Você está na página 1de 11

Contemporary Educational Psychology 37 (2012) 229–239

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Contemporary Educational Psychology


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cedpsych

Choosing a major in higher education: Profiles of students’ decision-making process


Veerle Germeijs a,⇑, Koen Luyckx a, Guy Notelaers b, Luc Goossens a, Karine Verschueren a
a
Department of Psychology, Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Tiensestraat 102, B-3000 Leuven, Belgium
b
Bergen Bullying Research Group, Department of Psychosocial Science, University of Bergen, Norway

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: This study identified decision-making profiles of students who make a choice of a major in higher edu-
Available online 18 January 2012 cation. These profiles were examined in a sample of Belgian students at the end of Grade 12, when the
educational system expects that these adolescents choose a specific major. Using latent class cluster anal-
Keywords: ysis on adolescents’ scores for coping with career decisional tasks (i.e., orientation, exploration, decisional
Educational choice status, and commitment), four clusters were identified. As expected, these profiles paralleled Marcia’s
Decision-making profiles (1966) identity statuses (i.e., the achievement, moratorium, foreclosure, and diffusion status). Results
Higher education
provided support for the external validity of the identified clusters through differential associations with
Identity
several person variables (i.e., career decision-making self-efficacy, career choice anxiety, and decision-
making style) and with academic functioning in higher education (i.e., commitment, academic and social
adjustment). Implications for current educational research and research on career decision-making are
discussed.
Ó 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction Educational choices such as choosing a major can be considered


as important career-related decisions. Because adolescents’ ideas
Each year many adolescents are confronted with the issue of about possible future careers develop while making such educa-
choosing a major in higher education. This educational decision tional decisions, these choices can be seen as a means for imple-
usually implies a complex decision-making process during which menting occupational choices (Lent, Brown, & Hackett, 1994) and
adolescents are faced with decisional tasks or activities such as as an integral part of career development (Super, 1980). More spe-
exploring different alternatives, reflecting on interests and skills, cifically, within Super’s theory of career development (Super,
comparing suitable alternatives, and choosing one option. Most 1980) educational choices can been seen as a mini-cycle (i.e., a
school counselors would agree that adolescents who are con- decision-making process) within the maxi-cycle of career develop-
fronted with making the choice of a major may differ in the way ment: at the end of such a mini-cycle an individual has selected a
they deal with these decisional tasks. For example, some students preferred plan of action (e.g., entering a major in higher education)
may be highly committed to their choice without having explored which in turn influences his or her career. By investigating adoles-
several alternatives, whereas others may show high levels of cents’ decision-making profiles with regard to choosing a major in
exploration but are still uncertain about their choice. Thus, differ- higher education, the current study aims to shed more light on the
ent groups of adolescents may exist that are characterized by dif- ways adolescents address one such mini-cycle.
ferent combinations of involvement in these decisional tasks From a theoretical viewpoint, investigating adolescents’ profiles
(Van Esbroeck, Tibos, & Zaman, 2005), each reflecting a different of choosing a major may add to the conceptualization of how peo-
way of approaching this educational decision. However, little re- ple make career-related decisions. Specifically, it may help to
search has addressed the question of whether different decision- understand whether or not it is important to pay attention to all
making profiles empirically exist among adolescents. The main decisional tasks advanced in career decision-making models. For
purpose of the present study was to identify decision-making pro- example, it has been hypothesized that a combination of being
files of adolescents when choosing a specific major in higher highly committed to a career choice (i.e., being attached to, certain
education. and self-confident about career preferences and choices; Blustein,
Ellis, & Devenis, 1989) and having experienced a period of career
⇑ Corresponding author. Fax: +32 16 326 144.
exploration (i.e., eliciting information about oneself and one’s envi-
E-mail addresses: veerle.germeijs@psy.kuleuven.be (V. Germeijs), koen.luyckx@
ronment and how both interact; Jordaan, 1963) is more beneficial
psy.kuleuven.be (K. Luyckx), guy.notelaers@uib.no (G. Notelaers), luc.goossens@
psy.kuleuven.be (L. Goossens), karine.verschueren@psy.kuleuven.be than a combination of the same level of commitment and limited
(K. Verschueren). exploration in terms of choice satisfaction, choice stability, and

0361-476X/$ - see front matter Ó 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2011.12.002
230 V. Germeijs et al. / Contemporary Educational Psychology 37 (2012) 229–239

adjustment (Brisbin & Savickas, 1994). However, to our knowledge, fers to learning generally about the self and possible careers,
this hypothesis has not been tested yet. whereas in-depth exploration refers to learning about a reduced
From a practical viewpoint, educational guidance may also ben- set of career alternatives that are more aligned with the self
efit from the identification of these profiles because they may pro- (Porfeli & Skorikov, 2010). In line with these newer models, the
vide a parsimonious way of describing differences between current study also relied on an elaborate set of dimensions (i.e.,
students’ educational decision-making processes. Such a parsimo- decisional tasks) to characterize students’ approach to deciding
nious description, in turn, facilitates communication among coun- on a major.
selors in educational settings. Insight into these profiles may also
guide students’ grouping for tailored assistance in their educa- 1.2. Career decisional tasks
tional decision-making process (Distefano & Kamphaus, 2006). In
the current study, the identity formation literature and its linkages To identify adolescents’ educational decision-making profiles in
with the career development literature are used as a guiding the current study, six dimensions or decisional tasks were used,
framework for our hypotheses on the types of educational deci- which are considered core aspects of the career decision-making
sion-making profiles that might emerge. process at the micro-level (Germeijs & Verschueren, 2006). More
specifically, we aimed to identify different intra-individual combi-
1.1. Identity formation and career decision-making nations of the following six decisional tasks: (a) orientation (i.e.,
awareness of the need to make a decision and motivation to en-
In personal identity research, the distinction between different gage in the career decision-making process), (b) self-exploration
types of dealing with personally relevant decisions has received a (i.e., gathering information about one’s characteristics, including
lot of attention. Personal identity formation involves defining who one’s interests, values, and abilities), (c) broad exploration of the
you are, what your goals and values are, and which directions you environment (i.e., gathering general information about career
choose to pursue in life. In Marcia’s (1966) identity status paradigm, alternatives), (d) in-depth exploration of the environment (i.e.,
which is one of the most important models for research on personal gathering detailed information about a reduced set of career alter-
identity formation, adolescent identity development is viewed as a natives), (e) decisional status (i.e., progress in choosing an alterna-
series of crises during which decisions in identity-relevant domains tive), and (f) commitment (i.e., strength of confidence in,
have to be made. Marcia (1966) mainly focused on individual attachment to, and identification with a particular career alterna-
differences in the way these decisions are made. More specifically, tive). With regard to adolescents’ process of choosing a major in
he distinguished among four different identity statuses, based on higher education, support was found for the distinctiveness of
combining high and/or low scores on the dimensions of identity these six career decisional tasks (Germeijs & Verschueren, 2006).
exploration (i.e., active consideration of potential alternative sets The present study uses these six tasks, which allows for a
of goals, values, and beliefs) and commitment (i.e., attaining a clear differentiated view on possible types of adolescents. The career
sense of self-definition and adhering to choices in identity-relevant decisional tasks of environmental (i.e., broad or in-depth) and
domains). These statuses are referred to as achievement (high self-exploration can be seen as equivalent to further refinements
exploration, high commitment), foreclosure (low exploration, high of Marcia’s exploration dimension and correspond with the
commitment), moratorium (high exploration, low commitment), refinements made in current identity and career decision-making
and diffusion (low exploration, low commitment). research (e.g., Porfeli & Skorikov, 2010). The distinction between
One important life domains for personal identity formation is decisional status and commitment made in the present study
the career domain (Grotevant, 1987; Marcia, 1966), in which nicely maps onto the distinction between commitment-making
exploration and commitment are also conceptualized as key com- and identification with commitment in the current personal
ponents for (career) development (Jordaan, 1963; Super, 1980). Ca- identity literature (e.g., Luyckx et al., 2006).
reer identity (also referred to as vocational or occupational In the country where the present study was conducted (i.e.,
identity; Skorikov & Vondracek, 2011) is regarded as a core compo- Belgium) students follow a major from the first year in higher
nent of one’s overall sense of identity, being not only important for education on. Hence, the educational system and the societal
choices in the career domain, but also for giving meaning and context expect from these adolescents to choose a specific major
structure to a person’s life (Kroger, Martinussen, & Marcia, 2010; by the end of high school. Because these environmental expecta-
Skorikov & Vondracek, 1998). Indeed, in several studies, support tions may encourage adolescents to work on the decisional tasks
has been found for a positive relationship between vocational iden- as they move towards the end of high school (Grotevant, 1987;
tity, career development, career exploration and commitment on Kalakoski & Nurmi, 1998), adolescents’ decision-making profiles
the one hand and identity statuses on the other (e.g., Blustein, are studied at the end of Grade 12.
Devenis, & Kidney, 1989; Savickas, 1985; Vondracek, Schulenberg,
Skorikov, Gillespie, & Wahlheim, 1995). 1.3. Career decision-making profiles
In more recent identity models, authors tried to ‘‘unpack’’ the
dimensions of exploration and commitment into a set of more spe- Our review of the literature on career decision-making yielded
cific processes. This trend towards more process-oriented models nine studies in which different career decision-making types or pro-
is also observed in the career development literature where files were identified through the use of cluster analysis (Callanan &
authors have emphasized additional dimensions within career Greenhaus, 1992; Chartrand et al., 1994; Kelly & Pulver, 2003;
exploration and commitment. With regard to commitment, recent Larson, Heppner, Ham, & Dugan, 1988; Lucas, 1993; Lucas &
identity models that build on Marcia’s work emphasize that com- Epperson, 1990; Rojewski, 1994; Savickas & Jarjoura, 1991;
mitment has different components to it (Bosma & Kunnen, 2001), Wanberg & Muchinsky, 1992). Cluster analytic techniques group
such as the making of commitments and feeling certain about persons in relatively homogeneous clusters based on multivariate
these commitments (Luyckx, Goossens, & Soenens, 2006). With re- observations in such a way that persons assigned to the same cluster
gard to exploration, both the identity literature (Luyckx et al., have more in common than persons assigned to different clusters
2006) and the career development literature (Flum & Blustein, (Gore, 2000). Three profiles emerged regularly across studies (Kelly
2000; Porfeli & Skorikov, 2010) state that it is important to distin- & Pulver, 2003): the chronically indecisive type (i.e., no or low
guish between broad or general exploration and more detailed or commitment to a career choice, high need for career information,
in-depth exploration. In the career domain, broad exploration re- and much negative affect), the developmentally undecided type
V. Germeijs et al. / Contemporary Educational Psychology 37 (2012) 229–239 231

(i.e., no or low commitment to a career choice, high need for career high school students which are similar to the four identity statuses.
information, and little negative affect), and the decided or ready to First and parallel to the achievement status, one profile would be
decide type (i.e., little career indecision or being decided upon a ca- characterized by high environmental (i.e., broad or in depth) and
reer, low need for self- or career information, and little negative self-exploration and high scores on decisional status. Based on
affect). the positive relationship between personal growth (i.e., active
In some studies, not all of these three types could be identified engagement in the change process of personal growth) and iden-
and/or additional profiles emerged. This may be due to the differ- tity (Robitschek & Cook, 1999), students in this profile are expected
ent sets of decisional variables and person characteristics used in to show high scores on orientation regarding making the educa-
the cluster analyses but also to the different samples used in the tional choice. Finally, because the achievement status has been
respective studies (Brown & Rector, 2008). For example, some characterized by high identification with the choices made
studies used a general high school sample (e.g., Rojewski, 1994), (Luyckx, Goossens, Soenens, Beyers, & Vansteenkiste, 2005), it is
whereas others selected samples from a general college population expected that students in this first profile score high on commit-
(e.g., Wanberg & Muchinsky, 1992). It is clear that the participants ment with the educational choice. Second, the profile parallel to
in these samples were in different developmental stages and, as a the foreclosed status would be characterized by low environmental
consequence, experienced different socio-cultural expectations (i.e., broad or in-depth) and self-exploration and high scores on
regarding their preparation for a career. These differences may ex- decisional status and commitment to their educational choice
plain why some studies could identify, for example, a ‘confident (Luyckx et al., 2005). As students in this profile make little effort
decided type’ (Wanberg & Muchinsky, 1992), whereas others only for exploration, we hypothesize that these students are less moti-
identified a ‘tentatively decided’ type (Rojewski, 1994). vated to engage in the decision-making process than the achieved
Typical of the studies on career decision-making types is that individuals and thus would score substantially lower on orienta-
they focused mostly on career decision-making at a general or tion. In contrast to existing studies that were not able to identify
macro level and not on a [specific mini-cycle or] decision-making a foreclosed career decision-making profile, we expect to find a
process at the micro-level. By using more general measures of ca- profile parallel to the foreclosed status by using a more differenti-
reer (in)decision (e.g., Career Decision Scale, Osipow, 1987) these ated set of decisional tasks. Third and parallel to the moratorium
studies could examine approaches to career decision-making at a status (Marcia, 1980), we expect to find a profile characterized
macro level (e.g., the future career) rather than the micro-level by high environmental (i.e., broad or in-depth) and self-exploration
(e.g., the choice of a major or the choice of a job). Second, in the but low decisional status and commitment. Because these students
case that measures regarding decisional tasks were used, only show engagement in the career decision-making process through
some of the six central career decisional tasks were included. Third, exploratory behavior, it is expected that this profile is character-
many of these studies used career decisional as well as person ized by moderate to moderately high orientation scores. Fourth
variables (e.g., anxiety, self-esteem) to characterize the decision- and finally, the profile parallel to the diffusion status would be
making type. However, researchers have suggested that the pro- characterized by low scores on all decisional tasks (Luyckx et al.,
files be based on career decisional variables only (Chartrand 2005; Robitschek & Cook, 1999).
et al., 1994). The person characteristics should rather be used to In addition, the external validity of the profiles derived was
examine the external validity of the identified career decision- tested by examining whether individuals in the different profile
making profiles. The latter strategy, which is used in the current groups differ as expected on several person variables (i.e., career
study, would allow for more straightforward conclusions about decision-making self-efficacy, career choice anxiety, and deci-
decision-making profiles without possible confounding influences sion-making style), and on academic functioning in higher educa-
from other related variables (Chartrand et al., 1994). tion (i.e., commitment, academic, and social adjustment).
When comparing Marcia’s (1966) four identity statuses with the
career decision-making profiles that emerged across different stud- 1.5. Personal correlates of educational decision-making profiles
ies, it seems that the ‘decided’ or ‘ready to decide’ type corresponds
most with the achievement status and the ‘chronically indecisive’ Person variables that have been used frequently in previous re-
type with the diffusion status. The ‘developmentally undecided’ search to differentiate between different career decision-making
type, and similar types described in the career decision literature, profiles are feelings of anxiety (i.e., trait anxiety or decision-mak-
such as ‘the undecided student who was exploring self and occupa- ing anxiety) and beliefs regarding the self (e.g., Chartrand et al.,
tions’ (Savickas & Jarjoura, 1991), seem to correspond with the mor- 1994; Kelly & Pulver, 2003; Wanberg & Muchinsky, 1992). In iden-
atorium status in the identity literature. However, no career tity research, anxiety and self-beliefs also discriminated well
decision-making profile has been identified empirically which among the identity statuses. Specifically, the two low-commitment
resembles the foreclosure status. This is in line with the results of statuses (i.e., diffusion and moratorium) have been found to show
Brisbin and Savickas (1994) who found that the most widely used higher anxiety and more negative self-beliefs than the high-com-
measures of career indecision do not measure foreclosure. mitment statuses (i.e., foreclosure and achievement) (Kroger,
Because specific educational choices can be seen as activities that 2003; Luyckx et al., 2008; Marcia, 1980). Therefore, in the current
help to shape the process of constructing one’s career identity study, we examined the external validity of the educational deci-
(Skorikov & Vondracek, 2011), in the current study the choice of a sion-making profiles obtained by investigating the associations
major in higher education is considered as an identity-relevant with anxiety and self-beliefs. Because associations with profiles
choice or as a micro-process in adolescents’ career identity develop- in the domain of career decision-making are examined, domain-
ment. Therefore, we studied whether educational decision-making specific variables were chosen (Goossens, 2001). More specifically,
profiles at the micro-level would parallel the four identity statuses we used measures of anxiety feelings related to the career deci-
in broader life domains (e.g., the career domain) or at the general sion-making process (i.e., career choice anxiety) and self-efficacy
level. beliefs concerning the ability to complete tasks necessary for mak-
ing a career decision (i.e., career decision-making self-efficacy).
1.4. Four educational decision-making profiles at the micro-level In addition to career choice anxiety and career decision-making
self-efficacy, the degree to which adolescents use a rational deci-
In the current study, we expected that cluster analysis would sion-making style was used to externally validate the educational
reveal at least four educational decision-making profiles among decision-making profiles obtained. People who use a rational
232 V. Germeijs et al. / Contemporary Educational Psychology 37 (2012) 229–239

decision-making style actively seek information and make deci- coping with the six career decisional tasks. The questionnaire
sions deliberately and logically (Buck & Daniels, 1985). Persons in was administered during regular classes and all 665 students filled
the achievement and moratorium statuses tend to use planful it out. In the current study, this sample was used to answer the re-
and reflective strategies in decision-making in contrast to persons search question regarding the identification of different career
in the foreclosure and diffusion statuses (Blustein & Phillips, 1990; decision making profiles (first research question). The second ques-
Marcia, 1980; Waterman & Waterman, 1974). Therefore, it is tionnaire probed person characteristics, including career choice
hypothesized that a rational decision-making style would discrim- anxiety, career decision-making self-efficacy, and rational decision
inate well between the educational decision-making profiles char- making style. This questionnaire was completed at home because
acterized by high exploration (i.e., achievement and moratorium) insufficient time was left to fill out both questionnaires within
and low exploration (i.e., moratorium and diffusion). one class. The questionnaires were delivered in a closed envelope
to the school. In the instructions of the home questionnaires stu-
1.6. Consequences of educational decision-making profiles for choice dents were encouraged to answer the questions independently.
implementation From the 665 students who filled out the first questionnaire, 422
students (168 boys and 254 girls) returned the second question-
The quality of the way a career decision is made, is expected to naire. The sample of 422 students was used to investigate the
influence the quality of actualization of the chosen direction in the external validity of the career decision-making profiles obtained
future (e.g., Gati & Asher, 2001; Harren, 1979; Tiedeman & O’Hara, by examining their concurrent associations with person character-
1963; Van Esbroeck et al., 2005). However, Kelly and Pulver (2003) istics (second research question). Attrition analyses revealed no
stressed that there have been no studies investigating academic or differences between the sample of 422 students and the group of
career outcomes associated with the different career decision- students who did not complete the second home questionnaire
making profiles. The current study examined longitudinally the on the six career decisional tasks (F (6, 565) = 1.81, p = .09,
associations between adolescents’ educational decision-making g2 = .02) neither on the three indices for choice implementation
profiles and the quality of subsequent choice implementation (i.e., commitment, academic adjustment, and social adjustment)
(i.e., academic functioning) in higher education. Specifically, we in higher education (F (3, 368) = 2.09, p = .10, g2 = .02).
examined whether the different profiles predict commitment to At the beginning of the second year in higher education
the chosen study and adjustment to higher education during the (November 2004), all students received a questionnaire by mail
second year in the chosen major. Because the transition to higher probing their commitment to the chosen major and adjustment.
education requires adolescents to adjust in both the social and Students were asked to return the questionnaires in a self-
the academic domain (Gall, Evans, & Bellerose, 2000; Gerdes & addressed stamped envelope. Students who did not respond were
Mallinckrodt, 1994), we investigated adjustment to the educa- phoned and asked to still participate in the study. From the 665
tional demands (i.e., academic adjustment) as well as adjustment students who participated at the end of Grade 12, a total of 373
to the interpersonal-societal demands (i.e., social adjustment) of students answered the questions on commitment to the chosen
higher education. major and adjustment during the second year in higher education.
Based on reviews by Marcia (1980, 1993) on the relationship This sample is used to examine the external validity of the deci-
between identity statuses and adjustment and based on studies sion-making profiles by investigating their predictive validity for
about the associations between identity in the career domain and the quality of choice implementation (second research question).
its outcomes (see Skorikov and Vondracek (2007), for an overview), The attrition analysis revealed no significant differences on the
we expected that adolescents in the achievement profile, and to a decisional tasks between the original sample and the students
lesser extent, in the foreclosure profile, would show the highest who dropped out of the study (F (6, 568) = 1.78, p = .10).2
commitment to their major and the highest levels of adjustment
in higher education. Students in the moratorium profile were ex-
2.2. Measures
pected to show less commitment and adjustment than students
in the achievement and foreclosure profiles. Students in the diffu-
All except two measures (i.e., Study Choice Task Inventory and
sion profile would show the lowest levels of commitment and
Commitment Scale in higher education) were originally created in
adjustment.
English and were translated into Dutch by the first author.3 A Ph.D.
student in psychology backtranslated the items into English and an-
2. Method other Ph.D. student in psychology matched the original items and
the items backtranslated into English. All items were correctly
2.1. Participants and procedure matched.

In all, 665 students (300 boys and 365 girls) participated in this 2
In line with a reviewer’s request, we checked whether the cluster solution on the
study. The students came from 25 high schools in Flanders, the decisional tasks measured at the end of Grade 12 is similar in the following two
Dutch-speaking part of Belgium. Nearly all students were White. samples: the sample of students who participated at the end of Grade 12 (n = 665)
They followed general education1 and came from the final year in and the subsample of students who participated at the end of Grade 12 as well as in
the second year of higher education (n = 373). The cluster solutions on the decisional
high school (Grade 12). All students received two questionnaires at
tasks at the end of Grade 12 in both samples turned out to be highly similar.
the end of Grade 12 (May 2003). Mean age for this sample at that 3
For all translated instruments (i.e., Career Decision Self-Efficacy Scale, Career
moment was 18; 0 years (SD = 6.3 months). Choice Anxiety Scale, Rational Decision-Making Style Scale, Academic Adjustment
The first questionnaire the students received at the end of Grade Scale, Social Adjustment Scale) the internal structure was investigated through
12 assessed background variables (i.e., age and gender) and their confirmatory factor analyses. For each scale we tested whether a model with one
underlying dimension holds in the current sample. The items from each scale were
divided at random into four parcels and these parcels were used as indicators of each
1
In Belgian high schools different educational forms are distinguished (i.e., dimension. With this procedure the number of measured variables, and hence the
education in the arts, general, technical and vocational education). After successfully number of parameters to be estimated in the models becomes smaller (Bentler &
completing high school and receiving a certificate, students from all educational Chou, 1987; Marsh & O’Neill, 1984). For each scale, the results showed that the fit of a
forms have unrestricted access to higher education. The group of students who follow one-factor model was good (as indicated by the Standardized Root Mean Square
general education in high school has the largest proportion of students starting higher Residual, the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation and the Comparative Fit
education (i.e., 97.3%; Coppieters et al., 2002). Index), supporting the factorial validity of the translated scales.
V. Germeijs et al. / Contemporary Educational Psychology 37 (2012) 229–239 233

2.2.1. Career decisional tasks 2.2.3. Career choice anxiety


To measure the six career decisional tasks (i.e., orientation, self- To measure the degree of anxiety related to the process of mak-
exploration, broad environmental exploration, in-depth environ- ing a choice of a major in higher education, the Career Choice Anx-
mental exploration, decisional status, and commitment) that char- iety scale of the Career Factors Inventory (CFI; Chartrand, Robbins,
acterize adolescents’ career decision-making profiles, six subscales Morril, & Boggs, 1990) was used. The six items were adapted to the
of the Study Choice Task Inventory (SCTI; Germeijs & Verschueren, situation of choosing a major in higher education (e.g., ‘‘When I
2006) were used. The items refer to attitudes (i.e., orientation and think about actually deciding for sure what I want to study, I feel
commitment), as well as to behaviors (i.e., exploratory behavior) (1) frightened, (5) fearless’’). Evidence was offered for the test–retest
that address the tasks. The subscale Orientation to Choice probes reliability, and the convergent and divergent validity of the Career
students’ awareness of the need to make a decision and their moti- Choice Anxiety scale (Chartrand et al., 1990; Vidal-Brown &
vation to engage in the career decision process (e.g., ‘‘I am moti- Thompson, 2001). In the current study, the internal consistency
vated to tackle choosing a major at the moment’’). A 9-point of this scale was .91 at the end of Grade 12.
scale (going from does not describe me to describes me well) is used
as a response scale. Three subscales of the SCTI assess exploratory
behavior: the Self-Exploratory Behavior scale (e.g., ‘‘I have talked 2.2.4. Rational decision-making style
with my friends about my interests’’), the Broad Exploratory The Rational Decision-Making Style Scale (DMS-R) of the
Behavior scale (e.g., ‘‘I glanced through general summaries about Assessment of Career Decision Making (Buck & Daniels, 1985)
the structure of higher education’’), and the In-depth Exploratory was used. The DMS-R contains 10 items (e.g., ‘‘I am very systematic
Behavior Scale (e.g., ‘‘I have talked to a teacher to get more infor- when I go about making an important decision’’). Participants have
mation about these studies’’). For these scales three response cate- to indicate whether each statement is (0) false or mostly false or (1)
gories (i.e., never, sometimes, often) are used to indicate the true or mostly true. Buck and Daniels (1985) described several stud-
frequency of exploratory behavior during the last and current ies that provided evidence for the test–retest reliability, internal
school year. Before probing In-depth Exploratory Behavior, we consistency and validity of the scale. In the current study, the
asked students to name the majors about which they had collected Kuder–Richardson 20 index was .81 at the end of Grade 12.
information. Students who had not looked up information about
any study did not have to fill out the In-depth Exploratory Behavior
Scale. 2.2.5. Commitment to major
The subscale Decisional Status consists of two questions (i.e., To probe the degree of commitment to the chosen major the
‘‘list all studies you are considering now’’ and ‘‘which study is your Commitment scale of the GIDS (Bosma, 1985) was used. In addition
first choice (if undecided, write ‘undecided’)’’). Numerical values to the eight items of the GIDS, which were also used in the current
are given to four possibilities in responding: (1) no first choice, study to probe the degree of commitment before choice implemen-
no alternatives, (2) alternatives without first choice, (3) first choice tation at the end of Grade 12 (see Commitment scale of SCTI), eight
with alternatives, and (4) first choice with no alternatives. The additional items of the GIDS (e.g., ‘‘Do you feel involved in this
Commitment scale of the SCTI (e.g., ‘‘Are you certain about this study?’’) were used to assess the degree of commitment to the ma-
study?’’) is based on the Commitment scale of the Groningen Iden- jor during choice implementation. Bosma (1992) found support for
tity Development Scale (GIDS; Bosma, 1985). Answers on the Com- the validity of the Commitment scale of the GIDS when investigat-
mitment scale are given on a 6-point scale ranging from (1) not at ing differences between groups that were expected to differ in
all to (6) yes, very. Only students who indicated having a first terms of strengths of commitments in several identity areas. In
choice in the Decisional Status scale (i.e., score 3 or 4) were asked the current study, Cronbach’s alpha for this scale was .92 at the
to rate the degree of commitment to their choice. beginning of the second year in higher education.
Germeijs and Verschueren (2006) provided evidence for the
reliability and validity of the SCTI. Evidence for the construct valid-
ity of the SCTI was obtained through confirmatory factor analyses 2.2.6. Academic and social adjustment
supporting its hypothesized multidimensional structure. Evidence To probe academic and social adjustment in higher education a
for the convergent validity was offered through connections in the Dutch translation (Beyers & Goossens, 2002) of the Academic
expected direction of the SCTI-subscales with existing measures of Adjustment scale and the Social Adjustment scale of the Student
career exploration and career decision-making difficulties. Con- Adaptation to College Questionnaire (SACQ; Baker & Syrik, 1989)
struct validity evidence was also provided by two known-groups was used. The Academic Adjustment Scale consists of 24 items
validation analyses. In the current study Cronbach’s alpha indices (e.g., ‘‘I have been keeping up to date on my academic work’’),
at the end of Grade 12 were .89 (Orientation to Choice, 12 items), referring to the educational demands of the experience in higher
.89 (Self-Exploratory Behavior, 20 items), .80 (Broad Exploratory education. The items probe students’ attitudes towards academic
Behavior, 5 items), .76 (In-depth Exploratory Behavior, 10 items), goals and academic work, how well they apply themselves to aca-
and .83 (Commitment, 8 items). demic work, and the effectiveness of their academic efforts. The So-
cial Adjustment Scale consists of 20 items (e.g., ‘‘I am quite
2.2.2. Career decision-making self-efficacy satisfied with my social life at college’’) that refer to the interper-
This construct was measured by the short form of the Career sonal demands characteristic of higher education. The items assess
Decision Self-Efficacy Scale (CDSE-SF; Betz, Klein, & Taylor, 1996). students’ involvement and relationships with other people at the
The CDSE-SF consists of 25 items assessing an individual’s degree campus, their satisfaction with the social aspects of higher educa-
of confidence in completing tasks important in career decision- tion, and the way they deal with being away from home. The items
making (e.g., ‘‘How much confidence do you have that you could are scored on a 5-point scale ranging from (1) doesn’t apply to me at
find information in the library about occupations you are inter- all to (5) applies very closely to me. Beyers and Goossens (2002) pro-
ested in?’’). Items are answered on a 5-point scale ranging from vided support for the validity of the Dutch translation of the SACQ
(1) no confidence at all to (5) complete confidence. Betz et al. in a sample of Belgian university students. In the current study,
(1996) provided evidence for the reliability and validity of the Cronbach’s alpha for the Academic Adjustment scale and the Social
CDSE-SF. In the current study, the internal consistency of this scale Adjustment scale was .89 and .88, respectively, at the beginning of
was .93 at the end of Grade 12. the second year in higher education.
234 V. Germeijs et al. / Contemporary Educational Psychology 37 (2012) 229–239

2.3. Plan of analysis 3. Results

To identify the decision-making profiles based on the six career 3.1. Educational decision-making profiles
decisional tasks, we used latent class cluster analysis. From the
sample of 665 students who participated at the end of Grade 12, Latent class cluster analysis on scores for the six career deci-
a subgroup of 93 students had missing scores on one or more of sional tasks yielded a four latent clusters solution as the most
the SCTI scales and could therefore not be included in the cluster appropriate. First of all, the BIC of the four latent cluster model
analysis. Most of these students had missing values on the In- was the lowest. Next, conditional bootstrap based upon the log
depth Exploratory Behavior Scale (n = 19) or on the Commitment likelihood showed that the four latent cluster solution outper-
Scale (n = 67) of the SCTI. They did not fill out these scales because formed the three latent cluster solution (p < .0001). Although the
they had not collected information on any major or they had no five latent cluster solution showed a higher BIC, indicating a dete-
first choice (see Measures). In sum, the latent class cluster analysis rioration of fit, the conditional bootstrap based upon Log Likeli-
was performed on a sample of 572 students. Because the group of hood bootstrap indicated that the five clusters would be a slight
93 students did not have a first choice and/or did not collect de- improvement compared to the four latent cluster solution
tailed information about any major at the end of Grade 12, it is ex- (p < .05). Inspection of the bivariate residuals (BVRs) revealed,
pected that these students resemble most the students in the however, that the five latent cluster solution did not represent an
diffusion cluster on the validation variables and on indices of improvement with regard to explaining away the associations
choice implementation. among the indicators.
Latent class cluster analysis is a statistical method which classi- The final four-cluster solution is presented in Fig. 1. In Fig. 1, the
fies respondents into mutually exclusive groups with respect to a clusters are defined using Z scores based on the total sample. The
latent (i.e., not directly observed) variable (Goodman, 1974a, distance between the cluster means and the total sample standard-
1974b; McCutcheon, 1987; Notelaers, Einarsen, De Witte, & ized mean, expressed in SD, can be interpreted as an effect size
Vermunt, 2006). Magidson and Vermunt (2001) refer to such mod- (Scholte, van Lieshout, de Wit, & van Aken, 2005). Analogous to
els as latent class cluster models because the number of nominal Cohen’s d (1988), 0.2SD is a small effect, 0.5SD is a medium or mod-
categories of the latent variable serves the same function as the erate effect, and 0.8SD is a large effect. Cluster 1 (n = 118) consisted
number of clusters desired in cluster analysis. An important differ- of adolescents scoring moderately high to high on decisional status
ence from traditional cluster methods is that latent class cluster and commitment and very high on environmental exploration (i.e.,
analysis is a model-based approach. Hence, the appropriateness broad and in-depth), self-exploration and orientation relative to
of the model can be statistically tested (Magidson & Vermunt, the sample mean. In accordance with Marcia’s (1966) definition
2002). As a consequence, determining the number of latent classes of the achievement status, Cluster 1 was labeled as the achieve-
is less arbitrary than when using traditional cluster methods. ment cluster. Cluster 2 (n = 183) scored relatively high on deci-
Another difference is that indicators of different measurement lev- sional status and commitment, but intermediate on the three
els can be included into the model as it has been implemented in exploration tasks. Adolescents in this cluster scored moderately
different statistical software packages like Latent Gold 4.5 high on orientation. This cluster resembled most the achievement
(Vermunt & Magidson, 2009). This versatility in handling different cluster in terms of decisional status and commitment. However,
measurement levels is very convenient in the current study, students in Cluster 2 had intermediate scores instead of high scores
because our indicator ‘decisional status’ cannot be conceived as a on the exploration tasks and scored lower on orientation than stu-
continuous measurement as it has only two response categories dents in the achievement cluster. In sum, students in Cluster 2 are
(i.e., 3 or 4). decided and did gather some information, but their exploration
To determine how many clusters are needed to explain the was substantially less intense compared to students in the achieve-
associations among the indicators, we used the Bayesian Criterion ment cluster. Based on Marcia’s model (1980) and on the corre-
Information (BIC) (Magidson & Vermunt, 2004; McCutcheon, spondence with the empirically identified foreclosure status in
1987). In addition, we assessed the improvement of a model by a identity research (e.g., Luyckx, Vansteenkiste, Duriez, & Goossens,
conditional bootstrap where the model with T clusters according 2009), this cluster was labeled as the foreclosure cluster. Cluster
to BIC can be compared to a model with T 1 and a model with 3 (n = 91) was labeled as the moratorium cluster because it con-
T + 1 clusters (Vermunt & Magidson, 2005). We also inspected sisted of adolescents who scored moderately low to very low on
the bivariate residuals (BVR), because the latent variable, as in a decisional status and commitment relative to the sample mean
traditional measurement model, explains the associations among and moderately high to high on the three exploration tasks. Cluster
the indicators. Furthermore, similar to evaluations of measurement 4 (n = 180), consisted of adolescents scoring relatively low to very
models, the ratio of v2 divided by the degrees of freedom should be low on all decisional tasks. Only for decisional status, this cluster
smaller than 3 (Magidson & Vermunt, 2004). However, the sensi- scored intermediate. Based on Marcia’s model (1980) and in line
tivity to sample size has led to a more practical threshold when with personal identity research (e.g., Luyckx et al., 2005), Cluster
this ratio is higher than 3, that is, the reduction in BVR should be 4 was labeled as the diffusion cluster.
at least 85% (Notelaers et al., 2006). This indicates that most of Chi-square analysis indicated a significant relationship between
the initial associations among the indicators is explained by the gender and classification in the four clusters (v2 (3) = 33.65,
T cluster solution. In this study, we used that threshold. p < .01). The standardized residuals showed that in the diffusion
To investigate the external validity of the clusters, we tested cluster more boys were observed than expected, and in the
whether the clusters differed on career choice anxiety, career deci- achievement cluster fewer boys were observed than expected. Be-
sion-making self-efficacy, and rational decision-making style cause of this gender effect, we controlled for gender in the remain-
through a MANCOVA. In addition, the consequences of the career der of the analyses.
decision-making profiles for choice implementation were examined
through a MANCOVA with commitment to the chosen major, social
adjustment, and academic adjustment as dependent variables. In 3.2. External validation: concurrent associations
both MANCOVAs, we controlled for gender and students’ grades
(i.e., students’ average percentage in Grade 12, collected from the A MANCOVA on the three person variables (i.e., career
school record data) by including them as independent variables. choice anxiety, career decision-making self-efficacy, and rational
V. Germeijs et al. / Contemporary Educational Psychology 37 (2012) 229–239 235

Fig. 1. Z-scores for career decisional tasks in the 4-cluster solution.

Table 1
External validation: mean scores on person variables and choice implementation as a function of the four extracted clusters.

Variable Clusters F1 g2
Achievement Foreclosure Moratorium Diffused
Person variables
Career choice anxiety 2.16a 2.14a 2.78b 2.55b 9.12* .09
Career decision-making self-efficacy 3.94a 3.73b 3.48c 3.38c 17.48* .15
Rational decision-making style 0.82a 0.67b 0.70b 0.56c 9.56* .09
Choice implementation
Academic adjustment 3.92a 3.85a 3.64b 3.48b 8.64* .09
Social adjustment 4.05a 3.95b 3.75bc 3.62c 9.38* .09
Commitment 5.18a 5.08a 4.78b 4.66b 10.17* .10

Note: Means in the same row that do not share subscripts differ at p < .05 on post hoc contrasts.
1
F (3, 281) for analyses on person variables, F (3, 253) for analyses on choice implementation.
*
p < .01.

decision-making style) with educational decision-making profile, differences (F (9, 611) = 4.60, p < .01, g2 = .05). Subsequent univari-
gender, and grades at the end of Grade 12 as independent variables ate ANOVAs revealed significant cluster differences on the three
yielded a significant main effect of the educational decision-making indices for choice implementation (see Table 1). The effect sizes
clusters (F (9, 679.16) = 9.11, p < .01, g2 = .09). The main effects of were medium (Cohen, 1988). As expected, students in the achieve-
the control variables gender and grades were not significant (F ment cluster exhibited the most adaptive pattern during the
(3, 279) = .71, p = .55, g2 = .01 and F (3, 279) = 1.58, p = .19, g2 = .02, second year in higher education. The achievement cluster scored
respectively). Subsequent univariate ANOVAs revealed significant higher on academic adjustment, social adjustment, and commit-
cluster differences on each person variable (see Table 1). The effect ment than the moratorium and diffusion cluster. In addition,
sizes for all three variables were medium to large (Cohen, 1988). students in the achievement cluster scored higher on social adjust-
As hypothesized, students in the achievement or foreclosure ment than students in the foreclosure cluster. The results also
cluster showed lower career choice anxiety and higher career showed that the two clusters with relatively low scores on
decision-making self-efficacy than students in the moratorium or commitment to their choice (i.e., the moratorium and diffusion
diffusion cluster. Table 1 also indicates that adolescents in the clusters4) did not differ significantly on the three indices of choice
achievement cluster, which is the only cluster characterized by implementation.
relatively high levels on all career decisional tasks, have the most Significant effects of the two control variables on choice imple-
positive beliefs concerning their ability to complete career mentation were obtained (F (3, 251) = 5.10, p < .01, g2 = .06 for
decisional tasks. The results also indicate that the two clusters students’ grades in Grade 12 and F (3, 251) = 6.59, p < .01, g2 = .07
characterized by a relatively low degree of commitment to their for gender). Subsequent univariate ANOVAs revealed a significant
educational choice (i.e., the moratorium and diffusion clusters) effect of grades in Grade 12 on the three indices of choice
did not differ significantly in career choice anxiety or career implementation (F (1, 253) = 14.94, p < .01, g2 = .05 on academic
decision-making self-efficacy. With regard to the rational deci- adjustment; F (1, 253) = 5.64, p < .05, g2 = .02 on social adjustment;
sion-making style, hypotheses were also confirmed. Students in F (1, 253) = 6.35, p < .05, g2 = .02 on commitment). Students with
the achievement and moratorium clusters showed higher levels higher grades at the end of Grade 12 tended to show higher levels
of a rational decision-making style than students in the diffusion of adjustment (i.e., both in the social and academic domain) and
cluster. In addition, the achievement cluster scored higher on more commitment to their major during the second year in higher
rational decision-making style than the foreclosure cluster. Only
the hypothesized difference between the moratorium and foreclo- 4
A small group of adolescents who did not collect any in-depth information on a
sure cluster failed to reach significance. major (n = 19) and/or who did not have a first choice at the end of Grade 12 (n = 67)
could not be included in the cluster analysis because of missing values on the in-
3.3. External validation: implications for choice implementation depth exploratory behavior scale and/or the commitment scale. Additional analyses
showed that this group of adolescents did not differ significantly from the diffusion
cluster on all validation variables and indices of choice implementation. Only for the
With regard to the effect on commitment and adjustment in validation variable career choice anxiety, this group of students scored even higher
higher education, the MANCOVA yielded significant cluster than students from the diffusion cluster.
236 V. Germeijs et al. / Contemporary Educational Psychology 37 (2012) 229–239

education. In addition, significant gender differences were revealed and commitment. In addition, students in the achievement cluster
on social adjustment during the second year in higher education (F showed the most adaptive pattern in terms of career choice anxi-
(1, 253) = 4.86, p < .05, g2 = .02), with girls showing higher scores ety and career decision-making self-efficacy. However, the results
on social adjustment than boys. of this study also show that, in contrast to previous career
decision-making research (Brisbin & Savickas, 1994; Skorikov &
Vondracek, 2007), we were able to distinguish a foreclosed group
4. Discussion
which is differentiated from the achievement group. These results
suggest that school counselors should not only focus on helping
The present study aimed to empirically derive by means of clus-
undecided clients, but that some decided clients (i.e., the foreclo-
ter analysis educational decision-making profiles among adoles-
sure group) may also benefit from interventions that foster
cents who make a choice of a major in higher education. In
self- and environmental exploration.
addition, the external validity of these profiles was examined.
The findings of the present study show that this approach can
4.2. Characterization of the two uncommitted clusters
add to our understanding of differences between adolescents in
their approach to educational decision-making.
Two clusters were identified with moderately low to low levels
Using a differentiated set of six career decisional tasks, four
of commitment: the moratorium cluster and the diffusion cluster.
clusters were retained which paralleled Marcia’s (1966) identity
However, both clusters were differentiated by their degree of
statuses. The bulk of previous identity research focusing on these
exploration and orientation. In accordance with the definitions of
identity statuses relied heavily on theory-based assumptions and
the uncommitted statuses by Marcia (1980), the moratorium clus-
techniques (such as median-split procedures; but see Luyckx
ter showed moderately high levels of self- and environmental
et al., 2005, 2008) to derive identity statuses. Given that the pres-
exploration (i.e., broad and in depth), whereas the diffusion cluster
ent study made use of the data-driven procedure of latent class
showed low levels on these decisional tasks. Important to note is
clustering, we provide important validity evidence for Marcia’s
that the diffusion cluster scored the lowest of all clusters on orien-
model. Moreover, statuses in identity research are usually derived
tation. Apparently, adolescents in this cluster are not aware of the
at an overall level (i.e., across several content domains) or at the le-
fact that they have to make an educational decision by the end of
vel of a broad domain (e.g., referring to identity statuses across a
the school year and are not motivated to engage in this decision-
broad range of career choices). The current study focused on one
making process.
specific identity-relevant decision in the career domain, thereby
In previous research on career decision-making profiles, two
demonstrating that Marcia’s model is also valid for describing
types of undecided clusters emerged frequently, that is a ‘‘develop-
the way individuals deal with a real and specific identity-relevant
mentally undecided’’ and a ‘‘chronically indecisive’’ group (Kelly &
decision or with a micro-process in identity development.
Pulver, 2003). Whereas adolescents in the former cluster were
characterized by low negative affect, adolescents in the latter clus-
4.1. Characterization of the two committed clusters ter tended to show high levels of negative affect (e.g., high levels of
anxiety, low self-esteem, and lack of decision-making confidence).
Two of the four clusters showed moderately high to high levels Taking into account the characterization of the diffusion cluster on
of decisional status and commitment: the achievement cluster and the decisional tasks together with the levels of career choice anxi-
the foreclosure cluster. Therefore, students in these clusters can all ety and career decision-making self-efficacy in this cluster, this
be labeled as more decided regarding their choice of a major and group seems to replicate the ‘‘chronically indecisive’’ type as found
more attached to and confident in this choice than students of in previous career research (Kelly & Pulver, 2003). The moratorium
the other two clusters. However, the two decided clusters were cluster identified in the current study does not seem to correspond
clearly differentiated by their exploratory behavior. In line with with the ‘‘developmentally undecided’’ group which was found in
definitions of the achievement and foreclosure statuses by Marcia previous research. The moratorium cluster was characterized by
(1980), the cluster with the highest levels of self- and environmen- relatively high scores on the three exploration tasks, whereas indi-
tal exploration (i.e., broad and in depth) was labeled as the viduals in the ‘‘developmentally undecided’’ group were not well
achievement cluster, whereas the cluster showing lower levels informed and showed a high need for information. Certain clusters
for these decisional tasks was labeled as the foreclosure cluster. that were identified in some career decision-making studies
These results confirm the idea that commitment to a career (Larson et al., 1988; Rojewski, 1994; Savickas & Jarjoura, 1991),
choice is not necessarily associated with a period of intense explo- such as the ‘‘transitional indecision’’ cluster (i.e., undecided but
ration (Blustein et al., 1989). The findings are also in line with the having started the process of career exploration) or the ‘‘informed
more or less orthogonal relationship between exploration and indecisives’’ (i.e., well informed but still unable to make a decision)
commitment that is hypothesized and demonstrated in the seem to correspond more with the moratorium cluster in the cur-
identity literature. Commitments can either be formed with or rent study.
without a period of intense exploration. Interestingly, in line with Important to note is that in the current study the group of ado-
the degree of exploration, the achievement cluster scored very high lescents on which the analyses were performed had all gathered
on orientation whereas the foreclosure cluster scored only in-depth information about at least one college major and all had
moderately high on this decisional task. Apparently, adolescents a first choice at the end of Grade 12. The group of adolescents
in the foreclosure cluster are less motivated to actively engage in who did not collect any in-depth information on a major and/or
the educational decision-making process than adolescents in the who did not have a first choice at the end of Grade 12 (n = 93)
achievement cluster which probably results in limited engagement was not included in the cluster analysis but turned out to resemble
in exploratory activities. most the diffusion cluster on the validation variables and indices of
When comparing the two decided clusters identified with the choice implementation in additional analyses. In sum, students
career decision-making profiles that emerged in previous studies, who did not collect detailed information about any major and/or
the achievement cluster seems to correspond most with the who did not have a first choice at the end of Grade 12 showed a less
‘‘decided’’ or ‘‘ready to decide’’ type (Kelly & Pulver, 2003). In the adaptive pattern of person characteristics and choice implementa-
current study, adolescents in the achievement cluster showed tion compared to students in the achievement, foreclosure, and
relatively high to very high scores on exploration, decisional status, moratorium clusters.
V. Germeijs et al. / Contemporary Educational Psychology 37 (2012) 229–239 237

4.3. Associations with person variables sufficiently to warrant confidence in their ability to make career
decisions.
The relationships between the identified clusters and the three
person variables provided evidence for the external validity of the 4.4. Consequences for choice implementation
four clusters. First and as expected, scores on the rational decision-
making style were in the same direction as the scores on the explo- The obtained career decision-making profiles showed differen-
ration tasks. The achievement and moratorium clusters scored tial associations with three indices of the quality of choice imple-
higher than the diffusion clusters on this decision-making style. mentation during the second year in higher education. In general,
In addition, the achievement cluster scored higher than the fore- students in the achievement cluster showed the most positive
closure cluster on rational decision-making style. The difference pattern of academic functioning in terms of academic adjustment,
in means between the moratorium and foreclosure cluster on ra- social adjustment, and commitment to the chosen major. Students
tional decision-making was in line with expectations, but failed in the moratorium and diffused clusters showed the least adaptive
to reach significance. Given that the rational decision-making style pattern on choice implementation.
was investigated with regard to making decisions in general, the An important result of the current study is the significant differ-
findings of this study suggest that the high and low career explora- ence between the achievement cluster and foreclosure cluster,
tion groups will also adopt different degrees of exploratory behav- with students in the achievement cluster showing higher scores
ior and planning when making decisions in other life domains than on social adjustment than students in the foreclosure cluster.
the career domain. The differences found between the high and These results are in line with identity research reporting negative
low exploration clusters on rational decision making style are in relationships between identity foreclosure and the formation of
line with research on the relationship between identity statuses mature social relationships (Berzonsky & Kuk, 2000) and showing
and information-processing styles. The degree of using an informa- that identity achievers have more adaptive interactional skills than
tion-oriented style (i.e., active exploration and information seek- the other statuses (Clancy & Dollinger, 1993; Read et al., 1984).
ing) when faced with identity-relevant decisions has been found Foreclosures tend to be less tolerant of others and tend to seek
to be higher for individuals in the achievement and moratorium approval and reassurance from others (Berzonsky & Kuk, 2000).
statuses than for individuals in the foreclosure and diffusion sta- This social behavior may interfere with interpersonal adjustment
tuses (Schwartz, Mullis, Waterman, & Dunham, 2000). in several contexts, including the integration in social life and the
With regard to career choice anxiety and career decision- formation of a support network in higher education.
making self-efficacy the results show that adolescents in the The findings regarding the differential relations between the ca-
achievement and foreclosure clusters displayed a better profile reer decision-making profiles and choice implementation in higher
than those in the moratorium and diffusion clusters. These findings education are in line with the idea that the way individuals arrive
are in line with prior research on career decision-making showing at a career decision may have important consequences for the
that career choice anxiety was negatively and career decision- quality of the implementation of the decision (e.g., Gati & Asher,
making self-efficacy was positively associated with career choice 2001; Van Esbroeck et al., 2005). The finding that the achievement
certainty and decidedness (e.g., Betz, 2001; Vidal-Brown & cluster showed the most adaptive pattern of choice implementa-
Thompson, 2001). The findings are also in line with the well- tion may be due to its combination of relatively high levels of
established results in the identity literature showing that adoles- involvement on all career decisional tasks. Hence, the results offer
cents in the foreclosure and especially achievement status showed important empirical support for theoretical models on career
the highest self-esteem (e.g., Marcia & Friedman, 1970; Read, decision-making (e.g., Harren, 1979; Tiedeman & O’Hara, 1963;
Adams, & Dobson, 1984) and adolescents in the diffusion and Van Esbroeck et al., 2005) in which these different decisional tasks
especially moratorium status displayed the most anxiety and are distinguished and hypothesized to be all important for the
depression (e.g., Marcia, 1967). quality of a person’s career decision-making process.
Hence, although it has been suggested that having no stable or
firmly established identity commitments might constitute an 4.5. Limitations and directions for future research
adaptive response in Western late-modern societies (e.g., Gergen,
1991), most research to date indicated that scoring high on identity The present study has a number of limitations, which may pro-
commitment was generally associated with optimal psychosocial vide suggestions for future research. First, this study only focused
functioning in Western contemporary societies (e.g., Luyckx on choosing a major in higher education. Future studies could give
et al., 2008; Schwartz et al., 2011). an indication about the generalizability of the findings to other
Due to the characteristics of the Belgian educational system, the educational or career decisions (e.g., choosing a job). A second lim-
end of Grade 12 is the moment at which students are expected to itation is the specificity of our sample. Because cluster analysis is a
have made a choice of a major in higher education. As a result, data-driven procedure, the nature of the sample is important and
students who are still uncertain about their major choice at that limits the generalizations that can be made. In the current study
moment may feel stressed and may develop negative beliefs all students were White and followed general education in high
regarding their ability to make this educational choice. However, school. In addition, the educational decision-making profiles were
given that the career decisional tasks as well as career choice anx- studied in the Belgian educational context which expects adoles-
iety and career decision-making self-efficacy were measured at the cents to make a choice of a major by the end of high school.
same moment, the findings may also indicate that anxiety and low Because the specific cultural–educational context may provide an
self-efficacy beliefs may take their toll in the failure to decide and incentive for working on the educational decisional tasks and for
to be certain and confident about an important educational choice. working on the broader developmental task of preparing for a
The results also showed that the foreclosure cluster scored career (Grotevant, 1987; Kalakoski & Nurmi, 1998), differences
lower than the achievement cluster on career decision-making among educational systems and related expectations can affect
self-efficacy. Although unexpected, similar results were reported the timing and the way of making educational decisions. For exam-
recently by Nauta and Kahn (2007), who found a negative ple, it may be that in other countries where choosing a major is less
association between the foreclosure identity status and career urgent at the age of 18 (e.g., North-American students start in
decision-making self-efficacy. These authors suggest that colleges and choose a major later on) the levels of the decisional
foreclosed individuals may recognize that they did not explore tasks are lower than in the current study. Therefore, it might be
238 V. Germeijs et al. / Contemporary Educational Psychology 37 (2012) 229–239

interesting for future cross-national studies to clarify the role of Buck, J. N., & Daniels, M. H. (1985). Assessment of career decision making: Manual. Los
Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
the educational system on educational decision-making profiles.
Callanan, G. A., & Greenhaus, J. H. (1992). The career indecision of managers and
Third, not all students from the original sample could be included professionals: An examination of multiple subtypes. Journal of Vocational
in the validation analyses because they did not complete the home Behavior, 41, 212–231.
questionnaire or did not participate at the second wave. For exam- Chartrand, J. M., Martin, W. F., Robbins, S. B., McAuliffe, G. J., Pickering, J. W., &
Calliotte, J. A. (1994). Testing a level versus an interactional view of career
ple, although parts of the questionnaire used in the second wave indecision. Journal of Career Assessment, 2, 55–69.
could also be completed by students who did not enter higher edu- Chartrand, J. M., Robbins, S. B., Morril, W. H., & Boggs, K. (1990). Development and
cation, it may be that especially this group of students decided to validation of the career factors inventory. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 37,
491–501.
participate no longer in the second wave because the focus of the Clancy, S. M., & Dollinger, S. J. (1993). Identity, self, and personality: I. Identity
study (i.e., choosing a major) was not relevant anymore to them. status and the five-factor model of personality. Journal of Research on
This attrition may affect the generalizability of our findings. How- Adolescence, 3, 227–245.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ:
ever, additional analyses indicate that the effect of attrition on the Erlbaum.
variables of interest was rather limited. Finally, given that the Coppieters, P., Creten, H., Glorieux, I., Lancksweerdt, P., Laurijssen, I., Leysen, V.,
career decisional tasks and the person variables were measured et al. (2002). Hoe maken Vlaamse jongeren de overgang van school naar werk?
[How do Flemish adolescents make the transition from school to work?].
at the same moment, no inferences can be drawn regarding the Eindrapport PB0’97 (Sonar). Leuven, Belgium: Steunpunt WAV.
directionality in relationships between the career decision-making DiStefano, C., & Kamphaus, R. W. (2006). Investigating subtypes of child
profiles and decision-making style, career choice anxiety, and development. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 66, 778–794.
Flum, H., & Blustein, D. L. (2000). Reinvigorating the study of vocational exploration:
career decision-making self-efficacy.
A framework for research. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 56, 380–440.
Gall, T. L., Evans, D. R., & Bellerose, S. (2000). Transition to first-year University:
4.6. Conclusion Patterns of change in adjustment across life domains and time. Journal of Social
and Clinical Psychology, 19, 544–567.
Gati, I., & Asher, I. (2001). The PIC model for career decision making: Prescreening,
The findings of the present study provide valuable information in-depth exploration, and choice. In T. L. Leong & A. Barak (Eds.), Contemporary
on differences between adolescents who are faced with an models in vocational psychology: A volume in honor of Samuel H. Osipow
(pp. 6–54). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
educational decision. Given that previous research on career deci-
Gerdes, H., & Mallinckrodt, B. (1994). Emotional, social, and academic adjustment of
sion-making profiles mostly focused on career decision-making at college students: A longitudinal study of retention. Journal of Counseling and
a general level, the current study emphasized the importance of Development, 72, 281–288.
distinguishing profiles in specific micro-processes during career Gergen, K. J. (1991). The saturated self: Dilemnas of identity in contemporary life. New
York: Basic Books.
development. To our knowledge, this study is also the first that Germeijs, V., & Verschueren, K. (2006). High school students’ career decision-
managed to differentiate between an achieved and foreclosed pro- making process: Development and validation of the study choice task
file in the career domain and to show that both decided profiles inventory. Journal of Career Assessment, 14, 449–471.
Goodman, L. A. (1974a). The analysis of quantitative variables when some of the
have different associations with person variables and the quality variables are unobserved: Part I – A modified latent structure approach.
of choice implementation. In line with the experience of career American Journal of Sociology, 79, 1179–1259.
counselors that different subtypes of adolescents who have to Goodman, L. A. (1974b). Exploratory latent structure models using both identifiable
and unidentifiable models. Biometrika, 61, 215–231.
make an educational choice may exist, the results of the current Goossens, L. (2001). Global versus domain-specific statuses in identity research: A
study do not only suggest that the differentiation between decided comparison of two self-report measures. Journal of Adolescence, 24, 681–699.
and undecided students is a relevant one, but that the type of inde- Gore, P. A. (2000). Cluster analysis. In H. E. A. Tinsley & S. D. Brown (Eds.), Handbook
of applied multivariate statistics and mathematical modeling (pp. 297–321). San
cision and of decidedness is important as well.
Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Grotevant, H. D. (1987). Toward a process model of identity formation. Journal of
References Adolescent Research, 2, 203–222.
Harren, V. A. (1979). A model of career decision-making for college students. Journal
of Vocational Behavior, 14, 119–133.
Baker, R. W., & Syrik, B. (1989). Student adaptation to college questionnaire (SACQ):
Jordaan, J. P. (1963). Exploratory behavior: The formation of self and occupational
Manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
concepts. In Career development: Self-concept theory. Essays in vocational
Bentler, P. M., & Chou, C. (1987). Practical issues in structural modeling. Sociological
development (pp. 42–78). Princeton, NJ: College Entrance Examination Board.
Methods and Research, 16, 78–117.
Kalakoski, V., & Nurmi, J. E. (1998). Identity and educational transitions: Age
Berzonsky, M. D., & Kuk, L. S. (2000). Identity status, identity processing style, and
differences in adolescent exploration and commitment related to education,
the transition to university. Journal of Adolescent Research, 15, 81–98.
occupation, and family. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 8, 29–47.
Betz, N. E. (2001). Career self-efficacy. In T. L. Leong & A. Barak (Eds.), Contemporary
Kelly, K. R., & Pulver, C. A. (2003). Refining measurement of career indecision types:
models in vocational psychology: A volume in honor of Samuel H. Osipow
A validity study. Journal of Counseling and Development, 81, 445–454.
(pp. 55–78). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kroger, J. (2003). Identity development during adolescence. In G. R. Adams & M. D.
Betz, N. E., Klein, K. L., & Taylor, K. M. (1996). Evaluation of a short form of the career
Berzonsky (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of adolescence (pp. 205–226). Malden, MA:
decision making self-efficacy scale. Journal of Career Assessment, 4, 47–57.
Blackwell.
Beyers, W., & Goossens, L. (2002). Concurrent and predictive validity of the Student
Kroger, J., Martinussen, M., & Marcia, J. E. (2010). Identity status change during
Adaptation to College Questionnaire in a sample of European freshman
adolescence and young adulthood: A meta-analysis. Journal of Adolescence, 33,
students. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 62, 527–538.
683–698.
Blustein, D. L., Devenis, L. E., & Kidney, B. A. (1989). Relationships between the
Larson, L. M., Heppner, P. P., Ham, T., & Dugan, K. (1988). Investigating multiple
identity formation process and career development. Journal of Counseling
subtypes of career indecision through cluster analysis. Journal of Counseling
Psychology, 36, 196–202.
Psychology, 35, 439–446.
Blustein, D. L., Ellis, M. V., & Devenis, L. E. (1989). The development and validation of
Lent, R. W., Brown, S. D., & Hackett, G. (1994). Toward a unifying social cognitive
a two-dimensional model of the commitment to career choices process. Journal
theory of career and academic interest, choice, and performance. Journal of
of Vocational Behavior, 35, 342–378.
Vocational Behavior, 45, 79–122.
Blustein, D. L., & Phillips, S. D. (1990). Relation between ego identity statuses and
Lucas, M. S. (1993). A validation of career indecision at a counseling center. Journal
decision-making styles. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 37, 160–168.
of Counseling Psychology, 40, 440–446.
Bosma, H. A. (1985). Identity development in adolescence: Coping with commitments.
Lucas, M. S., & Epperson, D. L. (1990). Types of vocational undecidedness: A
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Groningen, The Netherlands.
replication and refinement. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 37, 382–388.
Bosma, H. A. (1992). Identity in adolescence: Managing commitments. In G. R.
Luyckx, K., Goossens, L., & Soenens, B. (2006). A developmental-contextual
Adams, T. P. Gullotta, & R. Montemayor (Eds.), Adolescent identity formation
perspective on identity construction in emerging adulthood: Change
(pp. 91–121). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
dynamics in commitment formation and commitment evaluation.
Bosma, H. A., & Kunnen, S. E. (2001). Determinants and mechanisms in ego identity
Developmental Psychology, 42, 366–380.
development: A review and synthesis. Developmental Review, 21, 39–66.
Luyckx, K., Goossens, L., Soenens, B., Beyers, W., & Vansteenkiste, M. (2005). Identity
Brisbin, L. A., & Savickas, M. L. (1994). Career indecision scales do not measure
statuses based upon four rather than two identity dimensions: Extending and
foreclosure. Journal of Career Assessment, 2, 352–363.
refining Marcia’s paradigm. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 34, 605–618.
Brown, S. D., & Rector, C. C. (2008). Conceptualizing and diagnosing problems in
Luyckx, K., Schwartz, S. J., Berzonsky, M. D., Soenens, B., Vansteenkiste, M., Smits, I.,
vocational decision making. In S. D. Brown & R. W. Lent (Eds.), Handbook of
et al. (2008). Capturing ruminative exploration: Extending the four-
counseling psychology (pp. 392–407). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
V. Germeijs et al. / Contemporary Educational Psychology 37 (2012) 229–239 239

dimensional model of identity formation in late adolescence. Journal of Research Savickas, M. L. (1985). Identity in vocational development. Journal of Vocational
in Personality, 42, 58–82. Development, 27, 329–337.
Luyckx, K., Vansteenkiste, M., Duriez, B., & Goossens, L. (2009). Basic need Savickas, M. L., & Jarjoura, D. (1991). The career decision scale as a type indicator.
satisfaction and identity formation: Bridging self-determination theory and Journal of Counseling Psychology, 38, 85–90.
process-oriented identity research. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 56, Scholte, R. H. J., van Lieshout, C. F. M., de Wit, C. A. M., & van Aken, M. A. G. (2005).
276–288. Adolescent personality types and subtypes and their psychosocial adjustment.
Magidson, J., & Vermunt, J. K. (2001). Latent class factor and cluster models, bi-plots Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 51, 258–286.
and related graphical displays. Sociological Methodology, 31, 223–264. Schwartz, S., Beyers, W., Luyckx, K., Soenens, B., Zamboanga, B., Forthun, L., et al.
Magidson, J., & Vermunt, J. K. (2002). Latent class modelling as a probabilistic (2011). Examining the light and ark sides of emerging adults’ identity: A study
extension of K-means clustering. Quirk’s Marketing Research Review, 3(20), of identity status differences in positive and negative psychosocial functioning.
77–80. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40, 839–859.
Magidson, J., & Vermunt, J. K. (2004). Latent class models. In D. Kaplan (Ed.), The Schwartz, S. J., Mullis, R. L., Waterman, A. S., & Dunham, R. M. (2000). Ego identity
Sage handbook for quantitative methodology (pp. 175–198). Thousand Oake, CA: status, identity style, and personal expressiveness: An empirical investigation of
Sage. three convergent constructs. Journal of Adolescent Research, 15, 504–521.
Marcia, J. E. (1966). Development and validation of ego-identity status. Journal of Skorikov, V. B., & Vondracek, F. W. (1998). Vocational identity development: Its
Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 551–558. relationship to other identity domains and to overall identity development.
Marcia, J. E. (1967). Ego identity status: Relationship to change in self-esteem, Journal of Career Assessment, 6, 13–35.
‘‘general adjustment’’, and authoritarianism. Journal of Personality, 35, 119–133. Skorikov, V. B., & Vondracek, F. W. (2007). Vocational identity. In V. B. Skorikov & W.
Marcia, J. E. (1980). Identity in adolescence. In J. Adelson (Ed.), Handbook of Patton (Eds.), Career development in childhood and adolescence (pp. 143–168).
adolescent psychology (pp. 159–187). New York: Wiley. Rotterdam, The Netherlands: Sense.
Marcia, J. E. (1993). The status of the statuses: Research review. In J. E. Marcia, A. S. Skorikov, V. B., & Vondracek, F. W. (2011). Occupational identity. In S. J. Schwartz, K.
Waterman, D. R. Matteson, S. L. Archer, & J. L. Orlofsky (Eds.), Identity: A Luyckx, & L. Vignoles (Eds.), Handbook of identity theory and research
handbook for psychosocial research (pp. 22–41). New York: Springer. (pp. 693–714). New York: Springer.
Marcia, J. E., & Friedman, M. L. (1970). Ego identity status in college women. Journal Super, D. E. (1980). A life-span, life-space approach to career development. Journal
of Personality, 38, 249–263. of Vocational Behavior, 16, 282–298.
Marsh, H. W., & O’Neill, R. (1984). Self description questionnaire III: The construct Tiedeman, D. V., & O’Hara, R. P. (1963). Career development: Choice and adjustment.
validity of multidimensional self-concept ratings by late adolescents. Journal of New York: College Entrance Examination Board.
Educational Measurement, 21, 153–174. Van Esbroeck, R., Tibos, K., & Zaman, M. (2005). A dynamic model of career choice
McCutcheon, A. L. (1987). Latent class analysis. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. development. International Journal for Educational and Vocational Guidance, 5,
Nauta, M. N., & Kahn, J. H. (2007). Identity status, consistency and differentiation of 5–18.
interests, and career decision-making self-efficacy. Journal of Career Assessment, Vermunt, J. K., & Magidson, J. (2005). Technical guide for Latent Gold 4.0: Basic and
15, 55–65. advanced. Belmont, CA: Statistical Innovations.
Notelaers, G., Einarsen, S., De Witte, H., & Vermunt, J. K. (2006). Measuring exposure Vermunt, J. K., & Magidson, J. (2009). Latent Gold (Version 4.5). Belmont, CA:
to bullying at work: The validity and advantages of the latent class cluster Statistical Innovations.
approach. Work and Stress, 20, 288–301. Vidal-Brown, S. A., & Thompson, B. (2001). The career assessment diagnostic
Osipow, S. H. (1987). Career decision scale manual (Rev. ed.). Odessa, FL: inventory: A new career indecision assessment tool. Journal of Career
Psychological Assessment Resources. Assessment, 9, 185–202.
Porfeli, E. J., & Skorikov, V. B. (2010). Specific and diverse career exploration during Vondracek, F. W., Schulenberg, J., Skorikov, V., Gillespie, L. K., & Wahlheim, C.
late adolescence. Journal of Career Assessment, 18, 46–58. (1995). The relationship of identity status to career indecision during
Read, D., Adams, G. R., & Dobson, W. R. (1984). Ego identity status, personality, and adolescence. Journal of Adolescence, 18, 17–29.
social influence style. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 169–177. Wanberg, C. R., & Muchinsky, P. M. (1992). A typology of career decision status:
Robitschek, C., & Cook, S. W. (1999). The influence of personal growth initiative and Validity extension of the vocational decision status model. Journal of Counseling
coping styles on career exploration and vocational identity. Journal of Vocational Psychology, 39, 71–80.
Behavior, 54, 127–141. Waterman, C. K., & Waterman, A. S. (1974). Ego identity status and decision styles.
Rojewski, J. W. (1994). Career indecision types for rural adolescents from Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 3, 1–6.
disadvantaged and nondisadvantaged backgrounds. Journal of Counseling
Psychology, 41, 356–363.

Você também pode gostar