Você está na página 1de 14

J Bus Psychol

DOI 10.1007/s10869-010-9207-0

Examining the Relationships Between Personality,


Coping Strategies, and Work–Family Conflict
Boris B. Baltes • Ludmila S. Zhdanova •

Malissa A. Clark

Ó Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010

Abstract strong influence of individual factors on a person’s choice


Purpose The purpose of this study was to examine the of coping strategies.
processes through which personality characteristics may Originality/Value The examination of the processes
influence work–family conflict (WFC). Specifically, the through which personality characteristics may influence
mediating effects of selection, optimization, and compen- work–family conflict (WFC) has not received adequate
sation (SOC) behavioral stress-coping strategies on the attention. This article advances work–family conflict
relationship between personality characteristics and WFC research by examining the mediating role of behavioral
were tested. strategies aimed to cope with competing demands of work
Design/Methodology/Approach A snowball sampling and family roles in the relationship between individual
technique was used to recruit 289 working adults, who difference variables and WFC.
completed online questionnaires. The proposed model was
tested using path analysis. Keywords Work–family conflict  Work–family
Findings Conscientiousness and agreeableness were interference  Coping strategies  Personality  Stress
related to greater usage of work and family behavioral
coping strategies, and these behavioral strategies influ-
enced levels of experienced WFC. Negative affect was Work–family conflict (WFC) occurs when role, time, and
found to have direct effects on work interference with behavioral demands in one domain (e.g., work) are com-
family (WIF) and family interference with work (FIW), peting or interfering with responsibilities in another
and emotional stability was found to have a direct effect on domain (e.g., family; Edwards and Rothbard 2000). Since
WIF conflict. inter-role interference is an underlying foundation of a
Implications Findings suggest that different processes conflict between the two domains, WFC can also be
underlie the influence of specific personality characteristics referred to as work–family interference (WFI; Greenhaus
on WFC. These findings can have implications for the et al. 2006). Evidence suggests this conflict or interference
effectiveness of training programs and interventions aimed is directional, such that work responsibilities can interfere
at reducing work–family conflict levels of employees, in with responsibilities in the family domain (WIF), and
that trainers and managers should take into account the family responsibilities can interfere with responsibilities in
the work domain (FIW; Frone et al. 1992). It has been
demonstrated that the conflict between work and family
B. B. Baltes (&)
domains (i.e., WIF and FIW) negatively affects important
Wayne State University, Detroit, MI, USA
e-mail: b.baltes@wayne.edu individual level (e.g., depression, stress, and substance
abuse; Frone et al. 1992, 1997) and organizational level
L. S. Zhdanova outcomes (e.g., absenteeism, turnover; Eby et al. 2005).
Carleton University, Ottawa, ON, Canada
Considering the negative influence that WFC can have
M. A. Clark on individuals and organizations, researchers and practi-
Auburn University, Auburn, AL, USA tioners have begun identifying and examining factors that

123
J Bus Psychol

contribute to WFC. Recent meta-analyses and independent Personality Variables and WFC
studies have identified various important antecedents of
WIF and FIW. Generally, antecedents are categorized as Personality refers to the stable mental structures and pro-
work-related factors (e.g., job stressors and job involve- cesses that influence how people interpret and react,
ment), non-work-related factors (e.g., family stressors emotionally and behaviorally, to their environment (James
and family involvement), and individual differences (e.g., and Mazerolle 2002). Thus, not surprisingly, researchers
affectivity and extraversion; Bruck and Allen 2003; have theorized that personality should also influence indi-
Grzywacz and Marks 2000). vidual reactions to the characteristics of work and family
To date, relatively little has been done to advance the domains. Furthermore, studies have empirically demon-
understanding of the processes through which individual strated that personality traits indeed relate to perceptions of
differences (e.g., personality characteristics) influence WFC (e.g., Bruck and Allen 2003; Michel and Clark 2009).
WFC. Thus, the purpose of this study is to extend our Recently, researchers have called for a closer examination
understanding of the influence of personality characteris- of the relationship between personality traits and WFC
tics on WFC. Theoretically, personality traits can have (e.g., Eby et al. 2005). In this study, we included person-
direct, as well as indirect effects on WFC. Specifically, ality variables that have been identified as theoretically and
personality traits are believed to influence cognitions and empirically important antecedents to WFC: negative
perceptions of work and family situations, and thus have affectivity, locus of control, conscientiousness, emotional
the capacity to influence the extent to which one experi- stability, and agreeableness.
ences WFC (e.g., Eby et al. 2005). In addition, personality The two personality traits that have received the most
traits are also theorized to affect behavioral choices, and empirical attention in the WFC literature are negative
these behaviors, in turn, may increase or decrease the levels affectivity (NA) and locus of control. Trait NA is the
of WFC (e.g., Wayne et al. 2004). The examination of tendency to experience negative emotions such as anger,
these potential mediating links between individual differ- guilt, and nervousness, as well as increased levels of sub-
ence variables and WFC has not received adequate atten- jective stress (Watson 1988). These experiences generalize
tion. Thus, this study attempts to fill this gap by testing the across various domains, including work and family.
potential mediating effects of stress coping behaviors Researchers have linked negative affectivity to WFC based
aimed at reducing levels of WIF and FIW on the rela- on the argument that individuals high on NA have a ten-
tionship of personality characteristics with FIW and WIF. dency to assess situations (e.g., home, work) in negative
In the next sections, we provide theoretical rationale for the terms; thus, they are more likely to experience negative
relationships depicted in Fig. 1. emotions towards conditions at work and at home

Fig. 1 Proposed model of the


mediating effect of SOC
strategies on the relationship
between individual differences
and work–family conflict. Note:
Direct effects were estimated
for Negative Affectivity and
Emotional Stability. For figure
simplicity reasons, boxes are
used to group the variables into
relevant categories, and arrows
from a group of variables
indicate that direct effects were
estimated from every variable in
that group to every variable in
the affected group. Also,
variables within each box were
allowed to correlate

123
J Bus Psychol

(Eby et al. 2005). The results of several studies in the problem-focused (e.g., Penley and Tomaka 2002). Emo-
work–family research arena lead to the conclusion that tion-focused coping strategies aim at regulating emotions
individuals high on NA tend to report higher levels of WFC or distress (e.g., humor, denial); while problem-focused
(e.g., Bruck and Allen 2003). strategies aim at management of the problem that is
Locus of control refers to the extent to which individuals causing distress (e.g., working harder, seeking social sup-
feel that outcomes result from internal factors (e.g., skill, port; Folkman and Lazarus 1980). It was reported in a
effort) or from external factors (e.g., chance; Rotter 1966). recent meta-analyses that individuals with high levels of
This individual difference variable has been extensively extroversion and conscientiousness and low on neuroticism
studied in relation to WIF and FIW. Not surprisingly, are more likely to rely on problem-focused strategies, while
studies reported that internal locus of control was related to those high in neuroticism tend to use emotion-focused
lower levels of various types of WFC, even after control- strategies (Connor-Smith and Flachsbart 2007). Another
ling for important demographic variables (e.g., Andreassi study demonstrated that people with external locus of
and Thompson 2007; Noor 2002). control were more likely to report higher use of problem-
Recently, researchers began to link other personality solving strategies than people with internal locus of control
variables to WIF and FIW (e.g., Bruck and Allen 2003; (Strentz and Auerbach 1988).
Grzywacz and Marks 2000). A few studies have consis- Consequently, several researchers have hypothesized
tently linked higher levels of conscientiousness to lower that personality should relate in a similar manner to coping
levels of different types of WIF and FIW (e.g., Bruck and with stressful work and family environments (e.g., Freund
Allen 2003; Kinnunen et al. 2003; Wayne et al. 2004). and Baltes 2002; Wayne et al. 2004). Thus, when faced
Higher levels of emotional stability have also been con- with competing demands in multiple domains (i.e., work
sistently linked to lower levels of WFC (e.g., Bruck and and family), people with certain personality characteristics
Allen 2003; Grzywacz and Marks 2000; Wayne et al. might be more likely to choose effective strategies to cope
2004); although, Kinnunen et al. (2003) did not find a with these competing demands than people with other
significant relationship between these variables. Results personality characteristics, and in turn experience lower
regarding the direction of the relationship between agree- levels of WFC.
ableness and WFC have been mixed. Specifically, two Even though WFC researchers have theorized that the
studies reported agreeableness was negatively related to effects of personality traits on WFC may be mediated by
WIF in particular (Kinnunen et al. 2003; Wayne et al. the choice of coping strategies, these indirect and direct
2004). However, another study found a positive relation- effects have not been tested empirically. Thus, the main
ship with WIF and FIW (Bruck and Allen 2003). goal of this article is to address this important limitation of
Thus, previous research has found preliminary evidence the current literature and test the meditating effects of
for the importance of personality variables in experienced stress coping strategies on the relationship between per-
levels of WIF and FIW. However, as mentioned previously, sonality and WFC. The present study examined the medi-
the mechanisms through which personality characteristics ating effects of stress-coping behavioral strategies outlined
influence WFC has not received adequate attention. The by the Selection, Optimization, and Compensation (SOC)
next section provides an overview of stress-coping strate- model. The SOC model and its corresponding behavioral
gies as possible intervening variables in these relationships. strategies were chosen since prior research (e.g., Baltes and
Heydens-Gahir 2003; Young et al. 2007) has linked their
use to reduced levels of WFC. In the next section, we
Stress Coping Strategies as Intervening Variables provide a more detailed description of the SOC model.

Researchers have theorized that personality traits may affect


an individual’s perceived level of WFC, because these traits The SOC Model
influence other factors related to the WFC construct, such
as time management, energy levels, and choice of coping As mentioned previously, stress-coping researchers have
mechanism (e.g., Wayne et al. 2004; Friede and Ryan 2005). made a distinction between emotion-focused and problem-
In other words, researchers have theorized that personality focused coping strategies (e.g., Folkman and Lazarus
should influence how individuals behave when faced with 1980). Problem-focused strategies involve constructive
the competing demands of work and family roles, and in turn attempts to resolve the source of stress; whereas, emotion-
affect experienced levels of WFC. focused strategies involve regulating emotions that are
Indeed, in the general stress literature, personality experienced as a result of stress.
has been found to differentiately influence the choice The selection, optimization, and compensation (SOC)
of stress coping strategies, both emotion-focused and model is essentially a model of problem-focused coping

123
J Bus Psychol

strategies, as selecting goals and priorities, optimizing These strategies are theorized to work together as an
those goals and priorities, and compensating for losses in ensemble of life-management and problem-focused stress
energy and resources are all focused on reducing stress coping strategies aimed to improve one’s functioning.
through direct actions (Baltes and Baltes 1990; Freund and Thus, an overall SOC score should have the strongest
Baltes 2002; Wiese et al. 2000). The model is based on the relationship with outcome variables. Indeed, these four
assumption that the coordinated use of behaviors can strategies are generally positively correlated (e.g., Freund
(a) increase one’s resources to facilitate developmental and Baltes 2002). Therefore, in this manuscript we will
enhancement, (b) help maintain functioning in the face of focus on the SOC strategies as an ensemble. This model
challenges and limited resources, and (c) help regulate has been used to explain successful adaptation during all
impending losses in resources (Baltes and Baltes 1990; stages of human development and in various contexts (e.g.,
Freund and Baltes 2002). In sum, when faced with limited general, family, career; Baltes and Baltes 1990; Freund and
resources, those who use SOC strategies adapt more suc- Baltes 2002; Wiese et al. 2000). In addition, Baltes and
cessfully. The three general categories of the behaviors Dickson (2001) proposed that the SOC model could pro-
identified in the model are selection (elective and loss- vide a useful framework to explain how some individuals
based), optimization, and compensation. cope with conflicting demands of work and family
Selection (both elective and loss-based) is concerned with domains. In order for SOC behaviors to be plausible
the degree to which people identify and select goals, alter- intervening mechanisms in the relationship between per-
native contexts, outcomes, and goal structures. Behaviors sonality variables and WFC, there should be a relationship
associated with the selection strategy provide a direction to between (1) personality variables and SOC behaviors, and
behavior. As a result, limited resources are allocated in a non- (2) between SOC behaviors and WFC. In the next sections,
random way based on an identified hierarchy of goals. we describe these links. In addition, we develop hypotheses
Elective selection occurs when goal selection is based pri- for the direct influence of some personality variables on
marily on individual preferences or outside guidance (e.g., WFC, as well as the effects of personality variables on
social norms). For example, a career-driven woman may WFC though SOC behaviors.
choose to focus on achieving a certain level of career success
before getting married and/or having children. Loss-based
selection occurs when a loss of some internal (e.g., activity The Present Study
level, hearing) or external (e.g., loss of income) resource
forces individuals to change their goals. An example of this Personality and SOC
strategy would be choosing to focus on close family and not
extended family (e.g., visits, presents, long-distance con- In the general stress coping literature, personality traits
versations) when one of the spouse’s income is lost. have been found to relate to various coping strategies (e.g.,
Optimization involves an examination of the adequacy Penley and Tomaka 2002; Somech and Drach-Zahavy
of existing means, practices, and resources to achieve 2007). For example, neuroticism, extraversion, and NA
goals, as well as necessary improvements of practices and have been found to negatively relate to problem-focused
resources to achieve those goals. General examples of coping. These relationships have been also tested in the
optimization include practicing and learning of new skills, work and family domains. For example, Lee-Baggley et al.
and scheduling of time and energy. For example, a young (2005) reported that higher levels of neuroticism related to
professional may model her supervisors’ successful strat- the use of emotion-focused and not problem-focused
egies for balancing a career and family. strategies in response to interpersonal family stress. Simi-
Compensation strategies are aimed at gaining and larly, Parkes (1986) reported that people low on neuroti-
acquiring alternative means of achieving goals in situations cism tend to report higher levels of problem-focused
when resources are limited or lost or when faced with an strategies when confronted with moderate levels of work
overload situation. Although both optimization and com- demands. Since SOC is a model of problem-focused coping
pensation refer to the application of goal-relevant means, strategies, personality variables that influence problem-
they are different in that optimization refers to achieving focused coping are likely to influence one’s use of SOC
higher level functioning, whereas compensation refers to when dealing with competing work and family demands
counteracting losses. In the family domain, compensation (e.g., Freund and Baltes 2002; Wayne et al. 2004).
may involve hiring a nanny to help care for children at Trait NA has been demonstrated to relate to coping
home, or ordering food from a restaurant for dinner. In the strategies (e.g., Hooker et al. 1994). Studies generally report
work domain, compensation may include hiring an assis- a positive link between NA and emotion-focused coping, as
tant to help with administrative duties, or obtaining a faster well as a negative link between NA and problem-focused
computer. coping (e.g., Butt et al. 2002; Hooker et al. 1994). Pertinent

123
J Bus Psychol

to the current study, individuals with lower levels of NA more opportunities to reach out to their social networks in
also tend to report higher usage of SOC strategies in both both the work and family domains. Indeed, some studies
the work and family domains (Pratt 2006). Thus, we expect have linked agreeableness to the choice of problem-
that individuals high on NA are less likely to report the use focused strategies, such as planning (e.g. Watson and
of SOC behaviors in both domains. Hubbard 1996). In addition, Wayne et al. (2004) found that
agreeableness related positively to facilitation from the
Hypothesis 1 People with higher levels of trait NA report
family domain to the work domain. Surprisingly, two
lower levels of (a) work SOC behaviors and (b) family
studies that examined the relationship between agreeable-
SOC behaviors.
ness and general SOC behaviors did not find it to be sig-
Locus of control influences individuals’ perceptions of nificant at the bivariate level (Freund and Baltes 2002;
the extent to which their efforts and actions (including those Wiese et al. 2000). The study by Freund and Baltes (2002)
aimed at stress reduction) influence the outcome. People included very diverse participants as young as 14 and as
with a belief that their actions can affect outcomes (i.e., old as 87, and they examined the use of general SOC
internal locus of control) are more likely to initiate inter- strategies only; whereas, Wiese et al. (2000) excluded
ventions when faced with stressful situations than those participants with children and did not provide information
with a belief that their actions will not affect the levels of on the relationships between overall SOC strategies and
stress they experience (i.e., external locus of control). personality. Thus, considering that these prior studies did
Indeed, empirical evidence indicates that individuals with not examine the relationship between agreeableness and
internal locus of control are likely to use problem-focused overall SOC behaviors specifically in the work and family
strategies, and people with external locus of control gen- context or with samples that are traditionally used in WFC
erally choose not to use any coping strategies (e.g., Rees research, we believe it necessary to further examine this
and Cooper 1992). Despite the pervasive evidence of the relationship specifically in the context of coping with
relationship between locus of control and coping, Freund WFC. Furthermore, based on the evolutionary perspective,
and Baltes (1998) reported non-significant correlations we propose that highly agreeable individuals are more
between locus of control and general SOC behaviors. likely to use SOC strategies.
However, the sample in the study by Freund and Baltes
Hypothesis 3 People with higher levels of agreeableness
(1998) was comprised specifically of non-working elderly
report higher levels of (a) work SOC behaviors and (b)
people in Germany with ages ranging from 72 to 102; while
family SOC behaviors.
research that has linked internal locus of control to problem-
focused strategies has usually employed more traditional Conscientious individuals tend to be goal-oriented,
samples with participants from diverse demographics. responsible, hardworking, efficient, organized, responsible,
Elderly people face problems and stressors that are different and achievement-striving (McCrae and John 1992). The
from those faced by younger people with family and work definition of a goal-oriented individual implies that he/she
responsibilities. It is also possible that unique factors pre- is more likely to set goals and adapt those goals to the
dict whether elderly people use coping strategies (e.g., circumstances to deal with demands at work and home
Kahana et al. 2005). In addition, Freund and Baltes (1998) (i.e., selection strategies). In addition, conscientious indi-
measured the use of SOC strategies in relation to general viduals have a preference for organization and efficiency,
life management. Thus, considering the strong theoretical and these qualities are part of such strategies as compen-
and empirical connection of locus of control to problems- sation and optimization. Thus, it is not surprising that
focused strategies in the general working population, it is several studies have found that the reported use of SOC
important to examine the relationship between locus of strategies was related to higher levels of conscientiousness
control and the use of SOC strategies in the work and family (Freund and Baltes 2002; Wiese et al. 2000). Accordingly,
domains. Consistent with the theoretical assumptions stated we propose that conscientiousness is positively related to
previously, we expect that individuals with an internal locus the reported use of SOC strategies in both domains.
of control are more likely to utilize SOC strategies.
Hypothesis 4 People with higher levels of conscien-
Hypothesis 2 People with internal locus of control report tiousness report higher levels of a) work SOC behaviors
higher levels of (a) work SOC behaviors and (b) family and b) family SOC behaviors.
SOC behaviors.
McCrae and Costa (1986) argued that emotionally stable
From an evolutionary perspective, agreeable individuals individuals are more suited to deal with depression and
are more capable of creating the social alliances that help stress, including stressful circumstances resulting from
one to adapt and adjust to various situations (Caligiuri WFC. Indeed, out of all the personality factors, emotional
2000). Thus, highly agreeable individuals should have stability has been found to have one of the strongest

123
J Bus Psychol

positive relationships with stress coping (e.g., McCrae and presented in prior articles (e.g., Baltes and Dickson 2001;
Costa 1986). In addition, emotional stability has been Baltes and Heydens-Gahir 2003). Thus, we propose the
found to relate positively to the overall use of SOC strat- following hypotheses:
egies (Freund and Baltes 1998, 2002). However, no studies
Hypothesis 6a People who report higher level of work
have looked at these relationships specifically within the
SOC strategies experience lower levels of WIF.
family and work domains. Considering that emotional
stability has been shown theoretically and empirically to Hypothesis 6b People who report higher level of family
relate to stress coping across various domains, it is essential SOC strategies experience lower levels of FIW.
to include this personality trait in order to understand the
relative importance of various factors in their influences on
the choice of specific behavioral strategies aimed to reduce SOC as a Potential Mediator
WFC. Thus, we propose that emotionally stable individuals
are more likely to report higher levels of all SOC behaviors As mentioned previously, the theoretical link between
in both domains. personality and WFC is twofold. First, researchers have
proposed that some personality traits relate to reduced
Hypothesis 5 People with higher levels of emotional
levels of perceived WFC (e.g., conscientiousness), whereas
stability report higher levels of (a) work SOC behaviors
others relate to increased levels of perceived WFC (e.g.,
and (b) family SOC behaviors.
neuroticism), because these traits influence other factors
related to the WFC construct, such as choice of coping
mechanism (e.g., Freund and Baltes 2002; Wayne et al.
SOC and WFC 2004). In other words, personality traits influence choice of
behavioral coping strategies, and the use of these strategies
The SOC theory specifies that when faced with resource should lead to decreased levels of WFC (Wayne et al.
limitations, those individuals who use SOC strategies are 2004). Consistent with this theorization, we propose that
more likely to deal effectively with these demanding and SOC will mediate the relationship of NA, locus of control,
stressful situations. According to resource drain theory, agreeableness, conscientiousness, and emotional stability
time, attention, and energy are finite (Edwards and Roth- with WIF/FIW. Specifically, we are expecting the effects
bard 2000); thus, when individuals are faced with demands of the individual difference variables on WIF (FIW) to be
in both work and family domains, SOC strategies could channeled through SOC behaviors.
help individuals to more successfully manage the demands
Hypothesis 7a Work SOC strategies mediate the effects
of both work and family (Wiese et al. 2000).
of NA, locus of control, agreeableness, conscientiousness,
Several quantitative studies have demonstrated the link
and emotional stability on WIF.
between SOC strategies, job/family stressors, and WFC.
For example, Baltes and Heydens-Gahir (2003) demon- Hypothesis 7b Family SOC strategies mediate the effects
strated that individuals who use SOC behavioral strategies of NA, locus of control, agreeableness, conscientiousness,
in the work and/or family domain tend to experience lower and emotional stability on FIW.
amounts of job and family stressors, and in turn lower
Second, some personality variables, emotional stability
levels of WIF and FIW conflict. Another study also dem-
and NA in particular, have been theorized and empirically
onstrated that SOC strategies related to lower levels of
demonstrated to have a direct effect on perceptions and
family stressors (e.g., parental demands, marital tensions)
interpretations of work and family circumstances as
and job stressors (e.g., role overload, role ambiguity;
stressful or not (e.g., Brief et al. 1993; Friede and Ryan
Young et al. 2007). Wiese et al. (2000) demonstrated that
2005; Michel and Clark 2009). In other words, individuals
use of SOC strategies at work is associated with positive
with high negative affect and low emotional stability have
psychological outcomes, such as emotional balance and job
a tendency to interpret competing demands of work and
satisfaction, in that domain.
family responsibilities as more stressful than those with
A recent qualitative study (Clark et al. 2009) also pro-
low negative affect and high emotional stability. Thus, we
vided support for the argument that people rely on SOC
expect that these personality variables have direct effects
strategies to deal with work and family stressors. More
on WIF and FIW, even after the use of SOC strategies is
specifically, in semi-structured phone interviews partici-
taken into account.
pants were asked to identify strategies they have used that
were successful or unsuccessful in reducing job/family Hypothesis 8a NA and emotional stability directly relate
stress. The strategies provided by participants were con- to WIF, even after considering the effects of work SOC
sistent with the examples of specific SOC strategies behaviors on WIF.

123
J Bus Psychol

Hypothesis 8b NA and emotional stability directly relate used measures of the locus of control construct (Lefcourt
to FIW, even after considering the effects of work SOC 1991). High scores indicate an external locus of control,
behaviors on FIW. and low scores indicate an internal locus.

Emotional Stability, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness


Method
Thirty items from the IPIP instrument (Goldberg 1999)
Participants were used to measure emotional stability, agreeableness,
and conscientiousness. Participants were instructed to rate
Participants were 289 working adults living in a major how accurately the statements describe them on a 5-point
Midwestern metropolitan area. The sample only included scale from 1 ‘‘very inaccurate’’ to 5 ‘‘very accurate.’’
individuals who worked at least 20 h per week and had a
significant other and/or child. The average age of the SOC
sample was 37.64 (SD = 11.46), the average number of
hours worked was 39.69 (SD = 12.42), 73% were married, In this study, SOC strategies were assessed with the long
and 81% were parents. The sample was 61% females, and version (48 items) of the SOC questionnaire developed by
racial composition was 5% Arabic/Middle Eastern, 10% Baltes et al. (1999).
Asian, 28% Black, 4% Hispanic, 1% Multi-racial, and 47% The authors have intended this measure to be domain-
White. In this study, a snowball sampling technique was general and/or domain-specific (Baltes et al. 1999). This
utilized (Heckathorn 1997). More specifically, an adver- scale has been utilized across variety of domains, including
tisement (with the information about the study and a link to general life, work, career, family, and partnership (Baltes
a survey on a secure internet site) posted at a large urban and Heydens-Gahir 2003; Freund and Baltes 1998, 2002;
Midwestern university notified undergraduate psychology Wiese et al. 2000). Domain specificity of SOC question-
students about an opportunity to earn extra-credit. They naire is traditionally manipulated by changing instructions,
relayed information about the study to their friends, while items are kept identical.
acquaintances, and family members who fit the criteria for In this study, the questionnaire was adapted to work life
participation in the study in exchange for extra- credit. and family life; that is, participants completed the SOC
These recruited individuals contacted the study coordinator questionnaire twice referring to different parts of their lives
and were provided with a link to the on-line survey. The (i.e., work and family). As a result, scores were computed
questionnaires were presented in random order and took separately for family and work. The format of each item in
approximately 40 min to complete. All participants were the SOC measure consists of two response options. The
guaranteed confidentiality. first option reflects a typical SOC behavior (e.g., I con-
centrate my energy on a few things), and the second option
Measures reflects a non-SOC behavior (e.g., I divide my energy
among many things). The participants selected the behavior
Negative Affectivity that is most similar to them in the particular context (i.e.,
work or family). Sample items for each dimension and
Trait negative affectivity was measured with the 60-item domain are provided in Appendix. The items for work and
Positive and Negative Affect Schedule-Expanded Form family domains were presented in random order. Next,
(PANAS-X; Watson and Clark 1994). For the purpose of participants indicated the degree of similarity between
this study only scores on the 30 items assessing negative themselves and the behavior they selected on a 4-point
affectivity (NA) were utilized. Participants were instructed scale ranging from 1 ‘‘a little’’ to 4 ‘‘exactly’’. However,
to ‘‘indicate to what extent you tend to feel this way in the choice of non-SOC behavior resulted in score of 0,
general, that is, on the average’’ on a 5-point scale ranging regardless of the provided degree of similarity. Thus, the
from 1 ‘‘very slightly or not at all’’ to 5 ‘‘extremely’’. participants’ score on a SOC item may range from 0 (non-
SOC behavior selected) to 4 (participant selected SOC
Locus of Control behavior and indicated that the behavior described them
‘‘Exactly’’). To compute overall scores for work SOC and
Locus of control was measured with an abbreviated version family SOC, scores on the four components of SOC in each
(i.e., 11 items) of the Rotter’s I-E Scale short version domain were averaged. Test–retest reliability has been
proposed by Valecha and Ostrom (1974). Rotter’s I-E scale shown to be satisfactory for work SOC and family SOC
and its abbreviated version are among the most commonly (0.70 and 0.80, respectively; Wiese et al. 2000).

123
J Bus Psychol

WIF and FIW Conflict terms of the latent constructs: (1) the error terms of the
exogenous constructs were allowed to correlate; (2) the
WIF and FIW conflict were measured with a scale devel- error terms of the Work and Family SOC constructs were
oped by Carlson et al. (2000) that measured the six fol- allowed to correlate; and (3) the error terms of the WIF and
lowing dimensions: (1) time-based work interference with FIW constructs were allowed to correlate. Good fit was
family, (2) time-based family interference with work, (3) indicated by a non-significant chi-square, an RMSEA of
strain-based work interference with family, (4) strain-based 0.08 or below, and values above 0.90 for the NNFI and CFI
family interference with work, (5) behavior-based work (Hoyle 1995).
interference with family, and (6) behavior-based family
interference with work. Each dimension was measured by
three items (18 items total) with a response scale ranging Results
from 1 ‘‘strongly disagree’’ to 5 ‘‘strongly agree’’. Items for
both domains were presented in random order. Table 1 presents means, standard deviations, reliability
coefficients, and intercorrelations for the study variables.
Control Variables At the bivariate level, all individual difference variables
related significantly and in the hypothesized direction to
Two important variables (hours worked and gender) that reported use of SOC behaviors in both domains. Higher
have been identified as important to consider when exam- levels of reported use of work and family SOC strategies
ining the nomological network surrounding WFC were were related to lower levels of WIF and FIW, with stronger
included as control variables. Research shows that the relationships observed for domain-congruent strategies and
number of hours worked per week is positively related to interference. Also as expected, work and family SOC
experienced levels of WFC (e.g., Baltes and Heydens- behaviors were positively correlated.
Gahir 2003). In addition, hours worked per week can lead A good fit was obtained for the hypothesized model: v2
to an increased need for the use of various strategies (e.g., (6, N = 289) = 4.97, n.s., NNFI = 0.99, CFI = 0.99, and
SOC behaviors) to reduce WFC. In regards to gender, some RMSEA = 0.01. Generally, this model indicates that when
studies have found that women perceive more WFC (e.g., considered simultaneously, individual level variables dif-
Frone et al. 1992), while other studies have reported low/no ferentially predict the use of SOC strategies (see Fig. 2).
direct effect of gender on perceived levels of WFC (e.g., Specifically, hypotheses 1a, 2a, 3a, and 4a were supported,
Somech and Drach-Zahavy 2007). Considering the perva- indicating that locus of control, NA agreeableness, and
sive empirical evidence that gender and hours worked per conscientiousness predicted one’s use of work SOC strat-
week relate to WFC and coping strategies, it is important to egies. Hypotheses 3b, 4b, and 5b were also supported
include these variables as controls. Both variables were indicating that people with higher levels of conscientious-
assessed with single items. ness, agreeableness, and emotional stability reported
greater use of family SOC strategies. Locus of control and
Statistical Analyses NA did not relate to family SOC strategies; thus there was
no support for hypotheses 1b and 2b, respectively. Though
Structural equation modeling and LISREL 8.8 (Jöreskog significant at the bivariate level, when entered into the full
and Sörbom 2004) were used to test the theoretical model model emotional stability was no longer an important
depicted in Fig. 1. Given the large number of variables and predictor of work SOC strategies, providing a lack of
the sample size of the data set, a path analysis framework support for the hypothesis 5a. All together, individual
was used. The model was tested using a partial covariance difference variables explained 24% (i.e., R-square of 0.24)
matrix with variance due to the control variables having of the variance in work SOC and 27% (i.e., R-square of
been removed (Fletcher et al. 2006). While path analysis 0.27) of the variance in family SOC. In addition, the model
was used measurement error was dealt with when feasible. as a whole explained 22% (i.e., R-square of 0.22) of the
Specifically, where it was possible (i.e., when a scale score variance in WIF and 34% (i.e., R-square of 0.34) of the
was used as an indicator) reliability information was used variance in FIW.
for each construct to set measurement parameters. In set- Pathways between work SOC and WIF and family SOC
ting the measurement parameters, the factor loadings were and FIW were significant. Specifically, people who repor-
set equal to the square root of the reliabilities (i.e., coef- ted higher levels of work SOC behaviors experienced lower
ficient alphas), whereas the error variances were set equal levels of WIF, but not FIW. Similarly, higher levels of
to the variance of the measure multiplied by the value reported family SOC related to lower levels of FIW, but
(1 - reliability; Loehlin 1998). With respect to the error nor WIF. Thus, hypotheses 6a and 6b were supported. In

123
J Bus Psychol

Table 1 Means, standard deviations, intercorrelations, and reliability coefficients for the study variables
Variables Means SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 14 15 16 17

1 Locus of control 3.96 2.29 0.63


2 Negative affect 2.11 0.76 0.22 0.96
3 Agreeableness 3.71 0.62 -0.21 -0.40 0.84
4 Conscientiousness 3.48 0.55 -0.27 -0.39 0.43 0.84
5 Emotional stability 3.2 0.65 -0.13 -0.46 0.22 0.23 0.83
6 SOC-W 1.14 0.74 -0.31 -0.36 0.32 0.43 0.15 0.95
7 SOC-F 1.36 0.74 -0.26 -0.35 0.36 0.47 0.24 0.78 0.95
8 WIF 2.63 0.79 0.04 0.27 -0.13 -0.19 -0.32 -0.33 -0.31 0.85
9 FIW 2.35 0.82 0.15 0.43 -0.21 -0.31 -0.27 -0.28 -0.45 0.60 0.86
10 Gender 1.62 0.49 0.05 0.06 0.18 0.03 -0.12 -0.03 -0.06 0.06 -0.02 –
11 Hours worked 39.69 12.42 -0.16 -0.07 -0.08 0.01 0.02 0.16 0.19 0.02 -0.01 -0.36 –
Notes: N = 289. SOC selection, optimization, and compensation, W work, F family, WIF work interfering with family, FIW family interfering
with work; Gender 1 = Male, 2 = Female; High scores on Locus of Control variable indicate an external locus of control.; Correlations [ 0.11
are significant at p \ 0.05, Correlations [ 0.15 are significant at p \ 0.01, Correlations [ 0.17 are significant at p \ 0.001; Reliability coef-
ficients are presented on a diagonal

Fig. 2 Structural model with standardized path coefficients. Notes: For parsimony purposes only significant pathways are shown in a figure. The
bolded arrows indicate significant indirect effects of individual difference variables on WIF and FIW

sum, there were 4 non-significant pathways in the current one was the direct effect of emotional stability on FIW
model that were originally hypothesized to be significant. conflict.
Three of these were effects of personality variables on SOC We then examined the direct effects of the personality
behaviors (i.e., locus of control and negative affectivity and variables on WIF (FIW) and their indirect effects through
family SOC, and emotional stability and work SOC) while SOC to test for potential mediation. Hypotheses 7a and 7b

123
J Bus Psychol

were partially supported: SOC mediated the relationship It should be pointed out that the model was run again
between some individual difference variables and WFC without the non-significant pathways included and all the
and not others. Specifically, agreeableness and conscien- results (in terms of overall fit, pathway estimates, and
tiousness had significant indirect effects on WIF (b = variance explained) were virtually unchanged. However,
-0.04, t = -2.22, p \ 0.01 and, b = -0.07, t = -3.17, since such model modification should be accompanied by
p \ 0.001, respectively), indicating that their influence cross-validation on a new sample, we only present and
on WIF conflict was mediated by SOC behaviors. By discuss results from the original model and analysis.
decomposing the total indirect effects to see what per- As mentioned previously, the SOC strategies are theo-
centage of the indirect effect went through work SOC and rized to work as an ensemble. More specifically, while the
which percentage went through family SOC one saw that dimensions of SOC do work together as an ensemble, and
the majority of both were through work SOC (64% of the thus, the overall score should have the strongest relationship
indirect effect for agreeableness and 67% of the indirect with outcome variables, it is possible the exact nature of the
effect for conscientiousness). Also, consistent with the relationships between each SOC dimension and the out-
results from the work domain, agreeableness and consci- come of interest may differ. Thus, for the exploratory pur-
entiousness (b = -0.06, t = -2.49, p \ 0.01, and b = poses, we have included and examined the bivariate
-0.10 t = -3.60, p \ 0.001, respectively) had significant correlations of specific facets of SOC with WIF and FIW
indirect effects on FIW conflict. In this case the primary (Table 2). Consistent with previous research, facets of SOC
mediating variable was family SOC as indicated by the behaviors in both domains were moderately (work) and
indirect effect decomposition (i.e., 93% of the indirect strongly (family) related to each other at the bivariate level
effect for agreeableness and 92% of the indirect effect for (0.46–0.80); with stronger correlations observed for corre-
conscientiousness). Finally, emotional stability had a sig- sponding strategies across domains (e.g., Freund and Baltes
nificant indirect effect (b = -0.06, t = -2.28, p \ 0.01) 1998; Wiese et al. 2000). All the SOC strategies were found
on FIW. Decomposing the indirect effect indicated that to significantly relate to both WIF and FIW in the expected
99% of this indirect effect was through family SOC. direction. Family optimization and compensation strategies
Hypothesis 8a was supported. Specifically, both NA and had the strongest relationships with FIW, followed by
emotional stability each had a significant direct effect on family selection strategies. Elective selection strategies at
WIF, in such that higher levels of NA and lower levels of work were least effective in reducing WIF and FIW.
emotional stability predicted higher levels of WIF. These
two personality traits did not have indirect effects on WIF,
indicating that their influences on WIF were not mediated Discussion
by work SOC. Hypothesis 8b was partially supported, since
only NA had a significant direct effect on FIW conflict. The general purpose of this study was to extend our
However, NA did not have a significant indirect effect on understanding of the mechanisms through which person-
FIW, indicating that family SOC did not mediate the effects ality variables influence WFC. Researchers have theorized
of NA on FIW. that some personality variables influence the way people

Table 2 Means, standard deviations, intercorrelations, and reliability coefficients for SOC facets, WIF, and FIW
Variables Means SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

1 ES-W 1.18 0.75 0.81


2 LBS-W 1.38 0.84 0.66 0.77
3 O-W 1.63 0.93 0.62 0.75 0.85
4 C-W 1.45 0.89 0.51 0.69 0.76 0.85
5 ES-F 1.11 0.76 0.65 0.56 0.56 0.46 0.83
6 LBS-F 1.35 0.82 0.56 0.75 0.68 0.64 0.62 0.78
7 O-F 1.56 0.93 0.55 0.68 0.80 0.67 0.58 0.74 0.83
8 C-F 1.43 0.88 0.46 0.64 0.67 0.72 0.52 0.71 0.78 0.86
9 WIF 2.63 0.79 -0.19 -0.29 -0.28 -0.29 -0.23 -0.24 -0.29 -0.30 0.85
10 FIW 2.35 0.82 -0.19 -0.31 -0.36 -0.27 -0.32 -0.36 -0.45 -0.40 0.60 0.86
Notes: N = 289. ES elective selection, LBS loss-based selection, O optimization, C compensation, W work, F family, WIF work interfering with
family, FIW family interfering with work; Correlations [ 0.11 are significant at p \ 0.05, Correlations [ 0.15 are significant at p \ 0.01,
Correlations [ 0.17 are significant at p \ 0.001. Reliability coefficients are presented on a diagonal

123
J Bus Psychol

perceive, interpret, and react to different environmental resources that results from participating in two roles (i.e.,
characteristics. As such, people with certain personality work and family) are less likely to emotionally reflect on
traits might perceive participating in two roles (i.e., work this stressful environment and more likely to actively cope
and family) as stressful and as a result experience higher with WFC. Agreeable individuals are characterized by
levels of WFC (Friede and Ryan 2005). Another potential kindness, likeability, trust, friendliness, and helpfulness, as
explanation specifies that personality characteristics influ- well as are more likely to have greater social networks in
ence the choice of behavioral strategies aimed at dealing both work and family that allow them to rely on strategies
with WFC; in turn, these behavioral strategies influence the like optimization and compensation in both domains
amount of WFC people experience. Thus, a specific pur- (McCrae and John 1992). For example, colleagues may be
pose of this study was to assess the mediating role of more likely to offer or agree to help with certain tasks (i.e.,
behavioral strategies aimed at coping with the competing compensation) or share information about a more efficient
demands of work and family roles in the relationship way to accomplish tasks (i.e., optimization) to an agreeable
between individual difference variables and WFC. To test coworker. In a similar manner, a neighbor may be more
this proposition we have examined personality character- likely to agree to watch children for an agreeable
istics that have been identified as important predictors of individual.
WFC, as well as life management coping strategies (i.e., However, it appears that some personality characteris-
SOC) that have been demonstrated to be effective in low- tics, while influencing behaviors to some extent, have a
ering WIF and FIW conflict (Baltes and Heydens-Gahir greater influence on the way people feel and interpret their
2003; Young et al. 2007). environment (e.g., Brief et al. 1993). For example, people
It should first be pointed out that the personality vari- high on NA and low on emotional stability tend to expe-
ables used in the current study were related, for the most rience feelings of anger, distress, subjective stress, and fear
part, with the coping strategies examined. This is not sur- (e.g., Watson 1988). These individuals also tend to per-
prising, but does provide more evidence that personality ceive stressful situations as threatening and difficult. These
variables predict what type of coping strategies an indi- tendencies might potentially explain why these personality
vidual may use. traits had a direct effect on perceptions of WIF.
With respect to the mediation hypothesis, in general the
results provide preliminary evidence that some personality
characteristics indeed influence experienced levels of WFC Limitations and Future Research
through the choice of behavioral coping strategies. For
example, consistent with previous research, conscien- One main limitation of this study is our reliance on cross-
tiousness and agreeableness were significantly related to sectional data to make our conclusions about the relation-
WIF and FIW at the bivariate level. However, when con- ships between personality traits, SOC behaviors, and WFC.
sidered in conjunction with our proposed mediators, these With the exception of a study by Wiese et al. (2000) that
personality characteristics no longer had direct effects on examined the ability of SOC measures to predict family,
FIW and WIF conflict; rather, the effects of conscien- career, and general life functioning using longitudinal
tiousness and agreeableness on WFC were mediated analysis, the majority of studies using the SOC model have
through SOC strategies. relied on cross-sectional data. Thus, future research should
In the work domain, negative affect and emotional sta- examine how personality traits influence both coping
bility had significant direct effects on experienced levels of behaviors as well as perceptions relating to WFC using
WIF conflict, even in the presence of mediators. While longitudinal data.
negative affect related to the choice of SOC strategies, Although this study provides preliminary evidence of
there was no indirect effect on WIF conflict. These results the mediating effect of behavioral coping strategies on the
are consistent with recent research that has found trait NA relationship between personality variables and WFC, future
to have a direct effect on such outcomes as perceptions of research may want to consider how other factors (e.g.,
WFC and job satisfaction because of general tendency for environmental characteristics) influence these relation-
high trait NA individuals to perceive demanding circum- ships. There has been a call for identifying meaningful
stances as stressful (Michel and Clark 2009). situational influences on the relationships between per-
Thus, it appears that different processes underlie the sonality and work outcomes (e.g., Stewart and Barrick
influence of specific personality characteristics on WFC. 2004). For example, under certain environmental condi-
Conscientiousness has been found to strongly relate to the tions, the influence of personality traits on perceptions and
outcomes that require action, such as job and academic behavioral choices can be stronger or weaker (Pfeffer
performance (e.g., Barrick and Mount 1991). Thus, it is 1998). It is possible that in situations with low resources,
possible that these individuals, when faced with a loss of personality becomes a stronger predictor of stress-coping

123
J Bus Psychol

strategies, and the relationship between stress coping the extent to which personality interacts with occupation,
strategies and experienced levels of WFC is stronger as as well as student status, in predicting use of SOC behav-
well. Thus, future studies should examine the relationships iors as well as levels of perceived WFC. Similarly, future
between personality, SOC strategies, and WFC under cir- studies should examine to what extent specific subgroups,
cumstances with different levels of demands on and/or such as single parents or married couples with grown
supplies of resources. children, experience different pressures, and thus, poten-
A further important direction for future research tially experience the meditational process differently.
involves examination of the role of work and family
stressors in these relationships. As it has been mentioned
previously, personality may influence one’s appraisal of General Conclusions
whether or not certain circumstances are stressful or not.
According to this proposition, when confronted with spe- This study makes an important contribution to the WFC
cific job (family) stressors some individuals would interpret literature by examining the mediating effects of behavioral
these stressors as detrimental to their well-being (i.e., stress-coping strategies on the link between personality
experience stress and WFC), while others would not characteristics and WFC. Results indicate that conscien-
interpret these stressors as detrimental (e.g., Wayne et al. tiousness and agreeableness are related to greater usage of
2004). These differences in the interpretation and appraisal work and family behavioral coping strategies, and that
of stressors might influence the choice and effectiveness of these behavioral strategies influence levels of experienced
coping strategies. Thus, future studies should include WFC. Negative affect was found to have direct effects on
measures of relevant job/family stressors in order to WIF and FIW, and emotional stability was found to have a
examine the interactive effects of personality and job/ direct effect on WIF conflict.
family stressors on choice and effectiveness of SOC Understanding how individual factors influence SOC
behaviors. strategies and WFC has important practical implications.
Another limitation of the present study was that partic- This study has implications for the effectiveness of
ipants were not asked to provide any information about training programs and interventions aimed at reducing
their job or occupations. Researchers have suggested that WFC levels of employees. When creating these programs,
occupations partially constrain how individuals manage trainers and managers should take into account the strong
their work and family roles (e.g., Kossek et al. 2005). influence of individual factors on a person’s choice of
Indeed, Dierdorff and Ellington (2008) found that occu- coping strategies. For example, trainers and managers
pation accounted for a significant proportion of the vari- could focus additional attention on helping individuals
ance in reported levels of WFC, and that individuals with high levels of negative affectivity and low levels of
working in occupations requiring a great deal of interde- emotional stability to manage competing demands of work
pendence and responsibility for others reported greater and family roles.
WFC. We also were not able to assess what percentage of
the sample were full- or part-time students in addition to
working. Being a student might introduce additional Appendix
competing demands, and thus potentially affect the choice
of coping strategies. Thus, future research should examine See Table 3.

Table 3 Sample items from the SOC questionnaire


Target behavior Alternative/distracter behavior

Elective selection
I concentrate my efforts on a few things I divide my energy among many things
Loss-based selection
When things don’t go as well as they used to, I keep trying When things don’t go as well as they used to, I accept it
other ways until I can achieve the same result I used to
Compensation
When something in my life isn’t working as well When something in my life isn’t working as well as it used to, I decide
as it used to, I ask others for advice what to do about it myself, without involving other people
Optimization
I keep working on what I have planned until I succeed When I do not succeed right away at what I want to do, I don’t try other
possibilities for very long

123
J Bus Psychol

References Fletcher, T. D., Selgrade, K. A., & Germano, L. M. (2006, May). On


the use of partial covariances in structural equation modeling.
Andreassi, J. K., & Thompson, C. A. (2007). Dispositional and Paper presented at the 21st Annual Conference of the Society for
situational sources of control: Relative impact on work–family Industrial and Organizational Psychology, Dallas, TX.
conflict and positive spillover. Journal of Managerial Psychol- Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1980). An analysis of coping in a
ogy, 22, 722–740. doi:10.1108/02683940710837697. middle-aged community sample. Journal of Health and Social
Baltes, P. B., & Baltes, M. M. (Eds.). (1990). Successful aging: Behavior, 21, 219–239.
Perspectives from the behavioral sciences. Cambridge, MA: Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (1998). Selection, optimization and
Cambridge University Press. compensation as strategies of life management: Correlations
Baltes, P. B., Baltes, M. M., Freund, A. M., & Lang, F. R. (1999). The with subjective indications of successful aging. Psychology and
measurement of selection, optimization, and compensation Aging, 13, 531–543. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.13.4.531.
(SOC) by self-report: Technical report 1999. Berlin: Max Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2002). Life-management strategies of
Planck Institute for Human Development. selection, optimization, and compensation: Measurement by self-
Baltes, B. B., & Dickson, M. W. (2001). Using life-span models report and construct validity. Journal of Personality and Social
in industrial-organizational psychology: The theory of selective Psychology, 82, 642–662. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.82.4.642.
optimization with compensation. Applied Developmental Friede, A., & Ryan, A. M. (2005). The importance of the individual:
Science, 5, 51–63. doi:10.1207/S1532480XADS0501_5. How self-evaluations influence the work–family interface. In E.
Baltes, B. B., & Heydens-Gahir, H. (2003). Reduction of work–family E. Kossek & S. Lambert (Eds.), Work and life integration:
conflict through the use of selection, optimization, and compen- Organizational, cultural, and individual perspectives. New
sation behaviors. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 1005–1018. Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
doi:10.1037/0021-9010.88.6.1005. Frone, M. R., Russell, M., & Cooper, M. L. (1992). Antecedents and
Barrick, M. R., & Mount, M. K. (1991). The Big Five personality outcomes of work–family conflict: Testing a model of the work–
dimensions and job performance: A meta-analysis. Personnel family interface. Journal of Applied Psychology, 77, 65–78. doi:
Psychology, 41, 1–26. doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.1991.tb00688.x. 10.1037/0021-9010.77.1.65.
Brief, A. P., Butcher, A. H., George, J. M., & Link, K. E. (1993). Frone, M. R., Russell, M., & Cooper, M. L. (1997). Relation of work–
Integrating top-down and bottom-up theories of subjective well- family conflict to health outcomes: A four-year longitudinal
being: The case of health. Journal of Personality and Social study of employed parents. Journal of Occupational and
Psychology, 64, 646–653. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.64.4.646. Organizational Psychology, 70, 325–335.
Bruck, C. S., & Allen, T. D. (2003). The relationship between big five Goldberg, L. R. (1999). A broad-bandwidth, public-domain, person-
personality traits, negative affectivity, type A behavior, and ality inventory measuring the lower-level facets of several five-
work–family conflict. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 63, factor models. In I. Mervielde, I. Deary, F. De Fruyt, & F.
457–472. doi:10.1016/S0001-8791(02)00040-4. Ostendorf (Eds.), Personality psychology in Europe (Vol. 7,
Butt, Z. A., Strauss, M. E., Smyth, K. A., & Rose-Rego, S. A. (2002). pp. 7–28). Tilburg, The Netherlands: Tilburg University Press.
Negative affectivity and emotion-focused coping in spouse care- Greenhaus, J. H., Allen, T. D., & Spector, P. E. (2006). Health
givers of persons with Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Applied consequences of work–family conflict: The dark side of the
Gerontology, 21, 471–483. doi:10.1177/073346402237633. work–family interface. In P. L. Perrewe & D. C. Ganster (Eds.),
Caligiuri, P. M. (2000). The big five personality characteristics as Research in occupational stress and well-being (Vol. 5,
predictors of expatriate’s desire to terminate the assignment and pp. 61–98). Amsterdam: JAI Press/Elsevier.
supervisor-rated performance. Personnel Psychology, 53, 67–88. Grzywacz, J. G., & Marks, N. F. (2000). Reconceptualizing the work–
doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.2000.tb00194.x. family interface: An ecological perspective on the correlates of
Carlson, D. S., Kacmar, K. M., & Williams, L. J. (2000). Construction positive and negative spillover between work and family.
and initial validation of a multidimensional measure of work– Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 5, 111–126. doi:
family conflict. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 56, 249–276. 10.1037/1076-8998.5.1.111.
doi:10.1006/jvbe.1999.1713. Heckathorn, D. D. (1997). Respondent-driven sampling: A new
Clark, M. A., Bal, A., Zhdanova, L., & Baltes, B. B. (2009, April). A approach to the study of hidden populations. Social Problems,
qualitative analysis of strategies for coping with work–family 44, 174–199. doi:10.1525/sp.1997.44.2.03x0221m.
stressors. Interactive poster presented at the annual meeting of Hooker, K., Frazier, L. D., & Monahan, D. J. (1994). Personality and
the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, New coping among caregivers of spouses with dementia. The
Orleans, LA. Gerontologist, 34, 386–392.
Connor-Smith, J. K., & Flachsbart, C. S. (2007). Relations between Hoyle, R. H. (1995). Structural equation modeling. Thousand Oaks,
personality and coping: A metaanalysis. Journal of Personality CA: Sage Publications.
and Social Psychology, 6, 1080–1107. doi:10.1037/0022-3514. James, L. R., & Mazerolle, M. D. (2002). Personality in work
93.6.1080. organizations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Dierdorff, E. C., & Ellington, J. K. (2008). It’s the nature of the work: Jöreskog, K., & Sörbom, D. (2004). LISREL 8.8: User’s reference
Examining behavior-based sources of work–family conflict guide. Chicago: Scientific Software International.
across occupations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, Kahana, E., Kahana, B., & Zhang, J. (2005). Motivational antecedents
883–892. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.93.4.883. of preventive proactivity in late life: Linking future orientation
Eby, L. T., Casper, W. J., Lockwood, A., Bordeaux, C., & Brinley, A. and exercise. Motivation & Emotion, 29, 438–459. doi:10.1007/
(2005). Work and family research in IO/OB: Content analysis s11031-006-9012-2.
and review of the literature (1980–2002). Journal of Vocational Kinnunen, U., Vermulst, A., Gerris, J., & Makikangas, A. (2003). Work–
Behavior, 66, 124–197. doi:10.1016/j.jvb.2003.11.003. family conflict and its relation to well-being: The role of personality
Edwards, J. R., & Rothbard, N. P. (2000). Mechanisms linking work as a moderating factor. Personality and Individual Differences, 35,
and family: Clarifying the relationship between work and family 1669–1683. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00389-6.
constructs. Academy of Management Review, 25, 178–199. doi: Kossek, E. E., Lautsch, B. A., & Eaton, S. C. (2005). Flexibility
10.2307/259269. enactment theory: Implications of flexibility types, control and

123
J Bus Psychol

boundary management for work–family effectiveness. In E. Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus
E. Kossek & S. J. Lambert (Eds.), Work and life integration: external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs,
Organizational, cultural, and individual perspectives. Mahwah, 80, 1–28.
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Somech, A., & Drach-Zahavy, A. (2007). Strategies for coping with
Lee-Baggley, D., Preece, M., & DeLongis, A. (2005). Individual work–family conflict: The distinctive relationships of gender role
differences in coping across time: Role of the Big 5 Personality ideology. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 12, 1–19.
Dimensions. Journal of Personality, 73, 1141–1180. doi:10.1111/ doi:10.1037/1076-8998.12.1.1.
j.1467-6494.2005.00345.x. Stewart, G. L., & Barrick, M. R. (2004). Four lessons learned from
Lefcourt, H. M. (1991). Locus of control. In J. P. Robinson, the person-situation debate: A review and research agenda. In
P. R. Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of D. B. Smith & B. Schneider (Eds.), Personality and organiza-
personality and social psychological attitudes (Vol. 1, tions. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
pp. 413–499). San Diego, CA: Academic Press, Inc. Strentz, T., & Auerbach, S. M. (1988). Adjustment to the stress of
Loehlin, J. C. (1998). Latent variable models: An introduction to simulated captivity: Effects of emotion-focused vs. problem-
factor, path, and structural analysis. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. focused preparation on hostages differing in locus of control.
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1986). Personality, coping, and coping Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 652–660. doi:
effectiveness in an adult sample. Journal of Personality, 54, 10.1037/0022-3514.55.4.652.
385–405. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1986.tb00401.x. Valecha, G. K., & Ostrom, T. M. (1974). An abbreviated measure of
McCrae, R. R., & John, O. (1992). An introduction to the five-factor internal–external locus of control. Journal of Personality
model and its applications. Journal of Personality, 60, 175–215. Assessment, 38, 369–376.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1992.tb00970.x. Watson, D. (1988). Intraindividual and interindividual analyses of
Michel, J. S., & Clark, M. A. (2009). Has it been affect all along? A positive and negative affect: Their relation to health complaints,
test of work-to-family and family-to-work models of conflict, perceived stress, and daily activities. Journal of Personality and
enrichment, and satisfaction. Personality and Individual Differ- Social Psychology, 54(6), 1020–1030. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.
ences, 47, 163–168. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.02.015. 54.6.1020.
Noor, N. M. (2002). Work–family conflict, locus of control, and women’s Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1994). The PANAS-X: Manual for the
well-being: Tests of alternative pathways. Journal of Social positive and negative affect schedule-expanded form. Cedar
Psychology, 142, 645–662. doi:10.1080/00224540209603924. Rapids: University of Iowa.
Parkes, K. R. (1986). Coping in stressful episodes: The role of Watson, D., & Hubbard, B. (1996). Adaptational style and disposi-
individual differences, environmental factors and situational tional structure: Coping in the context of the five factor model.
characteristics. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Journal of Personality, 64, 735–774. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.
51, 1277–1292. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1277. 1996.tb00943.x.
Pratt, A. K. (2006). Role of cognitive ability and personality factors in Wayne, J. H., Musisca, N., & Fleeson, W. (2004). Considering the
coping with work-family conflict. Doctoral dissertation, Wayne role of personality in the work–family experience: Relationships
State University, 2006. Dissertation Abstracts International, of the big five to work–family conflict and facilitation. Journal of
AAT 3211006. Vocational Behavior, 64, 108–130. doi:10.1016/S0001-8791
Penley, J. A., & Tomaka, J. (2002). Associations among the Big Five, (03)00035-6.
emotional responses, and coping with acute stress. Personality Wiese, B. S., Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2000). Selection,
and Individual Differences, 32, 1215–1228. doi:10.1016/S0191- optimization, and compensation: An action-related approach to
8869(01)00087-3. work and partnership. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 57,
Pfeffer, J. (1998). Understanding organizations: Concepts and con- 273–300. doi:10.1006/jvbe.2000.1752.
troversies. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The Young, L. M., Baltes, B. B., & Pratt, A. K. (2007). Using selection,
handbook of social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 733–777). New optimization, and compensation to reduce job/family stressors:
York: McGraw-Hill. Effective when it matters. Journal of Business and Psychology,
Rees, D. W., & Cooper, C. L. (1992). The occupational stress 21(4), 511–540. doi:10.1007/s10869-007-9039-8.
indicator locus of control scale: Should this be regarded as a state
rather than trait measure? Work and Stress, 6, 45–48. doi:
10.1080/02678379208257038.

123

Você também pode gostar