Você está na página 1de 25

Nadkarni & Gupta/Task-Based Model of Perceived Website Complexity

RESEARCH ARTICLE

A TASK-BASED MODEL OF PERCEIVED


WEBSITE COMPLEXITY1

By: Sucheta Nadkarni research have created an important debate: Does complexity
College of Business Administration enhance or inhibit user experience at a website? In this
University of Nebraska, Lincoln study, we draw on the task complexity literature to develop a
Lincoln, NE 68588-0491 broad and holistic model that examines the antecedents and
U.S.A. consequences of PWC. Our results provide two important
snadkarn@unlnotes.unl.edu insights into the relationship between PWC and user out-
comes. First, the positive relationship between objective
Reetika Gupta complexity and PWC was moderated by user familiarity.
College of Business and Economics Second, online task goals (goal-directed search and experi-
Lehigh University ential browsing) moderated the relationship between PWC
Bethlehem, PA 18015 and user satisfaction. Specifically, the relationship between
U.S.A. PWC and user satisfaction was negative for goal-directed
reg205@lehigh.edu users and inverted-U for experiential users. The implications
of this finding for the practice of website design are
discussed.
Abstract
Keywords: Perceived website complexity, user perception,
In this study, we propose that perceived website complexity website usability
(PWC) is central to understanding how sophisticated features
of a website (such as animation, audio, video, and rollover
effects) affect a visitor’s experience at the site. Although pre- Introduction
vious research suggests that several elements of perceived
complexity (e.g., amount of text, animation, graphics, range Businesses spend billions of dollars annually to add a wide
and consistency of webpages configuring a website, ease of range of sophisticated features, such as animation, audio,
navigating through it, and clarity of hyperlinks) affect impor- video, and rollover effects, to improve users’ experience with
tant user outcomes, conflicting results yielded by previous their websites. However, these features are of value only
when online users find them interesting and when the user
1 experience at the website is satisfying. For example,
Deborah Compeau was the accepting senior editor for this paper. Sue
Brown was the associate editor. Dov Te'eni, Vicki McKinney, and Alex Brynjolfsson and Smith (2000) find that by providing efficient
Ramirez served as reviewers. search features at a website, online retailers can charge a
price-premium to time-sensitive customers. In contrast, when
Earlier versions of this paper were presented at the INFORMS Conference, website features inhibit information search, users do not buy
San Antonio, TX, 2000; American Marketing Association Educators’
Conference, Orlando, FL, 2002; and the OCIS Division of the Academy of products at the website, resulting in loss of sales for online
Management National Conference, Seattle, WA, 2003. firms (Hof 2001). Moreover, Kotha, Rajgopal, and Venkata-

MIS Quarterly Vol. 31 No. 3, pp. 501-524/September 2007 501


Nadkarni & Gupta/Task-Based Model of Perceived Website Complexity

chalam (2004) argue that website design layout can provide ways. First, we distinguish between objective website com-
online retailers cost advantages by reducing the cost of plexity and PWC. Our study shows that the relationship
acquiring new customers and the need for expensive offline between objective website complexity and PWC is moderated
support as customers navigate the site and help themselves. by user familiarity. Second, our study demonstrates that
In other words, website features that provide users a satisfying online task goals—goal-directed (focused on information
experience can act as differentiators in a cluttered market gathering to achieve a predetermined end goal) and experien-
place and can provide online retailers with a sustainable com- tial (focused on information browsing for recreation and navi-
petitive advantage (Kotha et al. 2004). Thus, understanding gational experience) (Hoffman and Novak 1996; Schlosser
how the information cues presented at a website affect user 2003; Te'eni and Feldman 2001)—are important in under-
experience is critical for the success of online companies. standing the relationship between PWC and user satisfaction.
We show that the PWC–user satisfaction relationship is
We propose that perceived website complexity (PWC) is different for goal-directed and experiential users as the two
central to understanding how these information cues affect a online task goals induce users to adopt separate mechanisms
user’s experience at the website. There is evidence that in interacting with the online environment. An important
elements of PWC affect the degree to which users find a implication of our results is that web designers need to create
website appealing and satisfying (Geissler et al. 2001; websites that can accommodate different levels of complexity
Stevenson et al. 2000). This implies that by effectively in order to maximize the satisfaction of both goal-directed and
managing a website’s level of complexity, a firm can experiential users. Taken as a whole, the comprehensive set
differentiate its website from other sites and produce a of antecedent and outcome relationships of PWC examined in
compelling navigation experience for users. Drawing on this study can provide useful insights for web evaluation and
Wood’s (1986) framework of task complexity, we define design.
PWC as a function of three facets: component (density and
dissimilarity of visual features such as text, graphics, video,
and animation presented at a website), coordinative (range of
topics covered by the website and interrelationships between Theory Development
these topics), and dynamic (ambiguity and clarity of action-
outcome relationship in a hyperlink). Definition of Perceived Website Complexity
Research suggests that several elements of PWC (e.g., amount We use Wood’s (1986) comprehensive framework of task
of text, animation, graphics, range and consistency of complexity to define perceived website complexity. This
webpages configuring a website, ease of navigating through task-based framework is especially important for the online
it, and clarity of hyperlinks) affect important user outcomes environment, where users visit websites mainly to fulfill task
such as perceived web-information and web-system quality goals such as goal-directed search and experiential browsing
(McKinney et al. 2002), perceived ease of use (Agarwal and (Hoffman and Novak 1996; Novak et al. 2003). Wood
Venkatesh 2002), communication effectiveness (Geissler et contends that information cues (pieces of information about
al. 2001), and satisfaction (Stevenson et al. 2000). However, the stimulus that individuals must process in performing a
a particularly important debate remains unresolved. Results task) are central to understanding perceived complexity. For
of some studies suggest that simple websites are easy to use online users, websites are the primary medium through which
and effective (Agarwal and Venkatesh 2002; Shneiderman they interact with the information cues in the online environ-
1998), whereas others suggest that complexity increases the ment to achieve their online task goals. Because websites
richness of information presentation and thereby enhances represent a major task stimulus for online users (Agarwal and
user satisfaction (Palmer 2002). In addition, other studies Venkatesh 2002; Venkatesh and Agarwal 2006; Hoffman and
suggest an inverted-U relationship between website com- Novak 1996; McKinney et al. 2002; Palmer 2002),
plexity and communication effectiveness (Geissler et al. 2001; information cues (e.g., text, animation, hypertext structure,
Stevenson et al. 2000). Because different studies present con- and navigation tools) presented at a website are central to
flicting findings, it is unclear whether complexity enhances or online users’ perceptions of task complexity.
inhibits user experience at a website. We attempt to address
this question by theorizing that user familiarity and online Wood specifies perceived complexity (TCt) as a linear com-
task goals play important roles in determining how PWC bination of three dimensions that capture distinct elements of
affects an important user outcome, user satisfaction. the information cues that make up a task stimulus: component
(TC1), coordinative (TC2), and dynamic (TC3):
The model of PWC developed in our study clarifies the
relationship between complexity and user outcomes in two TCt = "TC1 + $TC2 + (TC3

502 MIS Quarterly Vol. 31 No. 3/September 2007


Nadkarni & Gupta/Task-Based Model of Perceived Website Complexity

Perceived component complexity refers to the users’ per- sentation formats, multimedia and search tools, hierarchical
ceptions of the density and dissimilarity of information cues menu structure, and download time for webpages) and has its
in the task stimulus. A task stimulus with dense and dis- roots in the system design literature (Bucy et al. 1999; Nielsen
similar information cues is perceived as more complex than 2000; Schubert and Selz 1998; Shneiderman 1998). In
one with sparse information cues. For a website, dense cues contrast, perceived complexity is rooted in the human–
are represented by long text, many images, and colors; computer interaction (HCI) literature and captures users’
whereas dissimilarity is reflected in the use of varied formats personal interpretation of the website and their interaction
(e.g., text, graphics, and animation) such as dissimilar with it (e.g., how uncertain and ambiguous users find the
graphics and dissimilar information items on a webpage. hyperlinks, how dense and dissimilar users find the infor-
mation cues presented at a website) (Agarwal and Venkatesh
Perceived coordinative complexity describes users’ percep- 2002; McKinney et al. 2002; Te'eni 1989, 2001). The central
tions of the range of and interdependencies among the dif- tenet of the perceived complexity literature is that users may
ferent information clusters (groups or chunks of related perceive the same level of objective complexity differently
topics) in the task stimulus. The wider the range of informa- because of their different backgrounds and experience.
tion clusters and interrelationships among the clusters, the
greater the perceptions of coordinative complexity. For a
website, high coordinative complexity is reflected in a wide PWC and Web Evaluation Outcomes
range of topics covered by the website, high number of
webpages configuring a website, and many paths linking Web evaluation studies suggest that elements of component
webpages. (e.g., amount of text, animation, and graphics on webpages),
coordinative (e.g., range and consistency of webpages con-
Perceived dynamic complexity refers to the ambiguity figuring a website), and dynamic complexity (e.g., ease of
(number of different possible interpretations of the same piece navigating through it and clarity of hyperlinks) that make up
of information) and uncertainty (clarity of action–outcome PWC determine important user outcomes such as web-
relationships) that individuals face in performing a task. information and web-system quality (McKinney et al. 2002),
Ambiguous hyperlinks and search procedures and unpre- perceived ease of use (Agarwal and Venkatesh 2002; Davis
dictable click streams can increase the dynamic complexity of 1989; Venkatesh 2000; Venkatesh et al. 2003), communica-
a website. tion effectiveness (Geissler et al. 2001), and satisfaction
(Stevenson et al. 2000). However, the varied interpretations
Previous literature posits that simultaneous sources of resulting from these studies have produced controversy as to
complexity, from all of the elements of a stimulus, are percep- whether PWC increases or hinders user satisfaction at a
tually integrated to produce a general level of perceived website. One stream of research suggests that PWC increases
complexity (Berlyne 1960; Wood 1986). Thus, we treat PWC the richness of information presentation, thereby providing a
as a unified aggregate construct and test our model for the rich and satisfying experience (Hall and Hanna 2004; Nack et
aggregate PWC rather than for individual facets of PWC al. 2001; Palmer 2002). Other research suggests that PWC
(component, coordinative, and dynamic). creates confusion and frustration in users, resulting in a
negative impact on key user outcomes such as perceived ease
of use (Agarwal and Venkatesh 2002; Shneiderman 1998;
Objective Complexity and Venkatesh 2000; Venkatesh et al. 2003). A third stream
Perceived Complexity suggests an inverted-U relationship between PWC and user
outcomes, such that low levels of PWC create boredom for
The literature on task complexity distinguishes between users, whereas high levels of PWC create confusion and con-
objective and perceived complexity of a task stimulus flict for users (Geissler et al. 2001; Stevenson et al. 2000).
(Campbell 1988; Earley 1985; Te'eni 1989, 2001; Wood Thus, this research posits that medium levels of PWC maxi-
1986). The objective complexity is defined by the number mize user satisfaction by arousing users’ curiosity and
and configuration of information cues in the stimulus itself, engaging them in the navigation process without excessively
whereas perceived complexity, which is based on the indi- burdening them.
vidual’s perception of the stimulus, focuses on the person–
stimulus interaction. The web evaluation literature also treats We address this theoretical conflict regarding website com-
objective and perceived complexity as distinct constructs plexity by developing a broad and holistic model that inte-
capturing different facets of websites. Objective complexity grates objective website complexity, PWC, user familiarity,
is defined by a universal set of design characteristics that online task goals, and user satisfaction. We discuss this
encompass the technological aspects of a website (e.g., pre- model in the following section.

MIS Quarterly Vol. 31 No. 3/September 2007 503


Nadkarni & Gupta/Task-Based Model of Perceived Website Complexity

Objective
Website
Complexity

H1
User H2
Familiarity
Component
Complexity

PWC Coordinative
Complexity

Dynamic
Online task goals: Complexity
Goal-directed vs. H3, H3a, H3b
Experiential

User
Satisfaction

Figure 1. Theoretical Model of Perceived Website Complexity

Hypotheses 1985; Lindsay and Norman 1977; Wood 1986). This cogni-
tive load translates into the perceived complexity of the
Drawing on the task complexity literature (Campbell 1984, information cues in the task environment.
1988; Campbell and Gingrich 1986; Early 1985; Wood 1986),
we develop a model (shown in Figure 1) that examines the When individuals face information cues, they spend cognitive
antecedents and consequences of PWC. We propose that resources to encode the cues and decide how to respond to
(1) PWC mediates the relationship between objective com- these cues (Lindsay and Norman 1977). Objectively complex
plexity and user satisfaction, (2) user familiarity moderates cues require significantly more cognitive resources to encode
the relationship between objective complexity and PWC, and and respond to than simple cues. Because users have limited
(3) online task goals moderate the relationship between PWC cognitive resources, the excessive information processing
and user satisfaction. Thus our model addresses how user demands of objectively complex cues may create cognitive
familiarity and online task goals determine the varying rela- overload. The notion of cognitive load (Lindsay and Norman
tionships between objective complexity, PWC, and user 1977) is consistent with Miller’s (1956) seminal work on “the
satisfaction. We discuss each of these propositions in the magical number seven, plus or minus two,” which suggests
following sections. that human working memory can hold up to seven bits of
information, plus or minus two, at one time. This limitation
of human information processing in short-term memory
Objective Website Complexity and PWC requires that displays be kept simple by minimizing anima-
tion, wild background patterns, and contrasting text colors;
The task complexity literature contends that perceived com- that multiple page displays be consolidated; and that window-
plexity mediates the relationship between objective com- motion frequency be reduced (Nielsen 1994; Shneiderman
plexity and task outcomes. Thus, it is perceived and not 1998). Thus, simple layout, clear content, and straightforward
objective complexity that directly affects user outcomes. navigation procedures reduce the cognitive strain on users.
Objective complexity of a task influences the cognitive load When users experience less cognitive strain in interacting
associated with performing the task—the information pro- with a website, they are likely to find it less complex.
cessing effort that individuals need to spend in order to see
and understand information cues in the task stimulus Hypothesis 1: Objective website complexity will be
(Campbell 1984, 1988; Campbell and Gingrich 1986; Early positively related to PWC.

504 MIS Quarterly Vol. 31 No. 3/September 2007


Nadkarni & Gupta/Task-Based Model of Perceived Website Complexity

User Familiarity PWC, Online Task Goals, and User Satisfaction

The task complexity literature suggests that the relationship The concept of user satisfaction occupies a central position in
between objective complexity and perceived complexity is a information systems as well as web evaluation research.
function of the individual’s familiarity with the task stimulus Information systems studies have shown that when users are
and/or task domain (Campbell 1988; Earley 1985; Huber satisfied with a system, they are more likely to use the system
1985; Jacoby et al. 1971; Keisler and Sproull 1982; Taylor (Delone and McLean 1992). User satisfaction is also central
1981). Familiarity increases individuals’ tolerance of com- to website evaluation research (Palmer 2002; Te'eni and
plexity by (1) allowing them to better understand inter- Feldman 2001). When users experience satisfaction at a
relationships between elements of the task stimulus and website, they are likely to return to the website (Hoffman and
(2) helping them to distinguish relevant from irrelevant Novak 1996; Te'eni and Feldman 2001), purchase products at
information in the task stimulus. the website, and recommend the website to others (McKinney
et al. 2002). Conversely, when users are dissatisfied with the
Individuals familiar with the task stimulus or domain have website, they are likely to develop a negative impression of
superior knowledge of a given task stimulus than others have, the website, which is likely to hurt the overall image of the
which allows them to develop a better understanding of the website and online sales through the website. In other words,
relationships between different elements of that task stimulus user satisfaction is critical to website success. Thus, we chose
(Cox and Cox 1988; Hong et al. 2002; Russo and Johnson user satisfaction as the outcome in our model of PWC.
1980). Individuals familiar with the task stimulus or domain
can encode new information in the task stimulus more effi- We posit that online task goals will play a significant role in
ciently than other individuals can. Results of recent studies determining the relationship between PWC and user satis-
support the conclusion that the richer store of knowledge faction with the website. Researchers have classified online
possessed by users familiar with the web contributes to a task goals into two distinct categories: goal-directed and
clearer and better understanding of the content, organization, experiential (Hoffman and Novak 1996; Novak et al. 2003;
and browsing procedures of the website or the products Schlosser 2003; Te'eni and Feldman 2001). A goal-directed
offered than is possible for individuals who are unfamiliar activity consists of using the Internet for its informative value
with the web (Agarwal and Venkatesh 2002; Cox and Cox and purchase utility, such as directly searching for specific
2002). Thus, for the same level of objective website com- information to accomplish a task or to reduce purchase uncer-
plexity, users familiar with the websites or products available tainty, whereas an experiential goal refers to browsing the
at the website may experience less PWC than unfamiliar website in a relatively unstructured manner for recreational
users. purposes. Research suggests that goal-directed and experien-
tial task goals induce users to adopt separate mechanisms as
A critical facet of processing task-related information is the they interact with the online environment (Hoffman and
ability to select relevant information while ignoring infor- Novak 1996; Schlosser 2003), implying that PWC may inter-
mation irrelevant to the task at hand (Berlyne 1970; Larkin et act with online task goals to modify users’ satisfaction with
al. 1985). Individuals familiar with a task stimulus or task the website.
domain may use their domain knowledge to limit their atten-
tion to task-related information, thus minimizing their Goal-directed users have a clearly definable goal hierarchy,
cognitive efforts toward redundant information at a website. putting more effort into reaching the end goal rather than into
Therefore, we expect that familiarity will moderate the undirected exploration. Goal-directed users consider chal-
relationship between objective complexity and PWC. lenge a deterrent to their main effort, as they do not want to
expend unnecessary effort in processing challenging infor-
Hypothesis 2: User familiarity will moderate the mation (Wolfinbarger and Gilly 2001). As the complex cues
relationship between objective website complexity in the environment shift goal-directed users’ attention away
and PWC such that, for the same level of objective from their end-goals, medium and high levels of PWC may
website complexity, the PWC ratings of users pose a challenge to these users. Therefore, we predict that the
familiar with the website or products available at a distraction experienced by goal-directed users at medium and
website will be lower than those of users not familiar high levels of PWC may decrease their satisfaction at the
with the website or products available at a website. website. On the other hand, goal-directed users will experi-

MIS Quarterly Vol. 31 No. 3/September 2007 505


Nadkarni & Gupta/Task-Based Model of Perceived Website Complexity

ence satisfaction at low levels of PWC, characterized by few used the construct measures developed in the pilot phase to
information cues and clusters, and by clear, unambiguous test our hypotheses. We describe each phase briefly in the
hyperlinks. Such a relatively uncluttered and easily navigable following paragraphs.
environment facilitates the efficient scanning of goal-relevant
information.
Pilot Phase
In contrast, experiential users are not guided by a specific end
outcome. They adopt a spontaneous stance, focusing on the Because we drew on measures of PWC from different
process of exploration (Schlosser 2003; Wolfinbarger and research areas, the pilot phase was important to ensure that
Gilly 2001). Because of this focus, they seek a higher level these measures were relevant to our online website context
of challenge and put greater effort into processing challenging and to allow for modification of the wording of measurement
information. Accordingly, we predict that experiential users items, making them meaningful to online users. We devel-
will derive satisfaction from websites with medium levels of oped the PWC scale in the pilot phase through a series of
PWC, whose multitude of cues and clusters and ambiguous steps. First, on the basis of a thorough search of the website
hyperlinks provide them with the stimulating and challenging evaluation, HCI, and information systems literatures, we
environment that they seek. This supports the stimulus identified existing measurement items that fit the definitions
complexity view (Berlyne 1960), which posits that medium of component, coordinative, and dynamic complexity. This
levels of complexity provide the ideal scenario to users. literature search yielded 22 items—15 web-specific and 7 not
Experiential users will not be satisfied with low-PWC specific to websites (Cox and Cox 1988; Daft and Lengel
websites because these do not provide the engagement and 1986; Kieras and Polson 1985).
challenge that they need in order to enjoy the browsing
process. However, at very high levels of PWC, experiential Second, we conducted a free-association task (Aaker 1997) to
users will experience confusion and conflict, and ultimately ensure that the measurement items comprehensively captured
low satisfaction levels, as they lack the resources needed to our specified constructs and they were meaningful to people
process the excessive number of cues and clusters and the (Aaker 1997; Bagozzi 1980). We asked 40 subjects (mean
completely ambiguous information. age = 23, female = 30 percent) to write down complexity
characteristics that first came to mind when thinking about the
We therefore expect the relationship between PWC and user three websites that they most often visited. We used the
satisfaction to be different for goal-directed and experiential specific characteristics yielded by the free-association task to
users. recast the 22 items in a language meaningful to website users.

Hypothesis 3: Online task goals will moderate the Third, we conducted a Q-sort analysis to qualitatively validate
relationship between PWC and user satisfaction. the dimensionality of the 22 items (Anderson and Gerbing
1991; Bagozzi 1980). We provided three professors and two
Hypothesis 3a: There will be a negative relationship web designers the definitions of component, coordinative, and
between PWC and user satisfaction for goal-directed dynamic complexity and the 22 measurement scales and
users. asked them to sort the scales by the construct (W = 0.81).

Hypothesis 3b: There will be an inverted-U relation- Fourth, we pilot tested the questionnaire on a voluntarily
ship between PWC and user satisfaction for experi- recruited pool of 50 undergraduates and 30 web experts (web-
ential users. site designers and researchers) using 6 websites representing
high (2), medium (2), and low (2) levels of objective com-
plexity (which were a subset of websites used in the testing
phase) and online task goals (goal-directed and experiential)
Research Methods similar to the ones used in the testing phase of the study. We
did not provide any time limit for task completion in the pilot.
Research Design and Data Collection However, we asked the pilot subjects to answer the total time
that they typically spend on a website for both searching and
The design of this study comprises two phases: pilot and browsing. The pilot subjects suggested that they spend
testing. In the pilot phase, we developed the PWC measures between 20 and 25 minutes at the website for both browsing
and conducted preliminary testing of the reliability and and searching, which is consistent with the findings of pre-
validity of our construct measures. In the testing phase, we vious web evaluation studies (e.g., Agarwal and Venkatesh

506 MIS Quarterly Vol. 31 No. 3/September 2007


Nadkarni & Gupta/Task-Based Model of Perceived Website Complexity

2002). After completing the pilot questionnaire, each respon- ponent complexity: t = 1.14, n.s.; coordinative complexity:
dent reviewed all questions for content, clarity, meaning- t = 1.05, n.s.; dynamic complexity: t = 0.97, n.s.; product
fulness, and the ability to measure the construct (Bagozzi familiarity: t = 1.18, n.s.; website familiarity: t = 1.03, n.s.;
1980). We also used item-total correlation and discrimination user satisfaction: t = 0.95, n.s.). Therefore, we combined the
based on the t-statistic to eliminate redundant items (Churchill two samples to test our model (n = 452).
1979). Items with low item-total correlations (two items:
0.25, n.s. and 0.19, n.s.) and nonsignificant t-statistic (same We used the same procedures for all subjects. Student
two items: 1.07, n.s. and 1.24, n.s.) between the ratings of volunteers were recruited for the study by circulation of a
respondents above 74th percentile and those below the 26th notice describing the experiment to students from the sections
percentile were eliminated. Based on these results of the pilot of a senior-level required course in business policies and of a
test, we retained 20 items for the testing phase of the study. junior-level required introductory course in marketing (parti-
cipation rate: 89 percent). Nonstudent volunteers were
In the pilot phase, we also conducted a preliminary analysis recruited by circulating the notice to visitors of a local com-
of the dimensionality and validity of other construct measures munity public library (participation rate: 78 percent). We
before using them in the testing phase. In the exploratory collected the data in a computer laboratory setting for all
factor analyses on a sample of 80 subjects, consistent with subjects by using Dell computers with Windows XP, and
previous literature, the user familiarity construct yielded two comparable hard drive space and RAM.
dimensions (factor 1: eigen value = 3.24, factor loadings =
0.82, 0.86; factor 2: eigen value = 2.84, factor loadings = Participants were randomly assigned to one of two online task
0.84, 0.88) explaining 79 percent of the variance. Consistent goal conditions (goal-directed and experiential) and one of
with our conceptualization of PWC, the 20 items of PWC three objective complexity conditions (high, medium, and
yielded three dimensions—component (eigen value = 4.51), low). Each participant was given an instruction sheet that
coordinative (eigen value = 3.67), and dynamic (eigen value explained the specific task assigned (goal-directed or
= 2.98)—that explained 84 percent of the variance. The experiential) and that instructed him/her to visit the site for 20
factor loadings on each factor ranged from 0.82 to 0.93. minutes (based on previous web evaluation studies and pilot
Finally, consistent with McKinney et al. (2002), the user results), to complete the task assigned, and to complete the
satisfaction scale yielded a single factor (eigen value = 3.95) questionnaire (which had been placed in a sealed envelope
explaining 78 percent of the variance in the pilot study. The under the instruction sheet) after completing the task.
results provide preliminary evidence of validity for our
construct measures.
Objective Website Complexity Conditions

Testing Phase We used a series of steps to select websites representing high,


medium and low levels of objective complexity. First, we
In the testing phase, we collected data from 332 under- randomly selected from Yahoo! Directories 150 websites
graduate students at a major eastern (170) and a major representing 10 product categories: travel, auction, entertain-
midwestern (162) university to test our hypotheses. Most of ment, news or media, sports, computer hardware components,
the subjects (67 percent) were 18 to 24 years old and 43 office software, auto, astronomy, and fertilizers. Using mul-
percent of the subjects were female. All subjects had over 3 tiple stimuli (e.g., websites) to represent the treatment cells
years of experience in using the Internet, and their weekly decreases the likelihood of skewed results and potential
Internet use ranged from 4 to 10 hours. To ensure that results confounds and increases the robustness and generalizability
were not idiosyncratic to student subjects, we replicated the of the results (Aaker 1997; Cox and Cox 2002).
study with subjects drawn from the population at large,
collecting data from 120 nonstudent subjects (male: 75, Based on a thorough review of the web design literature, we
female: 45) belonging to the age groups 9 to 14 years (40) identified 13 objective complexity metrics that capture the
and 30 to 60 years (80). Subjects, who regularly visited the underlying technological characteristics of website design
library in the local community, comprised a cross-section of (hypertext structure and presentation) and that are relevant to
people from different educational backgrounds and had an component (e.g., percentage of white space, graphics count
average age of 42. We used the unpaired t-test (n = 452) to and size, word count, color count), coordinative (e.g., number
determine whether there were significant differences in our of webpages, average depth of pages, average internal and
study variables between the two samples. There were no dif- external, and same page links on the webpage, and coefficient
ferences in the results obtained with the two samples (com- of variation in the number of presentation forms such as text,

MIS Quarterly Vol. 31 No. 3/September 2007 507


Nadkarni & Gupta/Task-Based Model of Perceived Website Complexity

graphics, and video), and dynamic complexity (e.g., average of websites in each complexity level was equal for the three
pop-up advertisements per webpage, average webpage down- categories. This process yielded a total of 48 websites—16 in
load time, number of support tools such as site map, search each complexity level. The 48 websites represented eight
option and help links) (Bucy et al. 1999; Nielsen 2000; product categories: travel, auction, news or media, sports,
Palmer 2002; Schubert and Selz 1998; Shneiderman 1998). computer hardware components, office software, auto, and
We acknowledge that objective complexity measures do not astronomy. We developed a categorical variable of objective
have a perfect mapping with measures of component, coordi- website complexity (high, medium, and low) rather than con-
native, and dynamic complexity because, as discussed earlier, tinuous individual measures to ensure an adequate number of
objective complexity and PWC have roots in different streams subjects per treatment cell (Churchill and Surprenant 1982).
of literature and encompass different assumptions about com- We used ANOVA and mean-difference tests to confirm that
plexity. However, we chose objective complexity measures there was a significant difference in aggregate objective
that were relevant to the three facets of PWC. For example, complexity metrics measured across the three levels (F =
the objective complexity measures of “pop-up advertise- 19.21, p < 0.001).
ments,” “page download time,” and “support tools” are rele-
vant to dynamic complexity; the higher the number of pop-up
advertisements and page download time, the higher the Online Task Goal Conditions
uncertainty (dynamic complexity) associated with the links on
the webpages. On the other hand, the higher the support tools We designed the two online task goal conditions—goal-
available at a website, the lower the uncertainty associated directed and experiential—based on existing, valid manipu-
(dynamic complexity) with the links on the webpage. lations (Novak et al. 2003; Schlosser 2003). Those assigned
to a goal-directed task condition were instructed to go to their
We used a web metrics software package to calculate the 13 site with “the goal of efficiently finding something specific
complexity measures. We then computed an aggregate mea- within that site” (Schlosser 2003, p. 188). Those assigned to
sure of objective complexity2 (coefficient " = 0.79) based on the experiential task goal condition were instructed to “have
the mean z scores of the 13 variables. Such formative treat- fun, looking at whatever you consider interesting or enter-
ment of objective complexity is consistent with the web taining.” What to look for was not specified, so that goal-
design literature cited above. We then classified the 150 directed and experiential users would have similarly hetero-
websites into high (top 33.33 percentile: 50 websites), geneous information needs. Thus, both goal-directed and
medium (middle 33.33 percentile) and low (bottom 33.33 experiential users could adapt their experience to match their
percentile) objective complexity levels. own information or entertainment needs (Schlosser 2003).

Thirty experienced web designers rated the degree to which To ensure that users viewed the website in the light of the
each of the ten objective metrics makes the website complex assigned online task goal, we designed a scale of manipu-
(seven-point scale: not complex at all, 1; neutral, 4; highly lation check for the two task conditions based on codes
complex, 7) based on “universal web design principles” to constructed by Novak et al. (2003). We show the items in the
define robust factor levels. Each website was rated by three manipulation check scale in Table 1. Goal-directed users
designers. There was a high correlation between the objective reported a significantly lower score (M = 2.71) than experi-
metrics and designers’ ratings (ranging from 0.59, p < 0.001 ential users (M = 3.80; F = 29.17; p < .001) on the mani-
to 0.91, p < 0.0001) for the 150 websites. pulation scale, suggesting that they were more focused and
had an identifiable purpose. The scale was an agree–disagree
We selected the final websites in two stages. First, the web- scale where 1 suggested “high goal-directedness” and 7 “low
sites’ level of complexity were categorized as high, medium, goal-directedness.”
and low based on objective metrics as well as on ratings of the
designers. Second, websites were chosen so that the number
Measures
2
We also conducted partial least squares analyses in which we measured Perceived Website Complexity
objective website complexity as follows, which is in line with our definition
of PWC: TCt = "TC1 + $TC2 + (TC3. We then classified the 48 websites into
high (top 33.33 percentile), medium (middle 33.33 percentile), and low We used the 20-item scale developed in the pilot phase to
(bottom 33.33 percentile). Because this classification of websites based on measure PWC. We show the 20 items and the supporting
PLS analyses matched completely the classification based on the objective
literature in Table 1 and illustrate them graphically in
complexity measure based on the mean z-scores of the 13 metrics, we do not
include this analysis in the paper. Appendix A .

508 MIS Quarterly Vol. 31 No. 3/September 2007


Nadkarni & Gupta/Task-Based Model of Perceived Website Complexity

Table 1. Theoretically Derived Measurement Scales of Component, Coordinative, and Dynamic


Complexity
Construct Theoretical
Definition Dimensions Website Complexity Measures of Each Theoretical Dimension Source of Items
Component Similarity/ 1. The clarity between text and images was High (1) – Low (7) Geissler et al. 2001
Complexity: dissimilarity 2. The images (or graphics) on the webpages were Similar (1) – Stevenson et al. 2000
The degree to (Berlyne 1960) Dissimilar (7)
which users 3. The information items on the webpages were Similar (1) –
find the form Dissimilar (7)
and content
cues at the Visual density 4. The text on the webpages was Short (1) – Long (7)
individual (Berlyne 1960; 5. The webpage backgrounds were Not visually dense at all (1) –
webpages Campbell 1988) Visually Dense (7)
visually dense 6. The graphics on the webpages were Not visually dense at all
and dissimilar (1) – Visually dense (7)
7. The layout of the webpages was Not visually dense at all (1) –
Visually dense (7)
Coordinative Range 1. The range of the alternative links to find information was Broad Agarwal and Venkatesh 2002
Complexity: (Campbell 1988; (1) – Narrow (7) Daft and Lengel 1986
Users’ percep- Wood 1986) 2. The choice of both image and text clicks was Broad (1) – Ha and James 1998
tions of the Narrow (7) Kieras and Polson 1985
range of and 3. The variety of information clusters (groups of related McKinney et al. 2002
the degree of information) was Low (1) – High (7) Nielsen 2000
connectedness Palmer 2002
among the Connectedness/ 4. The links at the website were Logical (1) – Illogical (7) Schubert and Selz 1998
information Interrelationships 5. The layout across the webpages was Uniform (1) – Not Shneiderman 1998
clusters at a (Steinmann Uniform at all (7) Steuer 1992
website 1976) 6. The backgrounds across the webpages were Uniform (1) – Not
Uniform at all (7)
7. The information clusters (groups of related information) were
Interrelated (1) – Not at all interrelated (7)
Dynamic Ambiguity 1. Procedures to browse the websites were Unclear (1) – Clear Agarwal and Venkatesh 2002
Complexity: (Campbell 1988) (7) McKinney et al. 2002
Users’ percep- 2. Hyperlinks on the website were Unambiguous (1) – Oinas-Kukkonen 1998
tions of ambi- Ambiguous (7) Palmer 2002
guity of hyper- 3. Information presented on the websites was Unambiguous (1) – Steuer 1992
links and Ambiguous (7) Te’eni 2001
uncertainty of
the relationship Action-outcome 4. Information on the succeeding links from the initial page was
between the Uncertainty Predictable (1) – Unpredictable (7)
hyperlink and (March and 5. Individual links took me to desired webpages: Always (1) –
the ensuing Simon 1958) Never (7)
webpages 6. Information presented on the website was Uncertain (1) –
Certain (7)
Objective 1. Percentage of white space: Percentage of a page not taken Bucy et al. 1999
Website up by text and graphics Geissler et al. 2001
Complexity 2. Graphics count: The mean number of graphics on the Nielsen 1994, 2000
webpages Palmer 2002
3. Graphics size: The mean size of the graphics on the Perrow 1986
webpages Schubert and Selz 1998
4. Word count: The mean number of words on the webpages Shneiderman 1998
5. Color count: The mean number of colors on the webpages Took 1990
6. Average number of different presentation forms used on a
webpage (text, graphics, video, audio, animation)

MIS Quarterly Vol. 31 No. 3/September 2007 509


Nadkarni & Gupta/Task-Based Model of Perceived Website Complexity

Table 1. Theoretically Derived Measurement Scales of Component, Coordinative, and Dynamic


Complexity (Continued)
Construct Theoretical
Definition Dimensions Website Complexity Measures of Each Theoretical Dimension Source of Items
7. Average internal, external and same page links on the
webpage
8. Number of webpages configuring a website
9. Average depth of pages: The average number of pages from
a home page to a page with no more forward links or only
links external to the website
10. Coefficient of variation in the number of different presentation
forms (e.g., text, graphics, video, audio, animation) used
across webpages
11. Average pop-up advertisements per webpage
12. Average webpage download time
13. Number of support tools (e.g., site map, search option, help
links)
Familiarity Product 1. My knowledge of the product/s served by the website is: Very Cox and Cox 1988, 2002
familiarity Low (1) – High (7) Hong et al. 2002
2. I have used the products served by the website: Very often (1)
– Never (7)

Website 3. My knowledge of the website is: Very High (1) – Low (7)
familiarity 4. I have visited the website: Never (1) – Very often (7)
Online Task Goal-directed When I was at the XX website….. Novak et al. 2003
Goal and Experiential 1. I had a distinct identifiable purpose: Strongly agree (1) –
Manipulation Strongly disagree (7)
Check 2. I was looking up specific information: Strongly agree (1) –
Strongly disagree (7)
3. I was very focused: Strongly agree (1) – Strongly disagree (7)
4. I was absorbed in finding specific information: Strongly agree (1)
– Strongly disagree (7)
5. I was clicking often and went to many different webpages:
Strongly agree (1) – Strongly disagree (7)
6. I was absorbed in seeing where I could go next: Strongly agree
(1) – Strongly disagree (7)
7. I was randomly surfing through the website: Strongly agree (1)
– Strongly disagree (7)
User 1. After using this website, I am: Very dissatisfied (1) – Very McKinney et al. 2002
Satisfaction satisfied (7)
2. After using this website, I am: Very displeased (1) – Very
pleased (7)
3. Using this website made me: Frustrated (1) – Contented (7)
4. After using this website, I feel: Terrible (1) – Delighted (7)
5. After using this website, I will: Never recommend it to my friends
(1) – Strongly recommend it to my friends (7)
6. After using this website, I will: Never use it again (1) – Most
likely use it again (7)

510 MIS Quarterly Vol. 31 No. 3/September 2007


Nadkarni & Gupta/Task-Based Model of Perceived Website Complexity

Appendix A1 illustrates websites with high and low perceived User Familiarity
component complexity based on visual density and dis-
similarity. The website with low component complexity has The literature has identified two measures of familiarity:
sparse information cues (use of fewer graphics and colors) knowledge/expertise and exposure (Cox and Cox 2002; Hong
presented in a single format (text only and content of the et al. 2002; Jacoby et al. 1971; Johnson and Russo 1984;
items is related), whereas the one with high component com- Russo and Johnson 1980). We measured product familiarity
plexity has visually dense information cues (long text, many by the user’s knowledge of the product and frequency of
graphics, and colors) presented in different formats (use of product use and we measured website familiarity by the user’s
text, graphics, and video; dissimilar graphics; dissimilarity of knowledge of the website and frequency of visits to it. Based
content items on the same webpage—movie trailer, cooking, on previous research and results of the pilot study, we defined
news, weather, directories). user familiarity as a higher order factor comprising two
dimensions: product familiarity and website familiarity (Law
Appendix A2 illustrates websites with high and low perceived and Wong 1998).
coordinative complexity based on the range of information
clusters and the interrelationships between them. The coordi-
natively simple website includes few information clusters User Satisfaction
(e.g., top stories, world, domestic country, business, science
technology, and sports) with few interrelationships between We adapted the six-item scale of user satisfaction developed
information clusters (e.g., few click choices in that only head- by McKinney et al. (2002) (shown in Table 1). This scale is
lines can be clicked, uniformity in layout across webpages). especially useful for our study for three reasons. First, it was
On the other hand, the coordinatively complex website covers developed specifically for websites, which is the focus of our
a broad range of information clusters (e.g., more news topics, study. Second, it measures users’ overall satisfaction with the
more external links to domestic and international news website rather than their satisfaction with specific attributes
sources) and many interrelationships between information of the website. Third, the psychometric properties of this
clusters (e.g., more click choices—both headlines and photos scale indicated promising validity and reliability in a previous
can be clicked, layout across webpages is not uniform). study (McKinney et al. 2002) as well as in our pilot study.

Appendix A3 demonstrates the ambiguity and uncertainty


with elements of perceived dynamic complexity. In Appendix Control Variables
A3, icons that resemble the shopping carts used in real stores
allow users to clearly interpret the hyperlink, based on their We controlled for several variables that may serve as alterna-
experiences in a real store, resulting in low levels of perceived tive explanations of variance in PWC and user satisfaction.
ambiguity with the hyperlink. In contrast, icons that resemble First, a potential alternative explanation of PWC and user
“normal bags” or a “line of interlocked shopping carts” may satisfaction is initial likeability. Grush (1976) suggests that
produce confusion and multiple interpretations of what a likeability of initially liked stimulus tends to improve with
hyperlink represents, creating high levels of perceived ambi- exposure, whereas that of initially disliked stimulus declines.
guity associated with the link. Similarly, in Appendix A3, by We also controlled for the users’ liking of the products served
clicking on the “Chicago Bears@Green Bay packers at by the website. For example, users who are movie enthusiasts
Lambeau field” link, users expect to reach a webpage where are likely to find a movie site usable regardless of website
they can place a bid for these specific tickets, as is shown in complexity (perceived or objective) and user familiarity.
the certain link. However, in the case of the uncertain link, Thus, in our empirical analyses, we controlled for the initial
when a user reaches an external webpage (Joe’s Green Bay liking of the website and the products it serves. Because the
Packers’ corner) that does not allow him/her to place the bid, two measures were highly correlated (r = 0.61, p < 0.001), we
the individual action-outcome expectancy is not met. This created a composite measure of initial liking.
increases users’ perception of uncertainty associated with this
hyperlink. We also controlled for four individual demographics (gender,
age, education, and Internet experience) that have been shown
The final design of the questionnaire, including clarity and to affect users’ perceptions of and attitudes toward computer-
specification of instructions, choice of rating scales, reverse related technologies in general and websites specifically.
coding and sequencing of items, was based on suggestions to Several HCI and web evaluation studies have shown that
reduce method bias (Torangeau 1999; Williams and Anderson males and females interact differently with, and have different
1994). attitudes toward, computer-related technologies in general

MIS Quarterly Vol. 31 No. 3/September 2007 511


Nadkarni & Gupta/Task-Based Model of Perceived Website Complexity

(Harrison and Rainer 1992; Wilder et al. 1985) and websites 1998). Then, in line with the work of Chin et al. (2003), we
in particular (Venkatesh and Agarwal 2006), and these dif- represented LIVs by creating all possible products from the
ferences may lead to differences in perceptions of website two set of indicators. Finally, we used the LIV, objective
complexity and user satisfaction. Studies have also shown website complexity × familiarity, to estimate the interaction
that younger users hold more positive attitudes toward com- effect of familiarity and we used the LIVs, PWC × online task
puters than do older users (Harrison and Rainer 1992; goals, and PWC² × online task goals, to estimate the inter-
Venkatesh and Agarwal 2006), which may lead to differences action effect of online task goals in the structural model (Chin
in perceptions of website complexity and user satisfaction et al. 2003; Kenny and Judd 1984).
between older and younger users. Several HCI studies have
found that education is positively related to favorable com-
puter attitude and negatively related to computer anxiety
(Harrison and Rainer 1992; Igbaria and Parasuraman 1989). Results
Thus, users with higher education are likely to be more
tolerant of complexity and find the websites more satisfying
than users with lower education levels. Finally, because Assessment of Measures
Internet experience positively affects attitudes toward web-
sites and anxiety in navigation (Hong et al. 2002), experi- The descriptive statistics of the constructs are shown in
enced Internet users are likely to be more tolerant of com- Table 2. As mentioned earlier, results of unpaired t-tests sug-
plexity and find complex websites more satisfying than less gested no significant differences between the student and the
experienced users. nonstudent samples.

We assessed the reliability of individual items by inspecting


Data Analyses the loadings of the items on their corresponding construct
(Chin 1998) and their internal consistency values (Fornell and
We used partial least squares to analyze the data (Chin 1998; Larcker 1981). As shown in Table 3, all measures satisfied
Venkatesh and Morris 2000). The categorical measures of requirements for reliability (reliability greater than 0.70). The
objective website complexity, online task goals, and the inter- internal consistency values for all constructs (Table 3) exceed
action variables in our model violate the multivariate the 0.70 guideline that Nunnally (1978) recommends.
normality required by maximum likelihood estimation. PLS
is particularly useful for our study because it is robust to non- We assessed the discriminant validity of the first-order con-
normal data distribution (Chin 1998). structs by assessing their cross-loadings on other constructs.
The range of cross-loadings shown in Table 3 is considerably
We used the measurement variables to generate first order lower than the corresponding factor loadings. These results
factor scores (objective complexity, product familiarity, web-
support the discriminant validity of all first order construct
site familiarity, component complexity, coordinative com-
measures.
plexity, dynamic complexity, and user satisfaction). We esti-
mated second order factor scores for PWC and user
familiarity using the repeated indicators method based on the For the second-order factor of PWC, the structural coeffi-
hierarchical component model suggested by Wold (1981). cients of component (0.85), coordinative (0.83), and dynamic
This method is especially suited for our study because it can complexity (0.75) were considerably higher than the recom-
estimate formative indicators such as those of PWC and user mended value of 0.70 (Chin 1998). The two subdimensions
familiarity and works best when the number of indicators is of product (0.87) and website (0.81) familiarity also loaded
approximately equal for each construct (which is the case for highly on the second-order factor, user familiarity. The inter-
the subconstructs of both PWC and user familiarity) (Chin et factor correlations (component–coordinative: 0.35; compo-
al. 2003). nent–dynamic: 0.37; coordinative–dynamic: 0.34) were con-
siderably lower than the structural coefficients of PWC.
Before estimating the structural models, we created three Similarly, the inter-factor correlations between website and
latent interaction variables (LIVs): objective complexity × product familiarity (0.45) were lower than the structural
familiarity, PWC × online task goals, and PWC² × online task coefficients of user familiarity. Collectively, these results
goals. First, to reduce inflation in path coefficients, we stan- provide evidence of reliability and validity for the higher
dardized and centered the indicators of each construct (Chin order factors.

512 MIS Quarterly Vol. 31 No. 3/September 2007


Nadkarni & Gupta/Task-Based Model of Perceived Website Complexity

Table 2. Descriptive Statistics and Cross-Sample Differences in Study Constructs


Descriptive Statistics
Undergraduate Sample Library Sample Unpaired t-test
(n = 332) (n = 120) (n = 452)
Constructs Mean SD Mean SD t-value
1. Component website complexity 4.05 1.89 4.12 1.95 1.14
2. Coordinative website complexity 4.21 1.75 4.15 2.01 1.51
3. Dynamic website complexity 4.29 1.94 4.32 1.76 0.79
4. Product familiarity 4.22 1.55 4.17 1.63 1.01
5. Website familiarity 4.11 1.73 4.24 1.88 0.86
6. User satisfaction 4.54 1.92 4.43 2.17 0.97

Table 3. Loadings, Cross-Loadings, and Reliability of First-Order Factors


Range of Factor Reliability
Items Factor Loadings Cross-Loadings (n = 452)
1. Component website complexity (COMP) 0.87
COMP1 0.88 0.21 – 0.32
COMP2 0.89 0.25 – 0.39
COMP3 0.85 0.19 – 0.29
0.24 – 0.35
COMP4 0.90 0.21 – 0.37
COMP5 0.87 0.17 – 0.41
COMP6 0.83 0.25 – 0.44
COMP7 0.91 0.28 – 0.36
2. Coordinative website complexity (COOD) 0.92
COOD1 0.85 0.21 – 0.39
COOD2 0.89 0.16 – 0.42
COOD3 0.82 0.25 – 0.40
0.28 – 0.37
COOD4 0.89 0.22 – 0.41
COOD5 0.81 0.18 – 0.33
COOD6 0.89 0.15 – 0.42
COOD7 0.84 0.17 – 0.32
3. Dynamic complexity (DYN) 0.85
DYN1 0.82 0.29 – 0.35
DYN2 0.85 0.22 – 0.38
0.27 – 0.43
DYN3 0.81
0.18 – 0.29
DYN4 0.79 0.25 – 0.36
DYN5 0.83 0.20 – 0.32
DYN6 0.88 0.33 – 0.45
4. Product Familiarity (PFAM) 0.89
PFAM1 0.91 0.15 – 0.40
PFAM2 0.87 0.22 – 0.38
5. Website Familiarity (WFMA) 0.84
WFAM3 0.85 0.29 – 0.42
WFAM4 0.82 0.23 – 0.32
6. User Satisfaction (SAT) 0.92
SAT1 0.88 0.18 – 0.39
SAT2 0.85 0.24 – 0.43
0.21 – 0.39
SAT3 0.84
0.26 – 0.33
SAT4 0.87 0.19 – 0.31
SAT5 0.89 0.24 – 0.36
SAT6 0.91 0.28 – 0.42
Because they are products of other items, indicators for interaction terms are typically not included in a confirmatory factor analysis of the measure-
ment model. Their inclusion would violate assumptions about the item’s independence (Yang Jonson 1998).

MIS Quarterly Vol. 31 No. 3/September 2007 513


Nadkarni & Gupta/Task-Based Model of Perceived Website Complexity

Assessment of Structural Model3 Second, we tested the full model of objective website com-
plexity, PWC, online task goals, and user satisfaction,5 which
The intercorrelations among our study constructs are shown is shown in Part C of Table 5. The results show that once the
in Table 4, whereas the results of our PLS analyses are shown PWC variables are entered in the model, the effect of objec-
in Table 5. We discuss these results in the following sections. tive complexity on user satisfaction becomes insignificant,
confirming that objective website complexity influences user
satisfaction through PWC. The negative PWC² term (B =
Antecedents of PWC –0.17, p < 0.05) suggests an overall inverted-U relationship
between PWC and user satisfaction. The significant negative
Table 5 presents the results for the antecedents of PWC.4 interaction term—PWC² × online task goals (B = –0.32, p <
None of the control variables were significantly related to 0.001)—confirms the moderating effect of online task goals
PWC (R² = 0.09). Objective website complexity (B = 0.31, in the relationship between PWC and online task goals, sup-
p < 0.001) had a positive relationship with PWC, supporting porting H3. This moderating effect is depicted graphically in
hypothesis 1. Familiarity (B = –0.22, p < 0.05) had a negative Figure 2; a negative linear relationship is seen between PWC
relationship with PWC. The significant negative interaction and user satisfaction for experiential users, and an inverted-U
terms of objective website complexity × familiarity (B = relationship between PWC and user satisfaction for goal-
–0.27, p < 0.001) suggests that the lower the familiarity, the directed users. These results support H3a and H3b.
higher the PWC for a given level of objective website com-
plexity. These results support H2. Figure 2 depicts these
relationships graphically for familiar and unfamiliar users
(split on the median rating: 4).The graphs indicate that there Discussion
are no differences in slopes of familiar and unfamiliar users
between low and high complexity levels, indicating no inter- This research was motivated by an interest in examining how
action effects of familiarity. However, there are differences and why PWC affects the user outcomes in an online environ-
in the slopes of familiar and unfamiliar users in moving from ment. To this end, we developed and tested a holistic model
low to medium levels of PWC and from medium to high comprising objective website complexity, user familiarity,
levels of PWC. This suggests that the interaction effects of PWC, online task goals, and user satisfaction. Specifically,
familiarity are driven by medium levels of PWC. by focusing on user familiarity and online task goals, we
attempted to resolve the theoretical debate regarding the
nature of the relationship between complexity and user out-
Outcomes of PWC comes. Not only does our model synthesize and integrate
research on complexity, it extends this body of work by
We tested the mediating effect of PWC between objective clarifying how PWC affects user satisfaction. Our results pro-
website complexity and user satisfaction in two separate vide two important insights into the relationship between
models. In the first model, we tested the direct effect of PWC and user outcomes. First, the positive relationship
objective website complexity on user satisfaction. The results between objective complexity and PWC was moderated by
of this analysis are shown in Part B of Table 5. There is an user familiarity. Second, online task goals moderated the
inverted-U relationship between objective website complexity relationship between PWC and user satisfaction. Specifically,
and user satisfaction as is indicated by the significant negative the relationship between PWC and user satisfaction was nega-
objective website complexity square term (B = –0.16, p < tive for goal-directed users and inverted-U for experiential
0.05). Moreover, the significance of the objective website users. We discuss the theoretical and practical implications
complexity square × online task goals suggests that online of these results in the following sections.
task goals moderate this inverted-U relationship (B = –0.22,
p < 0.05).
Limitations
3
We conducted PLS analyses separately for each dimension of PWC: com- Limitations that circumscribe the interpretation of our
ponent, coordinative, and dynamic complexity. The results of each indi-
vidual dimension of PWC were consistent with the overall model of PWC.
findings must be acknowledged. First, our conceptualization

4
We compared the full model including interactions (objective website
5
complexity × familiarity) with the model without the interaction term. The Similarly, we compared the full model including the online task goals ×
variance explained increased significantly by adding the interaction term PWC interaction with the model without this interaction. The variance
()R² = 0.14, p < 0.001). explained increased significantly ()R² = 0.12, p < 0.001).

514 MIS Quarterly Vol. 31 No. 3/September 2007


Nadkarni & Gupta/Task-Based Model of Perceived Website Complexity

Table 4. Intercorrelations among Study Constructs


Intercorrelations (n = 452)
Constructs 1 2 3 4 5
1. Objective Complexity —
2. PWC 0.30*** —
3. User Satisfaction 0.24* 0.25* —
4. User Familiarity –0.07 –0.21* –0.22 —
5. Online Task Goals 0.09 –0.32*** –0.19 0.08 —
*p < 0.05 **p < 0.01 ***p < 0.001

a
Table 5. PLS Results
PWC
Parameter Estimate (Standard Error)
A. Objective Website Complexity, Familiarity, and PWC (n = 452)
b
Control Variables
R² 0.09
Model Variables:
R² 0.25
Objective Website Complexity 0.31*** (0.09)
Familiarity –0.22* (0.07)
Objective Website Complexity × Familiarity –0.27*** (0.04)
c
B. Objective Website Complexity, Online Task Goals, and User Satisfaction (n = 452)
Model Variables:
R² 0.17
Objective Website Complexity 0.23* (0.11)
Objective Website Complexity² –0.16* (0.05)
Online Task Goals 0.12 (0.02)
Objective Website Complexity × Online Task Goals 0.19* (0.02)
Objective Website Complexity² × Online Task Goals –0.22* (0.06)
C. Objective Website Complexity, PWC, Online Task Goals, and User Satisfaction (n = 462)
Model Variables:
R² 0.23
Objective Website Complexity
Objective Website Complexity 0.12 (0.07)
Objective Website Complexity² –0.09 (0.05)
Online Task Goals 0.11 (0.08)
Objective Website Complexity × Online Task Goals 0.08 (0.04)
Objective Website Complexity² × Online Task Goals –0.17 (0.07)
PWC
PWC 0.28*** (0.07)
PWC² –0.17* (0.04)
Online Task Goals 0.13 (0.02)
PWC × Online Task Goals 0.27** (0.02)
PWC² × Online Task Goals –0.32*** (0.05)
a
We also ran a PLS model for each dimension of PWC (component, coordinative, and dynamic complexity) to confirm that the results of the overall
model were consistent with each dimension of complexity. Because we found that the results for each individual dimension of PWC were consistent
with the overall results for the PWC construct, we do not report the results in the paper.
b
Control variables include age, education, gender, computer use in hours per week, computer use in years, and initial satisfaction with the website.
c
Online task goals were represented by a dummy variable: Goal-directed = 0 and Experiential = 1.
*p < .05 **p < 0.01 ***p < 0.001

MIS Quarterly Vol. 31 No. 3/September 2007 515


Nadkarni & Gupta/Task-Based Model of Perceived Website Complexity

Objective Website Complexity, Familiarity, and PWC PWC, Online Task Conditions, and User Satisfaction

Unfamiliar 7 Goal-directed
7 Familiar 6 Experiential
6

User Satisfaction
5
PWC

4 4

3 3

2 2

1
1

Low Medium High Low Medium High

PWC
Objective Website Complexity

Figure 2. Moderating Effects of Familiarity and Online Task Goals in the Antecedent and Consequence
Relationships of PWC

of PWC was based on Wood’s (1986) framework of per- literature on perceived complexity (Berlyne 1960; Wood
ceived task complexity, which we chose because it compre- 1986). However, it would be interesting in future research to
hensively captured the user–task interactions that are impor- investigate the effects of each dimension of PWC (com-
tant to defining complexity in an online environment. Thus, ponent, coordinative, and dynamic) on user satisfaction and
our findings are unique to this specific view of complexity. the underlying processes driving these effects.
We acknowledge that there are other frameworks that define
complexity differently. For example, complexity has been Finally, goal-directed and experiential categories represent
defined by the cognitive load—the number of resources it just one way of classifying online task goals. Other classifi-
uses (Kramer et al. 1983; Moray 1977; Sheridan 1980; cations of online task goals, such as transacting (purchasing)
Welford 1978). Although, we developed the relationships and communicating (chatting) (Hoffman and Novak 1996),
between objective website complexity and PWC on the basis may yield different results. A related task classification per-
of these cognitive load arguments, we did not define PWC as tains to task uncertainty (e.g., routine or nonroutine) (Gal-
cognitive load. Examining our model of PWC by using this braith 1973; Perrow 1967, 1986). The antecedent and conse-
definition of complexity may yield some important additional quence relationships of PWC may play out differently for
insights. routine tasks (repetitive, predictable, well understood) and
nonroutine tasks (unique or ever changing situations, difficult
Second, we created three levels (high, medium, and low) for to understand).
our objective website complexity condition so that we had an
adequate number of subjects in each treatment cell (Churchill
and Surprenant 1982). However, because of this categoriza- Theoretical Implications and Future Research
tion, we could not assess whether the 16 websites in each
category affected this result. In other words, the 16 websites Our finding that user familiarity moderated the positive rela-
in each treatment cell could represent diverse characteristics tionship between objective complexity and PWC is consistent
that could affect the results differently. Although, we used a with the task complexity literature (Campbell 1988; Earley
number of websites to achieve representativeness, we do 1985; Huber 1985; Jacoby et al. 1971; Taylor 1981). Users
understand that collapsing the websites to create three levels with relatively low familiarity experienced higher PWC than
could have created a within-cell bias (Kirk 1982). users with higher familiarity for a given level of objective
complexity (shown in Figure 2). This difference points out
Third, we examined complexity at an aggregate level to the importance of user familiarity in managing complexity in
determine task outcomes, which is consistent with previous an online environment. Although the role of user familiarity

516 MIS Quarterly Vol. 31 No. 3/September 2007


Nadkarni & Gupta/Task-Based Model of Perceived Website Complexity

has been emphasized in literature on information systems use complexity of the website by balancing the cognitive pro-
(Harrison and Rainer 1992), website evaluation research has cessing elements with the fun and entertainment value.
largely ignored the role of familiarity (an exception is Hong Empirical examination of this contention is an important area
et al. 2002). Our results suggest that future studies should for future research.
incorporate user familiarity in testing relationships among
web evaluation constructs in an online environment. Our findings on the nature of the PWC–user satisfaction rela-
tionship also contribute to the literature on online task goals
The second and more important set of results explains how (Moe 2003; Schlosser 2003). Online task goal studies have
PWC affects user satisfaction and reconciles the conflicting examined how these goals affect the search procedures and
findings regarding this relationship in previous literature. The shopping behaviors that users adopt. However, these studies
PWC–user satisfaction relationship was negative for goal- have ignored the role of the perceived attributes of the online
directed users. Because goal-directed users focus on the end- task environment, such as complexity, in the relationship
goal and minimize their cognitive effort on the navigation between user task goals and user outcomes. Our research
process, medium and high levels of perceived complexity shows that the fit between PWC and online task goals is an
distract and frustrate them. In contrast, the inverted-U rela- important predictor of user satisfaction.
tionship between PWC and user satisfaction for experiential
users suggests that experiential users are as frustrated with Further, the importance of PWC–online task goal fit sug-
low levels of PWC as they are with high levels of PWC; their gested by our study raises an interesting question: How do
satisfaction is maximized at medium levels of PWC. Thus, goal-directed and experiential users cope with PWC? Based
our results address the theoretical conflict on whether com- on Gollwitzer’s (1999) theory of implementation intentions,
plexity inhibits (Agarwal and Venkatesh 2002; Shneiderman there could be two strategies of coping with complex environ-
1998; Venkatesh 2000; Venkatesh et al. 2003) or improves ments: complexity-resolving and task-facilitating. As the
(Hall and Hanna 2004; Nack et al. 2001; Palmer 2002) posi- mechanisms driving PWC–user outcome relationships are
tive user outcomes. Our results suggest that the nature of the different for goal-directed (cognition-based processes) and
relationship between PWC and user satisfaction is too com- experiential (affect-based processes) users, these two cate-
plex to be explained completely by a single stream of gories of users may adopt different strategies to cope with
research. The different explanations of the PWC–user out- complexity in an online environment. For example, when
come relationship are not contradictory; rather, they are com- goal-directed users face complexity, they may employ
plementary in explaining this relationship for goal-directed complexity-resolving strategies by relying on navigational
and experiential users; complexity inhibits user satisfaction aids (e.g., tool bars, search function) available at the site to
for goal-directed users, whereas medium levels of complexity relieve some of the cognitive load that they experience. On
enhance user satisfaction for experiential users. the other hand, experiential users who face complexity may
be motivated to resolve this complexity on their own and may
Although we did not empirically test the specific processes adopt task-facilitating strategies where they continue their
underlying the differences in the PWC–user satisfaction exploration in an undirected manner. Examining these
relationships for goal-directed and experiential users, our differences in coping strategies adopted by goal-directed and
results do raise an interesting question: Are the processes experiential users is an important area of future research.
underlying the PWC–user satisfaction relationships different
for goal-directed and experiential users? For example, the
negative relationship between PWC and user satisfaction sug- Practical Implications
gests that cognitive processing mechanisms may be driving
this relationship for goal-directed users. In contrast, the The results of our study have some important practical impli-
inverted-U relationship between PWC and user satisfaction cations. We found that user satisfaction for goal-directed
for experiential users suggests that cognitive processing users was maximized at low levels of complexity, whereas
mechanisms may not completely explain how PWC affects experiential users found medium levels of complexity most
user satisfaction for these users. The cognitive processing satisfying. This suggests that to maximize user satisfaction,
mechanisms gain prominence at high levels, but not at low web designers must provide different levels of complexity to
and medium levels, of PWC. Other important aspects may be goal-directed and experiential users.
critical in explaining this relationship for experiential users:
curiosity and interest (Berlyne 1960), which capture the fun, Web designers can create distinct websites with low and
entertainment, and enjoyment value of the website (Hall and medium levels of complexity in many ways. One way would
Hanna 2003; Nack et al. 2001). To maximize the satisfaction be to provide two hyperlinks for entering the site: Text Only
of experiential users, web designers may need to manage the and Rich Graphics. Clicking on the Text Only hyperlink

MIS Quarterly Vol. 31 No. 3/September 2007 517


Nadkarni & Gupta/Task-Based Model of Perceived Website Complexity

could take goal-directed users to a simple website with few containing search product domains (e.g., a university website
graphics, little or no animation, few colors, clearly spelled out with information about application procedures, specific
text hyperlinks, limited information clusters, and an open majors, etc.) will attract goal-directed users seeking detailed
layout with adequate blank space. In contrast, experiential information about the product to facilitate their decision
users could click on the Rich Graphics hyperlink to access a making. Conversely, at websites containing experiential pro-
complex but visually rich website with a wide range of inter- ducts (e.g., music, wine), users are not looking at information
related information categories, rollover effects in navigation, cues at a website to influence their purchase decision.
and audio and video formats, which could provide experien- Instead, such websites will attract experiential users who are
tial users a stimulating and enjoyable experience at the web- seeking to enjoy the process of browsing. Because the pro-
site. A second way to create websites with different levels of duct category at a website determines the type of users (goal-
complexity is to provide users a login option by registering at directed or experiential) that visit the website, web designers
the website. Registered users could access more visually rich, can use this information to discern the optimum level of
complex, and expanded information at the website than non- complexity to be provided at these websites to maximize the
registered users. Because goal-directed users are focused on satisfaction of their users.
the goal, they would prefer to search a simple website without
logging in, whereas experiential users could use the log-in
option to access a medium complexity website.
Conclusions
As web designers provide higher levels of complexity to
experiential users (than goal-directed users), web designers The overarching goal of this paper was to enrich our under-
can use methods such as consumer profiling (Raghu et al. standing of how website complexity affects important user
2001) to identify experiential users, to determine the range of outcomes. We proposed perceived website complexity as a
information that may be of interest to them, and to allow key construct in understanding how the use of sophisticated
experiential users to better manage perceived complexity at a website design features such as animation, audio, video, and
website. Current technology available on the web such as rollover effects affect user satisfaction. The results suggest
dynamic HTML, applets, and cookies provides the means to that online task goals (goal-directed and experiential) deter-
acquire an individual customer profile. By use of these mine how PWC affects user outcomes. Our study represents
applications, customers can be profiled in many ways, a first step in integrating disparate explanations to develop a
including registration forms completed by users, history of more complete understanding of the complex relationships
users’ actions, and current user activity. For example, a site between PWC and user outcomes. Given the undeniable
that sells both compact disks and books may profile a user reality that complexity is inevitable as web design technology
who always goes to the compact disks sections and never to becomes more sophisticated and as the scope of online acti-
the book section as a music lover. The site could then provide vities expands, research that sheds light on how to manage
this user a wide range of music-related information, including and adjust this complexity to maximize user outcomes has
hyperlinks to external musical sites in a variety of formats value to both theory development and practice. Several ave-
such as audio, graphics, and video. Although such richness nues for future work remain, and we hope that this study will
of form and content cues increases the complexity of the stimulate others to extend this line of research further.
website, it will also make the website more enjoyable to
experiential users because the information content matches
the interests of the users. Acknowledgments

Web designers may also be able to use the type of products The authors would like to thank Professor Deborah Compeau (senior
editor), the associate editor, and three anonymous reviewers for their
served at the website to determine whether their user pool is
valuable comments and suggestions, which helped improve this
typically goal-directed or experiential, and may be able to
paper considerably.
adjust the complexity of the website to maximize the satis-
faction of their users. Recent research seems to suggest that
the nature of website experience desired by users may depend References
on whether the online product is a search product (products
that can be evaluated based on prepurchase or pre-use Aaker, J. “Dimensions of Brand Personality,” Journal of Marketing
information) or an experience product (products that can be Research (34:3), 1997, pp. 347-357.
evaluated only by using or buying the product) (Schlosser Agarwal, R., and Venkatesh, V. “Assessing a Firm’s Web Presence:
2003). As users can gain complete information on search A Heuristic Evaluation Procedure for the Measurement of Usa-
products before purchase (Wright and Lynch 1995), websites bility.” Information Systems Research (13:2), 2002, pp. 168-186.

518 MIS Quarterly Vol. 31 No. 3/September 2007


Nadkarni & Gupta/Task-Based Model of Perceived Website Complexity

Anderson, J. C., and Gerbing, D. W. “Predicting the Performance Delone, W. H., and McLean, E. R. “Information Systems Success:
of Measures in a Confirmatory Factor Analysis with a Pretest The Quest for the Dependent Variable,” Information Systems
Assessment of Their Substantive Validities,” Journal of Applied Research (3:1), 1992, pp. 60-95.
Psychology (76:5), 1991, pp. 732-740. Earley, C. “The Influence of Information, Choice and Task Com-
Bagozzi, R. Causal Methods in Marketing, John Wiley and Sons, plexity upon Goal Acceptance, Performance, and Personal
New York, 1980. Goals,” Journal of Applied Psychology (70:1), 1985, pp.
Berlyne, D. E. Conflict, Arousal, and Curiosity, McGraw-Hill, New 481-491.
York, 1960. Fornell, C., and Larcker, D. F. “Evaluating Structural Equations
Berlyne, D. E. “Novelty, Complexity, and Hedonic Value,” Percep- Models with Unobservable Variables and Measurement Error,”
tion & Psychophysics (8:5A), 1970, pp. 279-286. Journal of Marketing Research (18:1), 1981, pp. 39-50.
Brynjolfsson, E., and Smith, M. D. “Frictionless Commerce? A Galbraith, J. A. Designing Complex Organizations, Addison-
Comparison of Internet and Conventional Retailers,” Manage- Wesley Longman Publishing Co., Inc., Boston, MA, 1973.
ment Science (46:4), 2000, pp. 563-585. Geissler, G., Zinkhan G., and Watson R. “Web Homepage Com-
Bucy, E. P., Lang, A., Potter, R. F., and Grabe, M. E. “Formal Fea- plexity and Communication Effectiveness,” Journal of the
tures of Cyberspace: Relationships between Web Page Com- Association for Information Systems (2:2), 2001, pp. 1-46.
plexity and Site Traffic,” Journal of the American Society for Gollwitzer P .M. “Implementation Intentions: Strong Effects of
Information Science (50:13), 1999, pp. 1246-1256. Simple Plans,” American Psychologist (54:7), 1999, pp. 493-503.
Campbell, D. J. “The Effects of Goal-Contingent Payment on the Grush, J. “Attitude Formation and Mere Exposure Phenomena: A
Performance of a Complex Task,” Personnel Psychology (37:1), Nonartifactual Explanation of Empirical Findings,” Journal of
1984, pp. 23-40. Personality and Social Psychology (33:1), 1976, pp. 281-290.
Campbell, D. J. “Task Complexity: A Review and Analysis,” Ha, L., and James, E. L. “Interactivity Reexamined: A Baseline
Academy of Management Review (13:1), 1988, pp. 40-52. Analysis of Early Business Web Sites,” Journal of Broadcasting
Campbell, D. J., and Gingrich, K. F. “The Interactive Effects of and Electronic Media (42:4), 1998, pp. 457-474.
Task Complexity and Participation on Task Performance: A Hall, R. H., and Hanna, P. “The Impact of Web Page Text-
Field Experiment,” Organizational Behavior and Human Deci- Background Color Combinations on Readability, Retention,
sion Processes (38:2), 1986, pp. 162-180. Aesthetics, and Behavioral Intention,” Behavior & Information
Chin, W. “The Partial Least Squares Approach to Structural Equa- Technology (23:3), 2004, pp.183-195.
tion Modeling,” in Modern Business Research Methods, G. A. Harrison, A. W., and Rainer, R. K. “The Influence of Individual
Marcoulides (ed.), Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ, Differences on Skills in End User Computing,” Journal of
1998, pp. 295-336. Management Information Systems (9:1), 1992, pp. 93-111.
Chin, W. W., Marcolin, B. L., and Newsted, P. R. “A Partial Least Hof, R. D. “Desperately Seeking Search Technology,” Business
Squares Latent Variable Modeling Approach for Measuring Week, September 24, 2001, p. 89.
Interaction Effects: Results from a Monte Carlo Simulation Hoffman, D. L., and Novak, T. P. “Marketing in Hypermedia
Study and Electronic Mail Emotion/Adoption Study,” Informa- Computer-Mediated Environments: Conceptual Foundations,”
Journal of Marketing (60:3), 1996, pp. 50-68.
tion Systems Research (14:2), 2003, pp. 189-217.
Hong, W., Tam, K. Y., and Yim, C. K. “E-Service Environment:
Churchill, G. A. “A Paradigm for Developing Better Measures of
Impacts of Web Interface Characteristics on Consumers’ Online
Marketing Constructs,” Journal of Marketing Research (16:1),
Shopping Behavior,” in E-Service: New Directions in Theory
1979, pp. 64-73.
and Practice, R. T. Rust and P. K. Kannan (eds.), M. E. Sharpe,
Churchill, G. A. Jr., and Surprenant, C. “An Investigation into the
Armonk, NY, 2002, pp. 108-128.
Determinants of Customer Satisfaction,” Journal of Marketing
Huber, V. “Effects of Task Difficulty, Goal Setting and Strategy on
Research (19:4), 1982, pp. 491-504.
Performance of a Heuristic Task,” Journal of Applied Psychology
Cox, D. S., and Cox, A. D. “Beyond First Impressions: The
(70:3), 1985, pp. 759-768.
Effects of Repeated Exposure on Consumer Liking of Visually
Igbaria, M., and Parasuraman S. “A Path Analytic Study of Indi-
Complex and Simple Product Designs,” Journal of the Academy vidual Characteristics, Computer Anxiety and Attitudes Towards
of Marketing Science (30:2), 2002, pp. 119-130. Microcomputers,” Journal of Management (15:3), 1989, pp.
Cox, D. S., and Cox, A. D. “What Does Familiarity Breed? Com- 373-388.
plexity as a Moderator of Repetition Effects in Advertisement Jacoby, J., Olson, J. C., and Haddock, R. A. “Price, Brand Name,
Evaluation,” Journal of Consumer Research (15:1), 1988, pp. and Product Composition Characteristics as Determinants of
111-116. Perceived Quality,” Journal of Applied Psychology (55:4), 1971,
Daft, R. L., and Lengel, R. H. “Organizational Information Require- pp. 470-479.
ments, Media Richness and Structural Design,” Management Johnson, E. J., and Russo, J. E. “Product Familiarity and Learning
Science (32:5), 1986, pp. 554-571. New Information,” Journal of Consumer Research (11:1), 1984,
Davis, F. D. “Perceived Usefulness, Perceived Ease of Use, and pp. 542-550
User Acceptance of Information Technology,” MIS Quarterly Keisler, S., and Sproull, L. “Managerial Response to Changing
(13:3), 1989, pp. 318-339. Environments: Perspectives on Problems Sensing from Social

MIS Quarterly Vol. 31 No. 3/September 2007 519


Nadkarni & Gupta/Task-Based Model of Perceived Website Complexity

Cognition,” Administrative Science Quarterly (27:1), 1982, pp. Palmer, J. “Website Usability, Design and Performance Criteria,”
548-570. Information Systems Research (13:2), 2002, pp. 150-167.
Kenny, D. A., and Judd, C. M. “Estimating the Nonlinear and Perrow, C. Y. “A Framework for the Comparative Analysis of
Interactive Effects of Latent Variables,” Psychological Bulletin Organizations,” American Sociological Review (32:1), 1967, pp.
(99:1), 1984, pp. 422-431. 194-208.
Kieras, D. E., and Polson, P. G. “An Approach to the Formal Perrow, C. Y. Complex Organizations: A Critical Essay (3rd ed.),
Analysis of User Complexity,” International Journal of Man- McGrfaw-Hill, Inc., New York, 1986.
Machine Studies (22:1), 1985, pp. 365-394. Raghu, T. S., Kannan, P. K., Rao, H. R., and Whinston, A. B.
Kirk, R. E. Experimental Design: Procedures for the Behavioral “Dynamic Profiling of Consumers for Customized Offerings
Sciences (2nd ed.), Brooks/Cole, Pacific Grove, CA, 1982. Over the Internet: A Model and Analysis,” Decision Support
Kotha, S., Rajgopal, S., and Venkatachalam, M. “The Role of Systems (32:2), 2001, pp. 117-134.
Online Buying Experience as a Competitive Advantage: Russo, J .E., and Johnson, E. J. “What Do Customers Know About
Evidence from Third-Party Ratings for E-Commere Firms,” Familiar Products?,” in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol.
Journal of Business (77:2), 2004, pp. 109-133. VII, J. C. Olson (ed.), Association for Consumer Research, Ann
Kramer, A. F., Wickens, C. D., and Donchin, E. “An Analysis of Arbor, MI, 1980, pp. 417-424.
the Processing Requirements of a Complex Perceptual–Motor Schlosser, A. “Experiencing Products in the Virtual World: The
Task,” Human Factors (25:1), 1983, pp. 597-621. Role of Goal and Imagery in Influencing Attitudes Versus
Larkin, J., McDermott, J., Simon D. P., and Simon, H. A. “Expert Purchase Intentions,” Journal of Consumer Research (30:2),
and Novice Performance in Solving Physics Problems,” Science 2003, pp. 184-198.
(208), 1985, pp. 1335-1342. Schubert, P., and Selz, D. “Web Assessment: A Model for the
Law, K. S., and Wong, C. “Toward a Taxonomy of Multi- Evaluation and Assessment of Successful Electronic Commerce
dimensional Constructions,” Academy of Management Review Applications,” in Proceedings of the 31st Hawaii International
(23:4), 1998, pp. 741-755. Conference on System Sciences, IEEE Computer Society Press,
Lindsay, P., and Norman, D. Human Information Processing: An Los Alamitos, CA., 1998.
Introduction to Psychology (2nd ed.), Academic Press, New York, Sheridan, T. B. “Mental Workload –What Is It? Why Bother with
1977. It?,” Human Factors Society Bulletin (23:2), 1980, pp. 1-2.
March, J., and Simon, H. Organizations, Wiley, New York, 1958. Shneiderman, B. Designing the User Interface: Strategies for
McKinney, V., Yoon, K., and Zahedi, F. M. “The Measurement of Effective Human-Computer Interaction, Addison-Wesley,
Web-Customer Satisfaction: An Expectation and Disconfirma- Reading, MA, 1998.
tion Approach,” Information Systems Research (13:3), 2002, pp. Steinmann, D. “The Effects of Cognitive Feedback and Task Com-
296-315. plexity in Multiple-Cue Probability Learning,” Organizational
Miller, G G. A. “The Magical Number Seven, Plus or Minus Two: Behavior and Human Performance (15:4), 1976, pp. 168-179.
Some Limits on Our Capacity for Processing Information,” Stevenson, J. S., Bruner G. C. II, and Kumar A. “Web Page Back-
Psychological Review (63:2), 1956, pp. 81-97. ground and Viewer Attitudes,” Journal of Advertising Research
Moe, W. W. “Buying, Searching, or Browsing: Differentiating (40:1&2), 2000, pp. 29-34.
between Online Shoppers Using In-Store Navigational Click- Steuer, J. “Defining Virtual Reality: Dimensions Determining
stream,” Journal of Consumer Psychology (13:1&2), 2003, pp. Telepresence,” Journal of Communication (42:4), 1992, pp.
29-40. 73-93.
Moray, N. Mental Workload: Its Theory and Measurement, Plenum Taylor, M. “The Motivational Effects of Task Challenge: A
Press, New York, 1977. Laboratory Investigation,” Organizational Behavior and Human
Nack, F., Dorai, C., and Venkatesh, S. “Computational Media Performance (29:2), 1981, pp. 255-278.
Aesthetics: Finding Meaning Beautiful,” IEEE Multimedia (8:4), Te'eni, D. “Determinants and Consequences of Perceived Com-
2001, pp. 10-12. plexity in Human–Computer Interaction,” Decision Sciences
Nielsen, J. Designing Web Usability, New Riders Publishing, (20:1), 1989, pp. 166-181.
Indianapolis, IN, 2000. Te'eni, D. “Review: A Cognitive–Affective Model of Organi-
Nielsen, J. “Heuristic Evaluation,” in Usability Inspection Methods, zational Communication for Designing IT,” MIS Quarterly
J. Nielsen and R. L. Mack (eds.), John Wiley and Sons, New (25:2), 2001, pp. 251-312.
York, 1994, pp. 25-64. Te'eni, D., and Feldman, R. “Performance and Satisfaction in
Novak, T. P., Hoffman, D. L., and Duhachek, A. “The Influence of Adaptive Websites: An Experiment on Searchers Within a Task-
Goal-Directed and Experiential Activities on Online Flow Adapted Website,” Journal of the Association for Information
Experiences,” Journal of Consumer Psychology (13:1&2), 2003, Systems (2:3), 2001, pp. 1-28.
pp. 3-16. Took, R. “Putting Design into Practice: Formal Specification and
Nunnally, J. C. Psychometric Theory, McGraw-Hill, New York, the User Interface,” in Formal Methods in Human-Computer
1978. Interaction, M. Harrison and H. Thimbleby (eds.), Cambridge
Oinas-Kukkonen, H. “Evaluating the Usefulness of Design University Press, Cambridge, UK, 1990, pp. 82 -96.
Rationale in CASE,” European Journal of Information Systems Torangeau, R. “Context Effects on Answers to Attitude Questions,”
(7:3), 1998, pp.185-191. in Cognition and Survey Research, M. G. Sirken, D. J. Hermann,

520 MIS Quarterly Vol. 31 No. 3/September 2007


Nadkarni & Gupta/Task-Based Model of Perceived Website Complexity

S. Schechter, N. Schwarz, J. M. Tanur, and R. Tourangeau (eds.), Wolfinbarger, M., and Gilly, M. C. “Shopping Online for Freedom,
Wiley, New York, 1999, pp. 111-131. Control and Fun,” California Management Review (43:2), 2001,
Venkatesh, V. “Determinants of Perceived Ease of Use: Integrating pp. 34-55.
Control, Intrinsic Motivation, and Emotion into the Technology Wood, R. E. “Task Complexity: Definition of a Construct,”
Acceptance Model,” Information Systems Research (11:4), 2000, Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes (31:1),
pp. 342-365. 1986, pp. 60-82.
Venkatesh, V., and Agarwal, R. “Turning Visitors into Customers: Wright, A. A., and Lynch, J. G. Jr. “Communication Effects of
A Usability-Centric Perspective on Purchase Behavior in Elec- Advertising Versus Direct Experience When Both Search and
tronic Channels,” Management Science (52:3), 2006, pp. Experience Attributes Are Present,” Journal of Consumer
367-382.. Research (21:4), 1995, pp. 708-718.
Venkatesh, V., and Morris, M. G. “Why Don’t Men Ever Stop to Yang Jonson, F. “Modeling Interaction and Nonlinear Effects: A
Ask for Directions? Gender, Social Influence, and Their Role in Step-by-Step LISREL Example,” in Interaction and Nonlinear
Technology Acceptance and Usage Behavior,” MIS Quarterly Effects in Structural Equation Modeling, R. E. Schumacker and
(24:1), 2000, pp.115-139. G. A. Marcoulides (eds.), Lawrence Erlbaum Associates,
Venkatesh, V., Morris, M. G., Davis, G. B., and Davis, F. D. “User Mahwah, NJ, 1998, pp. 17-42.
Acceptance of Information Technology: Toward a Unified
View,” MIS Quarterly (27:3), 2003, pp. 425-478.
Welford, A. T. “Mental Work-Load as a Function of Demand,
About the Authors
Capacity, Strategy, and Skill,” Ergonomics (21:3), 1978, pp.
Sucheta Nadkarni is an associate professor of Management at the
151-167.
University of Nebraska–Lincoln. She received her Ph.D. from the
Wilder, G., Mackie, D., and Cooper, J. “Gender and Computers:
University of Kansas. Her research focuses on cognitive issues in
Two Surveys of Computer-Related Attitudes,” Sex Roles
strategic decision making and management information systems.
(13:3&4), 1985, pp. 215-228. She has published or has papers forthcoming in journals such as
Williams, L. J., and Anderson, S. E. “An Alternative Approach to Strategic Management Journal, Journal of International Business
Method Effects Using Latent-Variable Models: Applications in Studies, Organization Science, and MIS Quarterly.
Organizational Behavior Research,” Journal of Applied Psycho-
logy (79:2), 1994, pp. 323-331. Reetika Gupta is an assistant professor of Marketing at Lehigh
Wold, H. “The Fix-Point Approach to Interdependent Systems: University. She received her Ph.D. from Baruch College, City
Review and Current Outlook,” in The Fix-Point Approach to University of New York. Her research interests include consumer
Interdependent Systems, H. Wold (ed.), North-Holland, Amster- behavior in interactive consumption environments and consumer
dam, 1981, pp. 1-35. learning of new products.

MIS Quarterly Vol. 31 No. 3/September 2007 521


Nadkarni & Gupta/Task-Based Model of Perceived Website Complexity

Appendix A
Graphical Illustrations of Sale Items
A1. Illustration of Component Complexity
Low Component Complexity High Component Complexity

Finance Music Shopping Mail MyGoget it! Messenger

ChooseSearchCategory: WEB l Picturesl DirectorylLocal l NEW!l Newsl Products

•Advanced
SearchtheWeb: Goget it!! Search •Preferences
Web Pictures Groups News About Go get it!! More>>

Advanced Search
Preferences
Language Tools

Search Go Get It!! I feel good!


What’sin News 9:00am, wedNov10

•Tasteechocolatecompanystartedanewplant
ReplayAnimation-AdFeedback •Hurricaneshit somepartsof theworld
•Therehasbeendomesticunrest against current
FunThings >>Funthings
government inacountryin Asia
•Tourismindustryprospersdomestically
PreviewLindonHogan's'That ‘80sShow'debut •Currencyfluctuationscontinuetoplague
•Trailer Premiere: DavidBeaconin'Thetreeman' international business
•LennyRepptalksparenthoodand'Noland' •President resignsinamajor countryintheworld
•Meet DRIVEBuddingArtist RonnieMcFee
•Markets:
Hohan'sTVDebut
Dow+0.3%·Nasdaq-0.3%StockQuotes:

InvogueLog–What everyoneislookingfor >>MoreInvogue Log


What’stheweather like 9:00am, wedNov10
Great waytobeginyour morning. Digintoourtopbreakfast
searches! More...
Enter U.S. Cityor ZipCode
TopBreakfastSearches

1. BedandBreakfast 3. Breakfast Recipes Go


2. Breakfast at Tiffany's 4. TheBreakfast Club
»Findworldwideweather

Wow!! Onlinedirectory >>MoreGoget it!! Onlinedirectory Savelocationonthispage

Arts Business Computers Education Entertainment Health About Goget it!!


News Recreation Reference Regional Science Society

522 MIS Quarterly Vol. 31 No. 3/September 2007


Nadkarni & Gupta/Task-Based Model of Perceived Website Complexity

A2. Illustration of Coordinative Website Complexity

Linked Page

Pre vious Home Help Ne xt

Web Pict ures Gr ou ps News Abo u t G o ge t it !! More >>

NEWS PAGE
Tastee chocolate
company started a new
T op Stories plant
Tastee chocolate company opened its new plant
W orld in California. This plant employs 1,000 full time
employees and is located Applecounty.
D omes tic C ountry Tasteeinvested a tfotal of 20 million dollars and
the company expects to produce over 200,000
Bus iness units of chocolate a month
More>>
Sc ienc e/Tec hnology
Hurricanes hit some
Sports parts of the world

Home Page S ome countries inS outhAmerica were hit by


some powerful hurricanes which destroyed
several houses along the coastline. The local
government dispatched approximately 2000

Low Coordinative
rescue workers to affected areas and allocated 1
million dollars to helpin the reconstruction
process.
More>>
Sear ch Go Get It!!

Domestic unrest against

Complexity current government in an


Asian country
There were several demonstrations and strikes
against the local government in anA siancountry.
50 people were injured and no casualties were
reported. The government officials continued their
negotiations with theleaders of the demonstrating

Previous Home Help Next organization to restore peace.


More>>

Web Pictures Groups News About Go get it!! More>>


Linked Page
Advanced Search
Preferences
Language Tools

Search Go Get It!! I feel good!


Top Stories
NEWS PAGE
Global
Tastee chocolate company
High Coordinative Local
started a new plant
Business
Complexity Science/Technology
Tastee chocolate company opened its new plant in
California. This plant employs 1,000 full time
employees and is located Apple county.
Sports Tasteeinvested a tfotal of 20 million dollars and the
Entertainment company expects to produce over 200,000 units
of chocolate a month
Health More>>
Weather Hurricanes hit some parts of
the world
Search Go Get It!! Some countries in South America were hit by some
powerful hurricanes which destroyed several
houses along the coastline. The local government
dispatched approximately 2000 rescue workers to
affected areas and allocated 1 million dollars to
NEWS SOURCES help in the reconstruction process.
Press More>>

Television Domestic unrest against


Domestic current government in an Asian
International country
There were several demonstrations and strikes
against the local government in an Asian country.
50 people were injured and no casualties were
reported. The government officials continued their
negotiations with the leaders of the demonstrating
organization to restore peace.
More>>

MIS Quarterly Vol. 31 No. 3/September 2007 523


Nadkarni & Gupta/Task-Based Model of Perceived Website Complexity

A3. Illustration of Dynamic Website Complexity


Ambiguous Hyperlinks for Shopping Cart Unambiguous Hyperlinks for Shopping Cart

J O E ’S G R E E N B A Y P A C K E R S ’
C O R N E R

O n e -s to p s h o p fo r G re e n B a y P a c k e r fa n s !

H is to r y o f G re e n
B a y P a c ke rs
G re e n B a y P a c k e rs
T ic k e t F in d e r

N ew s a n d B u zz V e n u e S ta te /P r o v in c e

Hyperlink: NFL ticket bid A n y

V e n u e C it y
G re e n B a y
P a c k e r s S p o r t in g
M o n th
G o o d s

Uncertain P h o to s
D ay

Link
Great Green Bay
Y e a r

Packer tickets⎯ T ic k e t s
S h o w ite m s

Chicago Bears@
Green Bay Packers
Venue:
Lambeau Field
Current bid US $139.45
Certain Place Bid >

Link Time left: 22 hours 14 mins


1- day listing. Ends Dec - 08
05 16:52:10 PST
Start time: Dec - 07 - 05 16: 52:10 PST

History: 12 bids (US $0.01 starting bid)

High Bidder: bistrobistro (128 )

Item location: Cincinnati, Ohio


United States

Shipping, payment details and return policy

524 MIS Quarterly Vol. 31 No. 3/September 2007

Você também pode gostar