Você está na página 1de 12

Reviews and Overviews

A Meta-Analysis of Cognitive Remediation in Schizophrenia

Susan R. McGurk, Ph.D. Objective: This study evaluated the ef- chosocial functioning (0.36), and a small
fects of cognitive remediation for improving effect size for symptoms (0.28). The ef-
Elizabeth W. Twamley, Ph.D. cognitive performance, symptoms, and fects of cognitive remediation on psycho-
psychosocial functioning in schizophrenia. social functioning were significantly stron-
David I. Sitzer, Ph.D. Method: A meta-analysis was conducted ger in studies that provided adjunctive
of 26 randomized, controlled trials of cog- psychiatric rehabilitation than in those
Gregory J. McHugo, Ph.D. nitive remediation in schizophrenia in- that provided cognitive remediation
cluding 1,151 patients. alone.
Kim T. Mueser, Ph.D. Results: Cognitive remediation was asso- Conclusions: Cognitive remediation pro-
ciated with significant improvements duces moderate improvements in cogni-
across all three outcomes, with a medium tive performance and, when combined
effect size for cognitive performance with psychiatric rehabilitation, also im-
(0.41), a slightly lower effect size for psy- proves functional outcomes.

(Am J Psychiatry 2007; 164:1791–1802)

C ognitive impairment is a core feature of schizophre-


nia, with converging evidence showing that it is strongly
its presumed effects on psychosocial functioning and im-
proved response to rehabilitation. Therefore, a critical ex-
related to functioning in areas such as work, social rela- amination of the effects of cognitive remediation on func-
tionships, and independent living (1, 2). Furthermore, tional outcomes is necessary in order to determine its
cognitive functioning is a robust predictor of response to potential role in the treatment of schizophrenia.
psychiatric rehabilitation (i.e., systematic efforts to im- In recent years, the number of studies that examined
prove the psychosocial functioning of persons with severe psychosocial functioning has grown sufficiently to permit
mental illness) (3), including outcomes such as work, so- a closer look at the impact of cognitive remediation. We
cial skills, and self-care (1, 4, 5). Because of the importance conducted a meta-analysis of controlled studies to evalu-
of cognitive impairment in schizophrenia, it has been ate the effects of cognitive remediation on cognitive func-
identified as an appropriate target for interventions (6). tioning, symptoms, and functional outcomes. We also ex-
Currently available pharmacological treatments have amined whether characteristics of cognitive remediation
limited effects on cognition in schizophrenia (7, 8) and programs (e.g., hours of cognitive training), the provision
even less impact on community functioning (9). To address of adjunctive psychiatric rehabilitation, treatment set-
the problem of cognitive impairment in schizophrenia, a tings, patient demographics, or type of control group was
range of cognitive remediation programs has been devel- related to improved outcomes. We hypothesized that cog-
oped and evaluated over the past 40 years. These programs nitive remediation would improve both cognitive func-
employ a variety of methods, such as drill and practice ex- tioning and psychosocial adjustment. We also hypothe-
ercises, teaching strategies to improve cognitive function- sized that programs that provided more hours of cognitive
ing, compensatory strategies to reduce the effects of per- training would have stronger effects on cognitive func-
sistent cognitive impairments, and group discussions. tioning and that adjunctive psychiatric rehabilitation
would be associated with greater improvements in func-
Several reviews of research on cognitive rehabilitation in
tional outcomes.
schizophrenia have been published (10–13). The general
conclusions from these reviews have been that cognitive
remediation leads to modest improvements in perfor- Method
mance on neuropsychological tests but has no impact on
Studies for the meta-analysis were identified by conducting
functional outcomes. However, these reviews were limited MEDLINE and PsycINFO searches for English language articles
by the relatively small number of studies that actually mea- published in peer-reviewed journals. The following search terms
sured psychosocial functioning, precluding any definitive were used: cognitive training, cognitive remediation, cognitive re-
conclusions about the effects of cognitive remediation on habilitation, and schizophrenia. Studies meeting the following cri-
teria were included: 1) a randomized, controlled trial of a psycho-
psychosocial adjustment or the identification of program
social intervention designed to improve cognitive functioning; 2)
characteristics that may contribute to such effects. The ra- an assessment of performance with at least one neuropsychologi-
tionale for cognitive remediation is chiefly predicated on cal measure that had the potential to reflect generalization of ef-

Am J Psychiatry 164:12, December 2007 ajp.psychiatryonline.org 1791


COGNITIVE REMEDIATION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA

TABLE 1. Neuropsychological Assessments Included in Each Cognitive Domain


Domain Assessment
Attention/vigilance Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS) information and mental control subtests
Search-a-Word
Cancellation tasks
Continuous Performance Tests
Span of apprehension
Labyrinth Test
Sustained Attention Test
Span: hits, time, and overall
Preattentional processing
Cross-over reaction time
Cross-modal reaction time
Embedded Figures Test
COGLAB apprehension/masking
Dichotic listening tasks
Speed of processing Trail Making Test, Parts A and B
WAIS, WAIS-R, or WAIS-III digit symbol subtest
Stroop Test, color and word conditions
Reaction time tests
Letters and category fluency
Verbal working memory WAIS, WAIS-R, WAIS-III, or WMS digit span
WAIS-III letter-number sequencing and arithmetic subtests
Digit Span Distractibility Test
Other digit span tasks
Trained Word Recall Task
Other arithmetic tasks
Sentence span
Dual span
Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test
Nonverbal working memory Wechsler Memory Scale—Revised (WMS-R) visual span
Dual span
Verbal learning and memory WMS, WMS-R, or WMS-III logical memory and verbal paired associates subtests
California Verbal Learning Test
Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test
Hopkins Verbal Learning Test
Word List Recall Task
Verbal learning paradigm
Denman Neuropsychological Memory Test
Span-Completeness Verbal Learning Test
Visual learning and memory WMS, WMS-R, or WMS-III visual recall, visual reproduction, faces, and figural memory subtests
Memory for Designs Test
Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test
Kimura recurring figures
Denman Neuropsychological Memory Test
Reasoning and problem solving WAIS, WAIS-R, or WAIS-III similarities and picture arrangement subtests
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children mazes subtest
Stroop Test interference condition
Independent Living Scale—problem solving
Gorham’s Proverbs Test and other proverb interpretation tasks
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test
Trail Making Test (B – A)
Hinting Task
Labyrinth Test
Tower of Hanoi
Tower of London
Response inhibition
Six elements
Categories
COGLAB card sorting test
Social cognition Social perception (Emotion Matching Test and Emotion Labeling Test)
Bell-Lysaker Emotion Recognition Test
Social cognition
Other cognitive
Cognitive measures of multiple domains Global cognitive scores
Mini-Mental State Examination
Cognitive measures not considered sensi-
tive to change Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test
Shipley Institute of Living Scale IQ estimate
WAIS-R comprehension subtest
Verbal IQ
Cognitive measures lacking consensus Cognitive style
Hayling Sentence Completion Task
(continued)

1792 ajp.psychiatryonline.org Am J Psychiatry 164:12, December 2007


McGURK, TWAMLEY, SITZER, ET AL.

TABLE 1. Neuropsychological Assessments Included in Each Cognitive Domain (continued)


Purdue Pegboard
Tactile performance
WMS orientation subtest
Symptoms Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms
Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms
Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale
Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale
Holtzman Inkblot Test
Paranoid Depression Scale
Present State Exam
Thought, language, and communication
Functioning Bay Area Functional Performance Evaluation
Percent “sick talk”/incoherence during the interview
Life skills profile
Global Assessment Scale
Nurses’ Observation Scale for Inpatient Evaluation
Disability Assessment Schedule
Employment
Social Behaviour Schedule
Micro-Module Learning Test
Assessment of Interpersonal Problem-Solving Skills
Social adjustment

fects rather than assessments on trained tasks only; 3) data avail- evaluated by computing the Q statistic (18). Then the significance
able on either group means and standard deviations for baseline level of the mean effect sizes was computed by conducting fixed-
and postintervention cognitive tests or statistics from which effect effects linear models except when the Q statistic indicated signif-
sizes could be calculated; 4) a minimum of 75% of the sample re- icant within-group heterogeneity, in which case we used random
ported to have schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, or schizo- effects models. Moderator analyses were then conducted on
phreniform disorder. those domains with significant heterogeneity, based on the Q sta-
tistic, to determine whether any participant, setting, or program
Categorization of Neuropsychological Tests variables explained variations between studies in effect sizes.
Neuropsychological tests were grouped into the following cog- These analyses were performed by clustering studies into two
nitive domains described by the Measurement and Treatment Re- contrasting groups based on the moderator variable and comput-
search to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia (MATRICS) con- ing the Q between and Q within statistics (18).
sensus panel (6): attention/vigilance, speed of processing, verbal
Moderator Variables
working memory, nonverbal working memory, verbal learning
and memory, visual learning and memory, reasoning/problem Several variables were considered as potential moderators of
solving, and social cognition. Each of the neuropsychological cognitive remediation. Each moderator variable was divided into
measures used in the studies meeting the inclusion criteria was two levels based on a median split. The moderator variables and
assigned to one cognitive domain by consensus of the first three levels were 1) participant characteristics: age (years) (15–37/38–
authors. Measures for which no consensus could be reached, that 50), 2) the setting (inpatient/outpatient), 3) the type of control
were judged to reflect more than one cognitive domain, or that group (active control [e.g., another intervention, such as cogni-
the MATRICS panel deemed not sensitive to change were not in- tive behavior therapy or motivational interviewing]/passive con-
cluded in the meta-analysis. Table 1 summarizes which neuro- trol [e.g., viewing educational videos or treatment as usual]), 4)
psychological tests were included in each cognitive domain. program characteristics: type of intervention (drill and practice/
drill and practice plus strategy coaching or strategy coaching
Calculation of Effect Sizes alone), hours of practice (determined for the overall program as
Effect sizes were calculated by using posttreatment group well as individual cognitive domains), and the provision of ad-
means and standard deviations (14), pre-post difference scores, junctive psychiatric rehabilitation (no/yes).
or analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) or multivariate analysis of co- Some programs that provided training in social cognition em-
variance (MANCOVA) F values that covaried baseline scores on ployed a combination of cognitive remediation and other rehabil-
the dependent measures. Effect sizes can generally be catego- itation approaches, such as social skills training (19, 20), whereas
rized as small (0.2), medium (0.5), or large (0.8) (15). When a study others employed strictly cognitive remediation methods, such as
reported data from multiple measures classified in the same cog- computer-based training tasks (21). The number of hours of social
nitive domain, the mean of the effect sizes from those measures cognition training was included in the total number of cognitive
was used. Effect size distributions were evaluated for outliers, re- remediation hours only for the programs that did not combine the
sulting in exclusion of results of one study from the functional training with another rehabilitation approach. A variety of psychi-
outcome analyses (16). atric rehabilitation approaches were provided in conjunction with
cognitive remediation, including social skills training (20, 22), so-
Meta-Analytic Procedure cial skills/social perception training (19, 23), supported employ-
ment (24), vocational rehabilitation (25), and vocational rehabili-
Meta-analyses were conducted with BioStat software (17). In
tation and social information processing groups (26).
order to control for study differences in sample size when mean
effect sizes were computed, studies were weighted according to
their inverse variance estimates. To determine whether mean ef- Results
fect sizes were statistically significant, the confidence interval (CI)
and z transformation of the effect size were used. The homogene- Data from 26 studies (1,151 subjects) were included.
ity of the effect sizes across studies for each outcome domain was The studies, characteristics of participants and programs,

Am J Psychiatry 164:12, December 2007 ajp.psychiatryonline.org 1793


COGNITIVE REMEDIATION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA

TABLE 2. Description of Randomized, Controlled Trials of Cognitive Remediation in Schizophrenia


Sample Characteristics— Cognitive Remedia-
Inpatient/Outpatient; Mean Age Drill and Practice/ tion Program In-
(years); Mean Education (years); Drill and Strategy cluded Psychiatric
Author % Male Treatment group (N) Coaching Rehabilitation
Wagner (1968) (45), Inpatients; 44.8; not reported; Noncomputerized attention Drill and practice No
treatment 1 100% training (N=8)

Wagner (1968) (45), Inpatients; 44.8; not reported; Noncomputerized abstraction Drill and practice No
treatment 2 100% training (N=8)

Wagner (1968) (45), Inpatients; 44.8; not reported; Noncomputerized attention and Drill and practice No
treatment 3 100% abstraction training (N=16)

Meichenbaum & Cameron Inpatients; 36.0; not reported; Noncomputerized training using Drill and practice No
(1973), includes 3-week 100% self-talk (N=5)
follow-up (16)

Benedict & Harris (1989) Inpatients; 30.3; not reported; Computerized training with Drill and practice No
(46) not reported advancement criteria (N=10)

Olbrich & Mussgay (1990) Inpatients; 30.7; 10.2; 57% Noncomputerized training (N=15) Drill and practice No
(43)

Hermanutz & Gestrich Inpatients; 31.0; 11.0; not Computerized attention training Drill and practice No
(1991) (42) reported (N=10)

Benedict et al. (1994) (47) Outpatients; 38.8; 11.0; 52% Computerized attention training Drill and practice No
(N=16)

Burda et al. (1994) (48) Inpatients; 46.6; 12.5; 97% Computerized training using Drill and practice No
Captain’s Log (N=40)

Field et al. (1997) (49) Outpatients; 28.6; not reported; Computerized training (N=5) Drill and practice No
90%

Medalia et al. (1998) (50) Inpatients; 32.5; 10.8; 78% Computerized attention training Drill and practice No
using orientation remedial mod-
ule (N=27)
Spaulding et al. (1999) (20) Inpatients; 35.7; 11.9; 63% Noncomputerized training using Drill and strategy Social skills training
integrated psychological therapy coaching groups
(N=49)

Wykes et al. (1999) (27); Outpatients; 38.5; 12.0; 76% Noncomputerized training with Drill and strategy No
Wykes et al. (2003) (30), errorless learning (N=17) coaching
6-month follow-up

Medalia et al. (2000) (51), Inpatients; 37.7; 11.5; 59% Computerized problem-solving Drill and strategy No
treatment 1 training (N=18) coaching
Medalia et al. (2000) (51), Inpatients; 36.5; 10.5; 59% Computerized memory training Drill and practice No
treatment 2 (N=18)

1794 ajp.psychiatryonline.org Am J Psychiatry 164:12, December 2007


McGURK, TWAMLEY, SITZER, ET AL.

Hours/Weeks of Average Effect


Cognitive Remediation Cognitive Effect Size Average Effect Size Average Effect Size for
Control Group (active/ (excluding other (follow-up effect size in for Cognitive Size for Symptom Functioning
passive control, N) treatment) parentheses) Measures Measures Measures
Viewing treatment group 3 hours/1 week Visual learning memory= 1.10
stimuli without 0.80; reasoning and
responding (active problem solving=1.40
control, N=8)
Viewing treatment group 3 hours/1 week Visual learning memory= –0.04
stimuli without –1.21; reasoning and
responding (active problem solving=1.12
control, N=8)
Viewing treatment group 3 hours/1 week Visual learning memory= 0.32
stimuli without –0.09; reasoning and
responding (active problem solving=0.55
control, N=8)
Task practice without self- 4.1 hours/3 weeks Verbal working memory= 1.02 (1.65) 1.89 (2.08) 3.50 (3.99)
talk (active control, N=5) 0.77 (1.31); reasoning
and problem solving=
1.26 (1.99)
1. Computerized training 12.5 hours/8–14 Speed of processing=1.57 1.57
without advancement weeks
criteria (active control,
N=10); 2. Treatment as
usual (passive control,
N=10)
Arts and crafts groups 12 hours/3 weeks Attention/vigilance=0.52; 0.40 0.00
(active control, N=15) Verbal working mem-
ory=0.43; reasoning and
problem solving=0.26
1. Integrated psychologi- 7.5 hours/3–4 weeks Treatment versus active Treatment versus ac- Treatment versus Treatment
cal therapy focusing on control: attention/vigi- tive control=0.18; active control= versus active
cognitive, communica- lance=0.18; treatment treatment versus 0.43; treatment control=–0.47;
tion, and social training versus passive control: passive control= versus passive treatment
(active control, N=10); attention/vigilance= –0.09 control=0.24 versus passive
2. Treatment as usual –0.09 control=–0.46
(passive control, N=10)
Treatment as usual 12.5 hours/3–5 weeks Attention/vigilance=0.41; 0.27
(passive control, N=17) verbal learning and
memory=0.13
Treatment as usual 12 hours/8 weeks Speed of processing=0.58; 0.51
(passive control, N=29) attention/vigilance=
0.65; verbal working
memory=0.89; verbal
learning and memory=
0.69; visual learning
memory=–0.26
Graphics-based computer 6 hours/3 weeks Speed of processing=0.74; 0.79
games (active control, attention/vigilance=
N=5) 1.08; reasoning and
problem solving=0.54;
other=0.00
Watching National Geo- 6 hours/6 weeks Attention/vigilance=0.19 0.19 0.24
graphic documentaries
(passive control, N=27)
Group supportive therapy 68.3 hours/26 weeks Speed of processing=0.02; 0.08 0.55 0.53
emphasizing social skills attention/vigilance=
(active control, N=42) 0.38; verbal learning
and memory=–0.06;
visual learning memory=
–0.06; reasoning and
problem solving=0.14
Intensive occupational 24–40 hours/8–10 Speed of processing=0.26; 0.20 (0.20) 0.59 0.05
therapy (active control, weeks verbal working mem-
N=16) ory=0.27 (0.07); nonver-
bal working memory=
0.06; reasoning and
problem solving=0.20
(0.33)
Treatment as usual 4.2 hours/5 weeks Verbal learning and –0.43
(passive control, N=18) memory=–0.43
Treatment as usual 4.2 hours/5 weeks Verbal learning and –0.39
(passive control, N=18) memory=0.39 (continued)

Am J Psychiatry 164:12, December 2007 ajp.psychiatryonline.org 1795


COGNITIVE REMEDIATION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA

TABLE 2. Description of Randomized, Controlled Trials of Cognitive Remediation in Schizophrenia (continued)


Sample Characteristics— Cognitive Remedia-
Inpatient/Outpatient; Mean Age Drill and Practice/ tion Program In-
(years); Mean Education (years); Drill and Strategy cluded Psychiatric
Author % Male Treatment group (N) Coaching Rehabilitation
Bell et al. (2001) (26) Outpatients in work therapy; Computerized training using Drill and practice Vocational rehabilita-
43.6; 13.2; 78% CogReHab plus weekly social tion and social
information processing group information
(N=31) processing groups

Bell et al. (2003) (52), Same as above (N=47)


includes follow-up
Fiszdon et al. (2004) (53), Same as above (N=45)
includes follow-up
Fiszdon et al. (2005) (54) Same as above (N=57)

Van der Gaag (2002) (23) Inpatients; 31.1; not reported; Noncomputerized training (N=21) Drill and strategy Social skills/social
64% coaching perception training

Bellucci et al. (2002) (34) Outpatients; 42.0; 12.6; 47% Computerized training using Drill and practice No
Captain’s Log (N=17)

López-Luengo & Vázquez Outpatients; 33.5; not reported; Noncomputerized training using Drill and practice No
(2003) (55) 83% attention process training (N=13)

Hogarty et al. (2004) (19), Outpatients; 37.3; not reported; Computerized training using Drill and strategy Social skills/social
includes 12-month 59% orientation remedial module and coaching perception training
follow-up CogReHab plus group social groups
cognition exercises (N=67)

Ueland & Rund (2005) (21); Inpatients; 15.3; not reported; Computerized and non- Drill and strategy No
Ueland & Rund (2004) 50% computerized training (N=14) coaching
(44), 12-month follow-up

McGurk et al. (2005) (24) Outpatients in supported Computerized training using Drill and strategy Supported
employment; 37.5; 11.2; 55% Cogpack (N=23) coaching employment

Sartory et al. (2005) (56) Inpatients; 36.4; 10.3; 67% Computerized training using Drill and practice No
Cogpack (N=21)

Silverstein et al. (2005) (22) Inpatients; 39.3; 10.6; 87% Noncomputerized training using Drill and practice Social skills training
attention process training and groups
shaping (N=18)

Vauth et al. (2005) (25) Inpatients in vocational Computerized training using Drill and strategy Vocational
rehabilitation; 30.0; 12.5; 65% Cogpack and noncomputerized coaching rehabilitation
training, plus cognitive
adaptation therapy (N=47)

1796 ajp.psychiatryonline.org Am J Psychiatry 164:12, December 2007


McGURK, TWAMLEY, SITZER, ET AL.

Hours/Weeks of Average Effect


Cognitive Remediation Cognitive Effect Size Average Effect Size Average Effect Size for
Control Group (active/ (excluding other (follow-up effect size in for Cognitive Size for Symptom Functioning
passive control, N) treatment) parentheses) Measures Measures Measures
Treatment as usual 36 hours/26 weeks Speed of processing=0.31; 0.50
(passive control, N=34) verbal working mem-
ory=0.30; reasoning and
problem solving=0.50;
social=0.90
Passive control, N=55 Verbal working memory= 0.40 (0.48)
0.40 (0.48)
Passive control, N=49 Verbal working memory= 0.53 (0.66)
0.53 (0.66)
Passive control, N=68 Verbal learning and mem- 0.36
ory=0.36
Leisure/games group 4 hours/11 weeks Speed of processing= 0.26
(active control, N=21) –0.02; attention/vigi-
lance=0.09; verbal
learning and memory=
0.41; visual learning
memory=0.47; reason-
ing and problem solv-
ing=0.09; social=0.51
Treatment as usual 8 hours/8 weeks Speed of processing=0.56; 0.52 0.32
(passive control, N=17) verbal working mem-
ory=0.52; verbal learn-
ing and memory=0.49
Treatment as usual 24 hours/43 weeks Speed of processing=0.32; 0.53
(passive control, N=11) attention vigilance=
0.54; verbal working
memory=0.48; verbal
learning and memory=
0.57; reasoning and
problem solving=0.45
Enriched supportive ther- 75 hours/104 weeks Speed of processing=0.83 0.60 (0.67) –0.07 (0.09) 0.37 (0.51)
apy including psychoed- (0.86); social=0.36 (0.66)
ucation, illness self-
management, and stress
management (active
control, N=54)
Treatment as usual 30 hours/12 weeks Attention/vigilance=0.31 0.37 (0.33) 0.25 (0.51) 0.31 (0.16)
(passive control, N=12) (0.28); verbal working
memory=0.54 (0.60);
verbal learning and
memory=0.33 (0.29);
visual learning mem-
ory=0.11 (0.17); reason-
ing and problem solv-
ing=0.57 (0.31)
Treatment as usual 24 hours/12 weeks Speed of processing=0.27; 0.33 0.45 1.76
(passive control, N=21) verbal working mem-
ory=0.42; verbal learn-
ing and memory=0.45;
reasoning and problem
solving=0.18
Treatment as usual 15 hours/3 weeks Speed of processing=0.69; 0.78
(passive control, N=21) verbal learning and
memory=0.88
Treatment as usual 18 hours/6 weeks Attention/vigilance=0.25; 0.37 0.41 0.68
(passive control, N=13) verbal working mem-
ory=0.38; verbal learn-
ing and memory=0.48
1. Vocational rehabilita- 24 hours/8 weeks Treatment versus active Treatment versus Treatment versus Treatment
tion and self-manage- control; attention/vigi- active control= active control= versus active
ment training for nega- lance=0.46; verbal 0.54; treatment 0.44; treatment control=0.10;
tive symptoms (active learning and memory= versus passive versus passive treatment
control, N=45); 2. Treat- 0.61; treatment versus control=0.54 control=0.19 versus passive
ment as usual (passive passive control; atten- control=0.46
control, N=46) tion/vigilance=0.46; ver-
bal learning and mem-
ory=0.55; reasoning and
problem solving=0.60 (continued)

Am J Psychiatry 164:12, December 2007 ajp.psychiatryonline.org 1797


COGNITIVE REMEDIATION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA

TABLE 2. Description of Randomized, Controlled Trials of Cognitive Remediation in Schizophrenia (continued)


Sample Characteristics— Cognitive Remedia-
Inpatient/Outpatient; Mean Age Drill and Practice/ tion Program In-
(years); Mean Education (years); Drill and Strategy cluded Psychiatric
Author % Male Treatment group (N) Coaching Rehabilitation
Penadés et al. (2006) (29), Outpatients; 35.1; 10.2; 57% Noncomputerized training using Drill and practice No
includes 6-month follow- frontal/executive program with
up errorless learning (N=20)

and effect sizes are displayed in Table 2. The mean sample hours of cognitive remediation were unrelated to program
size was 50 (SD=36, range=10–138). The mean age of the type (χ2=0.4, df=1, n.s.).
participants was 36.3 years (SD=6.0, range of means=15–
47), the mean years of education was 11.8 (SD=1.0, range Effects on Symptoms and Functioning
of means=10–13), 69% of the participants were men, and Cognitive remediation was associated with a small ef-
60% were inpatients. The mean duration of cognitive re- fect size for symptoms (0.28) and between a small and a
mediation programs was 12.8 weeks (SD=20.9, range=1– medium effect size for functioning (0.35). There was sig-
104). Programs targeted for training an average of 2.9 cog- nificant heterogeneity in the effect sizes for functioning
nitive domains (SD=1.6, range=1–6), whereas changes in (Q=25.7, df=11, p<0.01), but not for symptoms. Moderator
cognitive functioning were assessed on an average of 3.1 analyses indicated that cognitive remediation resulted in
cognitive domains (SD=1.6, range=1–6). Sixty-nine per- stronger effect sizes for improved psychosocial function-
cent of the programs used a drill and practice interven- ing in studies that provided adjunctive psychiatric rehabil-
tion; 23% provided adjunctive psychosocial rehabilitation. itation (0.47) compared to no psychiatric rehabilitation
(0.05) (Q=5.5, df=1, p<0.01.), cognitive remediation pro-
Effects on Cognitive Performance grams that used drill and practice plus strategy coaching
Only one study examined changes in nonverbal working (0.62) compared to drill and practice only (0.24) (Q=4.6,
memory (27), so this domain was not included in the df=1, p<0.05), and studies that included older (0.55) rather
meta-analysis. The effect sizes and related statistics for than younger (0.18) patients (Q=5.7, df=1, p<0.05). Pro-
overall cognition and the other seven individual cognitive gram type was unrelated to age and to adjunctive psychi-
domains are provided in Table 3. In addition, the effect atric rehabilitation, but age and adjunctive psychiatric re-
sizes for overall cognitive functioning for each study are habilitation were significantly associated (χ2=6.7, df=1,
depicted in Figure 1. The effect size for overall cognition p<0.05). Studies that provided psychiatric rehabilitation
was significant, as well as for six of the seven domains of tended to serve older patients.
cognitive performance. Most of the effects were in the me-
dium or low-medium effect size range, indicating im-
Discussion
proved cognitive performance after cognitive remedia-
tion. The effect size for visual learning and memory was The results provide support for the effects of cognitive
not significant (0.09). remediation on improving cognitive functioning in
Six studies also reported cognitive data at follow-up (16, schizophrenia, with effect sizes in the medium range for
19, 21, 28–30). For these studies, the average effect size at overall cognitive functioning (0.41) and six of the seven
posttreatment was 0.56 (t=4.8, df=5, p<0.001, CI=0.33– cognitive domains (0.39–0.54). The effects of cognitive re-
0.79; Q=3.4, df=5, n.s.), and at follow-up, it was 0.66 (t=5.7, mediation on cognitive performance were remarkably
df=5, p<0.001, CI=0.43–0.89; Q=7.8, df=5, n.s.). Similar to similar across the 26 studies included in the analysis de-
the results at posttreatment, cognitive remediation was spite differences in length and training methods between
associated with improved overall cognitive performance cognitive remediation programs, inpatient/outpatient
an average of 8 months later. setting, patient age, and provision of adjunctive psychiat-
Hedges’s Q was significant for only one cognitive do- ric rehabilitation. The results indicate that cognitive reme-
main, verbal learning and memory, indicating significant diation produced robust improvements in cognitive func-
heterogeneity in effect sizes to evaluate the effects of mod- tioning across a variety of program and patient conditions.
erators. For this domain, a larger effect size was associated The effect sizes of cognitive remediation were homoge-
with more hours of cognitive remediation (0.57) com- neously distributed across studies for overall cognitive
pared with fewer hours (0.29) (Q=3.7, df=1, p<0.05) and functioning and six of the seven cognitive domains, pre-
with drill and practice (0.48) compared with drill and prac- cluding the examination of moderators of treatment ef-
tice plus strategy coaching (0.23) (Q=2.0, df=1, p<0.05); fects for most cognitive outcomes. Thus, contrary to our

1798 ajp.psychiatryonline.org Am J Psychiatry 164:12, December 2007


McGURK, TWAMLEY, SITZER, ET AL.

Hours/Weeks of Average Effect


Cognitive Remediation Cognitive Effect Size Average Effect Size Average Effect Size for
Control Group (active/ (excluding other (follow-up effect size in for Cognitive Size for Symptom Functioning
passive control, N) treatment) parentheses) Measures Measures Measures
Cognitive behavioral ther- 40 hours/16 weeks Speed of processing=0.56 1.01 (1.42) –0.15 (–0.15) 0.42 (0.45)
apy for psychosis (active (1.02); verbal working
control, N=20) memory=0.80 (0.76);
verbal learning and
memory=1.19 (2.07);
visual learning mem-
ory=0.75 (1.22); reason-
ing and problem solv-
ing=1.76 (2.05)

hypothesis, the number of hours programs devoted to Cognitive remediation also had a significant effect on
cognitive remediation was not related to the amount of improving psychosocial functioning, with an average ef-
improvement in overall cognitive functioning. However, fect size of 0.35, just slightly lower than the average effect
hours of training, as well as use of drill and practice rather size of 0.41 for improved cognitive performance. For ex-
than combined drill and practice with strategy coaching, ample, patients who participated in cognitive remediation
were related to improvements in verbal learning and showed greater improvements in obtaining and working
memory, suggesting that this domain may be more sensi- competitive jobs (24, 25), the quality of and satisfaction
tive to the method and extent of cognitive remediation. with interpersonal relationships (19), and the ability to
It is possible that a relatively limited amount of cogni- solve interpersonal problems (20). These findings are
tive remediation (e.g., 5–15 hours) is sufficient to produce unique because until recently a sufficient number of stud-
ies had not measured functional outcomes from which to
improved cognitive functioning and that all studies pro-
draw firm conclusions. The impact of cognitive remedia-
vided an adequate amount of treatment. Alternatively, the
tion on improved functioning is important because the
amount of cognitive remediation may not be related to
primary rationale for cognitive remediation in schizo-
immediate gains in cognitive functioning but could con-
phrenia is to improve psychosocial functioning (35).
tribute to the retention of improvements following the ter-
mination of treatment. The impact of amount of cognitive In contrast to the uniform effects across studies of cog-
nitive remediation on overall cognitive performance and
remediation on the maintenance of treatment effects
symptoms, there was significant variability in its effects on
could not be evaluated in this meta-analysis because only
psychosocial functioning. Furthermore, as hypothesized,
six studies conducted follow-up assessments an average
cognitive remediation programs that provided adjunctive
of 8 months after completion of the program. However,
psychiatric rehabilitation had significantly stronger ef-
the mean effect size for overall cognitive performance for
fects on improving functional outcomes (0.47) than pro-
these studies was in the medium range (0.66), comparable
grams that did not (0.05). This effect is consistent with pre-
in magnitude to the immediate effects of cognitive reme-
vious research showing that cognitive impairment
diation. These findings provide preliminary support for
attenuates response to psychiatric rehabilitation (1, 36,
the longer-term benefits of cognitive remediation on cog-
37) and suggests that improved cognitive performance
nitive performance and point to the need for more re-
may enable some patients to benefit more from rehabilita-
search on the maintenance of treatment effects. tion. The findings are also consistent with the results of a
The overall effect size of cognitive remediation on im- meta-analysis of integrated psychological therapy (38) in
proving symptoms was significant but in the small range which the strongest effects on functioning were found in
(0.28). Previous reviews of the effects of cognitive remedi- programs that integrated cognitive remediation and social
ation either have not examined symptoms (10, 11) or were skills training rather than programs that provided either
inconclusive because of the small number of studies (12, intervention alone (39).
13). The apparently limited impact of cognitive remedia- Cognitive remediation programs that included strategy
tion on symptoms is consistent with numerous studies coaching had stronger effects on functioning than pro-
showing that cognitive impairment is relatively indepen- grams that focused only on drill and practice. Strategy
dent of other symptoms of schizophrenia (31–33). Cogni- coaching typically targets memory and executive func-
tive remediation may have some beneficial effects on tions by teaching methods such as chunking information
symptoms by providing positive learning experiences that to facilitate recall and problem-solving skills. It is unclear
serve to bolster self-esteem and self-efficacy for achieving whether strategy coaching is more effective because peo-
personal goals, thereby improving depression. Several ple are better able to transfer skills from the training set-
studies have reported that cognitive remediation im- ting into their daily lives (35) or because teaching such
proved mood (24, 27, 34). strategies helps patients compensate for the effects of per-

Am J Psychiatry 164:12, December 2007 ajp.psychiatryonline.org 1799


COGNITIVE REMEDIATION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA

TABLE 3. Results of Meta-Analysis of Randomized, Controlled Trials of Cognitive Remediation in Schizophreniaa


Cognitive
Subjects Remediation Hours Analysis
Outcome Domain Effect Size 95% CI T Score (N) Median Range Hedges’s Q df
Global cognition 0.41 0.29 to 0.52 6.9*** 1,214 12.5 3–75 35.3 28
Attention/vigilance 0.41 0.25 to 0.57 5.1*** 659 6.6 0–14 9.8 14
Speed of processing 0.48 0.28 to 0.69 5.9*** 655 0.0 0–3 20.7 13
Verbal working memory 0.52 0.33 to 0.72 5.2*** 428 0.4 0–8 3.9 10
Verbal learning and memory 0.39 0.20 to 0.58 5.5*** 858 0.0 0–8 26.6* 15
Visual learning and memory 0.09 –0.26 to 0.43 0.6 424 0.0 0–3 14.5* 7
Reasoning/problem solving 0.47 0.30 to 0.64 5.4*** 564 3.0 0–32 21.8 14
Social cognition 0.54 0.22 to 0.88 3.9*** 228 26.0 2–84 2.8 2
Symptoms 0.28 0.13 to 0.43 3.6*** 709 12.2 14
Functioning 0.35 0.07 to 0.62 1.9* 615 25.7** 10
aAfter consideration of the consistency of effect sizes across six of the seven cognitive domains and overall cognitive functioning, the clinical
and theoretical significance of these moderator effects on verbal learning and memory is unclear.
*p<0.05. ** p<0.01. ***p<0.001.

FIGURE 1. Effect Sizes for Overall Cognition in Random- sistent cognitive impairments on functioning (24) or both.
ized, Controlled Trials of Cognitive Remediation in Schizo- Further research is needed to address this question.
phrenia
The effects of cognitive remediation were not influ-
Benedict & Harris (46), treatment as usual enced by the nature of the control condition. Thus, simply
Benedict & Harris (46), active control actively or passively engaging patients in treatments de-
Wagner (45), treatment 1
signed to control for the amount of clinician contact did
not appear to confer any benefit in cognitive functioning
Meichenbaum & Cameron (16)
beyond the provision of usual services. These findings are
Penadés et al. (29)
consistent with the meta-analysis of the cognitive remedi-
Field et al. (49) ation-based integrated psychological therapy program
Sartory et al. (56) (39) but differ from the psychotherapy literature, where
Hogarty et al. (19) there is ample evidence for nonspecific effects related to
Vauth et al. (25), active control therapist attention (40). The mechanisms underlying the
Vauth et al. (25), treatment as usual
effects of cognitive remediation on improved cognitive
performance, functioning, and symptoms appear to differ
López-Luengo & Vázquez (55)
from those involved in psychotherapy. The results raise
Bellucci et al. (34)
questions about the need to control for the amount of cli-
Burda et al. (48) nician attention given to treatment control groups in re-
Bell et al. (26, 52); Fiszdon et al. (53, 54) search on cognitive remediation.
Olbrich & Mussgay (43) So what has been learned after almost 40 years of re-
Silverstein et al. (22) search on cognitive remediation for schizophrenia? Al-
Ueland & Rund (44) though a great deal more is known about schizophrenia
McGurk et al. (24) and its neurocognitive underpinnings and the technology
for assessing and remediating cognitive impairments has
Wagner (45), treatment 3
evolved (e.g., most programs now employ at least some
Benedict et al. (47)
computer-based training), the effect sizes on cognitive
van der Gaag et al. (23) functioning do not appear to have increased appreciably
Wykes et al. (27) in recent years. The failure to develop more potent pro-
Medalia et al. (50) grams could be due to limitations imposed by the illness
Hermanutz & Gestrich (42), active control itself and not the fault of treatment developers. It may be
Spaulding et al. (20) argued that a similar phenomenon has occurred in the
pharmacological treatment of schizophrenia, where de-
Wagner (45), treatment 2
spite the enormous investment of resources into the de-
Hermanutz & Gestrich (42),
treatment as usual velopment of new drugs, the clinical gains in treating
Medalia et al. (51), treatment 2 symptoms over the past 50 years are debatable (41).
Medalia et al. (51), treatment 1 Alternatively, the ability to improve the effectiveness of
cognitive remediation may depend on attention to critical
–2.0 –1.0 0.0 1.0 2.0 3.0
issues in research design. Two such issues deserve special
Effect Size
consideration: the evaluation of the persistence of reme-

1800 ajp.psychiatryonline.org Am J Psychiatry 164:12, December 2007


McGURK, TWAMLEY, SITZER, ET AL.

diation effects on cognitive functioning and the assess- Building, 105 Pleasant St., Concord, NH 03301; susan.r.mcgurk@
dartmouth.edu (e-mail).
ment of the impact of remediation on functional out-
All authors report no competing interests.
comes. Despite the number of controlled studies of Supported by NIMH grant MH77210 and National Institute on Dis-
cognitive remediation, only six studies (16, 19, 21, 28–30) ability and Rehabilitation Research grant H133G050230.

examined whether improvements in cognitive function-


ing were maintained at a posttreatment follow-up, pre-
References
cluding the exploration of moderators of treatment ef-
fects. The relative lack of data addressing this question 1. McGurk SR, Mueser KT: Cognitive functioning, symptoms, and
work in supported employment: a review and heuristic model.
may be important because different program, patient, or
Schizophr Res 2004; 70:147–174
setting factors could influence the long-term mainte- 2. Mueser KT: Cognitive functioning, social adjustment and long-
nance of cognitive effects compared to short-term effects. term outcome in schizophrenia, in Cognition in Schizophrenia:
Similarly, only 11 studies evaluated functional out- Impairments, Importance, and Treatment Strategies. Edited by
Sharma T, Harvey P. Oxford, UK, Oxford University Press, 2000,
comes (16, 19, 20, 22, 24, 25, 27, 29, 42–44), and this was
pp 157–177
the first meta-analysis to quantitatively demonstrate that 3. Corrigan PW, Mueser KT, Bond GR, Drake RE, Solomon P: The
cognitive remediation improved psychosocial function- Principles and Practice of Psychiatric Rehabilitation: An Empir-
ing. Furthermore, the impact of cognitive remediation on ical Approach. New York, Guilford, 2007
4. Smith TE, Hull JW, Romanelli S, Fertuck E, Weiss KA: Symptoms
functioning was moderated by several factors, including
and neurocognition as rate limiters in skills training for psy-
the provision of adjunctive psychiatric rehabilitation, cog- chotic patients. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:1817–1818
nitive training method, and patient age, suggesting poten- 5. Wykes T, Dunn G: Cognitive deficit and the prediction of reha-
tially important factors for improving the impact of treat- bilitation success in a chronic psychiatric group. Psychol Med
ment programs. Thus, the ability to make cognitive 1992; 22:389–398
6. Nuechterlein KH, Barch DM, Gold JM, Goldberg TE, Green MF,
remediation programs more effective may have been con-
Heaton RK: Identification of separable cognitive factors in
strained by the neglect of most studies to measure the schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2004; 72:29–39
long-term effects of remediation and its impact on func- 7. Marder SR: Initiatives to promote the discovery of drugs to im-
tional outcomes, resulting in the inability to identify mod- prove cognitive function in severe mental illness. J Clin Psychi-
erators of treatment that could be the focus of efforts to atry 2006; 67(suppl 9):31–35
8. Rund BR, Borg NE: Cognitive deficits and cognitive training in
hone and refine the intervention. Future research on cog-
schizophrenic patients: a review. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1999;
nitive remediation should routinely evaluate psychosocial 100:85–95
functioning and the long-term effects of treatment on all 9. Harvey PD, Green MF, Keefe RS, Velligan DI: Cognitive function-
outcomes of interest. In addition, research that systemati- ing in schizophrenia: a consensus statement on its role in the
cally examines the interactions between cognitive remedi- definition and evaluation of effective treatments for the ill-
ness. J Clin Psychiatry 2004; 65:361–372
ation and psychiatric rehabilitation is warranted. 10. Krabbendam L, Aleman A: Cognitive rehabilitation in schizo-
In summary, cognitive remediation was found to have phrenia: a quantitative analysis of controlled studies. Psycho-
consistent effects on improving cognitive performance, pharmacology 2003; 169:376–382
11. Kurtz MM, Moberg PJ, Gur RC, Gur RE: Approaches to cognitive
functioning, and symptoms. In addition, the impact of
remediation of neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenia: a
cognitive remediation on functional outcomes was signif- review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychology Rev 2001; 11:
icantly greater in studies that also provided psychiatric re- 197–210
habilitation, suggesting that these two treatment ap- 12. Pilling S, Bebbington P, Kuipers E, Garety P, Geddes JR, Martin-
proaches may work together in a synergistic fashion. dale B, Orbach G, Morgan C: Psychological treatments in
schizophrenia, II: meta-analyses of randomized controlled tri-
These findings challenge the assumption that simply im-
als of social skills training and cognitive remediation. Psychol
proving cognitive functioning in schizophrenia will spon- Med 2002; 32:783–791
taneously lead to better psychosocial outcomes. The re- 13. Twamley EW, Jeste DV, Bellack AS: A review of cognitive train-
sults do suggest, however, that cognitive remediation may ing in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 2003; 29:359–382
improve the response of some patients to psychiatric re- 14. Thalheimer W, Cook S: How to calculate effect sizes from pub-
lished research: a simplified methodology. www.work-learn-
habilitation. Overall, this meta-analysis indicates that cog-
ing.com/effect_sizes.htm
nitive remediation may have an important role to play in 15. Cohen J: Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences.
improving both cognitive performance and functional Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1988
outcomes in schizophrenia. 16. Meichenbaum D, Cameron R: Training schizophrenics to talk to
themselves: a means of developing attentional controls. Behav
Ther 1973; 4:515–534
Received June 8, 2007; revision received Aug. 27, 2007; accepted
17. Borenstein M, Rothstein H: Comprehensive Meta-Analysis: A
Aug. 28, 2007 (doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07060906). From the
Computer Program for Research Synthesis. Englewood, NJ, Bio-
Dartmouth Psychiatric Research Center, the Department of Commu-
stat, 1999
nity and Family Medicine, and the Department of Psychiatry, Dart-
mouth Medical School; the Department of Psychiatry, University of 18. Hedges LV, Olkin I: Statistical Methods for Meta-Analysis. New
California, San Diego; and the School of Psychology, Argosy Univer- York, Academic Press, 1985
sity, Santa Monica, Calif. Address correspondence and reprint 19. Hogarty GE, Flesher S, Ulrich RF, Carter M, Greenwald D, Pogue-
requests to Dr. McGurk, Dartmouth Psychiatric Research Center, Main Geile MF, Kechavan M, Cooley S, Di Barry AL, Garrett A, Pare-

Am J Psychiatry 164:12, December 2007 ajp.psychiatryonline.org 1801


COGNITIVE REMEDIATION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA

pally H, Zoretich R: Cognitive enhancement therapy for schizo- 38. Brenner H, Roder V, Hodel B, Kienzle N, Reed D, Liberman R:
phrenia: effects of a 2-year randomized trial on cognition and Integrated Psychological Therapy for Schizophrenic Patients.
behavior. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004; 61:866–876 Seattle, Hogrefe & Huber, 1994
20. Spaulding WD, Reed D, Sullivan M, Richardson C, Weiler M: Ef- 39. Roder V, Mueller DR, Mueser KT, Brenner HD: Integrated psy-
fects of cognitive treatment in psychiatric rehabilitation. chological therapy (IPT) for schizophrenia: is it effective?
Schizophr Bull 1999; 25:657–676 Schizophr Bull 2006; 32(suppl 1):S81–S93
21. Ueland T, Rund BR: Cognitive remediation for adolescents with 40. Crits-Christoph P, Baranackie K, Kurcias JS, Beck AT, Carroll K,
early onset psychosis: a 1-year follow-up study. Acta Psychiatr Perry K, Luborsky L, McLellan AT, Woody GE, Thompson L, Gal-
Scand 2005; 111:193–201 lagher D, Zitrin C: Meta-analysis of therapist effects in psycho-
22. Silverstein SM, Hatashita-Wong M, Solak BA, Uhlhaas P, Landa therapy outcome studies. Psychotherapy Res 1991; 1:81–91
Y, Wilkniss SM, Goicochea C, Carpiniello K, Schenkel LS, Savitz A, 41. Lieberman JA, Stroup TS, McEvoy JP, Swartz MS, Rosenheck RA,
Smith TE: Effectiveness of a two-phase cognitive rehabilitation Perkins DO, Keefe RS, Davis SM, Davis CE, Lebowitz BD, Severe
intervention for severely impaired schizophrenia patients. Psy- J, Hsiao JK: Clinical Antipsychotic Trials of Intervention Effec-
chol Med 2005; 35:829–837 tiveness (CATIE) investigators: effectiveness of antipsychotic
23. van der Gaag M, Kern RS, van den Bosch RJ, Liberman RP: A drugs in patients with chronic schizophrenia. N Engl J Med
controlled trial of cognitive remediation in schizophrenia. 2005; 353:1209–1223
Schizophr Bull 2002; 28:167–176 42. Hermanutz M, Gestrich J: Computer-assisted attention training
24. McGurk SR, Mueser KT, Pascaris A: Cognitive training and sup- in schizophrenics: a comparative study. Eur Arch Psychiatry
ported employment for persons with severe mental illness: Clin Neurosci 1991; 240:282–287
one year results from a randomized controlled trial. Schizophr 43. Olbrich R, Mussgay L: Reduction of schizophrenic deficits by
Bull 2005; 31:898–909 cognitive training: an evaluation study. Eur Arch Psychiatry
25. Vauth R, Corrigan PW, Clauss M, Dietl M, Dreher-Rudolph M, Neurol Sci 1990; 239:366–369
Stieglitz R-D, Vater R: Cognitive strategies versus self-manage- 44. Ueland T, Rund BR: A controlled randomized treatment study:
ment skills as adjunct to vocational rehabilitation. Schizophr the effects of a cognitive remediation program on adolescents
Bull 2005; 31:55–66 with early onset psychosis. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2004; 109:70–
26. Bell MD, Bryson G, Greig T, Corcoran C, Wexler RE: Neurocogni- 74
tive enhancement therapy with work therapy. Arch Gen Psychi- 45. Wagner BR: The training of attending and abstracting re-
atry 2001; 58:763–768 sponses in chronic schizophrenics. J Exper Res Personality
27. Wykes T, Reeder C, Corner J, Williams C, Everitt B: The effects of 1968; 3:77–88
neurocognitive remediation on executive processing in pa- 46. Benedict RHB, Harris AE: Remediation of attention deficits in
tients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 1999; 25:291–307 chronic schizophrenic patients: a preliminary study. Br J Clin
28. Fiszdon JM, Whelahan H, Bryson GJ, Wexler BE, Bell MD: Cogni- Psychol 1989; 28:187–188
tive training of verbal memory using a dichotic listening para- 47. Benedict RHB, Harris AE, Markow T, McCormick JA, Nuechter-
digm: impact on symptoms and cognition. Acta Psychiatr lein KH, Asarnow RF: Effects of attention training on informa-
Scand 2005; 112:187–193 tion processing in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 1994; 20:537–
29. Penadés R, Catalán R, Salamero M, Boget T, Puig O, Guarch J, 546
Gastó C: Cognitive remediation therapy for outpatients with 48. Burda PC, Starkey TW, Dominguez F, Vera V: Computer-assisted
chronic schizophrenia: a controlled and randomized study. cognitive rehabilitation of chronic psychiatric inpatients. Com-
Schizophr Res 2006; 87:323–331 puters Hum Behav 1994; 10:359–368
30. Wykes T, Reeder C, Williams C, Corner J, Rice C, Everitt B: Are 49. Field CD, Galletly C, Anderson D, Walker P: Computer-aided
the effects of cognitive remediation therapy (CTR) durable? re- cognitive rehabilitation: possible application to the attentional
sults from an exploratory trial in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res deficit of schizophrenia: a report of negative results. Percept
2003; 61:163–174 Mot Skills 1997; 85:995–1002
31. Harvey PD, Serper MR, White L, Parrella M, McGurk SR, Moriarty 50. Medalia A, Aluma M, Tryon W, Merriam AE: Effectiveness of at-
PJ, Bowie C, Vadhan N, Friedman J, Davis KL: The convergence tention training in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 1998; 24:
of neuropsychological testing and clinical ratings of cognitive 147–152
impairment in patients with schizophrenia. Compr Psychiatry 51. Medalia A, Dorn H, Watras-Gans S: Treating problem solving
2001; 42:306–313 deficits on an acute psychiatric inpatient unit. Psychiatry Res
32. Liddle PF: The symptoms of chronic schizophrenia: a re-exam- 2000; 97:79–88
ination of the positive-negative dichotomy. Br J Psychiatry 52. Bell M, Bryson G, Wexler BE: Cognitive remediation of working
1987; 151:145–151 memory deficits: durability of training effects in severely im-
33. Mueser KT, Curran PJ, McHugo GJ: Factor structure of the Brief paired and less severely impaired schizophrenia. Acta Psychi-
Psychiatric Rating Scale in schizophrenia. Psychol Assessment atr Scand 2003; 108:101–109
1997; 9:196–204 53. Fiszdon JM, Bryson GJ, Wexler BE, Bell MD: Durability of cogni-
34. Bellucci DM, Glaberman K, Haslam N: Computer-assisted cog- tive remediation training in schizophrenia: performance on
nitive rehabilitation reduces negative symptoms in the se- two memory tasks at 6-month and 12-month follow-up. Psy-
verely mentally ill. Schizophr Res 2002; 59:225–232 chiatry Res 2004; 125:1–7
35. Wykes T, Reeder C: Cognitive Remediation Therapy for Schizo- 54. Fiszdon JM, Cardenas AS, Bryson GJ, Bell MD: Predictors of re-
phrenia: Theory and Practice. London, Routledge, 2005 mediation success on a trained memory task. J Nerv Ment Dis
36. Mueser KT, Bellack AS, Douglas MS, Wade JH: Prediction of so- 2005; 193:602–608
cial skill acquisition in schizophrenic and major affective disor- 55. López-Luengo B, Vázquez C: Effects of attention process train-
der patients from memory and symptomatology. Psychiatry ing on cognitive functioning of schizophrenic patients. Psychi-
Res 1991; 37:281–296 atry Res 2003; 119:41–53
37. Wykes T, Sturt E, Katz R: The prediction of rehabilitative success 56. Sartory G, Zorn C, Groetzinger G, Windgassen K: Computerized
after three years—the use of social, symptom and cognitive cognitive rehabilitation improves verbal learning and process-
variables. Br J Psychiatry 1990; 157:865–870 ing speed in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2005; 75:219–223

1802 ajp.psychiatryonline.org Am J Psychiatry 164:12, December 2007

Você também pode gostar