Você está na página 1de 14

Journal of International Business Studies (2008) 39, 996–1009

& 2008 Academy of International Business All rights reserved 0047-2506


www.jibs.net

Understanding consumer animosity in


an international crisis: nature, antecedents,
and consequences

Siew Meng Leong1, Abstract


Joseph A Cote2, The nature, antecedents, and consequences of consumer animosity during the
1997 Asian economic crisis are investigated, based on a large-scale survey of
Swee Hoon Ang1, 2000 adult consumers representative of five affected nations (Indonesia,
Soo Jiuan Tan1, Kwon Jung3, Malaysia, Singapore, South Korea, and Thailand). An animosity model was
Ah Keng Kau1 and developed and tested with the US and Japan as target countries. As predicted,
stable and situational animosity reduced willingness to buy products from a
Chanthika Pornpitakpan4 perceived hostile national entity. Affective evaluations and cognitive judgments
1
were negatively influenced by situational animosity but not by stable
Department of Marketing, NUS Business animosity. As expected, situational animosity was increased by external
School, National University of Singapore,
attribution, perceived external control, and stable animosity. Implications of
Singapore, Republic of Singapore; 2Washington
State University, Vancouver, USA; 3KDI School of these findings are discussed, and directions for future research suggested.
Public Policy and Management, Korea; 4Faculty Journal of International Business Studies (2008) 39, 996–1009.
of Business Administration, University of Macau, doi:10.1057/palgrave.jibs.8400392
China
Keywords: animosity; Asia; crisis; international marketing
Correspondence: SM Leong, Department of
Marketing, NUS Business School, National
University of Singapore, 1 Business Link, INTRODUCTION
Singapore 117592, Republic of Singapore. Expanding globalization makes it necessary that marketers have a
Tel: þ 65 6516 3172;
Fax: þ 65 6779 5941;
better understanding of foreign consumer reactions toward their
E-mail: bizlsm@nus.edu.sg brands. Recent research suggests that perceptions of a product’s
home country affiliation can affect consumers’ buying decisions
directly and independently of product performance (Klein, 2002;
Klein, Ettenson, & Morris, 1998). While country affiliation can
convey positive attributes, it may also create brand animosity,
particularly when it is ignited during times of crisis. For example,
the ongoing Iraq conflict has fanned anti-US sentiment overseas,
which may result in American brands being shunned in some
markets. Indeed, the Asian edition of BusinessWeek’s ranking of the
world’s most valuable brands came under the headline ‘‘Brands in
an Age of Anti-Americanism’’ (Khermouch, Brady, Holmes, Ihlwan,
Kripalani, & Picard, 2003; see also Kripalani, 2004). Another
notable example of animosity is the Korean ban on Japanese
products, which was completely lifted only in 2002, more than 50
years after the end of World War II (Miami Herald, 2003).
The possibility that a country’s political actions in the interna-
Received: 21 July 2005
Revised: 1 September 2006
tional arena may create animosity towards brands affiliated with
Accepted: 20 June 2007 that nation has motivated research into consumer animosity, its
Online publication date: 15 May 2008 antecedents, and its effects on purchase intentions and behavior.
Consumer animosity in an international crisis Siew Meng Leong et al
997

Animosity is the emotional antagonism towards a on an understanding of the psychological determi-


particular entity (Averill, 1982). Jung, Ang, Leong, nants of animosity, international marketers can
Tan, Pornpitakpan, and Kau (2002) found that devise strategies to defuse potential damage to their
animosity can be split into situational animosity, brands arising from animosity towards the country
which is sparked by a particular episode, and stable with which their brands are associated.
animosity, which tends to accumulate over time, Our third contribution is methodological in
evolving into a long-lasting hostility. nature. Past animosity research has typically
Continuing this line of inquiry, we propose and employed respondents from a single country, and
test a model that examines the antecedents and has focused on a single reference nation as a target
consequences of situational and stable animosity of hostility (Klein et al., 1998). In contrast, our data
associated with the Asian economic crisis. Our provide more robust insights into the nature,
contribution is threefold. Conceptually, we seek to antecedents, and consequences of animosity, as
provide insights into the extent to which animosity they were derived from a large, representative
translates into economic preferences. Instead of sample of adult consumers from five Asian nations,
identifying animosity as a single construct with and employed two major nations – the US and
specific manifestation, such as war or an economic Japan – as target nations.
event (Klein et al., 1998), we define animosity as a We next review the animosity literature to
two-dimensional construct that can be episodic develop our conceptual model and hypotheses.
(situational animosity) or enduring (stable animos- We then provide our research methodology,
ity). Unlike past research, which explored mainly followed by the results of our survey. We conclude
demographic and socio-economic antecedents of by discussing the implications, limitations, and
animosity (Klein & Ettenson, 1999), we draw on future research directions of our work.
Weiner’s (1986) attribution theory to propose
external control and external attribution as two
important psychological antecedents to situational DEVELOPMENT OF CONCEPTUAL MODEL
animosity. Extant animosity research has explored Animosity refers to strong feelings of dislike and
only the direct effect of animosity on willingness to enmity based on beliefs arising from previous or
buy and on product judgment (Klein, 2002; Liefeld, ongoing military, political, or economic actions
1993). We propose that, given its emotional core, between nations and peoples that are perceived as
situational and stable animosity will also influence hostile, unwarranted, or violating social norms
affective evaluations of products, and that stable (Averill, 1982). Initial marketing research explored
animosity will impact on situational animosity. the effects of animosity as a unidimensional
Our second contribution pertains to marketing construct (e.g., Klein et al., 1998). More recent
practice. Marketing experts have long extolled the research has demonstrated animosity to be multi-
value of country of origin on international brands dimensional. Nijssen and Douglas (2004) found
(Heslop, Panadopoulos, & Bourk, 1998; Maheswaran, that war animosity had a strong effect on reluc-
1994). For example, Japan has been cited as a good tance to buy foreign products, while economic
example of a country that has succeeded rapidly in animosity affected reluctance to buy indirectly
altering its value as a provenance brand (Anholt, through consumer ethnocentrism. Tse, Gu, and
2000). It has also been contended that ‘‘because a Yim (2005) found that national animosity is a
country’s brand is usually highly complex and multidimensional and emotionally based construct
robust, and built up over centuries, it is relatively that biases recall and affective judgments.
hard to alter or damage it except through major Unlike ethnocentrism, where anything other
political, economic or social upheaval’’ (Anholt, than one’s own country is viewed unfavorably
2000: 22). In this era of heightened political and (Shimp & Sharma, 1987), animosity is targeted
social sensitivity, it is important for global brand towards a particular country, ethnic group, or
marketers to recognize and detect any such senti- entity. Animosity has been found to be a hostile
ments in the foreign markets that could destroy attitude comprising emotion and belief compo-
brands with strong country-of-origin linkages. nents toward national out-groups (Jung et al.,
Hence, on the managerial front, our examination 2002). Brummett et al. (1988) conceptualized
of the animosity construct helps decision-makers hostility as having both a cognitive and an
recognize whether and how animosity affects attitudinal component. The cognitive dimension
consumer preferences. By using early cues based involves cynical beliefs and mistrust of others, and

Journal of International Business Studies


Consumer animosity in an international crisis Siew Meng Leong et al
998

the attitudinal dimension involves negative and the extent to which they were willing to pay
emotions of anger, contempt, and disgust. premium prices to obtain these items. Thus we
This suggests that animosity can influence both hypothesize:
cognitive judgments and affective evaluations of
products (cf. Klein, 2002). Thus the hostile beliefs Hypothesis 1: The more animosity a consumer
regarding one country can be extended to judg- feels towards a country, the less favorable are his
ments regarding quality of products from that or her other cognitive judgments and affective
country. Beliefs about product quality are tainted evaluations of products from that country.
by beliefs about the country that it comes from.
Krisnakumar (1974) has shown that Indian stu- Hypothesis 2: The more animosity a consumer
dents evaluate British products more positively feels towards a country, the less willing he or she
than do students from Taiwan, a difference attribu- is to buy products from that country.
table to the past colonial link between the United
Kingdom and India. In the Cold War years there Hypothesis 3: The less favorable the cognitive
was a backlash against anything Western in China. judgments and affective evaluations a consumer
However, with the opening up of China, and its has of products from a country, the lower his or
normalization of relationships with countries in her willingness to purchase products from that
North America and Europe since the late 1980s, country.
imported (Western) products have been viewed
favorably over domestic products in China, even Since animosity is based on beliefs arising from
at prices that are three to four times higher (Tefft, previous or ongoing hostility, the emotion can be
1994). situationally driven by a particular event, or can
Affective evaluations would likewise be influ- evolve over time through a series of events ( Jung
enced, where the greater the animosity, the less et al., 2002). The former is referred to as situational
favorable is the liking for products from the animosity, and the latter as stable animosity.
concerned country. Indeed, the strong emotional Whereas situational animosity refers to a specific
core of the animosity construct suggests that it circumstance, stable animosity is long lasting and
should correspondingly influence consumers’ affec- deep rooted, becoming ingrained over time. For
tive evaluations of products. In turn, the lower instance, the 1997 Asian economic crisis may have
levels of product judgment and affect lead to prompted situational animosity towards countries
decreased willingness to purchase. For example, perceived to have contributed to the crisis. The
the recent success of such brands as Mecca-Cola animosity is situational, as it pertained to a specific
and Qibla-Cola in Muslim nations is derived partly event. In contrast, if the event becomes drawn out,
from Islamic consumers’ disapproval of American or several economic crises occur one after the other,
foreign policy following the invasion of Iraq. with the same country being viewed as responsible
Affected Middle Eastern consumers have switched for the occurrences, then the animosity can become
to such offerings to show active support for brands ingrained and stable over time. War between two
that advertise a political and ethical alternative to countries, for example, arouses situational animos-
American brands (Parmar, 2004). Aside from its ity at the time of occurrence, but can evolve into
indirect impact via cognitive judgments and affec- stable animosity over time as the atrocities of war
tive evaluations posited here, animosity has also are remembered, though not necessarily personally
been found to have a direct, negative impact on experienced, for generations.
willingness to buy (Klein et al., 1998). For instance, We predict that Hypotheses 1–3 would hold for
following an incident in 2003 when a group of both stable and situational animosity. Thus, regard-
Japanese tourists attended a brothel-type party in less of whether animosity is derived from a current
Zhuhai, China, on the anniversary of Japan’s attack or an ongoing series of events, it should negatively
on China during World War II, about 30% of the impact on willingness to buy, as well as on
Chinese college students’ chat room comments cognitive judgments and affective evaluations of,
rallied for actions against Japan, including the products from the perceived target of hostility.
boycott of Japanese goods (Tse et al., 2005). Moreover, we expect a positive relationship
Empirically, Shimp, Dunn, and Klein (2004) found between stable and situational animosity. When
that animosity correlated with the degree to which situational animosity towards a particular entity is
consumers chose products from their own region aroused, information pertaining to the current

Journal of International Business Studies


Consumer animosity in an international crisis Siew Meng Leong et al
999

event may be processed in a manner biased by


stable animosity towards that entity. Research has
shown that, when consumers have strongly held
preferences, they engage in motivated processing
( Jain & Maheswaran, 2000). For instance, the anti-
Japanese protests and demonstrations that spread
throughout China in mid-April 2005 over the
Japanese Ministry of Education’s approval to print
a school textbook that glossed over Japanese war
atrocities (Marquardt, 2005) are a manifestation of
the deep-rooted negative feelings that the Chinese
have over Japan’s war atrocities in China. The
Japanese act also caused the South Korean president
to caution Japan about a possible diplomatic war,
and reduced economic activities between the two
countries (Marquardt, 2005). Hence: Figure 1 Hypothesized model.
Note: Correlations among exogenous factors were modeled but
Hypothesis 4: The greater the stable animosity a are not shown in the figure.
consumer feels towards a country, the greater the
situational animosity that will be felt towards
that country. control. Federn (1985) found that resentment was
elevated when the instigating party was perceived
Weiner’s (1986) attribution theory suggests two to have control over the provocation. Formally:
other antecedents of situational animosity that will
be investigated here: locus of attribution, and Hypothesis 6: The greater the external controll-
controllability of the cause and effect. Locus of ability, the greater the situational animosity
attribution addresses the ‘‘who is to blame?’’ towards the outside entity.
question. It concerns the attribution of outcomes.
External attribution occurs when an external party The hypothesized model is shown in Figure 1.
is blamed for the occurrence, whereas internal
attribution relates to inferences made towards METHOD
dispositional causes for the event’s occurrence: the
greater the external attribution, the greater the Background
animosity towards the entity concerned. If Asian The research context was the 1997 Asian economic
consumers blame America for the 1997 economic crisis, an event of significant international political,
crisis, the greater is their situational animosity economic, and marketing proportions. Data were
towards the US. Hence: collected from nationally representative samples
of adult consumers from five Asian countries:
Hypothesis 5: The greater the external attribu- Indonesia, South Korea, Malaysia, Singapore, and
tion made, the more situational animosity will be Thailand. These countries varied in how severely
felt towards the outside entity. affected they were by the crisis (Indonesia, South
Korea, and Thailand were hurt most), the type of
Controllability concerns the extent to which the economic controls instituted (Indonesia looked
entity perceived to be responsible for an outcome towards IMF for help, whereas Malaysia instituted
has the power to modify the course of events. Low capital controls to contain the crisis), and their
controllability suggests that there is little that the historical relationship with the US and Japan
party responsible for the event can do to avoid the (Indonesia, Malaysia, Singapore, and Korea were
outcome, whereas high controllability implies that occupied by Japan during World War II, whereas
much can be done by the party responsible for the Thailand was never under foreign jurisdiction).
outcome to change the consequences. If the Adults in these five countries were asked for their
external entity is viewed to have control, animosity views regarding two major countries – the US and
towards that entity is expected to be greater than Japan – in general, as well as specific to the crisis.
when the external entity is viewed as having low The Asian economic crisis reflects the situational

Journal of International Business Studies


Consumer animosity in an international crisis Siew Meng Leong et al
1000

animosity felt by Asian consumers. In Malaysia, for employed were appropriate, adequate, and mean-
instance, there were widespread accusations that ingful for respondents. The research instrument
the root of the crisis was the intense currency was then translated and back-translated indepen-
speculation by American fund managers (The New dently by professionals engaged by the market
Republic, 1999), suggesting situational animosity research agency for linguistic and conceptual
towards the US. The animosity arising from histor- equivalence (Bhalla & Lin, 1987). Pretests were
ical military occupation in the region is suggestive then conducted to ensure that the intended mean-
of stable animosity. For instance, Korea was under ings were conveyed and understood by respon-
Japanese occupation for 35 years, and experienced dents. The final questionnaire was produced in
some of the worst war atrocities in Asia, implying both English and the local language. Bilingual
stable animosity towards Japan. interviewers used the version that respondents felt
more comfortable with in administering the survey.
Respondents The animosity measures were taken from Jung
The data were collected in the fourth quarter of et al. (2002). Three items were used to measure
1998, when most Asian countries were reeling from stable animosity. These items concerned views about
the effects of the economic crisis. A leading how well America/Japan had behaved towards their
international research company was commissioned country over the years. Situational animosity was
to collect the data. Professional interviewers blind measured by six items using the Asian economic
to the purpose of the research surveyed 400 adult crisis as the context. The items measured how they
nationals aged 20–59 years from the capital city of personally felt towards Americans/Japanese during
each country, giving a total sample size of 2000. this crisis.
In-home face-to-face interviews were conducted The external attribution and external control
using stratified random sampling to obtain a items were adapted from Weiner (1986) to reflect
representative sample for each country. Specifically, the context of the Asian crisis. Four items were used
sampling areas for each country were selected based to measure external attribution. The items reflected
on electoral divisions or polling wards containing a attributions made concerning the country’s
certain population of households. To ensure wide- economic crisis. External control was measured using
spread coverage, each electoral division was divided four items concerning views about how much
into primary sampling units (PSUs) of about equal influence the external party had over the economic
size of around 500 dwelling units. Each PSU was situation of respondents’ country.
then selected to represent the polling division. Affective evaluation was measured using four items
Some 30–40 PSUs were employed for each of the that have been employed in other work (e.g., Tse
capital cities in which the survey was conducted. et al., 2005). They included how much respondents
Half the respondents in each country were asked to liked, trusted, were in favor of buying, and found it
respond with regard to the US, and the other half to appealing to buy products from America/Japan. The
Japan. scale for cognitive judgment consisted of five items
taken from Klein et al. (1998). They concerned
Questionnaire perceptions of workmanship, technological
The questionnaire consisted of several sections, advancement, color and design, reliability, and
with items using five-point scales anchored on value for money of American/Japanese products.
‘‘Strongly disagree’’ (1) and ‘‘Strongly agree’’ (5). Finally, willingness to buy was measured using four
Items employed to measure the various constructs items, three of which were from Klein et al. (1998).
of interest are contained in the Appendix, which The items included how willing they were to buy,
also reports the average composite reliabilities and felt guilty buying, avoided buying, and liked the
average variance extracted of the scales. The items idea of owning American/Japanese products.
were first developed in English. Given that several
measures were included from scales used in pre- RESULTS
vious studies with Western consumers, the initial
draft survey was assessed for cultural compatibility Tests of Measure Equivalence
and adequacy for an Asian sample by native We adopted Steenkamp and Baumgartner’s (1998)
speakers fluent in English (Douglas & Craig, approach for comparing multi-country data and
1984). Minor modifications were made, based on assessing measure invariance. They recommend
their feedback, to ensure that all items to be fitting a sequential set of increasingly constrained

Journal of International Business Studies


Consumer animosity in an international crisis Siew Meng Leong et al
1001

multi-group structural equation models to demon- achieved. After relaxing 35 of the 486 constraints in
strate that results are comparable across countries. the metric invariance model, a good fit was
The first test is for configural invariance, which achieved CFI¼0.863, RMSEA¼0.023). Only four of
shows that the pattern of salient and nonsalient the released constraints were factor loadings, while
loadings defines the structure of the measurement 31 were associated with measurement error. Most of
instrument. This test involves constraining model the modifications were for Korea (11) and Malaysia
specifications to be identical for all countries, but (10), but all countries had constraints released.
allowing the parameters to be uniquely estimated. There were greater differences between the target
In other words, the factor structures for the scales country data. The Japanese ratings accounted for 26
and the specified relationships among the con- of the 35 released constraints. Based on these
structs are identical, but estimates of the loadings results, we conclude that adequate metric invari-
and path coefficients differ among countries. ance was demonstrated.
Metric invariance tests whether consumers in The fully constrained factor invariance model
different countries respond to the scale items in had a moderate fit (CFI¼0.850, RMSEA¼0.024).3
the same way. In other words, is the size of the Releasing only 22 of the 586 constraints brought
factor loadings equal across countries? To test the the fit close to the metric invariance model
metric model, we constrained the loadings between (CFI¼0.859, RMSEA¼0.023). Ten of the released
a given indicator and its factor to be equal across all constraints were disturbance terms, indicating that
10 country/target pairs (e.g., the loading for Korean the model explained different amounts of variance
evaluations of the US were the same as Thai among the countries/target pairs. Twelve of the
evaluations of Japan). Finally, factor invariance1 constraints related to country differences in the
specifies that the relationships among constructs structural relationships. Eleven of these differences
are the same for all countries. We tested factor were the effect of stable animosity (six constraints)
invariance by imposing equality constraints on the and situational animosity (five constraints) on the
structural relationships, factor variances, and factor dependent variables.
covariances across countries. If there is configural,
metric, and factor invariance across the multiple Hypotheses Tests
groups, we can conclude that the relationships The structural estimates are presented in Table 1,
among the constructs are the same for all countries. and country differences are noted. In general, the
Steenkamp and Baumgartner (1998) suggest using results support the hypotheses. Overall, the model
RMSEA, CFI, CAIC, and/or NNFI when comparing was able to explain a large amount of variance in
fit across models. For simplicity, we compare only willingness to buy (R2¼0.615) and situational
the RMSEA and CFI values for each model. animosity (R2¼0.528). The model explained a
The configural invariance model fitted well modest amount of variance for affective evalua-
(unrestricted model CFI¼0.876, RMSEA¼0.023) tions (R2¼0.293) and a small amount of the
using the raw data (covariance matrix).2 However, variance in cognitive judgments (R2¼0.057). With
the metric invariance model failed to fit due to few exceptions, the results were the same across
numerous estimation problems. Given these pro- country–target pairs. Situational animosity leads to
blems, we analyzed normed data per Mullen (1995). less favorable affective evaluations and cognitive
The norming process was done separately for each judgments. Overall, the effect on affective evalua-
country/data set combination (e.g., the correlation tions was much higher than for cognitive judg-
matrix was calculated separately for each country/ ments. The effects of situational animosity on
target combination). affective evaluations and cognitive evaluations
Using normed data, the configural invariance were the same for all countries, except for:
model fitted well (CFI¼0.877, RMSEA¼0.023). Malaysia, where the effect on affective evaluations
The full metric invariance model did not fit was more negative; Singapore, where it was more
well (CFI¼0.836, RMSEA¼0.025). Steenkamp and negative only towards the US; and Korea, where it
Baumgartner (1998) note that it is unrealistic to was positive for the US. However, the influence of
expect fully constrained metric and factor invar- stable animosity on affective evaluations and
iance models to fit well, and suggest using mod- cognitive judgment was not supported. The excep-
ification indices to relax constraints until a tions were for Singapore, where stable animosity
reasonable fit is achieved. If relatively few para- had a large negative effect on affective evaluations
meters are freed, then partial invariance has been for both target countries and cognitive judgments

Journal of International Business Studies


Consumer animosity in an international crisis Siew Meng Leong et al
1002

Table 1 Structural estimates for the factor invariance model

Affective evaluations Cognitive judgments Situational animosity Stable animosity Disturbance R2

(a) Willingness to buy


US
Indonesia 0.551* 0.015 0.182* 0.234* 0.631 0.602
Malaysia 0.680* 0.014 0.163* 0.216* 0.368 0.864
Singapore 0.503* 0.015 0.169* 0.219* 0.592 0.649
Thailand 0.556* 0.016 0.172* 0.239* 0.645 0.583
Korea 0.552* 0.015 0.201* 0.230* 0.621 0.614

Japan
Indonesia 0.569* 0.016 0.184* 0.118 0.777 0.396
Malaysia 0.508* 0.014 0.177* 0.215* 0.582 0.662
Singapore 0.502* 0.016 0.161* 0.213* 0.576 0.668
Thailand 0.589* 0.015 0.176* 0.225* 0.607 0.631
Korea 0.603* 0.017 0.054 0.262* 0.708 0.499

Situational animosity Stable animosity Disturbance R2

(b) Affective evaluations


US
Indonesia 0.369* 0.060 0.913 0.167
Malaysia 0.654* 0.058 0.724 0.476
Singapore 0.376* 0.445* 0.667 0.555
Thailand 0.346* 0.060 0.924 0.146
Korea 0.408* 0.059 0.897 0.196

Japan
Indonesia 0.361* 0.060 0.918 0.158
Malaysia 0.687* 0.060 0.679 0.539
Singapore 0.359* 0.571* 0.536 0.713
Thailand 0.333* 0.054 0.929 0.137
Korea 0.340* 0.061 0.935 0.126

Situational animosity Stable animosity Disturbance R2

(c) Cognitive judgments


US
Indonesia 0.102* 0.062 0.989 0.022
Malaysia 0.099* 0.062 0.990 0.021
Singapore 0.345* 0.058 0.923 0.148
Thailand 0.094* 0.062 0.990 0.019
Korea 0.231* 0.061 0.979 0.042

Japan
Indonesia 0.099* 0.062 0.990 0.021
Malaysia 0.107* 0.062 0.987 0.026
Singapore 0.084* 0.486* 0.840 0.294
Thailand 0.102* 0.062 0.989 0.023
Korea 0.092* 0.062 0.993 0.014

Stable animosity External attribution External control Disturbance R2

(d) Situational animosity


US
Indonesia 0.402* 0.319* 0.205* 0.636 0.596
Malaysia 0.415* 0.330* 0.211* 0.656 0.569

Journal of International Business Studies


Consumer animosity in an international crisis Siew Meng Leong et al
1003

Table 1 continued

Stable animosity External attribution External control Disturbance R2


Singapore 0.405* 0.322* 0.206* 0.641 0.589
Thailand 0.434* 0.345* 0.221* 0.686 0.529
Korea 0.357* 0.284* 0.182* 0.745 0.445

Japan
Indonesia 0.413* 0.328* 0.210* 0.653 0.573
Malaysia 0.636* 0.302* 0.144 0.601 0.639
Singapore 0.414* 0.329* 0.211* 0.654 0.572
Thailand 0.400* 0.318* 0.204* 0.634 0.599
Korea 0.078 0.355* 0.228* 0.852 0.275
*Indicates parameter significant at po0.05.
Italic indicates constraints that were freed.

for Japan. However, it appears that the effect of towards a country, the greater the situational
stable animosity was mediated by situational animosity felt towards that country (the effect was
animosity. When situational animosity’s effects on stronger for Malaysian evaluations of Japan, and
evaluations and judgment were dropped, stable not significant for Korean evaluations of Japan).
animosity had a significant negative effect. This Hypotheses 5 and 6 were both supported, with
provides further evidence that the effect of stable estimates equal across all countries. As suggested by
animosity on affective evaluations and cognitive Hypothesis 5, the greater the external attribution
judgments was mediated by situational animosity. made, the more situational animosity was felt
These results partially support Hypothesis 1. towards the outside entity. Similarly, as Hypothesis
Hypothesis 2 was fully supported. Both situa- 6 posited, greater external controllability was
tional and stable animosity had a significant effect related to higher levels of situational animosity
on willingness to buy. As hypothesized, the more towards the target entity.
animosity a consumer felt towards a country, the
less willing he or she was to buy products from that DISCUSSION
country. The exceptions were for Korea, where This study examined the antecedents and conse-
situational animosity had no effect on willingness quences of stable and situational animosity using a
to buy Japanese products, and Indonesia, where large sample of representative adult consumers
stable animosity had a positive effect on willingness from five Asian nations against the backdrop of
to buy Japanese products. Possibly, the stable the economic crisis of the late 1990s. The survey
animosity of Koreans towards the Japanese may results generally supported the proposed model.
be so strong that their situational animosity has Situational and stable animosity reduced willing-
little effect on willingness to buy. Holloway (1991) ness to buy both US and Japanese products.
noted that many Indonesians who were not overly Situational animosity also negatively influenced
influenced by Western thinking perceive that the cognitive judgments and affective evaluations of US
Japanese occupation was beneficial, despite the fact and Japanese products. However, stable animosity
that Indonesia suffered as much as other Asian did not impact on cognitive judgments and
countries during World War II. Moreover, Japanese affective evaluations, except through situational
trade, aid, and investments were crucial ingredients animosity. In terms of antecedents, feelings of
to the country’s success, and signaled Japan’s long- situational animosity were increased by external
term commitment to Indonesia. attribution, perceived external control, and stable
Hypothesis 3 was generally supported. Positive animosity.
affective evaluations led to higher willingness to Our findings cast added conceptual light on the
buy (the effect was stronger for the Malaysia–US animosity construct on several counts. First, we
pairing). This effect was also quite large. However, extend recent research that explores the nature of
cognitive judgments did not have an effect on animosity (Nijssen & Douglas, 2004). Whereas such
willingness to buy. As Hypothesis 4 posited, the work focused on different sources of the construct’s
greater the stable animosity a consumer felt manifestation (war and economic), we distinguish

Journal of International Business Studies


Consumer animosity in an international crisis Siew Meng Leong et al
1004

between stable and situational animosity, a dichot- situational animosity (an expression of anger) for
omy that is consistent with extant theorizing in the both the Japan and US data provides empirical
emotional aggression literature (Jung et al., 2002). support for this linkage.
Berkowitz (1994) had suggested that emotional
aggression could be triggered by situational influ- Implications for Practice
ences or personality dispositions. Our stable– Our results also have implications for marketing
situational dichotomy is thus more generalizable managers and policymakers. While much has been
than previous accounts, since it accommodates written regarding the use of marketing to advance
various sources of animosity. For example, stable political candidates and agendas (Newman, 1999),
animosity could arise from non-military events, as the stream of research on animosity illustrates the
illustrated by the defensive attitude the French politicization of marketing. For example, in the
adopt towards ‘‘American’’ culture. They appear context of American foreign policy, Huntington
‘‘fascinated by the American Way of Life as an (1999) noted that the US corporate interests abroad
exotic item but many French people, including have been promoted under the slogans of free trade
politicians, would be horrified to have it at home’’ and open markets, through the shaping of World
(Usunier, 1993: 199). Bank and International Monetary Fund policies,
Second, our results suggest that while situational and through getting foreign countries to adopt
and stable animosity reduced consumer willingness economic and social policies that benefit American
to buy products from perceived targets of hostility, economic interests. Such actions are sometimes
these effects work through different mechanisms. viewed with hostility by foreign countries. Negative
In particular, our findings suggest that while reactions to the US imposing unpopular policies
situational animosity has both a direct and an may well affect America’s economic performance
indirect influence on willingness to buy via affec- internationally. This is especially true as the
tive evaluation, the impact of stable animosity is perceived hostile policies imposed by the US may
mediated by situational animosity. It would appear be viewed as controllable, and can accumulate
that stable animosity exacerbates animosity based over time into stable animosity. The current
on current events. In turn, affective evaluations are unpopularity of US military policy can further
adversely impacted, which then reduces purchase increase animosity. Thus any political, economic,
intentions. It appears that intense emotions initi- or military actions viewed as being hostile
ally formed may have persistent effects on future will lower product judgment and evaluation,
emotions triggered by hostile events, and that such as well as reduce willingness to buy brands from
emotions can overwhelm cognition. Our findings the aggressor nation.
thus underscore the more prominent role of affect Klein et al. (1998) provided several sound recom-
over cognition in animosity models. mendations for foreign marketers in such situa-
Aside from stable animosity, our results demon- tions, including: exporters conducting marketing
strate that situational animosity is also influenced research to gauge the level of animosity in foreign
positively by external controllability and external target markets; downplaying ‘‘Made in’’ labels and
attribution. These findings were robust across the brand names associated with the aggressor country;
US and Japanese data. Our research thus augments engaging in strategic marketing and manufacturing
the scant body of attribution research in the alliances to make and offer hybrid products that de-
international consumer behavior literature (Folkes, emphasize the source of the products; and employ-
1988; Leigh & Choi, 2003). In social psychology, it ing public relations and other communications
has been found that people of collectivistic cultures efforts to improve country perceptions and address
characteristic of most East Asian nations tend to sources of international tension.
make external inferences (Lee, Hallahan, & Herzog, In addition, we suggest that foreign marketers
1996; Morris & Peng, 1994). We extend this line may localize their brands by using local production
of research by finding a relationship between and raw materials, or by launching local versions of
such attributional inferences and animosity levels. their offerings. Investing in the affected country,
Weiner (2000) had proposed an attributional possibly in conjunction with a local partner, may
process that links attribution to emotions (such as also demonstrate a long-term commitment to that
anger, hope, and fear) and, in turn, to consumer country (Ang, Jung, Kau, Leong, Pornpitakpan, &
behavior. Our examination and confirmation of Tan, 2004). Cause-related marketing may also be
the significant impact of external attribution on employed in this connection. Foreign firms may

Journal of International Business Studies


Consumer animosity in an international crisis Siew Meng Leong et al
1005

support local causes, such as sponsoring events that becoming more comparable over time. This implies
raise money for the adversely affected, or be that negative affect will be harder to overcome, and
involved in activities of local community organiza- that as developing countries improve product
tions (Vence, 2004). Hiring local talent and placing quality, such affective evaluations will play a bigger
them in key management roles, as General Electric role in choice. Thus, while major global brands
does, may reduce animosity in addition to promot- have been able to generally ignore their home
ing diversity (Khermouch et al., 2003). Foreign country’s political actions, this may be less true in
marketers may also foster closer partnerships with the future. For example, French wine is considered
their local distributors. Setting up local subsidiaries better by US consumers, but sales dropped drama-
to handle and manage their dealers, and providing tically after the French failed to support US Iraqi
them with financial and marketing assistance, may policies.
likewise contribute to strengthening their roots in More generally, by understanding the psycholo-
affected markets (Slater, 1999). To the extent that gical determinants of the animosity construct,
hostility is peculiarly favorable to social cohesion international marketers can devise strategies to
(Mead, 1929), foreign businesses may engage in defuse damage to their brands arising from animos-
discussions of which both they and consumers in ity towards the country with which their brands
the affected countries have a common dislike have strong linkage. For example, they can focus on
(Rawwas, Rajendran, & Wuehrer, 1996). Collec- other aspects of brand personality (apart from
tively, these initiatives may be more imperative for performance or quality) that are less related to
marketers of iconic brands strongly identified country of origin during crisis periods when
with their national heritage (e.g., McDonald’s and animosity is invoked. Since situational animosity
Coca-Cola with the US). negatively impacts on affective evaluations, inter-
Firms could also consider ways in which they can national marketers should downplay the country
dissociate themselves from the policies and actions linkage when animosity is aroused against their
of the home government. Such actions may also brand’s country of origin, especially since our
redirect affected consumers’ external attributions results demonstrate that external attribution has a
away from themselves, to reduce perceptions of the strong impact on situational animosity. However,
firms’ controllability over the event. Foreign mar- situational animosity is sporadic, and has a short
keters should not engage in actions that may lifespan (e.g., the Chinese students’ call for boycott
exacerbate feelings of animosity in affected coun- of Japanese products has abated, and many Japanese
tries. During the Asian economic crisis, nearly 2000 businesses are back to normal in China). Thus
Malaysian companies were wound up by their international marketers need to be alert to events
creditors. The Malaysian registrar of companies that may arouse situational animosity and, in turn,
noted that ‘‘If every company that is in trouble is their brand.
just wound up by creditors, it is tantamount to Less attention has focused on what domestic
killing the development of local companies.’’ He marketers may do under such circumstances. Klein
urged creditors to be more patient, and consider et al. (1998) suggest that importers identify the
flexible repayment schemes (Pereira, 1998). Acquir- level of animosity towards particular foreign sup-
ing local businesses and assets (e.g., distributorships pliers. Domestic marketers may also defend them-
and retail locations) at deep discounts is another selves by promoting ‘‘buy local’’ campaigns. For
example. Thus US investment banks and multi- example, during the Asian economic crisis, the Thai
nationals have been perceived to scour the region government urged its citizens to ‘‘Eat Thai, Use
on ‘‘vulture tours’’ during the crisis to establish and Thai, Buy Thai, and Travel Thai.’’ More extreme
strengthen their foothold in Asia (Head, 1998). actions may involve organizing boycotts against
While such actions appear financially and products of businesses from the hostile nation.
strategically well motivated, they should be However, proactive domestic marketers may exploit
handled with care and sensitivity, to avoid reinfor- the situation by launching niche brands to appeal
cing consumer resentment and inviting a possible to consumers sympathetic to their cause and
backlash. resentful of that of the hostile nation. For example,
In addition, our findings suggest to international Arab cola marketers have offered alternatives for
marketers that having a better product may not be Muslims and related sympathizers to show their
sufficient to compensate for animosity. Indeed, disapproval of American Middle East policy and
for most products, functional performance is support for alternative values and lifestyles (Parmar,

Journal of International Business Studies


Consumer animosity in an international crisis Siew Meng Leong et al
1006

2004). Thus Qibla Cola’s tagline of ‘‘Liberate your animosity on product judgment and affective
taste’’ furthers the metaphor that consumers of the evaluation, which was not a focus of our research.
two leading cola brands may now find a viable Second, our research analyzed the effects of
substitute. Yet leveraging on anti-American senti- animosity using willingness to buy as the ultimate
ment alone may be insufficient for long-term dependent measure. Future research might consider
growth. Expansion beyond the niche markets they a broader range of dependent variables, including
appeal to may require a broadened positioning, as actual brand purchase behavior, brand choice,
well as depend on how successfully these word-of-mouth recommendation, ownership and
brands market their alternative values and lifestyles usage patterns, and product disposal.
globally. Third, improvements could also be made on the
Policymakers responsible for influencing interna- psychometric front. Extant measures of animosity
tional marketing performance with potentially vary from study to study, limiting the comparabil-
hostile actions may also help to reduce animosity ity of research results. For example, our animosity
levels. For example, during the Asian economic items were developed to capture the construct’s
crisis Japan was repeatedly encouraged by the stable and situational dimensions, whereas pre-
regional and international communities to take vious scales tapped its sources of occurrence, or
bold initiatives to revive its economy to absorb were unidimensional in nature. Developing more
imports from affected Asian nations (Lee, 1998). standardized inventories in this area would thus be
Other measures may involve governmental efforts a productive avenue for future work.
to correct attributions of blame, as well as proactive Finally, our respondents hailed from five coun-
efforts such as sending aid and trade missions to tries in Asia. This furnishes insights into the
affected countries. In the absence of such actions, robustness of the relationships across nations, as
the contemporary and cumulative effects of unpop- well as facilitates the uncovering of possible
ular policy may well reverberate into the rejection cultural variations in our findings. Our results
of offerings from perceived hostile nations, and suggest that while some cross-national differences
consequently reduce their exports to affected occurred, they did not detract from the overall
international markets. At the minimum, public consistent pattern of results. This implies that the
policymakers should be cognizant of the interna- proposed model of animosity’s antecedents and
tional business repercussions of their actions (e.g., consequences appears to be generalizable. None-
Chua, 2005), particularly in a crisis situation, where theless, as Asia represents a myriad of cultures and
emotions run high and are fueled by angry religions, future research might involve respon-
sentiments drawn from past provocations. dents from more countries in the region. Indeed,
animosity research would also benefit from
Limitations and Directions for Future data collection efforts in other continents and
Research cultures (e.g., the Middle East). Expanding
Several directions for future research can be recom- the targets of hostility to other nations besides the
mended, based on the findings and limitations of US and Japan examined here would also be
our work. First, we propose that animosity research instructive.
employ a more theory-driven approach by examin-
ing its psychological antecedents. We empirically ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
assessed two attribution-related constructs to this The authors thank the National University of Singapore
end. Although this is promising, future work may for funding this research, and the two anonymous JIBS
investigate other aspects of attribution, such as reviewers and departmental editor David K. Tse for
globality (whether the cause is universal, or unique their helpful comments.
to specific circumstances) and intentionality
(whether the entity intended for the outcome to
occur), for added insights (Meyer, 1980). Examining NOTES
1
other psychological constructs as antecedents as Steenkamp and Baumgartner (1998) differentiate
well as potential moderators of animosity will lead between factor covariance and factor variance invar-
to the development of a more comprehensive and iance. Since we do both simultaneously, we use the
generalizable nomological framework for the con- term ‘‘factor invariance’’ to imply both variance and
struct. In particular, it would be useful to assess the covariance invariance, as well as invariance in the
boundary conditions relating to the effects of structural estimates.

Journal of International Business Studies


Consumer animosity in an international crisis Siew Meng Leong et al
1007

2
Sharma, Mukherjee, Kumar, and Dillon (2005) note value. Our very complex model, with numerous
that for a model with as few as 24 indicators and a constraints, exceeds the cutoffs suggested by Sharma
sample size of 200, cutoff CFI values should fall to et al. (2005).
3
about 0.85 (or less) and 0.07 to 0.08 for RMSEA. The constraints released in the metric invariance
Increased model complexity would suggest lowering model remained released in the factor invariance
the CFI cutoff value and raising the RMSEA cutoff model.

REFERENCES
Ang, S. H., Jung, K., Kau, A. K., Leong, S. M., Pornpitakpan, C., Klein, J. G., Ettenson, R., & Morris, M. D. 1998. The animosity
& Tan, S. J. 2004. Animosity towards economic giants: What model of foreign product purchase: An empirical test in the
the little guys think. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 21(3): People’s Republic of China. Journal of Marketing, 62(1):
190–207. 89–100.
Anholt, S. 2000. The nation as brand. Across the Board, 37(10): Kripalani, M. 2004. Ban outsourcing? Bad idea. BusinessWeek
22–27. Asian Edition, March 29: http://www.businessweek.com.
Averill, J. 1982. Anger and aggression: An essay on emotion. New Krisnakumar, P. 1974. An exploratory study of the influence of
York: Springer-Verlag. country of origin on the product images of persons from
Berkowitz, L. 1994. On the escalation of aggression. In M. selected countries. Unpublished PhD Dissertation, University
Potegal & J. F. Knutson (Eds), The dynamics of aggression: of Florida.
Biological and social processes in dyads and groups: 33–41. Lee, F., Hallahan, M., & Herzog, T. 1996. Explaining real-life
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. events: How culture and domain shape attributions. Person-
Bhalla, G., & Lin, L. 1987. Cross-cultural marketing research: A ality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22(7): 732–741.
discussion of equivalence issues and management strategies. Lee, P. P. 1998. Japan and the Asian economic crisis. The Business
Psychology and Marketing, 4(4): 275–285. Times (ISEAS Trends), April 25–26: 9.
Brummett, B. H., Maynard, K. E., Babyak, M. A., Haney, T. L., Leigh, J. H., & Choi, Y. T. 2003. Attributions about life events
Siegler, I., Helms, M. J., & Barefoot, J. C. 1988. Measures of and perceptions of foreign products: Contrasts in individual-
hostility as predictors of facial affect during social interaction: ism and collectivism. In U.S. Tate (Ed), Proceedings of the 2003
Evidence for construct validity. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, Association of Collegiate Marketing Educators Conference: 22–
20(3): 168–173. 23. Texas: College of Business University of North Texas
Chua, C. H. 2005. China’s message to Japan over shrine visits. Association of Collegiate Marketing Educators.
The Straits Times, May 24: 1. Liefeld, J. P. 1993. Experiments on country-of-origin effects:
Douglas, S. P., & Craig, C. S. 1984. Establishing equivalence in Review and meta-analysis of effect size. In N. Panadopoulos &
comparative consumer research. In E. Kaynak & R. Savitt (Eds), L. A. Heslop (Eds), Product-country images: Impact and role in
Comparative marketing systems: 93–113. New York: Praeger. international marketing: 117–146. New York: International
Federn, E. 1985. The therapeutic management of violence. Business Press.
Milieu Therapy, 4: 41–49. Maheswaran, D. 1994. Country-of-origin as a stereotype:
Folkes, V. S. 1988. Recent attribution research in consumer Effects of consumer expertise and attribute strength on
behavior: A review and new directions. Journal of Consumer product evaluations. Journal of Consumer Research, 21(2):
Research, 14(4): 548–565. 354–365.
Head, M. 1998. US leads attack on Japan at G-7 meeting. World Marquardt, E. 2005. The price of Japanese nationalism. Asia
Socialist Web Site http://www.wsws.org/news/1998/feb1998/ Times Online, April 14: http://www.atimes.com.
japf26.shtml. Accessed 17 July 2003. Mead, G. H. 1929. National-mindedness and international-
Heslop, L. A., Panadopoulos, N., & Bourk, M. 1998. An inter- mindedness. The International Journal of Ethics, 39(4):
regional and intercultural perspective on subculture differ- 385–407.
ences in product evaluations. Canadian Journal of Adminis- Meyer, J. P. 1980. Causal attribution for success and failure: A
trative Sciences, 15(2): 113–127. multivariate investigation of dimensionality, formation, and
Holloway, N. 1991. Japan in Asia. Hong Kong: Review Publishing consequences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Company Ltd. 38(5): 704–718.
Huntington, S. P. 1999. The lonely superpower. Foreign Affairs, Miami Herald, The 2003. Ban on Japan’s imports to end in
78(2): 35–49. celebration. December 31: http://www.mercurynews.com.
Jain, S. P., & Maheswaran, D. 2000. Motivated reasoning: A Morris, M. W., & Peng, K. 1994. Culture and cause: American
depth-of-processing perspective. Journal of Consumer Research, and Chinese attributions for social and physical events. Journal
26(4): 358–371. of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(2): 949–971.
Jung, K., Ang, S. H., Leong, S. M., Tan, S. J., Pornpitakpan, C., & Mullen, M. R. 1995. Diagnosing measurement equivalence in
Kau, A. K. 2002. A typology of animosity and its cross-national cross-national research. Journal of International Business Stu-
validation. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 33(6): 525–539. dies, 26(3): 573–596.
Khermouch, G., Brady, D., Holmes, S., Ihlwan, M., Kripalani, M., Newman, B. I. 1999. The mass marketing of politics: Democracy in
& Picard, J. 2003. Brands in an age of anti-Americanism. an age of manufactured images. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
BusinessWeek Asian Edition, August 4: http://www.business- New Republic, The 1999. The plot thickens. December 6: 18–20.
week.com. Nijssen, E. J., & Douglas, S. P. 2004. Examining the animosity
Klein, J. G. 2002. Us versus them, or us versus everyone? model in a country with a high level of foreign trade.
Delineating consumer aversion to foreign goods. Journal of International Journal of Research in Marketing, 21(1): 23–38.
International Business Studies, 33(2): 345–363. Parmar, A. 2004. Drink politics. Marketing News, February 5: 1,
Klein, J. G., & Ettenson, R. 1999. Consumer animosity and 11–12.
consumer ethnocentrism: An analysis of unique antecedents. Pereira, B. 1998. Economic crisis kills off 2,000 firms. The Straits
Journal of International Consumer Marketing, 11(4): 5–24. Times, April 20: 59.

Journal of International Business Studies


Consumer animosity in an international crisis Siew Meng Leong et al
1008

Rawwas, M. Y. A., Rajendran, K. N., & Wuehrer, G. A. 1996. The 1. The Americans/Japanese caused my country’s
influence of worldmindedness and nationalism on consumer
evaluation of domestic and foreign products. International
current economic problems.
Marketing Review, 13(2): 20–38. 2. I blame the American/Japanese government for
Sharma, S., Mukherjee, S., Kumar, A., & Dillon, W. R. 2005. A my country’s current economic problems.
simulation study to investigate the use of cutoff values for
assessing model fit in covariance structure analysis. Journal of 3. We are poorer now because of what Americans/
Business Research, 58(7): 935–943. Japanese business people have done.
Shimp, T. A., & Sharma, S. 1987. Consumer ethnocentrism: 4. Our currency is worth less now because of a(n)
Construction and validation of the CETSCALE. Journal of
Marketing Research, 24(3): 280–289. American/Japanese conspiracy.
Shimp, T. A., Dunn, T. H., & Klein, J. G. 2004. Remnants of the
US Civil War and modern consumer behavior. Psychology & External control (average composite reliability¼
Marketing, 21(2): 75–91. 0.57; average variance extracted¼0.36)
Slater, J. 1999. Squeezing the middlemen. Far Eastern Economic
Review, 162 (January 14): 20–21. 1. Whatever the Americans/Japanese do will affect
Steenkamp, J.-B., & Baumgartner, H. 1998. Assessing measure-
ment invariance in cross-national consumer research. Journal my country’s current economic problems.
of Consumer Research, 25(1): 78–90. 2. American/Japanese business people can solve my
Tefft, S. 1994. China’s savvy shoppers load carts with expensive
imported goods. Advertising Age, June 20: 1–21.
country’s economic problems.
Tse, D. K., Gu, F. F., & Yim, C. K. 2005. National animosity: 3. The Americans/Japanese could have prevented
Conceptualization, measurement characteristics, and salience in my country’s economic problems from happening.
international business decisions. Working paper, School of
Business, University of Hong Kong.
Usunier, J. C. 1993. International marketing: A cultural approach. Cognitive judgment (average composite reliability
London: Prentice-Hall. ¼0.78; average variance extracted¼0.50)
Vence, D. L. 2004. You talkin’ to me? Marketing News, March 1:
1, 9–11. 1. Products made in America/Japan are carefully
Weiner, B. 1986. An attributional theory of motivation and produced and have fine workmanship.
emotion. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Weiner, B. 2000. Attributional thoughts about consumer 2. Products made in America/Japan show a very
behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 27(3): 382–387. high degree of technological advancement.
3. Products made in America/Japan usually show a
APPENDIX very clever use of color and design.
4. Products made in America/Japan are usually
CONSTRUCT MEASURES
quite reliable and seem to last the desired length
Stable animosity (average composite reliability¼
of time.
0.66; average variance extracted¼0.49)
5. Products made in America/Japan are usually
1. America/Japan is a friend of my country only in good value for the money.
good times.
2. America/Japan has never been fair to my country. Affective evaluation (average composite reliability¼
3. America/Japan is not reliable. 0.82; average variance extracted¼0.65)

Situational animosity (average composite reliability 1. I dislike American/Japanese products. (Reversed)


¼0.84; average variance extracted¼0.58) 2. American/Japanese products do not appeal to
me. (Reversed)
1. I feel upset that the Americans/Japanese caused 3. I do not trust American/Japanese products.
my friends to lose their jobs during this economic (Reversed)
crisis. 4. I do not favor buying American/Japanese products.
2. I resent the Americans/Japanese for making my (Reversed)
family poorer.
3. I find it hard to forgive the Americans/Japanese Willingness to buy (average composite reliability¼
for exploiting me during this economic crisis. 0.73; average variance extracted¼0.53)
4. The Americans/Japanese have made me feel
1. I would feel guilty if I bought a(n) American/
more insecure now.
Japanese product. (Reversed)
5. The Americans/Japanese have made me more
2. From now on, I am less willing to buy American/
anxious about my future.
Japanese products. (Reversed)
6. I am unhappy with the Americans/Japanese as
3. Whenever possible, I avoid buying American/
they have gained a lot from this economic crisis.
Japanese products. (Reversed)
External attribution (average composite reliability¼ 4. I do not like the idea of owning American/
0.82; average variance extracted¼0.66) Japanese products. (Reversed)

Journal of International Business Studies


Consumer animosity in an international crisis Siew Meng Leong et al
1009

ABOUT THE AUTHORS native of Singapore. Her research interests are in the
Siew Meng Leong is Professor of Marketing at the areas of global marketing strategies, consumer
National University of Singapore (NUS) Business values and lifestyles, game theoretic applications
School. He obtained his PhD from the University of in marketing, and new product and brand
Wisconsin at Madison and is a native of Singapore. management. She can be reached at biztansj@
His research in consumer behavior and interna- nus.edu.sg.
tional marketing has been published in Journal of
Marketing, Journal of Marketing Research, and Journal
of Consumer Research. He can be reached at Kwon Jung is Associate Professor of Marketing, KDI
bizlsm@nus.edu.sg. School of Public Policy and Management. He
received his PhD from University of Illinois,
Urbana-Champaign and is a native of Korea. He is
Joseph A Cote is Professor of Marketing at interested in cross-cultural research, marketing
Washington State University, Vancouver. He simulation exercises, and lifestyle segmentation
obtained his PhD from the University of Arizona and can be reached at jungk@kdischool.ac.kr.
and is a US citizen. His research interests include
consumer behavior and marketing theory. He has
published in Journal of Consumer Research, Journal of Ah Keng Kau is deputy director of the NUS
Marketing Research, Journal of Marketing, and Journal Entrepreneurship Centre. He received his PhD from
of Applied Psychology. He can be reached at cote@ the London Graduate School of Business Studies
wsu.edu. and is a native of Malaysia. His research focuses on
entrepreneurship and consumer behavior. He has
published in Journal of Marketing Research, Interna-
Swee Hoon Ang is Associate Professor of Marketing tional Marketing Review, Psychology and Marketing,
at the NUS Business School. She received her PhD and Social Indicators Research. He can be reached at
from the University of British Columbia and is a neckauak@nus.edu.sg.
citizen of Singapore. Her research in consumer
behavior, advertising, and cross-cultural issues
have been published in Journal of the Academy of Chanthika Pornpitakpan is Associate Professor of
the Marketing Science, Journal of Advertising, and the Faculty of Business Administration, University
Marketing Letters. She can be reached at bizangsh@ of Macau. She obtained her PhD from University of
nus.edu.sg. British Columbia and is a citizen of Thailand. Her
research interests are in cross-cultural consumer
behavior, persuasion, consumer information pro-
Soo Jiuan Tan is Associate Professor of Marketing at cessing, advertising and packaging effects, brand
the NUS Business School. She obtained her PhD management, and personality assessment. She can
from Washington University in St Louis and is a be reached at ynvynv@yahoo.com.

Accepted by David Tse, Departmental Editor, 20th June 2007. This paper has been with the authors for two revisions.

Journal of International Business Studies

Você também pode gostar