Você está na página 1de 8

Alcohol & Alcoholism Vol. 43, No. 2, pp. 163–170, 2008 doi: 10.

1093/alcalc/agm160
Advance Access publication 31 January 2008

REVIEW
A REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE ON THE COGNITIVE EFFECTS OF ALCOHOL HANGOVER
RICHARD STEPHENS1,∗ , JONATHAN LING1 , THOMAS M. HEFFERNAN2 , NICK HEATHER2 and KATE JONES3
1 School of Psychology, Keele University, Staffordshire, United Kingdom, ST5 5BG, 2 Division of Psychology, Northumbria University, Newcastle-upon-Tyne,
United Kingdom, NE1 8ST and 3 Biological Monitoring Section, Health and Safety Laboratory, Buxton, United Kingdom, SK17 9JN

(Received 20 July 2007; first review notified 23 August 2007; in revised form 2 October 2007; accepted 4 October 2007;
advance access publication 31 January 2008)

Abstract —Aims: Alcohol misuse is a prime social and health problem in the UK. This paper presents a critical review of literature on
the performance effects in the morning after binge drinking – during the alcohol hangover. Several pathophysiological changes that both
follow and outlast acute intoxication may give rise to alcohol hangover effects. We have identified 27 English language peer-reviewed
studies that investigate aspects of psychological performance during alcohol hangover following controlled alcohol ingestion. However,
the majority of studies had basic methodological shortcomings. Of eight laboratory studies rigorous enough to warrant serious attention,
only two showed effects. We interpret these largely negative findings as evidence of an insensitivity that is intrinsic to laboratory-

Downloaded from http://alcalc.oxfordjournals.org by Marisa Levy on March 28, 2010


based studies of performance under the influence of alcohol. Several studies have investigated the cognitive consequences of hangover
subsequent to naturalistic consumption, where participants have chosen what and where to drink. Although these studies have tended to
show effects, participants were always informed at the outset that hangover effects were to be assessed, and participants knew which was
the hangover condition. Under these circumstances expectancy effects have possibly contaminated the results significantly. Therefore,
naturalistic alcohol consumption studies (and laboratory studies that did not employ a placebo) can be considered as being suggestive of
hangover effects, but should not be interpreted as providing definitive evidence of such effects. In conclusion, although there is empirical
evidence showing impaired performance as a result of the alcohol hangover, future studies should confirm these findings and overcome
the shortcomings of previous research.

INTRODUCTION (£2 billion) that were made 20 years ago and without refer-
ence to the original author’s caveat concerning the crude na-
Alcohol misuse appears to be rising to the extent that the UK ture of the costings (Crofton, 1987). A recent report by the
Government considers tackling problem drinking as a social Prime Minister’s Strategy Unit (2004) estimates that the cost to
and health priority (Prime Minister’s Strategy Unit, 2004). The the UK economy of alcohol-related absenteeism from work
alcohol-related death rate in the UK has increased from 6.9 (due in part to hangover) is between £1.2 and £1.8 billion per
per 100,000 population in 1991 to 12.9 in 2005 (Office for year. However, this estimate does not take into account effects
National Statistics, 2006). While the dangers of binge drink- of depleted worker performance and includes lost days due to
ing have been highlighted (e.g. Pincock, 2003), 35% of all long-term health problems associated with alcohol dependency.
men (42% of those aged 16–24 years) and 20% of all women In the grey literature, the BBC (BBC, 2004) cites research car-
(36% of those aged 16–24 years) still reported exceeding daily ried out by an employment agency estimating that hangovers
benchmarks on at least 1 day in the previous week to the 2005 cost the UK economy £2.8 billion a year due to the average
Great Britain General Household Survey (Goddard, 2006). A of 2.3 sick days per person per year, augmented by a further
recent government report estimated the societal costs of heavy 2.5 days per year that workers spend, on average, hungover on
drinking, including costs to the NHS in treating alcohol-related the job. Wiese et al. (2000) cited an estimate of hangover costs
injuries and illnesses (estimated at £1.7 billion per year), costs to the US economy of $148 billion a year, but this estimate was
associated with alcohol-related crime and disorder (estimated criticized by Becker (2001). A fair conclusion would be that
at £7.3 billion per year), and costs due to lost productivity hangover costs are indeterminate but significant.
through illness and absence from work (estimated at up to £6.4 Several reviews of the hangover state have been undertaken.
billion per year; Prime Minister’s Strategy Unit, 2004). The Finnigan and Hammersley (1992) report findings from a small
latter estimate includes the costs to the economy of alcohol re- number of hangover and performance studies, although this
lated deaths. A further, less visible societal cost arises because review has now become dated. In a brief review of hangover
of alcohol-related impairment in the morning after an evening’s appearing as an editorial piece, Calder (1997) recounted the
binge drinking–due to the so-called alcohol hangover. findings of several performance studies but did not conduct
Estimating the societal costs of hangover is prone to inac- any critical evaluation. Swift and Davidson (1998) gave a de-
curacy when one considers that hangover effects may include tailed account of the mechanisms and mediators of hangover
lateness, accident risk, poorly performed work and disputes but did not discuss performance effects. Wiese et al. (2000)
(Crofton, 1987) in addition to absenteeism. Even the recent conducted a detailed systematic review of the causes, patho-
report by Pittler et al. (2005) cites hangover cost estimates physiological characteristics and treatment of alcohol-induced
hangover. They defined hangover as ‘the presence of at least
two symptoms (out of: headache, poor sense of overall well-
being, diarrhoea, anorexia, tremulousness, fatigue, and nausea)
∗ Author to whom correspondence should be addressed at: School
occurring after the consumption and full metabolism of alco-
of Psychology, Keele University, Keele, Staffordshire, ST5 5BG UK.
Tel: (+44) (0)1782 583600; Fax: (+44) (0)1782 583387; e-mail:
hol with sufficient severity to disrupt the performance of daily
r.stephens@psy.keele.ac.uk tasks and responsibilities’ (p. 898). However, the notion that


C The Author 2008. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Medical Council on Alcohol. All rights reserved
164 R. STEPHENS et al.

hangover disrupts performance of certain tasks is not as safe 2001). On the other hand, increased cardiac output is associated
an assumption to make as it might at first appear. Certainly, with improved cognitive performance (e.g. Tomporowski and
Wiese et al. (2000) do not make a convincing case. They re- Ellis, 1986). The wide range of mechanisms and the lack of
viewed several studies claiming to show performance effects a unitary direction of effect make predicting the effects of
of hangover, but they recounted findings without any critical hangover complex. Fisk and Scerbo (1987) found that, during
evaluation, and they omitted a large number of published peer- acute alcohol intoxication, controlled processes were affected
reviewed hangover performance studies. more than automatic processes, implicating an effect on exec-
The present review offers a more critical analysis of all pub- utive function. Such an effect may persist into the hangover
lished peer-reviewed papers on hangover performance that we phase. Likewise, Jones and Harrison (2001) found executive
could find. For the purposes of this review, performance effects function decrements following modest amounts of experimen-
are defined primarily as changes in cognitive functioning as- tally induced sleep loss, one of the mechanisms thought to
sessed using cognitive tests. However, more everyday aspects of underlie hangover. Therefore, executive function, or certainly
performance, such as driving or performance in a management- higher cognitive functions, should be considered as candidate
style decision-making game are also considered. We began by functions to be affected by hangover.
searching the PSYCInfo database but this missed a large num- Humans have been drinking alcohol, and presumably expe-
ber of relevant studies and produced many false positives (e.g. riencing hangovers, since the first mead was brewed from fer-
non-performance based studies). Therefore, while some papers mented honey around 8000 BC (Meyer and Quenzer, 2005).

Downloaded from http://alcalc.oxfordjournals.org by Marisa Levy on March 28, 2010


were identified from database searches, the majority were iden- Nevertheless, although a number of studies have been car-
tified from references cited in papers already obtained. Some ried out addressing hangover effects on cognition and perfor-
further papers were identified using citation searching of ob- mance, we argue in the following sections that their interpre-
tained papers (i.e. searching forwards for papers that cite the tation is severely limited due to a variety of methodological
papers we had obtained). This was done using the MIMAS Web considerations.
of Knowledge database.
We briefly outline the biological mechanisms that may un-
derlie hangover effects before embarking on a detailed review LABORATORY STUDIES
of the literature on the performance effects of alcohol hangover.
We set out to include all the peer-reviewed studies published We identified 27 English language peer-review studies that have
to date but it is possible that one or two of the less well-cited investigated some aspect of psychological performance during
studies will have been omitted. alcohol hangover following controlled alcohol ingestion. Typ-
ically, in a between-subjects design, alcohol is given to one
MECHANISMS group and placebo is given to another group. After the pas-
sage of sufficient time for the acute intoxication effects to wear
Several pathophysiological changes that both follow and outlast off (typically 11 hours), one or more cognitive/performance
acute alcohol intoxication (i.e. are present after all the acute al- tests are applied. Decrements in the alcohol group relative
cohol has been metabolized) may give rise to the alcohol hang- to the placebo group are interpreted as hangover effects. A
over. Wiese et al. (2000) note increased levels of acetaldehyde, similar procedure may be applied in a related design where
hormonal alterations due to deregulated cytokine pathways an analogous comparison is made but the same participants
and the inhibition of the availability of glucose via a process are tested twice, once following alcohol and once following
mediated by insulin. Calder (1997) lists additional phenom- placebo. However, close reading reveals that these relatively
ena associated with dehydration, metabolic acidosis, disturbed straightforward designs have not been well implemented in
prostaglandin synthesis, increased cardiac output and vasodila- practice in this literature, with the majority of studies having
tion. Other potential mechanisms include sleep deprivation and basic methodological shortcomings.
insufficient eating (Verster et al., 2003). Calder (1997) sug- Several early studies either present no data at all or do
gests that the complex organic molecules found in alcoholic not present inferential statistical analyses of the data, and
beverages known as congeners may have an important role in it is not possible to estimate statistics, such as effect sizes,
producing hangover effects because some, such as methanol, based on the information presented (Carroll et al., 1964;
are metabolized to the notably toxic substances formaldehyde Ekman et al., 1964; Ideström and Cadenius, 1968; Dowd
and formic acid. Congeners tend to be present in greater con- et al., 1973). Future studies clearly were able to learn from
centrations in darker drinks (e.g. whisky) compared with clear these pioneering efforts. Nevertheless, without data or a rig-
drinks (e.g. vodka). orous data analysis it is not possible to draw meaningful con-
These mechanisms have in common the prediction of the clusions from these studies. In an even earlier ground-breaking
presence of hangover effects when the blood alcohol level study Takala et al. (1958) compared hangover performance in
(BAL) has returned to zero, after having become elevated medical students with non-hangover performance in a group of
during and following the drinking episode. Indeed, a defin- psychology and technical students. Unfortunately, in compar-
ing characteristic of alcohol hangover effects is their presence ing intact groups of participants it is not possible to distinguish
at zero BAL. This is necessary for research purposes in order hangover effects from pre-existing participant differences.
to distinguish between hangover and acute alcohol intoxica- Some other studies used a no-alcohol control condition rather
tion. However, there appears to be inconsistency in the way than a placebo (Yesavage and Leirer, 1986; Taylor et al., 1996;
that some of these physiological changes would be predicted to Kruisselbrink et al., 2006). In the laboratory, placebo controlled
affect cognitive functioning. For example, sleep deprivation is trials have become the gold standard for research into effects
known to impair executive functioning (e.g. Jones and Harrison, of imbibed substances such as alcohol. Results from laboratory
ALCOHOL HANGOVER EFFECTS ON COGNITION 165

studies that do not implement a placebo control have little delayed recall of items from a 15-word list the morning af-
credibility. Without a placebo, participants would have known ter 1.4 g/kg of alcohol consumption (mean recall 9.4 items,
when they were consuming alcohol and so any effects shown SD 3.4) compared with placebo (mean recall 11.5 items, SD
could be expectancy effects, rather than genuine effects arising 3.5). Four of the eight rigorous laboratory studies assessed di-
from the experimental treatment. In the case of alcohol, which vided attention (or dual task performance which amounts to
is renowned amongst the general public for producing perfor- the same thing) but only Roehrs et al. (1991) showed a signif-
mance decrements, participants may unwittingly have put in icant hangover decrement, with null effects in the three other
less effort when completing the performance tests following studies (Lemon et al., 1993; Chait and Perry, 1994; Finnigan
alcohol ingestion. Morrow et al. (1990) did employ a placebo et al., 1998). This low ‘hit rate’ of effects across studies ques-
but they used a repeated measures design without properly tions the reliability of the finding by Roehrs et al. and this
controlling for condition order effects. Participants becoming reliability is further questioned by the small sample size those
fatigued during the experiment could explain their findings. investigators employed. Therefore, there are only limited data
Another common problem is not verifying that BALs have to support the hypothesis that hangover produces decrements
returned to zero at the time the performance testing is carried in divided attention. Only one of the eight rigorous labora-
out. As already mentioned, a defining characteristic of alcohol tory studies showed a significant hangover decrement in de-
hangover effects is their presence at zero BAL. Ensuring BAL layed recall, although this was the sole study to have assessed
genuinely is zero (or so low as to be at the limits of detection) is long-term memory (Verster et al., 2003). At this juncture it

Downloaded from http://alcalc.oxfordjournals.org by Marisa Levy on March 28, 2010


necessary for research purposes in order to distinguish between is worth considering the extent to which long-term memory
hangover and acute alcohol intoxication. Lemon et al., (1993) decrements have been shown in the less rigorous studies. Takala
purport to assess hangover effects without verifying that BAL et al. (1958) noted a poorer rate of improvement due to prac-
is zero. In addition several supposed hangover studies report tice (later trial performance was compared with earlier trial
carrying out performance testing at elevated BALs. Such stud- performance) on a visual search task after 1.3 g/kg beer, al-
ies are likely to be picking up acute alcohol intoxication effects though, as already stated, this result was based on an intact
and cannot be interpreted as showing genuine hangover effects groups comparison and it is possible that the technical students
(Kelly et al., 1970; Seppela et al., 1976; Collins, 1980; Myrsten in the control group had generally superior visual search ability
et al., 1980; Kim et al., 2003). Several other studies that are relative to the medical students in the hangover group. Ekman
sometimes cited as assessing hangover effects were actually et al. (1964) assessed immediate and 7-minute delayed recog-
concerned with acute alcohol effects on the descending limb nition of letter pairs but neither presented nor analysed the test
of the blood-alcohol curve. As these studies conducted testing scores. A small effect is claimed but it is not clear whether
at raised BALs, they do not elucidate alcohol hangover effects this is an acute intoxication effect. In a related design, Kim
(Ekman et al., 1963; Jones and Vega, 1972; Peeke et al., 1980; et al. (2003) showed long-term memory ‘decrements’ follow-
McCaul et al., 1991; Millar et al., 1999). ing 1.5 mg/kg alcohol consumption. However, again it is not
Seven laboratory hangover studies are sufficiently rigorous clear whether this is an acute intoxication effect.
to warrant serious attention (Collins et al., 1971; Collins and We find these mainly null findings surprising and strongly
Chiles, 1980;. Roehrs et al., 1991; Chait and Perry, 1994; suspect their explanation lies with study insensitivity rather
Streufert et al., 1995; Finnigan et al., 1998; Verster et al., than a genuine absence of hangover effects. There are three
2003). One further laboratory study that we have criticized for reasons for this. First, five of the above studies employed
not verifying zero BAL at testing will also be reviewed fur- crossover placebo designs, yet such designs have been argued
ther. Lemon et al., (1993) found no effects and so this study to be unsuitable for ingestion studies. A combination of pro-
is immune from the criticism that, in not verifying zero BAL, prioceptive changes due to alcohol ingestion and inferential
hangover results are contaminated by acute alcohol intoxica- reasoning on the part of participants allows some participants
tion effects. Key aspects of the design, procedure and results of to determine which is the placebo condition and diminishes
these eight studies are summarized in Tables 1 and 2. study sensitivity (Finnigan and Hammersley, 1992). Second,
Each study involved the administration of between 0.7 and all the above studies employ the pharmacological model of
1.6 g/kg of alcohol (median 1 g/kg, roughly equivalent to drug action, i.e. the alcohol is administered as a single large
9 units, that is 3 pints of typical 5% ABV lager, or one bot- dose under as close to possible double-blinded conditions.
tle of 13% ABV wine), as pure ethanol or vodka mixed with However, this model may not be applicable to study the so-
orange juice, tonic, lime or lemonade. The placebo was usually cial phenomenon of drinking alcohol (Finnigan and Hammer-
orange juice or another mixer with a small quantity of alco- sley, 1992). For example, in real life people may control their
hol to provide an appropriate olfactory sensation. The majority drinking rate so that they can continue to function socially and
of studies employed all-male samples, the consumption-to-test will often take food with the alcohol. Third, it is possible that
interval ranged from 7.5–12 hours (median 10 hours), and ap- the alcohol doses employed in laboratory studies are insuffi-
propriate steps were taken to control condition order effects. cient for a hangover to occur; insufficient; due to research-
Each study verified that BAL was zero at testing (except Lemon ethics-imposed consumption limits. Nevertheless, five of the
et al., 1993, but see above). A range of cognitive and other per- identified eight rigorous laboratory studies assessed subjec-
formance tests were applied across the memory, attention, pro- tive hangover symptoms and in all cases hangover symptoms
cessing speed, executive function and psychomotor domains, were detected (Collins and Chiles, 1980; Roehrs et al., 1991;
although attention was tested in more studies than any other Streufert et al., 1995; Finnigan et al., 1998; Verster et al., 2003).
function. It is possible that the alcohol dose required to produce cogni-
Only two of the studies showed hangover effects. In a tive hangover effects is larger than that for somatic hangover
between-subjects design, Verster et al. (2003) showed poorer symptoms.
166

Table 1. Summary of design and procedural aspects of the eight rigorous laboratory-based hangover studies

Collins et al. Collins and Chiles Roehrs et al. Lemon et al. (1993) Chait and Perry Streufert et al. (1995) Finnigan et al. (1998) Verster et al. (2003)
(1971) (1980) (1991) (1994)

Country USA USA USA Australia USA USA Scotland Netherlands


Funder FAA Army and – NIH, NIAAA Federal Office of NIDA, USPHS NIDA AERC –
Navy Road Safety, Canberra
Design Independent Related Related Independent Related Related Related Independent
Participants 20 college 11 healthy pilots 5 healthy males 64 healthy males 14 healthy volunteers 21 healthy 40 male healthy heavy 48 healthy moderate
students professionals social drinkers, drinkers
non-smokers
Male:female ratio 20/0 7/4 5/0 64/0 10/4 21/0 40/0 24/24
Age Range 21–30 Mean 39.6 (SD not Range 21–34 Mean 24.6 (SD 6.6) Mean 24.5 (SD Under 46 (Mean not Mean 25.6 (SD Mean 21.9 (SD 17.1)
stated) 21–34) stated) 18–41)
Alcohol 1.6 (2 ml/kg) 1.3 0.8 0.5–1.0 (max 7–8 Males: 1.2 Females: 1.0 0.7 (100 mg/100 ml 1.4
consumed (g/kg) units) 1.0 blood)
Alcoholic drink Vodka and OJ Bourbon and 7-Up Vodka and tonic Ethanol and OJ Ethanol, tonic and Ethanol, tonic and Vodka, water and Ethanol and OJ
or vodka and 7-Up lime peppermint extract diabetic OJ
Placebo drink OJ with few drops Rum extract and Tonic with three 0.5 ml ethanol floated 1% ethanol, tonic and Tonic and peppermint Diabetic OJ, rim OJ and Grand Marnier
R. STEPHENS et al.

rum extract colouring drops of ethanol on OJ lime extract sprayed with swabbed with vodka essence consumed with
floated atomized 10% ethanol and lozenges nose clip
Restricted before – Coffee/ caffeine Caffeine – Eating, smoking, Drugs, alcohol Eating and drinking Eat, smoke, coffee,
morning test coffee anything but water excessive water
session
Consumption to 10 (max) 7.5 9 11 10 12 10 10
test interval (h)
BAL zero at test Yes (at 8 and 10 h Very low (under Yes Not stated Yes (but 2 participants Yes Yes Yes
post-drink) 0.007%) 10 mg/dl)

OJ = orange juice.

Downloaded from http://alcalc.oxfordjournals.org by Marisa Levy on March 28, 2010


ALCOHOL HANGOVER EFFECTS ON COGNITION 167
Table 2. Summary of cognitive domains and tests used in the eight rigorous laboratory-based hangover studies (underlined tests showed hangover decrements)

Collins et al. Collins and Chiles Roehrs et al. Lemon et al. Chait and Perry Streufert et al. Finnigan et al. Verster et al.
(1971) (1980) (1991) (1993) (1994) (1995) (1998) (2003)

Memory – – – – Word recall task – Probed- (short 15 word list


term) memory learning
recall task (immediate
recall; 60 min
delayed recall
and
recognition)
Attention – Choice RT, meter Divided Divided attention, Visual divided – Sustained Mackworth
monitoring, attention simple RT, attention, attention, dual clock test
pattern ID, Mackworth backwards task (tracking (vigilance)
clock test digit span and RT)
(vigilance)
Processing – Mental arithmetic – – Baddeley logical – – –
speed reasoning,
digit symbol
Executive – Problem solving – – Time production Management – –

Downloaded from http://alcalc.oxfordjournals.org by Marisa Levy on March 28, 2010


function simulation
(developing
country/floods)
Psycho- Tracking Tracking – – Standing – – –
motor steadiness

While some aspects of the design of laboratory studies on amount of alcohol consumed, each ensured BAL was zero
can easily be addressed to improve sensitivity, research ethics at testing and each used a related design with comparable n.
limitations on alcohol dose and the limitations of the pharmaco- Key aspects of the design, procedure and results of the five
logical model of drug action applied to alcohol ingestion studies naturalistic drinking hangover studies conducted to date are
have led some investigators to look beyond the laboratory. summarized in Tables 3 and 4.
In keeping with the laboratory studies reported earlier, natu-
ralistic consumption studies indicate that hangover affects as-
NATURALISTIC ALCOHOL CONSUMPTION pects of long-term memory and attention, as well as psychomo-
tor tasks. Although the quantity of alcohol reportedly consumed
Using an alternative approach, three studies have investigated in these naturalistic studies appears to be similar to that adminis-
the cognitive consequences of hangover subsequent to more tered in the laboratory studies, consumption data reported in the
naturalistic consumption, i.e. after participants have been al- naturalistic studies are averages. Therefore, some participants
lowed to drink what and sometimes where they choose. One would have drunk substantially more in the naturalistic studies
of these showed decrements on several tests of attention – than would ethically be allowed in the laboratory. While this un-
but, as zero BAL at testing was not confirmed, these could be doubtedly contributes to the sensitivity of naturalistic studies,
due to acute alcohol intoxication rather than hangover effects there is a substantial problem in the interpretation of these re-
(Anderson and Dawson, 1999). McKinney and Coyle (2004) sults. In these studies participants were always informed at the
used a related design with n = 48 and found impairments for outset that hangover effects were to be assessed and they knew
free recall of a word list, delayed recognition of words in the list, which was the hangover condition. Under these circumstances
and both simple and complex reaction times. Testing was car- expectancy effects are likely to have significantly contaminated
ried out at least 7 hours after reported consumption of an aver- the results. Therefore, while naturalistic alcohol consumption
age of 1.6 g/kg of alcohol and BAL was zero at testing for all ex- studies can be considered as being suggestive of hangover ef-
cept two participants whose readings were very low. Finnigan fects they should not, on their own, be interpreted as providing
et al. (2005) used a between-subjects design with n = 25 in definitive evidence of hangover effects.
the hangover group but showed no cognitive effects at zero
BAL the morning after reported consumption of an average of
1.7 g/kg of alcohol. We estimate the power of these studies to be
0.94 and 0.47, respectively, for the detection of medium-sized REVISITING LABORATORY STUDIES LACKING A
effects using two-tailed hypotheses (Cohen, 1988). Therefore PLACEBO CONTROL
the absence of effects in the latter study, in common with the
laboratory studies, is likely to be due to poor study sensitivity Earlier we argued that findings from laboratory studies that did
because of low statistical power. not employ a placebo control were likely to be biased as any
Two further naturalistic consumption studies have assessed effects found were likely to be contaminated by expectancy ef-
different aspects of driving ability during hangover. Laurell and fects, rather than genuine effects arising from the experimental
Törnros (1983) showed impaired cone avoidance while driving treatment. However, we went on to review naturalistic alcohol
a real car. Törnros and Laurell (1991) showed no difference in consumption studies on the grounds that these studies do not
the ability to go as fast as possible without losing control on employ the pharmacological model of drug action and so may
a driving simulator. These studies were otherwise comparable have greater sensitivity relative to laboratory studies employing
168 R. STEPHENS et al.
Table 3. Summary of design and procedural aspects of the five naturalistic alcohol consumption hangover studies

Laurell and Törnros (1983) Törnros and Laurell (1991) Anderson and McKinney and Coyle Finnigan et al. (2005)
Dawson (1999) (2004)

Country Sweden Sweden South Africa Northern Ireland Scotland


Funder None stated None stated None stated None stated AERC
Design Related Related Mixed Related Independent
Participants 22 healthy volunteers 24 healthy volunteers 16 hangover versus 48 students 25 hangover versus 33
(majority were students) 10 controls control versus 13 acute
(students) + hangover
(volunteers)
Male:female ratio 16/6 23/1 8/8 vs. 5/5 15/33 36/35
Age Range 19–38 Range 22–46 Mean 21.7 (SD 1.1) Mean 23.4 (SD 5.3) Mean 24.3 (SD ns)
Alcohol consumed 0.6 approx. (based on mean 0.7 approx. (based on 1.0 (minimum) 1.6 (mean) 1.75 (mean)
(g/kg) peak BAL 147 mg%) man peak BAL 176 mg%)
Alcoholic drink Beer, wine, spirits Beer, wine, spirits Not reported Not reported Not reported
Control condition Abstention ‘Non-alcoholic drinks’ Abstention Abstention Abstention
Restricted before None stated None stated None stated Caffeinated drinks None stated
morning test session
Consumption to test 8+ 14 (at 2 pm) 12–16 7+ Not stated

Downloaded from http://alcalc.oxfordjournals.org by Marisa Levy on March 28, 2010


interval (h)
BAL zero at test Yes Yes (at 2 pm) Not stated Yes (low readings of Yes
5 ml/100 ml for
two participants)

Table 4. Summary of cognitive domains and tests used in the five naturalistic alcohol consumption hangover studies (underlined tests showed hangover
decrements)

Laurell and Törnros and Laurell (1991) Anderson and Dawson (1999) McKinney and Coyle (2004) Finnigan et al. (2005)
Törnros (1983)

Memory – – – Word recall, delayed word Probe (short-term)


recognition memory recall
Attention – – Digit symbol modalities test, Reaction time (simple and Dual task tracking/ RT,
PASAT, Letter cancellation, 5-choice), selective Vigilance (repeating
star cancellation, symbol attention, divided attention, three-digit numbers)
cancellation spatial attention, sustained
attention,
Processing speed – – – 5-Choice reaction time –
Executive function – – – Stroop –
Psychomotor Car driving (cone Driving simulator – cover – – –
avoidance) 20 km as fast as possible

such models. Nonetheless, these naturalistic consumption stud- study employing a much smaller quantity of alcohol (0.6 g/kg)
ies also did not control expectancy effects. In the interests of did not show any effect – probably due to the low alcohol dose.
balance the findings of the non-placebo controlled laboratory However, it must be remembered that these studies did not
studies are considered in this section as further examples of sug- control expectancy effects pertinent to alcohol consumption,
gestive hangover effects, albeit effects which may be primarily and this is likely to have biased these participants’ performance
due to expectancy processes. to a sub-optimal level.
Yesavage and Leirer (1986) found evidence of poorer per-
formance piloting a flight simulator in 10 male navy pilots 14 DISCUSSION
hours following administration of approximately 1 g/kg of al-
cohol served as ethanol added to a soft drink. Blood alcohol In the Introduction we highlighted executive function as likely
was zero. Taylor et al. (1996) did not show any decrements on a to be susceptible to hangover effects, based on analogy with re-
flight simulator in 23 male and female pilots 8 hours following search into acute alcohol and sleep deprivation. However, only
ingestion of 0.6 g/kg of alcohol served as ethanol and diet soda. one study has assessed executive functioning during hangover
Individual blood alcohol levels were not reported, but the mean and no effect was observed. This may be due to insensitivity as-
BAL had dropped to zero 1 hour prior to testing. Kruisselbrink sociated with the manner of assessment – using a management
et al. (2006) showed an increase in choice reaction time errors simulation game rather than using well-validated neuropsycho-
in 12 female students 8.5 hours following consumption of ap- logical testing. Future research should employ well-validated
proximately 1.2 g/kg of alcohol served as beer, compared with executive function tests.
after abstaining. Blood alcohol was zero. Encouragingly, there is some consistency in the kinds of
In summary, these studies have shown decrements in ability cognitive effect shown across the laboratory and naturalis-
to pilot a simulated aircraft and in attentional processing (choice tic alcohol consumption studies. Verster et al. (2003) and
reaction time) at alcohol doses ranging from 1 to 1.2 g/kg. The McKinney and Coyle (2004) showed hangover-related memory
ALCOHOL HANGOVER EFFECTS ON COGNITION 169

decrements. The latter study also shares the finding of attention hangover. In this respect, the hangover and performance litera-
decrements with the laboratory-based studies of Roehrs et al. ture resembles the acute alcohol intoxication and performance
(1991) and Kruisselbrink et al. (2006). A convergent finding literature, which has also yielded largely inconclusive data.
from differing methodologies is known as triangulation and is Future research must overcome the shortcomings of previous
considered to be a sign of validity in psychological research research identified in this review if a full understanding of the
(e.g. Howitt and Cramer, 2005). As more studies are carried performance effects of the alcohol hangover is to be gained.
out, there may be a continued trend of convergent findings, For naturalistic drinking studies, the main issue is control-
which could lead future reviewers to be able to reach more ling expectancy effects. A novel approach would be for future
definitive conclusions. Nevertheless, we have identified some studies to exploit the predictability with which social drinking
serious problems with the two methodological approaches used occurs, e.g. on Friday evenings. Such regularity could be used
to study hangover effects. to assess hangover effects in individuals believing that they are
Rigorous laboratory-based studies, where participants are taking part in research with other aims, by simply inviting them
blinded to alcohol consumption, have tended not to show ef- to attend for testing on mornings likely to follow an evening
fects of hangover on performance. This insensitivity may arise spent drinking, and on mornings likely to follow an evening
partly because the pharmacological model of drug action, where of abstinence. Not all participants would have hangovers but
a certain drug dose is predicted to affect aspects of behaviour, those that do would arrive in the laboratory in a frame of mind
may be of little relevance to normal drinking (Finnigan and similar to any hungover person arriving at work or college –

Downloaded from http://alcalc.oxfordjournals.org by Marisa Levy on March 28, 2010


Hammersley, 1992). This is because the typical laboratory- aiming to have a reasonable go under the circumstances. This
based controlled intake study ignores potentially important ev- scenario is potentially very useful for understanding hangover
eryday aspects of drinking that are usually set by the drinker – effects and could contribute a further strand to a convergent
for example, the number and types of drink consumed, the pace alcohol hangover and performance literature.
of consumption, whether food is also consumed, and the social
setting. On the other hand, naturalistic alcohol consumption
studies, which allow for all these factors, have tended to show REFERENCES
effects of hangover on performance. However, as participants
were unblinded in these studies, the significant results are likely Anderson, S., and Dawson, J. (1999) Neuropsychological correlates of
to be contaminated by expectancy effects. alcoholic hangover. South African Journal of Science 95, 145–147.
BBC (2004) Hangovers cost UK £2.8bn annually. Retrieved
Finnigan and Hammersley (1992) reviewed all the acute al- 14th September 2007 from http://news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/business/
cohol studies published from 1980 to 1991 and this remains 3588632.stm.
the most-up-to-date, comprehensive review published. They Becker, J. (2001) Letter to the editor. Annals of Internal Medicine 134,
concluded that the research is ‘ambitious rather than rigorous’ 533.
Calder, I. (1997) Editorial: Hangovers. British Medical Journal 314,
(p. 74) and ‘while ample evidence exists that alcohol is capable 2.
of impairing performance. . . it is premature to conclude that it Carroll, J. R., Ashe, W. F., and Roberts, B. E. (1964) Influence of
invariably does so’ (p. 74). The criticisms they raise with respect the after-effects of alcohol combined with hypoxia on psychomotor
to acute alcohol studies may also be applied to the hangover performance. Aerospace Medicine 35, 990–993.
studies reviewed here. In both literatures there are studies with Chait, L. D., and Perry, J. L. (1994) Acute and residual effects of
alcohol and marijuana, alone and in combination, on mood and
flaws in basic experimental design, such as very small sam- performance. Psychopharmacology 115, 340–349.
ple size and ineffective control of expectancy, for example by Cohen, J. (1988) Statistical power for the behavioural sciences. . New
not including a placebo. Additionally, in both literatures the Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
majority of publications describe laboratory-controlled alcohol Collins, W. (1980) Performance effects of alcohol intoxication and
hangover at ground level and simulated altitude. Aviation, Space,
consumption studies employing the pharmacological model of and Environmental Medicine 51, 327–335.
drug action. As outlined in the previous paragraph, this model Collins, W. E., and Chiles, W. D. (1980) Laboratory performance
may be of little relevance to normal drinking. Finnigan and during acute intoxication and hangover. Human Factors 22, 445–
Hammersley (1992) argued that: ‘Natural intoxication may lead 462.
to more impairment, less impairment or different impairments’ Collins, W. E., Schroeder, D. J., Gilson, R. D. et al. (1971) Effects of
alcohol ingestion on tracking performance during angular acceler-
(p. 78). This is likely to be true for hangover effects as well as ation. Journal of Applied Psychology 55, 559–563.
for acute intoxication. Crofton, J. (1987) Editorial: Extent and costs of alcohol problems in
employment: A review of British data. Alcohol and Alcoholism 22,
321–325.
Dowd, P. J., Wolfe, J. W., and Cramer, R. L. (1973) After effects
CONCLUSION of alcohol on the perception and control of pitch attitude during
centripetal acceleration. Aerospace Medicine 44, 928–930.
One could say that the literature on performance effects of Ekman, G., Frankenhaeuser, M., Goldberg, L. et al. (1963) Effects of
the alcohol hangover resembles a Catch 22. Each of the two alcohol intake on subjective and objective variables over a five-hour
methodological approaches employed in this literature has period. Psychopharmacologia 4, 28–38.
Ekman, G., Frankenhaeuser, M., Goldberg, L. et al. (1964) Subjective
its own interpretative problems. Controlled-intake laboratory- and objective effects of alcohol as functions of dosage and time.
based studies appear to lose a significant quantity of variability Psychopharmacologia 6, 399–409.
attributable to user-controlled aspects of social drinking. On the Finnigan, F., and Hammersley, R. (1992) The effects of alcohol on
other hand, data from naturalistic alcohol consumption studies performance. In: Handbook of Human Performance, Smith, A. P.,
and Jones, D. M., eds. London: Academic Press, pp. 73–126.
are likely to be contaminated by expectancy effects. Currently Finnigan, F., Hammersley, R., and Cooper, T. (1998) An examination
there is little definitive empirical evidence determining what, of next-day hangover effects after a 100 mg/100 ml dose of alcohol
if any, effects on performance arise as a result of the alcohol in heavy social drinkers. Addiction 93, 1829–1838.
170 R. STEPHENS et al.

Finnigan, F., Schulze, D., Smallwood, J. et al. (2005) The effects Myrsten, A. L., Rydberg, U., Ideström, C. M. et al. (1980) Alcohol
of self-administered alcohol-induced ‘hangover’ in a naturalistic intoxication and hangover: modification of hangover by chlorme-
setting on psychomotor and cognitive performance and subjective thiazole. Psychopharmacology 69, 117–125.
state. Addiction 100, 1680–1689. Office for National Statistics, General Register Office for Scotland,
Fisk, A. D., and Scerbo, M. W. (1987) Automatic and control process- Northern Ireland Statistics and Research Agency. (2006) Alcohol-
ing approach to interpreting vigilance performance: A review and related deaths. Rates rise since 1990s. Retrieved December 14th,
reevaluation. Human Factors 29, 653–660. 2006 from http://www.statistics.gov.uk/cci/nugget.asp?id=1091.
Goddard, E. (2006) Smoking and drinking among adults. London: Peeke, S. C., Callaway, E., Jones, R. T. et al. (1980) Combined effect
Office for National Statistics. of alcohol and sleep deprivation in normal young adults. Psycophar-
Howitt, D., and Cramer, D. (2005) Introduction to research methods macology 67, 279–287.
in psychology. Harlow: Pearson Education Limited. Pittler, M. H., Verster, J. C., and Ernst, E. (2005) Interventions
Ideström, C. M., and Cadenius, B. (1968) Time relations of the effects for preventing or treating alcohol hangover: systematic review of
of alcohol compared to placebo. Psychopharmacologia 13, 189– randomised controlled trials. British Medical Journal 331, 1515–
200. 1518.
Jones, K., and Harrison, Y. (2001) Frontal lobe function, sleep loss Prime Minister’s Strategy Unit (2004) Alcohol harm reduction strategy
and fragmented sleep. Sleep Medicine Reviews 5, 463–475. for England. London: Cabinet Office.
Jones, B. M., and Vega, A. (1972) Cognitive performance measured Roehrs, T., Yoon, J., and Roth,T. (1991) Nocturnal and next-day effects
on the ascending and descending limb of the blood alcohol curve. of ethanol and basal level of sleepiness. Human Psychopharmacol-
Psychopharmacologia 23, 99–114. ogy 6, 307–311.
Kelly, M., Myrsten, A., Neri, A. et al. (1970) Effects and after-effects Seppälä, T., Leino, T., Linnoila, M. et al. (1976) Effects of hang-

Downloaded from http://alcalc.oxfordjournals.org by Marisa Levy on March 28, 2010


of alcohol on physiological and psychological functions in man – a over on psychomotor skills related to driving: modification by fruc-
controlled study. Blutalkohol 7, 422–436. tose and glucose. Acta Pharmacologica et Toxicologica 38, 209–
Kim, D. J., Yoon, S. J., Lee, H. P. et al. (2003) The effects of alcohol 218.
hangover on cognitive functions in healthy subjects. International Streufert, S., Pogash, R., Braig, D. et al. (1995) Alcohol hangover and
Journal of Neuroscience 113, 581–594. managerial effectiveness. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental
Kruisselbrink, L. D., Martin, K. L., and Megeney, M. (2006) Physical Research 19, 1141–1146.
and psychomotor functioning of females the morning after con- Swift, R., and Davidson, D. (1998) Alcohol hangover: Mecha-
suming low to moderate quantities of beer. Journal of Studies on nisms and mediators. Alcohol Health & Research World 22,
Alcohol 67, 416–420. 54–60.
Laurell, H., and Törnros, J. (1983) Investigation of alcoholic hangover Takala, M., Siro, E., and Toivainen, M. A. (1958) Intellectual functions
effects on driving performance. Blutalkohol 20, 489–499. and dexterity during hangover. Quarterly Journal of Studies on
Lemon, J., Chester, G., Fox, A. et al. (1993) Investigation of the Alcohol 19, 1–29.
“hangover” effects of an acute dose of alcohol on psychomotor Taylor, J. L., Dolhert, N., Friedman, L. et al. (1996) Alcohol elimi-
performance. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 17, nation and simulator performance of male and female aviators: a
665–668. preliminary report. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine
McCaul, M. E., Turkkan, J. S., Svikis, D. S. et al. (1991) Alcohol and 67, 407–413.
secobarbital effects as a function of familial alcoholism: Extended Tomporowski, P. D., and Ellis, N. R. (1986) Effects of exercise on
intoxication and increased withdrawal effects. Alcoholism: Clinical cognitive processes: A review. Psychological Bulletin 99, 338–346.
and Experimental Research 15, 94–101. Törnros, J., and Laurell, H. (1991) Acute and hang-over effects
McKinney, A., and Coyle, K. (2004) Next day effects of a normal of alcohol on simulated driving performance. Blutalkohol 28,
night’s drinking on memory and psychomotor performance. Alcohol 24–30.
and Alcoholism 39, 509–513. Verster, J. C., van Duin, D., Volkerts, E. R. et al. (2003) Alcohol
Meyer, J. S., and Quenzer, L. F. (2005) Psychopharmacology; drugs, hangover effects on memory functioning and vigilance performance
the brain, and behavior. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates. after an evening of binge drinking. Neuropsychopharmacology 28,
Millar, K., Finnigan, F., and Hammersley, R. H. (1999) Is residual im- 740–746.
pairment after alcohol an fffect of repeated performance? Aviation, Wiese, J. G., Shlipak, M. G., and Browner, W. S. (2000) The alcohol
Space, and Environmental Medicine 70, 124–130. hangover. Annals of Internal Medicine 132, 897–902.
Morrow, D., Leirer, V., and Yesavage, A. (1990) The influence of Yesavage, A., and Leirer, V. O. (1986) Hangover effects of aircraft
alcohol and aging on radio communication during flight. Aviation, pilots 14 h after alcohol ingestion: A preliminary report. American
Space, and Environmental Medicine 61, 12–20. Journal of Psychiatry 143, 1546–1550.

Você também pode gostar