Você está na página 1de 7

Available online at www.sciencedirect.

com

Journal of Consumer Psychology 22 (2012) 558 – 564

Research Report
When promoting a charity can hurt charitable giving:
A metacognitive analysis☆
Robert W. Smith a,⁎, Norbert Schwarz a, b, c

a
Ross School of Business, University of Michigan, USA
b
Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan, USA
c
Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, USA

Received 9 April 2011; received in revised form 4 January 2012; accepted 6 January 2012
Available online 23 February 2012

Abstract

Charities need to come to mind to enter a potential donor's consideration set. However, feeling familiar with a charity and its cause can facilitate
or impair giving. In most cases, perceived good memory for details of the cause fosters the impression of personal importance, which increases
giving (Studies 1 and 3). But when the charity aims to increase awareness of a cause, good memory for the cause suggests that awareness is already
high, which impairs giving (Studies 2 and 3). Hence, promotions for awareness-raising charities can actually have negative consequences, confirm-
ing the predictions of a metacognitive analysis.
© 2012 Society for Consumer Psychology. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Perceived memory; Charitable giving; Metacognitive inference; Lay theories; Promotion; Subjective knowledge

Introduction have deleterious consequences. The present research tests this


possibility. It asks under which conditions it helps or hurts a
Common wisdom holds that charities need extensive promo- charity when donors feel that they are well familiar with its
tional efforts to foster awareness and receive donations. Text- cause.
books emphasize the importance of promotion for nonprofits A metacognitive analysis suggests that being familiar with a
and advocate strategies designed to increase awareness and charity and its cause can convey different things to a potential
knowledge (Kotler & Andreasen, 2000). Scholarly articles donor. On one hand, the potential donor may conclude that
echo this belief with statements like, “To succeed in such a cli- the charity is very important to her or else she would not be
mate, a charity must rely on an effective promotional strategy” familiar with details of its cause and work; on the other hand,
(Bendapudi, Singh, & Bendapudi, 1996, p. 33). Analysts value she may conclude from her own familiarity that the charity is
nonprofit brands based in part on media exposure (Cone, 2010). already so well known that her donation will make little differ-
Indeed, charities require promotion to come to mind and to ence. Conversely, being unfamiliar with the charity may con-
enter the consideration set of potential donors. Accordingly, a vey that one never cared about this cause or, alternatively,
key goal of many charities is to achieve high visibility. Unfor- that the charity and its cause are not yet widely known and
tunately, some marketing tactics can backfire for charities therefore in particular need of support. Which of these consid-
(Krishna, 2011) and high visibility and awareness may also erations is likely to dominate may depend on the charity's
goals. Charities that emphasize awareness-raising may be par-

We thank Katherine Burson, Jesse Chandler, and the members of the Social ticularly likely to suffer from the perception that their cause is
Cognition Lab at the University of Michigan for helpful discussions. already highly familiar, as reflected in one's own good knowl-
⁎ Corresponding author at: Department of Marketing, Stephen M. Ross School of
Business, University of Michigan, 701 Tappan Street, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA.
edge about it. In contrast, charities that pursue other goals—
E-mail addresses: robwill@umich.edu (R.W. Smith), such as helping people in need—may benefit from the percep-
nschwarz@umich.edu (N. Schwarz). tion that one knows their cause well.
1057-7408/$ -see front matter © 2012 Society for Consumer Psychology. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jcps.2012.01.001
R.W. Smith, N. Schwarz / Journal of Consumer Psychology 22 (2012) 558–564 559

Drawing on metacognitive theorizing, we test these conjec- processes they bring to bear (Schwarz, 2004a, 2010; Schwarz,
tures in three experiments. To manipulate potential donors' per- Song, & Xu, 2009). The most widely studied lay theory of
ception of how much they know about a charity's cause we memory is at the heart of Tversky and Kahneman's (1973) avail-
present them with memory tests that are easy or difficult to ability heuristic. It correctly holds that it is easier to recall exam-
complete, leaving them with the impression that they remember ples when many rather than few exist in the world; hence people
quite a lot or very little about the respective charity. Confirming infer frequency and typicality from ease of recall (Schwarz et al.,
what most charity officers seem to assume, the impression that 1991; Tybout, Sternthal, Makaviya, Bakamitsos, & Park, 2005;
one knows a lot about the cause increases intentions to donate Wanke, Bohner, & Jurkowitsch, 1997). However, people hold
money and time (Study 1) as well as actual monetary donations many other, usually correct, lay theories of memory. One holds
(Study 3) when the charity asks for money to help people in that we forget over time; hence, people infer that an event oc-
need. In contrast, the impression that one knows a lot about curred more recently, the easier it is to bring to mind (Schwarz
the cause decreases intentions to donate money and time et al., 2009). Another assumes that repetition helps memory;
(Study 2) as well as actual monetary donations (Study 3) hence people infer that they heard or saw something more often
when the charity asks for money to raise awareness of its cause. when it is easy rather than difficult to recall (Schwarz, Sanna,
With regard to charitable giving, these findings draw atten- Skurnik, & Yoon, 2007; Weaver, Garcia, Schwarz, & Miller,
tion to the role of metacognitive processes in donors' decision 2007). Similarly, people correctly believe that personally relevant
making and illuminate the conditions under which donors' and important information is remembered better than unimpor-
sense of familiarity helps or hurts a charity. Most importantly, tant information (Schwarz, 2004b); hence, they infer personal
they identify a dilemma for charities that aim to raise awareness: importance from ease of recall (Schwarz et al., 2009).
when their cause does not come to mind to begin with, they will Which of these and many other lay theories of memory people
go without donations—but when potential givers believe they draw on when making a judgment depends on the specific task
know the cause well, this also reduces donations. For these and related context variables, reflecting that thinking is for
charities, extensive promotions may impair charitable giving. doing and sensitive to one's current goals in context (Schwarz,
Charities that pursue other goals—such as relieving suffering, 2004a, 2010). We propose that a charity's goal is one of the vari-
advancing self-reliance, or improving the environment—do ables that influence theory selection when people consider
not face this dilemma; for them, extensive promotions are likely whether to make a donation or not.
to improve giving. With regard to basic metacognitive processes,
the findings highlight that the same metacognitive experience can Impact of the charity's goal
have either positive or negative implications, depending on the
specifics of the current task and context. We return to these issues Not surprisingly, a charity's goal is a primary characteristic on
in the discussion. which charities are evaluated by potential donors (Sargeant,
1999; Small & Simonsohn, 2008). Even subtle changes in the
Theoretical background framing of the goal can have pronounced effects on donations
(Das, Kerhof, & Kuiper, 2008; Smith et al., 2011) and can elicit
Not surprisingly, people are more likely to donate to chari- different mind-sets in potential donors (Liu & Aaker, 2008;
ties whose work they find meaningful, important, and relevant Small, Loewenstein, & Slovic, 2007). It is therefore very likely
(Sargeant, 1999). Numerous variables can contribute to the that potential donors attend to the charity's goal when they con-
assessment of a charity's personal importance and relevance, in- sider relevant inputs, including the information provided by
cluding its cause (Bennett, 2003; Small & Simonsohn, 2008) their own charity-related metacognitive experiences.
and the donor's past experience with the charity (Sargeant & In most cases, the importance of the cause will loom large in
Jay, 2004; Sargeant & Woodliffe, 2007). Moreover, people's af- donors' decision making. This, in turn, is likely to bring to
fective response to a charitable appeal can signal its importance, mind an applicable (and correct) lay theory that holds that im-
resulting in increased donations when the victims are identified portant things are better remembered than unimportant ones
and coherent (Small & Loewenstein, 2003; Smith, Faro, & (Schwarz et al., 2009). If so, good memory for details of the
Burson, 2011), when the appeal elicits sympathy for the victims cause will suggest that one finds the cause important, a variable
(Small & Verrochi, 2009; Weiner, 1980), or when considerations that is known to increase donations (Sargeant, 1999). Hence,
of one's own benevolence elicit positive feelings (Manucia, perceived good memory should increase donations under
Baumann, & Cialdini, 1984). these conditions. Not so, however, when the charity's goal is
to raise awareness of a cause, such as the need to prevent de-
Metacognitive inferences struction of the rainforest; in this case, noting that one remem-
bers a lot about the cause provides ambiguous information. On
Here, we draw attention to a different set of variables, namely the one hand, it may indicate that one found this cause impor-
potential donors' metacognitive experiences when thinking about tant and paid close attention to it. On the other hand, it may in-
a charity and its cause. Every component of thinking can feel easy dicate that one has heard about the need to prevent rainforest
or difficult, from reading new information to recalling material destruction many times before, suggesting that it is already an
from memory. What people conclude from experiences of ease issue of public discourse. The latter interpretation is consistent
or difficulty depends on which of many lay theories of mental with the (also correct) lay theory that repetition improves
560 R.W. Smith, N. Schwarz / Journal of Consumer Psychology 22 (2012) 558–564

memory, which makes good recall an indicator of frequent pre- Study 1: The more I remember, the more I give
vious exposure and high popularity (Schwarz et al., 2007;
Weaver et al., 2007). If potential donors draw the latter infer- Study 1 tests whether people donate more to a charity that
ence, they may donate less when they believe they remember aims to relieve suffering the more they seem to remember
the charity well—after all, further awareness-raising efforts about its cause.
may not be needed.
Method
Present research
45 undergraduates from a large Midwestern University par-
The present research tests these conjectures. In three studies, ticipated for course credit. They read a one-page description of
participants read an appeal from a charity and subsequently a charity called Children of Uganda, which included a highly
take a memory test. Depending on conditions, the memory detailed account of the plight of Ugandan orphans and a de-
test is easy or difficult, leaving test takers with the impression scription of the charity's numerous activities designed to help
that they remember a lot versus very little about the charity. them. To manipulate participants' perceptions of how well
We accomplish this by asking about the same content at differ- they remember this information, they were randomly assigned
ent levels of detail (e.g., “Did the number of orphans increase or to an easy or difficult memory test. Each test consisted of
decrease last year?” versus “By how many did the number of seven questions of comparable content, except that the difficult
orphans increase or decrease last year?”); this ensures that the test asked for more intricate details than the easy test (e.g., the
same issues are salient in all conditions while allowing for var- direction of change in Uganda orphan numbers versus the
iation in the experienced ease of recall. Importantly, random quantity of change in Uganda orphan numbers).
assignment to an easy or difficult memory test avoids any con- Participants were then asked, in an open response format,
found between participants' actual memory for the charity and “Hypothetically, how much would you be willing to donate to
their metacognitive experience. As noted, the lay theory that the Children of Uganda charity?” They also reported their be-
important information is better remembered than unimportant havioral intentions (“I plan on getting involved with this
information is correct—those who find an issue more important cause,” and “I plan on telling a friend about this cause”,
attend more to it and remember more about it (Conway, 1990). 1 = strongly agree; 7 = strongly disagree); these items correlated
Hence, natural variations in memory performance are inade- r(43) = .634, p b .001, and were averaged to form an intended
quate for isolating the influence of metacognitive processes, involvement index. In addition, participants indicated how dif-
making experimental manipulations of perceived memory per- ficult they found the memory test (1 = very easy; 7 = very
formance the method of choice. difficult).
Study 1 uses a charity that helps orphans in Uganda. We pre-
dict, and find, that participants assigned to an easy memory test Results and discussion
intend to donate more to this charity than participants assigned
to a difficult memory test. Study 2 uses a charity that aims to Participants answered more questions correctly on the easy
raise awareness about rain forest destruction. We predict, and (M = 6.1) than on the difficult test (M =2.67; t(43)= 9.6, pb .001).
find, that participants assigned to an easy memory test intend They were aware of this difference and rated the easy test
to donate less to this charity than participants assigned to a dif- (M= 3.43) as easier than the difficult one (M= 4.75; t(43) =2.7,
ficult memory test. Study 3 uses the same charity, concerned p= .01).
with childhood heart disease, for both types of goals in a single We predicted that consumers who believe they remember a
design and replicates the results with actual (rather than charity well will (i) donate more money and (ii) report a higher
intended) monetary donations. intention to get involved. As is often the case with charitable
Note that our key prediction is an interaction of the charity's donations, the response variable was non-normally distributed
goal and potential donors' metacognitive experience: thinking with a large right skew. Accordingly, we follow the widely
that one remembers well will decrease donations for awareness- used method of analyzing donations with a non-parametric
raising charities, but increase donations for other charities. Mann–Whitney U test on donation–amount–ranks (Andreoni,
While many other variables may be plausibly related to one's per- 1995; Landry, Lange, List, Price, & Rupp, 2006; Lehmann,
formance on a memory test and one's willingness to donate 1975). As expected, those who took the easy test intended to
money, these variables predict main effects of test performance donate more than those who took the difficult test (U = 151,
rather than the above interaction. For example, better perfor- p = .02). Removing three outliers ($3000, $30,000, and
mance on a test may improve one's mood, self-esteem, or the $100,000) and conducting a t-test yielded similar results; partic-
accessibility of success related thoughts; while these and many ipants who took the easy test reported higher donation inten-
other variables can affect charitable giving (for reviews, see tions (M = $187.11) than those who took the difficult test (M =
Bekkers & Wiepking, 2007; Gardner, 1985; Piliavin & Charng, $59.67; t(40) = 2.02, p = .05). Also as predicted, the former par-
1990), we are not aware of any alternative theorizing that predicts ticipants reported greater intentions to get involved (M = 3.62)
that the impact of one's performance on a memory test on chari- than the latter (M = 4.46, t(43) = 2.25, p = .03).
table giving is contingent on whether the charity aims to raise In sum, participants assigned to an easy memory test
awareness or pursues another goal. responded more favorably to the charity and its cause than
R.W. Smith, N. Schwarz / Journal of Consumer Psychology 22 (2012) 558–564 561

participants assigned to a difficult memory test. Because the While test performance can be used as a mood manipulation when
memory test manipulated participants' beliefs about how well it is framed as success or failure in a test of ability (see Gerrards-
they remember the charity, it also illustrates how mere measure- Hesse, Spies, & Hesse, 2011), this prerequisite is not satisfied in
ment can affect consumer behavior (see also Morwitz & the present experiments. Further reducing the plausibility that
Fitzsimons, 2004). The finding is compatible with common mood plays a key role in the present findings, a difficult test was
wisdom: Potential donors who think they know a lot about a associated with high donation intentions in Study 2 but low dona-
charity and its cause are more likely to donate, making exten- tion intentions in Study 1, in contrast to the main effect predictions
sive advertising a crucial element of philanthropic work. Our that follow from diverse mood accounts (for reviews of relevant
theorizing predicts, however, that this wisdom does not apply mood theories, see Clore, Schwarz, & Conway, 1994; Morris,
when the charity pursues awareness-raising goals. Study 2 1989; Schwarz & Clore, 2007).
tests this prediction.
Study 3: Behavioral consequences
Study 2: The more I remember, the less I give
Studies 1 and 2 point to an important role of metacognitive
Method inferences in charitable giving decisions. However, the studies
were limited to hypothetical donations of money and time and
135 adults from the online pool MindField completed this the charities used in these studies differed not only in their
study and others in exchange for payment of $5. They read a goals, but also in the issues they addressed. Study 3 addresses
one-page description of The Prince's Rainforest Foundation, these ambiguities by soliciting real monetary donations for the
which emphasized the goal of raising awareness of the need same charity. In all conditions, participants learn about a charity
to prevent tropical deforestation. Subsequently, participants that seeks to reduce the impact of childhood heart disease; how-
completed either an easy or difficult memory test involving 7 ever, the charity attempts to do so either by raising awareness or
questions of comparable content (e.g. whether the charity's by supporting treatment. Next, participants take an easy or dif-
website has a certain feature versus what year that feature was ficult memory test; the tests are identical across the two charita-
launched). Participants then indicated in an open-ended re- ble goals. Finally, participants have the opportunity to make an
sponse format how much money (dollars) and time (hours) anonymous donation to the charity. This replicates Studies 1
they would hypothetically be willing to donate. and 2 in a single design with a behavioral outcome variable,
Participants also rated the difficulty of the test (1 = very easy; using the same charity in all conditions. We predict that con-
7 = very difficult) and completed a four-item mood scale sumers donate more to a charity that emphasizes the provision
(α = .92) with the ends of 5-point scales labeled: sad/happy, of help when perceived memory is good rather than poor, but
bad mood/good mood, irritable/pleased, and depressed/cheerful more to a charity that emphasizes awareness-raising when per-
(Lee & Sternthal, 1999; Swinyard, 1993). ceived memory is poor rather than good.

Results and discussion Method

Participants assigned to the easy memory test answered 255 adults from a paid-participant pool in a Midwestern col-
more questions correctly (M = 5.63) than those assigned to the lege town completed this study and others in exchange for a pay-
difficult test (M = 3.29; t(133) = 10.15, p b .001). They were ment of $15. Participants read a one-page description of Heart 2
aware of this difference and rated the easy test (M = 3.06) as Heart, a charity devoted to reducing the impact of childhood
easier than the difficult one (M = 4.21; t(133) = 4.42, p b .001). heart disease. There were two versions of this description. The
The donation data was again non-normally distributed and helping-purpose version emphasized the goal of treating child-
analyzed with a Mann–Whitney U test on donation–amount– hood heart disease, whereas the awareness-purpose version em-
ranks. As predicted, the previously observed pattern reversed. phasized the goal of raising awareness about childhood heart
Participants who took the difficult memory test intended to disease; the descriptions were identical in all other respects. Par-
donate more money to help the charity raise awareness (M = ticipants then took either an easy or difficult 7-question memory
$30.77) than participants who took the easy test (M = $13.63, test; the tests were identical across charity-purpose conditions and
U = 1571.5, p = .02). An unequal variances t-test yielded similar of comparable content across quiz-difficulty conditions. Finally,
results (t(83) = 1.94, p = .055). Paralleling this finding, partici- participants had the opportunity to make anonymous monetary
pants who took the difficult test also reported that they would donations by placing their survey and donations in an envelope.
donate more hours (M = 13.11) than those who took the easy Experimenters delivered all donated money to the charity. In
test (M = 6.65, t(114) = 2.07, p = .04). These results are opposite addition, participants reported how easy or difficult they found
to the results of Study 1, where a difficult memory test was the memory test (1 = very easy; 7 = very difficult).
associated with lower intentions to donate to a charity that pur-
sues a helping goal. Results and discussion
Finally, participants' mood reports showed no difference be-
tween conditions (M's= 3.63 and 3.60, t(133) b 1, for the easy Participants answered more questions correctly when the test
and difficult test, respectively). This is consistent with expectations. was easy (M = 5.82) rather than difficult (M = 4.22; t(250) = 9.2,
562 R.W. Smith, N. Schwarz / Journal of Consumer Psychology 22 (2012) 558–564

1.4
p b .001). They were aware of this difference and rated the easy
test (M = 4.1) as easier than the difficult one (M = 4.91; t(251) = 1.2

Mean Donation ($)


4.49, p b .001). 1
Overall, 39% of the participants chose to make a donation.
Due to the prevalence of $0 donations, we used a Tobit 0.8
model (Amemiya, 1985; Mitchell & Dacin, 1996) to examine 0.6
the extent to which the difficulty of the memory test (easy ver-
0.4
sus hard) and the purpose of the charity (helping versus
awareness-raising) affected the amount donated. The Tobit 0.2
model was run with zero (no donation) and 8 (highest donation 0
received) as the lower and upper limits, respectively; see Easy Hard
Table 1 for results. The key finding is the predicted interaction Quiz Difficulty
between the difficulty of the memory test and the purpose of the
charity (β = − 2.31, t(251) = − 2.58, p = .01), shown in Fig. 1; it Awareness Purpose Helping Purpose
was diagnosed with one-tailed Tobit contrasts. When the char-
ity's helping goal was emphasized, participants donated more
money when the easy quiz conveyed that they remembered Fig. 1. The implications of perceived memory depend on the target. This figure
includes participants who donated $0.
the details well (M = $1.26, SD = 1.77) than when the difficult
quiz conveyed that they remembered them poorly (M = $.78,
SD = 1.42; β = − 1.22, t(124) = − 1.87, p = .03). This replicates awareness-raising conditions, ensuring that their differential im-
Study 1 with real monetary donations. Also as predicted, this pact is not driven by differential memory content or differential
pattern reversed when the charity's awareness-raising goal affective responses across these conditions.
was emphasized. In this case, participants donated less when
the easy quiz conveyed that they remembered the details well General discussion
(M = $.51, SD = 1.06) than when the difficult quiz conveyed
that they remembered them poorly (M = $.85, SD = 1.33; In three studies with different charities, we found that poten-
β = 1.07, t(127) = 1.79, p = .04). This replicates Study 2 with tial donors' perceptions of how much they remember about a
real monetary donations. Mann–Whitney contrasts yielded sim- charity and its cause influence their intended (Studies 1 and
ilar results (p's b .04). 2) and actual (Study 3) charitable giving. When a charity has
Three points are worth noting. First, the robust replications a goal of helping people in need, perceptions of good memory
across different charities in Studies 1 to 3 highlight that the crucial for the cause increase charitable giving; but when a charity has
variable is indeed whether the charity pursues an awareness- a goal of raising awareness for a cause, perceptions of good
raising versus another goal, not the domain of their cause. This memory for the cause impair charitable giving. These effects
reduces potential concerns about idiosyncratic aspects of the re- were reliable for each individual study and combined analyses
spective charities. Second, the actual monetary donations in this (following the adding-z's procedure of Rosenthal, 1978)
study were substantially lower than the hypothetical donations result in overall effects of z = 2.98, p = .001, for the helping
in Studies 1 and 2, while replicating the theoretically predicted charities and z = 2.89, p = .002, for the awareness-raising chari-
patterns. The difference in the absolute size of hypothetical and ties. These findings are consistent with the theoretical rationale
actual donations is consistent with other findings (Murphy, developed in other domains (Schwarz, 2004a, 2010) and con-
Allen, Stevens, & Weatherhead, 2005). Third, the memory tests tribute to our understanding of charitable giving as well as
used in Study 3 were identical under help-providing and metacognition.

Table 1 Implications for metacognition


Tobit coefficients for amount of money donated to charity.
Full model Coefficient t-statistic p-value
Our results highlight that consumers' use of metacognitive
experiences as a source of information is sensitive to the specific
Constant − 1.83 − 3.45 .001
Difficulty (easy = 0) 1.13 1.76 .079
context in which they make a decision. The observation that one
Purpose (awareness = 0) 1.97 3.13 .002 remembers a lot about a charity one recently heard about is inher-
Difficulty x purpose interaction − 2.31 − 2.58 .010 ently ambiguous—it may reflect that one found the material per-
sonally important and paid close attention to it or that one had
Helping Purpose previously heard about the charity and was already familiar
Constant .061 .14 .89
Difficulty (replicating Study 1) − 1.22 − 1.87 .032
with some of the material. Which of the underlying lay theories
of memory is likely to come to mind depends on which goal
Awareness Purpose the charity emphasizes. When the charity aims to raise awareness,
Constant − 1.64 − 3.03 .003 potential donors are likely to assess whether such awareness-
Difficulty (replicating Study 2) 1.07 1.79 .038 raising is needed. Noting that they know little about the charity,
Note. Contrasts within purpose levels are single-tailed. they may conclude that awareness-raising is indeed required,
R.W. Smith, N. Schwarz / Journal of Consumer Psychology 22 (2012) 558–564 563

resulting in higher donations when memory is poor rather than Hence, the more than 20,000 awareness-raising charities
good. When the charity pursues another goal—such as wanting (Guidestar, 2011) face an important dilemma in their promotion
to help people, animals, or the environment—awareness related goals. For example, consider VH1's Save the Music Founda-
considerations are less likely to come to mind and consumers tion, which aims to raise “awareness about the importance of
may focus on the importance of the cause. In this case, good music as part of each child's complete education” (Vh1 Save
memory for details will suggest that one found the charity person- the Music, 2011). To acquire the necessary donations, the charity
ally important, resulting in higher donations when memory is needs to promote itself to potential donors. However, if the pro-
good rather than poor. These context sensitive inferences reflect motions lead potential donors to believe that they are already
a high level of sophistication in thinking about one's own mem- familiar with the charity and its cause, they may conclude that
ory processes and their context specific implications, indicating awareness of the need for music education in schools is already
that the use of metacognitive experiences as a source of informa- high and choose not to donate. Fortunately, there are several
tion is often part of a thoughtful and deliberate assessment of strategies that Save the Music Foundation can adopt to avoid
the available information in context (Schwarz, 2010). this conundrum. First, the organization can attempt to segment
This analysis may seem more compelling if it were accom- its population of potential donors. Like many awareness-raising
panied by data that show the implied differences in partici- charities, they have multiple goals and could emphasize different
pants' perception of the personal importance of the cause or goals for different segments. For existing donors, who are al-
the need to raise awareness, preferably complete with media- ready familiar with the charity, they might focus on providing
tion analyses. Unfortunately, such data would be nondiagnos- musical instruments to schools; for new potential donors, who
tic. First, suppose that we assessed perceived importance at are presumably less familiar with the organization, they could
the end of the experiment, after participants donated. In this focus on their awareness-raising goal. This strategy could
case, the act of donating may itself serve as an input into later allow the charity to take advantage of positive effects of high per-
importance judgments, providing apparent evidence for “media- ceived memory among existing donors as well as of positive
tion” that may well reflect reverse causality. Alternatively, sup- effects of low perceived memory among new potential donors.
pose that we assessed personal importance prior to donations. Second, for charities that focus exclusively on awareness-
In this case, the importance judgment would recruit the corre- raising, donation appeals should facilitate the attribution that
sponding lay theory of memory in all conditions, thus thwarting being highly knowledgeable about the cause is indicative of
the ability to observe goal-contingent differences. Similar consid- one's own personal engagement rather than of broad societal
erations apply to measures of the need to raise awareness. As awareness. Future research may fruitfully test these strategies
metacognitive analyses indicate, measurement changes the and may extend the analysis to other organizations that attempt
thought process and the desire to collect many measures in a to raise awareness, including political interest groups devoted
single study is more likely to mislead than to illuminate to changing the public's awareness of specific issues. If success-
(Schwarz, 2010). fully implemented, either of these strategies would put
awareness-raising organizations in the same situation as other or-
Implications for charitable giving ganizations, allowing them to benefit from the memorability of
their important and worthwhile causes.
With regard to charitable giving, our findings challenge con-
ventional wisdom: Knowing a lot about a charity and its cause
can hurt rather than help charitable giving, provided the charity Future directions
emphasizes awareness-raising as a goal. Hence, awareness-
raising charities face different marketing considerations than Theoretically, the influence of other metacognitive variables
charities that pursue other goals, most notably the relief of that can convey that one is or is not highly familiar with a charity
suffering. For both types of charities, a simple truism holds: should parallel the impact of perceived memory in the present
to enter a consumer's consideration set, they need to be suffi- studies. Such variables include all manipulations of fluency,
ciently memorable to come to mind when the consumer con- from print fonts to rhyme (for a review see Schwarz, 2004a).
siders making charitable donations. But once they pass the Moreover, the conceptual rationale developed here is not limited
threshold of entering the consideration set, both types of chari- to charities. Other product categories in which extensive promo-
ties differ in whether high memorability helps or hurts their pur- tion can have negative effects include identity signaling products,
pose. When the charity aims at relieving suffering, consumers for which consumers prefer moderately low societal awareness
infer from high knowledge about the charity that they care (Berger & Heath, 2007), and products that benefit from exclu-
about the cause, which increases the likelihood and size of sivity, such as some vacation destinations (Pocheptsova, Labroo,
donations (Studies 1 and 3). But for awareness-raising charities, & Dhar, 2010). If promotion of these products leads to perceptions
high memorability can backfire when consumers infer that of high societal awareness, willingness to buy may suffer.
awareness is already high and conclude that additional dona-
tions are not needed. Our findings suggest that this conclusion
comes easily to potential donors (Studies 2 and 3) and does References
not require prompts beyond the stated goal of raising Amemiya, T. (1985). Advanced econometrics. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni-
awareness. versity Press.
564 R.W. Smith, N. Schwarz / Journal of Consumer Psychology 22 (2012) 558–564

Andreoni, J. (1995). Warm-glow versus cold-prickle: The effects of positive Rosenthal, R. (1978). Combining results of independent studies. Psychological
and negative framing on cooperation in experiments. Quarterly Journal of Bulletin, 85, 185–193.
Economics, 110, 1–21. Sargeant, A. (1999). Charitable giving: Towards a model of donor behavior.
Bekkers, R., & Wiepking, P. (2007). Generosity and philanthropy: A literature Journal of Marketing Management, 15, 215–238.
review. Retrieved 8/13/2011 from SSRN. http://ssrn.com/abstract=1015507. Sargeant, A., & Jay, E. (2004). Building donor loyalty: The fundraiser's guide
Bendapudi, N., Singh, N. S., & Bendapudi, V. (1996). Enhancing helping to increasing lifetime value. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
behavior: An integrative framework for promotion planning. Journal of Sargeant, A., & Woodliffe, L. (2007). Building donor loyalty: The antecedents
Marketing, 3, 33–49. and role of commitment in the context of charity giving. Journal of Non-
Bennett, R. (2003). Factors underlying the inclination to donate to particular profit and Public Policy Sector Marketing, 18, 47–68.
types of charities. International Journal of Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Schwarz, N. (2004a). Metacognitive experiences in consumer judgment and
Marketing, 8, 12–29. decision making. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14, 332–348.
Berger, J. A., & Heath, C. (2007). Where consumers diverge from others: Identity Schwarz, N. (2004b). Metacognitive experiences: Response to commentaries.
signaling and product domains. Journal of Consumer Research, 34, 121–134. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14, 370–373.
Clore, G. L., Schwarz, N., & Conway, M. (1994). Affective causes and conse- Schwarz, N. (2010). Meaning in context: Metacognitive experiences. In B.
quences of social information processing. In R. S. Wyer, & T. K. Srull Mesquita, L. F. Barrett, & E. R. Smith (Eds.), The mind in context
(Eds.), Handbook of social cognition, Vol. 1. (pp. 323–418). (2nd Ed). (pp. 105–125). New York: Guilford Press.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Schwarz, N., Bless, H., Strack, F., Klumpp, G., Rittenauer–Schatka, H., &
Cone (2010). The Cone nonprofit power brand 100. Retrieved from. http:// Simons, A. (1991). Ease of retrieval as information: Another look at the avail-
www.coneinc.com/content2300. ability heuristic. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 195–202.
Conway, M. A. (1990). Autobiographical memory. Buckingham, UK: Open Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (2007). Feelings and phenomenal experiences. In
University Press. A. Kruglanski, & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Social psychology. Handbook of
Das, E., Kerhof, P., & Kuiper, J. (2008). Improving the effectiveness of fun- basic principles (pp. 385–407). (2nd Ed). New York: Guilford.
draising messages: The impact of charity goal attainment, message framing, Schwarz, N., Sanna, L. J., Skurnik, I., & Yoon, C. (2007). Metacognitive expe-
and evidence on persuasion. Journal of Applied Communication Research, riences and the intricacies of setting people straight: Implications for debias-
36, 161–175. ing and public information campaigns. Advances in Experimental Social
Gardner, M. P. (1985). Mood states and consumer behavior: A critical review. Psychology, 39, 127–161.
Journal of Consumer Research, 12, 281–300. Schwarz, N., Song, H., & Xu, J. (2009). When thinking is difficult: Metacogni-
Gerrards-Hesse, A., Spies, K., & Hesse, F. W. (2011). Experimental inductions tive experiences as information. In M. Wanke (Ed.), Social psychology of
of emotional states and their effectiveness: A review. British Journal of consumer behavior (pp. 201–223). New York: Psychology Press.
Psychology, 85, 55–78. Small, D. A., & Loewenstein, G. (2003). Helping a victim or helping the victim:
Guidestar (2011). Search conducted 3/15/2011 at. http://www.guidestar.org. Altruism and identifiability. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 26, 5–16.
Kotler, P., & Andreasen, A. R. (2000). Strategic marketing for nonprofit orga- Small, D. A., Loewenstein, G., & Slovic, P. (2007). Sympathy and callousness:
nizations. In M. J. Baker (Ed.), Marketing theory: A student text The impact of deliberative though on donations to identifiable and statistical
(pp. 244–263). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. victims. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 102,
Krishna, A. (2011). Can supporting a cause decrease donations and happiness? 143–153.
The cause marketing paradox. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 21, 338–345. Small, D. A., & Simonsohn, U. (2008). Friends of victims: Personal experience
Landry, C., Lange, A., List, J. A., Price, M. K., & Rupp, G. (2006). Toward an and prosocial behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 35, 532–542.
understanding of the economics of charity: Evidence from a field experi- Small, D. A., & Verrochi, N. M. (2009). The face of need: Facial emotion
ment. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 121, 747–782. expression on charity advertisements. Journal of Marketing Research, 46,
Lee, A. Y., & Sternthal, B. (1999). The effects of positive mood on memory. 777–787.
Journal of Consumer Research, 26, 115–127. Smith, R. W., Faro, D., & Burson, K. (2011). More for the many: The influence
Lehmann, E. L. (1975). Nonparametrics: Statistical methods based on ranks. of entitativity on charitable giving. working paper. Ann Arbor, MI: Univer-
San Francisco: Holden-Day. sity of Michigan.
Liu, W., & Aaker, J. (2008). The happiness of giving: The time-ask effect. Jour- Swinyard, W. R. (1993). The effects of mood, involvement, and quality of store
nal of Consumer Research, 35, 543–557. experience on shopping intentions. Journal of Consumer Research, 20,
Manucia, G. K., Baumann, D. J., & Cialdini, R. B. (1984). Mood influences on 271–280.
helping: Direct effects or side effects. Journal of Personality and Social Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging
Psychology, 46, 357–364. frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5, 207–232.
Mitchell, A. A., & Dacin, P. (1996). The assessment of alternative measures of Tybout, A. M., Sternthal, B., Makaviya, P., Bakamitsos, G. A., & Park, S.
consumer expertise. Journal of Consumer Research, 23, 219–239. (2005). Information accessibility as a moderator of judgments: The roles
Morris, W. N. (1989). Mood: The frame of mind. New York: Springer Verlag. of content versus retrieval ease. Journal of Consumer Research, 32, 76–85.
Morwitz, V. G., & Fitzsimons, G. J. (2004). The mere-measurement effect: Vh1 Save the Music (2011). Who we are. Accessed May 25, 2011 from. http://
Why does measuring intentions change behavior? Journal of Consumer www.vh1savethemusic.com/.
Psychology, 14, 64–73. Wanke, M., Bohner, G., & Jurkowitsch, A. (1997). There are many reasons to
Murphy, J. J., Allen, P. G., Stevens, T. H., & Weatherhead, D. (2005). A drive a BMW: Does imagined ease of argument generation influence atti-
meta-analysis of hypothetical bias in stated preference valuation. Environ- tudes? Journal of Consumer Research, 24, 170–178.
mental and Resource Economics, 30, 313–325. Weaver, K., Garcia, S., Schwarz, N., & Miller, D. T. (2007). Inferring the
Piliavin, J. A., & Charng, H. W. (1990). Altruism: A review of recent theory popularity of an opinion from its familiarity: A repetitive voice can sound
and research. Annual Review of Sociology, 16, 27–65. like a chorus. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 821–833.
Pocheptsova, A., Labroo, A., & Dhar, R. (2010). Making products feel special: Weiner, B. (1980). A cognitive (attribution)–emotion–action model of motivated
When metacognitive difficulty enhances evaluation. Journal of Marketing behavior: An analysis of judgments of help-giving. Journal of Personality and
Research, 47, 1059–1069. Social Psychology, 39, 186–200.

Você também pode gostar