Você está na página 1de 46

12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention

Part VIII - Accidents and Safety Management Français

Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention


INTRODUCTION
Jorma Saari

According to International Labour Office statistics, 120 million occupational accidents occur annually at workplaces worldwide. Of these, 210,000 are fatal
accidents. Every day, more than 500 men or women do not come home because they were killed by accidents at work. These are dramatic numbers which draw
fairly little public attention. Considering the fact that accidents take a considerable economic toll from nations, companies and individuals, accidents do not get
much publicity.

Fortunately there are people working with a sense of purpose, often behind the scenes, towards understanding and managing safety and accident prevention
better, and their efforts have not been wasted. Our understanding of accident prevention and safety is on a far higher level than ever before. Many world-class
researchers and safety practitioners share this new knowledge with us through their articles in this Encyclopaedia. During the past twenty decades, knowledge
about accidents has evolved considerably. We have left behind the simplistic model of dividing behaviour and conditions into two categories: safe or unsafe.
The rigid belief that any activity can be placed into either category has been put aside as more sophisticated systemic models have been developed and proven
effective in managing safety.

The important observation is that two safe conditions which by themselves are safe, may not be safe together. Workers are the connecting link, as their
behaviour changes according to the environment and their physical surroundings. As an example, power saws caused many accidents when they came into use
in the 1960s due to a hazard known as “kickback”, which takes the operator by surprise when the chain-saw blades hit a branch, knot or harder point in wood.
Kickback killed and injured hundreds of people before a guard was invented to protect the operator. When Sweden implemented regulations requiring the
kickback guard, the number of power saw injuries went down from 2,600 in 1971 to 1,700 in 1972. This was a major breakthrough in power saw accident
prevention.

Every user of power saws knows from personal experience that this noisy, vibrating and obviously sharp cutting tool appears to be very dangerous to use, and
the beginner operator is very cautious. However, after hours of experience operators lose their sense of any hazard and start handling the saw less carefully. The
kickback guard may produce a similar effect. Operators who know kickback is possible try to avoid it. When operators know that there is a mechanical device
preventing the saw from hurting them in event of kickback, they become less cautious. In another forestry industry chain-saw example, studies have shown that
leg protection makes workers less careful and they expose themselves more often to kickbacks, because they believe they are protected.

Despite the fact that kickback protection has helped prevent injuries, the mechanism is not straightforward. Even if these protective arrangements have been
successful, in the final analysis their effects do not have a linear relationship with safety. Two safe conditions, kickback guard and leg protection, do not double
the safety. The normal arithmetic of one plus one equals two (1 + 1 = 2), does not apply in this case, as one plus one makes less than two. Fortunately, one plus
one (1 + 1) makes more than zero in some cases. In other cases, however, the sum may even be negative.

These are phenomena which safety practitioners have started to understand better than before. The simple division of behaviours and conditions into safe and
unsafe does not lead very far toward prevention. The credit for progress has to be given to systems management. After understanding that humans, their tasks,
their equipment and the environment make up a dynamic system, we have made considerable progress towards more effective accident prevention. The
following examples demonstrate the dynamic nature of people and work. If one component is changed, the others do not remain the same, and the ultimate
safety effect is hard to estimate in advance.

In aviation and in other highly engineered and automated systems, we have seen that increased automation may not necessarily result in improved safety. For
example, operators may not get enough practice to maintain their skills in a highly automated system. When they then are required to intervene, they may not
have the necessary competence or ability.

Some paper manufacturers have indicated that younger employees do not understand the functions of a paper machine as well as the older employees. The older
employees have operated non-automated machines, and they have seen how these work. The new automated machines are operated from control rooms through
computer keyboards and screens. The operators do not know the exact location of each component of the machines they operate. Therefore they may bring a
component into a state which, for example, causes a hazard to the maintenance people in the vicinity. A technical improvement in the machinery or controls
without simultaneous improvement in operators’ skills, knowledge and values may not result in improved safety.

Accident prevention has been traditionally based on learning from accidents and near accidents (near misses). By investigating every incident, we learn about
causes and can take actions towards mitigating or removing the causes. The problem is that we have not been able to develop, in the absence of sufficiently
good theories, investigation methods which would bring up all the relevant factors for prevention. An investigation may give a fairly good picture about the
causes. However, this picture is usually relevant only for the specific case investigated. There may be conditions and factors which contributed to the accident
whose connections the investigators do not recognize or understand. Generalizing from one accident to other situations bears a degree of risk.

The good news is that we have made considerable progress in the area of predictive safety management. A number of techniques have been developed and have
become routine for industrial safety and risk analysis. These techniques allow us to study industrial production plants systematically for the identification of
potential hazards and to institute appropriate action before anything happens.

The chemical and petrochemical industries have shown leadership in this area worldwide. As a result of major catastrophes, such as Bhopal and Chernobyl, the
use of the new predictive techniques has increased. Remarkable progress has been made since the mid-1970s in the area of safety. Many governments have also
been leaders in making safety analysis mandatory. Sweden, Finland, Japan and the Federal Republic of Germany have all reduced fatal occupational accidents
by 60 to 70% during this time. Many other countries show similar progress. Now, the challenge is to transfer our knowledge from research into practical
applications and further improve our preventive efforts.

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 1/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention
One of the new steps in safety management is the notion of safety culture. It may be a difficult concept, since culture is not a visible entity. It is an abstract
concept prevailing within an organization or society. There are no direct ways of adjusting it. Safety culture is, however, a crucial concept for understanding the
possibilities of prevention. One of the goals of this edition is to explore this new concept.

This new edition of the Encyclopaedia provides a comprehensive review of accident prevention theories and models in order to develop better design and more
effective preventive strategies. It is possible to prevent occupational accidents. We do not need to tolerate this unnecessary toll to our well-being and economy.

CONCEPTS OF ACCIDENT ANALYSIS


Kirsten Jørgensen

The purpose of this article is to provide a guide for calculating the magnitude of the accident problem; it is not a description of the magnitude itself. In dealing
with occupational accidents, the magnitude of the problem can be estimated in different ways, depending on one’s need to estimate how big the problem has
been or how big it will be in the future. (Some people may say that this distinction is an unnecessary one, since knowledge of the current extent of the problem
will serve to indicate what is to be expected in the future.) The magnitude of the problem as well as its types differs from country to country, from industry to
industry and from workplace to workplace.

An accident may be described as a result of a chain of events in which something has gone wrong, resulting in an undesired conclusion. It has been shown that
human intervention may prevent the injury or damage to which such a chain of events would otherwise lead. However, given the fact of human intervention, the
potential exists for far more dangerous possible chains of events than those actually leading to injury or damage. These possibilities must be considered in
assessing the full extent of workplace risk. Assuming that events that might lead to injury or damage occur because of factors in the workplace, one is led to
conclude that the magnitude of the problem has to be determined on the basis of the existence and frequency of such factors.

When dealing with accidents in the workplace, one can estimate the magnitude of the problem retrospectively by comparing the number of accidents (incidence
rate) with the severity of the accidents (lost work days). However, if one wants to estimate the magnitude of the problem prospectively, it is done by evaluating
the presence of risk factors in the workplace—that is, factors that might lead to accidents.

A sufficiently complete and accurate view of the state of affairs with respect to workplace accidents can be gained by means of a comprehensive reporting and
record-keeping system. Analyses of well-prepared accident reports can give a picture of the basic relationships essential to understanding the causes of the
accidents. In order to estimate the magnitude of the problem in detail, a determination of risk factors is essential. Knowledge of the relevant risk factors can be
obtained by analysing the detailed information provided with each accident record as to where workers and operators were located when the accident occurred,
what they were doing or handling, by what means, what damages or injuries occurred and other particulars surrounding the accident.

Risk
Risk measurement must be made on the basis of information regarding the number and seriousness of injuries that have occurred in the past, yielding a
retrospective measurement. The risk of injury to individuals may be described by two types of data:

· Measurement of risk provides a calculated frequency of injuries and a measurement of the seriousness of the injury. This could be described as the number
of lost work day cases (or fatalities) per number of workers (e.g., in Denmark, the risk of dying in an occupational accident is 3 fatalities per 100,000
employees).

· Type of risk or element of danger assessment provides not only an indication of the exposure sources and other harmful factors which may cause an
accident, but also an indication of the circumstances leading to injury or damage. Work performed at a height, for example, will involve a risk of falling, with
serious injury as a possible result. Similarly, work with cutting tools involves a risk of cuts from contact with sharp components, and work with noisy machines
for a long period of time may result in hearing damage.

There exists a good deal of common-sense knowledge on many types of risks. For example, if you work at a height, you may fall; if it is slippery underfoot,
then you may slip; and if there are sharp objects nearby, you may cut yourself. Many types of risk, not apparent to common sense, may be overlooked. With
regard to these, the worker must be informed of the risk (e.g., that noise causes hearing damage, that some solvents cause brain damage and that certain
chemicals cause acute poisoning by inhalation). Our knowledge of types of risk, from the most to the least conspicuous, whether gained through everyday
experience or through research efforts, is based on past events. However, it is one thing to know what has happened, and another to assess what will happen in
the future. It should be noted that the very knowledge of the exposure sources and other potentially harmful factors which may cause damage or injury in
connection with tasks of various sorts, as well as knowledge of the factors that can either heighten or reduce those risk factors that influence risk measurement,
can provide a basis for recognition of the risk.

Factors Determining Risk

The factors which are of greatest relevance in determining risk are:

· factors which determine the presence or absence (or potential) of risks of any sort

· factors which either increase or minimize the probability of these risks resulting in accidents or injuries

· factors affecting the seriousness of accidents associated with these risks.

To clarify the first point, it is necessary to identify the causes of the accident—namely, exposure sources and other harmful factors; the two latter points
constitute the factors which influence the measurement of risk.

The primary factors in the working environment which are the direct causes of harm, either by way of occupational diseases or occupational accidents, are as
follows:

Exposure sources and occupational disorders

The concept of injuries due to exposure sources is often linked to the concept of disease (or disorder) because a disease can be viewed as caused by exposure to
one or several agents over a short (acute exposure) or long (chronic exposure) period of time. Chronic exposure agents are usually not directly harmful, but take

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 2/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention
effect rather after a relatively constant and extended period of exposure, whereas acute exposures are almost instantaneously harmful. The intensity, harmfulness
and period of action is of importance to the development of the injury, which may often be a matter of a combination of the effects of several different agents.
This fact makes it difficult to point out and determine the exposure sources because (among other reasons) monocausal correlations between specific disorders
and specific exposure sources are almost never found.

Examples of exposure sources which may result in an injury in the form of a disease-like condition are:

· chemical exposures (solvents, cleaning agents, degreasing agents, etc.)

· physical exposures (noise, radiation, heat, cold, inadequate lighting, lack of oxygen, etc.)

· physiological exposures (heavy loads, bad work postures or repetitive work)

· biological exposures (viruses, bacteria, flour, animal blood or leather, etc.)

· psychological exposures (work in isolation, threat of violence, changing working hours, unusual job demands, etc.).

Harmful factors and occupational accidents

The concept of harmful factors (not including exposure sources) is linked to occupational accidents, because this is where damages occur and workers are
exposed to the type of actions that cause instant injury. This type of action is easily identified because the damage or injury is recognized immediately when it
occurs. The difficulty attached to this type of injury is the unexpected contact with the harmful factor.

Examples of harmful factors which may result in persons being injured by an accident are often linked to different energy forms, sources or activity, such as the
following:

· energy that involves cutting, dividing or planing, usually in connection with such types of sharp objects as knives, saws and edged tools

· energy that involves pressing and compressing, usually in connection with different shaping means such as presses and clamping tools

· the conversion of kinetic energy into potential energy—for example, when something hits or falls against a worker

· the conversion of potential energy in a person into kinetic energy, such as occurs in falls from one level to another

· heat and cold, electricity, sound, light, radiation and vibration

· toxic and corrosive substances

· energy exposing the body to excessive stress in such actions, for example, as the moving of heavy loads or twisting of the body

· mental and psychological stresses such as the threat of violence.

Controlling Exposures

Exposure sources or other harmful factors are to a great extent governed by the nature of the processes, technologies, products and equipment to be found in the
workplace, but may also be governed by the way in which the work is organized. From the point of view of measurable risk, it should be recognized that control
of the probability of exposures and the seriousness of injuries to workers often depends on the following three factors:

· Elimination/substitution safety measures. Workplace hazards in the form of exposure sources or other harmful factors may be eliminated or mitigated by
substitution (e.g., a less harmful chemical may replace a toxic chemical in a process). It should be noted that this is not totally possible, as exposure sources and
other harmful factors will always be present in human surroundings (not least with respect to human working conditions).

· Technical safety measures. These measures, often called engineering controls, consist of separating persons from harmful factors by encapsulating the
harmful elements, or installing barriers between workers and the factors which may cause injury. Examples of these measures include, but are not limited to,
automation, remote control, use of ancillary equipment and machine protection (guarding).

· Organizational safety measures. Organizational safety measures, also known as administrative controls, include separating persons from harmful factors
either by means of special working methods or by separation in time or space. Examples of these controls include, but are not limited to, reduced exposure time,
preventive maintenance programmes, encapsulating the individual workers with personal protective equipment, and expedient organization of work.

Controlling Human Conduct


It is often not possible to isolate all hazards using the above control measures. It is commonly supposed that accident prevention analysis ends here because it is
believed that the workers will then be able to take care of themselves by acting “according to the rules”. This means that safety and risk will at some point
depend on factors which control human conduct—namely, whether the individual person has the knowledge, the skills, the opportunity and the will to act so as
to ensure safety in the workplace. The following illustrates the role of these factors.

· Knowledge. Workers must first be aware of the types of risk, potential hazards and elements of danger that may be found in the workplace. This usually
requires education, training and job experience. The risks also need to be identified, analysed, recorded and described in a readily understandable manner so that
workers know when they are in a specific risk situation and what consequences are liable to follow from their actions.

· The opportunity to act. It must be possible for the workers to act safely. It is necessary for workers to be able to make use of the available technical and
organizational—as well as physical and psychological—opportunities for action. Positive support of the safety programme must be forthcoming from
management, supervisors and the surroundings, including concern about risk taking, designing and following working methods with safety in view, safe use of
the proper tools, clearly defining tasks, establishing and following safe procedures, and providing clear instructions on how equipment and materials are to be
safely handled.

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 3/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention
· The will to act safely. Technical and organizational factors are important with respect to workers’ readiness to behave in ways that will ensure workplace
safety, but social and cultural factors are at least equally important. Risks will arise if, for example, safe conduct is difficult or time-consuming, or if it is not
desired by management or colleagues, or is not appreciated by them. Management must be clearly interested in safety, taking steps to prioritize it and displaying
a positive attitude towards the need for safe conduct.

Information on the causes of accidents serves the following purposes:

· It can demonstrate where something is wrong and what needs to be changed.

· It indicates the types of harmful factors that cause accidents (or near accidents) and also describes the situations that result in damage and injuries.

· It identifies and describes the underlying circumstances that determine the presence of potential hazards and risky situations and that will result in optimum
safety by their being altered or eliminated.

Information of a general sort can be obtained by a thorough analysis of the damage or injuries and the circumstances under which they occurred. Information
obtained from other similar accidents may point out more general factors of importance, thus disclosing less immediately visible causal relationships. However,
as very detailed and specific information can be obtained by analysing an individual accident, this information may help uncover the specific circumstances
which should be addressed. Often, analysis of the individual accident will provide information which it is not possible to obtain from the general analysis,
whereas the general analysis may point out factors which the specific analysis does not elucidate. Data from both of these kinds of analyses are important to
help disclose obvious and direct causal relations at the individual level.

Analysis of Individual Accidents


Analysis of individual accidents has two primary purposes:

First, it can be used to determine the cause of an accident and the specific work factors that contributed to it. Following analysis, one can assess the extent to
which the risk has been recognized. One may also decide upon technical and organizational safety measures and the degree to which more job experience might
have diminished the risk. Furthermore, a clearer view is gained of the possible actions that might have been taken to avoid the risk, and the motivation that a
worker must have to take these actions.

Second, one can gain knowledge which may be used for analyses of many similar accidents at both the enterprise level and at more comprehensive (e.g.,
organization-wide or national) levels. In this connection, it is important to assemble information such as the following:

· the identity of the workplace and the work itself (that is, information relating to the sector or the trade in which the workplace is positioned), and the work
processes and the technology that characterize the work

· the nature and the seriousness of the accident

· factors causing the accident, such as exposure sources, the way in which the accident occurred and the specific working situation causing the accident

· general conditions at the workplace and the working situation (comprising the factors mentioned in the foregoing paragraph).

Types of Analyses

There are five primary types of analyses of accidents, each having a distinct purpose:

· Analyses and identification of where and which types of accidents occur. The goal is to determine the incidence of the injuries, as associated, for example,
with sectors, trade groups, enterprises, work processes and types of technology.

· Analyses with respect to monitoring developments in the incidence of accidents. The purpose is to be warned of changes, both positive and negative.
Measuring the effect of preventive initiatives may be the result of such analyses, and increases in new types of accidents within a specified area will constitute
warning of new risk elements.

· Analyses to prioritize initiatives that call for high degrees of risk measurement, which in turn involve calculating the frequency and seriousness of accidents.
The goal is to establish a basis for prioritization to determine where it is more important to carry out preventive measures than elsewhere.

· Analyses to determine how the accidents occurred and, especially, to establish both direct and underlying causes. This information is then applied to the
selection, elaboration and implementation of concrete corrective action and preventive initiatives.

· Analyses for elucidation of special areas which have otherwise attracted attention (a sort of rediscovery or control analyses). Examples include analyses of
incidences of a special injury risk or the discovery of a hitherto unrecognized risk identified in the course of examining an already known risk.

These types of analyses can be carried out at several different levels, ranging from the individual enterprise to the national level. Analyses at several levels will
be necessary for preventive measures. Analyses involving general accident incident rates, monitoring, warning and prioritization will be carried out chiefly at
higher levels, whereas analyses describing direct and underlying accident causes will be conducted at lower levels. The results of the analyses will accordingly
be more specific at the individual level and more general at the higher level.

Phases of the Analysis


Irrespective of the level from which an analysis starts, it will usually have the following phases:

· identification of where the accidents occur at the general level selected

· specification of where the accidents occur at a more specific level within the general level

· determination of goals in view of the incidence (or frequency) and seriousness of the accidents

· description of exposure sources or other harmful factors—that is, the direct causes of damage and injury
www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 4/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention
· examination of the underlying causal relation and causal development.

Examples of different levels of analysis are given in figure 56.1 .

Figure 56.1 Different levels of accident analysis

Summary

Identification of accidents nationwide may provide knowledge of the sectors, trade groups, technologies and working processes within which damages and
injuries occur. The goal is solely to identify where the accidents occurred. Measurement of accidents by frequency and seriousness partly establishes where
something is wrong in particular and partly indicates where the risk has changed.

The type of workplace risk is established by descriptions of the types of accidents that take place and the way in which they arise within the individual
workplace areas. In this way, knowledge is obtained of the exposure sources and other harmful factors present in the workplace in the event that preventive
measures—attention to safety conditions, awareness of risk, providing opportunity for action and the appeal to the workers’ will—have proved insufficient to
avert the accident.

Identification, measurement and description of accidents together provide the basis for what is to be done and who is to do it in order to reduce the risk. If, for
example, specific exposure sources can be linked to specific technologies, it will help determine what special safety measures are necessary to control the risk.
This information may also be used to influence manufacturers and suppliers associated with the technology in question. If it can be demonstrated that frequent
and very serious accidents occur in connection with specific processes, the attempt may be made to adjust the nature of the equipment, machinery, operations or
work procedures that are associated with these processes. Unfortunately, a typical feature of such initiatives and adjustments is that an almost unambiguous
monocausal correlation between accidents and causes is required, and this is available for only a few cases.

Analyses of accidents within an enterprise may also be carried out from a general to a more specific level. However, the problem often is to assemble a
sufficiently extensive database. If accident injury data covering a number of years are gathered at an enterprise (including information regarding minor injuries
and near accidents), it will be possible to establish a useful database even at this level. The overall analysis of the enterprise will show whether there are special
problems in specific sections of the enterprise, or in connection with specific tasks or with the use of specific types of technology. The detailed analysis will
then show what is wrong and thus lead to an evaluation of preventive measures.

If workers’ conduct within a sector, trade group or enterprise, or the conduct of an individual is to be influenced, knowledge regarding many accidents is
required in order to increase workers’ awareness. At the same time, information must be made available about the factors which increase the probability of
accidents and about known possibilities of action that may minimize the risk of damage or injury. At this point, safety becomes a matter of motivating those
responsible for people’s conduct at the level of a given sector, an industrial organization, a trade organization, the employer or the employee.

THEORY OF ACCIDENT CAUSES


Abdul Raouf

Accidents are defined as unplanned occurrences which result in injuries, fatalities, loss of production or damage to property and assets. Preventing accidents is
extremely difficult in the absence of an understanding of the causes of accidents. Many attempts have been made to develop a prediction theory of accident
causation, but so far none has been universally accepted. Researchers from different fields of science and engineering have been trying to develop a theory of
accident causation which will help to identify, isolate and ultimately remove the factors that contribute to or cause accidents. In this article, a brief outline of
various accident causation theories is presented, followed by a structure of accidents.

Accident Causation Theories

The domino theory


www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 5/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention
According to W.H. Heinrich (1931), who developed the so-called domino theory, 88% of all accidents are caused by unsafe acts of people, 10% by unsafe
actions and 2% by “acts of God”. He proposed a “five-factor accident sequence” in which each factor would actuate the next step in the manner of toppling
dominoes lined up in a row. The sequence of accident factors is as follows:

1. ancestry and social environment

2. worker fault

3. unsafe act together with mechanical and physical hazard

4. accident

5. damage or injury.

In the same way that the removal of a single domino in the row would interrupt the sequence of toppling, Heinrich suggested that removal of one of the factors
would prevent the accident and resultant injury; with the key domino to be removed from the sequence being number 3. Although Heinrich provided no data for
his theory, it nonetheless represents a useful point to start discussion and a foundation for future research.

Multiple causation theory

Multiple causation theory is an outgrowth of the domino theory, but it postulates that for a single accident there may be many contributory factors, causes and
sub-causes, and that certain combinations of these give rise to accidents. According to this theory, the contributory factors can be grouped into the following two
categories:

Behavioural. This category includes factors pertaining to the worker, such as improper attitude, lack of knowledge, lack of skills and inadequate physical and
mental condition.

Environmental. This category includes improper guarding of other hazardous work elements and degradation of equipment through use and unsafe procedures.

The major contribution of this theory is to bring out the fact that rarely, if ever, is an accident the result of a single cause or act.

The pure chance theory

According to the pure chance theory, every one of any given set of workers has an equal chance of being involved in an accident. It further implies that there is
no single discernible pattern of events that leads to an accident. In this theory, all accidents are treated as corresponding to Heinrich’s acts of God, and it is held
that there exist no interventions to prevent them.

Biased liability theory

Biased liability theory is based on the view that once a worker is involved in an accident, the chances of the same worker becoming involved in future accidents
are either increased or decreased as compared to the rest of workers. This theory contributes very little, if anything at all, towards developing preventive actions
for avoiding accidents.

Accident proneness theory

Accident proneness theory maintains that within a given set of workers, there exists a subset of workers who are more liable to be involved in accidents.
Researchers have not been able to prove this theory conclusively because most of the research work has been poorly conducted and most of the findings are
contradictory and inconclusive. This theory is not generally accepted. It is felt that if indeed this theory is supported by any empirical evidence at all, it probably
accounts for only a very low proportion of accidents without any statistical significance.

The energy transfer theory

Those who accept the energy transfer theory put forward the claim that a worker incurs injury or equipment suffers damage through a change of energy, and that
for every change of energy there is a source, a path and a receiver. This theory is useful for determining injury causation and evaluating energy hazards and
control methodology. Strategies can be developed which are either preventive, limiting or ameliorating with respect to the energy transfer.

Control of energy transfer at the source can be achieved by the following means:

· elimination of the source

· changes made to the design or specification of elements of the work station

· preventive maintenance.

The path of energy transfer can be modified by:

· enclosure of the path

· installation of barriers

· installation of absorbers

· positioning of isolators.

The receiver of energy transfer can be assisted by adopting the following measures:

· limitation of exposure
www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 6/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention
· use of personal protective equipment.

The “symptoms versus causes” theory

The “symptoms versus causes” theory is not so much a theory as an admonition to be heeded if accident causation is to be understood. Usually, when
investigating accidents, we tend to fasten upon the obvious causes of the accident to the neglect of the root causes. Unsafe acts and unsafe conditions are the
symptoms—the proximate causes—and not the root causes of the accident.

Structure of Accidents

The belief that accidents are caused and can be prevented makes it imperative for us to study those factors which are likely to favour the occurrence of
accidents. By studying such factors, the root causes of accidents can be isolated and necessary steps can be taken to prevent the recurrence of the accidents.
These root causes of accidents can be grouped as “immediate” and “contributing”. The immediate causes are unsafe acts of the worker and unsafe working
conditions. The contributing causes could be management-related factors, the environment and the physical and mental condition of the worker. A combination
of causes must converge in order to result in an accident.

Figure 56.2 shows the structure of accidents, including the details of immediate causes, contributing causes, types of accidents and results of accidents. This
accounting is not exhaustive by any means. However, an understanding of the “cause and effect” relation of the accident-causing factors is required before
continuous improvement of safety processes can be undertaken.

Figure 56.2 Structure of accidents

Summary

Accident causation is very complex and must be understood adequately in order to improve accident prevention. Since safety lacks a theoretical base, it cannot
be regarded as being a science yet. This fact should not discourage us, as most of the scientific disciplines—mathematics, statistics and so on—passed through a
similarly tentative phase at one time or the other. Accident causation study holds great promise for those who are interested in developing the pertinent theory.
At present, theories of accident causation are conceptual in nature and, as such, are of limited use in preventing and controlling accidents. With such a diversity
of theories, it will not be difficult to understand that there does not exist one single theory that is considered right or correct and is universally accepted. These
theories are nonetheless necessary, but not sufficient, for developing a frame of reference for understanding accident occurrences.

HUMAN FACTORS IN ACCIDENT MODELLING


Anne-Marie Feyer and Ann M. Williamson

Human factors are a major component of the causes of accidents in the workplace. Estimates of the actual extent of the involvement vary markedly, but a study
in the early 1980s of the causes of all work-related fatalities occurring in Australia over three years revealed that behavioural factors were involved in more than
90% of fatal accidents. In view of data like these, it is important to have an understanding of the role of human factors in accidents. Traditional models of
accident causation placed superficial emphasis on human factors. Where human factors were included, they were depicted as linked to error occurring in the
immediate sequence of events leading to the accident. A better understanding of how, why and when human factors become involved in accidents enhances our
ability to make predictions about the role of human factors and helps to prevent accidents. A number of models have been put forward that attempt to describe
the part that human factors play in accidents.

Accident Causation Models

Recent models have extended the role of human factors beyond the immediate causal events leading to the accident. Models now tend to include additional
factors in the wider circumstances of the accident. Figure 56.3 shows details of such an approach: For example, human factors, such as work practices and
supervision, can be included both as error in the event sequence leading immediately to the accident and as pre-existing human factors contributing to the

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 7/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention
accident event sequence. The two main components (contributing factors and event sequences) of this human factors model should be envisaged as occurring on
a notional time line on which the order—contributing factors followed by a sequence of errors—is fixed, but the time base on which they occur is not. Both of
these components are essential parts of accident causation.

Figure 56.3 Model of accident causation

The Nature of Error

An essential component of accident prevention, therefore, is gaining an understanding of the nature, timing and causes of error. One of the important and unique
characteristics of error, distinguishing it from other factors involved in accidents, is that error is a normal part of behaviour. Error plays a fundamental role in
learning new skills and behaviours and in maintaining those behaviours. Through testing the boundaries of interactions with the environment, and consequently
making errors, humans learn just what the boundaries are. This is essential not only for learning a new skill but also for updating and maintaining ones they
have already learned. The degree to which humans test the boundaries of their skills is related to the level of risk that they are prepared to accept.

It seems that errors are a constant feature of all behaviour. Studies show also that they occur in the causes of approximately two-thirds of work-related fatal
accidents. It is essential therefore to develop some ideas about the form they are likely to take, and when and why they might occur. While there are many
aspects of human error that are not yet understood, our current level of understanding allows some predictions to be made about error types. Knowledge of these
types of error will, it is to be hoped, guide our efforts to prevent error or at least to modify the adverse consequences of error.

One of the most important features of the nature of error is that it is not a unitary phenomenon. Even though traditional accident analysis often treats error as if it
were a singular entity which cannot be dissected further, there are a number of ways that errors can occur. Errors differ depending on the information-processing
function being challenged. For example, errors can take the form of false sensations due to poor or degraded stimulation of the sensory organs, attentional
failures due to the demands of prolonged or very complex stimulation from the environment, various types of memory lapses, errors of judgement and reasoning
errors. All of these types of errors are distinguishable in terms of the context or task characteristics in which they occur. They signify breakdown in different
information-processing functions and consequently would require different approaches to overcoming each of them.

Different types of error can also be distinguished with respect to skilled and unskilled behaviour. Training is often said to be a solution to problems of human
error since skilled behaviour means that the required sequence of actions can be performed without conscious, constant attention and feedback, requiring only
intermittent conscious checking to ensure that the behaviour is on track. The advantages of skilled behaviour are that once triggered, it requires little effort from
the operator. It allows other activities to be undertaken at the same time (for instance, one can drive an automobile and talk at the same time) and allows the
operator to plan for future aspects of the task. Furthermore, skilled behaviour is usually predictable. Unfortunately, while greater skill reduces the likelihood of
many types of error, it increases the likelihood of others. Errors during skilled behaviour occur as absent-minded or unintended actions or lapses and are distinct
from the mistakes which occur during unskilled behaviour. Skill-based errors tend to be associated with switching in the nature of attentional control of the task.
They can occur during the conscious checking mode or they may be due to conclusion of similar patterns of skilled behaviour.

A second characteristic of errors is that they are not novel or random. Error forms are limited. They take similar forms in all types of functions. For example,
“place losing” errors occur in speech and perceptual tasks as well as in knowledge-related or problem-solving tasks. Similarly, the timing and location of error
in the accident causation sequence does not appear to be random. An important characteristic of information processing is that it is expressed in the same way
regardless of the setting. This means that the error forms that occur in everyday life in the kitchen, for example, occur in the same manner in the most high-risk
industries. The consequences of these errors, however, are very different and are determined by the setting in which the error occurs, rather than by the nature of
the error itself.

Models of Human Error


In the categorization of error and the development of models of human error, it is important to take all aspects of error into account to the extent possible. The
resulting categorization, however, needs to be usable in practice. This is possibly the biggest constraint. What can be done in developing a theory of accident
causation can be very difficult to apply in practice. In attempting to analyse the causes of an accident, or to predict the role of human factors in a process, it is
not possible to understand all aspects of human information processing which contributed or might contribute. It may never be possible, for example, to know
the role of intention before an accident occurred. Even afterwards, the very fact that the accident had occurred can change a person’s recall of the events
surrounding it. The error categorizations that have been most successful to date therefore focus on the nature of the behaviour that was performed at the time the
error occurred. This allows error analysis to be relatively objective and as reproducible as possible.

These categorizations of error distinguish between those occurring during skilled behaviour (slips, lapses or unintended acts) and those occurring during
unskilled or problem-solving behaviour (mistakes).

Slips or skill-based errors are defined as unintended errors occurring when the behaviour is a highly practised routine or automatic in nature.

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 8/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention
Mistakes have been further categorized into two types:

· rule-based errors, which occur when the behaviour requires the application of rules

· knowledge-based errors, which occur during problem solving when the person has no skill or rule to apply.

This means that knowledge-based errors occur through lack of expertise, rule-based errors through the failure to apply the expertise appropriately, and skill-
based errors through a disruption of the execution of the programme of actions, usually due to changes in attentional level (Rasmussen 1982).

Application of these categories in a population study of work-related fatal accidents showed that they could be used reliably. The results showed that skill-based
errors occurred most frequently overall and that occurrences of the three error types were distributed differently across the event sequence. Skill-based errors,
for example, occurred most commonly as the last event immediately before the accident (79% of fatalities). Since, at this point, there is little time for recovery,
their consequences may be more severe. Mistakes, on the other hand, appear to be distributed earlier in the accident sequence.

Human Factors in the Wider Circumstances of Accidents


Elaboration of the involvement of human factors other than human error in the circumstances immediately surrounding the accident represents a major advance
in understanding accident genesis. While there is no question that error is present in most accident sequences, human factors are also involved in a broader
sense, taking the form, for example, of standard operating work procedures and the influences that determine the nature and acceptability of work procedures,
including the earliest decisions of management. Clearly, flawed work procedures and decisions are related to error since they involve errors of judgement and
reasoning. However, flawed work procedures are distinguished by the characteristic that the errors of judgement and reasoning have been permitted to become
standard ways of operating, since, having no immediate consequences, they do not make their presence urgently felt. They are, nevertheless, recognizable as
unsafe work systems with fundamental vulnerabilities that provide the circumstances which may later, unintentionally, interact with human action and lead
directly to accidents.

In this context, the term human factors covers a wide range of elements involved in the interaction between individuals and their working environment. Some of
these are direct and observable aspects of the ways in which work systems function that do not have immediate adverse consequences. Design, use and
maintenance of equipment, the provision, use and maintenance of personal protective and other safety equipment and standard operating procedures originating
from management or workers, or both, are all examples of such ongoing practices.

These observable aspects of human factors in system functioning are to a large extent manifestations of the overall organizational setting, a human element even
more removed from direct involvement in accidents. Characteristics of organizations have been collectively termed organizational culture or climate. These
terms have been used to refer to the set of goals and beliefs an individual holds and the impact of the organization’s goals and beliefs on those of the individual.
Ultimately, the collective or norm values, reflecting the characteristics of the organization, are likely to be influential determinants of attitude and motivation for
safe behaviour at all levels. The level of risk tolerated in a work setting, for example, is determined by such values. Thus, the culture of any organization, clearly
reflected in its work system and the standard operating procedures of its workers, is a crucial aspect of the role of human factors in accident causation.

The conventional view of accidents as a number of things suddenly going wrong at the time and the place of the accident, concentrates attention on the overt
measurable event at the time of the accident. In fact, errors occur in a context which itself may allow the unsafe act or error to have its consequences. In order to
reveal accident causes that originate in pre-existing conditions in work systems, we need to take into account all of the various ways in which the human
element can contribute to accidents.This is perhaps the most important consequence of taking a broad view of the role of human factors in accident causation.
Flawed decisions and practices in work systems, while not having an immediate impact, act to create the setting conducive to operator error—or to the error’s
having consequences—at the time of the accident.

Traditionally, organizational aspects of accidents have been the most neglected aspect of accident analysis design and data collection. Because of their distant
relationship in time from the occurrence of the accident, the causal link between accidents and organizational factors has often not been obvious. Recent
conceptualizations have specifically structured analysis and data collection systems in such a way as to incorporate the organizational components of accidents.
According to Feyer and Williamson (1991), who used one of the first systems designed to specifically include the organizational contribution to accidents, a
considerable proportion of all occupational fatalities in Australia (42.0%) involved pre-existing and ongoing unsafe work practices as a causal factor. Waganaar,
Hudson and Reason (1990), using a similar theoretical framework in which the organizational contribution to accidents was recognized, argued that
organizational and managerial factors constitute latent failures in work systems that are analogous to resident pathogens in biological systems. Organizational
flaws interact with triggering events and circumstances in the immediate circumstances surrounding accidents much as resident pathogens in the body combine
with triggering agents such as toxic factors to bring about disease.

The central notion in these frameworks is that organizational and managerial flaws are present long before the onset of the accident sequence. That is, they are
factors which have a latent or delayed-action effect. Thus, to understand how accidents occur, how people contribute to them and why they behave the way that
they do, it is necessary to ensure that analysis does not begin and end with the circumstances that most directly and immediately lead to harm.

The Role of Human Factors in Accidents and Accident Prevention

In acknowledging the potential aetiological significance of the wider circumstances surrounding the accident, the model best describing accident causation has
to take into account the relative timing of elements and how they relate to each other.

First, causal factors vary in terms of their causal importance, and also in terms of their temporal importance. Furthermore, these two dimensions can vary
independently; that is, causes can be important because they occur very close in time to the accident and therefore they reveal something about the time of the
accident, or they can be important because they are a prime cause underlying the accident, or both. By examining both the temporal and causal importance of
factors involved in the wider circumstances as well as the immediate circumstances of the accident, analysis focuses on why the accident happened, rather than
just describing how it happened.

Second, accidents are generally agreed to be multicausal. Human, technical and environmental components in the work system can interact in critical ways.
Traditionally, accident analysis frameworks have been limited in terms of the range of categories defined. This, in turn, limits the nature of the information
obtained and so limits the range of options highlighted for preventive action. When the wider circumstances of the accident are taken into consideration, the
model has to deal with an even more extensive range of factors. Human factors are likely to interact with other human factors and also with non-human factors.
The patterns of occurrences, co-occurrences and inter-relationships of the wide range of possible different elements within the causal network provides the most
complete and therefore most informative picture of accident genesis.

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 9/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention
Third, these two considerations, the nature of the event and the nature of its contribution to the accident, interact. Although multiple causes are always present,
they are not equivalent in role. Accurate knowledge of the role of factors is the essential key to understanding why an accident happens and how to prevent it
from recurring. For example, immediate environmental causes of accidents may have their impact because of earlier behavioural factors in the form of standard
operating procedures. Similarly, pre-existing aspects of work systems may provide the context in which routine errors committed during skill-based behaviour
can precipitate an accident with harmful consequences. Normally these routine errors would have benign consequences. Effective prevention would be best
served if it were targeted towards the latent underlying causes, rather than the immediately precipitating factors. This level of understanding of the causal
network and how it influences outcome is possible only if all types of factors are included for consideration, their relative timing is examined and their relative
importance is determined.

Despite the potential for an almost infinite variety in the ways that human action can directly contribute to accidents, relatively few patterns of causal pathways
account for the majority of accident causation. In particular, the range of underlying latent conditions which set the scene for later human and other factors to
have their effect are limited predominantly to a small number of aspects of work systems. Feyer and Williamson (1991) reported that only four patterns of
factors accounted for the causes of approximately two-thirds of all occupational fatalities in Australia over a 3-year period. Not surprisingly, almost all of these
involved human factors at some point.

Summary
The nature of human involvement varies as to type and timing and as to its importance in terms of causing the accident (Williamson and Feyer 1990). Most
commonly, human factors in the form of a limited range of pre-existing, flawed work systems create the underlying prime causes of the fatal accidents. These
combine with later lapses during skilled performance or with hazards in environmental conditions to precipitate the accident. These patterns illustrate the
layered role typical of the involvement of human factors in accident genesis. To be of use in preventive strategy formulation, however, the challenge is not to
simply describe the various ways in which the human element is involved but rather to identify where and how it may be possible to intervene most effectively.
This is possible only if the model used has the capacity to describe accurately and comprehensively the complex network of interrelated factors involved in
accident causation, including the nature of the factors, their relative timing and their relative importance.

ACCIDENT MODELS: RISK HOMEOSTASIS


Gerald J.S. Wilde

Give me a ladder that is twice as stable, and I will climb it twice as high. But give me a cause for caution, and I’ll be twice as shy. Consider the following
scenario: A cigarette is invented that causes half the frequency of smoking-related deaths per cigarette smoked as compared to present-day cigarettes, but in all
other ways it is indistinguishable. Does this constitute progress? When the new cigarette replaces the current one, given that there is no change in people’s
desire to be healthy (and that this is the only factor inhibiting smoking), smokers will respond by smoking twice as much. Thus, although the death rate per
cigarette smoked is cut in half, the death risk due to smoking remains the same per smoker. But this is not the only repercussion: the availability of the “safer”
cigarette leads fewer people to stop smoking than presently is the case and seduces more current non-smokers to yield to the temptation to smoke. As a
consequence, the smoking-related death rate in the population increases. However, as people are willing to take no more risks with their health and lives than
they see fit in exchange for the satisfaction of other desires, they will cut down on other, less appealing, unsafe or unhealthy habits. The end result is that the
lifestyle-dependent death rate remains essentially the same.

The above scenario illustrates the following basic premises of risk homeostasis theory (RHT) (Wilde 1988; 1994):

The first is the notion that people have a target level of risk—that is, the level of risk they accept, tolerate, prefer, desire or choose. The target level of risk
depends on perceived benefits and disadvantages of safe and unsafe behaviour alternatives, and it determines the degree to which they will expose themselves to
safety and health hazards.

The second premise is that the actual frequency of lifestyle-dependent death, disease and injury is maintained over time through a closed-loop, self-regulating
control process. Thus, fluctuations in the degree of caution people apply in their behaviour determine the ups and downs in the loss to their health and safety.
Moreover, the ups and downs in the amount of actual lifestyle-dependent loss determine the fluctuations in the amount of caution people exercise in their
behaviour.

Finally, the third premise holds that the level of loss to life and health, in so far as this is due to human behaviour, can be decreased through interventions that
are effective in reducing the level of risk people are willing to take—that is, not through measures of the “safe cigarette” variety or other such efforts towards a
“technological fix” of the problem, but by means of programmes that enhance people’s desire to be alive and healthy.

The Risk Homeostasis Theory of Accident Causation and Prevention

Among the many psychological contributions to the literature on occupational accidents and disease, traffic accidents and lifestyle-dependent ill health, only a
relatively few deal with motivational factors in the causation and the prevention of these problems. Most of the publications deal with variables such as
permanent or semi-permanent traits (e.g., gender, personality or experience), transient states (fatigue, blood-alcohol level), information overload or underload
(stress or boredom ), training and skills, environmental factors and workstation ergonomics. It may be reasoned, however, that all variables other than
motivational ones (i.e., those impinging upon the target level of risk) merely have a marginal influence upon the frequency of accidents per operator-hour of
task execution. Some, though, may well have a favourable effect upon the accident rate per unit of productivity or per unit distance of mobility.

When applied, for instance, to road traffic, RHT posits that the traffic accident rate per time unit of road-user exposure is the output of a closed-loop control
process in which the target level of risk operates as the unique controlling variable. Thus, in contrast with temporary fluctuations, time-averaged accident risk is
viewed as independent of factors such as the physical features of the vehicle and road environment and of operator skills. Instead, it ultimately depends on the
level of accident risk accepted by the road-user population in exchange for the perceived benefits received from motor-vehicle mobility in general (like driving
a lot), and from specific risky acts associated with that mobility in particular (like driving well in excess of the average speed).

Thus, it is reasoned that at any moment of time, vehicle operators, equipped with their perceptual skills, perceive a certain level of accident risk and they
compare this with the amount of accident risk they are willing to accept. The level of the latter is determined by the pattern of trade-offs between expected costs
and benefits associated with the available alternatives for action. Thus, the target level of risk is that level of risk at which the overall utility of manner and
amount of mobility is thought to maximize. The expected costs and benefits are a function of economic, cultural and person-related variables, and their long-
term, short-term and momentary fluctuations. These control the target level of risk at any specific moment of time.

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 10/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention
Whenever road users perceive a discrepancy between target risk and experienced risk in one direction or the other, they will attempt to restore the balance
through some behavioural adjustment. Whether the balance is achieved or not depends upon the individual’s decision-making and psychomotor skills. However,
any action taken carries a certain likelihood of accident risk. The sum total of all actions taken by the road users in a jurisdiction in a given time period (like 1
year), produce the frequency and severity of the traffic accidents in that jurisdiction. It is hypothesized that this accident rate has an influence (through
feedback) upon the level of accident risk perceived by the survivors and thus upon their subsequent actions and subsequent accidents, and so forth. Thus, as
long as the target level of risk remains unchanged, accident toll and behavioural caution determine each other in a circular causal chain.

The Risk Homeostatic Process


This homeostatic process, in which the accident rate is both consequence and cause of changes in operator behaviour, is modelled in figure 56.4 . The self-
correcting nature of the homeostatic mechanism can be recognized in the closed loop that runs from box e to box b, to box c, to box d, and then back to box e. It
may take some time for people to become aware of a change in the accident rate (the feedback may be delayed, and this is symbolized by f). Note that box a is
located outside the closed loop, meaning that interventions that lower that target level of risk can bring about a lasting reduction in the accident rate (box e).

Figure 56.4 Homeostatic model relating changes in accident loss to changes in operator behaviour and vice versa, with the target level of risk as the
controlling variable

The process described herein can be further and quite clearly explained by another example of homeostatic regulation: the thermostatic control of the
temperature in a house. The set temperature (comparable to box a) on the thermostat is at any point in time being compared with the actual temperature (box b).
Whenever there is a difference between the two, there is a need for adjustment (box c), which triggers an adjustment action (i.e., the provision of colder or
warmer air, box d). As a result, the air that is distributed through the house becomes colder (via air conditioning) or warmer (via heating—box e), as desired.
After some time (symbolized by f) the air at the new temperature reaches the point set on the thermostat and gives rise to a new temperature reading, which is
compared with the set-point temperature (box a), and so on.

The house temperature will show major fluctuations if the thermometer is not very sensitive. The same thing will happen when the adjustment action is slow to
set in, either due to inertia of the switching mechanism or to a limited capacity of the heating/cooling system. Note, however, that these deficiencies will not
alter the time-averaged temperature in the house. Note too that the desired temperature (analogous to box a in figure 56.4) is the only factor outside the closed
loop. Resetting the thermostat to a new target temperature will produce durable changes in the time-averaged temperature. Just as a person chooses a target level
of risk on the basis of the perceived benefits and costs of safe and risky behaviour alternatives, so is the target temperature selected in consideration of the
pattern of expected costs and benefits of higher or lower temperatures (e.g., energy expenditures and physical comfort). A lasting discrepancy between target
risk and actual risk can occur only in the case of consistent over- or under-estimation of risk, just as a thermometer that produces a temperature reading that is
consistently too high or too low will cause real temperature to deviate systematically from target temperature.

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 11/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention

Evidence in Support of the Model

It may be deduced from the model described above that the introduction of any accident countermeasure that does not alter the target level of risk is followed by
road users making an estimate of its intrinsic effect upon safety—that is, the change in accident rate that would occur if operator behaviour did not change in
response to the new countermeasure. This estimate will enter into the comparison between perceived and accepted level of risk and thus influence subsequent
adjustment behaviour. If the initial estimates are incorrect on average, a disturbance in the accident rate will occur, but only temporarily, because of the
correcting effect due to the feedback process.

This phenomenon has been discussed in an OECD report. The greater opportunity for safety and the increased level of skill may not be utilized for greater
safety, but instead for improved performance: “Behavioural adaptations of road users which may occur following the introduction of safety measures in the
transport system are of particular concern to road authorities, regulatory bodies and motor vehicle manufacturers particularly in cases where such adaptations
may decrease the expected safety benefit” (OECD 1990). This report mentions numerous examples, as follows:

Taxicabs in Germany equipped with anti-lock brake systems were not involved in fewer accidents than taxis without these brakes, and they were driven in a
more careless manner. Increases in lane width of two-lane highways in New South Wales in Australia have been found to be associated with higher driving
speeds: a speed increase by 3.2 km/h for every 30 cm additional lane width. This was found for passenger cars, while truck speed increased by about 2 km/h for
every 30 cm in lane width. A US study dealing with the effects of lane-width reduction found that drivers familiar with the road reduced their speed by 4.6 km/h
and those unfamiliar by 6.7 km/h. In Ontario it was found that speeds decreased by about 1.7 km/h for each 30 cm of reduction in lane width. Roads in Texas
with paved shoulders as compared to unpaved shoulders were driven at speeds at least 10% higher. Drivers have generally been found to move at a higher speed
when driving at night on roads with clearly painted edge markings.

Recently, a Finnish study investigated the effect of installing reflector posts along highways with an 80 km/h speed limit. Randomly selected road sections
which totalled 548 km were equipped with these posts and compared with 586 km that were not. The installation of reflector posts increased speed in darkness.
There was not even the slightest indication that it reduced the accident rate per km driven on these roads; if anything, the opposite happened (Kallberg 1992).

Numerous other examples could be mentioned. Seat-belt-wearing legislation has not been seen to reduce traffic fatality rates (Adams 1985). Habitual non-users
of seat-belts who were made to buckle up, increased their moving speed and decreased their following distance (Janssen 1994). Following the change-over from
left- to right-hand traffic in Sweden and Iceland, there were initially major reductions in the occurrence of serious accidents, but their rates returned to the pre-
existing trend when road users found out that the roads had not become as dangerous as they thought at first (Wilde 1982). There have been major reductions in
the accident rate per km driven in the course of this century, but the traffic accident rate per head of population has not shown a downward trend (when account
is taken of periods of high unemployment in which the target level of accident risk is reduced; Wilde 1991).

Motivation for Accident Prevention

Interestingly, most of the evidence for the phenomena that are postulated by RHT comes from the area of road traffic, while the prospects this theory holds for
accident prevention have largely been confirmed in occupational settings. In principle, there are four ways in which workers and drivers may be motivated to
lower their target level of risk:

· Reduce the expected benefits of risky behaviour alternatives.

· Increase the expected costs of risky behaviour alternatives.

· Increase the expected benefits of safe behaviour alternatives.

· Decrease the expected costs of safe behaviour alternatives.

While some of these approaches have been found to be more effective than others, the notion that safety may be enhanced by acting upon motivation has a long
history, as is obvious from the universal presence of punitive law.

Punishment

Although enforcement of punitive law is one of society’s traditional attempts at motivating people towards safety, the evidence for its effectiveness has not been
forthcoming. It suffers from several other problems as well, some of which have been identified in the context of organizational psychology (Arnold 1989).

First is the “self-fulfilling prophecy” effect of attribution. For example, labelling people with undesirable characteristics may stimulate individuals to behave as
if they had these characteristics. Treat people as if they were irresponsible and eventually some will behave as if they were.

Second, the emphasis is on process controls; i.e., on specific behaviours such as using a piece of safety equipment or obeying the speed limit, instead of
focusing on the end result, which is safety. Process controls are cumbersome to design and implement, and they can never totally encompass all undesirable
specific behaviours of all people at all times.

Third, punishment brings negative side-effects. Punishment creates a dysfunctional organizational climate, marked by resentment, uncooperativeness,
antagonism and even sabotage. As a result, the very behaviour that was to be prevented may in fact be stimulated.

Encouragement

In contrast to punishment, incentive programmes have the effect for which they are intended, as well as the positive side-effect of creating a favourable social
climate (Steers and Porter 1991). The effectiveness of incentives and recognition programmes in enhancing safety has been clearly established. In a recent
review of over 120 published evaluations of different types of occupational accident prevention, incentives and recognition were generally found more effective
towards safety than engineering improvements, personnel selection and other types of intervention which included disciplinary action, special licensing, and
exercise and stress-reduction programmes (Guastello 1991).

Behavioural Adaptation

According to risk homeostasis theory, the accident rate per person-hour of task performance or the annual accident rate per head of population do not primarily
depend upon a person’s ability to be safe, nor upon the opportunity to be safe, but instead upon that person’s desire to be safe. Thus, it is reasoned that, although

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 12/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention
education and engineering may provide the ability or the opportunity for greater safety, these approaches to accident prevention will fail to reduce the accident
rate per hour, because they do not reduce the amount of risk people are willing to take. The response to these interventions, therefore, will usually take the form
of some behavioural adjustment in which the potential safety advantage is in fact consumed as an addition to performance in terms of greater productivity, more
mobility and/or higher speed of mobility.

This can be explained as the consequence of a homeostatic control process in which the degree of behavioural caution determines the accident rate and the
accident rate determines the extent of caution in operator behaviour. In this closed-loop process, the target level of risk is the only independent variable that
ultimately explains the accident rate. The target level of risk depends on the person’s perception of the advantages and disadvantages of various action
alternatives. To maintain that safety is its own reward is to ignore the fact that people knowingly take risks for various contingencies that are open to
modification.

Therefore, of all accident countermeasures that are currently available, those that enhance people’s motivation towards safety seem to be the most promising.
Furthermore, of all countermeasures that affect people’s motivation towards safety, those that reward people for accident-free performance seem to be the most
effective. According to McAfee and Winn’s literature review: “The major finding was that every study, without exception, found that incentives or feedback
enhanced safety and/or reduced accidents in the workplace, at least in the short term. Few literature reviews find such consistent results” (1989).

Summary
Of all possible schemes that reward people for accident-free performance, some promise better results than others because they contain the elements that appear
to enhance motivation towards safety. Examples of empirical evidence for the risk-homeostatic process have been selected from the larger information base
(Wilde 1994), while the ingredients for effective incentive programming have been discussed in greater detail in Chapter 60.16. The under-reporting of
accidents has been mentioned as the only identified negative side-effect of incentive schemes. This phenomenon, however, is limited to minor accidents. It may
be possible to conceal a broken finger; it is more difficult to hide a corpse.

ACCIDENT MODELLING
Andrew R. Hale

Humans play important roles in most of the processes leading up to accidents and in the majority of measures aimed at accident prevention. Therefore, it is vital
that models of the accident process should provide clear guidance about the links between human actions and accidents. Only then will it be possible to carry
out systematic accident investigation in order to understand these links and to make predictions about the effect of changes in the design and layout of
workplaces, in the training, selection and motivation of workers and managers, and in the organization of work and management safety systems.

Early Modelling

Up until the 1960s, modelling human and organizational factors in accidents had been rather unsophisticated. These models had not differentiated human
elements relevant to accidents beyond rough subdivisions such as skills, personality factors, motivational factors and fatigue. Accidents were seen as
undifferentiated problems for which undifferentiated solutions were sought (as doctors two centuries ago sought to cure many then undifferentiated diseases by
bleeding the patient).

Reviews of accident research literature that were published by Surry (1969) and by Hale and Hale (1972) were among the first attempts to go deeper and offer a
basis for classifying accidents into types reflecting differentiated aetiologies, which were themselves linked to failures in different aspects of the man-
technology-environment relationships. In both of these reviews, the authors drew upon the accumulating insights of cognitive psychology in order to develop
models presenting people as information processors, responding to their environment and its hazards by trying to perceive and control the risks that are present.
Accidents were considered in these models as failures of different parts of this process of control that occur when one or more of the control steps does not
perform satisfactorily. The emphasis was also shifted in these models away from blaming the individual for failures or errors, and towards focusing on the
mismatch between the behavioural demands of the task or system and the possibilities inherent in the way behaviour is generated and organized.

Human Behaviour

Later developments of these models by Hale and Glendon (1987) linked them to the work of Rasmussen and Reason (Reason 1990), which classified human
behaviour into three levels of processing:

· automatic, largely unconscious responses to routine situations (skill-based behaviour)

· matching learned rules to a correct diagnosis of the prevailing situation (rule-based behaviour)

conscious and time-consuming problem solving in novel situations (knowledge-based behaviour).

The typical failures of control differ from one level of behaviour to another, as do the types of accidents and the appropriate safety measures used to control
them. The Hale and Glendon model, updated with more recent insights, is depicted in figure 56.5 . It is made up of a number of building blocks which will be
explained successively in order to arrive at the full model.

Figure 56.5 Individual problem solving in the face of danger

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 13/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention

Link to deviation models

The starting point of the Hale and Glendon model is the way in which danger evolves in any workplace or system. Danger is considered to be always present,
but kept under control by a large number of accident-prevention measures linked to hardware (e.g., the design of equipment and safeguards), people (e.g.,
skilled operators), procedures (e.g., preventive maintenance) and organization (e.g., allocation of responsibility for critical safety tasks). Provided that all
relevant dangers and potential hazards have been foreseen and the preventive measures for them have been properly designed and chosen, no damage will occur.
Only if a deviation from this desired, normal state takes place can the accident process start. (These deviation models are dealt with in detail in “Accident
deviation models”.)

The task of the people in the system is to assure proper functioning of the accident-prevention measures so as to avert deviations, by using the correct
procedures for each eventuality, handling safety equipment with care, and undertaking the necessary checks and adjustments. People also have the task of
detecting and correcting many of the deviations which may occur and of adapting the system and its preventive measures to new demands, new dangers and
new insights. All these actions are modelled in the Hale and Glendon model as detection and control tasks related to a danger.

Problem solving

The Hale and Glendon model conceptualizes the role of human action in controlling danger as a problem-solving task. The steps in such a task can be described
generically as in figure 56.6 .

Figure 56.6 Problem-solving cycle

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 14/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention

This task is a goal-seeking process, driven by the standards set in step one in figure 56.6 . These are the standards of safety which workers set for themselves, or
which are set by employers, manufacturers or legislators. The model has the advantage that it can be applied not only to individual workers faced with imminent
or future danger, but also to groups of workers, departments or organizations aiming to control both existing danger from a process or industry and future danger
from new technology or products at the design stage. Hence safety management systems can be modelled in a consistent way with human behaviour, allowing
the designer or evaluator of safety management to take an appropriately focused or a wide view of the interlocking tasks of different levels of an organization
(Hale et al. 1994).

Applying these steps to individual behaviour in the face of danger we obtain figure 56.7 . Some examples of each step can clarify the task of the individual.
Some degree of danger, as stated above, is assumed to be present all the time in all situations. The question is whether an individual worker responds to that
danger. This will depend partly on how insistent the danger signals are and partly on the worker’s own consciousness of danger and standards of acceptable
level of risk. When a piece of machinery unexpectedly glows red hot, or a fork-lift truck approaches at high speed, or smoke starts seeping from under the door,
individual workers skip immediately to considering the need for action, or even to deciding what they or someone else can do.

Figure 56.7 Behaviour in the face of danger

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 15/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention

These situations of imminent danger are rare in most industries, and it is normally desirable to activate workers to control danger when it is much less imminent.
For example, workers should recognize slight wear on the machine guard and report it, and realize that a certain noise level will make them deaf if they are
continuously exposed to it for some years. Designers should anticipate that a novice worker could be liable to use their proposed new product in a way that
could be dangerous.

To do this, all persons responsible for safety must first consider the possibility that danger is or will be present. Consideration of danger is partly a matter of
personality and partly of experience. It can also be encouraged by training and guaranteed by making it an explicit part of tasks and procedures at the design and
execution phases of a process, where it may be confirmed and encouraged by colleagues and superiors. Secondly, workers and supervisors must know how to
anticipate and recognize the signs of danger. To ensure the appropriate quality of alertness, they must accustom themselves to recognize potential accident
scenarios—that is, indications and sets of indications that could lead to loss of control and so to damage. This is partly a question of understanding webs of
cause and effect, such as how a process can get out of control, how noise damages hearing or how and when a trench can collapse.

Just as important is an attitude of creative mistrust. This involves considering that tools, machines and systems can be misused, go wrong, or show properties
and interactions outside their designers’ intentions. It applies “Murphy’s Law” (whatever can go wrong will go wrong) creatively, by anticipating possible
failures and affording the opportunity of eliminating or controlling them. Such an attitude, together with knowledge and understanding, also helps at the next
step—that is, in really believing that some sort of danger is sufficiently likely or serious to warrant action.

Labelling something as dangerous enough to need action is again partly a matter of personality; for instance, it may have to do with how pessimistic a person
may be about technology. More importantly, it is very strongly influenced by the kind of experience that will prompt workers to ask themselves such questions
as, “Has it gone wrong in the past?” or “Has it worked for years with the same level of risk with no accidents?” The results of research on risk perception and on
attempts to influence it by risk communication or feedback on accident and incident experience are given in more detail in other articles.

Even if the need for some action is realized, workers may take no action for many reasons: they do not, for example, think it is their place to interfere with
someone else’s work; they do not know what to do; they see the situation as unchangeable (“it is just part of working in this industry”); or they fear reprisal for
reporting a potential problem. Beliefs and knowledge about cause and effect and about the attribution of responsibility for accidents and accident prevention are
important here. For example, supervisors who consider that accidents are largely caused by careless and accident-prone workers will not see any need for action
on their own part, except perhaps to eliminate those workers from their section. Effective communications to mobilize and coordinate the people who can and
should take action are also vital at this step.
www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 16/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention
The remaining steps are concerned with the knowledge of what to do to control the danger, and the skills needed to take appropriate action. This knowledge is
acquired by training and experience, but good design can help greatly by making it obvious how to achieve a certain result so as to avert danger or to protect
one’s self from it—for instance, by means of an emergency stop or shutdown, or an avoiding action. Good information resources such as operations manuals or
computer support systems can help supervisors and workers to gain access to knowledge not available to them in the course of day-to-day activity. Finally, skill
and practice determine whether the required response action can be carried out accurately enough and with the right timing to make it successful. A difficult
paradox arises in this connection: the more alert and prepared that people are, and the more reliable the hardware is, the less frequently the emergency
procedures will be needed and the harder it will be to sustain the level of skill needed to carry them out when they are called upon.

Links with behaviour based on skill, rules and knowledge

The final element in the Hale and Glendon model, which turns figure 56.7 into figure 56.5, is the addition of the link to the work of Reason and Rasmussen.
This work emphasized that behaviour can be evinced at three different levels of conscious control—skill-based, rule-based and knowledge-based—which
implicate different aspects of human functioning and are subject to different types and degrees of disturbance or error on account of external signals or internal
processing failures.

Skill-based. The skill-based level is highly reliable, but subject to lapses and slips when disturbed, or when another, similar routine captures control. This level
is particularly relevant to the kind of routine behaviour that involves automatic responses to known signals indicating danger, either imminent or more remote.
The responses are known and practised routines, such as keeping our fingers clear of a grinding wheel while sharpening a chisel, steering a car to keep it on the
road, or ducking to avoid a flying object coming at us. The responses are so automatic that workers may not even be aware that they are actively controlling
danger with them.

Rule-based. The rule-based level is concerned with choosing from a range of known routines or rules the one which is appropriate to the situation—for example,
choosing which sequence to initiate in order to close down a reactor which would otherwise become overpressurized, selecting the correct safety goggles to
work with acids (as opposed to those for working with dusts ), or deciding, as a manager, to carry out a full safety review for a new plant rather than a short
informal check. Errors here are often related to insufficient time spent matching the choice to the real situation, to relying on expectation rather than observation
to understand the situation, or to being misled by outside information into making a wrong diagnosis. In the Hale and Glendon model, behaviour at this level is
particularly relevant to detecting hazards and choosing correct procedures in familiar situations.

Knowledge-based. The knowledge-based level is engaged only when no pre-existing plans or procedures exist for coping with a developing situation. This is
particularly true of the recognition of new hazards at the design stage, of detecting unsuspected problems during safety inspections or of coping with unforeseen
emergencies. This level is predominant in the steps at the top of figure 56.5. It is the least predictable and least reliable mode of operation, but also the mode
where no machine or computer can replace a human in detecting potential danger and in recovering from deviations.

Putting all the elements together results in figure 56.5, which provides a framework for both classifying where failures occurred in human behaviour in a past
accident and analysing what can be done to optimize human behaviour in controlling danger in a given situation or task in advance of any accidents.

ACCIDENT SEQUENCE MODELS


Ragnar Andersson

This article covers a group of accident models that all share the same basic design. The interplay between human, machine and environment, and the
development of this interplay into potential hazards, dangers, damages and injuries, is envisaged by means of a sequence of questions derived and listed in a
logical order. This sequence is then applied in a similar manner at different levels of analysis through the use of models. The first of these models was presented
by Surry (1969). A few years later, a modified version was presented by the Swedish Work Environment Fund (1983) and received the fund’s sobriquet, WEF.
A Swedish research team then evaluated the WEF model and suggested some further developments resulting in a third model.

These models are herein described one by one, with comments on the reasons for the changes and developments undertaken. Finally, a tentative synthesis of the
three models is proposed. Thus, a total of four models, with considerable similarities, are presented and discussed. Although this may seem confusing, it
illustrates the fact that there is no model that is universally accepted as “The Model”. Among other things, there is an obvious conflict between simplicity and
completeness with regard to accident models.

Surry’s Model

In 1969, Jean Surry published the book Industrial Accident Research—A Human Engineering Appraisal. This book contains a review of models and approaches
predominantly applied in accident research. Surry grouped the theoretical and conceptual frameworks she identified into five different categories: (1) chain-of-
multiple-events models, (2) epidemiological models, (3) energy-exchange models, (4) behaviour models, and (5) systems models. She concluded that none of
these models is incompatible with any of the others; each simply stresses different aspects. This inspired her to combine the various frameworks into one
comprehensive and general model. She made it clear, though, that her model should be regarded as provisional, without any pretensions of finality.

In Surry’s view, an accident can be described by a series of questions, forming a sequential hierarchy of levels, where the answers to each question determine
whether an event turns out as an accident or not. Surry’s model (see figure 56.8) reflects the principles of human information processing, and is based upon the
notion of an accident as a deviation from an intended process. It has three principal stages, linked by two similar cycles.

Figure 56.8 Surry’s model

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 17/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention

The first stage views human beings in their total environment, including all relevant environmental and human parameters. The potential injury agent is also
described at this stage. It is assumed that, through the actions (or non-actions) of the individual, dangers grow out of such an environment. For the purpose of
analysis a “danger-build-up” cycle is constituted by the first sequence of questions. If there are negative responses to any of these questions, the danger in
question will become imminent.

The second sequence of questions, the “danger-release cycle”, links the level of danger to possible alternative outcomes when the danger is triggered. It should
be noted that by following different routes through the model, it is possible to distinguish between deliberate (or consciously accepted) dangers and
unintentional negative outcomes. The differences between “accident-like” unsafe acts, mishaps (and so on) and completed accidents are also clarified by the
model.

The WEF Model


In 1973, a committee set up by the Swedish Work Environment Fund to review the state of occupational accident research in Sweden launched a “new” model
and promoted it as a universal tool that should be employed for all research in this field. It was announced as a synthesis of existing behavioural,
epidemiological and systems models, and was also said to encompass all relevant aspects of prevention. Reference was made to Surry, among others, but
without mention of the fact that the proposed model was almost identical to hers. Only a few changes had been made, all for the purpose of improvement.

As often occurs when scientific models and perspectives are recommended by central agencies and authorities, the model is subsequently adopted in only a few
projects. Nevertheless, the report issued by the WEF contributed to a rapidly rising interest in modelling and theory development among Swedish and
Scandinavian accident researchers, and several new accident models emerged within a short period.

The point of departure in the WEF model (in contrast to Surry’s “man and environment” level) lies in the concept of danger, here limited to “objective danger”
as opposed to the subjective perception of danger. Objective danger is defined as an integral part of a given system, and is basically determined by the amount
of resources available for investment in safety. Increasing a system’s tolerance to human variability is mentioned as a way of reducing danger.

When an individual comes into contact with a certain system and its dangers, a process begins. Due to system features and individual behaviour, a risk situation
may arise. What is most important (as regards the properties of systems) according to the authors, is how dangers are indicated through various kinds of signals.
Risk imminence is determined depending on the individual’s perception, understanding and actions in relation to these signals.

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 18/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention
The next sequence in the process, which is in principle identical to Surry’s, is directly related to the event and whether it will lead to injury or not. If the danger
is released, can it be, in fact, observed? Is it perceived by the individual in question and is he or she able to avoid injury or damage? Answers to such questions
explain the kind and degree of harmful outcomes that emanate from the critical period.

The WEF model (figure 56.9) was seen as having four advantages:

· It makes clear that safe working conditions presuppose the taking of action at the earliest possible stage.

· It illustrates the importance of working with disturbances and near-accidents, as well as those accidents that lead to damage or injury. Actual outcome is of
less importance in a preventive perspective.

· It describes the principles of human information processing.

· It provides for self-correcting safety systems by means of the feedback of results from studied incidents.

Figure 56.9 The WEF model

Evaluation and Further Development

By the time the WEF report was issued, an epidemiological study on occupational accidents was under way in the city of Malmö, Sweden. The study was based
on a modified version of the so-called Haddon Matrix, which cross-tabulates variables along two dimensions: time in terms of pre-accident, accident and post-
accident phases; and the epidemiological trichotomy of host, agent (or vehicle/vector) and environment. Although such a model provides a good basis for data
collection, it was found by the research team to be insufficient to understand and explain the causal mechanisms underlying accident and injury phenomena. The
WEF model appeared to represent a new approach, and was therefore received with great interest. It was decided to conduct an immediate evaluation of the
model by testing it on a random selection of 60 actual cases of occupational accidents that had been previously thoroughly investigated and documented by the
Malmö group as a part of its ongoing study.

The results of the evaluation were summarized in four points:

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 19/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention
· The model is not the comprehensive tool it was expected to be. Instead, it should be regarded primarily as a behavioural model. The “danger” is given, and
the analysis focuses on the individual’s behaviour in relation to that danger. Consequently, preventive options derived from such analysis are oriented to human
factors rather than to equipment or environment. The “danger” as such is hardly questioned within the framework of the model.

· The model does not take due consideration of technical or organizational constraints in the work process. It creates an illusion of free choice between
dangerous and safe alternatives. Some dangers were actually found to be unavoidable by the individual worker, although they were clearly avoidable by
management. Hence, it becomes irrelevant and misleading to ask whether people know how to avoid (and choose to avoid) something which is not really
avoidable unless they decide to quit their job.

· The model provides no insight into the important issue of why the dangerous activity was necessary to begin with, and why it was performed by the specific
individual. Sometimes dangerous tasks can be made unnecessary; and sometimes they can be performed by other, more appropriate persons with greater skills.

· The analysis is restricted to a single person, but many accidents occur in interaction between two or more persons. However, it was suggested that this
deficiency could be overcome by combining the results of parallel analyses, each made from the perspective of one of the different individuals involved.

Based on these remarks, the model was further developed by the research group in Malmö. The most important innovation was the addition of a third sequence
of questions to complement the other two. This sequence was designed to analyse and explain the existence and nature of “danger” as an inherent feature of a
human-machine system. General principles from systems theory and control technology were applied.

Moreover, the work process, thus understood in terms of human-machine-environment interaction, should also be seen in the light of its organizational and
structural contexts at both company and societal levels. The need to take personal characteristics and motives for the actual activity into consideration, as well as
for the individual to carry out that activity, was also indicated. (See figure 56.10.)

Figure 56.10 The WEF model developed by introduction of a new first sequence

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 20/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention

Summary
In reconsidering these early models today, more than twenty years later, against the background of progress made concerning theories and models in accident
research, they still seem surprisingly up-to-date and competitive.

The basic underlying assumption of the models—that accidents, as well as their causes, should be seen as deviations from intended processes—is still a
dominant perspective (see, inter alia, Benner 1975; Kjellén and Larsson 1981).

The models make a clear distinction between the concept of injury as a health outcome and the concept of accident as a preceding occurrence. Moreover, they
demonstrate that an accident is not just an “event”, but rather a process which can be analysed as a series of steps (Andersson 1991).

Many subsequent models have been designed as a number of “boxes”, organized in temporal or hierarchical order, and indicating various temporal phases or
levels of analysis. Examples of these include the ISA model (Andersson and Lagerlöf 1983), the deviation model (Kjellén and Larsson 1981) and the so-called
Finnish model (Tuominen and Saari 1982). Such levels of analysis are also clearly central to the models described here. But the sequence models also propose a
theoretical instrument for analysing the mechanisms which link these levels together. Important contributions in this respect have been made by authors such as
Hale and Glendon (1987) from a human factors perspective, and Benner (1975) from a systems point of view.

As clearly emerges when comparing these models, Surry did not give a key position to the concept of danger, as is done in the WEF model. Her starting point
was the human-environment interaction, reflecting a broader approach similar to that suggested by the Malmö group. On the other hand, like the WEF
committee, she did not refer to any further levels of analysis beyond the worker and environment, such as organizational or societal levels. Further, the
comments from the Malmö study cited here in relation to the WEF model also seem relevant to Surry’s model.

A modern synthesis of the three models presented above might include fewer details on human information processing and more information on “upstream”
conditions (further back in the casual “flow”) at organizational and societal levels. Key elements in a sequence of questions designed to address the relationship
between the organizational and human-machine levels might be derived from modern principles of safety management, involving quality assurance

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 21/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention
methodologies (internal control and so on). Similarly, a sequence of questions for the connection between societal and organizational levels might involve
modern principles of systems-oriented supervision and auditing. A tentative comprehensive model, based on Surry’s original design and including these
additional elements, is outlined in figure 56.11 .

Figure 56.11 Tentative comprehensive model on accident causation (based on Surry 1969 and descendants)

ACCIDENT DEVIATION MODELS


Urban Kjellén

An occupational accident may be regarded as an abnormal or unwanted effect of the processes in an industrial system, or something which does not work as
planned. Unwanted effects other than personal injury are also possible, such as material damage, accidental release of pollution to the environment, time delay
or reduced product quality. The deviation model is rooted in systems theory. When applying the deviation model, accidents are analysed in terms of deviations.

Deviations

The definition of deviations in relation to specified requirements coincides with the definition of nonconformities in the International Organization for
Standardization’s ISO 9000 series of standards on quality management (ISO 1994). The value of a systems variable is classified as a deviation when it falls
outside a norm. Systems variables are measurable characteristics of a system, and they can assume different values.

Norms
There are four different types of norms. These relate to: (1) specified requirements, (2) what has been planned, (3) what is normal or usual and (4) what is
accepted. Each type of norm is characterized by the way it has been established and its degree of formalization.

Safety regulations, rules and procedures are examples of specified requirements. A typical example of a deviation from a specified requirement is a “human
error”, which is defined as a transgression of a rule. The norms that relate to what is “normal or usual” and what is “accepted” are less formalized. They are
www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 22/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention
typically applied in industrial settings, where the planning is oriented to outcome and the execution of the work is left to the discretion of the operators. An
example of a deviation from an “accepted” norm is an “incidental factor”, which is an unusual event that may (or may not) result in an accident (Leplat 1978). A
further example is an “unsafe act”, which traditionally was defined as a personal action violating a commonly accepted safe procedure (ANSI 1962).

Systems Variables
In the application of the deviation model, the set or range of values of systems variables is divided into two classes, namely, normal and deviation. The
distinction between normal and deviation may be problematic. Differences of opinion about what is normal may arise, for example, among workers,
supervisors, management and systems designers. Another problem relates to the lack of norms in work situations which have not been encountered before
(Rasmussen, Duncan and Leplat 1987). These differences of opinion and the lack of norms may in themselves contribute to an increased risk.

The Time Dimension

Time is a basic dimension in the deviation model. An accident is analysed as a process rather than as a single event or a chain of causal factors. The process
develops through consecutive phases, so that there is a transition from normal conditions in the industrial system to abnormal conditions or a state of lack of
control. Subsequently, a loss of control of energies in the system occurs and the damage or injury develops. Figure 56.12 shows an example of the analysis of
an accident based on a model developed by the Occupational Accident Research Unit (OARU) in Stockholm, in relation to these transitions.

Figure 56.12 Analysis of an accident at a construction site on the basis of the OARU model

Focus on Accident Control

Each accident model has a unique focus, which is linked to an accident-prevention strategy. The deviation model puts the focus on the initial phase of the
accident sequence, which is characterized by the state of abnormal conditions or lack of control. Accident prevention is accomplished through feedback where
established information systems for production planning and control and safety management are used. The aim is to conduct a smooth operation with as few
disturbances and improvisations as possible, so as to not increase the risk of accidents.

A distinction is made between corrective and preventive actions. Correction of deviations coincides with the first order of feedback in Van Court Hare’s
hierarchy of feedback, and does not result in any organizational learning from the accident experiences (Hare 1967). Preventive actions are accomplished
through higher orders of feedback that involve learning. An example of a preventive action is the development of new work instructions based on commonly
shared norms about safe work routines. In general, there are three different aims of preventive actions: (1) to reduce the probability of deviations, (2) to reduce
the consequences of deviations and (3) to reduce the time from the occurrence of deviations to their identification and correction.

To illustrate the characteristics of the deviation model, a comparison is made with the energy model (Haddon 1980) which directs the focus of accident
prevention on the later phases of the accident process—that is, the loss of control of energies and subsequent harm. Accident prevention is typically
accomplished through limitation or control of energies in the system or by interposing barriers between the energies and the victim.

Taxonomies of Deviations
There are different taxonomies for the classification of deviations. These have been developed to simplify the collection, processing and feedback of data on
deviations. Table 56.1 presents an overview.

Table 56.1 Examples of taxonomies for the classification of deviations

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 23/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention

Theory or model and variable Classes


Process model
Duration Event/act, condition
Phase of the accident sequence Initial phase, concluding phase, injury phase
Systems theory
Subject-object (Act of) person, mechanical/physical condition
Systems ergonomics Individual, task, equipment, environment
Industrial engineering Materials, labour power, information, technical,
human, intersecting/parallel activities, stationary
guards, personal protective equipment
Human errors
Human actions Omission, commission, extraneous act, sequential
error, time error
Energy model
Type of energy Thermal, radiation, mechanical, electrical, chemical
Type of energy control system Technical, human
Consequences
Type of loss No significant time loss, degraded output quality,
equipment damage, material loss, environmental
pollution, personal injury
Extent of loss Negligible, marginal, critical, catastrophic

Source: Kjellén 1984.

A classical taxonomy of deviations is the distinction between “unsafe act of persons” and “unsafe mechanical/physical conditions” (ANSI 1962). This
taxonomy combines a classification with respect to duration and the subject-object split. The OARU model is based on an industrial engineering systems view
(Kjellén and Hovden 1993) wherein each class of deviations is related to a typical system for production control. It follows, for example, that deviations related
to work materials are controlled through material control, and technical deviations are controlled through inspection and maintenance routines. Stationary
guards are typically controlled through safety inspections. Deviations that describe the loss of control of energies are characterized by the type of energy
involved (Haddon 1980). A distinction is also made between failures in human and technical systems for the control of energies (Kjellén and Hovden 1993).

The Validity of the Deviation Concept


No general relationships exist between deviations and the risk of injury. Research results suggest, however, that some types of deviations are associated with an
increased risk of accidents in certain industrial systems (Kjellén 1984). These include defective equipment, production disturbances, irregular workload and
tools used for unusual purposes. The type and amount of energy that is involved in the uncontrolled energy flow are fairly good predictors of the consequences.

Application of the Deviation Model

Data on deviations are collected in safety inspections, safety sampling, near-accident reporting and accident investigations. (See figure 56.13.)

Figure 56.13 The coverage of different tools for use in safety practice

For example, Safety sampling is a method for the control of deviations from safety rules through performance feedback to the workers. Positive effects of safety
sampling on safe performance, as measured by the risk of accidents, have been reported (Saari 1992).

The deviation model has been applied in the development of tools for use in accident investigations. In the incidental factors analysis method, deviations of the
accident sequence are identified and arranged in a logical tree structure (Leplat 1978). The OARU model has been the basis for the design of accident
investigation forms and checklists and for the structuring of the accident investigation procedure. Evaluation research shows that these methods support a
comprehensive and reliable charting and evaluation of deviations (see Kjellén and Hovden 1993 for a review). The deviation model has also inspired the
development of methods for risk analysis.

Deviation analysis is a risk analysis method and encompasses three steps: (1) the summarizing of systems functions and operator activities and their division
into subsections, (2) the examination of each activity to identify possible deviations and to assess the potential consequences of each deviation and (3) the
development of remedies (Harms-Ringdahl 1993). The accident process is modelled as illustrated by figure 56.12, and the risk analysis covers all three phases.
Checklists similar to those applied in accident investigations are used. It is possible to integrate this method with design tasks; it is further effective in
identifying needs for remedial actions.

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 24/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention

Summary

Deviation models focus on the early part of the accident process, where there are disturbances in the operation. Prevention is accomplished through feedback
control in order to achieve a smooth operation with few disturbances and improvisations that may result in accidents.

MAIM: THE MERSEYSIDE ACCIDENT INFORMATION MODEL


Harry S. Shannon and John Davies

Generally speaking, the term accident is used to denote events resulting in undesired or unplanned physical injury or damage; an accident model is a conceptual
scheme applied to the analysis of such events. (Some models may explicitly declare that “near accidents”—sometimes known as “near misses”—are covered by
the model; however, the distinction is not important to this article.) Models of accidents can serve different purposes. First, they may provide a conceptual
understanding of how accidents occur. Second, models may be used to record and store information on accidents. Third, they may provide a mechanism for
investigating accidents. These three aims are not entirely distinct, but form a useful means of categorization.

This article describes MAIM, the Merseyside Accident Information Model, which is most naturally adapted to the second purpose—recording and storing
accident information. Following an outline of the rationale for MAIM, some early studies evaluating the model are described. The article ends with recent
progress with MAIM, including the use of “intelligent software” to collect and analyse information on injury accidents.

Early Accident Modelling

In Heinrich’s model (1931), the causal sequence leading to an accident was likened to a sequence of five falling dominoes, each of the first four being necessary
before the final event could occur. In a precursor to MAIM, Manning (1971) concluded that “the basic requirements of an accidental injury are the presence of a
host [a worker, for example] and an environmental object which contributes to the accident. The host or the object or both move in relation to each other.”
Kjellén and Larsson (1981) developed their own model, which posited two levels: the accident sequence and the underlying, determining factors. In a later
paper, Kjellén and Hovden (1993) described subsequent progress in the context of other literature and noted the need for “efficient use of existing information
from routine accident and near accident reports by means of a powerful information retrieval system”. This has been achieved for MAIM.

Rationale for MAIM


There appears to be a substantial consensus that useful information on accidents should not merely concentrate on the immediate circumstances of the damage
or injury, but should also include an understanding of the preceding chain of events and factors causing the accident sequence to occur. Some early classification
systems failed to achieve this. Understanding objects, movements (of people or objects) and events were commonly mixed and successive events were not
distinguished.

A simple example illustrates the problem. A worker slips on a patch of oil, falls and strikes his or her head on a machine and suffers a concussion. We can easily
distinguish the (immediate) cause of the accident (slipping on oil) and the cause of the injury (hitting the head on the machine). Some classification systems,
however, include the categories “falls of persons” and “striking against objects”. The accident could be allocated to either of these, although neither describes
even the immediate cause of the accident (slipping on oil) or causal factors (such as how did the oil get on the floor).

Essentially, the problem is that just one factor is considered in a multifactorial situation. An accident does not always consist of a single event; there may be
many. These points formed the basis for the development of MAIM by Derek Manning, an occupational physician.

Description of MAIM

The centrepiece of the accident is the first unforeseen (undesired or unplanned) event involving the damaged equipment or injured person (figure 56.14). This
will not always be the first event in the accident process described as a preceding event. In the example above, the slip counts as the first unforeseen event of the
accident. (Given the presence of oil patches on the floor, it is not unforeseen that someone will slip on one and fall, but the person walking does not foresee
this.)

Figure 56.14 The MAIM Accident Model

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 25/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention

The behaviour of the equipment or person is described by the general activity at the time and a more specific description of the type of corporal movement when
the first event occurred. Objects involved are described, and for those related to events, characteristics of objects include position, movement and condition. On
occasion, a second object which inter-relates with the first object may be involved (for example, striking a chisel with a hammer).

As noted above, there may be more than one event and the second event may also have an object (perhaps different) involved in it. Additionally, the equipment
or person may make an additional corporal movement, such as throwing out a hand to prevent or break a fall. These can be included in the model. A third fourth
or later event may occur before the sequence finally leads to an injury. The model can be expanded in all directions by recording factors related to each
component. For example, branches from activities and corporal movements would record psychological factors, medications or physical limitations of a worker.

In general, separate events may be easily distinguished intuitively, but a stricter definition is useful: an event is an unexpected change, or lack of change, in the
energy state of the situation. (The term energy includes both kinetic and potential energy.) The first event is always unexpected. Subsequent events may be
expected, even inevitable, after the first event, but are always unexpected before the accident. An example of unexpected lack of change of energy is when a
hammer being swung misses the nail at which it is aimed. The example of a worker who slips on a patch of oil, falls and hits his or her head provides an
illustration. The first event is “foot slipped”—instead of remaining still, the foot acquires kinetic energy. The second event is “fell”, when further kinetic energy
is acquired. This energy is absorbed by the collision of the worker’s head with the machine when the injury occurs and the sequence ends. This can be “plotted”
onto the model as follows:

1. 1st event: foot slipped on oil.

2. 2nd event: person fell.

3. 3rd event: head struck against machine.

Experience with MAIM


An earlier version of the MAIM model was used in a study of all 2,428 reported accidents in 1973 in a gearbox manufacturing plant on the grounds of an
automobile company. (See Shannon 1978 for further details.) The operations included cutting and grinding of gears, heat treatment and gearbox assembly. The
cutting process produced sharp metal splinters and shavings, and oil was used as a coolant. Purpose-designed forms were used to collect information. Each
accident was plotted independently onto the model by two people and discrepancies were resolved by discussion. For each accident, the components were given
numerical codes, so the data could be stored on a computer and analyses performed. The following outlines some basic results and presents an examination
made what was learned specifically from the use of the model.

The accident rate was substantially reduced (by nearly 40%), apparently as a result of the study being conducted. The researchers learned that because of the
additional questioning that the study called for (and the consequent time involved) many employees “could not be bothered” to report minor injuries. Several

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 26/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention
items of evidence confirmed this:

1. The rate rose again in 1975 after the study ended.

2. The rate of lost-time injuries was unaffected.

3. Visits to the medical centre for non-industrial complaints were unaffected.

4. Accident rates on the rest of the grounds were unaffected.

Thus the reduced rate did indeed appear to be an artefact of reporting.

Another interesting finding was that there were 217 injuries (8%) for which the workers involved could not be certain how or when they occurred. This was
discovered because workers were explicitly asked if they were sure of what happened. Typically, the injuries involved were cuts or splinters, relatively common
given the nature of work at this plant.

Of the remaining accidents, nearly half (1,102) consisted of just one event. Two- and three-event accidents were successively less common, and 58 accidents
involved four or more events. There was a marked increase in the proportion of accidents resulting in lost time with an increase in the number of events. One
possible explanation is that there was an increase in kinetic energy with each event, so that with more events, there was more energy to dissipate when the
worker and the object involved collided.

Further examination of differences between lost-time and non-lost-time accidents found very marked differences in the distributions for separate components of
the model. For example, when the first event was “person slipped”, nearly a quarter of the accidents resulted in lost time; but for “body punctured by”, only 1%
did so. For combinations of components, such differences were accentuated. For example, with respect to the final events and related objects, none of the 132
accidents in which the casualty was “punctured by” or “splintered” resulted in lost time, but when the final event was “strained/sprained” with “no object
involved”, 40% of the injuries caused lost time.

These results contradicted the view that the severity of injury is largely a matter of luck and prevention of all types of accidents would lead to a reduction of
serious injuries. This means that analysing all accidents and attempting to prevent the most common types would not necessarily have an effect on those causing
serious injuries.

A sub-study was conducted to assess the usefulness of information in the model. Several potential uses of accident data were identified:

· to measure safety performance—the extent to which accidents in a plant, or area of a plant, continue to occur over time

· to identify causes

· to identify errors (in the broadest meaning of the term)

· to check on control—that is, to see whether safety measures taken to prevent some type(s) of accident are indeed effective

· to provide a basis of expertise, as knowledge of a wide range of accident situations and circumstances might help provide accident prevention advice.

Three safety officers (practitioners) rated the usefulness of the verbal descriptions and the plotted models for a series of accidents. Each rated at least 75
accidents on a scale from 0 (no useful information) to 5 (perfectly adequate for use). For the majority of accidents, the ratings were identical—that is, no
information was lost in the transfer from the written descriptions to the model. Where there was a loss of information it was mostly just one point on the 0 to 5
scale—that is, only a small loss.

The information available, though, was rarely “perfectly adequate”. This was partly because the safety officers were used to conducting detailed on-the-spot
investigations, something not done in this study because all reported accidents, both minor and more serious ones, were included. It should be remembered,
however, that the information plotted onto the models was taken directly from written descriptions. Since relatively little information was lost, this suggested the
possibility of excluding the intermediate step. The more widespread use of personal computers and availability of improved software make automated data
collection possible—and allow checklists to be used by way of insuring that all relevant information is obtained. A program has been written for this purpose
and has undergone some initial testing.

MAIM Intelligent Software


The MAIM model was used by Troup, Davies and Manning (1988) to investigate accidents causing back injuries. A database was created on an IBM PC by
coding results from patient interviews conducted by an interviewer who was experienced with the MAIM model. The analysis of interviews to obtain the
MAIM description (figure 56.14) was done by the interviewer and it was only at this stage that the data were entered into the database. While the method was
quite satisfactory, there were potential problems in making the method generally accessible. In particular, two areas of expertise were required—interviewing
skills and the familiarity with the analysis needed to form the MAIM description of the accident.

Software was developed by Davies and Manning (1994a) to conduct a patient interview and produce a database of accidents using the MAIM model. The
purpose of the software was to provide two areas of expertise—the interview and the analysis to form the MAIM event structure. The MAIM software is, in
effect, an intelligent “front end” to a database, and by 1991 it was sufficiently developed to be tested in a clinical environment. The MAIM software was
designed to interact with the patient by means of “menus”—the patient selects options from lists which require only the use of cursor keys and the “Enter” key.
The choice of an item from the list of options affected to some extent the path through the interview and also had the effect of recording information at the
appropriate part of the MAIM description of the accident. This method of data collection eliminated the need for spelling and typing skills and also gave a
repeatable and consistent interview.

The event structure of the MAIM model uses verbs and objects to form simple sentences. Verbs in events can be associated with different accident scenarios,
and this property of the model forms the basis for the construction of a set of linked questions that form an interview. Questions are presented in such a way that
at any stage only simple choices are needed, effectively breaking the complex account of the accident into a set of simple descriptions. Once an event verb has
been identified, associated nouns can be found by locating the objects to form a sentence giving the full detail of the description of the particular event. It is
clear that this strategy requires the use of an extensive dictionary of objects which can be searched quickly and efficiently.

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 27/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention
The Home Accident Surveillance System (HASS) (Department of Trade and Industry 1987) monitors objects involved in accidents, and the list of objects used
by HASS was used as the basis of an object dictionary for the MAIM software and was extended to include objects found in the workplace. Objects can be
grouped into classes, and with this structure a hierarchical menu system can be defined—classes of objects form layers that correspond to menu lists. Thus a
linked list of associated objects can be used to locate individual items. As an example, the object hammer could be found by selecting, in order: (1) tools, (2)
hand tools and (3) hammer from three successive menu lists. A given object could potentially be classified into several different groups—for example, a knife
could be associated with kitchen items, tools or sharp objects. This observation was used to create redundant links in the object dictionary, allowing many
different paths to find the required object. The object dictionary currently has a vocabulary of about 2,000 entries covering work and leisure environments.

The MAIM interview also collects information on activities at the time of the accident, corporal movements, the location of the accident, contributory factors,
injuries and disability. All of these elements can occur more than once in an accident, and this is reflected in the structure of the underlying relational database
which was used to record the accident.

At the end of the interview, several sentences describing events in the accident will have been recorded and the patient is asked to put them in the correct order.
In addition, the patient is asked to link injuries with the recorded events. A summary of the information collected is then presented on the screen of the computer
for information.

An example of an accident summary as seen by the patient is shown in figure 56.15. This accident has been superimposed on the MAIM diagram in figure
56.15 . Details relating to factors and the location of the accident have been omitted.

Figure 56.15 Accident summary as recorded by patient interview

The first unforeseen or unintended event (first event) involving the injured person is usually the first event in the accident sequence. For example, when a
person slips and falls, the slip is normally the first event in the accident sequence. If, on the other hand, a person is injured by a machine because another person
operates the machine before the victim is standing clear, the first event involving the victim is “trapped by machine” but the first event in the accident sequence
is “other person operated machine prematurely”. In the MAIM software, the first event in the accident sequence is recorded and it may arise either from the first
event involving the injured person or as a preceding event (figure 56.14). Theoretically, this way of viewing matters may be unsatisfactory, but from the
accident prevention point of view, it identifies the beginning of the accident sequence, which can then be targeted to prevent similar accidents in the future. (The
term action of deviation is used by some authorities to describe the beginning of the accident sequence, but it is not yet clear if this is always synonymous with
the first event in the accident.)

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 28/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention
When the MAIM software was first used in a clinical setting it was clear that there were problems in correctly appraising some types of “underfoot” accidents.
The MAIM model identifies the first unforeseen event as the starting point of the accident sequence. Consider two similar accidents, one in which a worker
intentionally steps onto an object which then breaks, and a second accident in which a worker unintentionally steps onto an object which breaks. In the first
accident stepping on the object is a body movement and the first unforeseen event is the object breaking. In the second accident stepping on the object is the
first unforeseen event in the accident. The resolution of these two scenarios is to ask, “Did you accidentally step on something?” This demonstrates how
important the correct design of the interview is in obtaining accurate data. The analysis of these two accidents allows recommendations on accident prevention
as follows; the first accident could have been prevented by making the patient aware that the object would break. The second accident could have been
prevented by making the patient aware that the object was an underfoot hazard.

The MAIM software has been tested successfully in three clinical settings, including a 1-year project in the Accident and Emergency Department of the Royal
Liverpool University Hospital. Patient interviews took between 5 and 15 minutes, and on average two patients were interviewed per hour. In all, 2,500 accidents
were recorded. Work on publications based on these data is in progress.

PRINCIPLES OF PREVENTION: THE PUBLIC HEALTH APPROACH TO REDUCING


INJURIES IN THE WORKPLACE
Gordon S. Smith and Mark A. Veazie

A public health approach to occupational injury prevention is based on the assumption that injury is a health problem, and as such can either be prevented or its
consequences mitigated (Occupational Injury Prevention Panel 1992; Smith and Falk 1987; Waller 1985). When a worker falls from a scaffold, the tissue
damage, internal haemorrhage, shock and death that follow are, by definition, a disease process—and also by definition a concern for public health
professionals. Just as malaria is defined as a disease whose causal agent is a specific protozoan, injuries are a family of diseases caused by exposure to a
particular form of energy (kinetic, electrical, thermal, radiation or chemical) (National Committee for Injury Prevention and Control 1989). Drowning,
asphyxiation and poisoning are also considered injuries because they represent a relatively rapid departure from the body’s structural or functional norm, as does
acute trauma.

As a health problem, injuries are the leading cause of premature death (i.e., before age 65) in most countries (Smith and Falk 1987; Baker et al. 1992; Smith and
Barss 1991). In the United States, for example, injury is the third leading cause of death following cardiovascular disease and cancer, the leading cause of
hospitalization under the age of 45, and an imposed economic burden of 158 billion dollars in direct and indirect costs in 1985 (Rice et al. 1989). One out of
three nonfatal injuries and one out of six fatal injuries to working-aged persons in the United States occur on the job (Baker et al. 1992). Similar patterns apply
in most of the developed world (Smith and Barss 1991). In middle- and low-income countries, a rapid and relatively unregulated pace of industrialization may
result in a nearly global pandemic of occupational injuries.

Public Health Models for Injury Control


The traditional practice of workplace safety usually focuses on minimizing risks and losses within a single company. Public health practitioners engaged in
occupational injury control are interested not only in individual worksites but also in improving the health status of populations in geographic areas that may be
exposed to the hazards associated with multiple industries and occupations. Some events such as workplace fatalities may be rare at individual plants, but by
studying all fatalities in a community, risk patterns and prevention policy may become evident.

Most models of public health practice are based on three elements: (1) assessment, (2) development of prevention strategies, and (3) evaluation. Public health
practice is usually multidisciplinary and founded on the applied science of epidemiology. Epidemiology is the study of the distribution and determinants of
diseases and injuries in a population. The three main applications of epidemiology are surveillance, aetiological research and evaluation.

Surveillance is “the ongoing and systematic collection, analysis and interpretation of health data in the process of describing and monitoring a health event. This
information is used for planning, implementing and evaluating public health interventions and programs” (CDC 1988).

Aetiological research tests hypotheses regarding the determinants of disease and injuries through the use of controlled, usually observational, studies.

Evaluation in both applied social sciences and epidemiology is “a process that attempts to determine as systematically and objectively as possible the relevance,
effectiveness and impact of activities in light of their objectives” (Last 1988). Epidemiological evaluation usually entails the use of controlled study designs to
measure the effects of an intervention on the occurrence of health-related events in a population.

The basic model of public health practice is described by a cycle of epidemiological surveillance, research on causes, interventions (targeted to high-risk
populations and specific to severe health conditions), and epidemiological evaluation. Important modifications of this model include community-oriented
primary care (Tollman 1991), community-based health education and health promotion (Green and Kreuter 1991), community health development (Steckler et
al. 1993), participatory action research (Hugentobler, Israel and Schurman 1992) and other forms of community-oriented public health practice that rely on
greater participation of communities and labour—as opposed to government officials and industrial management—to define problems, develop solutions and
evaluate their effectiveness. Family farming, fishing and hunting, self-employed work, many small business operations and work in the informal economy are
all primarily influenced by family and community systems and occur outside the context of an industrial management system. Community-oriented public
health practice is a particularly viable approach to occupational injury prevention in these populations.

Outcomes of Interest

The public health approach to workplace safety moves from the concept of accident prevention to a broader approach to injury control where the primary
outcomes of interest are both the occurrence and severity of injury. Injury is by definition physical damage due to the transfer of energy. A transfer of
mechanical energy can cause trauma, as in the case of a fall or automobile crash. Thermal, chemical, electrical or radiation energy can cause burns and other
injuries (Robertson 1992). Not only is the occurrence of injury of interest to public health practitioners, but so is the severity and long-term outcome of injury.
Injury severity can be measured in several dimensions, including anatomical (the amount and nature of tissue damage in various regions of the body),
physiological (how close to death the patient is, based on vital signs), disability, impairment of quality of life, and indirect and direct costs. Of considerable
importance to injury epidemiologists is anatomical severity, often measured by the Abbreviated Injury Score and the Injury Severity Scale (MacKenzie,
Steinwachs and Shankar 1989). These measures can predict survival and are a useful indicator of the energy transferred in severe events, but are not sensitive
enough to discriminate between severity levels among the relatively less severe, but much more frequent occupational injuries such as sprains and strains.

Among the least useful, but most common measures of severity is days lost from work following injury. From an epidemiological point of view, lost work days
are often difficult to interpret because they are a function of some unknown combination of disability, demands of the job, availability of alternative light work,
www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 29/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention
workplace policies such as sick leave, disability qualification criteria and individual differences in pain tolerance, the propensity to work with pain, and possibly
the same factors that motivate attendance. More work is needed to develop and validate more interpretable occupational injury severity measures, particularly
anatomical scales, disability scales and measures of impairment in the various dimensions of quality of life.

Unlike traditional safety practice, the public health community is not restricted to an interest in unintentional (“accidental”) injuries and the events that cause
them. Through looking at the individual causes of fatalities in the workplace, it was found, for example, that in the United States, homicide (an intentional
injury) is the leading cause of death at work among women and the third leading cause among men (Baker et al. 1992; Jenkins et al. 1993). Such fatalities are
very rare events in individual workplaces and thus their importance is often overlooked, as is the fact that motor vehicle injuries are the single leading cause of
fatal injuries on the job (figure 56.16). Based on these surveillance data, injuries and death due to violence in the workplace and to motor vehicle crashes are
priorities in the public health approach to occupational injury prevention in the United States.

Figure 56.16 Leading causes of occupational injury death, United States 1980-1989

Assessment in Public Health

Assessment in public health is a multidisciplinary effort which involves surveillance, aetiological research, and community and organizational needs
assessment. The purpose of injury surveillance is to identify high-risk populations, to identify injuries with significant public health impact, to detect and
monitor trends and to generate hypotheses. Surveillance programmes can collect data on injury fatalities, non-fatal injuries, incidents with injury potential, and
exposure to hazards. Data sources for occupational injury surveillance include health care providers (hospitals and physicians), death certificates, medical
examiner/coroner reports, employer-based reporting to departments of labour or health, workers’ compensation agencies, periodic surveys of employers or
households, and individual corporate records. Many of these reports and records are required by law but often offer incomplete information due to a lack of
coverage of all workers, incentives to under-report, and a poor level of specificity in injury details.

In-depth investigations of individual incidents employ a variety of approaches which permit the use of expert judgement to draw conclusions about what caused
the event and how it could have been prevented (Ferry 1988). Preventive action is often taken based on the findings from a single incident. Rate-based
surveillance, on the other hand, has a broader significance than the individual incident. Indeed, some information from traditional accident investigations can
have little epidemiological interpretation when aggregated into statistics. Accident investigation in the tradition of Heinrich (1959), for example, often produces
statistics indicating that in excess of 80% of industrial injuries are caused solely by unsafe acts. From an epidemiological point of view, such statistics are
difficult to interpret except as a survey of value judgements, and are rarely included in rate-based surveillance. Many other risk factors such as shift work, work
stress, poorly designed work environments and so on, are often not included in investigation forms and thus are not considered in examining statistics on injury
causes.

One of the primary purposes of surveillance is to identify high-risk groups in order to target further investigation and prevention. Injuries, like infectious and
chronic diseases, have distinct patterns of risk that vary by age, sex, race, geographic region, industry and occupation (Baker et al. 1992). In the United States
during the 1980s, for example, surveillance by the National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health (NIOSH) revealed the following high-risk groups for
occupational injury fatality: males; elderly workers; blacks; workers in rural Western states; transportation and materials moving occupations; farming, forestry
and fishing occupations; and labourers (Jenkins et al. 1993). Another important aspect of surveillance is to identify the types of injury that occur with the
greatest frequency and severity, such as the leading external causes of work-related injury fatality in the United States (see figure 56.16). At an individual
company level, problems such as homicide and motor vehicle fatalities are rare events and thus are seldom addressed by many traditional safety programmes.
However, national surveillance data identified these among the three leading causes of occupational injury fatalities. Assessing the impact of non-fatal injuries
requires the use of severity measures in order to make meaningful interpretations. For example, back injuries are a common cause of lost days of work, but an
infrequent cause of hospitalization for work-related injuries.

Surveillance data alone do not represent a complete assessment in the public health tradition. Particularly in community-oriented public health practice, needs
assessment and community diagnosis using surveys, focus groups and other techniques are important steps to assess what problems workers or communities
perceive are important, what are the prevalent attitudes, intentions and barriers regarding the adoption of prevention measures, and how an organization or
community really functions. A community-based agricultural safety programme, for example, might need to identify whether or not farmers perceive that
tractor rollovers are a critical problem, what barriers such as financial or time constraints may prevent the installation of rollover protective structures, and
through whom an intervention strategy should be implemented (e.g., trade association, youth organization, organization of farm wives). In addition to a
diagnosis of the community, organizational needs assessment identifies an organization’s capacity, workload and constraints to implement fully any already
existing prevention programmes such as the enforcement activities of a governmental department of labour (or health) or the safety department of a large
corporation.

Investigating the aetiology or causation of loss incidents and injuries is another step in the public health approach to occupational injury control. Such studies of
occupational disease have been the mainstay of developing disease-control programmes in the workplace. Aetiological research involves the application of
epidemiology to identify risk factors for injury. It also involves applied social sciences to identify the determinants of organizational and individual behaviours
that lead to unsafe conditions. Epidemiological research seeks to identify modifiable risk factors through the use of controlled, usually observational study

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 30/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention
designs such as the case-control study, the cohort study, the panel study and the cross-sectional study. As with epidemiological studies of other acute health
events (e.g., asthma attacks, sudden cardiac arrests), aetiological research on injuries is challenged by the need to study either rare or recurrent events that are
highly influenced by situational exposures that occur immediately before the event (e.g., distraction by impact noise) and by social and behavioural constructs
that are difficult to measure (e.g., safety climate, job strain) (Veazie et al. 1994). Only recently have epidemiological and statistical methods been developed to
accommodate the study of these types of health events.

Epidemiological studies that focus on the occurrence of injury are expensive and are not always needed. It does not require a controlled epidemiological study
to document the impact of a lack of machine guarding on amputations due to a particular machine; a series of case investigations would suffice. Similarly, if an
easily measurable individual behaviour such as failing to use a seat-belt is already a known risk factor, then studies focusing on the determinants of the
behaviour and how to improve usage rates, are more useful than studying the injury. However, controlled epidemiological studies of injury and injury severity
are needed to provide an understanding of a variety of causal mechanisms that are responsible for decrements in the performance of humans or technology that
are difficult to measure. The effect of noise exposure or shift work, for example, on the risk and severity of injury is unlikely to be quantified by case
investigations or by studies of easy-to-measure behaviours.

A recent review of studies on risk factors of occupational injuries revealed that age, job title, physical attributes or impairments and experience in the job or task
were the most commonly studied human variables (Veazie et al. 1994). Shift work and scheduling were the most commonly studied job content variables. The
work environment was the least studied. Most environment factors related to design features or recognized material hazards. Some studies examined factors in
the organization and social environment. A few studies evaluated physical stressors such as heat and noise exposure as risk factors for injury. Many of these
studies were of poor methodological quality, and few were replicated in different populations. Thus, little is known about risk factors for injury at work, except
for the most obvious immediate causes. Future research may benefit by examining the impact on injury rates of risk factors predicted by theory in human
factors, ergonomics, occupational stress and organizational behaviour. These may include design and scheduling of tasks and jobs, psychosocial factors (e.g.,
worker control, social support, psychological demands), and organizational structure and change (e.g., continuous quality improvement and management
commitment to safety).

The public health approach also integrates injury epidemiology with the applied behavioural sciences (particularly health promotion, health behaviour and
health policy research) to identify the modifiable, environmental reasons for unsafe worker behaviour and, most important, for behaviours on the part of
employers and managers that lead to the creation and persistence of hazards. In the large organizational setting, this effort must involve research in
organizational behaviour and industrial psychology. Thus, the assessment phase in the public health approach involves epidemiological surveillance, in-depth
investigations, community and organizational needs assessment, and aetiological research based on the application of epidemiology and applied behavioural
sciences.

Prevention Strategies
A number of principles guide the selection and implementation of prevention measures in a public health approach to injury control. These include:

(1) The importance of basing prevention measures on prior assessment and evaluation. The first principle acknowledges the importance of selecting
interventions that are targeted to have a high impact on community health status and are likely to be successfully implemented. Thus, interventions selected on
the basis of a thorough assessment phase, rather than merely common sense, are more likely to be effective. Interventions that have been demonstrated as
effective in the past are even more promising. Unfortunately, very few occupational injury interventions have been scientifically evaluated (Goldenhar and
Schulte 1994).

(2) The relative importance of control measures that automatically protect the worker. The second principle emphasizes the continuum between active and
passive protection. Active protection is that which requires constant repetitive individual action; passive protection offers relatively automatic protection. For
example, seat-belts require individual action to initiate protection each time someone gets into a vehicle. An air bag, on the other hand, bestows protection on a
vehicle occupant without any initiating action—it automatically protects that person. Active interventions require modifying and sustaining individual
behaviour change, which has been the least successful of injury prevention strategies to date. This principle is similar to the traditional hierarchy of controls in
occupational safety which emphasizes the importance of engineering controls over administrative controls, personal protective equipment and training.

(3) The importance of behaviour modification rather than education. The third principle recognizes the importance of behaviour modification and that not all
hazards can be engineered out of the environment at the manufacturing stage. Modification of the behaviour of employers, managers and employees is central,
not only to the installation and maintenance of passive protection, but to most other occupational injury control strategies as well. Another important aspect of
this principle is that classroom instruction, posters, pamphlets and other forms of education that merely seek to increase knowledge, usually have little effect on
behaviour when used alone. Most health behaviour theories applied in health promotion focus on a variety of factors that motivate behaviour change other than
awareness of a physical hazard or safe behaviour. The Health Belief Model, for example, emphasizes that self-protective behaviour is most influenced by the
perception of risk, the perception of severity and the perception of the benefits and barriers associated with taking protective action (Green and Kreuter 1991).

While credible educational messages may alter some of these perceptions, sometimes the best way to alter these perceptions is to change the physical and social
environment. A potentially effective approach to behaviour modification is to redesign equipment and the physical environment to make safe behaviour easier,
quicker, and more comfortable or socially desirable than unsafe behaviour. If the layout of machine shop equipment is designed to make walking through
hazardous zones difficult and unnecessary, then this unsafe behaviour will be reduced. Similarly, if hard hats are designed to be comfortable and to enhance the
social image of the construction worker, they may be used more often.

The social environment can also be modified to change behaviour. For example, legislation and enforcement is another far-reaching strategy in injury
prevention that changes behaviour and extends beyond education alone. Seat-belt laws and laws requiring the use of infant safety seats, for example, have
dramatically reduced motor vehicle fatalities in the United States. The effect of legislation and enforcement on occupational safety, however, is less well-
described. One notable exception is the documented clear, dramatic decline in US mine fatalities that followed the implementation of the Federal Coal Mine
Health and Safety Act of 1969 (figure 56.17). The resources and administrative authority devoted to mine safety enforcement is, however, much greater than
that available to most other agencies (Weeks 1991).

Figure 56.17 The effects of coal mining regulations on underground coal mining mortality rates, United States 1950-1990

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 31/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention

Well-designed occupational safety training often involves modifying the social environment by including a role modelling process, incentives, and feedback on
safety performance (Johnston, Cattledge and Collins 1994). Another form of training, labour education, represents an altered social environment (Wallerstein
and Baker 1994). It empowers workers to recognize hazards and to modify the behaviour of their employers so as to reduce those hazards. While education
alone is not usually sufficient, it is usually a necessary component of any injury prevention programme (Gielen 1992). Educating employers and employees is a
necessary part of the implementation of a specific injury prevention programme. Educating legislators, policy makers, health care providers and others is also
important to initiating and sustaining community-wide injury prevention efforts. Indeed, interventions most likely to succeed in the field use a multifaceted
approach which combines environmental modifications with policy changes and education (National Committee for Injury Prevention and Control 1989).

(4) Systematic consideration of all available options, including those that reduce not only injury occurrence but the severity and long-term consequences of
injuries. The fourth principle is that the process of choosing interventions should systematically consider a wide range of options. The choice of
countermeasures should not be determined by the relative importance of the causal factors or by their earliness in the sequence of events; rather priority must be
given to those which most effectively reduce injury. A useful scheme for systematically considering injury control options was proposed by Haddon (1972). The
Haddon Matrix reveals that interventions targeted at humans, the vehicles which can transfer damaging energy (e.g., cars, machinery), or the physical or
psychosocial environment may operate to control injury in the pre-event, event or post-event phases. Table 56.2 shows the application of the Haddon Matrix to
the problem of preventing motor vehicle injuries, which are the leading cause of occupational injury fatalities in many countries.

Table 56.2 The Haddon Matrix applied to motor vehicle injuries

Phases Factors
Human Vehicles and equipment Environment
Pre-event Educate public in the use Safe brakes and tires Improved road design;
of seat-belts and child restrict alcohol
restraints advertising and
availability at gas
stations
Event Prevention of Air bags and a Breakaway utility poles
osteoporosis to decrease crashworthy vehicle and crash barriers
likelihood of fracture design
Post-event Treatment of Safe design of fuel tank Adequate emergency
haemophilia and other to prevent rupture and medical care and
conditions that result in fire rehabilitation
impaired healing

Source: National Committee for Injury Prevention and Control 1989.

Traditional occupational safety interventions most often operate in the pre-event phase to prevent the initiation of an incident with potential to cause injury (i.e.,
an accident). Event phase interventions such as building cars to be more crashworthy or using safety lanyards while working at elevations, do not prevent
accidents, but minimize the probability and severity of injury. After the event is over—the cars in a crash have stopped moving or the worker has stopped falling
—post-event interventions such as first aid and prompt transport to appropriate surgical care seek to minimize the health consequences of injury (i.e., the
probability of death or long-term disability).

In the public health approach, it is important to avoid getting locked into one phase of the matrix. Just as injury is multifactorial in causation, prevention
strategies should address as many phases and aspects of injury as possible (but not necessarily all). The Haddon Matrix, for example, emphasizes that injury
control is not limited to preventing accidents. In fact, many of our most effective control strategies do not prevent accidents or even injuries, but can
considerably reduce their severity. Seat-belts and air bags in cars, safety helmets, fall protection in construction, rollover protective structures in agriculture, and
emergency eyewash fountains in the laboratory are but a few examples of event-phase strategies that do nothing to prevent an accident from occurring. Instead,
they reduce the severity of injury after the accident has been initiated. Even after the anatomical damage has been done, much can be done to reduce the risk of
death and long-term disability. In the United States, it has been estimated that many major trauma deaths could be prevented by systems that minimize the time
delay between injury and definitive surgical care. This broader framework is called injury control and goes far beyond traditional accident prevention. A
commonly used phrase to illustrate this point is “Injuries are not an accident”. They can be predicted and their impact on society controlled.

Another useful scheme often used for systematically considering injury control options is Haddon’s Ten Countermeasure Strategies (Haddon 1973). Table 56.3
shows how these strategies can be applied to controlling injury from falls in construction. As shown, not all strategies will be applicable for specific problems.

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 32/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention
Table 56.3 Haddon’s Ten Countermeasure Strategies applied to injuries due to falls in construction

Countermeasure Intervention (and relevant notes)


Prevent the creation of the hazard. Do not construct buildings—generally not a
practical option, to be sure.
Reduce the amount of the hazard brought Lower the height of construction project to
into being. below fatal levels—usually not practical, but
may be possible in some work zones.
Prevent the release of the hazard. Install non-slip walking surfaces on roofs
and other heights.
Modify the rate of release of the hazard Use safety lanyards. Use safety nets.
from its sources.
Separate the hazard from the worker by Do not schedule unnecessary foot traffic
time and space. near fall hazards until the hazards are abated.
Separate the hazard from the worker by Install guardrails on elevated surfaces.
physical barriers.
Modify basic qualities of the hazard. Remove sharp or protruding projections on
the ground surface where workers can fall
—practical only for very low heights.
Make worker as injury resistant as Require, e.g., safety helmets.
possible.
Begin to counter damage done by the Apply first aid.
hazard.
Stabilize, treat and rehabilitate worker. Develop a regionalized trauma system;
provide for effective rehabilitation and
retraining.

Evaluation in Public Health

Evaluation in both applied social sciences and epidemiology is “a process that attempts to determine as systematically and objectively as possible the relevance,
effectiveness and impact of activities in light of their objectives” (Last 1988). Evaluation is an essential component of public health practice. It occurs at two
levels. The first level relies on surveillance systems to determine whether or not entire communities have met their disease and injury reduction objectives,
without attempting to determine what caused the observed changes. Federal, state and local government agencies in the United States, for example, have set
objectives for the year 2000. One of these objectives is to reduce work-related injures resulting in medical treatment, lost time from work, or restricted work
activity to no more than 6 cases per 100 full-time workers per year. Progress in meeting these objectives will be monitored with the national surveillance
systems in place.

The second level of evaluation focuses on determining the effectiveness of policies, programmes and specific interventions. Ideally, this requires the application
of controlled experimental or quasi-experimental study designs. Mohr and Clemmer (1989), for example, conducted a time series study of injury rates in those
mobile off-shore oil rigs that opted to implement a new technology to assist workers in connecting drill pipes, compared with rates in those rigs that did not
have the new technology. Although injury rates were going down over the period of the installation of the new equipment, the authors were able to attribute a
decrease of 6 injuries per 100 workers per year to the new safety equipment and to demonstrate that the savings from injury prevention resulted in a full
recovery of the initial capital and installation costs within 5.7 years. Unfortunately, this type of scientific evaluation of programmes and interventions in
occupational health and safety is rare and often methodologically flawed (Goldenhar and Schulte 1994).

Summary

The above-mentioned programme demonstrates well the various components in the public health approach to reducing injuries in the workplace. Assessing the
injury problem and establishing an ongoing surveillance system was an essential part of this and earlier studies of injuries on oil rigs that were conducted by
these authors. The subsequent development of a simple engineering prevention strategy was then followed by a rigorous evaluation strategy which included an
evaluation of cost savings. Such studies have been the mainstay of the public health approach to the prevention of other occupational diseases. In the future, the
integration of occupational injury prevention into the assessment, intervention and evaluation phases of public health practice has the potential to be an
important step toward more effective protection and promotion of health in communities.

THEORETICAL PRINCIPLES OF JOB SAFETY


Reinald Skiba

This presentation covers the theoretical principles of job safety and the general principles for accident prevention. The presentation does not cover work-related
illnesses, which, although related, are different in many respects.

Theory of Job Safety


Job safety involves the interrelationship between people and work; materials, equipment and machinery; the environment; and economic considerations such as
productivity. Ideally, work should be healthful, not harmful and not unreasonably difficult. For economic reasons, as high a level of productivity as possible
must be achieved.

Job safety should start in the planning stage and continue through the various phases of production. Accordingly, requirements for job safety must be asserted
before work begins and be implemented throughout the work cycle, so that the results can be appraised for purposes of feedback, among other reasons. The

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 33/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention
responsibility of supervision toward maintaining the health and safety of those employed in the production process should also be considered during planning.
In the manufacturing process, people and objects interact. (The term object is used in the broader sense as expressed in the customary designation “people-
(machine)-environment system”. This includes not only technical instruments of work, machines and materials, but all surrounding items such as floors, stairs,
electrical current, gas, dusts, atmosphere and so on.)

Worker-Job Relationships
The following three possible relationships within the manufacturing process indicate how personal injury incidents (especially accidents) and harmful working
conditions are unintended effects of combining people and the objective working environment for the purpose of production.

1. The relationship between the worker and the objective working environment is optimal. This means well-being, job safety and labour-saving methods for
the employees as well as the reliability of the objective parts of the system, like machines. It also means no defects, accidents, incidents, near misses (potential
incidents) or injuries. The result is improved productivity.

2. The worker and the objective working environment are incompatible. This may be because the person is unqualified, equipment or materials are not correct
for the job or the operation is poorly organized. Accordingly, the worker is unintentionally overworked or underutilized. Objective parts of the system, like
machines, may become unreliable. This creates unsafe conditions and hazards with the potential for near misses (near accidents) and minor incidents resulting in
delays in production flow and declining output.

3. The relationship between the worker and the objective working environment is completely interrupted and a disruption results, causing damage, personal
injury or both, thereby preventing output. This relationship is specifically concerned with the question of job safety in the sense of avoiding accidents.

Principles of Workplace Safety

Because it is apparent that questions of accident prevention can be solved not in isolation, but only in the context of their relationship with production and the
working environment, the following principles for accident prevention can be derived:

1. Accident prevention must be built into production planning with the goal of avoiding disruptions.

2. The ultimate goal is to achieve a production flow that is as unhindered as possible. This results not only in reliability and the elimination of defects, but
also in the workers’ well-being, labour-saving methods and job safety.

Some of the practices commonly used in the workplace to achieve job safety and which are necessary for disruption-free production include, but are not limited
to the following:

· Workers and supervisors must be informed and aware of the dangers and potential hazards (e.g., through education).

· Workers must be motivated to function safely (behaviour modification).

· Workers must be able to function safely. This is accomplished through certification procedures, training and education.

· The personal working environment should be safe and healthy through the use of administrative or engineering controls, substitution of less hazardous
materials or conditions, or by the use of personal protective equipment.

· Equipment, machinery and objects must function safely for their intended use, with operating controls designed to human capabilities.

· Provisions should be made for appropriate emergency response in order to limit the consequences of accidents, incidents and injuries.

The following principles are important in understanding how accident prevention concepts relate to disruption-free production:

1. Accident prevention is sometimes considered a social burden instead of a major part of disruption prevention. Disruption prevention is a better motivator
than accident prevention, because improved production is expected to result from disruption prevention.

2. Measures to ensure workplace safety must be integrated into the measures used to ensure disruption-free production. For example, the instructions on
hazards must be an integral part of the general directions governing the flow of production at the workplace.

Accident Theory
An accident (including those that entail injuries) is a sudden and unwanted event, caused by an outside influence, that causes harm to people and results from
the interaction of people and objects.

Often the use of the term accident in the workplace is linked with personal injury. Damage to a machine is often referred to as a disruption or damage, but not an
accident. Damage to the environment is often called an incident. Accidents, incidents and disruptions which do not result in injury or damage are known as
“near accidents” or “near misses”. So while it may be considered appropriate to refer to accidents as cases of injury to workers and to define the terms incident,
disruption and damage separately as they apply to objects and the environment, in the context of this article they will all be referred to as accidents.

The conceptual model for the term accident indicates that workplace accidents occur from workers and objects interacting with each other through the release of
energy. The cause of an accident can lie in the characteristics of the injured worker (e.g., not capable of performing the work safely) or of the object (e.g., unsafe
or unsuitable equipment). The cause can also be another worker (providing erroneous information), supervisor (receiving incomplete job instructions) or trainer
(receiving incomplete or incorrect training). The following can be derived for accident prevention:

Assuming that workers as well as their objective environments can be carriers of hazards or dangers, accident prevention would basically consist of eliminating
the hazards or dangers, or impeding the consequences by keeping the carriers apart or by minimizing the effects of the energy.

Potential Hazards and Risks

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 34/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention
Although a hazard or danger may exist in an object, if the worker and the object are so separate from one another that they cannot come into contact, no
accident is possible. For example, if the object has a potential hazard (e.g., a suspended load is moved by a crane), this potential hazard cannot cause an injury
so long as there are no people in the effective area of the suspended load. It is only when a worker comes into the area of the crane’s suspended load that an
actual hazard or danger to this worker is posed, because an interaction between the worker and the object is possible. It should be noted that objects can also
endanger other objects, such as vehicles parked under the crane’s load. Risk, defined as a means of quantifying the hazard, is the product of the anticipated
frequency of the damage and the anticipated scope of the damage. Accident risk is correspondingly the product of the anticipated frequency of accidents
(relative accident frequency) and anticipated accident severity. Relative accident frequency is the number of accidents per risk-time (accidents per 1 million
hours or injuries per work year). The seriousness of the accident can be shown quantitatively with the lost time (e.g., lost work days), class of injury (minor
accident or first aid case, reportable injury, lost-time compensation case and fatal accident), type of injury, and cost of injury. This risk data should be recorded
empirically and in terms of a theoretical prognosis.

The risks of accidents are different in various workplaces, under various conditions. For example, the risks involved in drilling for oil, using the same workers
and identical equipment, differ widely depending on the geography (drilling on land or off shore) and the climate (Arctic exploration or deserts). The level of
accident risk depends on:

· the anticipated frequency of error of the worker and the technology (number per 1 million hours, etc.)

· the probability of the errors resulting in accidents (accident: error = 1:x)

· the probability of the seriousness level of the accident.

The acceptance of accident risks also varies widely. High accident risk appears to be acceptable in road traffic whereas a zero base tolerance is expected in the
field of nuclear energy. For purposes of accident prevention, it therefore follows that the driving force is the smallest possible acceptance of accident risk.

Causes of Accidents
The occurrence of an accident requires classification on a scale from cause to effect. Three levels must be differentiated:

· the level of the causes of possible and actual accidents

· the level of the accident’s origins

· the level of the accident’s consequences in the form of personal and material damages.

Cause is the reason for the accident. Almost every accident has multiple causes such as hazardous conditions, combinations of factors, courses of events,
omissions and so on. For example, causes of an accident involving a burst boiler may include one or a combination of the following reasons: faulty materials in
the boiler wall, inadequate training to ensure safe operation, failure of a pressure relief device, or violation of an operating procedure such as overheating.
Without one or more of these deficiencies, an accident may not have happened. Other conditions, which are not causal to the accident, should be separated. In
the case of the burst boiler, these would include conditions such as information about the time, the ambient temperature and the size of the boiler room.

It is important to differentiate the factors associated with the production process from the accident causes linked to workers (conduct of the immediate operator),
the organization (safe work procedures or policies) and technical accident causes (environmental changes and object failures). However, in the final analysis,
every accident results from faulty conduct of people, because people are always at the end of the causal chain. For example, if faulty material is determined to
be the cause of a boiler bursting, then improper conduct existed either on the part of the builder, manufacturer, tester, installer or owner (e.g., corrosion due to
inadequate maintenance). Strictly speaking, there is no such thing as a “technical failure” or technical accident cause. The technology is only the intermediate
link to the consequences of the improper conduct. Nevertheless, the normal division of causes into behavioural, technical and organizational is useful, because
this points toward which group of people behaved improperly and also helps select the appropriate corrective measures.

As previously stated, most accidents are the result of a combination of causes.

For example, a person slips on an oil spot in a dark, unlighted passageway and hits the sharp edge of a replacement part that is lying there, resulting in a head
injury. The immediate causes of the accident are inadequate lighting in the passageway, unsafe floor (oil spot), inadequately skid-free shoe soles, not wearing
head protection, and the replacement part not in its proper place. The accident could not have happened if the combination of causes had been eliminated or the
causal chain had been broken. Successful accident prevention therefore consists of recognizing the causal chain that leads to an accident and breaking it, so that
the accident can no longer occur.

Effect of Strains and Demands

Mechanization and automation of production processes have advanced considerably in recent years. It may appear that the causes of many accidents have
shifted from human error to those related to the maintenance of and interface with automated processes. However, these positive consequences of technology
are counterposed to other, negative ones, particularly the increase in psychological strains and corresponding ergonomic physical demands on workers in
automated plants due to the increased attention and responsibility required for overseeing the automated operations process, impersonal working environment
and monotony of work. These strains and corresponding demands increase the occurrence of accidents and can be harmful to health.

1. Strains are effects on workers which originate in the workplace, such as environmental strains (temperature, heat, humidity, light, noise and air pollution),
or they can be static or dynamic strains originating directly from the work process (lifting, climbing, chemical exposure and so on). Strain levels can be
physically measured (noise, force, atmospheric exposures and so on), whereas strain factors are physically unmeasurable influences (fatigue, mental stress, plant
worker/management relationships and so on).

2. Demands on workers are dependent on the type and degree of the strain as well as differing individual capability to withstand the strain. Effects of
demands show up physically and psychologically in the human body. The effects of the demands can be desirable or undesirable, depending on the type and
degree. Undesirable effects, such as physical and psychological exhaustion, work aggravations, illness, lack of coordination and concentration, and unsafe
behaviour cause increased risk of accident.

For purposes of accident prevention, it follows that workers, based on their individual competencies, capabilities and willingness, should be able to physically
and psychologically work safely provided that there are no outside factors such as unsuitable equipment, poor environment or unsatisfactory work conditions.

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 35/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention
Safety may be improved by organizing the work process to include appropriate stimuli such as planned job changes, expansion of work and tasks, and work
enrichment.

Near Accidents (Near Misses)


A large part of production loss results from disruptions in the form of near misses (near accidents), which are the basis of occurrences of accidents. Not every
disruption affects work safety. Near accidents (near misses) are those occurrences or incidents in which no injury or damage resulted, but if injury or damage
had occurred, they would be classified as accidents. For example, a machine that unexpectedly stops running without damage to the equipment or work is
considered to be a near accident. Additionally, the disruption may cause another near accident if the machine suddenly starts up again while a worker is inside
trying to determine the cause of the stoppage, but the worker is not injured.

Accident Pyramid

Accidents are relatively rare occurrences, and usually the more serious the accident, the more rare the occurrence. Near accidents form the bottom, or base, of
the accident pyramid, whereas fatal accidents stand at the top. If lost time is used as a criterion for the seriousness of accidents, we find a relatively high degree
of correspondence with the accident pyramid. (There may be a slight deviation as a result of the reporting requirements of different countries, companies and
jurisdictions.)

The accident pyramid can be very different for individual types or classifications of accidents. For example, accidents involving electricity are
disproportionately serious. When accidents are classified by occupation, we see that certain types of work activities suffer disproportionately more serious
accidents. In both cases the accident pyramid is top-heavy due to the relatively high proportion of serious and fatal accidents.

From the accident pyramid, it follows for purposes of accident prevention that:

1. Accident prevention begins with avoiding near accidents (near misses).

2. Eliminating minor accidents usually has a positive effect on eliminating serious accidents.

Accident Prevention
The different paths of accident prevention for ensuring workplace safety are as follows:

1. Eliminate the hazard or danger so that injury or damage is no longer possible.

2. Provide for separation between the worker (or equipment) and the hazard (equal to elimination of the hazard). The danger remains, but an injury (or
damage) is not possible since we make sure that the natural zones of influence of workers (equipment) and object (hazard or danger) do not intersect.

3. Provide shielding, such as fireproofing, protective clothing and respirators to minimize the hazard. The hazard still exists, but the possibility of an injury or
damage is reduced by minimizing the chances of the hazard having an effect by shielding the danger.

4. Adapt to the hazard by providing measures such as warning systems, monitoring equipment, information about dangers, motivation for safe behaviour,
training and education.

Summary

In 1914, Max Planck (German physicist, 1858–1947) said: “In every science the highest watchword is the task of seeking order and continuity from the
abundance of individual experiences and individual facts, in order, by filling the gaps, to integrate them into a coherent view.” This principle also applies to the
complex scientific and practical questions of job safety because they not only interface with many different disciplines, but also are themselves multifaceted.
While it is difficult, for this reason, to systematize the many problems involved with job safety, it is necessary to properly organize the individual questions
according to significance and context, and to pose effective options for improving job safety.

PRINCIPLES OF PREVENTION: SAFETY INFORMATION


Mark R. Lehto and James M. Miller

Sources of Safety Information

Manufacturers and employers throughout the world provide a vast amount of safety information to workers, both to encourage safe behaviour and to discourage
unsafe behaviour. These sources of safety information include, among others, regulations, codes and standards, industry practices, training courses, Material
Safety Data Sheets (MSDSs), written procedures, safety signs, product labels and instruction manuals. Information provided by each of these sources varies in
its behavioural objectives, intended audience, content, level of detail, format and mode of presentation. Each source may also design its information so as to be
relevant to the different stages of task performance within a potential accident sequence.

Four Stages of the Accident Sequence


The behavioural objectives of particular sources of safety information correspond or “map” naturally to the four different stages of the accident sequence (table
56.4).

Table 56.4 Objectives and example sources of safety information mapped to the accident sequence

Task stage in accident sequence


Prior to task Routine task performance Abnormal task conditions Accident conditions
Objectives Educate and persuade worker of the Instruct or remind worker to Alert worker of abnormal Indicate locations of safety and first
www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 36/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention
(Behavioural) nature and level of risk, precautions, follow safe procedures or conditions. Specify needed aid equipment, exits and
remedial measures and emergency take precautions. actions. emergency procedures. Specify
procedures. remedial and emergency
procedures.
Example Training manuals, videos or Instruction manuals, job Warning signals: visual, Safety information signs, labels,
sources programmes, hazard communication performance aids, checklists, auditory, or olfactory. and markings, material safety data
programmes, material safety data written procedures, warning Temporary tags, signs, barriers sheets
sheets, safety propaganda, safety signs and labels or lock-outs
feedback

First stage. At the first stage in the accident sequence, sources of information provided prior to the task, such as safety training materials, hazard communication
programmes and various forms of safety programme materials (including safety posters and campaigns) are used to educate workers about risks and persuade
them to behave safely. Methods of education and persuasion (behaviour modification) attempt not only to reduce errors by improving worker knowledge and
skills but also to reduce intentional violations of safety rules by changing unsafe attitudes. Inexperienced workers are often the target audience at this stage, and
therefore the safety information is much more detailed in content than at the other stages. It must be emphasized that a well-trained and motivated workforce is
a prerequisite for safety information to be effective at the three following stages of the accident sequence.

Second stage. At the second stage in the accident sequence, sources such as written procedures, checklists, instructions, warning signs and product labels can
provide critical safety information during routine task performance. This information usually consists of brief statements which either instruct less skilled
workers or remind skilled workers to take necessary precautions. Following this approach can help prevent workers from omitting either precautions or other
critical steps in a task. Statements providing such information are often embedded at the appropriate stage within step-by-step instructions describing how to
perform a task. Warning signs at appropriate locations can play a similar role: for example, a warning sign located at the entrance to a workplace might state that
safety hard hats must be worn inside.

Third stage. At the third stage in the accident sequence, highly conspicuous and easily perceived sources of safety information alert workers of abnormal or
unusually hazardous conditions. Examples include warning signals, safety markings, tags, signs, barriers or lock-outs. Warning signals can be visual (flashing
lights, movements, etc.), auditory (buzzers, horns, tones, etc.), olfactory (odours), tactile (vibrations) or kinaesthetic. Certain warning signals are inherent to
products when they are in hazardous states (e.g., the odour released upon opening a container of acetone). Others are designed into machinery or work
environments (e.g., the back-up signal on a fork-lift truck). Safety markings refer to methods of non-verbally identifying or highlighting potentially hazardous
elements of the environment (e.g., by painting step edges yellow or emergency stops red). Safety tags, barriers, signs or lock-outs are placed at points of hazard
and are often used to prevent workers from entering areas or activating equipment during maintenance, repair or other abnormal conditions.

Fourth stage. At the fourth stage in the accident sequence, the focus is on expediting worker performance of emergency procedures at the time an accident is
occurring, or on the performance of remedial measures shortly after the accident. Safety information signs and markings conspicuously indicate facts critical to
adequate performance of emergency procedures (e.g., the locations of exits, fire extinguishers, first aid stations, emergency showers, eyewash stations or
emergency releases). Product safety labels and MSDSs may specify remedial and emergency procedures to be followed.

However, if safety information is to be effective at any stage in the accident sequence, it must first be noticed and understood, and if the information has been
previously learned, it must also be remembered. Then the worker must both decide to comply with the provided message and be physically able to do so.
Successfully attaining each of these steps for effectiveness can be difficult; however, guidelines describing how to design safety information are of some
assistance.

Design Guidelines and Requirements


Standards-making organizations, regulatory agencies and the courts through their decisions have traditionally both instituted guidelines and imposed
requirements regarding when and how safety information is to be provided. More recently, there has been a trend towards developing guidelines based on
scientific research concerning the factors which influence the effectiveness of safety information.

Legal requirements

In most industrialized countries, government regulations require that certain forms of safety information be provided to workers. For example, in the United
States, the Environmental Protection Agency (EPA) has developed several labelling requirements for toxic chemicals. The Department of Transportation (DOT)
makes specific provisions regarding the labelling of hazardous materials in transport. The Occupational Safety and Health Administration (OSHA) has
promulgated a hazard communication standard that applies to workplaces where toxic or hazardous materials are in use, which requires training, container
labelling, MSDSs and other forms of warnings.

In the United States, the failure to warn also can be grounds for litigation holding manufacturers, employers and others liable for injuries incurred by workers.
In establishing liability, the Theory of Negligence takes into consideration whether the failure to provide adequate warning is judged to be unreasonable conduct
based on (1) the foreseeability of the danger by the manufacturer, (2) the reasonableness of the assumption that a user would realize the danger and (3) the
degree of care that the manufacturer took to inform the user of the danger. The Theory of Strict Liability requires only that the failure to warn caused the injury
or loss.

Voluntary standards

A large set of existing standards provide voluntary recommendations regarding the use and design of safety information. These standards have been developed
by multilateral groups and agencies, such as the United Nations, the European Economic Community (EEC’s EURONORM), the International Organization for
Standardization (ISO) and the International Electrotechnical Commission (IEC); and by national groups, such as the American National Standards Institute
(ANSI), the British Standards Institute, the Canadian Standards Association, the German Institute for Normalization (DIN) and the Japanese Industrial
Standards Committee.

Among consensus standards, those developed by ANSI in the United States are of special significance. Since the mid-1980s, five new ANSI standards focusing
on safety signs and labels have been developed and one significant standard has been revised. The new standards are: (1) ANSI Z535.1, Safety Color Code, (2)
ANSI Z535.2, Environmental and Facility Safety Signs, (3) ANSI Z535.3, Criteria for Safety Symbols, (4) ANSI Z535.4, Product Safety Signs and Labels, and
(5) ANSI Z535.5, Accident Prevention Tags. The recently revised standard is ANSI Z129.1–1988, Hazardous Industrial Chemicals—Precautionary Labeling.
Furthermore, ANSI has published the Guide for Developing Product Information.

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 37/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention
Design specifications

Design specifications can be found in consensus and governmental safety standards specifying how to design the following:

1. Material Safety Data Sheets (MSDSs). The OSHA hazard communication standard specifies that employers must have a MSDS in the workplace for each
hazardous chemical used. The standard requires that each sheet be written in English, list its date of preparation and provide the scientific and common names
of the hazardous chemical mentioned. It also requires the MSDS to describe (1) physical and chemical characteristics of the hazardous chemical, (2) physical
hazards, including potential for fire, explosion and reactivity, (3) health hazards, including signs and symptoms of exposure, and health conditions potentially
aggravated by the chemical, (4) the primary route of entry, (5) the OSHA permissible exposure limit, the ACGIH threshold limit value or other recommended
limits, (6) carcinogenic properties, (7) generally applicable precautions, (8) generally applicable control measures, (9) emergency and first aid procedures and
(10) the name, address and telephone number of a party able to provide, if necessary, additional information on the hazardous chemical and emergency
procedures.

2. Instructional labels and manuals. Few consensus standards currently specify how to design instructional labels and manuals. This situation is, however,
quickly changing. The ANSI Guide for Developing User Product Information was published in 1990, and several other consensus organizations are working on
draft documents. Without an overly scientific foundation, the ANSI Consumer Interest Council, which is responsible for the above guidelines, has provided a
reasonable outline to manufacturers regarding what to consider in producing instruction/operator manuals. They have included sections entitled: “Organizational
Elements”, “Illustrations”, “Instructions”, “Warnings”, “Standards”, “How to Use Language”, and “An Instructions Development Checklist”. While the
guideline is brief, the document represents a useful initial effort in this area.

3. Safety symbols. Numerous standards throughout the world contain provisions regarding safety symbols. Among such standards, the ANSI Z535.3
standard, Criteria for Safety Symbols, is particularly relevant for industrial users. The standard presents a significant set of selected symbols shown in previous
studies to be well understood by workers in the United States. Perhaps more importantly, the standard also specifies methods for designing and evaluating safety
symbols. Important provisions include the requirement that (1) new symbols must be correctly identified during testing by at least 85% of 50 or more
representative subjects, (2) symbols which don’t meet the above criteria should be used only when equivalent printed verbal messages are also provided and (3)
employers and product manufacturers should train workers and users regarding the intended meaning of the symbols. The standard also makes new symbols
developed under these guidelines eligible to be considered for inclusion in future revisions of the standard.

4. Warning signs, labels and tags. ANSI and other standards provide very specific recommendations regarding the design of warning signs, labels and tags.
These include, among other factors, particular signal words and text, colour coding schemes, typography, symbols, arrangement and hazard identification (table
56.5). Among the most popular signal words recommended are: DANGER, to indicate the highest level of hazard; WARNING, to represent an intermediate
hazard; and CAUTION, to indicate the lowest level of hazard. Colour coding methods are to be used to consistently associate colours with particular levels of
hazard. For example, red is used in all of the standards in table 56.5 to represent DANGER, the highest level of hazard. Explicit recommendations regarding
typography are given in nearly all the systems. The most general commonality between the systems is the recommended use of sans-serif typefaces. Varied
recommendations are given regarding the use of symbols and pictographs. The FMC and the Westinghouse systems advocate the use of symbols to define the
hazard and to convey the level of hazard (FMC 1985; Westinghouse 1981). Other standards recommend symbols only as a supplement to words. Another area
of substantial variation, shown in table 56.4 , pertains to the recommended label arrangements. The proposed arrangements generally include elements discussed
above and specify the image (graphic content or colour), the background (shape, colour); the enclosure (shape, colour) and the surround (shape, colour). Many
of the systems also precisely describe the arrangement of the written text and provide guidance regarding methods of hazard identification.

Table 56.5 Summary of recommendations within selected warning systems

System Signal words Colour coding Typography Symbols Arrangement


ANSI Z129.1 Danger Not specified Not specified Skull-and-crossbones as Label arrangement not
Hazardous Industrial Warning supplement to words. specified; examples given
Chemicals: Caution Acceptable symbols for
Precautionary Poison 3 other hazards types.
Labeling (1988) optional words for
“delayed” hazards
ANSI Z535.2 Danger Red Sans serif, upper case, Symbols and Defines signal word, word
Environmental and Warning Orange acceptable typefaces, pictographs per ANSI message, symbol panels in 1 to
Facility Safety Signs Caution Yellow letter heights Z535.3 3 panel designs. 4 shapes for
(1993) Notice Blue special use. Can use ANSI
(general safety) Green Z535.4 for uniformity.
(arrows) as above; black and
white otherwise per
ANSI Z535.1
ANSI Z535.4 Product Danger Red Sans serif, upper case, Symbols and Defines signal word, message,
Safety Signs and Warning Orange suggested typefaces, letter pictographs per ANSI pictorial panels in order of
Labels (1993) Caution Yellow heights Z535.3; also SAE J284 general to specific. Can use
per ANSI Z535.1 safety alert symbol ANSI Z535.2 for uniformity.
Use ANSI Z129.1 for chemical
hazards.
NEMA Guidelines: Danger Red Not specified Electric shock symbol Defines signal word, hazard,
NEMA 260 (1982) Warning Red consequences, instructions,
symbol. Does not specify order.
SAE J115 Safety Danger Red Sans serif typeface, upper Layout to accommodate Defines 3 areas: signal word
Signs (1979) Warning Yellow case symbols; specific panel, pictorial panel, message
Caution Yellow symbols/ pictographs panel. Arrange in order of
not prescribed general to specific.
ISO Standard: ISO None. 3 kinds of labels: Message panel is added Symbols and pictographs Pictograph or symbol is placed
R557 (1967); ISO Stop/prohibition Red below if necessary inside appropriate shape with
3864 (1984)

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 38/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention
Mandatory action Blue message panel below if
Warning Yellow necessary
OSHA 1910.145 Danger Red Readable at 5 feet or as Biological hazard Signal word and major message
Specification for Warning (tags only) Yellow required by task symbol. Major message (tags only)
Accident Prevention Caution Yellow can be supplied by
Signs and Tags (1985) Biological Hazard, Fluorescent pictograph (tags only).
BIOHAZARD, or Orange/orange-red Slow-moving vehicle
symbol (SAE J943)
(safety instruction) Green
(slow-moving vehicle) Fluorescent yellow-
orange and dark red
per ANSI Z535.1
OSHA 1910.1200 Per applicable In English Only as Material Safety Data
(Chemical) Hazard requirements of EPA, Sheet
Communication FDA, BATF, and
(1985) CPSC; not otherwise
specified.
Westinghouse Danger Red Helvetica bold and Symbols and pictographs Recommends 5 components:
Handbook (1981); Warning Orange regular weights, signal word,
FMC Guidelines Caution Yellow upper/lower case symbol/pictograph, hazard,
(1985) Notice Blue result of ignoring warning,
avoiding hazard

Source: Adapted from Lehto and Miller 1986; Lehto and Clark 1990.

Certain standards may also specify the content and wording of warning signs or labels in some detail. For example, ANSI Z129.1 specifies that chemical
warning labels must include (1) identification of the chemical product or its hazardous component(s), (2) a signal word, (3) a statement of hazard(s), (4)
precautionary measures, (5) instructions in case of contact or exposure, (6) antidotes, (7) notes to physicians, (8) instructions in case of fire and spill or leak and
(9) instructions for container handling and storage. This standard also specifies a general format for chemical labels that incorporate these items. The standard
also provides extensive and specific recommended wordings for particular messages.

Cognitive guidelines

Design specifications, such as those discussed above, can be useful to developers of safety information. However, many products and situations are not directly
addressed by standards or regulations. Certain design specifications may not be scientifically proven, and, in extreme cases, conforming with standards and
regulations may actually reduce the effectiveness of safety information. To ensure effectiveness, developers of safety information consequently may need to go
beyond safety standards. Recognizing this issue, the International Ergonomics Association (IEA) and International Foundation for Industrial Ergonomics and
Safety Research (IFIESR) recently supported an effort to develop guidelines for warning signs and labels (Lehto 1992) which reflect published and unpublished
studies on effectiveness and have implications regarding the design of nearly all forms of safety information. Six of these guidelines, presented in slightly
modified form, are as follows.

1. Match sources of safety information to the level of performance at which critical errors occur for a given population. In specifying what and how safety
information is to be provided, this guideline emphasizes the need to focus attention on (1) critical errors that can cause significant damage and (2) the level of
worker performance at the time the error is made. This objective often can be attained if sources of safety information are matched to behavioural objectives
consistently with the mapping shown in table 56.4 and discussed earlier.

2. Integrate safety information into the task and hazard-related context. Safety information should be provided in a way that makes it likely to be noticed at
the time it is most relevant, which almost always is the moment when action needs to be taken. Recent research has confirmed that this principle is true for both
the placement of safety messages within instructions and the placement of safety information sources (such as warning signs) in the physical environment. One
study showed that people were much more likely to notice and comply with safety precautions when they were included as a step within instructions, rather than
separated from instructional text as a separate warning section. It is interesting to observe that many safety standards conversely recommend or require that
precautionary and warning information be placed in a separate section.

3. Be selective. Providing excessive amounts of safety information increases the time and effort required to find what is relevant to the emergent need.
Sources of safety information should consequently focus on providing relevant information which does not exceed what is needed for the immediate purpose.
Training programmes should provide the most detailed information. Instruction manuals, MSDSs and other reference sources should be more detailed than
warning signs, labels or signals.

4. Keep the cost of compliance within a reasonable level. A substantial number of studies have indicated that people become less likely to follow safety
precautions when doing so is perceived to involve a significant “cost of compliance”. Safety information should therefore be provided in a way that minimizes
the difficulty of complying with its message. Occasionally this goal can be attained by providing the information at a time and location when complying is
convenient.

5. Make symbols and text as concrete as possible. Research has shown that people are better able to understand concrete, rather than abstract, words and
symbols used within safety information. Skill and experience, however, play a major role in determining the value of concreteness. It is not unusual for highly
skilled workers to both prefer and better understand abstract terminology.

6. Simplify the syntax and grammar of text and combinations of symbols. Writing text that poor readers, or even adequate readers, can comprehend is not an
easy task. Numerous guidelines have been developed in attempts to alleviate such problems. Some basic principles are (1) use words and symbols understood
by the target audience, (2) use consistent terminology, (3) use short, simple sentences constructed in the standard subject-verb-object form, (4) avoid negations
and complex conditional sentences, (5) use the active rather than passive voice, (6) avoid using complex pictographs to describe actions and (7) avoid
combining multiple meanings in a single figure.

Satisfying these guidelines requires consideration of a substantial number of detailed issues as addressed in the next section.

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 39/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention

Developing Safety Information

The development of safety information meant to accompany products, such as safety warnings, labels and instructions, often requires extensive investigations
and development activities involving considerable resources and time. Ideally, such activities (1) coordinate the development of product information with design
of the product itself, (2) analyse product features which affect user expectations and behaviours, (3) identify the hazards associated with use and likely misuse of
the product, (4) research user perceptions and expectations regarding product function and hazard characteristics and (5) evaluate product information using
methods and criteria consistent with the goals of each component of product information. Activities accomplishing these objectives can be grouped into several
levels. While in-house product designers are able to accomplish many of the tasks designated, some of these tasks involve the application of methodologies
most familiar to professionals with backgrounds in human factors engineering, safety engineering, document design and the communication sciences. Tasks
falling within these levels are summarized as follows and are shown in figure 56.18 :

Figure 56.18 A model for designing and evaluating product information

Level 0: Product design status

Level 0 is both the starting point for initiating a product information project, and the point at which feedback regarding design alternatives will be received and
new iterations at the basic model level will be forwarded. At the initiation of a product information project, the researcher begins with a particular design. The
design can be in the concept or prototype stage or as currently being sold and used. A major reason for designating a Level 0 is the recognition that the
development of product information must be managed. Such projects require formal budgets, resources, planning, and accountability. The largest benefits to be
gained from a systematic product information design are achieved when the product is in the pre-production concept or prototype state. However, applying the
methodology to existing products and product information is quite appropriate and extremely valuable.

Level 1: Product type investigations

At least seven tasks should be performed at this stage: (1) document characteristics of the existing product (e.g., parts, operation, assembly and packaging), (2)
investigate the design features and accompanying information for similar or competitive products, (3) collect data on accidents for both this product and similar
or competitive products, (4) identify human factors and safety research addressing this type of product, (5) identify applicable standards and regulations, (6)
analyse government and commercial media attention to this type of product (including recall information) and (7) research the litigation history for this and
similar products.

Level 2: Product use and user group research

At least seven tasks should be performed at this stage: (1) determine appropriate methods for use of product (including assembly, installation, use and
maintenance), (2) identify existing and potential product user groups, (3) research consumer use, misuse, and knowledge of product or similar products, (4)
research user perceptions of product hazards, (5) identify hazards associated with intended use(s) and foreseeable misuse(s) of product, (6) analyse cognitive
and behavioural demands during product use and (7) identify likely user errors, their consequences and potential remedies.

After completing the analyses in Levels 1 and 2, product design changes should be considered before proceeding further. In the traditional safety engineering
sense, this could be called “engineering the hazard out of the product”. Some modifications may be for the health of the consumer, and some for the benefit of
the company as it attempts to produce a marketing success.

Level 3: Information design criteria and prototypes

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 40/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention
In Level 3 at least nine tasks are performed: (1) determine from the standards and requirements applying to the particular product which if any of those
requirements impose design or performance criteria on this part of the information design, (2) determine those types of tasks for which information is to be
provided to users (e.g., operation, assembly, maintenance and disposal), (3) for each type of task information, determine messages to be conveyed to user, (4)
determine the mode of communication appropriate for each message (e.g., text, symbols, signals or product features), (5) determine temporal and spatial
location of individual messages, (6) develop desired features of information based on messages, modes and placements developed in previous steps, (7) develop
prototypes of individual components of product information system (e.g., manuals, labels, warnings, tags, advertisements, packaging and signs), (8) verify that
there is consistency across the various types of information (e.g., manuals, advertisements, tags and packaging) and (9) verify that products with other brand
names or similar existing products from the same company have consistent information.

After having proceeded through Levels 1, 2 and 3, the researcher will have developed the format and content of information expected to be appropriate. At this
point, the researcher may want to provide initial recommendations regarding the redesign of any existing product information before moving on to Level 4.

Level 4: Evaluation and revision

In Level 4 at least six tasks are performed: (1) define evaluation parameters for each prototype component of the product information system, (2) develop an
evaluation plan for each prototype component of the product information system, (3) select representative users, installers and so on, to participate in evaluation,
(4) execute the evaluation plan, (5) modify product information prototypes and/or the design of the product based on the results obtained during evaluation
(several iterations are likely to be necessary) and (6) specify the final text and artwork layout.

Level 5: Publication

Level 5, the actual publication of the information, is reviewed, approved and accomplished as specified. The purpose at this level is to confirm that
specifications for designs, including designated logical groupings of material, location and quality of illustrations, and special communication features have
been precisely followed, and have not been unintentionally modified by the printer. While the publication activity is usually not under the control of the person
developing the information designs, we have found it necessary to verify that such designs are precisely followed, the reason being that printers have been
known to take great liberties in manipulating design layout.

Level 6: Post-sale evaluations

The last level of the model deals with the post-sale evaluations, a final check to ensure that the information is indeed fulfilling the goals it was designed to
achieve. The information designer as well as the manufacturer gains an opportunity for valuable and educational feedback from this process. Examples of post-
sale evaluations include (1) feedback from customer satisfaction programmes, (2) potential summarization of data from warranty fulfilments and warranty
response cards, (3) gathering of information from accident investigations involving the same or similar products, (4) monitoring of consensus standards and
regulatory activities and (5) monitoring of safety recalls and media attention to similar products.

WORK-RELATED ACCIDENT COSTS


Diego Andreoni

Workers who are the victims of work-related accidents suffer from material consequences, which include expenses and loss of earnings, and from intangible
consequences, including pain and suffering, both of which may be of short or long duration. These consequences include:

· doctor’s fees, cost of ambulance or other transport, hospital charges or fees for home nursing, payments made to persons who gave assistance, cost of
artificial limbs and so on

· the immediate loss of earnings during absence from work (unless insured or compensated)

· loss of future earnings if the injury is permanently disabling, long term or precludes the victim’s normal advancement in his or her career or occupation

· permanent afflictions resulting from the accident, such as mutilation, lameness, loss of vision, ugly scars or disfigurement, mental changes and so on, which
may reduce life expectancy and give rise to physical or psychological suffering, or to further expenses arising from the victim’s need to find a new occupation
or interests

· subsequent economic difficulties with the family budget if other members of the family have to either go to work to replace lost income or give up their
employment in order to look after the victim. There may also be additional loss of income if the victim was engaged in private work outside normal working
hours and is no longer able to perform it.

· anxiety for the rest of the family and detriment to their future, especially in the case of children.

Workers who become victims of accidents frequently receive compensation or allowances both in cash and in kind. Although these do not affect the intangible
consequences of the accident (except in exceptional circumstances), they constitute a more or less important part of the material consequences, inasmuch as they
affect the income which will take the place of the salary. There is no doubt that part of the overall costs of an accident must, except in very favourable
circumstances, be borne directly by the victims.

Considering the national economy as a whole, it must be admitted that the interdependence of all its members is such that the consequences of an accident
affecting one individual will have an adverse effect on the general standard of living, and may include the following:

· an increase in the price of manufactured products, since the direct and indirect expenses and losses resulting from an accident may result in an increase in
the cost of making the product

· a decrease in the gross national product as a result of the adverse effects of accidents on people, equipment, facilities and materials; these effects will vary
according to the availability in each country of workers, capital and material resources

· additional expenses incurred to cover the cost of compensating accident victims and pay increased insurance premiums, and the amount necessary to
provide safety measures required to prevent similar occurrences.

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 41/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention
One of the functions of society is that it must protect the health and income of its members. It meets these obligations through the creation of social security
institutions, health programmes (some governments provide free or low-cost medical care to their constituents), injury compensation insurance and safety
systems (including legislation, inspection, assistance, research and so on), the administrative costs of which are a charge on society.

The level of compensation benefits and the amount of resources devoted to accident prevention by governments are limited for two reasons: because they
depend (1) on the value placed on human life and suffering, which varies from one country to another and from one era to another; and (2) on the funds
available and the priorities allocated for other services provided for the protection of the public.

As a result of all this, a considerable amount of capital is no longer available for productive investment. Nevertheless, the money devoted to preventive action
does provide considerable economic benefits, to the extent that there is a reduction in the total number of accidents and their cost. Much of the effort devoted to
the prevention of accidents, such as the incorporation of higher safety standards into machinery and equipment and the general education of the population
before working age, are equally useful both inside and outside the workplace. This is of increasing importance because the number and cost of accidents
occurring at home, on the road and in other non-work-related activities of modern life continues to grow. The total cost of accidents may be said to be the sum
of the cost of prevention and the cost of the resultant changes. It would not seem unreasonable to recognize that the cost to society of the changes which could
result from the implementation of a preventive measure may exceed the actual cost of the measure many times over. The necessary financial resources are
drawn from the economically active section of the population, such as workers, employers and other taxpayers through systems which work either on the basis
of contributions to the institutions that provide the benefits, or through taxes collected by the state and other public authorities, or by both systems. At the level
of the undertaking the cost of accidents includes expenses and losses, which are made up of the following:

· expenses incurred while setting up the system of work and the related equipment and machinery with a view to ensuring safety in the production process.
Estimation of these expenses is difficult because it is not possible to draw a line between the safety of the process itself and that of the workers. Major sums are
involved which are entirely expended before production commences and are included in general or special costs to be amortized over a period of years.

· expenses incurred during production, which in turn include: (1) fixed charges related to accident prevention, notably for medical, safety and educational
services and for arrangements for the workers’ participation in the safety programme; (2) fixed charges for accident insurance, plus variable charges in schemes
where premiums are based on the number of accidents; (3) varying charges for activities related to accident prevention (these depend largely on accident
frequency and severity, and include the cost of training and information activities, safety campaigns, safety programmes and research, and workers’
participation in these activities); (4) costs arising from personal injuries (These include the cost of medical care, transport, grants to accident victims and their
families, administrative and legal consequences of accidents, salaries paid to injured persons during their absence from work and to other workers during
interruptions to work after an accident and during subsequent inquiries and investigations, and so on.); (5) costs arising from material damage and loss which
need not be accompanied by personal injury. In fact, the most typical and expensive material damage in certain branches of industry arises in circumstances
other than those which result in personal injury; attention should be concentrated upon the few points in common between the techniques of material damage
control and those required for the prevention of personal injury.

· losses arising out of a fall in production or from the costs of introducing special counter-measures, both of which may be very expensive.

In addition to affecting the place where the accident occurred, successive losses may occur at other points in the plant or in associated plants; apart from
economic losses which result from work stoppages due to accidents or injuries, account must be taken of the losses resulting when the workers stop work or
come out on strike during industrial disputes concerning serious, collective or repeated accidents.

The total value of these costs and losses are by no means the same for every undertaking. The most obvious differences depend on the particular hazards
associated with each branch of industry or type of occupation and on the extent to which appropriate safety precautions are applied. Rather than trying to place a
value on the initial costs incurred while incorporating accident prevention measures into the system at the earliest stages, many authors have tried to work out
the consequential costs. Among these may be cited: Heinrich, who proposed that costs be divided into “direct costs” (particularly insurance) and “indirect costs”
(expenses incurred by the manufacturer); Simonds, who proposed dividing the costs into insured costs and non-insured costs; Wallach, who proposed a division
under the different headings used for analysing production costs, viz. labour, machinery, maintenance and time expenses; and Compes, who defined the costs as
either general costs or individual costs. In all of these examples (with the exception of Wallach), two groups of costs are described which, although differently
defined, have many points in common.

In view of the difficulty of estimating overall costs, attempts have been made to arrive at a suitable value for this figure by expressing the indirect cost
(uninsured or individual costs) as a multiple of the direct cost (insured or general costs). Heinrich was the first to attempt to obtain a value for this figure and
proposed that the indirect costs amounted to four times the direct costs—that is, that the total cost amounts to five times the direct cost. This estimation is valid
for the group of undertakings studied by Heinrich, but is not valid for other groups and is even less valid when applied to individual factories. In a number of
industries in various industrialized countries this value has been found to be of the order of 1 to 7 (4 ± 75%) but individual studies have shown that this figure
can be considerably higher (up to 20 times) and may even vary over a period of time for the same undertaking.

There is no doubt that money spent incorporating accident prevention measures into the system during the initial stages of a manufacturing project will be offset
by the reduction of losses and expenses that would otherwise have been incurred. This saving is not, however, subject to any particular law or fixed proportion,
and will vary from case to case. It may be found that a small expenditure results in very substantial savings, whereas in another case a much greater expenditure
results in very little apparent gain. In making calculations of this kind, allowance should always be made for the time factor, which works in two ways: current
expenses may be reduced by amortizing the initial cost over several years, and the probability of an accident occurring, however rare it may be, will increase
with the passage of time.

In any given industry, where permitted by societal factors, there may be no financial incentive to reduce accidents in view of the fact that their cost is added to
the production cost and is thus passed on to the consumer. This is a different matter, however, when considered from the point of view of an individual
undertaking. There may be a great incentive for an undertaking to take steps to avoid the serious economic effects of accidents involving key personnel or
essential equipment. This is particularly so in the case of small plants which do not have a reserve of qualified staff, or those engaged in certain specialized
activities, as well as in large, complex facilities, such as in the process industry, where the costs of replacement could surpass the capacity to raise capital. There
may also be cases where a larger undertaking can be more competitive and thus increase its profits by taking steps to reduce accidents. Furthermore, no
undertaking can afford to overlook the financial advantages that stem from maintaining good relations with workers and their trade unions.

As a final point, when passing from the abstract concept of an undertaking to the concrete reality of those who occupy senior positions in the business (i.e., the
employer or the senior management), there is a personal incentive which is not only financial and which stems from the desire or the need to further their own
career and to avoid the penalties, legal and otherwise, which may befall them in the case of certain types of accident. The cost of occupational accidents,
therefore, has repercussions on both the national economy and that of each individual member of the population: there is thus an overall and an individual
incentive for everybody to play a part in reducing this cost.

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 42/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention

REFERENCES
Adams, JGU. 1985. Risk and Freedom; The Record of Read Safety Regulation. London: Transport Publishing Projects.

American National Standards Institute (ANSI). 1962. Method of Recording and Measuring Work Injury Experience. ANSI Z-16.2. New York: ANSI.

—. 1978. American National Standard Manual on Uniform Traffic Control Devices for Streets and Highways. ANSI D6.1. New York: ANSI.

—. 1988. Hazardous Industrial Chemicals—Precautionary Labeling. ANSI Z129.1. New York: ANSI.

—. 1993. Safety Color Code. ANSI Z535.1. New York: ANSI.

—. 1993. Environmental and Facility Safety Signs. ANSI Z535.2. New York: ANSI.

—. 1993. Criteria for Safety Symbols. ANSI Z535.3. New York: ANSI.

—. 1993. Product Safety Signs and Labels. ANSI Z535.4. New York: ANSI.

—. 1993. Accident Prevention Tags. ANSI Z535.5. New York: ANSI.

Andersson, R. 1991. The role of accidentology in occupational accident research. Arbete och halsa. 1991. Solna, Sweden. Thesis.

Andersson, R and E Lagerlöf. 1983. Accident data in the new Swedish information system on occupational injuries. Ergonomics 26.

Arnold, HJ. 1989. Sanctions and rewards: Organizational perspectives. In Sanctions and Rewards in the Legal System: A Multidisciplinary Approach. Toronto:
University of Toronto Press.

Baker, SP, B O’Neil, MJ Ginsburg, and G Li. 1992. Injury Fact Book. New York: Oxford University Press.

Benner, L. 1975. Accident investigations—multilinear sequencing methods. J Saf Res 7.

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). 1988. Guidelines for evaluating surveillance systems. Morb Mortal Weekly Rep 37(S-5):1–18.

Davies, JC and DP Manning. 1994a. MAIM: the concept and construction of intelligent software. Saf Sci 17:207–218.

—. 1994b. Data collected by MAIM intelligent software: The first fifty accidents. Saf Sci 17:219-226.

Department of Trade and Industry. 1987. Leisure Accident Surveillance System (LASS): Home and Leisure Accident Research 1986 Data. 11th Annual Report
of the Home Accident Surveillance System. London: Department of Trade and Industry.

Ferry, TS. 1988. Modern Accident Investigation and Analysis. New York: Wiley.

Feyer, A-M and AM Williamson. 1991. An accident classification system for use in preventive strategies. Scand J Work Environ Health 17:302–311.

FMC. 1985. Product Safety Sign and Label System. Santa Clara, California: FMC Corporation.

Gielen, AC. 1992. Health education and injury control: Integrating approaches. Health Educ Q 19(2):203–218.

Goldenhar, LM and PA Schulte. 1994. Intervention research in occupational health and safety. J Occup Med 36(7):763–775.

Green, LW and MW Kreuter. 1991. Health Promotion Planning: An Educational and Environmental Approach. Mountainview, CA: Mayfield Publishing
Company.

Guastello, SJ. 1991. The Comparative Effectiveness of Occupational Accident Reduction Programs. Paper presented at the International Symposium Alcohol
Related Accidents and Injuries. Yverdon-les-Bains, Switzerland, Dec. 2-5.

Haddon, WJ. 1972. A logical framework for categorizing highway safety phenomena and activity. J Trauma 12:193–207.

—. 1973. Energy damage and the 10 countermeasure strategies. J Trauma 13:321–331.

—. 1980. The basic strategies for reducing damage from hazards of all kinds. Hazard Prevention September/October:8–12.

Hale, AR and AI Glendon. 1987. Individual Behaviour in the Face of Danger. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Hale, AR and M Hale. 1972. Review of the Industrial Accident Research Literature. Research paper No. l, Committee on Safety & Health. London: HMSO.

Hale, AR, B Heming, J Carthey and B Kirwan. 1994. Extension of the Model of Behaviour in the Control of Danger. Vol. 3: Extended Model Description.
Sheffield: Health and Safety Executive project HF/GNSR/28.

Hare, VC. 1967. System Analysis: A Diagnostic Approach. New York: Harcourt Brace World.

Harms-Ringdahl, L. 1993. Safety Analysis. Principles and Practice in Occupational Safety. Vol. 289. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Heinrich, HW. 1931. Industrial Accident Prevention. New York: McGraw-Hill.

—. 1959. Industrial Accident Prevention: A Scientific Approach. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Company.

Hugentobler, MK, BA Israel, and SJ Schurman. 1992. An action research approach to workplace health: Intergrating methods. Health Educ Q 19(1):55–76.

International Organization for Standardization (ISO). 1967. Symbols, Dimensions, and Layout for Safety Signs. ISO R557. Geneva: ISO.
www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 43/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention
—. 1984. Safety Signs and Colors. ISO 3864. Geneva: ISO.

—. 1991. Industrial Automation Systems—Safety of Integrated Manufacturing Systems—Basic Requirements (CD 11161). TC 184/WG 4. Geneva: ISO.

—. 1994. Quality Management and Quality Assurance Vocabulary. ISO/DIS 8402. Paris: Association française de normalisation.

Janssen, W. 1994. Seat-belt wearing and driving behavior: An instrumented-vehicle study. Accident analysis and prevention. Accident Anal. Prev. 26: 249-261.

Jenkins, EL, SM Kisner, D Fosbroke, LA Layne, MA Stout, DN Castillo, PM Cutlip, and R Cianfrocco. 1993. Fatal Injuries to Workers in the United States,
1980–1989: A Decade of Surveillance. Cincinnati, OH: NIOSH.

Johnston, JJ, GTH Cattledge, and JW Collins. 1994. The efficacy of training for occupational injury control. Occup Med: State Art Rev 9(2):147–158.

Kallberg, VP. 1992. The Effects of Reflector Posts on Driving Behaviour and Accidents on Two-lane Rural Roads in Finland. Report 59/1992. Helsinki: The
Finnish National Road Administration Technical Development Center.

Kjellén, U. 1984. The deviation concept in occupational accident control. Part I: Definition and classification; Part II: Data collection and assesment of
significance. Accident Anal Prev 16:289–323.

Kjellén, U and J Hovden. 1993. Reducing risks by deviation control—a retrospection into a research strategy. Saf Sci 16:417–438.

Kjellén, U and TJ Larsson. 1981. Investigating accidents and reducing risks—a dynamic approach. J Occup Acc 3:129–140.

Last, JM. 1988. A Dictionary of Epidemiology. New York: Oxford University Press.

Lehto, MR. 1992. Designing warning signs and warning labels: Part I—Guidelines for the practitioner. Int J Ind Erg 10:105–113.

Lehto, MR and D Clark. 1990. Warning signs and labels in the workplace. In Workspace, Equipment and Tool Design, edited by A Mital and W Karwowski.
Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Lehto, MR and JM Miller. 1986. Warnings: Volume I: Fundamentals, Design, and Evaluation Methodologies. Ann Arbor, MI: Fuller Technical Publications.

Leplat, J. 1978. Accident analyses and work analyses. J Occup Acc 1:331–340.

MacKenzie, EJ, DM Steinwachs, and BS Shankar. 1989. Classifying severity of trauma based on hospital discharge diagnoses: Validation of an ICD-9CM to
AIS-85 conversion table. Med Care 27:412–422.

Manning, DP. 1971. Industrial accident-type classifications—A study of the theory and practice of accident prevention based on a computer analysis of
industrial injury records. M.D. Thesis, University of Liverpool.

McAfee, RB and AR Winn. 1989. The use of incentives/feedback to enhance work place safety: A critique of the literature. J Saf Res 20:7-19.

Mohr, DL and D Clemmer. 1989. Evaluation of an occupational injury intervention in the petroleum industry. Accident Anal Prev 21(3):263–271.

National Committee for Injury Prevention and Control. 1989. Injury Prevention: Meeting the Challenge. New York: Oxford University Press.

National Electronic Manufacturers Association (NEMA). 1982. Safety Labels for Padmounted Switch Gear and Transformers Sited in Public Areas. NEMA
260. Rosslyn, VA: NEMA.

Occupational Health and Safety Administration (OSHA). 1985. Specification for Accident Prevention Signs and Tags. CFR 1910.145. Washington DC: OSHA.

—. 1985. [Chemical] Hazard Communication. CFR 1910.1200. Washington DC: OSHA.

Occupational Injury Prevention Panel. 1992. Occupational injury prevention. In Centers for Disease Control. Position Papers from the Third National Injury
Control Conference: Setting the National Agenda for Injury Control in the 1990s. Atlanta, GA: CDC.

Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development (OECD). 1990. Behavioural Adaptation to Changes in the Road Transport System. Paris: OECD.

Rasmussen, J. 1982. Human errors. A taxonomy for describing human malfunction in industrial installations. J Occup Acc 4:311–333.

Rasmussen, J, K Duncan and J Leplat. 1987. New Technology and Human Error. Chichester: Wiley.

Reason, JT. 1990. Human Error. Cambridge: CUP.

Rice, DP, EJ MacKenzie and associates. 1989. Cost of Injury in the United States: A Report to Congress. San Francisco: Institute for Health and Aging,
University of California; and Baltimore: Injury Prevention Center, The Johns Hopkins University.

Robertson, LS. 1992. Injury Epidemiology. New York: Oxford University Press.

Saari, J. 1992. Successful implementation of occupational health and safety programs in manufacturing for the 1990s. J Hum Factors Manufac 2:55–66.

Schelp, L. 1988. The role of organizations in community participation—prevention of accidental injuries in a rural Swedish municipality. Soc Sci Med
26(11):1087–1093.

Shannon, HS. 1978. A statistical study of 2,500 consecutive reported accidents in an automobile factory. Ph.D. thesis, University of London.

Smith, GS and H Falk. 1987. Unintentional injuries. Am J Prev Medicine 5, sup.:143–163.

Smith, GS and PG Barss. 1991. Unintentional injuries in developing countries: The epidemiology of a neglected problem. Epidemiological Reviews :228–266.

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 44/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention
Society of Automotive Engineers (SAE). 1979. Safety Signs. SAE J115: SAE.

Steckler, AB, L Dawson, BA Israel, and E Eng. 1993. Community health development: An overview of the works of Guy W. Stewart. Health Educ Q Sup. 1:
S3-S20.

Steers, RM and LW Porter.1991. Motivation and Work Behavior (5th ed). New York: McGraw-Hill.

Surry, J. 1969. Industrial Accident Research: A Human Engineering Appraisal. Canada: University of Toronto.

Tollman, S. 1991. Community-oriented primary care: Origins, evolutions, applications. Soc Sci Med 32(6):633-642.

Troup, JDG, J Davies, and DP Manning. 1988. A model for the investigation of back injuries and manual handling problems at work. J Soc Occup Med 10:107–
119.

Tuominen, R and J Saari. 1982. A model for analysis of accidents and its applications. J Occup Acc 4.

Veazie, MA, DD Landen, TR Bender and HE Amandus. 1994. Epidemiologic research on the etiology of injuries at work. Ann Rev Pub Health 15:203–21.

Waganaar, WA, PT Hudson and JT Reason. 1990. Cognitive failures and accidents. Appl Cogn Psychol 4:273–294.

Waller, JA. 1985. Injury Control: A Guide to the Causes and Prevention of Trauma. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.

Wallerstein, N and R Baker. 1994. Labor education programs in health and safety. Occup Med State Art Rev 9(2):305-320.

Weeks, JL. 1991. Occupational health and safety regulation in the coal mining industry: Public health at the workplace. Annu Rev Publ Health 12:195–207.

Westinghouse Electric Corporation. 1981. Product Safety Label Handbook. Trafford, Pa: Westinghouse Printing Division.

Wilde, GJS. 1982. The theory of risk homeostasis: Implications for safety and health. Risk Anal 2:209-225.

—. 1991. Economics and accidents: A commentary. J Appl Behav Sci 24:81-84.

—. 1988. Risk homeostasis theory and traffic accidents: propositions, deductions and discussion of dissemsion in recent reactions. Ergonomics 31:441-468.

—. 1994. Target Risk. Toronto: PDE Publications.

Williamson, AM and A-M Feyer. 1990. Behavioural epidemiology as a tool for accident research. J Occup Acc 12:207–222.

Work Environment Fund [Arbetarskyddsfonden]. 1983. Olycksfall i arbetsmiljön—Kartläggning och analys av forskningsbehov [Accidents in the work
environment—survey and analysis]. Solna: Arbetarskyddsfonden

OTHER RELEVANT READINGS


Andersson, R, B Johansson, K Lindén, and L Svanström. 1978. Development of a model for research on occupational accidents. J Occup Acc 1.

Apple, J. 1972. Materials Handling Systems Design. New York: The Ronald Press.

Bainbridge, L. 1983. Ironies of automation. Automatica 19:775-779.

Bamber, L. 1979. Accident costing in industry. Health and Safety at Work (Croyden) 2/4:32–34.

Barnett, R and D Brickman. 1986. Safety hierarchy. J Saf Res 17:49–55.

Booth, R. 1979. Making factories safe for forklift truck drivers. Occup Health 4:193–197.

Collinson, IL. 1980. Safety—the cost of accidents and their prevention. The Mining Engineer (London), 561–571.

Corbett, JM. 1988. Ergonomics in the development of human-centred AMT. Appl Ergon 19:35-39.

Diekershoff, K, W Hamacher, and G Kliemt. 1986. Gefährdungen und Belastungen beim innerbetrieblichen Transport und Verkehr. Die Berufsgenossenschaft
7:378–384.

Franke, A and S Joki. 1975. Die volkwirtschaftlichen Kosten der Arbeitsunfälle [The economic costs of occupational accidents]. Bundesanstalt für
Arbeitsschutz und Unfallforschung. Forschungsbericht Nr. 148. Dortmund: Marten.

Goodstein, LP, HB Anderson, and SE Olsen. 1988. Tasks, Errors and Mental Models. London: Taylor & Francis.

Häkkinen, K. 1992. Failures in materials handling systems with the framework of EN 292. In Proceedings of the Fourth International Conference on Structural
Failure, Product Liability and Technical Insurance, Technical University Vienna July 6–9, 1992, edited by HP Rossmanith. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

—. 1991. Accidents and technological change in materials handling. In XIIth World Congress on Occupational Safety and Health, Hamburg 6–11 May 1990,
proceedings. Sankt Augustin: ILO.

Hale, AR and M Hale. 1970. Accidents in perspective. Occup Psychol 44:115–122.

Health and Safety Executive. 1992. Road Transport in Factories and Similiar Workplaces. Guidance Note GS9(R). London: HMSO.

—. 1982. Transport Kills. A Study of Fatal Accidents in Industry 1978–1980. London: HMSO.

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 45/46
12/23/2019 Chapter 56 - Accident Prevention
Hollnagel, E and D Woods. 1983. Cognitive systems engineering: New wine in new bottles. Int J Man Mach Stud 18:593-600.

Kidd, P. 1994. Skill-based automated manufacturing. In Organization and Management of Advanced Manufacturing Systems, edited by W Karwowski and G
Salvendy. New York: Wiley.

Kuivanen, R. 1990. The impact on safety of disturbances in flexible manufacturing systems. In Ergonomics of Hybrid Automated Systems II, edited by W
Karwowski and M Rahimi. Amsterdam:Elsevier.

LeNet, M. 1978. Le prix de la vie humaine [The cost of a human life]. Notes et études documentaires. No. 4455. Paris: La Documentation française.

Miller, JM, MR Lehto, and JP Frantz. 1994. Warnings and Safety Instructions: Annotated and Indexed. Ann Arbor, MI: Fuller Technical Publications.

Rasmussen, J. 1983. Skills, rules, and knowledge: signals, signs, and symbols, and other distinctions in human performance models. IEEE T Syst Man Cyb
13(3):257–266.

Sinclair, TC. 1972. A Cost-effectiveness Approach to Industrial Safety. London: HMSO.

—. 1975. Safety Alert Symbol for Agricultural, Construction, and Industrial Equipment. SAE J284: SAE.

—. 1983. Slow Moving Vehicle Identification Emblem. SAE J943: SAE.

Sugimoto, N. 1987. Subjects and problems of robot safety technology. In Occupational Safety and Health in Automation and Robotics, edited by K Noro.
London:Taylor & Francis.

Part VIII - Accidents and Safety Management Français

www.ilocis.org/documents/chpt56e.htm 46/46

Você também pode gostar