Você está na página 1de 8

See corresponding editorial on page 1120.

Effect of breastfeeding and sociodemographic factors on visual


outcome in childhood and adolescence1–3
Alicja R Rudnicka, Christopher G Owen, Marcus Richards, Michael EJ Wadsworth, and David P Strachan

ABSTRACT Vision is immature at birth and is programmed in response to


Background: It has been suggested that early life factors, including visual stimuli in early life. There has been considerable interest
breastfeeding and birth weight, program childhood myopia. in whether early nutrition might also influence visual develop-
Objective: We examined the relation of reduced unaided vision ment, particularly whether initial infant feeding may play a role.
(indicative of myopia) in childhood and adolescence with infant It has been suggested that those initially breastfed have better
feeding, parental education, maternal age at birth, birth weight, sex, vision and are less likely to be myopic in later life than are those
birth order, and socioeconomic status. who are formula fed (12–15). The presence of long-chain poly-
Design: Three British cohorts recruited infants born in 1946 (n ҃ unsaturated fatty acids (LCPUFAs) in breast milk, which are

Downloaded from www.ajcn.org by guest on February 12, 2012


5362), 1958 (n ҃ 18 558), and 1970 (n ҃ 16 567). Adjusted odds needed for rapid growth and neural development in early life
ratios (ORs) for unaided vision of 6/12 or worse at ages 10 –11 and (16, 17), has been proposed as a potential explanation for these
15–16 y from each cohort were pooled by using fixed-effects meta- findings, because formula milk does not universally contain
analyses. LCPUFAs (15). Although this has been supported by experimen-
Results: The prevalence of reduced vision ranged from 4.4% to tal evidence showing the benefit on visual outcome of formula
6.5% at 10 –11 y and from 9.4% to 11.4% at 16 y, with marginally milk supplemented with LCPUFAs relative to nonsupplemented
higher levels in later cohorts. Breastfeeding declined across succes- formula (13, 18), other experimental and observational evidence
sive cohorts (65%, 43%, and 22% in those breastfed for 쏜1 mo, has been less supportive (19 –23). Inconsistencies in results may
respectively). Pooled ORs showed no associations between infant reflect variation in statistical power and/or differences in the
feeding and vision after adjustment at either age. Parental education extent of adjustment for potentially confounding factors, such as
(OR: 1.48, high versus low education; 95% CI: 1.23, 1.79), maternal socioeconomic status (14, 15, 24), maternal factors at delivery
age (OR: 1.10, per 5-y increase; 95% CI: 1.04, 1.17), birth weight (14, 15, 25, 26), parental smoking and education (14, 15, 27),
(OR: 0.85, per 1-kg rise; 95% CI: 0.76, 0.95), number of older birth order (24, 27, 28), and differences in size at birth between
siblings (OR: 0.89, per older sibling; 95% CI: 0.83, 0.94), and sex feeding groups (14, 15, 20).
(OR: 1.10, girls versus boys; 95% CI: 0.98, 1.23) were related to Some evidence links these potential confounding factors to
adverse visual outcome in childhood. Stronger associations were visual outcomes in early life. Increasing maternal age at birth
observed in adolescence, except that the association with birth seems to be related to poorer visual outcomes in infancy (14) and
weight was null. is strongly associated with the degree of myopia in early child-
Conclusions: Infant feeding does not appear to influence visual hood (29). In agreement with this latter finding, recent work has
development. Consistent associations of reduced vision with paren- suggested a positive association between birth weight and eye
tal education, sex, maternal age, and birth order suggest that other size in young children (30, 31). A retrospective cohort study did
environmental factors are important for visual development and not find birth weight to be related to visual function in the elderly
myopia in early life. Am J Clin Nutr 2008;87:1392–9. (32). Higher levels of parental education (15, 27), higher socio-
economic status (15, 24, 33), and smaller family size (or birth
order) (24, 27) have been reported to be positively related to the
INTRODUCTION prevalence of myopia. Higher levels of myopia among girls than
Myopia is a leading cause of correctable visual impairment in 1
From the Division of Community Health Sciences, St George’s, Univer-
the developed world and a leading cause of blindness in devel- sity of London, London, United Kingdom (ARR, CGO, and DPS), and the
oping countries (1). It has been estimated that 30.4 million Amer- Medical Research Council National Survey of Health and Development,
icans aged 욷40 y are myopic (2). Reduced vision in childhood is Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, Royal Free Hospital, Uni-
predominantly due to myopia (3– 6) with a shift toward higher versity College Medical School, London, United Kingdom (MR and MEJW).
2
levels of myopia with increasing age (7). Marked geographic Supported by grant no. G0000934 from the Medical Research Council
variations in the prevalence of myopia have been reported in both (ARR) and grant no. PG/04/072 from the British Heart Foundation (CGO).
3
children and adults (8, 9). These variations coupled with the Address reprint requests and correspondence to AR Rudnicka, Division
of Community Health Sciences, St George’s, University of London, Cranmer
recent rapid increases in the prevalence of myopia, especially
Terrace, London SW17 ORE, United Kingdom. E-mail: arudnick@
among children in Asian (10) and other industrialized societies sgul.ac.uk.
(11), suggest that environmental factors are important determi- Received August 1, 2007.
nants of visual outcome. Accepted for publication November 1, 2007.

1392 Am J Clin Nutr 2008;87:1392–9. Printed in USA. © 2008 American Society for Nutrition

Supplemental Material can be found at:


http://www.ajcn.org/content/suppl/2008/05/15/87.5.1392.DC
1.html
INFANT FEEDING AND VISION 1393
among boys observed among some studies (33–35) may also conditions; each eye was tested separately. In the 1958 and 1970
suggest environmental influences on refractive outcome, al- cohorts, near vision was also tested at both ages using the Sheri-
though not all studies have shown similar sex differences (15, dan and Gardiner reduced Snellen near acuity cards at 앒25 cm
24). A comparable finding was observed in studies that examined (10 inches) (49 –51). Two dichotomous outcomes of presumed
the association between infant feeding and cognitive develop- myopia were derived from these vision outcomes. The first used
ment, a neurological outcome related to vision, where failure to a cutoff of unaided distance vision in the better eye of 6/12
adjust systematically for similar confounders (36, 37) may ex- Snellen acuity or worse. The second, used for the 1958 and 1970
plain the apparent inconsistencies in the findings (28, 37– 43). cohorts only, was based on an unaided distance vision of 6/12
To examine the association of early nutrition on vision further, Snellen acuity or worse and unaided near vision of 6/6 or 6/9 at
we examined the association between pattern of infant feeding 25 cm (in the better eye); those with both poor distance and near
and visual outcome in childhood and adolescence in 3 large vision were excluded. This latter definition of presumed myopia
British birth cohorts accounting for the potential confounding was used previously in the 1958 NCDS (24) and is considered to
factors identified above. The cross-cohort comparison allows for be analogous with Ҁ1 to Ҁ5 diopters of myopia. Results from our
the association between infant feeding and visual outcome to be analyses using this definition were not materially different from
gauged over a period when there has been stark changes in the
the visual outcome based on an unaided distance alone and,
rates of breastfeeding and social patterning of feeding in early
therefore, are not presented. This is not unexpected because it
life, together with considerable improvements in childhood nu-
was shown in the 1958 cohort that those with reduced distance
trition (44, 45). In addition, parental educational attainment, es-
vision at 16 y (91%) had myopia in adulthood (6). Children in the
pecially maternal education, would also have changed over this
1946 cohort with distance vision 6/12 or worse at 16 y received
time frame, and this would influence both diet and educational
attainment in the offspring. The available data also allow the an ophthalmic examination; the results showed that 욷61% were

Downloaded from www.ajcn.org by guest on February 12, 2012


strength and consistency of the association between vision and myopic, but 30% with reduced vision were not assessed, so the
other sociodemographic factors across cohorts to be gauged. proportion of myopia in this group is an underestimate and is
likely to be higher.

SUBJECTS AND METHODS


Definition of breastfeeding
Participants were drawn from 3 British birth cohorts with
information on infant feeding practices, vision outcome in child- In the 1946 NSHD, mothers were interviewed when the cohort
hood and adolescence, and information on potential confounding member was 2 y old; infant feeding status and duration of breast-
factors, including socioeconomic status in early childhood or feeding was ascertained for 4784 (89% of the cohort at the outset)
infancy, birth weight, maternal age at delivery, parental educa- participants. In the other 2 cohorts, infant feeding was recalled by
tion, and the number of younger and older siblings (birth order). parental interview when the child was 5 (1970 BCS) or 7 (NCDS)
Ethical approval for the examination of the cohort members was y of age, and the number with data on breastfeeding status was
obtained from the relevant ethical committees at the time of each 12 981 (74%) and 14498 (88%), respectively. Infant feeding
survey. status was classified as not breastfed at all, breastfed (partially or
wholly) for 쏝1 mo, and breastfed for 욷1 mo (in NSHD 1946,
British birth cohort studies duration of breastfeeding in months up to 10 mo was available
From all those born in England, Scotland, and Wales during 1 and in the 1970 BCS it was also possible to identify those breast-
wk in March 1946, a sample was selected for follow-up of single fed for 욷3 mo). Those with unknown infant feeding status were
infants born to married women. All such births to wives of non- excluded from the analyses throughout.
manual and agricultural workers were selected together with 1 in
4 of all such infants born to wives of manual workers to form the
Definition of other risk factors
Medical Research Council National Survey of Health and De-
velopment (NSHD; n ҃ 5362), also known as the British 1946 1946 NSHD
birth cohort (46). The next cohort recruited all persons born in
England, Scotland, and Wales during 1 wk in March 1958 for a In the 1946 cohort, socioeconomic status was defined in the
Perinatal Mortality Survey (n ҃ 17 638); this then became study sampling design according to the Registrar General system
known as the National Child Development Study (NCDS), or the and was dichotomized to manual and nonmanual groups at birth;
British 1958 birth cohort (47). The third cohort, the 1970 British this dichotomization was preserved for the analyses. Birth
Cohort Study (1970 BCS), recruited 98% of persons (n ҃ 16567) weight, sex, maternal age, parental education, and birth order
born in Great Britain during 1 wk in April 1970 (48). Participants were recorded during the birth survey. Parental education at birth
from all cohorts were followed up periodically from birth was classified as primary schooling only, primary or secondary
through childhood and into adulthood. The current analysis is education with formal qualifications after secondary school (but
based on data collected from birth up to age 16 y. no diploma), and diplomas and professional degrees (highest
group); the highest level achieved by either parent was used.
Definition of visual outcome From birth order the number of older siblings was defined, and
Vision was measured when the children were aged 11 and 15 y the number of children at age 11 y was obtained by parental
in the 1946 cohort, 11 and 16 y in the 1958 cohort, and 10 and 16 y questionnaire, thereby allowing the number of younger siblings
in the 1970 cohort. In all 3 cohorts, unaided distance vision (ie, to be determined. In the analyses, the number of younger and
without optical correction) was measured with the use of con- older siblings was used to take account of birth order and family
ventional Snellen charts of block capitals under standardized size.
1394 RUDNICKA ET AL

1958 NCDS RESULTS


Maternal age, birth weight, and sex were obtained from the The overall prevalence of reduced vision at 10 –11 and 15–16
1958 Perinatal Mortality Survey. Socioeconomic status in child- y in the 3 cohorts is summarized by all risk factors considered in
hood was based on Registrar General classification of the father’s the analyses in Table 1. Overall, the prevalence of distance
occupation in 1958 or at age 7 y if data were unavailable at birth. vision of 6/12 or worse at 10 –11 and 15–16 y was 6.0% and 9.4%
The age at which the father left full-time education was ascer- in the 1946 NSHD, 6.5% and 10% in the 1958 NCDS, and 4.4%
tained when the cohort member was 7 and 11 y of age. Data at age and 11.4% in the 1970 BCS. The lower prevalence of poor vision
7 y were preferred; however, data from age 11 y were used if data in childhood for the 1970 BCS may in part be explained by the
at age 7 y were missing. The age at which the mother left full-time fact that vision was assessed 1 y earlier in this cohort. It is known
education was also ascertained when the cohort member was that myopia increases with age, and rapid changes in prevalence
11 y. Parental education was classified into 3 groups: left school occur at these ages (11). Logistic regression of the log odds of
at the minimum age of 15 y (or younger), remained in full-time poor vision at 15–16 y showed some evidence of an increase from
education to the age of 18 y, and remained in full-time education 1948 to 1970 (P ҃ 0.004), but vision was assessed 1 y earlier in
beyond the age of 18 y; the highest level achieved by either parent the 1946 NSHD. Hence, this trend may have been partly or
was used in the analyses. At age 11 y, the number of younger wholly confounded by age.
siblings and the child’s position in relation to all siblings was The prevalence of breastfeeding fell markedly across the 3
obtained by parental questionnaire. successive cohorts. Of those with complete data for these anal-
yses, the proportions breastfed for 쏜1 mo were 65% in the 1946
1970 BCS NSHD, 43% in the 1958 NCDS, and 22% in the 1970 BCS. The
Birth weight and sex were recorded in the original birth survey. prevalence of breastfeeding for 쏜1 mo was more common in

Downloaded from www.ajcn.org by guest on February 12, 2012


Age of the mother at delivery was ascertained by parental inter- nonmanual social classes and with increasing parental education,
view when the child was 5 y of age. Socioeconomic status in especially among the latter 2 cohorts (See Table S1 under “Sup-
childhood was defined by paternal occupation (using the Regis- plemental data” in the online issue.). There is some evidence that
trar General classification) if available; if not available, it was by infants with lower birth weights and older siblings were less
maternal occupation when the child was 10 y old. Family size, likely to have been breastfed. No clear association of infant
including the number of older and younger siblings, and parental feeding to maternal age was observed (See Table S1 under “Sup-
education were determined at the same age. Parental education plemental data” in the online issue.). The patterns observed
was classified into 3 groups using qualifications indicative of among those with complete data on all risk factors did not ma-
leaving full-time education at 울16 y, 16 –18 y, or 쏜18 y of age. terially change when those without complete data were included.
Again, the highest level achieved by either parent was used in the
analyses. Infant feeding and visual outcome in later life
In the 1946 NSHD and 1958 NCDS, the prevalence of distance
Statistical analysis vision of 6/12 or worse at 10 –11 and 15–16 y of age was similar
Statistical analyses were performed by using STATA version in those breastfed and formula fed. In the 1970 BCS, the preva-
9 (STATA Corporation, College Station, TX). The prevalences lence of reduced vision was marginally lower in those who were
of unaided distance vision 6/12 or worse at ages 10 –11 and formula fed, but these crude differences were not statistically
15–16 y were compared by a chi-square test in those breastfed for significant (Table 1). None of the corresponding odds ratios
쏜1 mo and those never breastfed. Chi-square tests were also used showed any evidence of an association of breastfeeding with
to compare the prevalences of reduced vision by social class reduced vision after adjustment for potential confounders, in-
(manual versus nonmanual), sex, level of parental education, cluding socioeconomic status in childhood, sex, parental educa-
maternal age (쏝25 y, 25 to 쏝35 y, and 욷35 y), quintiles of birth tion, maternal age, birth weight, and number of younger and older
weight, and number of younger (0, 1–2, and 욷3) and older (0, siblings (Table 2). The pooled fixed-effects estimate of reduced
1–2, and 욷3) siblings. On the basis of the number of individuals vision comparing those breastfed for 쏜1 mo with those never
in each cohort with vision outcome data, we used logistic regres- breastfed was 1.03 (95% CI: 0.90, 1.18) at 10 –11 y and 1.02
sion of grouped data to examine trends in the log odds of poor (95% CI: 0.89, 1.16) at 15–16 y.
visual outcome over the 3 cohorts. Trends were examined for The duration of breastfeeding defined in each cohort was 욷1
child and adolescent age groups separately. Within each cohort, mo, and this potentially diluted associations with longer dura-
multiple variable logistic regression was used to obtain mutually tions of breastfeeding. It was possible to examine the association
adjusted odds ratios for the vision outcomes for infant feeding of breastfeeding for 욷3 mo in the 1970 BCS, and, in the 1946
group, socioeconomic position in childhood, sex, and parental NSHD, the duration of exclusive breastfeeding in months up to
education as categorical variables and maternal age at delivery, 12 mo was available. There was still no association between infant
birth weight, and number of younger and older siblings as con- feeding on visual outcome, either in childhood or adolescence,
tinuous variables. Adjusted odds ratios from each cohort were when this prolonged duration and exclusivity of breastfeeding
pooled by using a fixed-effects (inverse variance) meta-analysis (1946 birth cohort only) was considered (data not presented).
throughout. Heterogeneity across cohorts was examined by us-
ing chi-square tests (52), and tests for trend of the odds ratios Other factors associated with visual outcome in childhood
across the 3 cohorts were examined by using meta-regression and adolescence
analysis (metareg command in STATA). All analyses were re- In general, the adjusted odds ratios were consistent across
stricted to those with complete data on the relevant vision out- the cohorts, and tests for heterogeneity were not statistically
come and all risk factors considered above. significant for any risk factor, except for mid versus low level
INFANT FEEDING AND VISION 1395
TABLE 1
Prevalence of unaided distance vision of 6/12 or worse by all risk factors included in the analyses in the participants with complete data on vision outcome

Prevalence of unaided distance vision of 6/12 or worse

10 –11 y 15–16 y
1946 NSDH 1958 NCDS 1970 BCS 1946 NSHD 1958 NCDS 1970 BCS
Risk factor (n ҃ 3510) (n ҃ 10 588) (n ҃ 9076) (n ҃ 3288) (n ҃ 8239) (n ҃ 3925)

%
Infant feeding
Formula fed 6.0 6.4 4.0 9.3 9.4 10.5
Breast fed 쏜1 mo 5.7 6.8 5.2 9.6 10.6 12.1
P1 0.30 0.53 0.07 0.86 0.23 0.09
Sex
Male 5.7 6.3 4.1 8.5 9.0 10.5
Female 6.3 6.8 4.8 10.4 11.0 12.2
P1 0.42 0.25 0.09 0.07 0.002 0.09
Socioeconomic status
Nonmanual 6.1 7.4 4.9 10.8 12.4 13.3
Manual 5.8 6.2 4.2 7.8 9.0 10.0
P1 0.70 0.03 0.12 0.003 쏝0.001 0.001
Parental education

Downloaded from www.ajcn.org by guest on February 12, 2012


Left school at minimum age 5.7 5.8 4.1 7.6 8.9 10.0
Further education 6.5 6.8 4.5 12.5 9.8 10.6
Higher education 6.8 10.3 5.8 13.5 17.0 16.5
P1 0.55 쏝0.001 0.01 쏝0.001 쏝0.001 쏝0.001
Maternal age at birth
쏝25 y 6.0 5.8 4.2 9.2 8.4 9.8
25 to 쏝35 y 6.3 7.0 4.4 9.6 10.6 12.2
욷35 y 4.9 6.4 6.2 9.1 11.3 14.6
P1 0.38 0.05 0.05 0.93 0.003 0.01
Birth weight quintile2
1 6.3 7.2 5.5 9.7 10.2 11.4
2 6.5 6.7 5.3 10.7 10.2 10.0
3 6.0 7.0 3.4 8.7 10.7 8.7
4 4.9 5.9 4.6 6.9 8.7 14.1
5 5.8 6.0 3.4 10.2 10.1 12.1
P1 0.76 0.30 0.002 0.15 0.31 0.007
Number of younger siblings
0 6.4 6.0 4.1 9.3 10.0 10.8
1–2 5.7 7.0 4.8 9.4 10.3 12.1
욷3 5.3 6.2 4.5 10.1 8.6 5.6
P1 0.60 0.17 0.31 0.92 0.27 0.32
Number of older siblings
0 6.9 7.5 4.7 11.2 11.2 12.5
1–2 5.5 6.0 4.1 8.4 9.5 10.3
욷3 4.7 5.0 4.9 7.3 6.4 12.1
P1 0.13 0.02 0.35 0.01 쏝0.001 0.11
Total prevalence 6.0 6.5 4.4 9.4 10.0 11.4
1
P values are from chi-square tests for crude comparisons of proportions.
2
Birth weight quintile groups are as follows: 1946 Medical Research Council National Survey of Health and Development (NSHD): 1st ҃ 쏝2.9 kg, 2nd
҃ 2.9 to 쏝3.3 kg, 3rd ҃ 3.3 to 쏝3.6 kg, 4th ҃ 3.6 to 쏝3.8 kg, 5th 욷3.8 kg; 1958 National Child Development Study (NCDS): 1st ҃ 쏝2.9 kg, 2nd ҃ 2.9 to
쏝3.2 kg, 3rd ҃ 3.2 to 쏝3.5 kg, 4th ҃ 3.5 to 쏝3.8 kg, 5th 욷3.8 kg; and 1970 British Cohort Study (BCS): 1st ҃ 쏝2.9 kg, 2nd ҃ 2.9 to 쏝3.2 kg, 3rd ҃ 3.2
to 쏝3.4 kg, 4th ҃ 3.4 to 쏝3.7 kg, 5th 욷3.7 kg.

of parental education in adolescence (Table 2 and Table 3). manual versus nonmanual social classes (95% CI: 0.78, 0.99).
Females were more likely to have reduced vision than were In all 3 cohorts, unaided distance vision of 6/12 or worse was
males (Tables 2 and 3), especially in adolescence, with a more likely in those with higher levels of parental education
pooled odds ratio of 1.23 (95% CI: 1.11, 1.37). Individuals than in those with lower levels; this was highly statistically
from a manual social class were less likely to have poor vision significant at both ages (Tables 2 and 3) with stronger asso-
at both ages than were those from a nonmanual social class. ciations in adolescence. The pooled odds ratio for the com-
Although socioeconomic status did not show a strong associ- parison of the highest with the lowest level of parental edu-
ation within each cohort, the pooled estimate at 15–16 y sug- cation in adolescence showed a 69% increase in the odds of
gests a 12% reduction in the odds of reduced vision in the poor unaided distance vision (95% CI: from 43% to 100%
1396 RUDNICKA ET AL

TABLE 2
Adjusted odds ratios (and 95% CIs) for the association of each risk factor listed with unaided distance vision of 6/12 or worse at 10 –11 y of age1

Adjusted odds ratios (95% CI)

1946 NSHD 1958 NCDS 1970 BCS Pooled estimate2 P for


Risk factor (n ҃ 3510) (n ҃ 10 588) (n ҃ 9076) (n ҃ 23 174) heterogeneity3

Breast fed 쏜1 mo vs formula fed 0.91 (0.65, 1.29) 0.99 (0.83, 1.19) 1.19 (0.93, 1.53) 1.03 (0.90, 1.18) 0.39
Female vs male 1.13 (0.85, 1.50) 1.07 (0.92, 1.25) 1.14 (0.93, 1.40) 1.10 (0.98, 1.23) 0.85
Manual vs nonmanual socioeconomic status 1.02 (0.75, 1.38) 0.98 (0.81, 1.17) 1.00 (0.78, 1.28) 0.99 (0.87, 1.13) 0.97
Parental education
Mid vs low 1.10 (0.79, 1.54) 1.15 (0.96, 1.37) 1.10 (0.78, 1.56) 1.13 (0.98, 1.31) 0.96
High vs low 1.18 (0.72, 1.92) 1.76 (1.32, 2.34)4 1.34 (1.00, 1.80) 1.48 (1.23, 1.79)4 0.26
Maternal age at birth per 5-y increase 1.02 (0.88, 1.19) 1.09 (1.00, 1.18) 1.18 (1.06, 1.31)4 1.10 (1.04, 1.17)4 0.26
Birth weight
per 1-kg rise 0.95 (0.72, 1.27) 0.89 (0.76, 1.07) 0.75 (0.62, 0.91)4 0.85 (0.76, 0.95)5 0.26
Per no. of younger siblings 0.93 (0.80, 1.08) 1.04 (0.97, 1.11) 1.18 (1.03, 1.36)5 1.04 (0.99, 1.10) 0.06
Per no. of older siblings 0.88 (0.76, 1.01) 0.85 (0.78, 0.92)4 0.95 (0.86, 1.05) 0.89 (0.83, 0.94)4 0.24
1
Odds ratio within each cohort are from logistic regression adjusted for all exposures listed in the table, and those breast fed for 쏝1 mo were included in
the analysis as a separate group. NSHD, Maternal Research Council National Survey of Health and Development; NCDS, National Child Development Study;
BCS, British Cohort Study.
2
Fixed-effects meta-analysis using inverse variance weighting.

Downloaded from www.ajcn.org by guest on February 12, 2012


3
Chi-square test for heterogeneity.
4
P 쏝 0.01.
5
P 쏝 0.05.

increase). Parental education was related to social class in the outcome, especially in the 1970 BCS. However, no associa-
cohorts, and exclusion of social class from the model resulted tion was observed by adolescence. The number of younger
in stronger associations for parental education and vice versa siblings showed no consistent association with visual out-
(data not shown). A higher maternal age at birth was related to come at either age. However, an increase in the numbers of
reduced vision both in childhood (odds ratio: 1.10 per 5-y older siblings was consistently associated with a decreased
increase in maternal age; 95% CI: 1.04, 1.17) and in adoles- risk of reduced vision, both at 10 –11 and at 15–16 y of age and
cence (odds ratio: 1.15; 95% CI: 1.09, 1.22); this association across cohorts (Tables 2 and 3). In childhood, the risk of poor
possibly strengthened across successive cohorts (P value for vision decreased by 11% (95% CI: 6% to 17% reduction) and
trend in odds ratios: 0.06). In childhood, heavier birth weight by 16% (95% CI: 11% to 20% reduction) in adolescence per
was marginally associated with a reduced risk of poor visual number of older siblings.

TABLE 3
Adjusted odds ratios (and 95% CIs) for the association of each risk factor listed with unaided distance vision of 6/12 or worse at 15–16 y of age1

Adjusted odds ratios (95% CIs)

1946 NSHD 1958 NCDS 1970 BCS Pooled estimate2 P for


Risk factor (n ҃ 3288) (n ҃ 8239) (n ҃ 3925) (n ҃ 15 452) heterogeneity3

Breast fed 쏜1 mo vs formula fed 0.97 (0.73, 1.30) 1.05 (0.88, 1.25) 0.99 (0.77, 1.26) 1.02 (0.89, 1.16) 0.88
Female vs male 1.27 (1.00, 1.61) 1.24 (1.10, 1.43)4 1.20 (0.98, 1.47) 1.23 (1.11, 1.37)4 0.94
Manual vs nonmanual socioeconomic status 0.84 (0.65, 1.09) 0.85 (0.72, 1.01) 0.96 (0.75, 1.21) 0.88 (0.78, 0.99)5 0.69
Parental education
Mid vs low 1.60 (1.21, 2.10)4 1.09 (0.92, 1.29) 1.02 (0.72, 1.43) 1.18 (1.03, 1.35)5 0.04
High vs low 1.66 (1.13, 2.45)4 1.79 (1.38, 2.31)4 1.60 (1.21, 2.10)4 1.69 (1.43, 2.00)4 0.83
Maternal age at birth per 5-y increase 1.04 (0.92, 1.19) 1.15 (1.06, 1.24)4 1.23 (1.11, 1.37)4 1.15 (1.09, 1.22)4 0.16
Birth weight
per 1-kg rise 1.01 (0.79, 1.28) 0.96 (0.83, 1.10) 1.08 (0.89, 1.31) 1.00 (0.90, 1.11) 0.61
Per no. of younger siblings 1.03 (0.92, 1.16) 1.00 (0.94, 1.07) 0.94 (0.81, 1.10) 1.00 (0.95, 1.06) 0.65
Per no. of older siblings 0.88 (0.78, 0.99)5 0.82 (0.76, 0.89)4 0.85 (0.76, 0.96)4 0.84 (0.80, 0.89)4 0.57
1
Odds ratio within each cohort are from logistic regression adjusted for all exposures listed in the table, and those breast fed for 쏝1 mo were included in
the analysis as a separate group. NSHD, Maternal Research Council National Survey of Health and Development; NCDS, National Child Development Study;
BCS, British Cohort Study.
2
Fixed-effects meta-analysis using inverse variance weighting.
3
Chi-square test for heterogeneity.
4
P 쏝 0.01.
5
P 쏝 0.05.
INFANT FEEDING AND VISION 1397
DISCUSSION of age, children born in 1958 were 앒3.5 cm taller than those born
Analyses of data from these 3 birth cohorts showed that there in 1948. Similarly, at age 16 y, those born in 1970 were 앒2.5 cm
were no differences in visual outcome in childhood or adoles- taller than those born in 1958. However, there was no evidence
cence among those initially breastfed for 쏜1 mo compared with of any differential association of breastfeeding versus formula
those formula fed. Rates of breastfeeding fell across successive feeding, at either age, on vision by tertiles of height in childhood
cohorts considerably, but there was no evidence of a subsequent or adolescence (data not shown).
increase in adverse visual outcome in childhood, although, there
was a small increase in adverse visual outcome in adolescence Factors associated with unaided distance vision of 6/12 or
from 1946 to 1970. However, this latter observation does not take worse
into consideration important confounding factors, especially the
Although early feeding showed little association with visual
age at which vision was assessed. Notably, within each individ-
outcome, other sociodemographic factors showed stronger in-
ual cohort there was no relation between mode of infant feeding
fluences. A 10% higher prevalence of poor unaided vision in girls
and visual outcome. The strongest determinants of unaided dis-
than in boys was consistently observed at 10 –11 y of age across
tance vision of 6/12 or worse at both ages were educational
the 3 cohorts. Similar sex differences have been reported in visual
attainment of the parents, maternal age, and number of older
outcome with the use of the same cutoffs used in this study, in
siblings; birth weight appeared to be important at age 10 –11 y,
children aged as young as 6 y (35), and in levels of myopia among
with no influence at 15–16 y. A higher prevalence of adverse
Singaporean children aged 7–9 y (10). The magnitude of differ-
visual outcome in girls than in boys emerged in childhood and
ence between girls and boys was more marked in adolescence
was more apparent by adolescence.
(23%), and a higher prevalence of myopia in females has been
shown to persist into adulthood in contemporary cohorts (2). The

Downloaded from www.ajcn.org by guest on February 12, 2012


Infant feeding and adverse visual outcomes reason for this sex difference remains unclear, but the develop-
Previous studies have suggested better visual outcome in in- ment of the difference over time may reflect that this is environ-
fancy (12, 13) and early childhood (14) among those who were mentally programmed from an early age. There is a consistent
breastfed than among those who were formula fed. A cross- association across cohorts of parental education, maternal age,
sectional study in Singaporean children of an age (10 –12 y) and number of older siblings (indicating a higher risk for first
similar to that of the participants in the present study suggested births) to unaided distance vision, with trends emerging in child-
that this association may persist into later childhood, when the hood and becoming stronger by adolescence. These associations
risk of myopia was found to be 42% lower in those initially were observed previously, but were not quantified collectively,
breastfed than in those who were formula fed (adjusted odds with adjustment for potential confounders (24, 27, 49, 50). This
ratio: 0.58; 95% CI: 0.39, 0.84) (15). However, the refractive study has enabled us to show that parental education was more
status of this South Asian population was markedly different strongly related to poor vision than was social class in this age
from that of UK children (ie, Singaporean children had myopia group. This pattern appears to develop in childhood and appears
levels 10 times those of UK children), and the findings may not stronger in adolescence, which may explain why associations
be comparable. Although, we do not have direct data on refrac- were not observed in other subjects at an earlier age (35). The
tive status during childhood for these UK cohorts, impaired un- positive association between risk of adverse visual outcome and
aided distance vision was predominantly due to refractive error maternal age may partly be explained by maternal education,
(mostly myopia) in this age group. Reassuringly, the prevalence because highly educated mothers tend to postpone their first
of reduced vision in these cohorts was comparable with estimates birth. The average age at first birth was lowest in the 1970 BCS
from other white populations of similar age (3, 53) and agrees and highest in the 1946 NSHD. This agrees with population
well with estimates of myopia in contemporary studies of white values from England and Wales, which showed a decline in the
children.(54, 55) Unlike the South Asian study in this age group average age of first births during this time period (56). Despite
(15), these UK cohorts collected data on exposures prospectively the strong association observed between maternal age and visual
without knowledge of visual outcome, eliminating the potential outcome, these time trends in maternal age do not appear to have
for bias in recalling initial feeding status. The detailed informa- materially influenced levels of vision in these different cohorts.
tion ascertained on early feeding practices, particularly in the In the current analyses, a 1-kg increase in birth weight reduced
1946 and 1970 birth cohorts, enabled us to show that the duration the risk of poor vision in childhood, with no effect being observed
and exclusivity of breastfeeding (1946 birth cohort only) exerted by adolescence. Measures of growth in early life, including birth
little influence on visual outcome. In addition, childhood vision weight (31), length, and head circumference at birth (30, 31) and
has remained relatively stable over a time period when there has height in childhood have been found to be associated with re-
been a stark decline in rates of initial breastfeeding (65%, 43%, fractive error and ocular biometric measures in young children
and 22% in those breastfed for 쏜1 mo in the 1946, 1958, and (57, 58). A study from Denmark showed that the association
1970 cohorts, respectively). Hence, our findings provide strong between birth weight and reduced vision might persist into adult-
evidence that infant feeding is unrelated to visual outcome in hood (59). However, in the current study, it is not clear why the
childhood and adolescence in a European setting. This result may relation observed in childhood was not replicated in adolescence.
not be applicable to populations in developing countries with The degree to which myopia is determined by genes or envi-
poorer nutrition in early life. It remains possible that the benefits ronmental influences (or both) is much debated. Although family
of breastfeeding on visual outcome in later life might be masked, studies have reported correlations in refractive error across gen-
particularly in later cohorts, by overall improvements in early erations and within twins (60 – 63), this may reflect shared envi-
nutrition (44, 45). Comparison of heights, as a marker of early ronments rather than shared genes. We have shown that first
nutrition, across the cohorts at the same age showed that at 11 y births are at highest risk of poor vision, and this risk declines as
1398 RUDNICKA ET AL

the number of older siblings increases. It is likely that environ- among adults in the United States, Western Europe, and Australia. Arch
mental factors related to prolonged near vision and emphasis on Ophthalmol 2004;122:495–505.
3. Junghans B, Kiely PM, Crewther DP, Crewther SG. Referral rates for a
academic achievements are also likely to be correlated with sib- functional vision screening among a large cosmopolitan sample of Aus-
ship and birth order. In the 1946 cohort, those from smaller tralian children. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 2002;22:10 –25.
families received better care and had higher cognitive perfor- 4. Kleinstein RN, Jones LA, Hullett S, et al. Refractive error and ethnicity
mance than did those from relatively larger families (64). There in children. Arch Ophthalmol 2003;121:1141–7.
is a large body of evidence of an association between the indi- 5. Lam CS, Goldschmidt E, Edwards MH. Prevalence of myopia in local
and international schools in Hong Kong. Optom Vis Sci 2004;81:317–
vidual level of educational attainment and prevalence of myopia 22.
(11), but the causal pathways remain unclear. Certainly, the 6. Cumberland PM, Peckham CS, Rahi JS. Inferring myopia over the life-
worldwide urban versus rural comparisons of the prevalence of course from uncorrected distance visual acuity in childhood. Br J Oph-
myopia are consistent with a near work hypothesis that increased thalmol 2007;91:151–3.
7. Weale RA. Epidemiology of refractive errors and presbyopia. Surv Oph-
reading and computer use may be a risk factor for myopia (65).
thalmol 2003;48:515– 43.
Gene loci for high levels of myopia have been identified (66 –70), 8. Chow YC, Dhillon B, Chew PT, Chew SJ. Refractive errors in Singapore
but not for low to mid levels. Hence, these lower levels of my- medical students. Singapore Med J 1990;31:472–3.
opia, which are more common, may be more environmentally 9. Saw SM. A synopsis of the prevalence rates and environmental risk
determined. factors for myopia. Clin Exp Optom 2003;86:289 –94.
10. Saw SM, Tong L, Chua WH, et al. Incidence and progression of myopia
in Singaporean school children. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2005;46:
Conclusions 51–7.
11. Morgan I, Rose K. How genetic is school myopia? Prog Retin Eye Res
Eyes are part of the wider base of neurodevelopment, and our 2005;24:1–38.
findings are contrary to the results of studies linking improved 12. Birch E, Birch D, Hoffman D, Hale L, Everett M, Uauy R. Breast-feeding

Downloaded from www.ajcn.org by guest on February 12, 2012


cognitive development with breast feeding rather than formula and optimal visual development. J Pediatr Ophthalmol Strabismus 1993;
feeding (37, 39 – 42). It has been has suggested that these effects 30:33– 8.
13. Carlson SE, Ford AJ, Werkman SH, Peeples JM, Koo WW. Visual acuity
are subject to residual confounding (36), especially from mater- and fatty acid status of term infants fed human milk and formulas with
nal intelligence (43). Breastfeeding is associated with early life and without docosahexaenoate and arachidonate from egg yolk lecithin.
social environment, and this influences both childhood behavior Pediatr Res 1996;39:882– 8.
and lifestyle. It is important that such confounders operating 14. Williams C, Birch EE, Emmett PM, Northstone K. Stereoacuity at age
across the life course are identified and taken into consideration, 3.5 y in children born full-term is associated with prenatal and postnatal
dietary factors: a report from a population-based cohort study. Am J Clin
as we have done in the current study, to examine the association Nutr 2001;73:316 –22.
between early nutrition and vision outcomes in later life. Breast- 15. Chong YS, Liang Y, Tan D, Gazzard G, Stone RA, Saw SM. Association
feeding is beneficial for many reasons, but does not appear to between breastfeeding and likelihood of myopia in children. JAMA
offer any protection against poor unaided vision (as a marker of 2005;293:3001–2.
16. Huttenlocher PR, de Courten C. The development of synapses in striate
myopia) in childhood or adolescence in a Westernized setting. In cortex of man. Hum Neurobiol 1987;6:1–9.
countries with poorer nutrition in early life, the associations 17. Blakemore C. Mysteries in the making of the cerebral cortex. Ciba Found
between breastfeeding and visual outcome may be different. Symp 1995;193:1–20.
Whether early myopia can be regarded as a neurological outcome 18. Morale SE, Hoffman DR, Castaneda YS, Wheaton DH, Burns RA, Birch
is debatable, because it may represent a physiologic response to EE. Duration of long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids availability in the
diet and visual acuity. Early Hum Dev 2005;81:197–203.
the wider environment associated with educational levels and 19. Innis SM, Nelson CM, Lwanga D, Rioux FM, Waslen P. Feeding for-
near work that accompanies modern economic development mula without arachidonic acid and docosahexaenoic acid has no effect
(11). We have shown that more important predictors of poor on preferential looking acuity or recognition memory in healthy full-
unaided vision include parental education, maternal age, birth term infants at 9 mo of age. Am J Clin Nutr 1996;64:40 – 6.
order, and possibly birth weight. However, unlike infant feeding 20. Makrides M, Neumann MA, Jeffrey B, Lien EL, Gibson RA. A random-
ized trial of different ratios of linoleic to alpha-linolenic acid in the diet
practices, these latter factors are less modifiable. Hence, the of term infants: effects on visual function and growth. Am J Clin Nutr
burden of early myopia seems likely to increase because of in- 2000;71:120 –9.
creasing economic development. 21. Auestad N, Halter R, Hall RT, et al. Growth and development in term
infants fed long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids: a double-masked,
We acknowledge the original data creators, depositors, copyright holders, randomized, parallel, prospective, multivariate study. Pediatrics 2001;
and funders of the Data Collections and the UK Data Archive (University of 108:372– 81.
Essex, Colchester, United Kingdom) for use of data from the 1970 BCS and 22. Innis SM, Akrabawi SS, Diersen-Schade DA, Dobson MV, Guy DG.
the National Child and Development Survey. They bear no responsibility for Visual acuity and blood lipids in term infants fed human milk or formu-
the analysis or interpretation of this data. lae. Lipids 1997;32:63–72.
The authors’ responsibilities were as follows—ARR and CGO: drafted the 23. Krasevec JM, Jones PJ, Cabrera-Hernandez A, Mayer DL, Connor WE.
Maternal and infant essential fatty acid status in Havana, Cuba. Am J
manuscript, carried out the statistical analysis, and as guarantors accepted full
Clin Nutr 2002;76:834 – 44.
responsibility for the integrity of the work as a whole. All authors contributed 24. Peckham CS, Gardiner PA, Goldstein H. Acquired myopia in 11-year-
substantially to the conception and design of this study, had access to the data, old children. Br Med J 1977;1:542–5.
contributed to the revision of the manuscript, and approved the final version 25. Carlson SE, Cooke RJ, Rhodes PG, Peeples JM, Werkman SH. Effect of
to be published. None of the authors had any conflicts of interest to declare. vegetable and marine oils in preterm infant formulas on blood arachi-
donic and docosahexaenoic acids. J Pediatr 1992;120(suppl):S159 – 67.
26. Guesnet P, Pugo-Gunsam P, Maurage C, et al. Blood lipid concentrations
of docosahexaenoic and arachidonic acids at birth determine their rela-
REFERENCES tive postnatal changes in term infants fed breast milk or formula. Am J
1. Congdon NG, Friedman DS, Lietman T. Important causes of visual Clin Nutr 1999;70:292– 8.
impairment in the world today. JAMA 2003;290:2057– 60. 27. Douglas JWB, Ross JM, Simpson HR. The ability and attainment of
2. Kempen JH, Mitchell P, Lee KE, et al. The prevalence of refractive errors short-sighted pupils. J Royal Stat Soc (series A) 1967;130:479 –503.
INFANT FEEDING AND VISION 1399
28. Rodgers B. Feeding in infancy and later ability and attainment: a longi- children in the National Child Development Study (1958 cohort). Public
tudinal study. Dev Med Child Neurol 1978;20:421– 6. Health 1976;90:271– 80.
29. Huynh SC, Wang XY, Ip J, et al. Prevalence and associations of aniso- 51. Butler NR, Golding J, Haslum M, Stewart-Brown S. Recent findings
metropia and aniso-astigmatism in a population based sample of 6 year from the 1970 child health and education study: preliminary communi-
old children. Br J Ophthalmol 2006;90:597– 601. cation. J R Soc Med 1982;75:781– 4.
30. Ojaimi E, Robaei D, Rochtchina E, Rose KA, Morgan IG, Mitchell P. 52. Deeks JJ, Altman DG, Bradburn MJ. Statistical methods for examining
Impact of birth parameters on eye size in a population-based study of heterogeneity and combining results from several studies in meta-
6-year-old Australian children. Am J Ophthalmol 2005;140:535–7. analysis. In: Egger M, Davey Smith G, Altman DG, eds. Systematic
31. Saw SM, Tong L, Chia KS, et al. The relation between birth size and the reviews in health care: meta-analysis in context. London, United King-
results of refractive error and biometry measurements in children. Br J dom: BMJ Books, 2001:285–312.
Ophthalmol 2004;88:538 – 42. 53. Lee DJ, Gomez-Marin O, Lam BL, Ma F, Vilar NF. Prevalence of
32. Hall N, Sayer AA, Evans J, Rauf A, Cooper C, Wormald R. Visual acuity usual-corrected distance visual acuity impairment in Hispanic and non-
and its relationship to early growth, eye disease, and aging in north Hispanic children and adolescents. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol 2000;14:
Hertfordshire. Ophthalmic Epidemiol 1998;5:83–90. 357– 62.
33. Sperduto RD, Seigel D, Roberts J, Rowland M. Prevalence of myopia in 54. Laatikainen L, Erkkila H. Refractive errors and other ocular findings in
the United States. Arch Ophthalmol 1983;101:405–7. school children. Acta Ophthalmol (Copenh) 1980;58:129 –36.
34. Goldschmidt E. On the etiology of myopia. An epidemiological study. 55. Blum HL, Peter HB, Bettman JW. Vision screening for elementary
Acta Ophthalmol (Copenh) 1968;suppl 98:1. schools. Los Angeles, CA: University of California Press, 1959.
35. Robaei D, Rose K, Ojaimi E, Kifley A, Huynh S, Mitchell P. Visual 56. Office of National Statistics. Births: 1938-2004. Mean age of women at
acuity and the causes of visual loss in a population-based sample of marriage and at live birth, England and Wales. London, United King-
6-year-old Australian children. Ophthalmology 2005;112:1275– 82. dom: ONS, 2006. [Historical Series FM1, no, 13 (27, 28, 29, and 33)].
36. Drane DL, Logemann JA. A critical evaluation of the evidence on the 57. Ojaimi E, Morgan IG, Robaei D, et al. Effect of stature and other an-
association between type of infant feeding and cognitive development. thropometric parameters on eye size and refraction in a population-based
Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol 2000;14:349 –56. study of Australian children. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2005;46:
37. Jain A, Concato J, Leventhal JM. How good is the evidence linking 4424 –9.

Downloaded from www.ajcn.org by guest on February 12, 2012


breastfeeding and intelligence? Pediatrics 2002;109:1044 –53. 58. Saw SM, Chua WH, Hong CY, et al. Height and its relationship to
38. Rogers IS, Emmett PM, Golding J. The incidence and duration of breast refraction and biometry parameters in Singapore Chinese children. In-
feeding. Early Hum. Dev 1997;49(suppl):S45–74. vest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2002;43:1408 –13.
39. Lucas A, Morley R, Cole TJ, Lister G, Leeson-Payne C. Breast milk and 59. Olsen J, Sorensen HT, Steffensen FH, et al. The association of indicators
subsequent intelligence quotient in children born preterm. Lancet 1992; of fetal growth with visual acuity and hearing among conscripts. Epide-
339:261– 4. miology 2001;12:235– 8.
40. Golding J, Rogers IS, Emmett PM. Association between breast feeding,
60. Wu MM, Edwards MH. The effect of having myopic parents: an analysis
child development and behaviour. Early Hum Dev 1997;49(suppl):
of myopia in three generations. Optom Vis Sci 1999;76:387–92.
S175– 84.
61. Lee KE, Klein BE, Klein R, Fine JP. Aggregation of refractive error and
41. Anderson JW, Johnstone BM, Remley DT. Breast-feeding and cognitive
5-year changes in refractive error among families in the Beaver Dam Eye
development: a meta-analysis. Am J Clin Nutr 1999;70:525–35.
Study. Arch Ophthalmol 2001;119:1679 – 85.
42. Richards M, Hardy R, Wadsworth ME. Long-term effects of breast-
feeding in a national birth cohort: educational attainment and midlife 62. Lyhne N, Sjolie AK, Kyvik KO, Green A. The importance of genes and
cognitive function. Public Health Nutr 2002;5:631–5. environment for ocular refraction and its determiners: a population based
43. Der G, Batty GD, Deary IJ. Effect of breast feeding on intelligence in study among 20 – 45 year old twins. Br J Ophthalmol 2001;85:1470 – 6.
children: prospective study, sibling pairs analysis, and meta-analysis. 63. Wojciechowski R, Congdon N, Bowie H, Munoz B, Gilbert D, West SK.
BMJ 2006;333:945. Heritability of refractive error and familial aggregation of myopia in an
44. Prynne CJ, Paul AA, Mishra GD, Hardy RJ, Bolton-Smith C, elderly American population. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2005;46:
Wadsworth ME. Sociodemographic inequalities in the diet of young 1588 –92.
children in the 1946 British birth cohort. Public Health Nutr 2002;5: 64. Douglas JWB. The home and the school. London, United Kingdom:
733– 45. McGibbon & Kee, 1964:129 –32.
45. Wadsworth MEJ, Butterworth S, Montgomery S, Ehlin A, Bartley M. 65. Mutti DO, Mitchell GL, Moeschberger ML, Jones LA, Zadnik K. Pa-
Changing Britain, changing lives. In: Ferri E, Bynner J, Wadsworth rental myopia, near work, school achievement, and children’s refractive
MEJ, eds. London, United Kingdom: Institute of Education Press, 2003: error. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2002;43:3633– 40.
221. 66. Young TL, Ronan SM, Alvear AB, et al. A second locus for familial high
46. Wadsworth ME, Butterworth SL, Hardy RJ et al. The life course pro- myopia maps to chromosome 12q. Am J Hum Genet 1998;63:1419 –24.
spective design: an example of benefits and problems associated with 67. Young TL, Atwood LD, Ronan SM, et al. Further refinement of the
study longevity. Soc Sci Med 2003;57:2193–205. MYP2 locus for autosomal dominant high myopia by linkage disequi-
47. Power C, Elliott J. Cohort profile: 1958 British birth cohort (National librium analysis. Ophthalmic Genet 2001;22:69 –75.
Child Development Study). Int J Epidemiol 2006;35:34 – 41. 68. Naiglin L, Gazagne C, Dallongeville F, et al. A genome wide scan for
48. Bynner D, Butler N, Ferri E, Shepherd P, Smith K. The design and familial high myopia suggests a novel locus on chromosome 7q36. J Med
conduct of the 1999-2000 surveys of the national child development Genet 2002;39:118 –24.
study and the 1970 British birth cohort study. UK data archive. London: 69. Lam DS, Tam PO, Fan DS, Baum L, Leung YF, Pang CP. Familial high
Centre for Longitudinal Studies, Institute of Education, 2002. (CLS myopia linkage to chromosome 18p. Ophthalmologica 2003;217:
Cohort Studies Working Paper 1.) 115– 8.
49. Peckham C, Adams B. Vision screening in a national sample of 11-year- 70. Paluru P, Ronan SM, Heon E, et al. New locus for autosomal dominant
old children. Child Care Health Dev 1975;1:93–106. high myopia maps to the long arm of chromosome 17. Invest Ophthalmol
50. Peckham C, Pearson R. Preliminary findings at the age of 16 years on Vis Sci 2003;44:1830 – 6.

Você também pode gostar