Você está na página 1de 12

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/276125250

Risky sexual behaviours: The role of substance


use, psychopathic traits, and attachment
insecurity among adolescents and...

Article in The Canadian journal of human sexuality · December 2014


DOI: 10.3138/cjhs.2625

CITATION READS

1 49

5 authors, including:

Stéphane Sabourin Audrey Brassard


Laval University Université de Sherbrooke
134 PUBLICATIONS 2,308 CITATIONS 37 PUBLICATIONS 220 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Christopher Naud
Université du Québec à Trois-Rivières
2 PUBLICATIONS 3 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

A cluster-analytic study of pornography use in a community sample View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Audrey Brassard on 15 June 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. All in-text references underlined in blue are added to the original document
and are linked to publications on ResearchGate, letting you access and read them immediately.
ARTICLE

Risky sexual behaviours: The role of substance use, psychopathic


traits, and attachment insecurity among adolescents and
young adults in Quebec
Carmen Lemelin,1 Yvan Lussier,1 Stéphane Sabourin,2 Audrey Brassard,3 and Christopher Naud 1
1 Department of Psychology, Université du Québec à Trois-Rivières, Trois-Rivières, QC
2 Department of Psychology, Université Laval, Laval, QC
3 Department of Psychology, Université de Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC

The purpose of this study was to examine how substance use, psychopathic traits, and attachment repre-
sentations explain sexual risk-taking in adolescence. Variables used to define risky sexual behaviours
were age at first sexual intercourse and number of lifetime sexual partners. A sample of 1,553 adolescents
and young adults completed a battery of questionnaires including measures of substance use, psycho-
pathic traits, attachment representations, and sexual behaviours. Structural equation modelling demon-
strated that, in the present sample, risky sexual behaviours were best modelled through both direct effects
of avoidant attachment representations and indirect effects of psychopathic traits and abandonment anxiety
through increases in substance use. This model was gender-invariant and may represent a contemporary
trend toward homogenization of sexual practices among young people in Quebec.

KEY WORDS: Risky sexual behaviour, substance use, psychopathic traits, attachment insecurity, adolescents

INTRODUCTION Risky sexual behaviours in adolescence result from a variety


of neurobiological changes that increase responsiveness to
Whether studied from an evolutionary (Ellis et al., 2012), de- physical and socio-emotional rewards at a time when impulse-
velopmental psychopathological (Steinberg, 2008), or psycho- control safeguards against immediate gratification are still not
analytic (Kernberg, 2011) theoretical perspective, adolescent fully established (Dodge & Albert, 2012). However, there are
sexual risk-taking has been conceptualized as a behavioural marked individual differences in adolescent sexual behaviours,
strategy aimed at increasing physical pleasure, gaining social and neurobiological variation is also expressed through a wide
status and dominance in peer groups, achieving socio-emo- array of other disinhibited behaviours (e.g., use of psychoactive
tional independence from one’s parents, resolving intimacy/ substances), personality traits, and attachment representations.
identity issues, or enhancing future reproductive success and The prevalence of early sexual behaviours in adolescence
pair bonding. A relatively early age at first sexual intercourse has been examined in various population surveys. In a national
and an elevated number of sexual partners may lead to reward- survey conducted in 2003, 12% of Canadian males and 13%
ing life experiences and promote adaptive outcomes for some of Canadian females had experienced sexual intercourse by
adolescents. However, for others, early sexual debut may be the age of 14–15 years and for adolescents between 15 and 17
costly and trigger a chain of events resulting in immediate years of age, 28% had experienced sexual intercourse (Roter-
negative outcomes (e.g., sexually transmitted infection, un- mann, 2005). In the province of Quebec, where the present
wanted pregnancy, family conflicts, loss of reputation) that study was conducted, 17% of males and 22% of females 14 to
may extend into the future (e.g., lower educational achieve- 15 years-old had experienced sexual intercourse (Garriguet,
ments and occupational status, fragile adult couple relation- 2005). Early sexual experiences are often associated with a
ships, higher prevalence of mental or personality disorders) higher overall number of sexual partners. Indeed, the earlier
(Institute of Medecine and National Research Council, 2011). teenagers become sexually active, the higher the number of

Acknowledgements: This research was made possible by a Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada grant awarded to the
second author.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Yvan Lussier, Department of Psychology, Université du Québec à Trois-Rivières,
C.P. 500, Trois-Rivières, Québec, Canada G9A 5H1. E-mail: yvan.lussier@uqtr.ca.

The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality 23(3), 2014, pp. 189–199; doi:10.3138/cjhs.2625 189
Carmen Lemelin et al.

sexual partners they report (Chun & Mobley, 2010; Garriguet, chopathic traits. To examine these potential gender differences,
2005; Le Gall & Le Van, 2007; Petersen & Hyde, 2010). In the the invariance of our model was tested in this study. Empirical
present study, the determinants of observed variation in adoles- support for the proposed model is now briefly reviewed.
cent sexual risk-taking are examined using an individual differ-
ences framework. Early sexual behaviours are thought to be
facilitated by poor impulse control, opportunistic behaviours ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
stemming from psychopathic personality traits, and insecure
Attachment representations can be examined along two
attachment orientations which are mediated by substance
well-established empirical dimensions: anxiety and avoidance
use. In addition, adolescent sexual risk-taking behaviours are
(Bartholomew, 1990; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2003). Anxiety re-
more frequently observed in young males than in young
fers to fear of rejection, clinging behaviours, and hyper-
females, whereas the negative developmental burden of early
vigilance to relational threats fueled by a lack of self-esteem.
sexual activity is higher in young females (Mahalik et al., 2013).
Two models exist to describe typical adolescent life Avoidance is characterized by self-sufficiency and discomfort
with intimacy. Attachment avoidance reduces vulnerability to
choices, such as sexual risk-taking and the use of psychoactive
negative emotions that might result from rejection or lack of
substances, and psychopathic behaviour: problem-behaviour
availability and support from the attachment figure.
theory (Jessor, Donovan, & Costa, 1991) and the social devel-
Attachment orientations and adolescent sexuality exert a
opmental model (Lonczak, Abbott, Hawkins, Kosterman, &
great influence on the development of an individual’s identity
Catalano, 2002). Problem- behaviour theory conceptualizes
and on the development of knowledge about self and others
teenage sexual activity as part of a behavioural pattern that
(Guidano, 1987). As teens get older, they distance themselves
includes risky sexual behaviour, substance use and delinquent
activities (referred to as psychopathic behaviours). These from their parents and increasingly turn toward romantic
partners to obtain emotional support (Buhrmester & Furman,
behaviours co-vary and co-occur in adolescence and are influ-
1987). This attachment figure shifting process from parent to
enced by social (e.g., attachment to parents or lovers) and
partner develops progressively (Friedlmeier & Granqvist, 2006).
contextual variables (e.g., situational problems). The social
Adolescents even come to prefer and refer to their romantic
developmental model (Lonczak et al., 2002) refers to multiple
partner as their main attachment figure, particularly in ongoing
levels or sources of influence on adolescent development (e.g.,
relationships of two years or more (Hazan & Zeifman, 1994),
surrounding context, parents’ and the child’s emotional and
thus supplanting parents in the social/interpersonal hierarchy
social skills) that promote or hinder adolescent social devel-
opment. Together, these factors may influence teenagers to (Furman & Wehner, 1997). Adolescents with a history of in-
secure attachment to their mother who report high levels of
develop risky sexual behaviours, to use psychoactive substances,
abandonment anxiety are more likely to describe their roman-
or to become involved in delinquent activities. Both models
tic partner as their main attachment figure, compared to those
focus on sexual risk-taking, substance use and psychopathic
with a history of insecure attachment who report high levels
personality traits as core marker variables of social develop-
of attachment avoidance (Friedlmeier & Granqvist, 2006).
ment in adolescence. It is in light of these models that number
Furthermore, teens carry out exploration behaviours in the
of sexual partners and age at first intercourse were selected as
presence of their romantic partner, as do young children in
markers of risky sexual behaviour in relation to substance use
and psychopathy. In addition, during adolescence, psycho- the presence of their parents. They are also inclined to seek
protection and comfort from their partner when facing situa-
pathic personality traits and romantic attachment are in-
tions that appear threatening. Moreover, they seem to protest
creasingly conceptualized as an evolving set of internal repre-
in situations of involuntary separation, just as the departure
sentations that guide adolescent behaviours such as sexual
of a parent may elicit a separation call from a young child.
risk-taking and substance abuse (Catchpole, 2008; Kubak,
Lastly, the majority of adults involved in an intimate relation-
2009). As such, these variables are included in the present
ship report that their main attachment figure is their romantic
study as indicators of more stable individual differences that
partner (Doherty & Feeney, 2004).
may explain substance use and sexual activity.
In addition, it is important to determine whether modifi- Attachment-related avoidant adolescents have fewer inti-
mate relationships and experience loving feelings less often
cations need to be made to the proposed model to account for
than their securely- or anxiously-attached peers, whereas an-
gender differences. For example, a recent meta-analysis showed
xious adolescents fall in love more often and enter romantic
that, compared to females, males are more avoidant and less
relationships at an earlier age (Tracy, Shaver, Albino, & Cooper,
anxious in their attachment representations and behaviours
2003). Studies indicate that, during adolescence, secure individ-
(Del Giudice, 2011). Likewise, psychopathic traits are report-
uals experience more steady and exclusive dating relationships
edly more prevalent, severe, and consequential in men than in
(lasting six months or more) than those with insecure attach-
women (Patrick, 2006). It may well be that, in young females,
sexual risk-taking mainly emerges to assuage abandonment ment styles (Furman & Wehner, 1997). Later in adolescence,
secure teenagers also report experiencing high-quality relation-
anxiety and fear of rejection while in young males, these be-
ships (Simpson, 1990) and intimacy (Bartholomew & Horowitz,
haviours may be mostly determined by poor impulse control,
1991).
opportunistic attitudes, and the desire to impress one’s peers,
features present in individuals showing elevated levels of psy-

190 The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality 23(3), 2014, pp. 189–199; doi:10.3138/cjhs.2625
Risky sexual behaviours

ADOLESCENT ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT AND signs of decreased commitment and possible break-up threats.
SEXUAL RISK BEHAVIOUR While psychopathic traits are related to casual sex, promis-
cuous behaviours, mate poaching, and infidelity in adults
The role of attachment orientations is also significant for sexual (Schmitt, Realo, Voracek, & Allik, 2008), to our knowledge,
experience. In one study, attachment-related avoidant adoles- there are no studies examining whether these associations
cents were least likely to have ever had intercourse (52%) com- emerge in adolescence.
pared to secure (66%) or anxious adolescents (69%) (Tracy et
al., 2003). Anxious teenage girls were more likely to report
having had sexual intercourse than their peers, whereas in SUBSTANCE USE
boys, attachment security is associated with an increased like-
lihood of having had intercourse. However, Tracy et al. found Several studies have shown significant associations between
no significant association between attachment representations substance use and sexual behaviours among adolescents and
and the number of sexual partners. Likewise, Szielasko, young adults. Alcohol use in romantic relationships is posi-
Symons, and Price (2013) did not find a significant associa- tively correlated with sexual activity (Cooper & Orcutt, 1997).
tion between the number of sexual partners and attachment The use of psychoactive substances is also associated with age
anxiety or avoidance. Nevertheless, avoidance in sexual rela- (first intoxication typically occurs between the ages of 12 and
tionships and male gender status did predict multiple partners. 16 years; Johnston, O’Malley, Bachman, & Schulenberg, 2007),
gender (girls consume fewer drugs and less frequently than
boys; Johnston et al., 2007), socio-economic status (poverty
PSYCHOPATHY and living in the suburbs are related to increased substance
use; Droomers, Schrijvers, Casswell, & Mackenbach, 2003;
Psychopathy refers to attitudes and behaviours evocative of Hanson & Chen, 2007), and social network (substance use
delinquent conduct. Researchers have identified two distinct is increased when friends, relatives, or lovers also use alco-
dimensions of psychopathy: primary psychopathy and second- hol or drugs; Bachman, Wadsworth, O’Malley, Johnston, &
ary psychopathy (Harpur, Hare, & Hakstian, 1989; Karpman, Schulenberg, 1997).
1948). Primary psychopathy refers to a tendency to be insensi- In a study of 16-year-olds, there was a mediated relation-
tive, selfish, and show empathic dysfunction (Blair et al., 2004). ship between early sexual behaviours, alcohol use, and the
These individuals are also described as manipulative, cruel, number of sexual partners at age 19 (Zimmer-Gembeck,
dishonest, and fearless (Fowles, 1980; Harpur et al., 1989; Siebenbruner, & Collins, 2004). In the 2011 National Youth
Karpman, 1948). Secondary psychopathy is related to emo- Risk Behaviour Survey, among in the US, among currently
tion regulation deficits (impulsivity and poor management of sexually active high-school students, 26% of males and 18.1%
anxiety), intolerance for frustration, rapid mood changes, of females had drunk alcohol or used drugs before their last
concrete and short-term planning and an inability to pursue sexual intercourse (Eaton et al., 2012). Attachment also
goals over a long period (Harpur et al., 1989; Karpman, 1948). appears to affect teenagers’ substance use. As compared to
These deficits result partly from maladaptive family processes their secure peers, avoidant adolescents are more frequent
also associated with attachment insecurity. alcohol users and report being intoxicated more often (Tracy
Many similarities can be drawn between attachment-related et al., 2003).
avoidance, as defined by romantic attachment theory, and psy- Insecurely-attached adolescents are more likely to report
chopathy (Frodi, Dernevik, Sepa, Philipson & Bragesjö, 2001; having used alcohol or drugs during their first sexual relation-
Outcalt, 2007; Rosenstein & Horowitz, 1996). Both concepts ship (Tracy et al., 2003). Psychopathic traits may also increase
imply a reticence to express feelings. In both cases, individuals an individual’s tendency to use illicit substances. A study
have had a difficult childhood and have typically had repeated found that 14- and 15-year-old teenagers who reported using
experiences of rejection. This suggests that psychopathy may marijuana weekly had significantly more psychopathic traits
be associated with love and sexual relationships. To date, few and committed more crimes than those who did not use
studies have examined psychopathic traits, attachment orien- the drug as regularly (Monshower, Van Dorsselaer, Gorter,
tations, and sexual behaviours in adolescents. Verdurmen, & Vollebergh, 2004). It is not clear if this last
One study found that adolescents who show psychopathic result extends to risky sexual behaviours.
personality traits display insecure attachment styles (Catchpole,
2008). College students who obtain high scores of both attach- The Present Study
ment avoidance and attachment anxiety also obtain high pri-
mary and secondary psychopathy scores (Mack, Hackney, & This brief literature review indicates that there is a lack of
Pyle, 2011). Young adults who display avoidant attachment studies that simultaneously consider the interplay of attach-
and elevated psychopathic traits are more likely than others ment representations, psychopathic traits, and substance use
to experience relationship break-ups, and these individuals to understand adolescent high risk sexual behaviours. It is
report not being affected by these separations (Davis, Shaver, therefore difficult to ascertain the specific contribution of each
& Vernon, 2004). They do not seem to perceive the warning of these factors. The present study first proposes a model to

The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality 23(3), 2014, pp. 189–199; doi:10.3138/cjhs.2625 191
Carmen Lemelin et al.

test direct links between all these variables. In the second step, ing these two latent dimensions, items from each scale are
a mediational model will stipulate that the association between subdivided into three composite scores of six items. The
attachment and psychopathy on one hand, and sexual risk- authors of the questionnaire obtained alpha coefficients of
taking on the other hand, is solely explained by substance .94 for the avoidance scale and .91 for the anxiety scale
use. The choice of a mediational model, obtained through (Brennan et al., 1998). In the present study, the reliability co-
structural equation modelling, was justified by theoretical cri- efficients were respectively .92 and .88 for the scales of avoid-
teria outlined earlier and was also deemed acceptable con- ance and anxiety.
sidering the lack of well-established empirical guidelines in Psychopathy. The Self-Report Psychopathy Scale (Levenson,
this area of research. Past studies were strictly correlational Kiehl, & Fitzpatrick, 1995; Savard, Sabourin, & Lussier, 2006)
and did not consider these variables simultaneously. More- is an instrument containing 26 items. The questionnaire in-
over, many studies examining risky sexual behaviours also cludes two scales, one measuring primary psychopathy (16
used mediators rather than moderators to maximize their items) and the other measuring secondary psychopathy (10
understanding of adolescent sexuality (Brakefield, Wilson, & items). In the present study, the latent factor Psychopathy was
Donenberg, 2012; Kashubeck-West & Szymanski, 2008; Lester derived from four composite scores, two for primary and two
et al., 2010; Mezzich et al., 1997). Lastly, the gender invari- for secondary psychopathy in which the items were randomly
ance of the model will be tested because some sex differences distributed. Primary psychopathy refers to attitudes of selfish-
have been reported in the pattern of substance use, psycho- ness, manipulation, and malevolence toward others, whereas
pathic personality traits and sexual risk-taking (Ruiz, Stevens, secondary psychopathy measures impulsivity and pessimism.
Fuhriman, Bogart, & Korchmaros, 2009). Participants respond to the different items using a four-point
Likert scale ranging from ‘‘extremely disagree’’ to ‘‘extremely
agree.’’ The presence of psychopathic traits in an individual
METHOD
can be determined using cutoff scores developed by Brinkley,
Participants and Procedures Schmitt, Smith, and Newman (2001) on the global score of
psychopathy. Scores of 58 or more correspond to what is
The sample consisted of 1,553 participants. Of these, 1530
observed in prison inmates, scores between 49 and 57 reflect
reported gender information (1062 females, 468 males). The
the presence of moderate psychopathic traits, and scores of 48
participants ranged in age from 16 to 20 years (M ¼ 18.21,
or less reveal the absence of these traits in the individual.
SD ¼ 2.54). Participants had an average income of 5,375.04
Internal consistency coefficients for these scales are .82 for
CDN$ (SD ¼ 4.431) and an average of 11.58 years of educa- primary psychopathy and .63 for secondary psychopathy
tion (SD ¼ 1.31; 49.5% were recruited from high schools,
whereas the alpha for the total scale is .85 (Levenson et al.,
50.2% from colleges, and 0.3% from universities). Regarding
1995). In the present study, Cronbach alpha coefficients were
their level of relationship involvement, 15% reported they
.78 for primary psychopathy, .62 for secondary psychopathy,
had never been in any serious relationship, whereas nearly
and .77 for the global psychopathy scale. The alpha for sec-
one out of two respondents reported having been in one or
ondary psychopathy was not ideal, but acceptable for research
two serious romantic relationships (M ¼ 1.69 relationships;
purposes. In addition, in our structural equation models, its
SD ¼ 1.32). More specifically, six participants were married,
measurement contribution to the general psychopathy latent
78 were cohabiting, 727 had a partner with whom they were variable was strong.
not cohabiting, and 736 were single at the time of the study.
Substance use. Alcohol and drug use was assessed by three
Participants were solicited on a voluntary basis within high-
questions measuring consumption frequency of alcoholic bever-
schools, colleges, and universities in the province of Quebec,
ages, soft drugs (e.g., marijuana) and hard drugs (e.g., cocaine).
Canada. Questionnaires were distributed in class to partici-
A Likert-type scale with five different frequency choices (never,
pants. They were asked to complete several questionnaires at
once a month or less, 2 to 4 times a month, 2–3 times a week,
home, including those related to the present study, and to re-
4 times a week or more) is presented to participants who are
turn them by mail in a prepaid envelope. Participants spent an
asked to quantify the frequency of their consumption. The
average of one hour answering the questionnaires. Compensa- substance use latent factor is composed of these three indica-
tion of $5 was offered. The study was approved by the ethics
tors. In the present study, the reliability coefficient was .53.
committee of the Université du Québec à Trois-Rivières.
This coefficient is unsurprisingly low because the scale is
very short and also because teenagers who drink alcohol do
Measures
not necessarily use drugs. However, in our structural equation
Attachment. The Experience in Close Relationships question- analyses, these three items proved to be significant indicators
naire (ECR; Brennan, Clark, & Shaver, 1998, Lafontaine & of the latent substance use factor.
Lussier, 2003) assesses attachment representations in romantic Sexuality. Two questions measured sexual risk-taking: 1)
relationships. It contains 36 items divided into two dimensions: age at first consenting sexual intercourse and 2) number of
avoidance (18 items) and anxiety (18 items). Participants indi- sexual partners to date. Risky sexual behaviour was considered
cate their level of agreement or disagreement with each state- a latent factor composed of these two variables. Each was
ment on a scale of 1 to 7 (ranging from ‘‘strongly disagree’’ to treated as a continuous variable and represented an indicator
‘‘strongly agree’’). To estimate the internal consistency underly- of sexual risk-taking. For the structural equation analyses,

192 The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality 23(3), 2014, pp. 189–199; doi:10.3138/cjhs.2625
Risky sexual behaviours

Table 1. Correlations between participants’ romantic attachment dimensions, psychopathy, substance use and sexuality variables

M SD 1 2 3 4 5

1. Anxiety 3.68 .98


2. Avoidance 2.31 .92 .26***
3. Psychopathy 51.32 8.64 .29*** .29***
4. Substance use 2.37 1.77 .03 .05 .24**
5. Age at first sexual intercourse 15.31 1.47 .04 .30*** .03 .24***
6. Number of partners 3.15 3.41 .03 .05 .13*** .33*** .39***

Note: *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

the number of sexual partners was reverse-coded to ensure Among participants who had experienced intercourse,
factor consistency. 38.5% (n ¼ 455) had only had one sexual partner. Nearly half
Since the objective of the study was to examine the simul- of respondents (47%, n ¼ 519) reported at least two partners,
taneous interplay of attachment, psychopathy and substance but no more than five, whereas 8.6% (n ¼ 93) had between
use on the prediction of risky sexual behaviours, structural six and nine sexual partners, and 5.3% (n ¼ 58) had more
equation modelling analyses were conducted using robust than 10 partners. The average number of sexual partners was
estimation with maximum likelihood (ML) approach. Robust 3.15 (SD ¼ 3.41) and there was no significant difference
estimation was used because it is relatively insensitive to de- between young females (M ¼ 3.19, SD ¼ 3.46) and males
partures from normality assumptions. Each structural equa- (M ¼ 3.0, SD ¼ 3.20), t(1072) ¼ 0.91, p ¼ .36.
tion model was tested using several fit indices, some of which Means and standard deviations for all measures are pre-
are less influenced by sample size, namely the Comparative sented in Table 1. Applying the cutoff points described above
Fit Index (CFI; Bentler, 1990), the Non-standard Fitting In- revealed that 20% of participants showed clinically significant
dex (NNFI), and the Root Mean Square Error of Approxima- psychopathic traits (scores above 57). Alcohol was unques-
tion (RMSEA, Steiger, 2000). The CFI is used to compare the tionably the substance used by the largest number of partici-
fit of the theoretical model to an alternative model based on pants: 91% had used alcohol at least once. Approximately
the assumption that the observed variables are non-correlated. four out of five participants (81%) reported using alcohol be-
The index obtained by this comparison varies from 0 to 1. tween 1 and 4 times a month, while 10% used it at least twice
When the CFI is equal or superior to .95, it is possible to con- a week. A little less than a third of participants (30%) reported
clude that the model adequately fits the data (Bentler, 1990). using soft drugs between 1 and 4 times a month, and 9% used
The same reasoning is followed for the NNFI which also has a them at least twice a week. Finally, 91% of participants had
value ranging from 0 to 1. An index of .90 or higher demon- never used hard drugs, 8% did use hard drugs 1 to 4 times a
strates a good fit of the model. The RMSEA calculates the month, while less than 2% used them at least twice a week.
average error of fit between the submitted model and the Young females and males differed significantly on the
data. An index equal to or less than .10 corresponds to an frequency of alcohol use (girls: M ¼ 1.51, SD ¼ 0.79; boys:
adequate fit of the model (Browne & Cudeck, 1993). The Chi- M ¼ 1.71, SD ¼ 0.89, t(1521) ¼ 4.39, p < .001, Cohen’s
square test is perceived to decrease in validity as sample size d ¼ .24), soft drug use (girls: M ¼ 0.62, SD ¼ 1.00; boys:
increases. It is therefore preferable to use the w2/ df ratio M ¼ 0.90, SD ¼ 1.30, t(716.33) ¼ 4.15, p < .001, d ¼ .24),
(Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1993). When the value of the ratio falls primary psychopathy (girls: M ¼ 29.19, SD ¼ 5.93; boys:
between 1 and 5, the fit between the proposed theoretical M ¼ 33.04, SD ¼ 6.87, t(786.88) ¼ 10.52, p < .001, d ¼ .60),
model and the data is satisfactory. Chi-squares were com- and attachment-related avoidance (girls: M ¼ 2.23, SD ¼ 0.91;
puted using the Satorra and Bentler (1994) robust method of boys: M ¼ 2.48, SD ¼ 0.92, t(1508) ¼ 4.99, p < .001, d ¼ .27).
estimation. The effect sizes for gender differences were relatively small
(<.30, Cohen, 1992), except for primary psychopathy which
was moderate.
RESULTS
Descriptive Analyses Correlational Analyses
At the time of the study, 70.7% (n ¼ 1098) of the total sample Correlations between romantic attachment, psychopathy,
of participants (N ¼ 1,553) had experienced sexual intercourse. substance use, and sexual behaviour are presented in Table 1.
There were 7.3% (n ¼ 113) of respondents who reported an Inspection of the correlation matrix leads to three important
early sexual debut (i.e., before the age of 14). This threshold observations. First, with the exception of a link between
of sexual precocity was set by Garriguet (2005). There were avoidance and sexual behaviours, attachment representations
29.7% (n ¼ 461) of respondents who had experienced their and psychopathic traits are only weakly associated with risky
first sexual intercourse at age 14 to 15 and the average age at adolescent sexual behaviours. Second, alcohol and drug use
first sexual intercourse was 15.31 years (SD ¼ 1.47). are moderately correlated with sexual behaviours. Third, sub-

The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality 23(3), 2014, pp. 189–199; doi:10.3138/cjhs.2625 193
Carmen Lemelin et al.

Figure 1. Mediational structural equation model of romantic attachment, psychopathy, substance use and sexuality. All coefficients
are significant at p < .05. Explained variance for substance use: R 2 ¼ .13. Explained variance for sexuality: R 2 ¼ .43.

stance use is related to psychopathic traits, but not to attach- of this variable (B ¼ 1.675, p < .05). Attachment anxiety
ment representations. was not significantly related to risky sexual behaviours (B ¼
0.017, p ¼ .75). When we tested the direct effects of attach-
ment representations and psychopathic traits on substance use,
Structural Equation Modelling
the fit of the model remained satisfactory and the explained
Fit indices for measurement models of all latent variables (at- proportion of variance reached 15%, CFI ¼ .95; NNFI ¼ .90;
tachment, psychopathy, substance use, and sexual risk-taking) RMSEA ¼ .04; S-B w2/ df ¼ 3.20. Anxious (B ¼ 0.036, p <
indicated that factors were adequately related to their indica- .05) and avoidant representations (B ¼ 0.030, p < .05) were
tors, CFI ¼ .97; NNFI ¼ .95; RMSEA ¼ .04; S-B w2/ df ¼ 3.87 negatively related to substance use but positively related to
(see Figure 1). psychopathic traits (B ¼ 0.539, p < .05). Lastly, a mediational
A direct effect model from attachment representations and model between attachment, psychopathic traits, substance use,
psychopathic traits to risky sexual behaviours was first esti- and risky sexual behaviours was tested. Substance use was now
mated (CFI ¼ .96; NNFI ¼ .95; RMSEA ¼ .05; S-B w2/ df ¼ conceptualized as a mediator of the association between attach-
1.96). Fit indices were adequate. This model explained 14% ment and psychopathic traits on one hand, and risky sexual
of the variation in sexual behaviours. Whereas attachment- behaviours on the other hand.
related avoidance negatively predicted sexual risk-taking (B ¼ Results of the mediational model are presented in Figure 1.
0.490, p < .05), psychopathic traits were a positive predictor This model, combining direct and indirect effects, explained

194 The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality 23(3), 2014, pp. 189–199; doi:10.3138/cjhs.2625
Risky sexual behaviours

43% of the variance in sexual risk-taking and showed satisfac- onset of sexual intercourse in adolescence is already well-
tory fit indices, CFI ¼ .96; NNFI ¼ .96; RMSEA ¼ .044; w2/ documented (Boislard P. & Poulin, 2011; Capaldi, Stoolmiller,
df ¼ 3.87. Attachment anxiety and psychopathic traits signifi- Clark, & Owen, 2002). However, to our knowledge, the deter-
cantly predicted risky sexual behaviours through substance mining role of personality traits typical of psychopathy, such
use; anxiety was associated with reduced substance use while as social dominance, emotional detachment, impulsiveness
psychopathic traits predicted higher alcohol and drug use. In and aggressiveness, in understanding the sexual behaviours
addition to these indirect associations, avoidant attachment of adolescents has not been studied. In the present study, sub-
representations directly predicted less sexual risk-taking. Since stance use mediated the relationship between psychopathic
studies have shown gender differences on some of these vari- traits and early sexual behaviours. This finding is an important
ables, an analysis of the model’s between-gender invariance contribution to the literature because it supports the hypothesis
was performed. The model proved to be invariant between that sexual risk-taking is partly explained by disinhibited
young females and males, CFI ¼ .96; NNFI ¼ .96; RMSEA ¼ behaviours which emerge from dispositional impulsivity and
.044; w2 ¼ 335.42 df ¼ 160, p ¼ .55. insensitivity. Thus, for a specific sub-group of adolescents who
present elevated psychopathic traits, there may be a higher risk
that early sexual debut may produce maladaptive outcomes
DISCUSSION because sexual behaviours represent opportunistic behaviours
emitted in a context of coldness, coercion and striving for
Adolescent sexual risk-taking serves multiple developmental
dominance, facilitated by alcohol and/or drug use.
functions. For young females and males, these behaviours
The present study also provides novel findings regarding
contribute to the construction, affirmation, and consolidation
the relationship between attachment representations and ado-
of different aspects of one’s general sense of personal identity, lescent sexual behaviours; although the attachment model is
including gender identity, social status, intimacy regulation,
well-validated in adults, empirical investigations of this nature
and sexual identity. Because early sexual debut generally pre-
are extremely scarce in adolescent populations. Our results
dicts maladaptive outcomes, the search to identify precursors
revealed that avoidant and anxious attachment representa-
of these behaviours has important practical implications. In
tions play a protective role against adolescent sexual risk-
the present study, the prevalence of sexual risk-taking in ado-
taking. The findings for avoidance are partially consistent with
lescents under 14 years of age is noteworthy and reached 7%,
those of Tracy et al. (2003), who found that avoidant adoles-
a proportion that is congruent with what has been observed
cents became sexually active later than their secure and
in large population samples (Eaton et al., 2012). Furthermore, anxious peers, and had lower frequency of intercourse during
nearly a third of our respondents had had sexual intercourse
the 6 months before the study. Although the evidence for the
for the first time at age 14 or 15. These adolescents may not
role of substance use is somewhat mixed, along with Tracy
be well-prepared to deal with some of the potential negative
et al., our results indicate that, in adolescents, attachment-
outcomes of their sexual behaviours.
related avoidance is a significant obstacle to the development
There were no significant gender differences in this pattern
of sexual behaviours. Past studies have shown that early
of results. Teenage boys did not engage in sexual intercourse
sexual debut often occurs in stable romantic relationships
earlier or with more partners than girls. This finding is incon-
(Rodgers, 1996), and adolescents with avoidant attachment
sistent with results of previous studies which have found that representations are known to have fewer and less serious
boys become sexually active earlier and with more partners
romantic relationships (Tracy et al., 2003). This potential dis-
than girls (Eaton et al., 2012). Whereas one should be cautious
comfort with sexual intimacy in close relationships extends to
not to over-interpret results that support the null hypothesis,
cohabiting and married couples, in which avoidance predicts
the absence of gender effects does not support the main pre-
reduced sexual activity with one’s partner (Brassard, Shaver, &
dictions from evolutionary or social learning models that, for
Lussier, 2007) and more infidelity (Russell, Baker, & McNulty,
biological or cultural reasons, young females should be more
2013).
guarded against early sexual behaviours and multiple partners
Attachment anxiety also significantly slowed the transition
either because they are more protective of their future paren- to an active and diversified sex life but only when mediated
tal investments or because they are differentially rewarded for
by lower substance use. This finding is not consistent with
protecting their sexual security and reputation (Poppen, 1995).
the results of Tracy et al., or with the hypothesis that early
Whether this represents a cross-national difference between
sexual behaviours are a behavioural response to overwhelm-
American and French-Canadian teens, with whom the present
ing attachment fears. However, anxiously-attached cohabiting
study was conducted, a contemporary trend toward homogeni-
adults report less frequent sexual intercourse (Brassard et al.,
zation of sexual practices (Poppen, 1995) or a finding specific
2007). Future studies will be needed to clarify the role of
to a sub-group of adolescents with mental health problems
attachment anxiety in the transition from adolescence to
(Seth et al., 2012) will have to be determined in future studies. adulthood. It is not clear whether anxious attachment in ado-
Our findings show that sexual risk-taking is predicted by
lescence is a marker of future sexual inhibition, or if the shift in
substance use as well as by attachment representations and
primary attachment figures from parents to sexual/romantic
psychopathic traits. The role of alcohol and drug use in the
partners masks the ‘‘real’’ association between attachment

The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality 23(3), 2014, pp. 189–199; doi:10.3138/cjhs.2625 195
Carmen Lemelin et al.

anxiety and sexuality in adolescence. For example, Tobey Future research in this area would also benefit from a
(2001) showed that teenage girls whose attachment to their longitudinal examination of the association between attach-
mother is insecure are likely to experience early sexual inter- ment representations, psychopathic traits, substance use, and
course. In the present study, only attachment to the romantic sexual decision-making in adolescents. The relationship be-
partner was measured. It may be important in future investi- tween adolescent sexual decision-making and these predis-
gations with adolescents to assess attachment to parents and posing or precipitating factors is complex, and early sexual
to the romantic/sexual partner. behaviours may bring about negative changes in attachment
Finally, it is important to note that the structural relations security and intensify psychopathic traits. The validity of
tested in the present study were all gender-invariant. The dynamic, bidirectional models will need to be tested rigorously.
possible explanations for this lack of significant difference This may help to more accurately assess the consequences of
are numerous and should be examined in future studies: for early sexual behaviours and multiple partners on subsequent
instance, systematic misreporting of sexual behaviours by young outcomes and on longstanding relationships in emerging
males and females, cross-national disparities, progressive stan- adulthood.
dardization of sexual experiences, or emergent changes that
may take place more clearly only after the transition to adult-
hood is completed. Furthermore, because recent studies have
REFERENCES
identified the role of pubertal maturation in sexual risk-taking Bachman, J.G., Wadsworth, K.N., O’Malley, P.M., Johnston, L.D., &
(e.g., James, Ellis, Schlomer, & Garber, 2012), it may be im- Schulenberg, J.E. (1997). Smoking, drinking, and drug use in
portant to examine potential gender disparities in sexuality young adulthood: The impact of new freedoms and new responsi-
in light of early puberty. bilities. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Whatever the causal pathways, the present findings suggest Bartholomew, K. (1990). Avoidance of intimacy: An attachment
the simultaneous action of two distinct effects: insecurely- perspective. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 7(2),
attached adolescents are less willing than their securely- 147–178. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0265407590072001
attached peers to engage in early sexual intercourse and to Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L.M. (1991). Attachment styles
have more partners, whereas adolescents with psychopathic among young adults: A test of a four-category model. Journal of
traits may be impulsive decision-makers especially when under Personality and Social Psychology, 61(2), 226–244.
the influence of alcohol or drugs. Thus, our results are consis- http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.61.2.226
tent with an individual differences perspective on adolescent Bentler, P.M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models.
sexual risk-taking. Variations in these traits and behaviours Psychological Bulletin, 107(2), 238–246. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
may help to identify adolescents who are prone to sexual im- 0033-2909.107.2.238
pulsivity and who put themselves at risk of experiencing the Blair, R.J.R., Mitchell, D.G.V., Peschardt, K.S., Colledge, E., Leonard,
consequences of under-controlled decision-making in the R.A., Shine, J.H., . . . , & Perrett, D.I. (2004). Reduced sensitivity to
sexual domain. In adolescents, attachment representations, others’ fearful expressions in psychopathic individuals. Personality
psychopathic traits, and substance use are difficult to change and Individual Differences, 37(6), 1111–1122. http://dx.doi.org/
and can be moving targets. Multi-component programs de- 10.1016/j.paid.2003.10.008
signed to enhance self-regulation, socioemotional skills and Boislard P., M.-A., & Poulin, F. (2011). Individual, familial, friends-
attachment strategies may help reduce some of the negative related and contextual predictors of early sexual intercourse.
outcomes associated with early sexual onset and high numbers Journal of Adolescence, 34(2), 289–300. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.adolescence.2010.05.002
of sexual partners.
Although the present study examined sexuality as a risk Brakefield, T., Wilson, H., & Donenberg, G. (2012). Maternal models
factor, it is important to recognize that, in adolescence, sexual of risk: Links between substance use and risky sexual behavior in
African American female caregivers and daughters. Journal of
behaviours generally also have an adaptive function associated
Adolescence, 35(4), 959–968. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
with positive contributions to psychosocial outcomes and iden-
adolescence.2012.01.004
tity development. Future research should attempt to separate
Brassard, A., Shaver, P.R., & Lussier, Y. (2007). Attachment, sexual
adaptive from risky sexual behaviours and study their multiple
experience, and sexual pressure in romantic relationships: A dyadic
consequences simultaneously. To be sexually active allows teens
approach. Personal Relationships, 14(3), 475–493. http://dx.doi.org/
to experience, for the first time, a situation of great intimacy 10.1111/j.1475-6811.2007.00166.x
and interdependence, in which both partners can learn from
Brennan, K.A., Clark, C.L., & Shaver, P.R. (1998). Self-report mea-
each other while promoting self-disclosure and love (Patrick,
surement of adult attachment. In J.A. Simpson & W.S. Rholes
Maggs, & Abar, 2007). Sexual activity within a teenage couple (Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 46–76).
may confirm the maturity of the relationship and demonstrate New York, NY: Guilford Press.
commitment (Patrick et al., 2007). At a certain age, sexually
Brinkley, C.A., Schmitt, W.A., Smith, S.S., & Newman, J.P. (2001).
active adolescents may come to be perceived by their peers as Construct validation of a self-report psychopathy scale: Does
popular, desirable and mature (O’Sullivan, Meyer-Balhburg, & Levenson’s self-report psychopathy scale measure the same con-
Watkins, 2000). Thus, this brings about social gains (Kinsman, structs as Hare’s psychopathy checklist-revised? Personality and
Romer, Furstenberg, & Schwarz, 1998) by which adolescents Individual Differences, 31(7), 1021–1038. http://dx.doi.org/
may demonstrate their independence from their parents. 10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00178-1

196 The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality 23(3), 2014, pp. 189–199; doi:10.3138/cjhs.2625
Risky sexual behaviours

Browne, M.W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing Fowles, D.C. (1980). The three arousal model: Implications of Gray’s
model fit. In K.A. Bollen & J.S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural two-factor learning theory for heart rate, electrodermal activity,
equation models (pp. 136–162). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage and psychopathy. Psychophysiology, 17(2), 87–104.
Publications. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1980.tb00117.x
Buhrmester, D., & Furman, W. (1987). The development of com- Frodi, A., Dernevik, M., Sepa, A., Philipson, J., & Bragesjö, M.
panionship and intimacy. Child Development, 58(4), 1101–1113. (2001). Current attachment representations of incarcerated
http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1130550 offenders varying in degree of psychopathy. Attachment &
Capaldi, D.M., Stoolmiller, M., Clark, S., & Owen, L.D. (2002). Human Development, 3(3), 269–283. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
Heterosexual risk behaviors in at-risk young men from early 14616730110096889
adolescence to young adulthood: Prevalence, prediction, and Furman, W., & Wehner, E.A. (1997). Adolescent romantic relation-
association with STD contraction. Developmental Psychology, ships: A developmental perspective. In S. Schulman & W.A.
38(3), 394–406. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.38.3.394 Collins (Eds.), New directions for child development, (No. 78).
Catchpole, R.E.H. (2008). Attachment to caregivers and psychopathic Romantic relationships in adolescence: Developmental Perspectives
characteristics among adolescents at risk for aggression, (Unpub- (pp. 21–36). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass; 10.1002/
lished doctoral dissertation). Simon Fraser University, Canada. cd.23219977804.
Chun, H., & Mobley, M. (2010). Gender and grade-level compari- Garriguet, D. (2005). Relations sexuelles précoces [Early sexual rela-
sons in the structure of problem behaviors among adolescents. tions]. Rapports sur la santé, 16 (3), 11–22.
Journal of Adolescence, 33(1), 197–207. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ Guidano, V.F. (1987). Complexity of the self: A developmental
j.adolescence.2009.03.010 research approach to psychopathology and therapy. New York,
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), NY: Guilford Press.
155–159. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155 Hanson, M.D., & Chen, E. (2007). Socioeconomic status and
Medline:19565683 substance use behaviors in adolescents: The role of family
Cooper, M.L., & Orcutt, H.K. (1997). Drinking and sexual experience resources versus family social status. Journal of Health Psychol-
on first dates among adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, ogy, 12(1), 32–35. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1359105306069073
106(2), 191–202. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.106.2.191 Harpur, T.J., Hare, R.D., & Hakstian, A.R. (1989). Two-factor
Davis, D., Shaver, P.R., & Vernon, M.L. (2004). Attachment style conceptualization of psychopathy: Construct validity and
and subjective motivations for sex. Personality and Social assessment implications. Psychological Assessment, 1(1), 6–17.
Psychology Bulletin, 30(8), 1076–1090. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.1.1.6
0146167204264794 Hazan, C., & Zeifman, D. (1994). Sex and the psychological tether.
Del Giudice, M. (2011). Sex differences in romantic attachment: A In K. Bartholomew & D. Perlman (Eds.), Advances in personal
meta-analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(2), relationships (Vol. 5). Attachment processes in adulthood (pp.
193–214. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167210392789 151–177). London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
Dodge, K.A., & Albert, D. (2012). Evolving science in adolescence: Institute of Medecine and National Research Council. (2011). The
Comment on Ellis at al. (2012). Developmental Psychology, 48(3), science of adolescent risk-taking: Working summary. Washington,
624–627. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0027683 DC: National Academies Press.
Doherty, N.A., & Feeney, J.A. (2004). The composition of attachment James, J., Ellis, B.J., Schlomer, G.L., & Garber, J. (2012). Sex-specific
networks throughout the adult years. Personal Relationships, 11(4), pathways to early puberty, sexual debut, and sexual risk taking:
469–488. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.2004.00093.x Tests of an integrated evolutionary-developmental model. Devel-
opmental Psychology, 48(3), 687–702. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
Droomers, M., Schrijvers, C.T.M., Casswell, S., & Mackenbach, J.P.
a0026427
(2003). Occupational level of the father and alcohol consumption
during adolescence; patterns and predictors. Journal of Epidemi- Jessor, R., Donovan, J.E., & Costa, F.M. (1991). Beyond adolescence:
ology and Community Health, 57(9), 704–710. http://dx.doi.org/ Problem behavior and young adult development. New York, NY:
10.1136/jech.57.9.704 Cambridge University Press.
Eaton, D.K., Kann, L., Kinchen, S., Shanklin, S., Flint, K.H., Hawkins, Johnston, L.D., O’Malley, P.M., Bachman, J.G., & Schulenberg, J.E.
J., . . . , & Wechsler, H.; Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2007). Monitoring the future. National results on adolescent drug
(CDC). (2012). Youth risk behavior surveillance – United States, use: Overview of key findings, 2006 (NIH Publication No. 07 –
2011. MMWR. Surveillance Summaries, 61(4), 1–162. 6202). Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse.
http://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/preview/mmwrhtml/ss6104a1.htm Jöreskog, K.G., & Sörbom, D. (1993). LISREL 8: Structural equation
Ellis, B.J., Del Giudice, M., Dishion, T.J., Figueredo, A.J., Gray, P., modeling with the SIMPLIS command language. Chicago, IL:
Griskevicius, V., . . . , & Wilson, D.S. (2012). The evolutionary Scientific Software International.
basis of risky adolescent behavior: Implications for science, policy, Karpman, B. (1948). The myth of the psychopathic personality.
and practice. Developmental Psychology, 48(3), 598–623. American Journal of Psychiatry, 104(9), 523–534.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0026220 http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.104.9.523
Friedlmeier, W., & Granqvist, P. (2006). Attachment transfer among Kashubeck-West, S., & Szymanski, D.M. (2008). Risky sexual be-
Swedish and German adolescents: A prospective longitudinal havior in gay and bisexual men: Internalized heterosexism,
study. Personal Relationships, 13(3), 261–279. http://dx.doi.org/ sensation seeking and substance use. Counseling Psychologist, 36(4),
10.1111/j.1475-6811.2006.00117.x 595–614. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0011000007309633

The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality 23(3), 2014, pp. 189–199; doi:10.3138/cjhs.2625 197
Carmen Lemelin et al.

Kernberg, O.F. (2011). The inseparable nature of love and aggression: O’Sullivan, L.F., Meyer-Balhburg, H.F.L., & Watkins, B.X. (2000).
Clinical and theoretical perspectives. Washington, DC: American Social cognitions associated with pubertal development in a sample
Psychiatric Publishing. of urban, low-income, African-American and Latina girls and
Kinsman, S.B., Romer, D., Furstenberg, F.F., & Schwarz, D.F. (1998). mothers. Journal of Adolescent Health, 27(4), 227–235.
Early sexual initiation: The role of peer norms. Pediatrics, 102(5), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1054-139X(99)00111-1
1185–1192. http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.102.5.1185 Patrick, C.J. (2006). Back to the future: Cleckley as a guide to the
Kubak, F.A. (2009). Psychopathy and the role of anxiety in childhood next generation of psychopathy research. In C.J. Patrick (Ed.),
populations (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Alabama Handbook of psychopathy (pp. 605–617). New York, NY:
University, USA. Guilford Press.
Lafontaine, M.-F., & Lussier, Y. (2003). Structure bidimensionnelle Patrick, M.E., Maggs, J.L., & Abar, C.C. (2007). Reasons to have sex,
de l’attachement amoureux: Anxiété face à l’abandon et évitement personal goals, and sexual behavior during the transition to college.
de l’intimité. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 35(1), 56– Journal of Sex Research, 44(3), 240–249. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0087187 00224490701443759
Le Gall, D., & Le Van, C. (2007). La première fois. Le passage à la Petersen, J.L., & Hyde, J.S. (2010). A meta-analytic review of research
sexualité adulte. [The first time: The transition to adult sexuality] on gender differences in sexuality, 1993–2007. Psychology &
Paris: Payot. Health, 136, 21–38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0017504
Lester, P., Stein, J.A., Bursch, B., Rice, E., Green, S., Penniman, T., & Poppen, P.J. (1995). Gender and patterns of sexual risk taking in
Rotheram-Borus, M.-J. (2010). Family-based processes associated college students. Sex Roles, 32(7-8), 545–555. http://dx.doi.org/
with adolescent distress, substance use and risky sexual behavior 10.1007/BF01544188
in families affected by maternal HIV. Journal of Clinical Child Rodgers, J.L. (1996). Sexual transition in adolescence. In J.A. Graber,
and Adolescent Psychology, 39(3), 328–340. http://dx.doi.org/ J. Brooks-Gunn, & A.C. Peterson (Eds.), Transition through
10.1080/15374411003691677 adolescence: Interpersonal domains and context (pp. 85–110).
Levenson, M.R., Kiehl, K.A., & Fitzpatrick, C.M. (1995). Assessing Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
psychopathic attributes in a noninstitutionalized population. Rosenstein, D.S., & Horowitz, H.A. (1996). Adolescent attachment
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(1), 151–158. and psychopathology. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol-
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.68.1.151 ogy, 64(2), 244–253. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.64.2.244
Lonczak, H.S., Abbott, R.D., Hawkins, J.D., Kosterman, R., & Rotermann, M. (2005). Sex, condoms, and STDs among young
Catalano, R.F. (2002). Effects of the Seattle social development people. Health Reports, 16(3), 39–45.
project on sexual behavior, pregnancy, birth, and sexually trans- Ruiz, B.S., Stevens, S.J., Fuhriman, J., Bogart, J.G., & Korchmaros,
mitted disease outcomes by age 21 years. Archives of Pediatrics & J.D. (2009). A juvenile drug court model in Southern Arizona:
Adolescent Medicine, 156(5), 438–447. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/ Substance abuse, delinquency, and sexual risk outcomes by gender
archpedi.156.5.438 and race/ethnicity. Journal of Offender Rehabilitation, 48(5), 416–
Mack, T.D., Hackney, A.A., & Pyle, M. (2011). The relationship 438. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10509670902979637
between psychopathic traits and attachment behavior in a non- Russell, V.M., Baker, L.R., & McNulty, J.K. (2013). Attachment
clinical population. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(5), security and infidelity in marriage: Do studies of dating relation-
584–588. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.05.019 ships really inform us about marriage? Journal of Family
Mahalik, J.R., Levine Coley, R., McPherran Lombardi, C., Doyle Psychology, 27(2), 242–251. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032118
Lynch, A., Markowitz, A.J., & Jaffee, S.R. (2013). Changes in Satorra, A., & Bentler, P.M. (1994). Corrections to test statistics and
health risk behaviors for males and females from early adolescence standard errors in covariance structure analysis. In A. von Eye &
through early adulthood. Health Psychology, 32(6), 685–694. C.C. Clogg (Eds.), Latent variables analysis: Applications for
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0031658 developmental research (pp. 399–419). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
Mezzich, A.C., Tarter, R.E., Giancola, P.R., Lu, S., Kirisci, L., & Publications.
Parks, S. (1997). Substance use and risky sexual behavior in Savard, C., Sabourin, S., & Lussier, Y. (2006). Male sub-threshold
female adolescents. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 44(2-3), 157– psychopathic traits and couple distress. Personality and Individual
166. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0376-8716(96)01333-6 Differences, 40(5), 931–942. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P.R. (2003). The attachment behavioral 2005.10.001
system in adulthood: Activation, psychodynamics, and interper- Schmitt, D.P., Realo, A., Voracek, M., & Allik, J. (2008). Why can’t a
sonal processes. In M.P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental man be more like a woman? Sex differences in big five personality
social psychology, Vol. 35 (pp. 53–152). San Diego, CA: Elsevier traits across 55 cultures. Journal of Personality and Social
Academic Press. Psychology, 94(1), 168–182. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-
Monshower, K., Van Dorsselaer, S., Gorter, A., Verdurmen, J., & 3514.94.1.168
Vollebergh, W. (2004). Jeugd en riskant gedrag: kerngegevens uit Seth, P., Lang, D.L., Diclemente, R.J., Braxton, N.D., Crosby, R.A.,
het peilstationsonderzoek 2003. Utrect, Netherlands: Trimbos- Brown, L.K., . . . , & Donenberg, G.R. (2012). Gender differences
instituut. in sexual risk behaviours and sexually transmissible infections
Outcalt, J. (2007). The romantic relationships associated with among adolescents in mental health treatment. Sexual Health,
psychopathy: Approach or avoidance? (Unpublished doctoral 9(3), 240–246. http://dx.doi.org/10.1071/SH10098
dissertation). Southern Illinois University Carbondale, USA.

198 The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality 23(3), 2014, pp. 189–199; doi:10.3138/cjhs.2625
Risky sexual behaviours

Simpson, J.A. (1990). Influence of attachment styles on romantic Tobey, C.P. (2001). Parental attachment, adolescent sexual behaviour
relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59(5), and stressful events (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Seattle
971–980. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.59.5.971 Pacific University, USA.
Steiger, J.H. (2000). Point estimation, hypothesis testing, and interval Tracy, J.L., Shaver, P.R., Albino, A.W., & Cooper, M.L. (2003).
estimation using the RMSEA: Some comments and a reply to Attachment styles and adolescent sexuality. In P. Florsheim (Ed.),
Hayduk and Glaser. Structural Equation Modeling, 7(2), 149–162. Adolescent romance and sexual behavior: Theory, research, and
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15328007SEM0702_1 practical implications (pp. 137–159). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Steinberg, L. (2008). A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent Erlbaum Associates.
risk-taking. Developmental Review, 28(1), 78–106. Zimmer-Gembeck, M.J., Siebenbruner, J., & Collins, W.A. (2004).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.002 A prospective study of intraindividual and peer influences
Szielasko, A.L., Symons, D.K., & Price, E.L. (2013). Development on adolescents’ heterosexual romantic and sexual behaviour.
of an attachment-informed measure of sexual behavior in late Archives of Sexual Behavior, 33(4), 381–394. http://dx.doi.org/
adolescence. Journal of Adolescence, 36(2), 361–370. 10.1023/B:ASEB.0000028891.16654.2c
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2012.12.008

The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality 23(3), 2014, pp. 189–199; doi:10.3138/cjhs.2625 199
View publication stats

Você também pode gostar