Você está na página 1de 11

Mantras and Bird Songs

Author(s): Frits Staal


Source: Journal of the American Oriental Society, Vol. 105, No. 3, Indological Studies
Dedicated to Daniel H. H. Ingalls (Jul. - Sep., 1985), pp. 549-558
Published by: American Oriental Society
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/601529 .
Accessed: 10/06/2014 08:34

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

American Oriental Society is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Journal of
the American Oriental Society.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 91.229.229.56 on Tue, 10 Jun 2014 08:34:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
MANTRAS AND BIRD SONGS
FRITS STAAL
UNIVERSITYOF CALIFORNIA

Mantras, invariably regarded as ancient and occupying a realm "beyond language," are invariant
across linguistic boundaries and are used in a manner which is different from linguistic expressions:
for example, in the contexts of ritual, chant, recitation or meditation, the distinction between
meaningful and meaningless, which is basic to language, is irrelevant to their use. Mantras often
consist of fragments, and are repeated endlessly, or reduced to nothing. Vedic mantras result from
"le decoupage des vieux hymnes en formules ou mrme en fragments devenus des corps inertes dans
la trame liturgique"(Renou). In all these respects it looks as if mantras are the vestiges of something
different from language that originated for a different purpose or in response to a different
challenge. It is not surprising, therefore, that there are analogies in structure, function and status
between mantras and bird songs.

IN 1980, 1 CONTRIBUTED an essay entitled "Ritual such mantras is that their form is adapted to their new
Syntax" to the Ingalls Festschrift edited by Nagatomi, phonological environment, just as Sanskrit homa or
Matilal, Masson and Dimock.* In that essay I argued dhjydnabecomes Japanese goma or zen. Such transfor-
that in Vedic ritual, as in mantra recitation, the mations are akin not to translation but to translitera-
function of language is phonetic and syntactic, not tion. This applies equally to apparently "meaningless"
semantic. This implies that neither ritual, nor mantras, mantras such as on? or hum phat and to apparently
should be regarded as a kind of language; for the "meaningful" mantras such as namo buddhaiya or
primary semantic distinction, that between meaningful sivdya namah. The evidence for the untranslatability of
and meaningless, which is basic to language in all its mantras is plentiful all over Asia (see, e.g., Hooykaas,
uses, is absent from ritual and mantras. I also drew a Skorupski and Strickmann in AGNI II). Among the
further conclusion: syntax in language has a ritual "Bauddha Brahmans" of Bali, for example, namo
origin, and language developed from syntactic struc- buddhaya or sivdya namah are not translated as "Hail
tures to which meanings were added subsequently. The to Buddha!" or "Hail to Siva!," but remain in their
physical structures of ritual and the sound structures of original form (see Hooykaas 1973, passim). We meet
mantras are therefore older than language. I did not not only with such clear-cut cases but also with
specify how semantics came to be added to syntax, and mixtures of translated and untranslated (because
thus how language was born, because I did not pay untranslatable) forms. This is not confined to "Greater
sufficient attention to the missing link between ritual India," but occurs on the subcontinent itself as in the
and language, viz., mantras. following example "translated"from the Tamil: "Kan
One reason for this omission was that the Indian Kan Kal Kal Cunatin Pin Pin Kalai Kalai Separate
evidence presented what seemed to be an insurmount- Separate Tuva Tuva Tuma Mantirakali Lord of the
able obstacle to the theory: many mantras are expres- Smoke Svaha!" (Diehl, 1956, 290).
sions in Vedic or Sanskrit, and it seems therefore There are additional reasons which support the idea
preposterous to claim that they are "older than that mantras are older than language. Mantras may or
language." However, a closer inspection of the facts may not consist of language; but they are treated, in
convinced me that my first idea, though not always either case, in a manner which is totally different from
clearly formulated, had been correct. Mantras, first of
all. are not language-bound. Whereas languages can be * I wish to thank Luis Baptista and Ben-Ami Scharfstein for
translated into each other, mantras often cannot. In permission to quote from their unpublished manuscripts. I am
fact, when Vedic or Sanskrit is translated into another grateful for comments on an earlier draft to Robert Goldman,
language, mantras remain invariant. Thus we find the to Mark Juergensmeyerand especially to Pamela MacFarland
same or similar mantras in India, southeast Asia, who pinpointed not only stylistic infelicities but also insidious
China, Koiea and Japan. The only difference between non-sequiturs.

549

This content downloaded from 91.229.229.56 on Tue, 10 Jun 2014 08:34:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
550 Journal of the American Oriental Society 105.3 (1985)

the way we use language. The situation is similar to a Renou called it; or that they result from "le decoupage
common occurrence in biology when a part of the body des vieux hymnes en formules ou meme en fragments
that seems to have come into being for a specific devenus des corps inertes dans la trame liturgique"
purpose is used for something quite different; for (Renou, 1960, 76; quoted in Malamoud, 1983, 33).
example, when legs, which seem to have come into While form, in a natural language, is at most as
being for walking or running, and perhaps kicking, are important as meaning, the form of mantras is more
used for swimming. Legs are used, in that case, as if important than their meaning. In this respect, mantras
they were fins. If we assume that language was preceded are a typical product of Indian civilisation a civilisa-
by mantras in the course of biological evolution, but tion where form is all-important, where, as in
that the "original" mantras are no longer there, it Malamoud's summary of Renou's formulations, "c'est
follows that all we have access to at present is mantras- l'arrangement des formes et la speculation sur les
couched-in-language. These vestigal mantras are not formes qui rev&lentle plus clairement le contenu, qui
used for the expression of meaning, like the other sont, en fin de compte, le contenu" (Renou, 1978, 3).
forms of language, but they are used differently, like Mantras are not confined to India, but this insistence
the original mantras. In other words, we have an on form may explain why India has produced more
analogy: language is to mantras what legs are to fins. mantras than any other civilization. Be this as it may,
We may formulate this conclusion in slightly dif- there is every reason to accept as a well-established fact
ferent terms by distinguishing between apparently that mantras, even if they consist of language, are not
meaningless mantras such as om and apparently used in the manner of language. For further argument
meaningful mantras such as siva va narnah. Let us and discussion see Staal, 1984a (mantras are the missing
assume that om was an "original" mantra; that is, we link between ritual and language), Staal, 1984b (man-
assume it existed before language was born. Suppose tras are like music) and Staal 1985a (mantras are bits
that Pithecanthropus, for example, was humming oni and pieces from the Vedas put to ritual use).
but could not talk. Let us further assume that this om In spite of Aristotle's definition of man as Qc)ov
re-appeared in Sanskrit where it is now used in the k6yov 9-tov, it is not obvious that language is the
manner mantras are used, e.g., preceding, following, or defining characteristic of man. It is equally consistent
interspersed between chants and recitations, accom- with the scanty evidence on man's early activities that
panying rites, meditation, etc. This om is not used in language is a relatively recent acquisition in man's
the manner in which other words are used: it is not biological evolution. In either case, if mantras are prior
declined like a noun, for example, but it may be quoted to language, it should be possible to find them in
like other words (e.g., with iti). Now contrast this with earlier stages of evolution, either in early man or
namah, apparently a Sanskrit expression: this
sis'Ac5va elsewhere in the animal kingdom. Since the sounds of
can be construed as a sentence consisting of two nouns early man are no longer audible and our closest
with an implicit verb, there is a kdraka relation between relatives, the nonhuman primates, have not developed
the two nouns which is expressed through case endings, much in the way of vocalization, it may be necessary to
etc. This expression, then, is a mantra, like om, but it is look further afield. This is not uncommon in the study
couched in language: it belongs to language in the of biological evolution. To find parallels to certain
sense that it is governed by the rules of language; but it features of social organization in man, for example,
is also used in the context of ritual recitation, medita- one may have to turn to insects, who represent a phase
tion, etc. Though an expression of language, it may be of biological evolution entirely different from the
used in the manner of om, just as legs, though ordinary mammalian phase to which we ourselves belong.
human limbs and not piscine accretions, may be used Similarly, to find parallels to human music, one should
in the manner of fins. study whales, frogs, insects, and especially birds. And
The hypothesis that mantras are in some respects so it may be rewarding to consider birds and bird songs
unlike language, and are remnants of something that if one searches for parallels to mantras. Professor
preceded language, may explain certain curious and Ingalls has long been interested in birds such as the
hitherto baffling facts about mantras: for example that cakora, (cataka, crow, crane, cuckoo, dove, goose,
they are repeated endlessly, but also that they have a heron, moorhen, parrot, peacock, sheldrake, swan and
proclivity to reduce to nothing (via japa "mutterings" wagtail all of which find their place in "An Anthology
and updmniu"murmurs" to manasa "in thought" and of Sanskrit Court Poetry" (and which, incidentally,
tzisn-im"in silence": Renou, 1949; Renou and Silburn, inspired Thieme, 1975, on crane and heron). Since a
1954); that they consist of "poussiere vedilue," as pioneering spirit and an open mind have infused all his

This content downloaded from 91.229.229.56 on Tue, 10 Jun 2014 08:34:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
STAAL: Mantras and Bird Songs 551

work, an enquiry into mantras and bird songs may texts, and in the version given here, by slanted bars
appeal to our eminent colleague to whom this follow- ("/ "). But in addition we have to take into account
up essay is offered once more as an expression of that ha bu, vy ha vi, and 6 Ii Id are stobhas that
continued friendship and esteem. function as units in many other songs. The correctness
Let us begin with a specific example. When all the of (1) therefore depends on the correctness of other
bricks of the Agnicayana altar have been put in place forms. For example, when the same sort of analysis is
and have been consecrated, there are still many mantras applied to other recitations it yields the following
left in the repertoire of the Adhvaryu (viz., Taittirfya structures:
Samhita 4.4) though there are no more bricks to A A B .../AABC/ DDD// (2)
consecrate and there is no more space to put them. A (the first chant of the Udgata, sung during the
general principle governing Vedic ritual is that there foundation ceremonies of the altar: AGNI I, 410);
should be a one-one-correspondence between mantras AAABBBCCC/ ... / AAABBBCCC'/ D
and rites (see, e.g., Apastamba Srauta Sttra 24.1.38: D' D' / / (3)
ekamantraini karma-ni). Thus we witness the insertion (the first space-filler chant during the consecration of
of a curious rite: 120 black pebbles are put between the the first layer: AGNI 1, 443; note that we use the
bricks like cement and consecrated. These pebbles are convention that C and C' are similar, or have identical
called sarkara in Sanskrit and kilipparan, "chicken- beginnings, and the same holds for D and D');
fish," in Malayalam (see AGNI I, 493ff.). While these A A A / . . . / A A A' / / (4)
pebbles are being consecrated by the Adhvaryu on (agner vrata sung at the northern hip of the altar after
behalf of his patron the Yajamana, the Udgata sings the five layers have been consecrated: AGNI 1, 539);
his pebble songs. The first of these is Jaiminiya A A A B / ... / A A A C B' f, / (5)
Gramageyagana 45.2. 1: (agner arka sung at the head of the altar on the same
occasion); and so forth.
so no ha hu / idam idam pura ha hu / pra va pra Now let us look at a bird song: four stanzas sung by
vasva ia ha vi / nina ninava tam u vo ha hu / sti-4a vi the Black Flycatcher Ficedula hypoleuca published by
ya li / o li la / /
sakhai'a Ya ha li / cdramuoa J. C. Roche ("L'oiseau musicien" no. 6) and analysed
by Franqois-Bernard Mache (1983, 93) in the following
We can represent the structure of this chant by
form:
introducing capital letters to stand for its units, which
are either bits of mantras or stobhas, as follows: A B C B C B D E/
B' C B C B C B F E' /
"A" for iv no A BCBCD /
"B" for ha ho B' C B C B C B F E'/ / (6)
"C" for idam iwan pura Here the capital letters stand for small phrases,
"D" for pra va pra x'asi'a motifs or figures that each consist of a few tones, and
"F" for Ya ha yi that are therefore comparable to the units of mantras
'F" for nin a nina ia taoi u vo that each consist of a few syllables. I have used the
"G" for stasa li sakha i'a slanted bar "/" where Mache begins a new line or
"W"for dramiutavali' "strophe." Here too the selection of units on which the
"I" for S liIa. analysis is based derives from a study of many other
songs that consist of similar units, for example:
The structure of JGG 45.2.1 may now be represented A B B C/ A A A A B! A BBCB/ AAABCB/ A
by: A B C /A A A A CB/AAAC B/ AAB/ AA B
AB / CB / DE / FB / GE / H / J / / (1) C B / A A A B / A A B / A A B B ; A A / (7)
How do we arrive at an analysis of this type? How (song of A(o(rephalus .v(hoenohaenus: Mache 77);
arbitraryare the units we have selected to be represented A AAB/ AAC/ AACB7 AACB/1 AAAC
by capitals? The answer to these questions is that we B/ AAAC/ AB/ AACB/ AAC (8)
can only arrive at a nonarbitrary analysis provided we (song of A/crocephalus dumnetorum: Mache 78);
analyse a number of similar forms. First of all we make A B A B C A'B B'A" B'B (9)
use of the subdivision of the samans into smaller units, (song of SY/via coninfunis, Mache 97; note that A' and
called hhakti, that are preserved by the tradition A" are again similar, hence A and A" are similar, but
(because a new breath has to be taken at the beginning less similar than either A and A' or A' and A"). And so
of each hhakti) and that are represented in printed forth.

This content downloaded from 91.229.229.56 on Tue, 10 Jun 2014 08:34:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
552 Journal of the American Oriental Societi' 105.3 (1985)

If we compare these structures of mantras and bird before: typically, a brahmana interpretation would
songs with each other we find a certain similarity (e.g., analyse sivkya namnahas a sentence of the language,
the doublets, triplets, and refrains discussed in Staal, and assign to it an often arbitrary semantic interpreta-
1984b, occur in all) but no strict identity. However, all tion, whereas a Arauta manual would treat it as an
we are interested in is structural similarity. If the unanalysed unit but specify its ritual use or viniVoga.
structures happened to be strictly identical, it might In the grauta SUtras, mantras are accordingly defined
prove that GG 45.2.1 was caused, influenced or inspired in precise accordance with their ritual use. Katyayana
by the songs of Ficedula hypoleuca, perhaps because Srauta SUtra 1.3.1, for example, defines mantras as
that bird occurred in a part of India where the "rc, yajus, saman, nigada," which comprises all that is
composers of the saman might have heard it. Actually, different from the category of brahmana. In the
Mache (page 82) has argued that a certain structural Mimamsa, mantras are similarly defined and their
pattern in Stravinsky which also is found in a bird song formal character and difference from language are
was copied "plus ou moins consciemment" because similarly stressed (e.g., the order of words in mantras is
the bird is common on the banks of the river Bug fixed, as in Sanskrit within compounds, or in English,
in UJstilug where Stravinsky used to work. Such a and unlike the order of words in ordinary Sanskrit
causal explanation would certainly be interesting if which is free: Staal 1967, 45 sq.). According to such
we could establish it in the realm of the Sdmaveda, definitions, then, samans are mantras. It is not inci-
and it might demonstrate that there is a similarity dental that these ritual definitions are in accordance
in structure which makes such causal links possible. with the historical facts as we know them: for the
Actually, Acrocephalus dumetorum Blyth or "Blyth's stobhas of the Samaveda are the direct precursors in
Reed Warbler" of example (8), which breeds in the Vedic realm of the b-ijamantras of Tantrism (see
Baluchistan, is a winter visitor to the entire subcon- Staal, 1985a).
tinent, and Slv5ia communis Latham, the "White- So far we have chiefly looked at structural patterns.
throat" of example (9) is an autumn migrant to The structural similarity between mantras and bird
Pakistan and northwest India (Ripley 1982. 424). songs is certainly striking, but it would only be
However, similarities in the realm of biological evolu- significant if it were not also to occur in language, the
tion only rarely express specific causal links; what they domain to which mantras have been generally-albeit
do express is similar responses to such things as the unwittingly-assigned by the authors of the Brahmanas
pressure of selection-a feature of the biological theory as well as by contemporary scholars. What we find in
to which I shall return. this respect is unexpected, and therefore intriguing.
Before further discussing the similarity in structure Repetitions such AA, AAA, AAAA, ABBB, etc., and
between mantras and bird songs, I must first deal with refrain-like structures that seem to be common to both
a possible objection, viz., that these Samaveda songs mantras and bird songs, are entirely absent from the
are not mantras at all. But this objection is easily set syntax of ordinary language. In fact, any linguist who
aside. The earliest systematic use of the category of is familiar with syntactic structures cannot fail to be
mantras occurs in the Black Yajurveda: it is here that struck by the absence in almost all such structures of
mantras are distinguished from brahmanas. The man- the typical repetitive features of both mantras and bird
tras are the sounds that are recited during the ritual, songs. There is an area of overlap: the domain of
and the brahmanas are the interpretations given to poetry. However, poetry seems in several respects to
these recitations and their ritual uses. Brahmanas must constitute an intermediary area between mantras and
therefore be formulated in language; but mantras need ordinary language. Excluding poetry (which would
not be-in fact, they are not formulated: in the ritual obviously deserve separate treatment) and confining
texts of the Srauta Sutras, they are treated as units. It ourselves to mantras and bird songs, their similarity in
is true that mantras are interpreted in the Brahmana structure and dissimilarity from the structures of
literature as if they were language; but this is precisely language are certainly curious and suggestive. But are
what makes the Brdhmanas so unreliable as aids to the we not barking up the wrong tree because we are
understanding of their ritual uses. I have argued this confining ourselves to structures at the neglect of
elsewhere and in greater detail, so it may be sufficient meaning and function, categories in terms of which
in the present context to illustrate the difference in mantras and bird songs seem to be vastly different?
character between Brdhmanas and Srauta Siltras I have argued on several occasions that many mantras
(which in actual fact are often combined or overlap in a and rites do not possess a clear meaning or function
specific text) in terms of the examples we considered (e.g., Staal, 1979, and cf. 1985a and b). Many mantras,

This content downloaded from 91.229.229.56 on Tue, 10 Jun 2014 08:34:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
STAAL: Mantras and Bird Songs 553

as well as stobhas or the bIja mantras of Tantrism, strations. Include elections, schools, work groups and
consist of meaningless sound; but even those that are the rituals of family living, and you have covered much
sentences of Vedic or Sanskrit are often unrelated or of social life in Britain to-day" (Goody 1977, 26). There
ill-related to the rites they accompany. As for rites, it is is a partial solution to this problem (see Staal, 1985b),
especially the rites of the srauta ritual that seem to be but it is not relevant in the present context. We shall
devoid of meaning or function. Rites of the grhya or simply conclude that rites and mantras have no clear
"domestic" ritual, on the other hand, seem often quite meaning or function which sets them apart from other
meaningful and functional. But how can there be such things.
a difference in meaning or function if the actual forms We shall revert to this problem but only after having
of srauta and grhya rites, together with the accompany- paid attention to the corresponding problem in the
ing mantras, are so often similar and even identical? realm of birds and bird songs, which seem to have a
We shall approach this problem by first taking a much clearer function. Or do they? To begin with,
closer look at the meaning and function of the grhya ornithologists were satisfied with a general character-
rites, or, more generally, of the general category of ization, stressing first that bird song "probably always
non-srauta rites. This would be too vast a task to serves in one way or another to distinguish the sexes"
undertake within the compass of the present essay had (because bird songs are mostly found in males), and
not Gonda fortunately come to our aid by recently second that "there must be a need to communicate.
publishing a manual in the Handbuch der Orientalistik, The need may be to show the signaler's location
which assembles precisely the data we need under the (contact notes), his awareness of danger (alarm calls),
title: "Vedic Ritual: the non-solemn rites" (Gonda, or his readiness to mate (courtship songs) or defend a
1980). Here we witness rites performed not only in breeding or foraging territory (territorial song)"
connexion with the traditional samskara ceremonies (summarized in Hartshorne 1973, 15).
which correspond in part to the "rites de passage" Another account of the functions of bird song runs
familiar to anthropologists following Van Gennep, but as follows: "The singer, more usually a male than a
also on a great number of other occasions, e.g.: making female, has two primary reasons for which to sing, the
rain, warding off dangerous or annoying animals, one to defend its territory against other males, and the
averting dangers such as fires and robbers, curing other, to attract females. The same song or call (which
illness and preserving health, going on and returning is shorter and simpler than a song) may have different
from a journey, entering a house, conquering enemies, meanings, depending on its context-the call that in
inheriting property, a variety of occupations connected the mating season both defends territory and attracts
with cattle, anniversaries, attaining great age, auster- females functions, after the mating season is over, to
ities, prayers, vows, magic, witchcraft, exorcism, defend territory alone. As against such general signals,
divination, purity and impurity, auspiciousness and there may be specific ones, and these specific ones may
inauspiciousness, learning and concentration, not to be graded in structure, length, and other characteristics,
mention seasonal ceremonies of many kinds. "Thus by which I mean that different intensities may call
occasions were few," concludes Gonda, "when Vedic forth different intensities of response. The occasions
man did not seek the aid of rites and mantras to that have been said to elicit distinctive songs or song-
promote in some way or other his welfare or to avert variants are many. They include the search for a nest-
the ills, real or imaginary, which might invest him" site; the building of the nest; the incubation of the eggs;
(page 410). the spelling of the female in her incubation duties; the
So rites and mantras can have a meaning or function. leaving of the next by the young; migration; and
Our startling earlier conclusion, that rites have no autumn and winter. Calls, too, have many varieties and
meaning, seems to turn into the opposite conclusion functions. One classification includes those for plea-
which is not less startling: there is no meaning or sure, territorial defense, flight, feeding, nesting, flock-
function rites can not possess. But this has always been ing, aggression, general alarm and specialized alarm
a problem with regard to ritual and rites. The British (to distinguish flying from non-flying predators)"
anthropologist Jack Goody has in fact questioned the (Scharfstein, forthcoming).
category of ritual precisely because it seems to include That the same song or call may have different
everything and therefore be empty: according to one meanings depending on the context accords with our
author he takes to task, rituals range "from coronations, earlier observation that rites have all kinds of functions.
to funerals, Christmas, dances, football, theatre, gym- In fact, a similar observation was made by van Gennep
nastics, brass-bands, pop festivals, to student demon- in 1911 ("the same rite . . . can change its meaning

This content downloaded from 91.229.229.56 on Tue, 10 Jun 2014 08:34:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
554 Journal of the American Oriental Society 105.3 (1985)

depending on the position it is given in a ceremony": familiaris L.). The sounds from which these songs are
quoted Staal, 1984b, 5) and by Durkheim in 1915 constructed are primarily three, which Thielcke, using
("according to the circumstances, one and the same German phonology, represents as tir, tit and srih.
ceremony serves two distinct functions": quoted ibid.). Though the functions of some of the songs are clear,
Later in his essay Scharfstein specifies this context the functions of others are not, and ritualization is
dependence in the case of a blackbird song as follows: accordingly postulated. Tree-creepersmoreover exhibit
"Toward the end of the season, when the song has lost all the characteristics of ritualization that Lorenz (195 1)
its overt sexual function, it reaches its most highly and Morris (1957) have enumerated: the primary
organized, in the human sense: most musical, form" sounds are rhythmically repeated; the sequences are
(Scharfstein, forthcoming). changed in such a way that "eine ursprtinglich weitge-
In more specialized studies, ornithologists have felt hend veranderliche und aus verschiedenen Gliedern
the need to make use of the ethologists' concept of bestehende Folge von Verhaltensweisen zu einem
ritualization. This concept is invoked when the original einzigen, in sich starren Bewegungsablaufzusammenge-
function of a behavioral pattern is no longer visible or schweisst wird" (Thielcke, 1964, 405, after Lorenz,
known. Let us start with an example not involving 1951); and there are "typical intensities." The latter
song. In a general study of the behavior and songs of a concept, introduced by Desmond Morris, indicates, for
species of finch (Estrildida), Guttinger (1970) has tried example, that a bird in the course of a courtship dance
to explain a pattern of inflexion ("Verbeugung")during may repeatedl/ytwist its body four times (Morris 1957,
which the male bird bends its head either to the right or 4-5). Since the order of elements, the sequence of
to the left, independent of the position of the female pitches, the intervals between the elements, and the
even if she is close by. Guttinger says (1970, 1054): number of elements are much more constant in songs
"Aus dem Bewegungselement der bisher besprochenen than in sounds, Thielcke has argued that the phylo-
Arten kann infolge des hohen Grades der Ritualisierung genetic development of the song of the European tree-
die ursprungliche Bedeutung der Verhaltensweise nicht creeper has therefore taken place in the direction from
mehr ermittelt werden." sounds to song.
German ornithologists use "ritualization"in contrast I am not an ornithologist and this is not an
to "Ableitungsmdglichkeit," or "possibility to deduce": ornithological paper. I am not in a position, therefore,
the latter term is used when it is possible to exlain a to provide a detailed and exhaustive discussion of the
pattern of behavior, including a song, in functional vast amount of information on bird song that is
terms. Applying the distinction to the realm of Vedic nowadays available. However, a general trend in these
ritual we could say that there exists an "Ableitungsmo- studies seems to be the following. Whereas originally,
glichkeit" for the grhya rites, but not for the grauta most ornithologists stressed functional explanations,
rites. As in the Vedic realm, such a distinction requires and were convinced that there were good pragmatic or
a great deal of analysis in specific cases and can easily utilitarian reasons for the execution of songs by birds,
lead to discussions and controversies. Some young it has increasingly been found that there are no such
finches of the species Spermestes, for example, when in reasons, and that birds often seem to sing simply
the nest and about to be fed by the parents, raise their because they like to. This may be expressed in the more
wings in a special manner. Immelmann (1962) explained specific context of the "ontogeny" of bird song, for
this as a kind of contest between siblings; moreover, example by Nottebohm (1970, 951) when he writes:
the wings can be raised in such a way that the neighbor "One is forced to conclude that the achievement of a
is partly hidden, so that the young bird who performs relatively stable and predictable song does not imply
the act is more likely to be fed first. But the theory does the existence of a preconceived or acquired goal." Or it
not hold, as Gittinger (1970, 1063) has pointed out, may be expressed in much more general and theoretical
because the parents take great care to feed all their terms, as for example by Lorenz (1967, 75): "there are
young equally, including not only those that do not mechanisms in existence which reinforce economical
raise their wings, but also those that are sick. In such perfection in motor skills independently of the attain-
cases, there seemed at first to be an "Ableitungsmd- ment of the ultimate biological goal in whose pursuit
glichkeit," which on further analysis turned out to be the learned movement is developed." Lorenz mentions
spurious. as examples the swimming of a shark, the gallop of an
Detailed examples of the ritualization of songs have antelope, and the gymnastics of a gibbon. Here the
been studied by Thielcke (1964) in the European tree- singing of birds may be plausibly added: in fact, we
creeper (Certhia brachvdacltla Brehm and Certhia have already seen that "pleasure" is listed as one of its

This content downloaded from 91.229.229.56 on Tue, 10 Jun 2014 08:34:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
STAAL: Mantras and Bird Songs 555

possible reasons (Scharfstein quoted above). Lorenz man, the animal that speaks (4CpovXoyov i?Xov), is
adds: "The most convincing argument in favor of my obsessed by language so that we tend to assume that
speculative assumption lies in the fact that acquired any articulate sound is "language" in its usual sense.
motor skills of this type, more than any other types of This is exactly what happened in human history and
movements, are forever being performed for their own prehistory. It has generally been fancied that birds
sake in the obvious absence of any other motivating or could really talk even though the evidence has never
reinforcing factors. Indeed, the very concept of play is been telling. No wonder that mantras, which are often
based on this fact to a large extent." All of this does of indistinguishable from it, continue to be regarded as
course apply to the human animal: "we know that language.
when we skate, ski, or dance, we perform these activities Conceptual issues involving cross-overs between
for their own sake, for the enjoyment they give us" nature and culture tend to be complex. Functionalist
(ibid.). To add music, mantras and ritual is but a explanations in the realm of mantras are always closely
natural extension of this list. connected with problems of semantics, whereas func-
Two of the books and one of the articles from which tionalist explanations in the realm of bird song are not
I have already quoted are partly concerned with a very connected with semantics. However, in both areas,
similar problem: the relation between bird songs and emphasizing syntax combines easily with de-emphasiz-
music. The first book, Charles Hartshorne's Born To ing semantics. Thus there is a close association between
Sing (1973), is by an ornithologist who is also a "Ritual Syntax" and "The Meaninglessness of Ritual."
philosopher; the second, Franqois-Bernard Mache's Similarly, when ornithologists found that it was
Musique, Mothe, Nature (1983) is by an ornithologist difficult or impossible to provide functionalist explana-
who is also a musicologist and composer; the essay by tions, their interest in syntax increased. Thus we find,
Scharfstein, a chapter from a forthcoming book on the since roughly 1965, syntax mentioned in the studies of
nature of art, is by a philosopher and esthetician bird songs by Thielcke, Gtitinger, Baptista, Nicolai,
interested in birds. All are concerned to show that it is Payne, Beer, Nottebohm, Johnson, and Bertram. The
impossible to define "human"music in such a way that work of these scholars is discussed in an unpublished
it will not also encompass bird song. All stress that paper by Luis Baptista, "On Parallels between Bird
birds must accordingly be assumed to possess some- Song and Speech Development." However, the first
thing like an aesthetic sense. (Scharfstein has in fact principled use of syntactic methods and representations
cast doubt on the functionalist bias of some of the on a large scale is found in Mache's book of 1983. The
professional ornithologists: "They tend to assume that author is well aware of the novelty of his approach, for
every nuance of song or display has survival value or he formulates some of his principal questions "aban-
disvalue, and they try, often with conspicuous clever- donnant le point de vue phonetique qui est celui des
ness, to turn each apparent exception into further zoologistes pour un point de vue syntaxique" (Mache,
evidence for the truth of their theses.") In the present 1983, 64).
context I need only the least controversial feature of The contrast between functionalist and non-function-
the conclusion of these three authors: I don't need the alist explanations in biology is closely related to the
hypothesis that birds have an aesthetic sense (though it continuing debates on the theoretical foundations of
may very well be correct) but will merely accept that Darwinian natural selection a problem that can of
these authors have demonstrated beyond reasonable course not be adequately discussed in the present
doubt that bird songs cannot be explained in function- context, but to which at least a brief reference should
alist, utilitarian, or pragmatic terms. be made. The notion of "survival of the fittest," in
We may now return to mantras, only to find that we particular, can be easily misinterpreted in such a
are in a position to make an important observation. manner that it becomes more or less tautologous and
The idea that birds can talk, and that bird songs are a empty. Elredge exposes such views in the following
kind of language, belongs to the realm of myth and has terms: "Only (God, natural selection) could have
never been taken seriously by ornithologists. But the fashioned such a marvellous organic machine! There is
idea that mantras express meaning, and are a kind of a difference, of course: God, as a supernatural being,
language, has generally been taken for granted by does not belong in science, whereas natural selection
Sanskritists. Of course, there are good reasons for this patently does. But used in this inappropriate fashion,
silent assumption. It is due not merely to the circum- natural selection becomes a mere substitute for the
stance that Sanskritists tend to confine themselves to Creator. It tells us nothing, really, about tribolite eyes
Sanskrit, but to the much more important fact that or anything specific or meaningful about how they

This content downloaded from 91.229.229.56 on Tue, 10 Jun 2014 08:34:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
556 Journal of the American Oriental Society 105.3 (1985)

came into existence" (Elredge, 1982, 508-509; quoted are not mantras and language learned, whereas bird
by Koster, 1983. 4). songs are instinctive and inborn? The first reply to any
Such interpretations were actually given by Tom such objection should be that the difference between
Bethell (1976), according to whom "survival of the these two modes of acquisition may be large from the
fittest" involves no more than "differentialreproductive point of view of the individual, but is less important in
success," viz., the production of more surviving off- terms of biological evolution, and need not prevent
spring than other competing members of the popula- certain phenomena from being analogous to each
tion. This formulation, however, defines fitness in other. However, this first reply is not even relevant in
terms of survival only and is therefore empty because the present case, for the distinction between learned
survival of what survives is a tautology. Such interpre- and innate does not obtain in any straightforward
tations are similar to the functionalist bias of some fashion. Language, first of all, unlike languages, is
ornithologists who seem to be "strainingto accomodate partly inborn. One's first or mother language is learnt
awkward facts" (Scharfstein). in a relatively short period and through a process that
Gould (1977, 40-45), from whom the quote from is different from the learning of any subsequent
Bethell has been taken, has convincingly shown that language because "universals of language" are charac-
Darwin did not make such a basic logical mistake. teristic structures of the human mind. This much, if
Biologists define the fittest beforehand, not only by anything, has been established, and established conclu-
their subsequent survival. Natural selection is a theory sively, by Noam Chomsky. It seems entirely likely that
of local adaptation to immediate but changing environ- the same holds true of rites and mantras: though
ments; it does not preach general progress in the specific cases have to be learnt, their general underlying
cosmic sense as inherent in the workings of evolution, structures are innate.
which explains the Victorian unpopularity of the idea. Bird songs, on the other hand, are partly learnt.
In fact, the theory of natural selection did not triumph Kroodsma (1978) provides a comprehensive recent
until the 1940s. Voltaire's Dr. Pangloss asserted that survey on "Aspects of Learning in the Ontogeny of
the nose was fashioned to bear spectacles, but we can Bird Song: Where, From Whom, When, How Many,
now see clearly that our world is not, as Candide Which, and How Accurately?" Kroodsma (1974) had
thought, "the best of all possible worlds." We know, as already shown that juvenile males of the species
Franpois Jacob formulated it in Le jeu des possibles Bewick's wren (Thryomanes bewickii) "are probably
(1981, 33), that "le monde vivant aujourd'hui, tel que capable of learning from their father . .. but a premium
nous le voyons autour de nous, n'est qu'un parmis de is placed on matching the song types of neighboring
nombreux possibles." males with whom interractions will occur throughout
How then are mantras and bird songs related within life; thus, song types of the father may be modified or
the perspective of biological evolution? Their similaritiy new ones learned in order to match more closely the
is not what biologists call "homologous," but what songs of neighboring males." Many birds learn songs
they call "analogous." The difference is explained by only within a "critical period," for example, within the
Gould (1977, 254): "Similar features due to common first 10 months of a potential lifespan of 5 or more
genetic ancestry are 'homologous'; similarities due to years. "After the end of this sensitive period further
common function, but with different evolutionary acoustic stimuli do not alter the bird's repertoire"
histories, are 'analogous': the wings of birds and insects, (Nottebohm 1970, 952). However, in the indigo bird
for example--the common ancestor of both groups (Vidua chalybeata), "flexibility to change song types in
lacked wings." To apply this definition, we have to the repertoireof the individual is apparently maintained
broaden it in a curious fashion: for the similarity throughout life" (Kroodsma 1978, 216). Since so many
between mantras and bird songs is due not to common songs are learnt, "variation in song repertoire sizes
function, but to common non-functionality. Mantras among different populations of a given species may be
and bird songs share not only certain structural striking. Long-billed marsh wrens near Seattle,
properties, but also lack of an inherent or absolute Washington, may have 150 song types, while an Illinois
purpose. It is precisely these features that express the male may have as few as 30" (Kroodsma, 1978, 221).
common characteristic of both as essentially satisfying, That mantras are similar to bird songs may seem
pleasurable and playful features that, in the case of far-fetched to Western Sanskritists, but it would not
mantras, have remained even though language has cause surprise to the saman chanters, other composers
intervened. of mantras, or Indians generally. Following Genesis,
I shall briefly deal with a final possible objection to the Western tradition has generally stressed the dif-
the thesis that mantras and bird songs are analogous: ference between man and the other animals. But in

This content downloaded from 91.229.229.56 on Tue, 10 Jun 2014 08:34:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
STAAL: Mantras and Bird Songs 557

India, the names of many samans are in fact inspired derives from suparna which is not only a lotus leaf, but
by birds. The kraunicamelody evokes a kind of curlew. also a large bird of prey. Vedic schools have been
We also have samans called trtTvakraunicaand even called after birds, e.g., the Kausika after the owl and
vahnidhanakraufca, a kraunca melody "ending in the Taittirfya after tittiri, the partridge (a totemic bird
speech." Other samans are called plava after an aquatic according to Kosambi (1950) who concluded that the
bird, bhasa after a bird of prey, and vas'a after a Taittirfya was a totemic clan-a hypothesis criticized
croaking bird. The well known chant syena is called by Brough (1953)). No wonder that vac was used,
after a vulture (see AGNI I 88-90**), and sauparna already in the Rgveda, not only of men, but also of
other animals and especially of birds.
** There is cross-cultural support for the hypothesis de- Geese informed Janasruti Pautrayana about the
fended in AGNI that isyenawas originally a vulture, and not a great master Raikva-with-the-chariot (Chandogya
hawk, an eagle or a falcon (cf. Schneider 1971). Germaine Upanisad 4.1-3). A goose and a diving bird taught
Dieterlen, grande dame of African ethnology, told me in Paris Satyakama Jabala each a quarter of brahman (Chan-
that among the Komo and among all Bambara societies, the dogya Upanisad 4.7-8). Indian sages have always been
vulture is "l'oiseau de la connaissance initiatique." Its impor- eager to gain knowledge and insight from birds. So
tance derives from the fact that it does not kill. Meat left or why shouldn't we?
procured by others is neutral; in India too (among Buddhist
monks, for example) it is dissociated from himsd. See, e.g.,
Dieterlen and Ciss6 1972, 206.

REFERENCES

AGNI: see Staal 1983. Hartshorne, Charles (1973), Born to Sing. An Interpretation
Baptista, Luis (unpublished), "On Parallels between Bird and World Survey of Bird Song, Bloomington.
Song and Speech Development." Hooykaas, C. (1973), Balinese Bauddha Brahmans, Amster-
Bethel, T. (1976), "Darwin's Mistake," Harpers (February). dam.
Brough, J. (1953), The Early Brahmanical System of Gotra Hooykaas, C. (1983), "Agni Offerings in Java and Bali," in
and Pravara, Cambridge. AGNI 11,382-402.
Burghardt, Gordon M. and Bekoff, Marc (1978), The Immelmann, K. (1962), "Beitrdge zur einen vergleichenden
Development of Behavior: Comparative and Evolu- Biologie australischer Prachtfinken," Zoologisehes Jahr-
tionary Aspects, New York. buch 90.1-196.
Diehl, Carl Gustav (1956), Instrument and Purpose. Studies
Ingalls, Daniel H. H. (1965), An Antholog of Sanskrit Court
on Rites and Rituals in South India, Lund.
Poetry. Vidr'kara 's "Suhhdsiiaratnakosa," Cambridge,
Dieterlen, G. and Ciss6, Y. (1972), Les fondements de la
Mass.
societe d'initiation du Komo, Paris.
Jacob, Franqois (1981), Le jeu des possibles. Essai sur la
Durkheim, E. (1912), Les formes elementaires de la vie
diversite du vivant, Paris.
religieuse. Le systcme totemique en A ustralie, Paris.
Elredge, Niles (1982), "An Ode to Adaptive Transformation," Kosambi, D. D. (1950), "On the Origin of Brahmin Gotras,"
Nature 296, 8 April 1982, 508-509. Journal of the Royal Asiatic Society, Bombay Branch
van Gennep, A. (1911), "De la methode a suivre dans l'6tude 26.21-80.
des rites et des mythes," Revue de l'Universiue de Koster, Jan (1983), "Karel, Darwin en Chomsky. Over taal en
Bruxelles, 505-523. evolutie." Hollands Maandblad 3-21.
Goody, Jack (1977), "Against 'Ritual': Loosely Structured Kroodsma, Donald E. (1974), "Song Learning, Dialects, and
Thoughts on a Loosely Defined Topic," in Moore and Dispersal in the Bewick's wren," Zeitschrift fur Tier-
Myerhoff, eds., 1977, 25-35. psychologie 35.352-380.
Gould, Stephen Jay (1977), Ever Since Darwin. Reflections in Kroodsma, Donald E. (1978), "Aspects of Learning in the
Natural History, New York. Ontogeny of Bird Song: Where, From Whom, When,
GUttinger, Hans Rudolf (1970), "Zur Evolution von Verhal- How Many, Which, and How Accurately?"in Burghardt
tensweisen und Lutdusserungen bei Prachtfinken (Estri- and Bekoff, 215-230.
lididae)," Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie 27:8 (Novem- Lorenz, Konrad (1951), "Ober die Enstehung auslosender
ber) 1011- 1075. 'Zeremonien'," Die Vogelwarte 16.9-13.

This content downloaded from 91.229.229.56 on Tue, 10 Jun 2014 08:34:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
558 Journal of the American Oriental Society 105.3 (1985)

Lorenz, Konrad (1967), Evolution and Modification of Skorupski, Tadeusz (1983), "Tibetan Homa Rites," in AGNI
Behavior, Chicago. 11,403-417.
Mdche, Franqois-Bernard (1983), Musique, Mythe, Nature; Staal, J. F. (1967), Word Order in Sanskrit and Universal
ou les Dauphins d'Arion, Paris. Grammar, Dordrecht.
Malamoud, Charles (1983), "Exegese de rites. Exegese de Staal, Frits (1979), "The Meaninglessness of Ritual," Numen.
textes," Purusirtha 7.17-38. International Journal of the Histor, of Religions 26.
Moore, S. F. and Myerhoff, B. G., (eds.) (1977), Secular 2-22.
Ritual, Amsterdam. Staal, Frits (1980), "Ritual Syntax," in: Nagatomi, etc., eds.,
Morris, Desmond (1957), "'Typical Intensity'and its Relation 119-142.
to the Problem of Ritualization, " Behaviour 11.1- 12. Staal, Frits (1983), A GNI. The Vedic Ritual of the Fire A liar,
Nagatomi, N., Matilal, B. K., Masson, J. M. and Dimock E. 1-11, Berkeley (referred to as: AGNI).
C. (eds.) (1980), Sanskrit and Indian Studies. Essays in Staal, Frits (1984a), "Ritual, Mantras and the Origin of
Honour of Daniel H. H. Ingalls, Dordrecht. Language," in: Anmrfadhara.Professor R. N. Dandekar
Nottebohm, Fernando (1970), "Ontogeny of Bird Song," Felicitation Volume, Poona, 403-425.
Science 167/2, 13 February 1970, 950-956. Staal, Frits (1984b), "The Search for Meaning: Mathematics,
Renou, Louis (1949), "La valeur du silence dans le culte Music, and Ritual," American Journal of Semiotics
vedique," Journal of the American Oriental Society 2/4.1-57.
69.11-18. Staal, Frits (1985a), "Vedic Mantras," in H. P. Alper,
Renou, Louis (1960), Etudes vediques et pdnineennes, vol. 6, Understanding Mantras, New York.
Paris. Staal, Frits (1985b), Jouer avec lefeu. Pratique et theorie du
Renou, Louis (1978), L'Indefondamentale, ed. C. Malamoud, rituel vedique, Paris.
Paris. Strickmann, Michel (1983), "Homa in East Asia," in AGNI
Renou, Louis and Silburn, L. (1954), "Nirukta and Anirukta 11,418-455.
in Vedic," Sarupa BhdratT(Lakshman Sarup Memorial Thielcke, Gerhard (1964), "Zur Phylogenese einiger Lautaus-
Volume) Hoshiarpur68-79. serungen der europaischen Baumlaufer (Certhia brachy-
Ripley, Sidney Dillon (1982), A Synopsis of the Birds of India dactyla Brehm und Certhia familiaris L.)," Zeitschrift
and Pakistan, Bombay. fur zoologische Systematik und Evolutionsforschung,
Scharfstein, Ben-Ami (forthcoming), Like the Song of a Bird: 2.383-413.
On Art and the Esthetic Universal. Thieme, Paul (1975), "Kranich und Reiher in Sanskrit,"
Schneider, U. (1971), Der Somaraub des Manu. Mythus und Studien zur Indologie und Iranistik 1.3-36.
Ritual, Wiesbaden.

This content downloaded from 91.229.229.56 on Tue, 10 Jun 2014 08:34:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Você também pode gostar