Você está na página 1de 28

Chapter 4.

Principles of Clinical Practice for Assessment and Intervention 55

Principles of Clinical Practice for


Assessment and Intervention
Stanley I. Greenspan, M.D., and Serena Wieder, Ph.D.

The discussion in Chapter 2 states that focused on symptoms and groups of symp-
developmental and learning disorders involve toms comprising syndromes, giving only par-
varying degrees of impairment in critical tial attention to a broad range of important
central nervous system (CNS) functions, functional capacities and their related pro-
such as motor capacities, language, cogni- cessing differences. For example, a mother
tion, sensory, and social and emotional func- was told by the senior clinician overseeing an
tioning. This chapter continues the discussion evaluation team that because her child was
by describing the functional developmental “autistic,” he could not interact with purpose
approach and its relationship to general prin- and meaning. The clinician concluded that,
ciples of clinical practice. because an intensive intervention would not
be helpful, the therapeutic effort should be
THE FUNCTIONAL modest. Yet, this clinician did not assess the
DEVELOPMENTAL APPROACH relative strengths and weaknesses of all the
child’s areas of functioning and underlying
Most non-progressive developmental and processing capacities to see which ones
learning disorders, including disorders of required work and which ones could be har-
relating and communicating (e.g., autistic nessed immediately for interacting and learn-
spectrum or pervasive developmental disor- ing. For example, was this child’s lack of
ders [PDD], multisystem developmental dis- purposeful interaction related to auditory
orders, severe language and cognitive processing and motor problems? Did he have
deficits) are nonspecific with regard to their some relative strengths in visual-spatial pro-
etiology and pathophysiology. Non-progres- cessing that could be partially harnessed to
sive developmental disorders are, therefore, enable him to interact with others more pur-
best characterized by types and degrees of posefully? Would working with him on activ-
functional limitations and processing differ- ities that were associated with high
ences as well as by symptoms that often are motivation (e.g., his eagerness to go outside)
only one expression of a functional limitation help him become more purposeful? Or, was
(e.g., echolalia is a symptom; pragmatic lan- his lack of purposeful interaction part of a
guage is the functional limitation). biologically based CNS impairment which,
Yet, as indicated in Chapters 2 and 3, both by definition, would remain chronic and rel-
historically and recently, clinicians have atively untreatable?
56 ICDL Clinical Practice Guidelines

Cases such as this one suggest the impor- discussed by Rosenbloom, Chapter 30, this
tance of looking at functional capacities. In volume).
fact, there is mounting evidence that: Many practitioners, especially those in
1. Most complex, non-progressive develop- speech and language pathology, occupational
mental and learning disorders involve therapy, physical therapy, education, and psy-
processing dysfunctions, each with its chology have been expanding a dynamic
own natural history and complex, poorly functional approach that takes into account
understood etiology and neurobiology the dynamic relationship between behavior
(see Zimmerman & Gordon, Chapter 27, and the CNS. These practitioners are demon-
and Minshew & Goldstein, Chapter, 28, strating that interactions geared to individual
this volume). differences can facilitate important function-
2. Key functional capacities, as well as relat- al capacities such as relating, thinking, and
ed symptoms, learning problems, adaptive communicating.
capacities, and a variety of surface behav- For example, motor-planning (including
iors, in turn, are often the result of the oral-motor) exercises can help develop a
interaction between early and ongoing child’s preverbal vocal, motor, and affective
interactive experiences and these biologi- gesturing, imitation, and language develop-
cally based processing dysfunctions. ment. Visual-spatial, problem-solving approach-
3. The resulting functional capacities, es can aid logical thinking and abstract academic
symptoms, or behaviors are not tied to challenges. Sensory modulation work can help
these underlying processes in a rigid, a child learn to relate to others and with recep-
fixed manner. For example, a child who tive language, whereas auditory discrimina-
cannot use words to symbolize a wish for tion work can facilitate the child’s phonemic
juice may be able to learn to use a sign or awareness as a basis for reading.
a picture to symbolize her wish, suggest- In spite of an expanding functional approach,
ing many alternatives to symbolization. however, many clinicians continue to focus pre-
dominantly on symptoms and only a few of
Observed behavior, therefore, is often the the functional areas (e.g., more on verbal
result of dynamic interactions between the development and less on intimacy, relating,
environment and genetic, prenatal, perinatal, and preverbal affective gesturing). Similarly,
and maturational variations. For example, there is a tendency to focus only on a few of
genetic expression is influenced by a number the critical underlying processing differences.
of cellular and extracellular biological process- For example, some clinicians focus more on
es, including different hormones and toxic auditory processing and less on motor plan-
substances. Resultant physiologic levels are ning and sequencing, which is essential for
modified by different levels of interaction with imitation, social interaction, play, and lan-
the physical, cognitive, and social environment. guage. Others may focus more on rote cogni-
Resultant functional capacities and behaviors tive and social skills and less on spontaneous
are further influenced by interactions with dif- interaction to promote affective processing.)
ferent aspects of the environment. (These (See Tsakiris, Chapter 31, this volume).
processes, which can be systematically Consequently, it is important to fur-
described in terms of Biomedical traits, ther systematize and expand a functional
Original traits, Learned coping mechanisms, approach. In this approach, assessments and
and Derived behaviors [BOLD] are more fully interventions must include the contributions
Chapter 4. Principles of Clinical Practice for Assessment and Intervention 57

of all relevant areas of functioning, process- number of sessions with the child and family.
ing differences, and interactive relationships These sessions must begin with discussions
(see Chapter 2). Chart 1 summarizes the rel- and observations. Structured tests, if indicat-
evant areas. ed, should be administered later to further
understand specific functional areas. The ses-
THE FUNCTIONAL DEVELOPMENTAL
APPROACH TO ASSESSMENT sions should include:
• Review of current functioning with par-
Implementing an appropriate assessment ents and other caregivers, looking at both
of all the relevant functional areas requires a problems and adaptive capacities.

Chart 1. Relevant Areas to Consider in a Functional Developmental Approach

The functional developmental level of the child’s communicative, cognitive, and emo-
tional capacities (i.e., how the child integrates all his capacities to meet emotionally mean-
ingful goals, such as using a continuous flow of social problem-solving gestures to get
Daddy to reach for and get him a desired toy).
Includes:
1. Shared attention and regulation
2. Engagement
3. Purposeful preverbal/gestural and affective communication
4. Complex social problem-solving interactions
5. The creative and meaningful use of ideas
6. Building logical connections between ideas or symbols (Greenspan, 1992)

Individual differences in the functioning of the child’s motor, sensory, and affective
patterns
Includes:
1. Sensory modulation (e.g., the degree to which the child is over- or underreactive to sen-
sations in each sensory modality such as touch, movement, sound, sight)
2. Auditory and visual-spatial processing
3. Motor planning and sequencing and affective processing (i.e., the connection between
affect, motor planning, other processing capacities, and emerging symbols)

Relationship and affective interaction patterns, including developmentally appropri-


ate interactions (which utilize understanding of the child’s functional developmental
level and individual differences) or inappropriate interactions
Includes:
1. Existing caregiver, parent, family patterns
2. Educational patterns
3. Peer patterns
58 ICDL Clinical Practice Guidelines

• Review of prenatal, perinatal, and post- potential usefulness of Depakote or other


natal developmental history, including antiseizure medications). (See Robinson,
development in all the functional areas. Chapter 17, this volume).
• Observations of child-caregiver interac- • Additional developmental and learning
tions for 45 or more minutes, including assessments, such as speech and language
“coaching” or interactions with the clini- functioning, motor and sensory function-
cian to elicit the child’s highest adaptive ing, and specific aspects of cognitive func-
level. There should be at least two obser- tioning, should be conducted as needed (as
vation sessions, with additional sessions opposed to routinely) to answer specific
scheduled if there is a discrepancy between questions. For example, a child with a cir-
parental reports and observed functioning. cumscribed receptive language difficulty
These sessions should serve, together may benefit from structured language
with a history and review of current func- assessments to tease out more precisely the
tioning, as a partial basis for a preliminary nature of the deficit. A child with a circum-
impression of the child’s functional devel- scribed learning problem may benefit from
opmental capacities and individual pro- specific structured tasks to tease out areas
cessing differences, including those that of strength and weakness, such as visual-
are often underemphasized, such as motor spatial processing or motor planning (e.g., a
planning, visual-spatial processing and neuropsychological battery). Specific
affective processing, and current as well assessments, however, must always build on
as potentially optimal child-caregiver a basic evaluation because even circum-
interaction patterns. scribed processing difficulties often make it
• Discussion of caregiver personality pat- more difficult for a child to process and
terns and child-caregiver interactions, understand important functions of social
including identification of strengths and relationships. Therefore, the processing dif-
vulnerabilities as well as family and cul- ficulties may be associated with emotional,
tural patterns. behavioral, and/or family challenges.
• Review of all current interventions, edu-
cational programs, daily activities, In contrast, a child who is only fleetingly
behaviors, and interaction patterns. engaged or aimless and has receptive as well
There should be review and consideration as expressive language problems may be best
of developmentally appropriate interac- understood through ongoing interactive
tion patterns and practices at home, in observation and the child’s response to a
school, with peers, and in different inter- comprehensive intervention program. Such a
ventions in regard to their capacity to fos- child may not be able to fully cooperate in a
ter the next level in the child’s functional structured developmental assessment and
abilities. even the best evaluator may only be able to
• Review and assessment to rule out (or describe the child’s behavior in the assess-
identify) concurrent and/or contributing ment situation. In such circumstances, clinical
medical disorders. This includes con- observation of caregiver- and clinician-child
ducting appropriate biomedical assess- interactions to observe functional capacities
ments to identify biological interventions coupled with a review of current and past
that may contribute to clinical manage- functioning provide an initial picture of
ment (e.g., a 24-hour EEG may reveal the strengths and challenges. Further profiling
Chapter 4. Principles of Clinical Practice for Assessment and Intervention 59

may then best done as part of the intervention The Affect Diathesis Hypothesis
process. (See Gerber & Prizant, Chapter 5;
Long & Sippel, Chapter 9; Greenspan & Looking at children with autistic spectrum
Wieder, Chapter 12, this volume.) disorders within the context of their function-
al profiles highlights a number of important
THE FUNCTIONAL PROFILE findings. These findings provide clues regard-
ing a core psychological mechanism that may
The functional assessment leads to an indi- express the neurological differences character-
vidualized functional profile that captures izing autistic spectrum disorders. When chil-
each child’s unique developmental features dren with autism are compared to children
and serves as a basis for creating individually without autism, and level of intelligence as
tailored intervention programs. The profile measured with IQ tests is controlled for, there
describes the child’s functional developmental are a number of autism-specific functional
capacities, contributing biological processing deficits. These include deficits in the ability
differences, and environmental interactive pat- for empathy (theory of mind) (Baron-Cohen,
terns, including the different interaction pat- 1994); higher-level abstract thinking and skills
terns available to the child at home and at in making inferences (Minshew & Goldstein,
school, with peers, and in other settings. The 1998); and shared attention, including social
profile should include all areas of functioning, referencing and problem solving (Mundy,
not simply the ones that are more obviously Sigman, & Kasari, 1990). In addition, deficits
associated with pathologic symptoms. For in the capacities for affective reciprocity
example, a preschooler’s lack of ability to (Baranek, 1999; Dawson & Galpert, 1990;
symbolize a broad range of emotional interests Lewy & Dawson, 1992; Osterling & Dawson,
and themes in either pretend play or talk is just 1994; Tanguay, 1999; Tanguay, Robertson, &
as important, if not more important, than that Derrick, 1998), and functional (pragmatic)
same preschooler’s tendency to be persevera- language (Wetherby & Prizant, 1993) also
tive or self-stimulatory. In fact, clinicians have appear specific to autism.
often seen that as the child’s range of symbol- These functional deficits have a feature in
ic expression broadens, perseverative and self- common that may suggest an overriding or
stimulatory tendencies decrease. Similarly, a core psychological deficit. Clinical work
child’s areas of special strength may also be with infants and children without challenges
critical to the intervention program. For exam- and those with biological challenges and
ple, visual-spatial thinking capacities may environmental challenges demonstrates that
enable a child with severe language problems the capacities for empathy, psychological
to interact with others, learn, and reason. mindedness, abstract thinking, social prob-
Because the functional profile captures lem solving, functional language, and reci-
each child’s individual variations, children procity all stem from the infant’s ability to
with the same diagnosis (e.g., PDD) may connect affect or intent to motor-planning
have very different profiles, whereas children capacities and emerging symbols (Greenspan,
with different diagnoses may have similar 1979, 1989, 1997). Relative deficits in this
profiles. The functional profile is updated core capacity lead to problems in these high-
continually through clinical observations, er-level emotional and intellectual processes.
which serve as a basis for revising the child’s A child’s capacity to connect affect to
intervention program. motor planning and emerging symbols
60 ICDL Clinical Practice Guidelines

becomes relatively apparent between 9 and with autism from individuals without autism,
18 months of age as the infant shifts from as outlined earlier. For example, long chains of
simple patterns of engagement and reciproc- social reciprocity depend on affect guiding
ity to complex chains of affective reciprocity interactive social behavior. Shared attention,
that involve problem-solving interactions. which includes social referencing and shared
Consider a 14-month-old child who takes his problem solving, also depends on affect guid-
father by the hand and pulls him to the toy ing interactive social behavior. Empathy and
area, points to the shelf, and motions for a theory of mind capacities depend on the abili-
toy. After Dad picks him up, he nods, smiles, ty to understand both one’s own affects or feel-
and bubbles with pleasure. For this complex, ings and another person’s affects or feelings,
problem-solving social interaction to occur, and to project oneself into the other person’s
the infant needs to have a wish, desire, or mindset. This complex emotional and cognitive
intent (i.e., inner affects or emotions) that task begins with the ability to exchange affect
indicate what he wants. The infant then needs signals with another person and, through these
an action plan (i.e., a plan to get his toy) that exchanges, emotionally sense one’s own intent
may involve many steps. However, the direc- and sense of self in interaction with another.
tion-giving affects must connect to the action Similarly, higher-level abstract thinking skills,
plan in order for the child to create a pattern such as making inferences, depend on the abil-
of meaningful, social problem-solving inter- ity to generate new ideas from one’s own affec-
actions. Without this connection between tive experiences with the world and then reflect
affect and action plans (i.e., motor planning), on and categorize them (Greenspan, 1997).
complex interactive problem-solving patterns In observations of infants and toddlers
are not possible. Action plans without affec- heading into autistic patterns and in taking
tive direction or meaning will tend to become careful histories of older children with autism,
repetitive (perseverative), aimless, or self- we have noted that children with autistic spec-
stimulatory, which is what is observed when trum patterns did not fully make the transition
there is a deficit in this core capacity. from simple patterns of engagement and
As the ability to form symbols emerges, interaction into complex affect-mediated,
inner affects (intent) need to connect to sym- social problem-solving interactions. They, by
bols to create meaningful ideas, such as those and large, did not progress to a continuous
involved in functional language, imagination, flow of affective problem-solving inter-
and creative and logical thought. The mean- changes. Even affectionate children who were
ingful use of symbols usually emerges from repeating a few words or memorizing num-
meaningful (affect-mediated) problem-solv- bers and letters, and who went on to evidence
ing interactions that enable a toddler to under- autistic patterns, did not master, for the most
stand the patterns in her world and eventually part, this early capacity to engage in a con-
use symbols to convey these patterns in tinuous flow of affect-mediated, gestural
thought and dialogue. Without affective con- interactions. These children were then unable
nections, symbols such as action plans will to develop higher-level capacities for empa-
be used in a repetitive (perseverative) manner thy and creative and abstract thinking unless
(e.g., scripting, echolalia). an intervention was initiated that focused on
Affectively guided problem-solving inter- facilitating affect-mediated interactions.
actions and symbols are necessary for all the In a review of the functional profiles of
unique capacities that distinguish individuals 200 cases of children with autistic spectrum
Chapter 4. Principles of Clinical Practice for Assessment and Intervention 61

disorders, we observed that most of the chil- may be intensifying her difficulties. She is
dren shared this unique processing deficit. At likely to perplex, confuse, frustrate, and
the same time, the children differed with undermine purposeful, interactive communi-
regard to other processing deficits involving cation with even very competent parents.
their auditory, motor-planning, visual-spatial, This loss of engagement and intentional,
and sensory modulation abilities. Approx- interactive relatedness to key caregivers may
imately two-thirds of the children who devel- cause a child to withdraw more idiosyncrati-
oped autistic spectrum disorders had this cally into her own world and become even
unique type of biologically based processing more aimless and/or repetitive. What later
deficit that involved the connection of affect looks like a primary biological deficit may,
or intent to motor planning and sequencing therefore, be part of a dynamic process
capacities as well as to emerging symbolic through which the child’s lack of interactions
capacities (Greenspan and Wieder, 1997). has intensified specific, early, biologically
The hypothesis that explores this connec- based processing problems and derailed crit-
tion between affect and different processing ical social skills.
capacities is called the affect diathesis hypoth- Biologically based processing (regulatory)
esis. In this hypothesis, as indicated previous- difficulties, therefore, often contribute to, but
ly, a child uses his affect to provide intent are not decisive, in determining relationship
(i.e., direction) for his actions and meaning and communication difficulties. When problems
for his words. Typically, during the second are perceived early, appropriate professional
year of life, a child begins to use his affect to help can, to varying degrees, teach children
guide intentional problem-solving behavior and caregivers how to work with the process-
and, later on, meaningful use of language. ing (regulatory) dysfunctions, including
Through many affective problem-solving helping the toddler connect affect to emerg-
interactions, the child develops complex ing action plans and their associated relation-
social skills and higher-level emotional and ship and communication patterns. Such
intellectual capacities. children can often become capable of form-
Because this unique processing deficit ing warm relationships and climb the devel-
occurs early in life, it can undermine the tod- opmental ladder leading to language and
dler’s capacity to engage in expectable learn- thinking capacities.
ing interactions essential for many critical There are many children who do not evi-
emotional and cognitive skills. For example, dence autism but have developmental prob-
she may have more difficulty eliciting ordi- lems in which intentionality or purposeful
nary expectable interactions from her parents action is difficult in its own right and results
and the people in her immediate environ- in less practice or use (e.g., severe motor
ment. Without appropriate interaction, she problems). These children may either have
may not be able to comprehend the rules of difficulty forming or may secondarily lose
complex social interactions or to develop a their ability to connect intent or affect to
sense of self. A child normally develops these motor planning. In these circumstances, cre-
skills and capacities at an especially rapid ating purposeful interactions around any
rate between 12 and 24 months of age. By the motor skill (e.g., head or tongue movements)
time a child with processing difficulties may strengthen the affect-motor connection,
receives professional attention, her challeng- reduce aimless, repetitive behavior, and facil-
ing interaction patterns with her caregivers itate problem solving and thinking. Recent
62 ICDL Clinical Practice Guidelines

MRI studies suggest that practicing and observations are also consistent with work on
improving motor skills may enhance the the shifts to a more complex central nervous
developmental plasticity of neuronal connec- system organization, including hemispheric
tions (Zimmerman & Gordon, Chapter 27, connections that occur at the end of the first
this volume). year of life and early part of the second year,
In our review of 200 cases, we also found just as the ability to engage in affect-mediated
that, although many children with autistic spec- chains of social problem solving are on the
trum disorders shared a primary deficit (i.e., ascendancy (Benson & Zaidel, 1985;
connecting affect to processing capacities), Courchesne et al., 1994; Dawson, Warrenburg,
they differed in the levels of developmental & Fuller, 1982; Sperry, 1985).
functioning of their different processing capac-
ities or “component parts.” We noted that the Dynamic Assessments over Time and
relative strength of each component part tend- in the Context of Interventions
ed to determine symptoms and splinter skills,
such as whether a child lined up toys (which The assessment of the functional process-
requires some motor planning) or just banged ing and developmental capacities just described
them, or scripted TV shows (which requires cannot be implemented based only on struc-
some auditory memory) or was silent. It was tured tests or a short observation. A child’s
also found that children with relatively stronger “functioning” involves her ability to learn over
component parts tended to make rapid progress time and in contexts that provide learning
once they were helped to connect affect or opportunities. For example, observing how a
intent to their other processing capacities. child responds to interventions that foster
Children with weaker component parts tended engagement and interaction reveals more about
to make more gradual progress and required the child’s relationship capacity than a one-
more intensive, specific therapies, such as time evaluation of how she relates to the clini-
speech and occupational therapy, in order to cian in an office. Similarly, the child’s ability to
work with the component part directly. learn to gesture, imitate words, and use words
These observations are consistent with purposefully is best assessed by observing her
recent neuroscience studies suggesting that at home, in school, and in the clinician’s office
different processing capacities may compete when the child is highly motivated and provid-
for cortical access, depending on functional ed with developmentally meaningful, interac-
use (Zimmerman & Gordon, Chapter 27, this tive opportunities.
volume). They are also consistent with neu- Traditional ways of assessing children
ropsychological studies of individuals with with developmental problems, which include
autism but without mental retardation that time-limited observations and structured
show that “within affected domains, impair- tests, may often present a misleading picture
ments consistently involved the most complex because they do not deal with the child’s vari-
tasks dependent on higher-order abilities (i.e., ability and range of functioning by looking at
concept formation, complex memory, com- the child’s functioning in a dynamic learning
plex language, and complex motor abilities) situation over time. In fact, the use of struc-
(Minshew & Goldstein, 1998). Higher-level tured tests raises an interesting paradox.
capacities tend to depend more on “meanings” Because many children with special needs
which, in turn, depend on affective interac- are quite variable in their functioning, fixed
tions with the world. Furthermore, these (i.e., standard) presentations of stimuli,
Chapter 4. Principles of Clinical Practice for Assessment and Intervention 63

which may appear attractive from a research and thinking and more substantial challenges
point of view, are likely to produce a great that do derail them. For example, a toddler
deal of variability in functioning or bring out with a mild articulation problem who relates
the child’s lowest level of possible function- and communicates with purposeful gestures
ing. In contrast, optimal levels and stability (e.g., takes Dad to the toy chest) has a cir-
of performance are enhanced by the flexibil- cumscribed difficulty. This child is signifi-
ity of the presentation. cantly different from a toddler who cannot
use social gestures to show what he wants,
Criteria for Immediate Evaluation vs. even if he can repeat words and is being
“Wait and See” derailed in his fundamental ability to relate
and communicate.
A child with functional developmental
impairments involving foundation-building RETHINKING AUTISM,
capacities, such as relating and using ges- MENTAL RETARDATION, AND
tures to communicate and problem solve, SEVERE ATTENTIONAL AND
often requires immediate attention, not a LEARNING PROBLEMS
“wait-and-see” attitude. Because functional
capacities build on each other and there is Moving from standardized, one-time
mounting evidence of age and time limits to assessments to observing functional
developmental, neurobiological plasticity in impairments in the context of truly helpful
a child’s brain, delaying evaluation can interventions over time will certainly change
increase functional impairments. Delaying how therapists diagnose problems. It may, at
intervention until a child is 3 or 4 years old times, change the ultimate diagnosis chosen
and evidences a clear syndrome tends to for a child. These ongoing observations are
increase the therapeutic challenge and may especially relevant for autistic spectrum dis-
affect the ultimate prognosis. As the child falls orders, mental retardation, and many types of
further and further behind, there are more attentional and learning problems.
missed opportunities. Therefore, a determina-
tion of developmentally significant functional Autistic Spectrum Disorders
impairment in and of itself should serve as a
criterion for initiating an appropriate evalua- Some children who meet DSM-IV crite-
tion and intervention. This is especially true ria for autistic spectrum disorders have
for functional impairments that derail the responded very quickly to developmentally
child’s ability to attend, relate, communicate, based, comprehensive intervention programs
play, or think. Because the functional impair- and become warmly related, interactive, and
ments are the focus of the intervention, it is verbally communicative (Greenspan, 1992;
possible to begin the therapeutic work even Greenspan & Wieder, 1997, 1998). Within 1
while observing the child over time to deter- year, for example, many of these children
mine an appropriate diagnosis. As new obser- became engaged and interactive, overcoming
vations are made, the intervention program their perseverative and self-stimulatory pat-
can be revised. In assessing functional terns. After 2 years of intervention, many
impairments, distinctions must be made used language flexibly and creatively, though
between circumscribed problems that do not still with delays. If the diagnosis of these
derail the basics of relating, communicating, children had been delayed for a year while
64 ICDL Clinical Practice Guidelines

their response to intervention was observed, diagnostic challenge is that many symptoms
the diagnosis for this group of rapidly used by clinicians to diagnose autistic spectrum
improving children would be language disor- disorders, such as hand-flapping, repetitive
ders and motor-planning problems, rather behavior, and self-stimulation, are not specific
than the autistic spectrum disorders with to autism. These behaviors are also seen in chil-
which they initially presented. If diagnosis dren with severe motor-planning problems and
were delayed for 2 years, the diagnosis would sensory-modulation difficulties. Lack of relat-
be a less severe language disorder. Many of edness and affective reciprocity is a more spe-
these children subsequently developed excel- cific symptom, but it is often the first behavior
lent language and learning abilities as well as to improve, especially in the children who
a solid capacity to relate to others, including respond quickly to treatment.
peers, with warmth, empathy, creativity, and
a sense of humor. They often evidenced in Mental Retardation
their later school years more circumscribed
processing problems involving motor plan- Because mental retardation implies rela-
ning and sequencing. tively permanent cognitive deficits, observing
Other children who initially met the crite- the response to intervention may be even
ria for autistic spectrum diagnoses have made more important then with autistic spectrum
much slower progress: 1 and 2 years into the disorders. Often, children who have a number
intervention, they continued to meet the crit- of cognitive deficits are assessed as having
eria for autistic spectrum disorders, although relatively permanent global deficits and are
with a greater capacity to relate and commu- diagnosed with varying degrees of mental
nicate. Still others have made extremely slow retardation. Historically, intelligence tests
progress or no progress at all, continuing to (e.g., two standard deviations below the norm
meet the criteria for autism after many years. constitutes mild retardation) have been used
Although each of these groups had a differ- to make this determination. This method,
ent presenting profile in terms of developmen- however, tends to look at the child only at one
tal capacities (e.g., relatedness, motor planning point in time and not take into account her
and sequencing, and visual-spatial processing), learning curve over a longer period of time,
the most distinct difference among the groups is which is essential for observing changes in
the way in which they have responded to an her many functional capacities. Contrary to
optimal intervention program. The response to traditional expectations, many children who
intervention can further classify problems in are diagnosed with mental retardation evi-
relating and communicating. It raises a question dence different patterns of growth and, on
of whether or not children who make extreme- close scrutiny, evidence relative strengths and
ly rapid progress and no longer meet DSM-IV weaknesses even if their intelligence subtest
criteria for PDD should be diagnosed as having scores are all low.
an autistic spectrum disorder or a separate type The most accurate method for making a
of neurodevelopmental dysfunction. Should proper diagnosis is, therefore, to observe a
these children be diagnosed based on their ini- child’s progress while fostering her growth
tial presenting patterns or should therapists take with a truly optimal program. If the child’s
into account their response to an intervention learning curve levels off in spite of the best
program, thereby leading to alternative diag- efforts of a comprehensive, intensive, inter-
nostic considerations? Further complicating the vention approach, it then might be reasonable
Chapter 4. Principles of Clinical Practice for Assessment and Intervention 65

to conclude that the child has stopped making work difficult. Therefore, circumscribed atten-
progress. Therefore, although cognitive prob- tional and learning problems, even if seemingly
lems should be noted and described, a diag- localized to school, require a comprehensive
nosis of mental retardation should only be functional developmental evaluation.
given if the child is in an optimal program A child’s cognitive challenges and chang-
with family involvement, and her learning ing abilities should be continually assessed.
curve has leveled off in all functional areas Access to services should not be delayed
for 2 to 3 years. pending a definitive diagnosis but provided
to a child based on the individual profile of
Severe Attentional and functional developmental impairments. If
Learning Problems necessary for administrative purposes, a pro-
visional diagnosis can be used.
There are two challenges in working with
children with severe attentional and learning A COMPREHENSIVE,
problems. The first challenge is working with DEVELOPMENTALLY BASED
them in relation to their underlying processing APPROACH TO INTERVENTION
patterns rather than working only on their
symptoms or behaviors. For example, a A functional approach to assessment
motor-planning and sequencing problem will leads to a comprehensive approach to inter-
make it difficult for a child to plan and organ- vention that deals with all the relevant func-
ize actions, such as doing homework or lining tional areas in an integrated manner. An
up the numbers to do math problems. An essential part of intervention is a review of the
auditory processing problem may make learn- child’s functional developmental profile
ing to read difficult because of poor sound based on assessments of the child’s functional
discrimination (i.e., poor phonemic aware- developmental level and capacities, individual
ness). Underreactivity to sensations such as processing differences, and the different inter-
touch, pain, and sound may lead to sensory action patterns available at home and school,
craving, increased activity, and poor attention. with peers, and in other settings. The func-
The other challenge for clinicians is to tional assessment is updated in an ongoing
look at all areas of functioning. There is a ten- manner with continuing clinical observations
dency to focus only on the circumscribed as part of the intervention. These ongoing
learning, attentional, or processing problems observations are the basis for revising the
and ignore how, for example, they may influ- child’s intervention program.
ence areas of social and emotional function- A comprehensive intervention program
ing. For example, the same visual-spatial and involves working with the emotional interac-
motor-planning problems that may make plan- tions between the child and family to support
ning and organizing school work and math dif- each of the child’s critical developmental
ficult may also make it difficult for a child to capacities and related underlying processing
interpret other people’s facial expressions and differences. This Developmental, Individual
body posture, which leads to social and emo- Differences, Relationship-based (DIR) ap-
tional misperceptions. This type of emotional proach can be conceptualized as a pyramid.
problem is not a reaction to frustration or feel- Each of the components of the pyramid build
ings of failure but a direct consequence of the on each other and are described briefly in the
same processing problems that make school- following sections and by Figure 1.
66 ICDL Clinical Practice Guidelines

Protective, Stable, Secure capacities to relate, are in even greater need


Relationships of warm, consistent caregiving. Their care-
givers, however, often face challenges in sus-
At the foundation of the intervention taining intimate relationships because it is so
pyramid are the protective, stable, develop- easy to misperceive their children’s inten-
mentally supportive relationships and family tions. Understanding their children’s behav-
patterns that all children require, especially ior as attempts to cope with their difficulties
those with developmental challenges. This or as being overwhelmed by their difficulties
foundation includes physical protection and can often help caregivers recognize these
safety and an ongoing sense of security. misperceptions and develop more creative
Some families require a great deal of support, and empathetic ways of relating to their chil-
therapy, or both in order to stabilize and dren. For example, children who are hyper-
organize these basic family functions. For sensitive to touch may not be rejecting their
example, some families may be dealing with parents’ comfort and care. For such a child,
extreme poverty and chronic states of fearful- parents may have to avoid light touch and use
ness, abuse, and neglect. Some families deep pressure to help the child feel more
require counseling to explore family patterns comfortable. Or, parents may need to under-
and relationships, particularly in connection stand that the child who jumps on a toddler
to the challenges of coping with a child with who is crying may not be primarily aggres-
special needs and the effects on relationships sive but is so sensitive to the sounds of the
between spouses and siblings. cry that he panics and wants the noise
Intervention programs require staff stopped. Similarly, the child who has diffi-
trained to assess family needs, develop culty comprehending words may become
alliances, problem solve, and advocate, confused and avoid communication. He may
including advocating for social and econom- benefit from pictures or gestural signs to
ic support. They also need to provide family understand his environment and predict what
counseling and family or personal therapy will happen next in his interactions with care-
where indicated (Barber, Turnbull, Behr, & givers. The child who is generally avoidant or
Kerns, 1988; Bronfenbrenner, 1986; Dunst & self-absorbed may be underreactive to sensa-
Trivette, 1988; Powell, Hecimovic, & tions, have low muscle tone, and need greater
Christensen, 1992; Robbins, Dunlop, & “wooing” to get beyond his self-absorption.
Plienis, 1991; Turnbull et al., 1986; and The importance of interactive relation-
Shanok, Chapter 14, this volume). ships cannot be underestimated. Almost all
learning occurs in relationships, whether in
Ongoing, Nurturing, Trusting the classroom, with the family, or in therapeu-
Relationships tic sessions. No one would deny that the abil-
ity to enjoy and participate in relationships is
At the second level of the pyramid are the pivotal for learning to relate to others, experi-
ongoing and consistent relationships that ence intimacy and positive self-esteem, and
every child requires. Typically developing develop healthy coping strategies. In addition,
children require nurturing relationships to most cognitive or intellectual capacities
help them achieve emotional and cognitive learned in the first 4 or 5 years of life are also
competency. Children with special needs, based on emotions and relationships
who often already have compromises in their (Greenspan, 1997a). For example, infants
Chapter 4. Principles of Clinical Practice for Assessment and Intervention 67

Specific
interventions,
including
speech therapy,
occupational therapy,
educational programs,
biomedical approaches,
ongoing developmental
and family consultation, and
specific clinical strategies

Developmentally appropriate
practices and interactions,
matched to the child’s
functional developmental level, and
individualized differences in sensory
reactivity, processing, motor planning,
and sequencing in family, peer,
and educational settings

Formation of ongoing, nurturing, trusting relationships

Protective, stable, secure relationships,


including basic services and family support for safety and
security (e.g., physical and emotional contact and adequate food,
housing, and medical care

Figure 1. The Intervention Pyramid for Children with Special Needs


68 ICDL Clinical Practice Guidelines

first learn initial concepts of causality in early modes of communication, it remains more
relationships, as a smile lead to a smile back, fundamental: for example, adults tend to trust
crying leads to comfort, or reaching out leads a person’s nonverbal nod or look of approval
to being picked up, rather than in activities more than words of praise. The system of
such as banging objects on the floor, which reciprocal affective gesturing enables the
leads to learning that this action makes a child to negotiate with his caregivers (envi-
sound. Similarly, the meaning of words and ronment) in small, graduated increments.
gestures, the sense of time, and concepts of Therefore, self-regulation improves and
quantity are also learned as part of interactive, becomes context-dependent, and learning
affective relationships early in life (Greenspan, can become subtle and highly differentiated.
1997a). For example, for a toddler, “a lot” is Relationships are, therefore, the context
more than she expects; “a little” is less than she for learning which behaviors are appropriate
wants. Words are connected to the emotional and which are inappropriate (Greenspan,
experiences that define them, and gestures 1974, 1975). As children’s behavioral reper-
become organized into patterns associated with toires become more complex in the second
emotions and expectation. For example, Dad’s year of life, discriminative and reinforcing
smile leads to a hug and tickle. properties of relationships define which
Relationships serve a number of func- behaviors increase and which behaviors
tions for young children. Most important, decrease. Repertoires are built up through the
they must foster warmth, intimacy, and give-and-take between children and care-
pleasure. In addition, relationships provide givers (i.e., discriminative learning). In addi-
the context in which children experience tion to behaviors, relationships help organize
security, physical safety, protection from ill- a child’s wishes, emerging self-perceptions,
ness and injury, and fulfillment of their basic and a sense of self. The emotional tone and
needs. The regulatory aspects of relationships subtle affective interactions of relationships
(for example, protection of the child from are, therefore, just as important as more easi-
over- or understimulation) help the child ly observable behaviors.
maintain pleasure in intimacy and a secure, Relationships enable a child to learn to
alert, attentive state that permits new learning symbolize experience. The first objects with
and development to occur. which the child has a highly emotional expe-
Relationships provide the basis for com- rience are not playthings, but rather the
munication. Initially, the infant’s communica- human “objects” with which he interacts. In
tion system is nonverbal. It involves affect his interactions, the child goes from “acting
cueing (smiles, assertive glances, frowns), out” his desires or wishes to picturing them
contingent behavioral interactions (pointing, in his mind and labeling them with a word.
taking and giving back, negotiating), and the He goes from desiring Mom and grabbing
like. From the earliest reciprocal smiles to a her to saying “Mom” and looking at her lov-
child taking her mother’s hand, walking to the ingly. This transformation heralds symbolic
refrigerator, and pointing to a favorite food, awareness.
there emerges a complex system of affective, The ability to picture an object when it is
gestural, and behavioral interactions that con- displaced in both time and space is a much-
tinues throughout the life of the individual. used marker for the child’s achievement of
Even though this nonverbal system eventual- object permanence and the emergence of
ly works in conjunction with symbolic-verbal symbolic capacities. Pretend or imaginative
Chapter 4. Principles of Clinical Practice for Assessment and Intervention 69

play involving emotional human dramas dialogue with parents. The child can only
(e.g., the dolls are hugging or fighting) helps understand the meaning of the words or pic-
the child learn the types of affective-based tures in a book in the context of her daily
symbolization that will enable her to connect emotional experiences with others. Without
an image to a wish or intent and then use this emotional experience to abstract from, there
image to think, “If I’m nice to Mom, she will would be no symbolic meaning.
let me stay up late.” Figuring out the motives The starting point for mobilizing growth
of a character in a story as well as the differ- involves meeting a child at his current func-
ence between ten cookies and three cookies tional developmental level and forming a
will depend on the child’s capacity for affec- relationship and engaging in emotional inter-
tive symbolization (Greenspan, 1997a). actions at that level. Children will vary; some
The child’s ability to create mental pic- are unrelated and unpurposeful and require
tures of relationship and, later, other objects, work on engaging and intentional communi-
forms the basis for more advanced symbolic cation whereas others are purposeful but
thinking (Greenspan, 1997a & b). For exam- require extra help in using symbols. Still oth-
ple, a key element essential for future learn- ers use symbols or ideas in a fragmented way
ing and coping is the child’s ability for and need to learn how to be logical and more
self-observation, problem solving, and cre- abstract. For each developmental level, spe-
ative thought. The ability to self-observe is cial types of interactions can enable a child to
essential for self-monitoring of activities as master that level and its related capacities.
simple as coloring inside or outside the lines, To support the child’s ability to relate
or matching pictures with words or numbers. requires a significant amount of time, consis-
Self-observation also helps a child label tency, and understanding. Family difficulties
rather than act out feelings. It helps her to or frequent turnovers among childcare staff
empathize with others and match behavior to or teachers may compromise the requirement
the expectations of the environment. for consistency in a child who is beginning to
Self-observation is essential for advanced learn how to relate to others.
learning and social negotiation. The ability
for self-observation emerges from the ability Developmentally Appropriate
to observe oneself and another in a relation- Practices and Interactions
ship and is a product of the same emotional
interactive relationships as earlier abilities. At the third level of the pyramid lie con-
Similarly, advanced cognitive skills involving sistent relationships and interactions that
numbers, reading, and analytic thinking build have been adapted to the individual differ-
on fundamental, relationship-based capaci- ences and functional developmental needs of
ties. As relationships embrace symbolic capac- each child, which can be thought of as devel-
ities, language grows from words to sentences opmentally appropriate practices and inter-
and concepts derived from daily affective expe- actions for the child with special needs.
riences. Similarly, cognitive capacities involv- Developmentally appropriate practices
ing time and space emerge out of the must characterize family and all other inter-
day-to-day negotiation of waiting or not wait- actions, including home and educational pro-
ing, or of having a little more or less of this grams. They should also be integrated into
or that. Even reading comprehension abilities the different therapies. Developmentally
blossom from the natural give-and-take of appropriate interactions and practices, how-
70 ICDL Clinical Practice Guidelines

ever, are not always easy for caregivers, edu- Home-Based Component
cators, and therapists to implement. The Children spend many hours at home. When
child’s tendency for self-absorption, perse- a child has severe processing difficulties, his
veration, self-stimulation, impulsive actions, choice of activities may not be developmental-
or avoidance often elicits counterreactions ly appropriate or facilitating, such as hours of
that attempt to alter the child’s immediate television-watching, perseverative behavior, or
behavior rather than to build interactions that repetitive computer games. Children generally
will both promote growth and alter the imme- are happier, more productive, less stressed, and
diate behavior. make more progress when involved in develop-
Children who do not have special needs mentally appropriate interactions and prac-
often involve themselves in what the National tices. In fact, these types of interactions in the
Association for the Education of Young home can become the most important factor
Children (NAEYC) has described as devel- for aiding a child’s growth.
opmentally appropriate practices: that is, they Developmentally appropriate practices
play on their own (part of the time) or with often require one-on-one work with the child.
peers, siblings, or parents interactively and Parents must decide how much they can do on
using developmentally appropriate toys, their own and how much help to elicit from
games, and puzzles in a constructive, growth- volunteers, hired students, or home visitors
facilitating manner. For children with special from community, state, or county-supported
needs who, because of their processing intervention programs (e.g., wrap-around
difficulties, may find it very hard to interact services in the state of Pennsylvania provide a
with people or toys in a way that facilitates very useful model in which bachelor’s and
their development, the challenge is to help master’s level professionals often spend more
them become involved in their own special than 20 hours a week supporting the program
types of developmentally appropriate prac- at home).
tices and interactions. Doing this involves The home-based component of develop-
using the profile of the child’s functional mentally appropriate interactions and prac-
developmental level, individual differences in tices can be divided into two parts. One
sensory processing, sensory modulation, involves developmentally appropriate interac-
motor planning and sequencing, and caregiv- tions based on following the child’s natural
er and family interaction patterns to construct interests and emotional inclinations. The other
interactions that will be pleasurable as well as focuses on semistructured problem-solving
developmentally meaningful and facilitating. interactions that also harness the child’s
For example, a 4-year-old may only have the “affect” but involve semistructured created sit-
intermittent functional capacities of a 2-year- uations to facilitate mastery of specific pro-
old, understand visual-spatial experiences cessing capacities, and emotional, cognitive,
better than auditory ones, and be oversensi- language, and motor skills. These two compo-
tive to sensations. The focus, therefore, nents of the home-based program are
should involve working on engaging, gestur- described in the following sections.
ing, and beginning to elaborate symbols by
using a lot of visual support and pretend Following The Child’s Lead
play in a very soothing and regulating con- The caregiver should follow the child’s
text. (See Greenspan & Wieder, Chapter 12, emotional interests to engage him at his func-
this volume.) tional developmental level and challenge him
Chapter 4. Principles of Clinical Practice for Assessment and Intervention 71

to move to the next level, thereby gradually new word, such as “open,” the caregiver
moving the child toward negotiating the six might put the child’s favorite toy outside the
levels outlined in Chart 1. These spontaneous door so that she would want to open it. The
interactions in which the caregiver follows caregiver may then help the child imitate the
the child’s lead will often take on two quali- word “open.” The child, in this way, is prac-
ties. First, the caregiver helps the child move ticing gesturing and using words together.
in the direction that interests him by, for Imitating the sounds “ope” for “open” while
example, putting the ball the child is interest- opening the door also provides the child with
ed in on her head. The child may then take the immediate meaning. Tying the word to affect
ball off the caregiver’s head, thus being drawn or intent (and meaning) facilitates generaliza-
into focusing on the caregiver and engaging tion. This approach is in contrast, for example,
in pleasurable relating and purposeful, two- to the child saying the word “open” in
way communication. Second, the caregiver response to a picture card and only later trying
can become playfully obstructive. For exam- to use the word in real life to solve a problem.
ple, if the child perseveratively opens and In addition to working on language and
closes doors, the caregiver could get “stuck” cognitive and social capacities, semistruc-
behind the door. This action leads the child to tured problem-solving interactions should
focus on the caregiver as he tries to push her focus on activities that enable the child to
away from the door, giggling as he suc- engage in (1) sensory modulation and motor -
ceeds—only to have the caregiver run back to
planning exercises, such as jumping on a
the door again to resume the game. In this
trampoline or mattress, running, spinning,
way, the caregiver facilitates focus and
appropriate roughhousing with deep tactile
engagement. As the child purposefully tries
pressure, and obstacle courses; (2) perceptual-
to engineer moving the caregiver away from
motor exercises and looking and doing
the door, intentional, two-way communica-
games, such as throwing and catching a big
tion is facilitated. Eventually, complex prob-
lem-solving behaviors and even the use of Nerf ball, kicking, and reaching for moving
words, such as “go” or “away” can be elabo- objects; and (3) visual-spatial exercises, such
rated off of the child’s spontaneous, emotion- as treasure hunt games, hide-and-seek, and
al interest in the door. (Strategies for the building complex structures from visual cues.
spontaneous use of developmentally appropri- In general, it is most effective for the
ate interactions to help the child master each child’s therapeutic team, including parents,
of the six core functional emotional capacities educators, speech pathologists, and occupa-
are described in detail in Greenspan & tional therapists to meet weekly to design the
Wieder, Chapter 12, “Developmentally functional goals for the semistructured aspect
Appropriate Practices and Interactions.”) of the home-based intervention. At least one-
third to one-half of the child’s available time
Semistructured, Problem-Solving at home should be spent on spontaneous
Interactions interactions (following the child’s lead and
The caregiver should create learning working off of natural affect and inclination
challenges for the child to master. These may to foster the six functional levels described
involve social, motor, sensory, spatial reason- earlier) and the remaining two-thirds to one-
ing, language, and other cognitive skills. For half on developmentally appropriate, semi-
example, if the goal is to help a child learn a structured problem-solving activities.
72 ICDL Clinical Practice Guidelines

Semistructured, problem-solving activi- Specific therapies often work in a semi-


ties also need to be geared to each child’s structured manner on these types of impor-
unique profile. When put into a problem- tant capacities. (See the Table of Contents for
solving context with emotional intent, the related chapters.) Team meetings should sug-
following types of activities may also be gest the goals of this part of the home
included: program.
• Imitating new words and using concepts
that help the child solve a problem he Peer Interaction
wants to solve, for example, “open,” “up Peer play is especially important once
there,” or “go.” the child has mastered preverbal problem-
• Motor-based challenges, such as gross- solving skills and is moving into the early
motor movement, balance, movement in stages of using ideas in a functional and
space, running, jumping, spinning, percep- spontaneous manner. The now-engaged,
tual-motor activities (involving looking and intentional, partially verbal, and imaginative
doing and crossing the midline). child needs to practice his emerging skills not
• Spatial problem solving, such as treasure only with adults but also with other children
hunt games in which the child is given who are at a similar or higher developmental
clues about how to find her favorite toy, age (i.e., the other children need to be inter-
first in the box in front of her and, even- active, somewhat verbal, and imaginative).
tually, in the box upstairs near another However, the playmates need not be the same
box behind the blue chair. age as the child. For example, if the child is
• Motor-imitation exercises, such as copy- 41/2 years old, but has a functional, emotional
ing the caregiver by touching eyes, ears, developmental capacity of 3 years, he might
nose and, eventually, vocal (sound) imita- prefer the company (and vice versa) of 3-
tions leading to word development. year-old playmates.
• Spatial and quantity concepts, such as At this point, individual, one-on-one play
“here,” there,” “big,” “little,” and, eventu- dates should occur four or more times per
ally, including “more” or “less,” and asso- week for one hour or more. Initially, an adult
ciation of numbers, time, or distance may have to facilitate the interactions to help
(e.g., finding Mommy in different parts deter the children from drifting into parallel
of the house, negotiating one versus three play. The adult may create a game to help the
cookies, or showing with hands the dif- children work jointly, such as having both
ference between a little and a lot). children hide together while the adult tries to
• Facilitation of conceptual understanding find them. While following the children’s
by using cards where the word is under the lead, the caregiver is also free to create games
picture and is used to help the child get the that facilitate interactions among the chil-
juice or a favorite toy, or as a cue for pre- dren. The goal is to help the children “rub
tending what the word or sentence conveys. shoulders” with each other and to communi-
• Visualization exercises, as the child cate with gestures and words.
becomes older, to help the child picture The need for peer play occurs at about the
words, sentences, or quantities (2 + 2 = 4) same time that a child needs to be integrated,
to facilitate a deeper understanding of often with an aide, into a regular preschool
concepts. These concepts may also be program or into an ongoing inclusion or inte-
acted out. grated program.
Chapter 4. Principles of Clinical Practice for Assessment and Intervention 73

Setting Limits, Facilitating As a child makes progress and is purpose-


“Compliance” fully interactive, both encouragement and
Developmentally appropriate practices sensitive limits can help him work with
used to foster new functional capacities can groups of children and follow expectations.
help parents, clinicians, and educators with Dangerous, as opposed to noncompliant,
one of their most difficult challenges—how to behavior needs to be dealt with immediately
integrate the process of setting limits and com- with firm but gentle limits. These limits
pliance with other clinical and educational should be based on the child’s functional
goals. Following rules and maintaining safety developmental capacities, not on actual age or
are understandable goals. Not infrequently, expectations for the other children in a group.
however, the need for compliance and control Just as Congress has appropriately mandated
takes the form of strapping a child into a chair, that a child be educated in the least restrictive
physically forcing him to walk to the bath- environment, therapists should teach the child
room, or using other types of restraint. the least restrictive, developmentally appro-
The key to teaching a child to follow priate tactics to control his behavior and be
rules is to provide developmentally appropri- sensitive to the needs of others.
ate practices and interactions that meet the
child at his functional developmental level in Specific Therapies and
the context of his individual differences. For Educational Strategies
example, for a child who is impulsive and is
not yet capable of logical, verbal thinking and At the apex of the pyramid are the specif-
conversation, developmentally appropriate ic therapeutic and educational techniques that
interactions mean a one-on-one aide working build on and also facilitate the child’s basic
with the child on the basics of relating and capacities for attention, engagement, inten-
purposeful interaction. The back-and-forth tional two-way communication, and the cre-
signaling, which will include limits, however, ative use of symbols. In this way, new
will be of the type one would implement with capacities are tied to the child’s sense of pur-
a 1- to 2-year-old child. Through this type of pose and self (i.e., his affects). Integrating
one-on-one interaction, the child gradually therapeutic strategies into the child’s natural-
learns how to be a purposeful, preverbal ly occurring interests and activities can be
communicator. Gradually, responding to lim- very helpful in simultaneously fostering her
its becomes a part of this purposeful commu- capacities to initiate, engage, communicate,
nication. Expecting a child who can not yet problem solve, and think, as well as learn
negotiate basic needs with a series of back- new functional skills. While new skills are on
and-forth signals to follow group-oriented the ascendancy, perhaps in relation to matu-
rules will often result in frustration, anger, rational shifts, is an optimal time to intensify
impulsive behavior, and, more importantly, a particular therapy that supports that skill,
slower progress. Developmentally appropri- such as physical therapy to facilitate walking.
ate practices are more likely to tap into poten- A variety of strategies have been advocat-
tial plasticity from within the individual ed. Some attempt to offer a comprehensive
child’s own brain than will externally- approach whereas others focus on specific
imposed structured techniques (i.e., too com- issues. However, an approach cannot be truly
plex or restrictive) that bypass neural comprehensive unless it works with all the lev-
networks already in place. els of the pyramid. Both focused and partially
74 ICDL Clinical Practice Guidelines

comprehensive approaches must build on the There is often confusion between very
foundation previously described. These helpful clinical strategies that foster sensory
include educational approaches (e.g., Bailey modulation, muscle tone, and motor planning
& Wolery, 1992), cognitive, language, senso- and debates about the explanatory value of
ry, and motor processing approaches (e.g., sensory integration theory. Like all broad the-
Prizant & Wetherby, 1988; Bricker, 1993), ories, time and continuing research will be
peer models (Odom & Strain, 1986; Strain, needed for further refinement of sensory inte-
Shores, & Timm, 1977), behavioral gration theory. In the meantime, clinical tech-
approaches (Lovaas, 1980, 1987; Durand, niques that enable children to master
Berotti, & Wiener, 1993; Haring & Lovinger, functional developmental capacities should be
1989; Odom & Haring, 1993), work with employed based on their clinical usefulness.
family patterns (e.g., Barber et al., 1988; (For further discussion, see Williamson,
Bronfenbrenner, 1986; Dunst & Trivette, Anzalone, & Hanft, Chapter 8; Long &
1988; Powell et al., 1992; Robbins et al., 1991; Sippel, Chapter 9; Koller, Chapter 11; Wachs,
Turnbull et al., 1986), interactive “floor time” Chapter 20; Youssefi & Youssefi, Chapter 21;
approaches (Greenspan, 1992; Greenspan & Feuerstein, Chapter 22, this volume.)
Wieder, 1998), and work with the social milieu
(e.g., Ostrosky, Kaiser, & Odom, 1993; Educational Program
An educational program should be geared
Wolfberg & Schuler, 1993).
to a child’s functional developmental capaci-
ties and processing profile and must involve
Speech and Language Therapy
developmentally appropriate practices for
Speech-and-language therapy is especial-
children with special needs. For example,
ly helpful for preverbal, as well as other types
some programs attempt to have a child learn
of symbolic communication (e.g., verbal,
in a group even though the child requires a
pictures, signs). It can also be especially highly individualized approach. Often, chil-
valuable for oral-motor work and related dren are able to learn in groups only after
expressive language challenges. Three or they have advanced to the point of mastering
more individual sessions per week of 30 to 60 individual relationships and preverbal prob-
minutes each is often required, in addition to lem-solving interactions and are already
consultation to and integration with the home beginning to use words. Nonetheless, school
and educational program. (See Gerber & settings with other children can be very
Prizant, Chapter 5, and Madell, Chapter 6, enriching, even if initially most of the work is
this volume, for further discussion.) conducted on an individual basis. Gradually,
learning can move more toward group inter-
Occupational and Physical Therapy actions.
Occupational and physical therapy are A child who is not yet engaged or pur-
especially helpful for motor problems, motor- poseful needs to be involved in one-on-one
planning and sequencing difficulties, and sen- interactions with a teacher, aide, or volunteer
sory modulation and processing challenges. throughout most of the school day. The aide’s
Two to three individual sessions per week of 30 role is not to sit behind the child or help the
to 60 minutes each is often required, in addition child conform to noninteractive routines, but
to consultation to and integration with the home rather to “woo” the child into learning inter-
and school programs. actions (i.e., two-way purposeful interactions
Chapter 4. Principles of Clinical Practice for Assessment and Intervention 75

and a constant flow of back-and-forth ideas in a variety of contexts. To meet this


communication). Once a child progresses to goal, the child requires opportunities to inter-
purposeful gestural communication, complex act with peers (one-on-one and in small
imitation and, over time, to using symbols groups) who are related, interactive, and ver-
(e.g., words, pictures, and signs) and is able bal (often with an adult mediating). She
to relate to and communicate logically with requires an integrated setting or an aide in a
peers, two-way symbolic interactions need to mainstream setting.
be facilitated to promote logical thinking. As It is important to emphasize that educa-
a child becomes more logical and abstract, tional programs for a child with special needs
group learning and routine academic activi- should not comprise a series of disconnected
ties can provide constructive learning oppor- cognitive learning opportunities. The important
tunities. This stage, however, is the culmination academic goals just stated do not emerge as
of a long learning process. isolated cognitive skills by simply practicing
Many programs are not equipped to pro- them. Having a child sit and look does not
vide the needed one-on-one interaction for mean he understands or that he can learn
most of the school day, which is required by from listening. Nor does having a child mem-
the child who has not yet fully learned to orize a passage mean he understands it. These
engage with peers and adults or communicate skills are part of a progression where each
and problem solve with gestures. In addition, step builds on another. Comprehension, relat-
education for children with special needs ing, communicating, reading, math, writing,
may take many forms. Often, goals are and engaging in higher-level problem solving
derived from the academic objectives for must build on the six functional capacities
older children. When this occurs, there is described earlier. Education programs should
often little attention given to the sequence by identify where each child is in his unique pro-
which young children acquire the core func- gression, meet the child at his level, and work
tional abilities that will enable them to relate, on the next steps. Only this type of logical
communicate, think, and learn, including progression constitutes developmentally
mastering traditional academic tasks. Typically, appropriate practices for a child in school.
there is an overemphasis on compliance in a For example, to read with meaning or to
group situation before a child has learned to understand math, a child must learn how to:
negotiate one-on-one relationships or even • “Want” to attend and engage with others
understand simple expectations. For exam- (first with one person at a time and then a
ple, a child may be taught in a rote manner to few) in order to be part of experiences
match shapes before she can engage in basic that will enable him to discriminate
multistep, preverbal, problem-solving inter- sounds and sights necessary for reading
action sequences, such as getting a toy out of and math, and to eventually give words
a box, bringing it over, and motioning for an and quantity concepts meaning.
adult to play. • Communicate logically with simple ges-
The child who is already attentive, engaged, tures and affect cues, and then with com-
purposefully interactive, involved in complex, plex problem-solving behaviors in order
ongoing preverbal problem-solving communi- to understand patterns of cause-and-
cation (i.e., gesturing), and is beginning to use effect relationships and different levels of
words presents special opportunities. She needs logic. These capacities are essential for
to learn to elaborate and build bridges between any type of academic work.
76 ICDL Clinical Practice Guidelines

• Represent or symbolize emotional experi- to strengthen processing capacities such as


ences and build logical bridges between visual-spatial, auditory, and motor planning
them. What a child knows from preverbal are being developed and are available in
experience as well as new verbal experi- innovative schools and programs but are not
ence must be understood, communicated, yet sufficiently available in all education
and thought about logically with words, and special education programs. (See Miller
pictures, or other symbols. This is neces- and Eller-Miller, Chapter 19; Wachs, Chapter
sary for creative and logical thought, 20; Youssefi & Youssefi, Chapter 21;
abstract thinking, and all academic tasks Feuerstein, Chapter 22; and Bell, Chapter
requiring the comprehension of words or 25, this volume.)
the ability to reason with symbols. In addition, many intelligent children
with moderate to severe circumscribed learn-
In addition to the difficulties in providing ing and/or attention problems who are fully
one-on-one interaction, most educational capable of being mainstreamed often require
programs, due to administrative policies, both participation in regular classes and one-
change intervention teams for a child and on-one and very small-group (two to four
family when the child is 3 years old. For a children) learning opportunities to master
child with autistic patterns who is learning to specific processing challenges. Many require
relate to and trust others, this change can special learning opportunities for as much as
undermine educational progress. Therefore, half of the day, during which they can work
continuity of staff and program is critical dur- on their processing challenges with the most
ing the infancy and preschool years. up-to-date techniques. At present, unfortu-
Developing the most appropriate educa- nately, many children with learning and
tional program for children with special needs attention problems do not get the adequate
is extremely challenging and often requires time or techniques they require.
one-on-one or very small group learning. To
increase one-on-one learning opportunities Exploration of Biomedical
that enable children to climb the developmen- Interventions
tal ladder and eventually learn and communi- Certain children with special needs will
cate in groups, school programs should benefit from biomedical assessments and
provide more aides and allow parents to vol- considerations of interventions, including
unteer, as they do in cooperative preschools. medications. For example, medications in the
(See Wieder and Kalmanson, Chapter 13, this selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor group
volume, for further discussion.) may facilitate motor planning, sequencing,
Children with learning and attention prob- and attention and reduce perseverative or
lems who have progressed and mastered their repetitive behavior. To determine if a bio-
basic challenges in relating and communica- medical intervention should be a part of the
tion, as well as children who have already therapeutic regimen, a basic biomedical
mastered these skills but have problems with assessment needs to be implemented, as
attention, learning, or both, require an educa- described by Robinson, Chapter 17, on bio-
tional program that provides opportunities to medical approaches.
resolve specific processing challenges and In exploring a trial of medication, it is
progress to high levels of abstract thinking and essential to look carefully at the side effects
academic proficiency. Promising techniques and weigh the benefits and risks. Medications
Chapter 4. Principles of Clinical Practice for Assessment and Intervention 77

are not designed as precisely as would be opti- intervention program. Often, various levels
mal and will often affect nontargeted areas. In of support are needed.
addition, exceptional caution and close moni-
toring are required if more than one medica- SPECIFIC CLINICAL TECHNIQUES
tion at a time is being considered.
Specific clinical techniques to deal with
Ongoing Developmental and Family difficult symptoms or behaviors are often
Consultation needed as part of an overall program. There
In addition to conducting the initial diag- are two primary philosophies that tend to
nostic evaluation and monitoring progress, guide parents, educators, and clinicians. The
developmental specialists (e.g., a psychologist, behavioral approach attempts to decrease the
behavioral or developmental pediatrician, or target behavior directly by ignoring it, using
child psychiatrist) may be helpful in facilitat- negative consequences, or both, and attempt-
ing or coaching the family in constructing ing to replace it with another behavior sup-
developmentally appropriate interactions at ported by positive consequences (e.g.,
home and at school. For some families, this interrupting perseverative behavior and
consultation may take the form of regular insisting on and reinforcing another activity).
meetings one or more times per week, during Intensive behavioral approaches are often
which the therapist works with the parents suggested as an overall treatment method.
Although they have a circumscribed role for
and other caregivers on their interactions
selected children, they do not address many
with the child and, if needed, works directly
important functional capacities, including the
with the child. Consultations may also
capacity to relate with pleasure and warmth,
involve periodic home visits. In addition,
to connect affect or intent to motor planning
intensive family work with supports and
and emerging symbolic capacities (for cre-
coordination from community agencies will ative and reflective thinking), and to process
be necessary for some families. The form the sensory information and plan actions.
consultation takes will vary depending on the Although behavioral approaches are
needs of the child and the family. (For a more widely used in most communities, as indicat-
detailed discussion, see Greenspan, 1992; ed in Chapter 3, children with typical patterns
Shanok, Chapter 14, this volume) of autism who are in these programs general-
ly do not make clinically meaningful
Family Support and Dynamic Family progress. They are often unable to learn to live
Processes independently, work, and participate in a
Special clinical techniques are often need- range of age-expected social relationships.
ed to support families. Although there is a long (See, Chapter 3; and Tsakiris, Chapter 31, this
tradition of family-based programs for chil- volume, for a full discussion.) Nonetheless,
dren with special needs, many of these pro- specific behavioral exercises can be useful for
grams offer only minimal help to the whole a child who has severe motor-planning prob-
family, including siblings. Marital conflicts, lems and, therefore, is having difficulty learn-
misperceptions of the special-needs child or ing to imitate actions, sounds, and words.
her siblings, and higher levels of stress may When the child can begin using imitative
characterize the family functioning. It is also capacities at the moment he is trying to solve
difficult for a family to orchestrate an entire a problem, such as copying the act of opening
78 ICDL Clinical Practice Guidelines

a door to get out, or saying “up” to get his toy available on an as-needed basis. In this way,
from a high shelf, he can move into more behavioral strategies are part of the tactics
dynamic, interactive learning approaches. that are available to the clinician.
Therefore, as one part of the semistructured Interestingly, developmentally appropriate
component of a broad comprehensive pro- practices can be conceptualized in behavioral
gram, some children may, for a period of time, terms. In this model, the focus is on develop-
require behavioral exercises in conjunction mental processes (i.e., classes of behavior),
with dynamic, interactive work. such as creative, ongoing reciprocal interaction
Intensive behavioral intervention tech- (circles of communication) rather than on spe-
niques by themselves tend to be almost anti- cific behaviors, such as “looking.” The
thetical to harnessing the child’s natural processes selected for focus are from a hierar-
affect and intent as a basis for internal control chy of developmental capacities that need to be
and initiation of behavior and language. They mastered. The cues (discriminative stimuli)
have as their strength the external control of and reinforcers are based on internal affect
behavior through external discriminative and states rather than on external events (i.e., natu-
reinforcing stimuli (prompts and reinforcers). ral social cues and reinforcers). Interactions are
External control can help get some behaviors continuous rather than stop-start. Some inten-
started. While there has been the hope that sive behavioral approaches are moving toward
internal prompts and reinforcers can take such a developmentally based model (e.g.,
over as part of a generalization process, the Pivotal Response Training [(Schreibman,
child does far better if she can learn new Stahmer, & Pierce, 1996]).
behaviors and concepts under the influence The functional, developmental approach
of internal affect cues. These give her actions attempts to understand the broad category of
immediate meaning and direction. functioning that the worrisome behavior is
Intensive behavioral intervention tech- part of, and the broad functional categories
niques can be compared to the strategy of a that may be missing. In this context, func-
tennis coach using hand-over-hand learning. tional means a broad developmental capacity
In this approach, the coach holds the racket essential for the child’s progress, such as pur-
with the student and drops the ball in front of poseful gesturing or using ideas. It does not
the student only when teaching a difficult, refer to a narrow functional behavior, such as
new handgrip or stroke. Most of the learning dressing. Developmentally appropriate inter-
occurs either through playing actual games or actions and practices are used to help the
in dynamic drills where the student is learn- child master new functional capacities. For
ing to use his forehand or backhand while on example, repetitive opening and closing of a
the run. The behavioral strategy is similar to door is part of the larger category of repeti-
the hand-over-hand approach because a ther- tive and rigid ways of dealing with the world.
apist uses it temporarily to shore up a skill The missing functional category is the devel-
such as imitative capacities. Once the child opmentally more advanced capacity for flex-
reachs a certain level, however, most of the ible, creative interactions in the world. In this
learning needs to occur in the semistructured model, the worrisome, repetitive behavior
learning situations (such as the dynamic ten- serves as an opportunity for progressing to
nis drilling) and in the spontaneous learning higher functional developmental levels of
interactions (such as playing games). The flexible, creative interactions. Therefore, as
more structured approach would remain described earlier, a perseverative behavior,
Chapter 4. Principles of Clinical Practice for Assessment and Intervention 79

such as opening and closing a door, is turned SUMMARY


into a purposeful interaction. The functional
developmental approach keeps the larger This chapter reviewed the general clinical
therapeutic goal of higher-level functional principles that characterize a functional
developmental capacities in the forefront. developmental approach to assessment and
Both philosophies can be used in an inte- intervention. The chapters that follow review
grated manner. For example, a caregiver can intervention research, examine the evaluation
interrupt a child’s self-injurious behavior while and intervention process for each functional
simultaneously drawing him into interactions area, and explore the evaluation and diag-
that serve a similar sensory need, such as deep nostic process as well as the intervention
tactile pressure coupled with warm engage- process in more detail. Relevant neuro-
ment and two-way intentional communication. science research also is considered. ■
80 ICDL Clinical Practice Guidelines

REFERENCES

Bailey, D. B., & Wolery, M. (1992). (1994). Role of cerebellar and parietal dys-
Normalizing early intervention. Topics in function in the social and cognitive deficits
Early Childhood Special Education, 10, in patients with infantile autism. Paper pre-
33-47. sented at the Autism Society of America
Barber, P. A., Turnbull, A. P., Behr, S. K., & Annual Conference, Las Vegas, NV.
Kerns, G. M. (1988). A family systems per- Dawson, G., & Galpert, I. (1990). Mothers’ use
spective on early childhood special educa- of imitative play for facilitating social respon-
tion. In S. L. Odom & M. B. Karnes (Eds.), siveness and toy play in young autistic chil-
Early intervention for infants and children dren. Developmental and Psychopathology, 2,
with handicaps: An empirical base (pp. 151-162.
179-198). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. Dawson, G., Warrenburg, S., & Fuller, P.
Baranek, G. T. (1999). Autism during infan- (1982). Cerebral lateralization in individu-
cy: A retrospective video analysis of senso- als diagnosed as autistic in early childhood.
rimotor and social behaviors at 9-12 Brain and Language, 15, 353-368.
months of age. Journal of Autism and Dunst, C. J., & Trivette, C. M. (1988). A fam-
Developmental Disorders, 29(3), 213-224. ily systems model of early intervention with
Baron-Cohen, S. (1994). Mindblindness: An handicapped and developmentally at-risk
essay on autism and theories of mind. children. In D. Powell (Ed.), Parent educa-
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. tion as early childhood intervention:
Benson, F., & Zaidel, E. (1985). The dual Emerging directions in theory, research, and
brain. New York: Guilford Press. practice (pp. 131-180). New York: Ablex.
Bricker, D. (1993). Then, now, and the path Durand, V. M., Berotti, D., & Weiner, J. S.
between: A brief history of language inter- (1993). Functional communication train-
vention. In A. P. Kaiser & D. B. Gray ing: Factors affecting effectiveness, gener-
(Eds.), Enhancing children’s communica- alization, and maintenance. In J. Reichle &
tion: Research foundations for intervention D. P. Wacker (Eds.), Communicative alter-
(pp. 11-13). natives to challenging behavior:
Bristol, M., Cohen, D., Costello, J., Denckla, Integrating functional assessment and
M., Eckberg, T., Kallen, R., Kraemer, H., intervention strategies (pp. 317-340).
Lord, C., Maurer, R., McIllvane, W., Greenspan, S. I. (1974). The clinical use of
Minshew, N., Sigman, M., & Spence, A. operant learning techniques: Some complex
(1996). State of the science in autism: issues. American Journal of Psychiatry,
Report to the National Institutes of Health. 131(8), 852-857.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Greenspan, S. I. (1975). A consideration of
Disorders, 26, 121-154. some learning variables in the context of
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986). Ecology of the psychoanalytic theory: Toward a psychoan-
family as context for human development alytic learning perspective. Psychological
research perspectives. Developmental Issues, IX(1): Monograph 33. New York:
Psychology, 22, 723-742. International Universities Press.
Courchesne, E., Akshoomoff, N., Egaas, B., Greenspan, S. I. (1979). Intelligence and
Lincoln, A. J., Saitoh, O., Schreibman, L., adaptation: An integration of psychoanalyt-
Townsend, J., & Yeung-Courchesne, R. ic and Piagetian developmental psychology.
Chapter 4. Principles of Clinical Practice for Assessment and Intervention 81

Psychological Issues (Monograph 47/68). preschool children with autism. Journal of


New York: International Universities Press. the Association for Persons with Severe
Greenspan, S. I. (1989). The development of Handicaps, 14, 255-262.
the ego: Implications for personality theo- Lewy, A. L., & Dawson, G. (1992). Social
ry, psychopathology, and the psychothera- stimulation and joint attention in young
peutic process. Madison, CT: International autistic children. Journal of Abnormal
Universities Press. Child Psychology, 20(6), 555-566.
Greenspan, S. I. (1992). Infancy and early Lovaas, O. I. (1980). Behavioral training with
childhood: The practice of clinical assess- young autistic children. In B. Wilcox & A.
ment and intervention with emotional and Thompson (Eds.), Critical issues in educat-
developmental challenges. Madison, CT: ing autistic children and youth (pp. 220-
International Universities Press. 233). U.S. Department of Education, Office
Greenspan, S. I. (1997a). The growth of the of Special Education.
mind and the endangered origins of intelli- Lovaas, O. I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and
gence. Reading, MA: Addison Wesley normal educational and intellectual function-
Longman. ing in young autistic children. Journal of
Greenspan, S. I. (1997b). Developmentally Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 3-9.
based psychotherapy. New York: International McEachin, J. J., Smith, T., & Lovaas, O. I.
Universities Press.
(1993). Long-term outcome for children
Greenspan, S. I. (1998). Commentary:
with autism who received early intensive
Guidance for constructing clinical practice
behavioral treatment. American Journal on
guidelines for developmental and learning
Mental Retardation, 97, 359-372.
disorders: Knowledge vs. evidence-based
Minshew N., & Goldstein, G. (1998). Autism as
approaches. Journal of Developmental and
a disorder of complex information process-
Learning Disorders, 2(2), 171-192.
ing. Mental Retardation and Developmental
Greenspan, S. I., & Wieder, S. (1997).
Developmental patterns and outcomes in Disabilities, 4, 129-136.
infants and children with disorders in relat- Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990).
ing and communicating: A chart review of A longitudinal study of joint attention and
200 cases of children with autistic spectrum language development in autistic children.
diagnoses. The Journal of Developmental Journal of Autism and Developmental
and Learning Disorders, 1, 87-141. Disorders, 20(1), 115-128.
Greenspan, S. I., & Wieder, S. (1998). The Odom, S. L., & Haring, T. G. (1993). Con-
child with special needs: Intellectual and textualism and applied behavior analysis:
emotional growth. Reading, MA: Addison Implications for early childhood education
Wesley Longman. for children with disabilities. In R. Gardner et
Gresham, F. M., & MacMillan, D. L. (1998). al. (Eds.). Behavior analysis in education:
Early intervention project: Can its claims Focus on measurably superior instruction.
be substantiated and its effects replicated? Pacific Grove, CA: Brookes/Cole.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Odom, S. L., & Strain, P. S. (1986). A compar-
Disorders, 28(1), 5-12. ison of peer-initiation and teacher-antecedent
Haring, T. G., & Lovinger, L. (1989). interventions for promoting reciprocal social
Promoting social interaction through teach- interaction of autistic preschoolers. Journal
ing generalized play initiation responses to of Applied Behavior Analysis, 19, 59-71.
82 ICDL Clinical Practice Guidelines

Osterling, J., & Dawson, G. (1994). Early mental disorders. American Journal on
recognition of children with autism: A Mental Retardation, 102, 238-249.
study of first birthday home videotapes. Sperry, R. W. (1985). Consciousness, person-
Journal of Autism and Developmental al identity, and the divided brain. In F.
Disorders, 24(3), 247-257. Benson & E. Zaidel (Eds.), The dual brain
Ostrosky, M. M., Kaiser, A. P., & Odom, S. L.
(pp. 11-27). New York: Guilford Press.
(1993). Facilitating children’s social-com-
municative interactions through the use of Strain, P. S., Shores, R. E., & Timm, M.
peer-mediated interventions. In A. P. Kaiser (1977). Effects of peer social initiations on
& D. B. Gray (Eds.), Enhancing children’s the behavior of withdrawn preschool chil-
communication: Research foundations for dren. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
intervention: Vol. 2. (pp.159-185). Baltimore: 10, 289-298.
Paul H. Brookes. Tanguay, P. E. (1999). The diagnostic assess-
Powell, T. H., Hecimovic, A., & Christensen, ment of autism using social communication
L. (1992). Meeting the unique needs of domains. Paper presented at the Inter-
families. In D. E. Berkel (Ed.), Autism:
disciplinary Council on Developmental and
Identification, education, and treatment
Learning Disorders’ Third Annual Inter-
(pp. 187-224). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Prizant, B. M., & Wetherby, A. M. (1988). national Conference, Autism and Disorders
Providing services to children with autism of Relating and Communicating, McLean,
(ages 0 to 2 years) and their families. VA, November 12-14.
Topics in Language Disorders, 9, 1-23. Tanguay, P. E., Robertson, J., & Derrick, A.
Robbins, F. R., Dunlap, G., & Plienis, A. J. (1998). A dimensional classification of
(1991). Family characteristics, family train- autism spectrum disorder by social communi-
ing, and the progress of young children cation domains. Journal of the American
with autism. Journal of Early Intervention, Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
15, 173-184.
37(3), 271.
Rogers, S. (1996). Brief report: Early inter-
Turnbull, A. P., & Turnbull, H. R., with
vention in autism. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 26, 243-246. Summers, J. A., Brotherson, J. J., &
Schopler, E. (1987). Specific and non-specific Benson, H. A. (1986). Families, profession-
factors in the effectiveness of a treatment als and exceptionality: A special partner-
system. American Psychologist, 42, 262-267. ship. Columbus, OH: Merrill.
Schreibman, L., Stahmer, A. C., & Pierce, K. Wetherby, A. M., & Prizant, B. M. (1993).
L. (1996). Alternative applications of piv- Profiling communication and symbolic abil-
otal response training: Teaching symbolic ities in young children. Journal of Childhood
play and social interaction skills. In L. Communication Disorders, 15, 23-32.
Koegel, R. Koegel, &. G. Dunlap (Eds.),
Wolfberg, P. J., & Schuler, A. L. (1993).
Positive behavioral change. Baltimore,
MD: Paul H. Brookes. Integrated play groups: A model for pro-
Smith, T., Eikeseth, S., Morten, K., & moting the social and cognitive dimensions
Lovaas, I. (1997). Intensive behavioral of play in children with autism. Journal of
treatment for preschoolers with severe Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23,
mental retardation and pervasive develop- 467-468.

Você também pode gostar