Você está na página 1de 5

F

Frustration A Functional Perspective

Bertus F. Jeronimus1,3 and Odilia M. Laceulle2,4 The experience of brief and intense emotions is an
1
Department of Developmental Psychology, integral component of our everyday conduct.
University of Groningen, Groningen, Emotions influence how we make decisions and
The Netherlands navigate our worlds, via bodily changes that pro-
2
Department of Medical and Clinical Psychology, mpt us to action. Frustration is a key negative
Tilburg University, Tilburg, The Netherlands emotion that roots in disappointment and can be
3
Department of Psychiatry, Interdisciplinary defined as irritable distress in response to limita-
Center Psychopathology and Emotion regulation tion, exclusion, and failure (a state of dissatisfied
(ICPE), University Medical Center Groningen, insecurity). Frustration elicits negative affect to
University of Groningen, Groningen, The signal that interests and interactions must be
Netherlands adjusted, and emotional tension or “arousal” to
4
Department of Developmental Psychology, instigate defensive or aggressive behavioral
Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands responses, such as strive to reduce or eliminate
the blocking agent or circumstances.
Frustration evolved to deal with a particular,
evolutionarily recurrent situation type and is expe-
Synonyms
rienced when people encounter unresolved prob-
lems, such as contextual or psychological barriers
Annoyance; Dissatisfaction; Foiling; Hindrance;
or obstructions, which must be removed to fulfill
Interference; Obstruction; Thwarting; Vexation
personal goals, desires, drives, or needs. Techni-
cally, frustration is elicited when a goal-pursuit is
not fulfilled at the expected time in the behavioral
Definition sequence (an unexpected nonreward). The most
reliable trigger of frustration is an externally
Frustration is a key negative emotion that roots in attributed omission of a rewarding event or item
disappointment (Latin frustrā or “in vain”) and and especially a perceived obstruction by an
can be defined as irritable distress after a wish intentional antagonistic act (Jeronimus et al.
collided with an unyielding reality. 2016). The intensity of frustration is a function
of the reward value of the frustrated approach goal
(reward proximity and motivation), the degree of
interference (partial/total), the number of
# Springer International Publishing AG 2017
V. Zeigler-Hill, T.K. Shackelford (eds.), Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences,
DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-28099-8_815-1
2 Frustration

interferences per unit time, and one’s self- explanation to one of their “why” questions.
regulation abilities (Berkowitz 1989). Over childhood new sources of frustration
From a functional perspective, frustrated emerge, including new expectations, and compar-
arousal should facilitate approach tendencies isons with peers, older siblings, and adults.
when the problem is deemed controllable and Adults, finally, who predispose for frustration
the goal perceived as attainable, e.g., inflicting early in life tend to score higher on the “angry-
costs via anger to overcome problems. Con- hostility” facet of the neuroticism personality
versely, when the problem is appraised as uncon- domain, in which frustration clusters with trait
trollable, frustration should facilitate avoidance anger and bitterness (McCrae et al. 2005).
(withdrawal, via fear or anxiety), or low approach Importantly, developmental patterns of frustra-
when the goal is perceived as unattainable tion vary slightly across genders. Male infants are
(downregulation of expected benefits via sad- typically less able to regulate their frustration reac-
ness). After cues trigger the frustration mode, the tivity physiologically via behaviors. And while
way we see the world and feel about the world childhood frustration-proneness is comparable in
changes. The energizing effects of frustration can both genders, boys become somewhat more
thus catalyze a broad range of processes, which inclined to frustration than girls over early adoles-
may be positive, because when we are frustrated cence until age 16, and adult men typically remain
we make greater efforts and strive in other direc- slightly more angry and hostile than women.
tions, which resulted in the creation of the electric
light bulb, Internet, and Facebook, among others.
All people suffer from frustration, because our Causes and Consequences of
needs cannot always be adequately satisfied in Frustration: The Social Environment
all situations, and frustration can help us identify
these needs. The ability to effectively deal with Childhood frustration-intolerance has been asso-
frustration is therefore a very important skill to ciated with a broad range of outcomes, including
develop. psychological, social, and occupational function-
ing, well-being, and somatic and mental health
service use (Caspi et al. 2016). Both stability and
The Development of Frustration change in dispositional frustration emerges from
an interplay between individuals and their (social)
Experiences of frustration have a substantial environment. Easily frustrated infants are typi-
genetic basis (ca. 50%) which can be observed cally perceived to be less attentive, more active,
from very early in life. Generally, frustration and more distressed to novelty than their less
emerges during the first year of life and increases easily frustrated peers and are more likely to
over childhood to peak during early and middle develop an insecure-avoidant attachment style.
adolescence (Buss 2011; Putnam et al. 2001), Children and adolescents who are easily frus-
followed by slow declines with age. Specifically, trated report more stressful social events with
over childhood children usually lack the impulse parents and peers, in part due to the perception
control that is required to hold back from an of more frequent hostile intent, rejection, and dis-
intense immediate response. Moreover, in our approval in others (Laceulle et al. 2015). As such,
first 2 years we typically cannot stand frustration, dispositional frustration can have pervasive social
which may be expressed in tantrums. After our consequences, in term of social relationships and
second year this frustration-tolerance improves, interactions, but also with regard to occupational
also due to better language skills. A 3-year-old and job performances.
may say “I hate you” when frustrated by limits, The social environment can also affect our frus-
whereas many 4-year-olds experience frustration tration tolerance, either in terms of further
when they are unable to make sense of an reinforcing and stabilizing an already existing
Frustration 3

predisposition or in terms of contributing to small (“aroused/hot”), which can be either internally or


changes in frustration. For instance, more rigid and externally provoked, resulting in an emotional,
disciplinarian parents can increase the number of impulsive, and defensive or hostile/retaliatory reac-
frustrations their child faces. In addition, tion. Proactive aggression, in contrast, tends to be
frustration-tolerance may decrease after major calm and instrumental and is associated with popu-
social stressors, and this change can persist for larity, delinquency, and psychopathy, rather than
months and may get under the skin (Jeronimus frustration.
et al. 2016). Thus, frustration tolerance both affects Frustration intolerance may be particularly
and can be affected by the social environment, and notable in new situations and when the person is
these processes are known as person-environment tired or stressed. Low frustration tolerance is a
transactions (e.g., Laceulle and van Aken 2017). characteristic feature of personality disorders,
especially for borderline and antisocial personal-
ity disorder, and has been associated with narcis-
Frustration, Psychopathology, and Life- sistic, obsessive, paranoid, histrionic, and
Outcomes schizoid traits. Being easily frustrated is also com-
monly reported by adults with sleep problems,
Dispositional frustration, as well as interactions medically unexplained complaints, attention def-
between frustration and the social environment, icit (hyperactivity) disorder, dyslexia, dyspraxia,
can have profound consequences for an individ- dementia, Alzheimer’s, and traumatic brain
ual’s vulnerability for the development of psycho- injury. Also high functioning autistic people
pathology. Frustration explains a substantial part (previously known as Asperger) are typically
of the development of psychopathology over ado- more prone to frustration.
lescence, which suggests that frustration is close
to the origin of the causal pathways towards psy-
chopathology (Caspi et al. 2016; Jeronimus et al. Animal Models of Frustration
2015). High frustration during adolescence pre-
dicts increases in general distress and externaliz- Frustration has been extensively studied using
ing symptoms such as anger and substance abuse, animal models. In these models, frustration is
and an increased risk to develop anxiety, depres- often conceptualized as part of a cognitive “rage
sion, substance abuse, and thought disorders and system” that influences memory-retention and
their symptoms during adulthood (Jeronimus learning processes to suit a deliberative mind-set
et al. 2016, 2017). to deal with the source of danger or obstruction, or
Frustration has repeatedly been related to aggres- to retrieve or replace a lost resource. Individual
sion and attention deficits, such as in the famous differences in frustration have been observed in
frustration-aggression hypothesis (Berkowitz 1989). chimpanzees, pigs, rats, and birds. Even honey-
Frustration typically elicits anger (an emotion), bees show frustration-like responses when exper-
which in turn reduces inhibitions and narrows atten- imenters shift access from a high very-sweet
tion to cues for threat, which can lead to aggression concentration of sugar to one that is much less
(a behavior which causes harm or damage, either concentrated, thus presumably less desirable.
physical, verbal, or relational), as outlined. Impor- Frustration enables organism both individually
tantly, whereas frustration requires a blockage to and collectively to adapt, survive, and reproduce.
obtain a desired goal, this is not a necessity for
anger and aggression. The frustration-aggression
link has been refined via the dual aggression History
model which distinguishes reactive from proactive
aggression (Hubbard et al. 2010). Frustration elicits About 2500 years ago Gautama Buddha claimed
reactive aggression to a perceived blockage that frustration is often generated by desire and
4 Frustration

attachment. At the same time Graeco-Arabic med- Cross-References


ical traditions conceived frustration intolerance as
characteristic of the choleric type, a person who is ▶ Basic Emotions
quickly tempered (irritable/angry) due to highly ▶ Frustration-Aggression Hypothesis
active bodily fluids and a preponderance of yellow ▶ Neuroticism
bile. These “fire people” responded rapidly and ▶ Personality Structure
sustained their response for a relatively long time ▶ Temperament
(hot/dry).
Early in the twentieth century, Sigmund Freud
revived interest in frustration with his psychody- References
namic theory of neurosis, in which frustration
referred both to external barriers to goal attain- Berkowitz, L. (1989). Frustration aggression hypothesis –
ment and internal obstacles blocking need satis- Examination and reformulation. Psychological Bulle-
tin, 106(1), 59–73. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.106.1.59.
faction. Freud postulated that unresolved
Buss, A. H. (2011). Pathways to individuality: Evolution
frustrations from infancy and early childhood and development of personality traits. Washington,
played out unconsciously in adulthood and saw DC: American Psychological Association. doi:10.
frustration as a necessary condition for mental 1037/a0026164.
Caspi, A., Houts, R. M., Belsky, D. W., Harrington, H.,
illness and the most common cause for neurosis.
Hogan, S., Ramrakha, S., ... Moffitt, T. E. (2016).
The psychotherapist Albert Ellis’ rational emo- Childhood forecasting of a small segment of the popu-
tive behavior therapy (REBT) purported that emo- lation with large economic burden. Nature Human
tional disturbances typically root in frustration Behaviour, 1, 0005. doi:10.1038/s41562-016-0005
Hubbard, J. A., McAuliffe, M. D., Morrow, M. T., &
intolerance, viz. the belief that reality should be
Romano, L. J. (2010). Reactive and proactive aggres-
how we want it to be. Frustration was thought to sion in childhood and adolescence: Precursors, out-
be the typical response to irrational cognitions like comes, processes, experiences, and measurement.
“life should be fair.” Challenging the validity of Journal of Personality, 78(1), 95–118. doi:10.1111/j.
1467-6494.2009.00610.x.
these beliefs then challenged the frustration that
Jeronimus, B. F., Riese, H., Oldehinkel, A. J., & Ormel,
resulted from it. J. (2016). Why does frustration predict psychopathol-
Over the past century, the choleric and melan- ogy? Multiple prospective pathways over adolescence:
cholic temperament types evolved into the mod- A TRAILS study. European Journal of Personality,
31(1), 85–103. doi:10.1002/per.2086.
ern personality dimension neuroticism, whereas
Jeronimus, B. F., Kotov, R., Riese, H., & Ormel, J. (2017).
neurosis developed into anxiety, depression, and Neuroticism’s prospective association with mental dis-
somatic disorders. orders halves after adjustment for baseline symptoms
and psychiatric history, but the adjusted association
hardly decays with time: A meta-analysis on 59 longi-
tudinal/prospective studies with 443,313 participants.
Conclusion Psychological Medicine, 46, 2883–2906. doi:10.1017/
S0033291716001653.
Frustration is a key emotion that can elicit a wide Laceulle, O. M., & van Aken, M. A. G. (2017). Trans-
actions of personality and the social environment dur-
range of responses. Although frustration is known
ing development. In V. Zeigler-Hill & T. K.
to have a substantial genetic component, research Shackelford (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of personality
has provided support for both lifespan develop- and individual differences. Los Angeles: Sage.
ment and (small) environmentally driven changes Laceulle, O. M., Jeronimus, B. F., Van Aken, M. A. G., &
Ormel, J. (2015). Why not everybody gets their fair share
in frustration. Individual differences in frustration
of stress: Adolescent’s perceived relationship affection
can have profound consequences for major life mediates associations between temperament and subse-
outcomes, including how we shape our social quent stressful social events. European Journal of Per-
environment, our occupational functioning, our sonality, 29(2), 125–137. doi:10.1002/per.1989.
McCrae, R. R., Costa, J., Paul, T., & Martin, T. A. (2005).
well-being, and a (increased) risk for the develop-
The NEO–PI–3: A more readable revised NEO person-
ment of somatic problems, psychopathology, and ality inventory. Journal of Personality Assessment,
aggression. 84(3), 261–270. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa8403_05.
Frustration 5

Putnam, S. P., Ellis, S. K., & Rothbart, M. K. (2001). The in research on temperament (pp. 165–182). Berlin:
structure of temperament from infancy through adoles- Pabst Scientist Publicer.
cence. In A. Eliasz & A. Engleneiter (Eds.), Advances

Você também pode gostar