Você está na página 1de 8

Making Sense of Number Sense:

Implications for Children With Mathematical


Disabilities

Daniel B. Berch

Abstract
Drawing on various approaches to the study of mathematics learning, Gersten, Jordan, and Flojo (in this issue) explore the implications
of this research for identifying children at risk for developing mathematical disabilities. One of the key topics Gersten et al. consider in
their review is that of “number sense.” I expand on their preliminary effort by examining in detail the diverse set of components pur-
ported to be encompassed by this construct. My analysis reveals some major differences between the ways in which number sense is
defined in the mathematical cognition literature and its definition in the literature in mathematics education. I also present recent em-
pirical evidence and theoretical perspectives bearing on the importance of measuring the speed of making magnitude comparisons.
Finally, I discuss how differing conceptions of number sense inform the issue of whether and to what extent it may be teachable.

G
ersten, Jordan, and Flojo (in dren at risk for developing MD. Not- the following reports: Case & Sowder,
this issue) judiciously review withstanding Gersten et al.’s valuable 1990; Dantzig, 1954; Dehaene, 1997,
and appraise relevant empiri- contributions in this regard, several crit- 2001a; Fennell & Landis, 1994; Gersten
cal evidence and theoretical perspec- ical issues concerning the construct of & Chard, 1999; Howden, 1989; Kalch-
tives pertaining to issues of both the number sense remain unresolved and, man, Moss, & Case, 2001; Markovits &
early identification of mathematical therefore, merit a more detailed exam- Sowder, 1994; McIntosh, Reys, & Reys,
disabilities (MD) and their remedia- ination. Such an analysis seems timely, 1992, 1997; McIntosh, Reys, Reys,
tion. Among other topics they cover in given that half a century has passed Bana, & Farrell, 1997; Menon, 2004; Na-
their incisive treatment of several per- since the concept of number sense was tional Council of Teachers of Mathe-
tinent literatures, Gersten et al. exam- first delineated (Dantzig, 1954). matics [NCTM], 2000; Number Sense
ine the construct of number sense. They Research Group, 1995; Okamoto &
begin by defining it conceptually and Case, 1996a; B. J. Reys, 1994; R. Reys et
describing a strong research tradition What Are the Components al., 1999; Sowder, 1992; Verschaffel &
and theoretical foundation for inves- of Number Sense? De Corte, 1996; Yang, Hsu, & Huang, in
tigating its early development. Then, press; Zanzali & Ghazali, 1999).
building on previous efforts to opera- Gersten et al. (in this issue) point out Inspection of Figure 1 reveals that
tionalize this construct, they present that no two researchers define number number sense reputedly constitutes
new evidence indicating that measures sense in exactly the same way. What an awareness, intuition, recognition,
derived from this approach appear to makes this situation even more prob- knowledge, skill, ability, desire, feel,
be predictive of MD in kindergartners. lematic, however, is that cognitive sci- expectation, process, conceptual struc-
Gersten et al. conclude this section by entists and math educators define the ture, or mental number line. Possess-
discussing past research on both for- concept of number sense in very differ- ing number sense ostensibly permits
mal and informal instructional inter- ent ways. After perusing the relevant one to achieve everything from under-
ventions for improving number sense. literature in the domains of mathemat- standing the meaning of numbers to
Both their thoughtful review of this ical cognition, cognitive development, developing strategies for solving com-
topic and the interesting original data and mathematics education, I com- plex math problems; from making
they provide represent an important piled a list of presumed features of simple magnitude comparisons to in-
first step toward the design of valid number sense, shown in Figure 1. venting procedures for conducting nu-
screening measures for identifying chil- (These components were drawn from merical operations; and from recogniz-

JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIES


VOLUME 38, NUMBER 4, JULY/AUGUST 2005, PAGES 333–339
334 JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIES

ceptual” sense of quantity and a


1. A faculty permitting the recognition that something has changed in a “higher order” depiction as an ac-
small collection when, without direct knowledge, an object has been re- quired “conceptual sense-making” of
moved or added to the collection (Dantzig, 1954). mathematics. The former view limits
2. Elementary abilities or intuitions about numbers and arithmetic. the features of number sense to ele-
3. Ability to approximate or estimate. mentary intuitions about quantity, in-
4. Ability to make numerical magnitude comparisons. cluding the rapid and accurate percep-
5. Ability to decompose numbers naturally. tion of small numerosities and the
6. Ability to develop useful strategies to solve complex problems.
ability to compare numerical magni-
7. Ability to use the relationships among arithmetic operations to under-
tudes, to count, and to comprehend
stand the base-10 number system.
8. Ability to use numbers and quantitative methods to communicate, simple arithmetic operations (Dehaene,
process, and interpret information. 1997, 2001a; Geary, 1995). Although
9. Awareness of various levels of accuracy and sensitivity for the reason- these foundational components are in-
ableness of calculations. corporated in the higher order per-
10. A desire to make sense of numerical situations by looking for links be- spective as well, number sense from
tween new information and previously acquired knowledge. this standpoint is considered to be
11. Possessing knowledge of the effects of operations on numbers. much more complex and multifaceted
12. Possessing fluency and flexibility with numbers. in nature; it comprises a deep under-
13. Can understand number meanings. standing of mathematical principles
14. Can understand multiple relationships among numbers.
and relationships, a high degree of
15. Can recognize benchmark numbers and number patterns.
fluency and flexibility with operations
16. Can recognize gross numerical errors.
17. Can understand and use equivalent forms and representations of num- and procedures, a recognition of and
bers as well as equivalent expressions. appreciation for the consistency and
18. Can understand numbers as referents to measure things in the real regularity of mathematics, and a ma-
world. ture facility in working with numerical
19. Can move seamlessly between the real world of quantities and the math- expressions—all of which develop as a
ematical world of numbers and numerical expressions. byproduct of learning through a wide
20. Can invent procedures for conducting numerical operations. array of mathematics education activi-
21. Can represent the same number in multiple ways depending on the con- ties (Greeno, 1991; Verschaffel & De
text and purpose of the representation. Corte, 1996; see Note 1).
22. Can think or talk in a sensible way about the general properties of a nu-
Stanislas Dehaene (2001b), a cog-
merical problem or expression—without doing any precise computation.
nitive neuroscientist and author of The
23. Engenders an expectation that numbers are useful and that mathematics
has a certain regularity. Number Sense (Dehaene, 1997), con-
24. A non-algorithmic feel for numbers. tended that if only for the sake of econ-
25. A well-organized conceptual network that enables a person to relate omy, a single number sense (i.e., ana-
number and operation. log representation of quantity) should
26. A conceptual structure that relies on many links among mathematical re- be hypothesized, rather than a patch-
lationships, mathematical principles, and mathematical procedures. work of representations and abilities.
27. A mental number line on which analog representations of numerical However, he also suggested that this
quantities can be manipulated. core representation becomes connected
28. A nonverbal, evolutionarily ancient, innate capacity to process approxi- to other cognitive systems as a conse-
mate numerosities.
quence of both development and edu-
29. A skill or kind of knowledge about numbers rather than an intrinsic
cation. “What is at stake is to what
process.
30. A process that develops and matures with experience and knowledge. extent the initial quantity system is al-
tered through these interactions; and
to what extent the adult concept of
FIGURE 1. Alleged components of number sense.
number is based solely on the quantity
system as opposed to an integration
of multiple senses of numbers” (De-
ing gross numerical errors to using others regard it as an acquired skill set haene, 2001b, p. 89).
quantitative methods for communicat- that develops with experience. Although Dehaene’s position
ing, processing, and interpreting infor- The major disparity here is be- seems eminently reasonable, at least to
mation. With respect to its origins, tween what might be considered a the present author, it is unlikely that
some consider number sense to be part “lower order” characterization of num- math educators would embrace this
of our genetic endowment, whereas ber sense as a biologically based “per- view, given that the more expansive
VOLUME 38, NUMBER 4, JULY/AUGUST 2005 335

conception of number sense they es- dren with MD suggests that it may be is, the greater the numerical difference
pouse is already entrenched in various crucial to measure the speed of execut- between the two-digit numbers being
forms, for example (a) as one of the five ing such quantity discriminations, as compared, the shorter the time re-
content standards of the National Coun- this variable can reveal subtle yet im- quired to judge which number is larger
cil of Teachers of Mathematics Princi- portant differences in numerical infor- (see Notes 3 and 4). For example,
ples and Standards for Mathematics (2000); mation processing that may not be adults respond more quickly when
(b) in contemporary mathematics text- tapped by assessing accuracy alone. judging whether 57 is larger than 42
books; and (c) as a distinct set of test For example, by measuring response than when deciding whether 61 is
items included in the mathematics por- times on individual magnitude com- larger than 59. This result is counterin-
tions of the National Assessment parison trials composed of single-digit tuitive, as the size of the unit digits is
of Educational Progress (NAEP), the pairs, Landerl, Bevan, and Butterworth irrelevant when the decade digits dif-
Trends in International Mathematics (2004) found that 8- and 9-year-old fer in magnitude. Furthermore, the
and Science Study (TIMSS), and the children with dyscalculia were signifi- finding of a distance effect was taken to
Program for International Student As- cantly slower than controls for correct indicate that the decade and unit dig-
sessment (PISA). Nonetheless, the pres- responses, despite a lack of difference its are mapped holistically on the men-
ent analysis can, I hope, inform the in error rates (but the groups did not tal number line rather than separately.
effort recommended by Gersten at al. differ in their speed of making physi- However, Nuerk, Weger, and Willmes
(in this issue) to devise “more refined, cal size comparisons). Similarly, Pas- (2001) subsequently showed that in-
better operationalized definitions of solunghi and Siegel (2004) discovered compatible number pairs, in which the
number sense” to make further prog- that whereas fifth-grade children with unit and decade digits lead to different
ress in developing valid screening MD were no less accurate in comparing “decisions” (e.g., 47 and 62) were re-
measures. That being said, one must the magnitudes of one- to four-digit sponded to more slowly than compat-
bear in mind that the utility of opera- numbers than their typically achieving ible number pairs (e.g., 42 and 57). This
tional definitions will in part be deter- controls, they were significantly slower so-called unit–decade compatibility effect
mined by the clarity and soundness of overall in carrying out these compar- suggests that at least in adults, two-
the theoretical conceptualizations from isons for a set of 16 number pairs. digit numbers are processed separately
which they are derived. Indeed, Ger- As the vast majority of item pairs and in parallel rather than holistically.
sten et al. have proceeded in just such for the Passolunghi and Siegel (2004) By examining response times for
a manner by basing their screening study were composed of two- to four- similar types of two-digit number
measures on the models of both Geary digit numbers, it is likely that further comparisons in second to fifth grad-
(1993, 2004) and Case and colleagues investigation of numerical comparisons ers, Nuerk, Kaufmann, Zoppoth, and
(Kalchman et al., 2001; Okamoto & for multidigit numbers may prove use- Willmes (2004) recently demonstrated
Case, 1996; see Note 2). ful. Indeed, as Geary and Hoard (2002) that independent processing of the
have pointed out, although a good deal decade and unit digits begins around
is known about the magnitude com- the second grade. Moreover, their re-
Why Are Timed Tests of
parison (and other numerical compre- sults suggested that this strategy of de-
Number Sense Important? hension) skills of children with MD for composing the decade and unit digits
In discussing critical next steps for de- single-digit numbers, relatively little is develops from a sequential (left to
veloping screening measures of num- known about such abilities with re- right) processing mode to a more par-
ber sense in general and numerical spect to more complex numerals. Some allel processing mode. Nuerk et al.
magnitude comparisons in particular, recent research examining the speed contended that what develops with
Gersten et al. (in this issue) suggest with which typically achieving chil- age, then, is not the kind or number of
that future research be aimed at resolv- dren of various ages make magnitude representations, but the way in which
ing whether measures administered to comparisons with two-digit numbers children access and integrate them.
kindergartners and first graders should has yielded an intriguing hypothesis Of particular importance with re-
be timed or untimed. This recommen- about a source of deficits in the multi- spect to the present analysis are the im-
dation was based on the reputed lack digit number judgments of children plications of these findings for children
of evidence to indicate that the speed with MD. who experience MD. Namely, Nuerk
of making magnitude comparisons is Before discussing this study, it is et al. hypothesized that the develop-
an important variable to measure, at necessary to briefly review the results ment of rapid, quasi-parallel digit inte-
least with respect to its validity for pre- of prior, related work with adults. gration when working with multidigit
dicting mathematical proficiency over First, when making magnitude com- Arabic numbers may be deficient in
the short term. parisons with two-digit numbers, adults children with MD. As they pointed out,
Interestingly, however, recent evi- exhibit a so-called distance effect (De- if digit integration develops more
dence from studies of school-age chil- haene, Dupoux, & Mehler, 1990); that slowly or is still predominantly se-
336 JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIES

quential in such children, then the cal- under nondisadvantaged rearing con- sentation is as yet unresolved (Siegler
culation of multidigit numbers would ditions. & Booth, 2004). Nonetheless, there is a
demand considerably more working Furthermore, according to Geary growing body of evidence that this sys-
memory and attentional resources than (1995), the neurocognitive systems tem putatively evolved to handle only
if the digits are integrated in a rapid, supporting these elementary numeri- approximate numerical judgments, not
quasi-parallel fashion. cal abilities include what has been re- exact ones (Dehaene, 1997, 2001a; De-
It is interesting to note that both of ferred to as skeletal principles (Gelman, haene, Spelke, Pinel, Stanescu, & Tsiv-
the two-digit magnitude comparison 1990; Gelman & Meck, 1992), because kin, 1999; Pica, Lemer, Izard, & De-
items from the Number Knowledge Test they provide just the foundational haene, 2004).
(Okamoto & Case, 1996) shown in structure for the acquisition of these As Geary (in press) pointed out,
Table 1 of Gersten et al. (in this issue; abilities. Concomitantly, engaging in “the learning of specific quantities be-
i.e., “Which is bigger: 69 or 71?” and numerical kinds of games and activi- yond four and mapping number
“Which is smaller: 27 or 32?”) consti- ties is thought to “flesh out” these prin- words onto the representations of
tute incompatible number pairs. Based ciples (Geary, 1995). Consistent with these quantities is a difficult task, be-
on the findings of Nuerk et al. (2004), it this perspective, the effective use of cause the analog magnitude system
is possible that using only incompati- board games with children with low that must be adapted for this purpose
ble pairs may inadvertently yield evi- socioeconomic status (Griffin, Case, & functions to represent general amounts,
dence of even greater processing diffi- Siegler, 1994) mentioned by Gersten not specific quantities (Gallistel & Gel-
culties for children with MD than if et al. (in this issue) aptly leads them to man, 1992).” The difficulties associated
compatible pairs (e.g., 65 and 78) were the conclusion that both formal and with this mapping process can be es-
also included, at least from the second informal instruction can enhance num- pecially challenging for children with
grade on. Unfortunately, we do not as ber sense development prior to enter- MD who also experience reading diffi-
yet know whether a unit–decade com- ing school. The interested reader should culties. For example, Hanich, Jordan,
patibility effect would emerge in kin- consult Siegler and Booth (2004) for a Kaplan, and Dick (2001) found that
dergartners or first graders, even with thoughtful delineation of the multiple children with comorbid MD and read-
respect to error rates. Consequently, factors inherent in such games that are ing difficulties performed lower on a
further study of this issue is likely to ideal for enhancing the construction of measure of exact arithmetic than chil-
prove informative both for theory and a linear mental representation of nu- dren with MD only, although these
for early identification. merical magnitude, as well as for a va- groups did not differ on a test of ap-
riety of practical suggestions regarding proximate arithmetic. As Gersten et al.
other instructional strategies that can (in this issue) correctly note, different
Can Number Sense be used at home or at school. interventions may be required for ame-
Be Taught? Gersten et al. (in this issue) also liorating the difficulties experienced in
suggest that one goal of early interven- these two types of MD.
Gersten et al. (in this issue) claim that tion is to enhance the ability of children What are the pedagogical impli-
if number sense is viewed as a skill or to use a mental number line. That be- cations of viewing number sense as a
a kind of knowledge rather than an ing said, the most judicious approach much more complex and multifaceted
“intrinsic” process (Robinson et al., to selecting an intervention would be construct than “simply” possessing
2002), it should teachable. However, as to base it on one’s model of the mental elementary intuitions about quantity?
some theorists have claimed that num- number line, as Gersten et al. have Among other recommendations ema-
ber sense is rooted in our biological done with respect to Case’s central nating from this perspective, it has
makeup, what are the implications of conceptual structures model. How do been argued that number sense “can-
this perspective for “teaching” it or at these various models differ, and what not be compartmentalized into special
least fostering its development? Con- kinds of instructional interventions textbook chapters or instructional units”
trary to a strict nativist position, most can be derived from them? Given (Verschaffel & De Corte, 1996, p. 109)
theorists who adhere to the view that space limitations, only a brief account and that its development does not re-
number sense has a long evolutionary of these issues can be given here. Basi- sult from a selected subset of activities
history and a specialized cerebral sub- cally, there is general agreement among designed specifically for this purpose
strate do not judge that it thereby con- such theorists that at its most nascent (Greeno, 1991). Similarly, B. J. Reys
stitutes a fixed or immutable entity. level, numerical information is proba- (1994) contended that number sense
Rather, the emergence of rudimentary bly manipulated in an analog format in constitutes “a way of thinking that
components of number sense in young which numerosities are represented as should permeate all aspects of mathe-
children is thought to occur “sponta- distributions of activation on the men- matics teaching and learning” (p. 114).
neously without much explicit instruc- tal number line (Dehaene, 2001a). How- Finally, Greeno (1991) suggested that
tion” (Dehaene, 1997, p. 245), at least ever, the precise nature of this repre- “it may be more fruitful to view num-
VOLUME 38, NUMBER 4, JULY/AUGUST 2005 337

ber sense as a by-product of other Linchevski & Livneh, 1999; Picciotto & Wah, dren could not correctly judge the larger
learning than as a goal of direct in- 1993; Slavit, 1998). number for either small-value (e.g., 2 and 3)
struction” (p. 173). It should be noted 2. It should be pointed out that the compara- or large-value (e.g., 8 and 9) pairs at a dis-
that these strategies, along with other tively broad-based, higher order conception tance level of 1. Assessing these same chil-
of number sense espoused by mathematics dren again in second grade, Geary et al.
“constructivist” approaches, have been
educators has yielded a surfeit of numerical (2000) found that most of the MD + RD
considered by some to be only weakly
and mathematical tasks, problems, and test children could determine that 3 is more than
related to the theoretical conceptions items that operationalize number sense from 2, although they still did not know that 9 is
and empirical findings that have pre-K all the way through high school (see larger than 8 (a finding known as the size
emerged from the cognitive sciences Number Test Item Bank at http://www effect). These findings, limited though they
(Geary, 1995). .nzmaths.co.nz/numeracy/Other%20 may be, suggest to the present author that
material/Number%20Sense%20Items.pdf). the analog representations of arabic numer-
3. The distance effect for magnitude compar- als on the mental number line of MD + RD
Conclusion isons with single digits was first reported group children may be “noisier” than for
almost 40 years ago (Moyer & Landauer, typically achieving children. Clearly, further
The objective of the present analysis 1967). Moreover, there are several lines of investigation of the distance effect for single
evidence indicating the importance of exam- digits in children with MD is needed in
was to shed some light on the nature,
ining this speed-based effect (although it also order to test this hypothesis.
origins, and pedagogical implications
shows up in error rates): (a) the distance ef-
of differing conceptions of number fect has been found to occur in children as
sense. Although various definitional young as 5 years of age (Duncan & McFar- REFERENCES
and theoretical issues pertaining to this land, 1980; Sekular & Mierkiewicz, 1977;
construct remain unresolved, there ap- Arcavi, A. (1994). Symbol sense: Informal
Temple & Posner, 1998); (b) this effect is one
pear to be several promising, model- sense-making in formal mathematics. For
of the key markers of the mental number line;
the Learning of Mathematics, 14, 24–35.
based directions for both the early (c) the neuroanatomical substrate of the
comparison process that yields this effect ap- Case, R., & Sowder, J. T. (1990). The devel-
identification of potential mathemati-
pears to be the same in young children as in opment of computational estimation: A
cal difficulties and the design of in- neo-Piagetian analysis. Cognition and In-
structional interventions. By summa- adults (Temple & Posner, 1998); and (d) the
slope of the response time function repre- struction, 7, 79–104.
rizing the major issues in this domain, Dantzig, T. (1954). Number: The language of
senting the distance effect systematically de-
framing the critical questions, and sug- science. New York: MacMillan.
clines in children as they get older (Sekular
gesting some next steps in developing & Mierkiewicz, 1977). One explanation for Dehaene, S. (1997). The number sense: How
valid screening measures and effective the latter outcome is that the analog quan- the mind creates mathematics. New York:
intervention programs, Gersten et al. tity representations on the mental number Oxford University Press.
(in this issue) have performed a valu- line may become less “noisy” with age and Dehaene, S. (2001a). Precis of the number
able service for the field. I hope the experience. sense. Mind & Language, 16, 16–36.
preceding examination of number 4. The only study to date that has assessed Dehaene, S. (2001b). Author’s response: Is
sense will contribute to this important speed of magnitude comparisons at different number sense a patchwork? Mind & Lan-
distance levels in children with MD is the guage, 16, 89–100.
endeavor.
Landerl et al. (2004) experiment with 8- and Dehaene, S., Dupoux, E., & Mehler, J.
9-year olds described earlier. Neither the (1990). Is numerical comparison digital?
ABOUT THE AUTHOR MD-only nor the MD + RD group exhibited Analogical and symbolic effects in two-
a distance effect; however, as neither the RD- digit number comparison. Journal of Ex-
Daniel B. Berch, PhD, is director of the Math- perimental Psychology: Human Perception
only group nor the typically achieving con-
ematics and Science Cognition and Learning and Performance, 16, 626–641.
trols did either, the possibility of differences
Program at the National Institute of Child Dehaene, S., Spelke, E., Pinel, P., Stanescu,
in the distance functions between children
Health and Human Development. Address: R., & Tsivkin, S. (1999). Sources of math-
with and without MD remains to be deter-
Daniel B. Berch, Child Development and Be- ematical thinking: Behavioral and brain-
mined. Some evidence regarding this issue
havior Branch, National Institute of Child Health imaging evidence. Science, 284, 970–973.
can be gleaned from the only two other stud-
and Human Development, NIH, 6100 Execu- Duncan, E. M., & McFarland, C. E. (1980).
ies that have examined the effects of numer-
tive Blvd., Room 4B05, Bethesda, MD 20892- Isolating the effect of symbolic distance
ical distance on performance in children
7510; e-mail: berchd2@mail.nih.gov and semantic congruity in comparative
with MD, despite the fact that only accuracy
levels were measured (Geary, Hamson, & judgments: An additive-factors analysis.
NOTES
Hoard, 2000; Geary, Hoard, & Hamson, Memory & Cognition, 8, 612–622.
1999). In the first study (Geary et al., 1999), Fennell, F., & Landis, T. E. (1994). Number
1. Consistent with this broad conception, sev- no distance effect emerged in first grade for sense and operations sense. In C. A.
eral other types of mathematical “senses” either the MD-only, RD-only, or typically Thornton & N. S. Bley (Eds.), Windows of
have also been proffered: an operation sense, achieving children, with performance being opportunity: Mathematics for students with
a graphic sense, a spatial sense, a symbol near ceiling for the latter two groups. How- special needs (pp. 187–203). Reston, VA:
sense, and a structure sense (Arcavi, 1994; ever, one third of the MD + RD group chil- NCTM.
338 JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIES

Gallistel, C. R., & Gelman, R. (1992). Pre- Griffin, S. A., Case, R., & Siegler, R. S. (1994). National Council of Teachers of Mathe-
verbal and verbal counting and compu- Rightstart: Providing the central concep- matics. (2000). Principles and standards for
tation. Cognition, 44, 43–74. tual prerequisites for first formal learning school mathematics. Reston, VA: NCTM.
Geary, D. C. (1993). Mathematical disabili- of arithmetic to students at risk for school Nuerk, H.-C., Kaufmann, L., Zoppoth, S., &
ties: Cognitive, neuropsychological, and failure. In K. McGilly (Ed.), Classroom Willmes, K. (2004). On the development
genetic components. Psychological Bulletin, lessons: Integrating cognitive theory and of the mental number line: More, less, or
114, 345–362. classroom practice (pp. 24– 49). Cambridge, never holistic with increasing age? Devel-
Geary, D. C. (1995). Reflections of evolution MA: MIT Press. opmental Psychology, 40, 1199–1211.
and culture in children’s cognition: Im- Hanich, L., Jordan, N., Kaplan, D., & Dick, Nuerk, H.-C., Weger, U., & Willmes, K.
plications for mathematical development J. (2001). Performance across different (2001). Decade breaks in the mental num-
and instruction. The American Psycholo- areas of mathematical cognition in chil- ber line? Putting tens and units back into
gist, 50, 24–37. dren with learning disabilities. Journal of different bins. Cognition, 82, B25–B33.
Geary, D. C. (2004). Mathematics and learn- Educational Psychology, 93, 615–626. Number Sense Research Group. (1995, No-
ing disabilities. Journal of Learning Dis- Howden, H. (1989). Teaching number vember). Activities of the Number Sense
abilities, 37, 4–15. sense. The Arithmetic Teacher, 36, 6–11. Research Group (NSRG). Retrieved De-
Geary, D. C. (in press). Development of cember 1, 2004, from http://tiger.coe
Kalchman, M., Moss, J., & Case, R. (2001).
mathematical understanding. In D. Kuhl .missouri.edu/~mathed/number.html
Psychological models for the develop-
& R. S. Siegler (Vol. Eds.), W. Damon Okamoto, Y., & Case, R. (1996). Exploring
ment of mathematical understanding:
(Gen. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology the microstructure of children’s central
Rational numbers and functions. In S.
(6th ed.). Vol 2. Cognition, perception, and conceptual structures in the domain of
Carver & D. Klahr (Eds.), Cognition and
language. New York: Wiley. number. Monographs of the Society for Re-
instruction: Twenty-five years of progress
Geary, D. C., Hamson, C. O., & Hoard, search in Child Development, 61, 27–59.
(pp. 1–38), Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
M. K. (2000). Numerical and arithmetical Passolunghi, M. C., & Siegel, L. S. (2004).
cognition: A longitudinal study of pro- Landerl, K., Bevan, A., & Butterworth, B. Working memory and access to numeri-
cess and concept deficits in children with (2004). Developmental dyscalculia and cal information in children with disabil-
learning disability. Journal of Experimental basic numerical capacities: A study of ity in mathematics. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology, 77, 236–263. 8–9-year-old students. Cognition, 93, 99– Child Psychology, 88, 348–367.
Geary, D. C., & Hoard, M. K. (2002). Learn- 125. Pica, P., Lemer, C., Izard, V., & Dehaene, S.
ing disabilities in basic mathematics: Linchevski, L., & Livneh, D. (1999). Struc- (2004). Exact and approximate arithmetic
Deficits in memory and cognition. In J. M. ture sense: The relationship between al- in an Amazonian indigene group. Science,
Royer (Ed.), Mathematical cognition (pp. 93– gebraic and numerical contexts. Educa- 306, 499–503.
115). Greenwich, CT: Information Age tional Studies in Mathematics, 40, 173–196. Picciotto, H., & Wah, A. (1993). A new alge-
Publishing. Markovits, Z., & Sowder, J. T. (1994). De- bra: Tools, themes, and concepts. The
Geary, D. C., Hoard, M. K., & Hamson, veloping number sense: An intervention Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 12, 19–42.
C. O. (1999). Numerical and arithmetical study in Grade 7. Journal for Research in Reys, B. J. (1994). Promoting number sense
cognition: Patterns of functions and Mathematics Education, 25, 4–29. in middle grades. Teaching Mathematics in
deficits in children at risk for a mathe- McIntosh, A., Reys, B. J., & Reys, R. E. the Middle School, 1, 114–120.
matical disability. Journal of Experimental (1992). A proposed framework for exam- Reys, R., Reys, B., McIntosh, A., Emanuels-
Child Psychology, 74, 213–239. ining basic number sense. For the Learning son, G., Johansson, B., & Der, C. Y. (1999).
Gelman, R. (1990). First principles organize of Mathematics, 12, 2–8. Assessing number sense of students in
attention to and learning about rele- Australia, Sweden, Taiwan, and the Uni-
McIntosh, A., Reys, B. J., & Reys, R. E.
vant data: Number and the animate– ted States. School Science and Mathematics,
(1997). Number sense in Grades 6–8. Palo
inanimate distinction as examples. Cogni- 99, 61–70.
Alto, CA: Dale Seymour.
tive Science, 14, 79–106. Robinson, C., Menchetti, B., & Torgesen, J.
Gelman, R., & Meck, E. (1992). Early princi- McIntosh, A., Reys, B., Reys, R., Bana, J., & (2002). Toward a two-factor theory of one
ples aid initial but not later conceptions Farrell, B. (1997). Number sense in school type of mathematics disabilities. Learning
of number. In J. Bideaud, C. Meljac, & mathematics: Student performance in Disabilities Research & Practice, 17, 81–89.
J.-P. Fischer (Eds.), Pathways to number: four countries. MASTEC Monograph Se- Sekular, R., & Mierkiewicz, D. (1977). Chil-
Children’s developing numerical abilities ries No. 5. Perth, Western Australia: Edith dren’s judgments of numerical inequal-
(pp. 171–189). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Cowan University. ity. Child Development, 48, 630–633.
Gersten, R., & Chard, D. (1999). Number Menon, R. (2004, October 12) Elementary Siegler, R. S., & Booth, J. L. (2004). Devel-
sense: Rethinking arithmetic instruction school children’s number sense. Interna- opment of numerical estimation in young
for students with mathematical disabili- tional Journal for Mathematics Teaching and children. Child Development, 75, 428–444.
ties. The Journal of Special Education, 33, Learning. Retrieved December 3, 2004, Slavit, D. (1998). The role of operation sense
18–28. from http://www.exeter.ac.uk/cimt/ in transitions from arithmetic to algebraic
Greeno, J. G. (1991). Number sense as situ- ijmtl/ijmenu.htm thought. Educational Studies in Mathemat-
ated knowing in a conceptual domain. Moyer, R. S., & Landauer, T. K. (1967). Time ics, 37, 251–274.
Journal for Research in Mathematics Educa- required for judgments of numerical in- Sowder, J. (1992). Estimation and number
tion, 22, 170–218. equality. Nature, 215, 1519–1520. sense. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of
VOLUME 38, NUMBER 4, JULY/AUGUST 2005 339

research on mathematics teaching and learn- Laborde (Eds.), International handbook of Zanzali, N. A. A., & Ghazali, M. (1999, No-
ing (pp. 371–389). New York: Macmillan. mathematics education (pp. 99–137). Dor- vember). Assessment of school children’s
Temple, E., & Posner, M. (1998). Brain drecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer. number sense. Paper presented at the
mechanisms of quantity are similar in Yang, D.-C., Hsu, C.-J., & Huang, M.-C. H. meeting of the International Conference
5-year-olds and adults. Proceedings of the (in press). A study of teaching and learn- on Mathematics Education into the 21st
National Academy of Sciences of the United ing number sense for sixth grade stu- Century: Societal Challenges, Issues and
States of America, 95, 7836–7841. dents in Taiwan. International Journal of Approaches. Cairo, Egypt.
Verschaffel, L., & De Corte, E. (1996). Num- Science and Mathematics Education, 2, 407–
ber and arithmetic. In A. J. Bishop, K. 430.
Clements, C. Keitel, J. Kilpatrick, & C.

Test of Early Mathematics Ability—Third Edition


Herbert P. Ginsburg and Arthur J. Baroody
The TEMA–3 measures the mathematics performance of Several important improve-
children between the ages of 3-0 and 8-11 and is also useful ments were made in the TEMA–3.
with older children who have learning problems in mathe- a) A linear equating procedure is
matics. Used as a norm-referenced measure or a diagnostic used to adjust scores on the two
instrument, the test can be used to measure progress, evalu- tests so they may be used inter-
ate programs, screen for readiness, discover the bases for changeably. b) Bias studies show
poor school performance in mathematics, identify gifted stu- the absence of bias based on
dents, and guide instruction and remediation. The test mea- gender and ethnicity. c) The pic-
sures informal and formal (school-taught) concepts and tures of animals and money in the
skills in the following domains: numbering skills, number- Picture Book are now in color to make them more appealing
comparison facility, numeral literacy, mastery of number and more realistic in appearance.
facts, calculation skills, and understanding of concepts. It has
two parallel forms, each containing 72 items. COMPLETE TEMA–3 KIT INCLUDES: Examiner's Man-
The characteristics of the sample of 1,219 children ap- ual, Picture Book Form A, Picture Book Form B, 25 Exam-
proximate those in the 2001 U.S. Census. Internal consistency iner Record Booklets Form A, 25 Examiner Record Booklets
reliabilities are all above .92; immediate and delayed alterna- Form B, 25 Worksheets Form A, 25 Worksheets Form B, As-
tive form reliabilities are in the .80s and .90s. sessment Probes, 5" x 8" cards, 20 blocks, 20 tokens, and a
Also provided is a book of remedial techniques (Assess- mesh bag, all in a sturdy storage box. (2003)
ment Probes and Instructional Activities) for improving skills
in the areas assessed by the test. #10880 TEMA–3 Complete Kit $256.00

PRO-ED, Inc. • 8700 Shoal Creek Blvd. • Austin, Texas 78757-6897 • ph 800/897-3202 or
512/451-3246 • fax 800/FXPROED • All PRO-ED products are sold on a 30-day approval.
Shipping and handling: U.S. add 10%, Canada add 15%, others add 20%.

Você também pode gostar