M. H., & Willutzki, U. (2004). Fifty years Handbook of psychological change: Psychotherapy processes
of process-outcome research: Continuity and change. In M. J. and practice for the 21st century. New York: John Wiley.
Lambert (Ed.), Bergin and Garfield’s handbook of psychother- Weller, J. N. (2003). Observable client resistance and early
apy and behavior change (5th ed., pp. 307–389). New York: psychotherapy dropout. Unpublished doctoral dissertation.
Wiley. Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI.
Patterson, G. R., & Forgatch, M. S. (1985). Therapist behavior as a Zeligs, M. A. (1961). The psychology of silence. Journal of the
determinant for client noncompliance: A paradox for the American Psychoanalytic Association, 9, 7–43.

123

Você também pode gostar