Você está na página 1de 9

Neuropharmacology 61 (2011) 355e363

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Neuropharmacology
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neuropharm

Invited review

Serotonin transporter and memory


Alfredo Meneses a, *, Georgina Perez-Garcia a, Teresa Ponce-Lopez a, Ruth Tellez a, Carlos Castillo b
a
Depto. de Farmacobiología, CINVESTAV-IPN, Tenorios 235, Granjas Coapa, Mexico City 14330, Mexico
b
Escuela Superior de Medicina del IPN, Mexico City, Mexico

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The serotonin transporter (SERT) has been associated to diverse functions and diseases, though seldom to
Received 1 October 2010 memory. Therefore, we made an attempt to summarize and discuss the available publications implicating
Received in revised form the involvement of the SERT in memory, amnesia and anti-amnesic effects. Evidence indicates that
15 December 2010
Alzheimer’s disease and drugs of abuse like d-methamphetamine (METH) and (þ/)3,4-methyl-
Accepted 10 January 2011
enedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA, “ecstasy”) have been associated to decrements in the SERT
expression and memory deficits. Several reports have indicated that memory formation and amnesia
Keywords:
affected the SERT expression. The SERT expression seems to be a reliable neural marker related to
Serotonin
Transporter
memory mechanisms, its alterations and potential treatment. The pharmacological, neural and molecular
Memory mechanisms associated to these changes are of great importance for investigation.
Amnesia This article is part of a Special Issue entitled ‘Serotonin: The New Wave’.
AD Ó 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Schizophrenia
Drugs
Autoshaping
Memory tasks
Rats
Humans

1. Introduction & learning” in the PuBmed resulted into only 77 publications of


which 7 are reviews (November, 2010) stressing out the limited
In the last few years, serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine, 5-HT) has publication on this subject; but growing interest (see e.g., February,
become one of the neurotransmission systems which engaged 3, 2011: 122 publications, including 14 reviews).
growing interest in the area of learning and memory (for recent The SERT is localized on terminals of 5-HT neurons, ensures the
reviews see e.g., Bert et al., 2008; Dayan and Huys, 2009; Eagle recapture of 5-HT and is the pharmacological target of selective
et al., 2008; Francis, 2008; Gold, 2008; King et al., 2008; reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) mainly used as antidepressants (Baudry
Mendelsohn et al., 2009; Ogren et al., 2008; Perez-Garcia and et al., 2010). The SERT has been implicated in diverse functions (e.g.,
Meneses, 2008; Robbins and Roberts, 2007; Terry et al., 2008). neuroendocrine, sleep, body temperature, etc.) and diseases (e.g.,
Evidence indicates that brain areas implicated in these cognitive obsessiveecompulsive disorder, depression, bipolar disorder,
processes (Zola-Morgan and Squire, 1993) receive 5-HT pathways motor abnormalities, etc.) (see e.g., Caligiuri and Buitenhuys, 2005;
(Jacobs and Azmitia, 1992; Steinbush, 1984), including the Murphy et al., 2008). The SERT has been used as an index of
prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, amygdala, etc (Meneses, 1999). integrity of the axon terminals of brain serotonergic neurons
5-HT exerts its influence via 14 receptors, which have been char- (Buchert et al., 2006). The lack of SERT in brain areas such as the
acterized genetically, pharmacologically and functionally (Fink and neocortex, hippocampus, amygdala and cingulate cortex has been
Göthert, 2007; Hannon and Hoyer, 2008) and the reuptake of 5-HT associated to lobus pallidus hyper-innervated and greater levels of
synaptic levels is regulated by 5-HT transporter “SERT” (see e.g., 5-HT axonal markers (Selvaraj et al., 2009). 5-HT has been impli-
Hensler, 2006; Kalueff et al., 2010; Murphy et al., 2008). While the cated in Alzheimer’s disease (AD), for instance SERT expression was
number of publications about 5-HT receptors and memory is decreased in patients with AD into several brain areas (see below).
growing, the wording “SERT & memory or serotonergic transporter In addition, pharmacological and genetic manipulations of the SERT
are known to modify memory performance in human and rodents
(see e.g., Chow et al., 2007; Meneses, 1999, 2002, 2007b; Monleón
* Corresponding author. Tel.: þ52 55 54832869; fax: þ52 55 54832863. et al., 2008; Savaskan et al., 2008). Emerging evidence indicates
E-mail address: ameneses@msn.com (A. Meneses). that learning and memory modify the SERT expression. Hence,

0028-3908/$ e see front matter Ó 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.01.018
356 A. Meneses et al. / Neuropharmacology 61 (2011) 355e363

in this review an attempt was made to summarize and discuss et al., 2010). Preclinical evidence showed that in a triple trans-
available evidence about the interaction between SERT and genic (expressing both plaques amyloid beta; Ab and neurofibril-
memory, with focus on disorders such as AD as well as on phar- lary tangles) mouse model of AD (Noristani et al., 2010),
macological and genetic manipulations of SERT. As the SERT a significant increase in SERT fibres in the hippocampus CA1 in
expression is affected by several drugs of abuse (e.g., metham- a subfield-, strata- and age-specific manner, at 3 months (by 61%)
phetamine, ecstasy), which seem to be associated to memory and at 18 months (by 74%). These results may indicate an associ-
deficits (see e.g., Chummun et al., 2010; McCann et al., 2008; ation (or link to) AD cognitive impairment and imbalanced sero-
Thomasius et al., 2006 for reviews), hence the role of the SERT tonergic neurotransmission. Recent experimental studies involving
expression in amnesia models induced by these drugs is also dis- healthy human volunteers have revealed that manipulations of the
cussed. Although the focus of this review is the SERT, it should be central 5-HT system can produce quite specific changes in cogni-
noted that AD is associated with changes in a variety of 5-HT tive functioning, independent of overt mood changes (see e.g.,
markers (see e.g., King et al., 2008; Meneses, 1999; Meneses and Schmitt et al., 2006). Dual inhibitors of acetylcholinesterase and
Perez-Garcia, 2007; Terry et al., 2008) as in MDMA-induced 5-HT 5-HT uptake has been tested in rodents and it has been proposed
neurotoxicity (see below). as possible treatment of Alzheimer’s disease (Abe et al., 2003; see
also Mowla et al., 2007; Orjales et al., 2003; Toda et al., 2010).
2. Alzheimer’s disease and SERT Further investigation is necessary to clarify the interaction of the
SERT, memory and AD; however, preclinical pharmacological and
Probably the most known neurological alteration associated to genetic manipulations of the SERT have already revealed signifi-
memory deficit is the AD. The most commonly recognized cant clues.
symptom in the early stages of AD is memory loss, such as diffi-
culty in remembering recently learned facts. As the disease 3. Pharmacological manipulation of SERT and memory
advances, more symptoms arise like confusion, irritability and
aggression, mood swings, language breakdown, long-term Pharmacological manipulations of the SERT has a long tradition
memory loss, and the general withdrawal of the sufferer as their regarding memory (see Altman and Normile, 1988; Chow et al.,
senses decline (Arnsten and van Dyck, 1997; Meltzer et al., 1998; 2007; Flood and Cherkin, 1987; Meneses, 1999, 2002, 2007b;
Michelsen et al., 2008). AD has also been related to the SERT (for Meneses and Hong, 1995; Monleón et al., 2008; Savaskan et al.,
reviews see Arnsten and van Dyck, 1997; Azmitia and Whitaker- 2008). Thus, while pre-training administration of SSRIs has
Azmitia, 1997; Meltzer et al., 1998; Solodkin and van Hoesen, shown to either improve (with some isolated exceptions), have no
1997; Smith and Lakoski, 1997; Michelsen et al., 2008). For effect on memory or undermine it, post-training administration of
instance, the SERT expression is found to be decreased in patients SSRIs (e.g., fluoxetine, sertraline) has been demonstrated to
with AD into several brain areas such as hippocampus, frontal, improve memory or no change (see Monleón et al., 2008; and also
temporal and entorhinal cortices, putamen and dorsal raphe nuclei Meneses, 2002). Of the available memory tasks (Lynch, 2002;
(Arnsten and van Dyck, 1997; Azmitia and Whitaker-Azmitia, 1997; Myhrer, 2003; Peele and Vincent, 1989; Meneses and Perez-
Belcher et al., 2005; Lai et al., 2007; Solodkin and van Hoesen, Garcia, 2007) among those most used are: the water maze, novel
1997; Tejani-Butt et al., 1995; Terry et al., 2008). Serotonergic object recognition, passive avoidance, T-maze, stone-maze,
dysfunction appears to be closely linked to the behavioral aspects autoshaping (see e,g., Altman and Normile, 1988; Dere et al., 2007;
of AD (see Meltzer et al., 1998), including depression, aggressive Ennaceur and Delacour, 1988; Flood and Cherkin, 1987; McAdam
behavior and psychosis. Only scarce information is known about et al., 2008; Meneses, 2003, 2007b; Meneses and Hong, 1995).
AD, selective 5-HT reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) and memory func- Chronic fluoxetine did not affect matching-to-place or reference-
tions or dysfunctions. It is known that administration of citalopram memory performance in intact rats in the Morris water-maze task.
to healthy subjects has no effect on the working memory (Rose Surprisingly, chronic fluoxetine adversely affected recovery of
et al., 2006), however in patients with AD both fluoxetine and function on both tasks was in rats with dentate gyrus damage
sertraline have improved cognition (Lyketsos et al., 2000; Mowla (Keith et al., 2007). In contrast, chronically fluoxetine-injected rats
et al., 2007; Mossello et al., 2008; see also Wuwongse et al., did not show any impairment relative to the saline controls in
2010). Moreover, Chow et al. (2007) have highlighted that some either the acquisition or the retention phases of the water maze
SSRIs could modify AD through their effects on amyloid plaques (hippocampal-dependent task; but see Box 1), however they did
formation or hippocampal neurogenesis (see also Aboukhatwa spent significantly less time exploring the novel object in the novel

Table 1
Summary of data obtained in the autoshaping task in diverse laboratories.

Role of incentive salience, amygdala, opioid receptors and individual differences Mahler and Berridge, 2009
Determining individual differences, role of dopamine system and addiction Flagel et al., 2007, 2008, 2009
Detecting expression of 5-HT1A and 5-HT2 receptors protein in hippocampus and cortex Tomie et al., 2003
Detecting differences between strains of dextroamphetamine (AMPH) intracranial Rodriguez et al., 2008
self-administration (ICSA) into the nucleus accumbens
Measuring noradrenalin and 5-HT release Tomie et al., 2004
Separating the contributions of the orbitofrontal and infralimbic cortex to memory Chudasama and Robbins, 2003
Studying operant learning in 5-HT1A and 5-HT1B receptor knockout mice Pattij et al., 2003
Determining the inhibitory role of hippocampus Ito et al., 2005
Determining the basal forebrain involvement Steckler et al., 1993
Studying the participation of dopaminergic and glutamatergic receptors Dalley et al., 2005
Facilitatory effects of 5-HT6 receptor antagonists Schreiber et al., 2006, 2007
Effects of the cholinesterase inhibitor physostigmine Sparber, 2001
Facilitatory effects of 5-HT6 receptor antagonists in other memory tasks Hirst et al., 2006; Lieben et al., 2005;
King et al., 2004; Mitchell et al., 2006;
Schreiber et al., 2007; Woolley et al., 2003
A. Meneses et al. / Neuropharmacology 61 (2011) 355e363 357

object recognition (NOR) task and needed more time to learn the and Colombo, 2002; Schreiber et al., 2007). Thus, Pavlovian/
association between the conditional stimulus and the appetitive instrumental autoshaping learning task is mediated by brain
unconditional stimulus in the appetitive Pavlovian-conditioning areas involved in explicit and implicit memory (Perez-Garcia
task (according with Valluzzi and Chan, 2007 both tasks are and Meneses, 2008; Tellez et al., 2010). Certainly, habit and
hippocampal-independent). Although this is consistent with the autoshaping have been linked (see e.g., Correa, 2007; also
evidence that fluoxetine pre-training administration impairs Fone, 2008; King et al., 2008), however it is important to
memory (see e.g., Meneses, 2002; Monleón et al., 2008); it is remember that the learning of explicit rules are used to learn
noteworthy that in the short-delay Pavlovian-conditioning task of even a sequence of motor responses and once the behaviors
are repeated or well-learned, they become automatic and can
12 sessions, significant deficits on memory were observed in the
be called habits (see e.g., Izquierdo et al., 2006). This is
fluoxetine-treated groups only during the sessions 3e5 relative to
observable in diverse memory tasks, including the Morris
control animals (Valluzzi and Chan, 2007; see also Tellez et al., water maze, passive avoidance and autoshaping. Multiple
2008, 2010). These data suggest that SSRIs have a specific time- memory mechanisms can work in tandem to support perfor-
window to have an impairment effect on memory consolidation. As mance on an implicit memory task and even additional
mentioned before, acute fluoxetine administration after memory contribution of explicit memory can be observed in neuro-
training improved the memory consolidation in a Pavlovian/ logically healthy individuals (Koenig et al., 2008) and during
instrumental autoshaping (Meneses and Hong, 1995) or passive acquisition of an implicit learning task. Thus, during memory
avoidance task (Izquierdo et al., 2006; for review see Monleón et al., consolidation hippocampus is in charge and when perfor-
2008). These contradictory findings make it necessary to revise mance become asymptotic or “automatic”, engaging striatum
(and other areas including cortical ones). Instrumental
very briefly the interaction among brain areas, neurotransmitters
learning requires repeated protein synthesis in the nucleus
systems, drug administration, cognitive and behavioral demand of accumbens (Hernandez et al., 2002; Hernandez and Kelley,
learning tasks and the extent of training (see Box 1). 2004) and takes time to become observable. For instance,
vehicle-treated rats begin to consistently respond on the
correct lever starting approximately on training day 4, and
their responses progressively increased over subsequent
Box 1. Types of memory and behavioral tasks sessions; in contrast, animals given post-trial high dose of
anisomycin (a protein inhibitor) show evidence of learning
Memory may be defined according to its content or in rela- until day 7, two days after drug treatment had ended, even-
tion to time and neurobiological basis: in the former case, as tually reaching the control levels.
declarative/explicit or non-declarative/implicit memory, and
regarding time, as short- or working, and long-term memory Moreover, while autoshaping memory is progressive (0
(Meneses and Perez-Garcia, 2007; Meneses et al., 2010). e120 h), mRNA 5-HT6 receptor expression is not modified; in
Explicit memory is related to hippocampus and implicit is contrast, 5-HT1A or 5-HT7 receptors expression monotonically
related to striatum (see e.g., Adamantidis and de Lecea, 2009; augmented or declined in prefrontal cortex, hippocampus and
Kandel, 2001). Accordingly, behavioral memory tasks have raphe nuclei, respectively. However, under autoshaping
been classified by implication, lesion studies, neurobiolog- memory facilitation (with the 5-HT6 receptor antagonist SB-
ical changes, etc. For instance, the Morris water maze (the 399885) or amnesic states a differential 5-HT6 receptor mRNA
platform hidden and prove trials version) is the classical expression is observed (Huerta-Rivas et al., 2010; Meneses
example of memory hippocampus-dependent (Schenk and et al., 2007; Perez-Garcia and Meneses, 2009a,b; for recent
Morris, 1985; D’Hooge and De Deyn, 2001; Gerlai, 2001). review see Meneses et al., 2011). The 5-HT6 receptor mRNA
Other memory tasks requiring of hippocampus include a higher degree in prefrontal cortex, hippocampus and stria-
passive avoidance (see e.g., Izquierdo et al., 1999), fear tum in the amnesic model of scopolamine, while in the dizo-
conditioning (Albrechet-Souza et al., 2010) and Pavlovian/ cilpine model increased it more in the striatum. Memory and
instrumental autoshaping (Meneses, 2003; Meneses et al., mRNA are reestablished by SB-399885 (Huerta-Rivas et al.,
2009, 2011; Tellez et al., 2010). However, overtrained 2010). Similar complex changes are observed between 5-HT
animals engage more striatum and less hippocampus on receptors and cAMP (Perez-Garcia and Meneses, 2008). Con-
behavioral tasks such as autoshaping, passive avoidance firming the interaction of time, memory tasks and neural
(e.g., Meneses, 2003; Izquierdo et al., 2006; Tellez et al., 2010; markers, evidence from rats overexpressing dorsomedial
see also Talpos et al., 2010). striatum, but not dorsocentral striatum, 5-HT6 receptors
shows impaired performance in a simple operant learning
Autoshaping has been used to study diverse problems, task (a striatum-dependent learning model), but not in the
including brain areas mediating memory, neurotransmission hippocampus-dependent water-maze task (Mitchell et al.,
systems, addiction, genetic deletions, etc. (Table 1). Even 2007). This impairment effect was appreciable at the third
comparable effects to those observed in autoshaping have testing session or during the second extinction session of
reported in social recognition, Morris water maze, spatial (Mitchell et al., 2007). Hence, an important clarification about
DNMTS, etc (Millan et al., 2004). Pavlovian/instrumental “cognitive” (explicit) and “habit” (implicit) memory consists
autoshaping memory formation, resembles situations of self- of the timing of observation. This notion is important in the
taught (Meneses et al., 2009) and recruit diverse neural context of investigation of potential memory and amnesia
markers, including the SERT and 5-HT receptors expression in markers and loci.
brain areas such as dentate gyrus, hippocampal CA1, baso-
lateral amygdaloid nucleus and prefrontal cortex, striatum
(Perez-Garcia and Meneses, 2008; Tellez et al., 2010). Hipo-
campi of untrained vs. autoshaping trained control groups 4. SSRI and models of amnesia
showed hippocampal 315 up-regulated genes, including
dopamine D1 and D5, a-adrenergic1d, NMDA and AMPA
Some drugs induce memory impairments that resemble, in part,
receptors; and 365 down-regulated: 5-HT2C,4,6,7; b-adrenergic,
etc (Perez-Garcia and Meneses, 2009a,b). Hippocampal those observed in disorders such as AD, schizophrenia or drugs
lesions impaired memory consolidation in diverse memory causing addiction; including cholinergic (e.g., scopolamine) and
tasks, including autoshaping (Good and Macphail, 1994; Hall glutamatergic (e.g., dizocilpine, ketamine) antagonists, drugs of
et al., 1996; Ito et al., 2005; Liy-Salmeron and Meneses, 2007, abuse (e.g., methamphetamine, ecstasy), protein synthesis inhibi-
2008; Meneses, 2007a,b; Reilly and Good, 1989; Richmond tors (e.g., anisomycin), etc. Especially, the cholinergic or
358 A. Meneses et al. / Neuropharmacology 61 (2011) 355e363

glutamatergic antagonists represent partial models for amnesia resonance imaging, no performance deficits were evident in
and they have been very useful in determining whether SSRIs ecstasy (slight) users (Roberts and Garavan, 2010). Nevertheless,
reverse or prevent memory deficits. For instance, repeated whether ecstasy is contributing to or arising from using it, a dys-
administration of SSRIs such as fluvoxamine reverses the memory regulation in brain regions subserving cognitive function and
deficit induced by phencyclidine (PCP) a N-methyl-D-aspartate default-mode processes in current recreational drug users mirrored
receptor antagonist in the NOR test (Hashimoto et al., 2007). Also, effects previously observed for “harder” drugs of abuse (Roberts
citalopram reversed scopolamine-induced impairment of spatial and Garavan, 2010). Moreover, even though there is no clear
memory in the radial maze (Egashira et al., 2006). The post-training evidence supporting an interaction between harmful effects in
administration of the 5-HT uptake facilitator tianeptine (Fattaccini ecstasy users and age-related memory decline or mid-life depres-
et al., 1990) enhanced memory consolidation, normalized the sion (Schilt et al., 2010), certainly memory deficits and the SERT
impaired memory induced by scopolamine or dizocilpine, and it expression seem to be related in AD (see above).
partially reversed the memory deficit induced by TFMPP (5-HT1B/
1D/2Ae2C/7 agonist/antagonist) in the autoshaping Pavlovian/ 5. Genetic manipulation of SERT
instrumental learning task (Meneses, 2002). It should be noted that
glutamatergic antagonists such phencyclidine (PCP), ketamine and The importance of SERT on memory is substantiated by the
MK801 (dizocilpine) are used to produce cognitive disturbances, result that SERT (/) rats with acute tryptophan depletion
which are relevant to schizophrenia and memory deficits associ- showed impaired STM in the NOR task (Olivier et al., 2008). Inter-
ated to this disease (see e.g., Neill et al., 2010; also Liy-Salmeron and estingly, Kalueff et al. (2010) revised several aspects of SERT (/)
Meneses, 2008). Likewise, in pharmacological or pathological mice and rats, including cognitive functions. For instance, in mice
amnesia models such as abuse of drugs, the SERT and amnesia have no effects were observed on spatial working memory (open field
been linked. Few studies in humans have reported a loss of the SERT and elevated plus maze habituation) and Pavlovian-to-Instru-
in methamphetamine (METH) users, likely since primates are far mental transfer task; nonetheless, SERT deletion impaired spatial
less vulnerable to methamphetamine-induced 5-HT injury than to reversal learning task. In the Morris water-maze SERT (/) rats
dopamine injury (Easton and Marsden, 2006; Nordahl et al., 2003). took longer to find the hidden platform. According to Kalueff et al.
However, METH and (þ/)3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (2010), the absence of the SERT slightly impairs hippocampus-
(MDMA, “ecstasy”), reduced brain SERT density, which was asso- dependent spatial/object memory, in striking contrast with
ciated to cognitive deficits (Herring et al., 2008; McCann et al., improved amygdala-dependent emotional memory (e.g., fear
2008; Marshall et al., 2007; Scott et al., 2007). For instance, absti- conditioning) in SERT (/) rodents.
nent MDMA users with a history of using substantial MDMA (two
or more doses over a 3- to 12-h period) and age-, gender-, and 6. Are SERT and memory related?
education-matched controls participated (McCann et al., 2008).
Subjects in a positron emission tomography study to measure the Regarding whether or not SERT is involved in memory forma-
dopamine transporter (DAT) and SERT binding [11C]WIN 35,428 tion and/or amnesia, recent evidence indicates that MDMA
and [11C]DASB. MDMA users displayed significant reductions in pretreatment led to chronic unpredictable stress-induced learning
SERT binding in multiple brain regions (dorsolateral prefrontal, impairment in the Morris water maze and dramatic reductions of
orbitofrontal and parietal cortices), but were not observed in the SERT protein in MDMA-treated animals (Cunningham et al.,
striatal DAT binding. Memory performance in the aggregate subject 2009). In addition, autoshaping trained rats decreased cortical
population was correlated with SERT binding in brain regions SERT binding relative to untrained ones (Tellez et al., 2010).
implicated in memory function and prior exposure to MDMA Administration of amnesic doses of METH to trained and untrained
significantly diminished the strength of this relationship. These animals decreased the SERT binding in several areas including
data are the first to directly relate memory performance to human hippocampus and cortex (see Fig. 1). Interestingly, in the trained
brain SERT density (McCann et al., 2008). Importantly, SERT- animals fluoxetine improved memory, increased SERT binding,
immunoreactive fiber density is significantly reduced in the prevented the METH amnesic effect and reestablished the SERT
hippocampus but not in the neocortex of MDMA users, suggesting binding (Tellez et al., 2010). Thus, SERT expression might be
that the hippocampus may be particularly vulnerable to moderate important for memory formation, amnesia and the reestablishment
MDMA exposure during adolescence (Meyer et al., 2008). of memory. Apparently memory consolidation, and in a major
Furthermore, MDMA use has been associated with impairments of degree, amnesia make the SERT vulnerable to the effects of METH.
psychological well-being, verbal memory and altered serotonergic On the other hand, there is the possibility that memory perfor-
functioning in a number of cross-sectional studies (Thomasius mance and SERT expression are not related, since the effects of
et al., 2006). These authors have noted that reduced SERT avail- drugs such as fluoxetine and METH on the SERT expression might
ability might be a direct effect of heavy ecstasy use, since it partially appear to be more of a pharmacological nature rather than related
recovered when the current users reduced their MDMA consume. to memory. METH produces amnesia in diverse memory tasks,
Nonetheless, Thomasius et al. (2006) have observed that this including NOR (Kalueff et al., 2010; Tellez et al., 2010), egocentric
measure may not necessarily be a valid indicator of the number or learning in the Cincinnati water maze (Vorhees et al., 2010), water
integrity of serotonergic neurons. Verbal memory of ex-ecstasy maze (Camarasa et al., 2010; Yamazaki et al., 1995; but see Schröder
users did not improve even after 2.5 or more years of abstinence, et al., 2003), passive avoidance (Jia et al., 2008; but see Timár et al.,
therefore this effect may represent persistent functional conse- 2003), fear conditioning (Balci et al., 2008) and conditioned taste
quences of MDMA neurotoxicity; notwithstanding, pre-existing aversion (Harrod et al., 2010). Although these memory tasks have
group differences cannot be completely excluded. Likewise, both different behavioral, cognitive and neural demand (see e.g., Box 1;
semantic knowledge and retrieval are impaired in ecstasy users and see also Meneses et al., 2011), METH mainly affects the memory.
the verbal fluency deficit may be attributable to a disruption of Confirming the notion that METH acts on memory, recent evidence
frontal-striatal circuits directly related with the 5-HT function as shows that this drug in untrained animals slightly reduced SERT,
well as a depletion of lexical-semantic stores mediated by temporal but when it produced amnesia in autoshaping trained rats a higher
structures (Fagundo et al., 2010). Apparently, during performance reduction of the SERT expression occurred in specific regions,
of a response-inhibition GO/NOGO task using functional magnetic including cortices (prefrontal, cingulate, perirhinal, entorhinal,
A. Meneses et al. / Neuropharmacology 61 (2011) 355e363 359

Fig. 1. Distribution of SERT in rat brains after various experimental treatments as revealed by [3H]citalopram binding autoradiography. Representative Autoradiograms from control
untrained and trained treated animal, made of series of consecutive coronal sections, from rostral (top) to caudal (down). Optical density readings are represented from colors,
which were added following optical density readings and represent from (red) strong expression to weak (blue and purple). Strong (black) to weak (grey) binding. Distance: 200 m.
(For interpretation of the references to color in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)

etc.), lateral septum, hippocampus (dentate gyrus, CA1 and CA3 realized in vitro and vice versa. Taking this into account and
areas), basal ganglia (accumbens nucleus and caudate putamen), considering transient actions, it is reasonable to assume that reduced
amygdala (basolateral, basomedial nuclei and stria terminal), and SERT expression is accompanied with memory consolidation, which
the dorsal and median raphe nuclei and the ventral tegmental area might be related to a selective modulation of 5-HT concentration.
(see Fig. 1). In short, while memory formation is accompanied by Indeed it has been found that memory modifies 5-HT release, for
a moderate and selective reduction, confirming the previous sug- example in the matching-to-sample task in pigeons which shows
gesting that memory makes the SERT more liable to the effects of that WM is accompanied by a prefrontal cortex but not a striatal
METH (Tellez et al., 2010). Together with this, the use of substantial release of 5-HT (Karakuyu et al., 2007). In contrast, well-trained rats
MDMA doses is also associated with lasting decreases in human in the radial maze showed hippocampal acetylcholine increased
brain SERT (McCann et al., 2008). Thus, drugs of abuse like METH levels during the waiting period and further increases during the
and MDMA provoked cognitive deficits and reduced human SERT radial-maze performance, while 5-HT levels did not change during
density (Marshall et al., 2007; Scott et al., 2007) in the dorsolateral the waiting period (Stancampiano et al., 1999). These authors
prefrontal, orbitofrontal and parietal cortices (McCann et al., 2008). suggest that: (i) hippocampal acetylcholine could be involved in
attentional and cognitive functions underlying motivational
7. Possible mechanisms for SERT involvement processes; (ii) 5-HT could be implicated in non-cognitive processes
on memory and amnesia (i.e. in the control of motor and feeding behavior) (Stancampiano
et al., 1999). It should be emphasized that, during the extinction
A key step that determines the intensity and duration of mono- phase 5-HT, but not acetylcholine, release quickly declines (see
amines signaling at synapses is the reuptake of the released 5-HT Stancampiano et al., 1999). This suggests that 5-HT release is
into nerve terminals through the SERT. Thus, SERT mediates reup- involved in memory formation. Further investigation is necessary to
take of 5-HT into presynaptic terminals, fine-tunes serotonergic clarify the issue of 5-HT release, SERT and memory, amnesia and
neurotransmission and inactivates 5-HT. As a logical consequence, forgetting. While autoshaping memory formation was associated to
a reduction of the SERT will reduce 5-HT clearance resulting in down regulation of the SERT expression, an increased SERT expres-
persistently increased concentrations of synaptic 5-HT (see e.g., sion was associated to either facilitation of memory (i.e., fluoxetine
Hensler, 2006). Diverse mechanisms might be responsible for effects administration) or anti-amnesic actions (i.e., fluoxetine plus METH
of the 5-HT inhibitors, MDMA, METH and memory on the SERT, administration) (Tellez et al., 2010). In contrast, the METH-induced
including 5-HT or dopamine release, abnormal glutamatergic/ amnesia was associated to SERT down-regulation. Thus, it is possible
cholinergic function, redistribution of catecholamines from synaptic that higher/lower 5-HT levels are associated to lower/higher SERT
vesicles to the cytosol, induction of reverse transport of transmitter expression, respectively, which should affect 5-HT receptors sensi-
through plasma membrane uptake carriers, etc. (see e.g., Nakagawa tivity and/or up/down regulation. This notion is supported by
and Kaneko, 2008; Siegel et al., 2010; Sulzer et al., 2005). Recent pharmacological evidence indicating that the fluoxetine-facilitation
evidence (Tellez et al., 2008, 2010) clearly indicates to draw caution memory effects are reversed by selective 5-HT1A, 5-HT1B, 5-HT2Ae2C,
about extrapolating conclusions derived from ex vivo studies to those 5-HT3, 5-HT4, 5-HT6 and 5-HT7 receptor antagonists (Meneses, 2002,
360 A. Meneses et al. / Neuropharmacology 61 (2011) 355e363

2007a,b; Meneses and Hong, 1995). Moreover, as already mentioned neural markers for the improvement (and impairment) of memory,
the antidepressant tianeptine (which increases uptake of 5-HT in the as revised here with the SERT. Nonetheless, in the passive avoid-
cortex and hippocampus; see Fattaccini et al., 1990; Kasper and ance memory task, performance may be improved by physical
McEwen, 2008) also enhanced memory consolidation, which was exercise, which decreases the 5-HT level for the hippocampus and
reversed by fluoxetine or 5-HT depletion. the expression of 5-HT1A receptors on the amygdala without
Tianeptineefluoxetine co-administration had no effect on altering the transporter expression (Jen et al., 2008). Notably, in
memory consolidation; nevertheless, the acetylcholinesterase healthy subjects high SERT binding in fronto-striatal regions is
inhibitor, phenserine, potentiated the subeffective tianeptine or associated with better performance on tasks involving executive
fluoxetine doses (Meneses, 2002). Collectively, these data strongly function and logical reasoning but not LTM (Madsen et al., 2010).
suggest that endogenously 5-HT modulates, via uptake sites and 5- To conclude, the SERT expression is important for memory
HT1e7 receptors, memory consolidation; which is consistent with formation, amnesia and in the reestablishment of memory.
the evidence that protein and/or mRNA expression of diverse 5-HT Apparently, memory consolidation and, in major degree, amnesia
receptors are involved in memory consolidation (see Box 1 and make the SERT more vulnerable. Emerging evidence also indicates
Huerta-Rivas et al., 2010; Perez-Garcia and Meneses, 2008) that memory formation and amnesia affect the SERT expression.
and receptors of other neurotransmission systems (Perez-Garcia SERT seem to be reliable neural markers in the understanding of
and Meneses, 2009a,b). Importantly, regulation of SERT function memory mechanisms, its alterations and potential treatment.
can occur at the level of transcription, translation or post-trans- Behavioral and cognitive demands exert differential and selective
lational (see Hensler, 2006), which are also part of the memory influence over pre- and postsynaptic 5-HT markers which might be
signaling cascades (see e.g., Bailey and Kandel, 2008), including exacerbated by amnesic states and/or aging but reversed by phar-
cAMP, protein kinase C (PKC), the neurotrophin Brain-Derived macological treatments.
Neurotrophic Factor (BDNF), cAMP-responsive element binding In addition to the SERT, other 5-HT markers have been related to
protein (CREB)-mediated gene expression and neurogenesis. Post- memory formation and cognitive decline, hence some of them
translational modifications like phosphorylation of serine, threo- might also be useful not only for diagnosis of cognitive decline but
nine, and tyrosine, neurotransmitter release, vesicle trafficking and also for understanding the pathological mechanisms as well as
synaptosomal or synaptosomal-associated proteins, substrates of a basis for development of therapeutics. The memory and molec-
a series of specific protein kinases and their counterparts, and ular mechanisms associated to these changes represent promise
protein phosphatases play a major role in memory as well (Mansuy steps of investigation.
and Shenolikar, 2006; Sunyer et al., 2008). In addition, memory and
METH reduce the SERT expression. Studies concerning these Acknowledgments
underlying molecular mechanisms should also focus on the
expression of 5-HT receptors, CaMKII, as well as on the fact that This work was supported in part by CONACYT grant 80060. We
METH or MDMA may also down-regulate the SERT expression wish to express our thanks for the excellent comments and
(Zahniser and Doolen, 2001). suggestions made for two anonymous referees.
MDMA causes a redistribution of SERT from the cell surface to
the intracellular compartment by a mechanism independent of References
phospho-p38-mitogen activated protein kinase activation (Kivell
et al., 2010). Certainly, more work is necessary to unraveling the Abe, Y., Aoyagi, A., Hara, T., Abe, K., Yamazaki, R., Kumagae, Y., Naruto, S., Koyama, K.,
Marumoto, S., Tago, K., Toda, N., Takami, K., Yamada, N., Ori, M., Kogen, H.,
exact molecular mechanisms. Kaneko, T., 2003. Pharmacological characterization of RS-1259, an orally active
dual inhibitor of acetylcholinesterase and serotonin transporter, in rodents:
8. Serotonergic neurobiological changes and memory possible treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. J. Pharmacol. Sci. 93, 95e105.
Aboukhatwa, M., Dosanjh, L., Luo, Y., 2010. Antidepressants are a rational comple-
mentary therapy for the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. Mol. Neurodegener.
Although the effects of pharmacological and genetic manipu- 5, 10.
lation of 5-HT receptors and SERT on memory have been studied Adamantidis, A., de Lecea, L., 2009. A role for melanin-concentrating hormone in
learning and memory. Peptides 30, 2066e2070.
(see above), there are only a few publications exploring the
Albrechet-Souza, L., Borelli, K.G., Almada, R.C., Brandão, M.L., 2010 Sep 19. Mid-
neurobiological changes associated to this monoamine on azolam reduces the selective activation of the rhinal cortex by contextual fear
mammalian species. For instance, Belcher et al. (2008) reported stimuli. Behav. Brain Res. [Epub ahead of print] PMID: 20851717.
that an effective dose of METH regimen attenuated the acute Altman, H.J., Normile, H.J., 1988. What is the nature of the role of the serotonergic
nervous system in learning and memory: prospects for development of an
hyperthermic response to the subsequent METH binge and pre- effective treatment strategy for senile dementia. Neurobiol. Aging 9, 627e638.
vented the NOR impairments and SERT reductions in striatum, Arnsten, A., van Dyck, C., 1997. Monoamines and acetylcholine influences on higher
hippocampus and perirhinal cortex that otherwise occur one week cognitive functions in nonhuman primates: relevance of the treatment of Alz-
heimer’s disease. In: Brioni, J., Decker, M. (Eds.), Neuropathology and Functional
after the METH binge. In addition, repeated treatment with ser- Anatomy of Alzheimer’s Disease: Pharmacological Treatment of AD. Wiley-Liss,
traline (which has moderate affinity for the dopamine transporter) New York, pp. 63e86.
reduced the SERT expression in the hippocampus and induced Azmitia, E., Whitaker-Azmitia, P., 1997. Development and adult plasticity of sero-
tonergic neurons and their target cells. In: Baumgarten, H., Göthert, M. (Eds.),
a persistent antidepressant-like effect on forced-swim test (Zhao Serotonergic Neurons and 5-HT Receptors in the CNS. Springer, Berlin, pp. 1e39.
et al., 2008). Further investigations are required to determine Bailey, C.H., Kandel, E.R., 2008. Synaptic remodeling, synaptic growth and the
precisely, whether the changes were due to altered Kd or V-max storage of long-term memory in Aplysia. Prog. Brain Res. 169, 179e198.
Balci, F., Papachristos, E.B., Gallistel, C.R., Brunner, D., Gibson, J., Shumyatsky, G.P.,
values. Moreover, the molecular mechanisms of memory remain to 2008. Interval timing in genetically modified mice: a simple paradigm. Genes
be fully established as well as the molecular mechanisms mediating Brain Behav. 7, 373e384.
the effects of fluoxetine and METH on memory. In addition, it is Baudry, A., Mouillet-Richard, S., Schneider, B., Launay, J.M., Kellermann, O., 2010.
MiR-16 targets the serotonin transporter: a new facet for adaptive responses to
important to investigate the effects of METH on dopamine and/or
antidepressants. Science 329 (5998), 1537e1541.
norepinephrine systems and their transporters. Behavioral and Belcher, A.M., O’Dell, S.J., Marshall, J.F., 2005. Impaired object recognition memory
pharmacological controls such as separate groups of drug-treated following methamphetamine, but not p-chloroamphetamine- or d-amphet-
trained rats and for LTM without confound of being tested for STM amine-induced neurotoxicity. Neuropsychopharmacology 30, 2026e2034.
Belcher, A.M., Feinstein, E.M., O’Dell, S.J., Marshall, J.F., 2008. Methamphetamine
while the drugs were on board. It is important to emphasize that influences on recognition memory: comparison of escalating and single-day
the serotonergic system seems to offer different mechanisms and dosing regimens. Neuropsychopharmacology 33, 1453e1463.
A. Meneses et al. / Neuropharmacology 61 (2011) 355e363 361

Bert, B., Fink, H., Rothe, J., Walstab, J., Bönisch, H., 2008. Learning and memory in 5- Hashimoto, K., Fujita, Y., Iyo, M., 2007. Phencyclidine-induced cognitive deficits in
HT1A-receptor mutant mice. Behav. Brain Res. 195, 78e85. mice are improved by subsequent subchronic administration of fluvoxamine:
Buchert, R., Thomasius, R., Petersen, K., Wilke, F., Obrocki, J., Nebeling, B., role of sigma-1 receptors. Neuropsychopharmacology 32, 514e521.
Wartberg, L., Zapletalova, P., Clausen, M., 2006. Reversibility of ecstasy-induced Hensler, J.G., 2006. Serotonin. In: Siegel, G.J., Albers, R.W., Brady, S.T.,
reduction in serotonin transporter availability in polydrug ecstasy users. Eur. J. Price, D.L. (Eds.), Basic Neurochemistry. Elsevier Academic Press, London,
Nucl. Med. Mol. Imaging 33, 188e199. pp. 227e248.
Caligiuri, M.P., Buitenhuys, C., 2005. Do preclinical findings of methamphetamine- Hernandez, P.J., Kelley, A.E., 2004. Long-term memory for instrumental responses
induced motor abnormalities translate to an observable clinical phenotype? does not undergo protein synthesis-dependent reconsolidation upon retrieval.
Neuropsychopharmacology 30, 2125e2134. Learn. Mem. 11, 748e754.
Camarasa, J., Rodrigo, T., Pubill, D., Escubedo, E., 2010 May 27. Memantine is a useful Hernandez, P.J., Sadeghian, K., Kelley, A.E., 2002. Early consolidation of instrumental
drug to prevent the spatial and non-spatial memory deficits induced by learning requires protein synthesis in the nucleus accumbens. Nat. Neurosci. 5,
methamphetamine in rats. Pharmacol. Res. [Epub ahead of print]PMID: 1327e1331.
20553881 [PubMed e as supplied by publisher]. Herring, N.R., Schaefer, T.L., Gudelsky, G.A., Vorhees, C.V., Williams, M.T., 2008. Effect
Chow, T.W., Pollock, B.G., Milgram, N.W., 2007. Potential cognitive enhancing and of þ-methamphetamine on path integration learning, novel object recognition,
disease modification effects of SSRIs for Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychiatr Dis and neurotoxicity in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 199, 637e650.
Treat 3 (5), 627e636. Hirst, W.D., Stean, T.O., Rogers, D.C., Sunter, D., Pugh, P., Moss, S.F., et al., 2006. SB-
Chummun, H., Tilley, V., Ibe, J., 2010. 3,4-methylenedioxyamfetamine (ecstasy) use 399885 is a potent, selective 5-HT6 receptor antagonist with cognitive
reduces cognition. Br. J. Nurs. 19 (2), 94e100. enhancing properties in aged rat water maze and novel object recognition
Correa, M., 2007. Functional neuroanatomy of implicit learning: associative, motor models. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 553, 109e119.
and habit. Rev. Neurol. 44, 234e242. Huerta-Rivas, A., Pérez-García, G., González-Espinosa, C., Meneses, A., 2010 Jan.
Chudasama, Y., Robbins, T.W., 2003. Dissociable contributions of the orbitofrontal Time-course of 5-HT6 receptor mRNA expression during memory consolidation
and infralimbic cortex to Pavlovian autoshaping and discrimination reversal and amnesia. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 93 (1), 99e110.
learning: further evidence for the functional heterogeneity of the rodent frontal Ito, R., Everitt, B.J., Robbins, T.W., 2005. The hippocampus and appetitive Pavlovian
cortex. J. Neurosci. 23, 8771e8780. conditioning: effects of excitotoxic hippocampal lesions on conditioned loco-
Cunningham, J.I., Raudensky, J., Tonkiss, J., Yamamoto, B.K., 2009. MDMA pretreat- motor activity and autoshaping. Hippocampus 15 (6), 713e721.
ment leads to mild chronic unpredictable stress-induced impairments in spatial Izquierdo, I., Medina, J.H., Vianna, M.R., Izquierdo, L.A., Barros, D.M., 1999. Separate
learning. Behav. Neurosci. 123, 1076e1084. mechanisms for short- and long-term memory. Behav. Brain Res. 103, 1e11.
Dalley, J.W., Laane, K., Theobald, D.E., Armstrong, H.C., Corlett, P.R., Chudasama, Y., Izquierdo, I., Bevilaqua, L.R., Rossato, J.I., Bonini, J.S., Da Silva, W.C., Medina, J.H.,
Robbins, T.W., 2005. Time-limited modulation of appetitive Pavlovian memory Cammarota, M., 2006. The connection between the hippocampal and the
by D1 and NMDA receptors in the nucleus accumbens. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. striatal memory systems of the brain: a review of recent findings. Neurotox.
A. 102, 6189e6194. Res. 10, 113e121.
Dayan, P., Huys, Q.J., 2009. Serotonin in affective control. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 32, 95e126. Jacobs, B.L., Azmitia, E.C., 1992. Structure and function of the brain serotonin system.
D’Hooge, R., De Deyn, P.P., 2001. Applications of the Morris water maze in the study Physiol. Rev. 72, 165e229.
of learning and memory. Brain Res. Rev. 36, 60e90. Jen, C.J., Lin, L.C., Wu, F.S., Ho, Y.C., Chen, H.I., 2008. Treadmill exercise enhances
Dere, E., Huston, J.P., De Souza Silva, M.A., 2007. The pharmacology, neuroanatomy passive avoidance learning in rats: the role of down-regulated serotonin system
and neurogenetics of one-trial object recognition in rodents. Neurosci. Bio- in the limbic system. Front. Hum. Neurosci. Conference Abstract: 10th Inter-
behav. Rev. 31, 673e704. national Conference on Cognitive Neuroscience. doi:10.3389/conf.neuro.09.
Eagle, D.M., Bari, A., Robbins, T.W., 2008. The neuropsychopharmacology of action 2009.01.285.
inhibition: cross-species translation of the stop-signal and go/no-go tasks. Jia, F., Mobarakeh, J.I., Dai, H., Kato, M., Xu, A., Okuda, T., Sakurai, E., Okamura, N.,
Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 199, 439e456. Takahashi, K., Yanai, K., 2008. Blocking histamine H(1) improves learning and
Easton, N., Marsden, C.A., 2006. Ecstasy: are animal data consistent between species mnemonic dysfunction in mice with social isolation plus repeated metham-
and can they translate to humans? J. Psychopharmacol. 20, 194e210. phetamine injection. J. Pharmacol. Sci. 107, 167e174.
Egashira, N., Matsumoto, Y., Mishima, K., Iwasaki, K., Fujioka, M., Matsushita, M., Kalueff, A.V., Olivier, J.D.A., Nonkes, L.J.P., Homberg, J.R., 2010. Conserved role for the
Shoyama, Y., Nishimura, R., Fujiwara, M., 2006. Low dose citalopram reverses serotonin transporter gene in rat and mouse neurobehavioral endophenotypes.
memory impairment and electroconvulsive shock-induced immobilization. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34 (3), 373e386.
Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 83, 161e167. Kandel, E.R., 2001. The molecular biology of memory storage: a dialogue between
Ennaceur, A., Delacour, J., 1988. A new one-trial for neurobiological studies of genes and synapses. Science 294 (5544), 1030e1038.
memory in rats. 1 Behavioral data. Behav. Brain Res. 31, 47e59. Karakuyu, D., Herold, C., Güntürkün, O., Diekamp, B., 2007. Differential increase of
Fagundo, A.B., Cuyàs, E., Verdejo-Garcia, A., Khymenets, O., Langohr, K., Martín- extracellular dopamine and serotonin in the ‘prefrontal cortex’ and striatum of
Santos, R., Farré, M., de la Torre, R., 2010. The influence of 5-HTT and COMT pigeons during working memory. Eur. J. Neurosci. 26 (8), 2293e2302.
genotypes on verbal fluency in ecstasy users. J. Psychopharmacol. 24, 1381e1393. Kasper, S., McEwen, B.S., 2008. Neurobiological and clinical effects of the antide-
Fattaccini, C.M., Bolaños-Jimenez, F., Gozlan, H., Hamon, M., 1990. Tianeptine pressant tianeptine. CNS Drugs 22, 15e26.
stimulates uptake of 5-hydroxytryptamine in vivo in the rat brain. Neuro- Keith, J.R., Wu, Y., Epp, J.R., Sutherland, R.J., 2007. Fluoxetine and the dentate gyrus:
pharmacology 29, 1e8. memory, recovery of function, and electrophysiology. Behav. Pharmacol. 18,
Fink, K.B., Göthert, M., 2007 Dec. 5-HT receptor regulation of neurotransmitter 521e531.
release. Pharmacol. Rev. 59, 360e417. King, M.V., Sleight, A.J., Woolley, M.L., Topham, I.A., Marsden, C.A., Fone, K.C., 2004.
Flagel, S.B., Watson, S.J., Robinson, T.E., Akil, H., 2007. Individual differences in the 5-HT6 receptor antagonists reverse delay-dependent deficits in novel object
propensity to approach signals vs. goals promote different adaptations in the discrimination by enhancing consolidationdan effect sensitive to NMDA
dopamine system of rats. Psychopharmacology 191, 599e607. receptor antagonism. Neuropharmacology 47, 195e204.
Flagel, S.B., Watson, S.J., Akil, H., Robinson, T.E., 2008. Individual differences in the King, M.V., Marsden, C.A., Fone, K.C., 2008. A role for the 5-HT1A, 5-HT4 and
attribution of incentive salience to a reward-related cue: influence on cocaine 5-HT6 receptors in learning and memory. Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 29, 482e492.
sensitization. Behav. Brain Res. 186, 48e56. Kivell, B., Day, D., Bosch, P., Schenk, S., Miller, J., 2010. MDMA causes a redistribution
Flagel, S.B., Akil, H., Robinson, T.E., 2009. Individual differences in the attribution of of serotonin transporter from the cell surface to the intracellular compartment
incentive salience to reward-related cues: implications for addiction. Neuro- by a mechanism independent of phospho-p38-mitogen activated protein kinase
pharmacology 56 (Suppl. 1), 139e148. activation. Neuroscience 168 (1), 82e95.
Flood, J.F., Cherkin, A., 1987. Fluoxetine enhances memory in mice. Psychophar- Koenig, P., Smith, E.E., Troiani, V., Anderson, C., Moore, P., Grossman, M., 2008.
macology 93, 36e43. Medial temporal lobe involvement in an implicit memory task: evidence of
Fone, K.C., 2008. An update on the role of the 5-hydroxytryptamine6 receptor in collaborating implicit and explicit memory systems from FMRI and Alzheimer’s
cognitive function. Neuropharmacology 55, 1015e1022. disease. Cereb. Cortex 18, 2831e2843.
Francis, P.T., 2008. Glutamatergic approaches to the treatment of cognitive and Lai, M.K.P., Ramirez, M.J., Tsang, S.W.Y., Francis, P.T., 2007. Alzheimer’s disease as
behavioural symptoms of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurodegener. Dis. 5 (3e4), a neurotransmitter disease. In: Dawbarn, D., Allen, S.J. (Eds.), Neurobiology of
241e243. Alzheimer’s Disease. Oxford University Press, New York, pp. 245e282.
Gerlai, R., 2001. Behavioral tests of hippocampal function: simple paradigms Lieben, C.K., Blokland, A., Sik, A., Sung, E., van Nieuwenhuizen, P., Schreiber, R.,
complex problems. Behav. Brain Res. 125, 269e277. 2005. The selective 5-HT6 receptor antagonist Ro4368554 restores memory
Gold, P.E., 2008. Protein synthesis inhibition and memory: formation vs. amnesia. performance in cholinergic and serotonergic models of memory deficiency in
Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 89, 201e211. the rat. Neuropsychopharmacology 30, 2169e2179.
Good, M., Macphail, E.M., 1994. The avian hippocampus and short-term memory for Liy-Salmeron, G., Meneses, A., 2007. Role of 5-HT1e7 receptors in short- and long-
spatial and non-spatial information. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. B 47, 293e317. term memory for an autoshaping task: intrahippocampal manipulations. Brain
Hall, G., Purves, D., Bonardi, C., 1996. Contextual control of conditioned responding Res. 1147, 140e147.
in rats with dorsal hippocampal lesions. Behav. Neurosci. 110, 933e945. Liy-Salmeron, G., Meneses, A., 2008. Effects of 5-HT drugs in prefrontal cortex
Hannon, J., Hoyer, D., 2008. Molecular biology of 5-HT receptors. Behav. Brain Res. during memory formation and the ketamine amnesia-model. Hippocampus 18,
195, 198e213. 965e974.
Harrod, S.B., Lacy, R.T., Ballina, L.E., 2010. Persistent expression of methamphet- Lyketsos, C.G., Sheppard, J.M., Steele, C.D., Kopunek, S., Steinberg, M., Baker, A.S.,
amine-induced CTA in periadolescent rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 96, Brandt, J., Rabins, P.V., 2000. Randomized, placebo-controlled, double-blind
515e520. clinical trial of sertraline in the treatment of depression complicating
362 A. Meneses et al. / Neuropharmacology 61 (2011) 355e363

Alzheimer’s disease: initial results from the depression in Alzheimer’s Disease antidepressant treatment associated with reduced cognitive decline in Alz-
study. Am. J. Psychiatry 157, 1686e1689. heimer’s disease? Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 25, 372e379.
Lynch, G., 2002. Memory enhancement: the search for mechanism-based drugs. Mowla, A., Mosavinasab, M., Pani, A., 2007. Does fluoxetine have any effect on the
Nature Neurosci. 5, 1035e1038. cognition of patients with mild cognitive impairment? A double-blind, placebo-
Madsen, K., Erritzoe, D., Mortensen, E.L., Gade, A., Madsen, J., Baaré, W., controlled, clinical trial. J. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 27, 67e70.
Knudsen, G.M., Hasselbalch, S.G., 2010 Jul 10. Cognitive function is related to Murphy, D.L., Fox, M.A., Timpano, K.R., Moya, P.R., Ren-Patterson, R., Andrews, A.M.,
fronto-striatal serotonin transporter levels e a brain PET study in young Holmes, A., Lesch, K.P., Wendland, J.R., 2008. How the serotonin story is being
healthy subjects. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) [Epub ahead of print] PMID: rewritten by new gene-based discoveries principally related to SLC6A4, the
20623110. serotonin transporter gene, which functions to influence all cellular serotonin
Mahler, S.V., Berridge, K.C., 2009. Which cue to “want?” Central amygdala opioid systems. Neuropharmacology 55, 932e960.
activation enhances and focuses incentive salience on a prepotent reward cue. Myhrer, T., 2003. Neurotransmitter systems involved in learning and memory in the
J. Neurosci. 29, 6500e6513. rat: a meta-analysis based on studies of four behavioral tasks. Brain Res. Rev. 41,
Mansuy, I.M., Shenolikar, S., 2006. Protein serine/threonine phosphatases in 268e287.
neuronal plasticity and disorders of learning and memory. Trends Neurosci. 29, Nakagawa, T., Kaneko, S., 2008. Neuropsychotoxicity of abused drugs: molecular
679e686. and neural mechanisms of neuropsychotoxicity induced by methamphetamine,
Marshall, J.F., Belcher, A.M., Feinstein, E.M., O’Dell, S.J., 2007. Methamphetamine- 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (ecstasy), and 5-methoxy-N, N-diiso-
induced neural and cognitive changes in rodents. Addiction 102 (Suppl. 1), propyltryptamine (foxy). J. Pharmacol. Sci. 106, 2e8.
61e69. Neill, J.C., Barnes, S., Cook, S., Grayson, B., Idris, N.F., McLean, S.L., Snigdha, S.,
McAdam, T.D., Brien, J.F., Reynolds, J.N., Dringenberg, H.C., 2008. Altered water- Rajagopal, L., Harte, M.K., 2010 Aug 10. Animal models of cognitive dysfunction
maze search behavior in adult guinea pigs following chronic prenatal ethanol and negative symptoms of schizophrenia: focus on NMDA receptor antagonism.
exposure: lack of mitigation by postnatal fluoxetine treatment. Behav. Brain Pharmacol. Ther. [Epub ahead of print] PMID: 20705091 [PubMed e as supplied
Res. 191, 202e229. by publisher].
McCann, U.D., Szabo, Z., Vranesic, M., Palermo, M., Mathews, W.B., Ravert, H.T., Nordahl, T.E., Salo, R., Leamon, M., 2003. Neuropsychological effects of chronic
Dannals, R.F., Ricaurte, G.A., 2008. Positron emission tomographic studies of methamphetamine use on neurotransmitters and cognition: a review.
brain dopamine and serotonin transporters in abstinent (þ/)3,4-methyl- J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 15, 317e325.
enedioxymethamphetamine (“ecstasy”) users: relationship to cognitive Noristani, H.N., Olabarria, M., Verkhratsky, A., Rodríguez, J.J., 2010. Serotonin fibre
performance. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 200, 439e450. sprouting and increase in serotonin transporter immunoreactivity in the CA1
Meltzer, C.C., Smith, G., Dekosky, S.T., Pollock, B.G., Mathis, C.A., Moore, R.Y., area of hippocampus in a triple transgenic mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease.
Kupfer, D.J., Reynolds, C.F., 1998. Serotonin in aging, late-life depression and Eur. J. Neurosci. 32, 71e79.
Alzheimer’s disease: the emerging role of functional imaging. Neuro- Ogren, S.O., Eriksson, T.M., Elvander-Tottie, E., D’Addario, C., Ekström, J.C.,
psychopharmacology 18, 407e430. Svenningsson, P., Meister, B., Kehr, J., Stiedl, O., 2008. The role of 5-HT1A
Mendelsohn, D., Riedel, W.J., Sambeth, A., 2009 Jun. Effects of acute tryptophan receptors in learning and memory. Behav. Brain Res. 195, 54e77.
depletion on memory, attention and executive functions: a systematic review. Olivier, J.D., Jans, L.A., Korte-Bouws, G.A., Korte, S.M., Deen, P.M., Cools, A.R.,
Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 33 (6), 926e952. Ellenbroek, B.A., Blokland, A., 2008. Acute tryptophan depletion dose depen-
Meneses, A., 1999. 5-HT system and cognition. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 23, dently impairs object memory in serotonin transporter knockout rats.
1111e1125. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 200, 243e254.
Meneses, A., 2002. Tianeptine: 5-HT uptake sites and 5-HT1e7 receptors modulate Orjales, A., Mosquera, R., Toledo, A., Pumar, M., García, N., Cortizo, L., Labeaga, L.,
memory formation in an autoshaping Pavlovian/instrumental task. Neurosci. Innerárity, A., 2003. Syntheses and binding studies of new [(aryl)(aroloxy)
Biobehav. Rev. 26, 309e319. methyl]piperidine derivatives and related compounds as potential antidepres-
Meneses, A., 2003. A pharmacological analysis of an associative learning task: 5-HT1 sant drugs with high affinity for serotonin (5-HT) and norepinephrine (NE)
to 5-HT7 receptor subtypes function on a Pavlovian/instrumental autoshaped transporters. J. Med. Chem. 46, 5512e5532.
memory. Learn. Mem 10, 363e372. Pattij, T., Broersen, L.M., van der Linde, J., Groenink, L., van der Gugten, J., Maes, R.A.,
Meneses, A., 2007a. Do serotonin1e7 receptors modulate short and long-term Olivier, B., 2003. Operant learning and differential-reinforcement-of-low-rate
memory? Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 87, 561e572. 36-s responding in 5-HT1A and 5-HT1B receptor knockout mice. Behav. Brain
Meneses, A., 2007b. Stimulation of 5-HT1A, 5-HT1B, 5-HT2A/2C, 5-HT3 and 5-HT4 Res. 141, 137e145.
receptors or 5-HT uptake inhibition: short- and long-term memory. Behav. Peele, D.B., Vincent, A., 1989. Strategies for assessing learning and memory,
Brain Res. 184, 81e90. 1978e1987: a comparison of behavioral toxicology, psychopharmacology, and
Meneses, A., Hong, E., 1995. Effect of fluoxetine on learning and memory involves neurobiology. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 13, 317e322.
multiple 5-HT systems. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 52, 341e346. Perez-Garcia, G., Meneses, A., 2008. Memory formation, amnesia, improved
Meneses, A., Perez-Garcia, G., 2007. 5-HT1A receptors and memory. Neurosci. Bio- memory and reversed amnesia: 5-HT role. Behav. Brain Res. 195, 17e29.
behav. Rev. 31, 705e727. Perez-Garcia, G., Meneses, A., 2009a. Memory time-course: mRNA 5-HT1A and 5-
Meneses, A., Perez-Garcia, G., Liy-Salmeron, G., Ponce-Lopez, T., Tellez, R., Flores- HT7 receptors. Behav. Brain Res. 202, 102e113.
Galvez, D., 2009. Associative learning, memory and serotonin: a neurobiological Perez-Garcia, G., Meneses, A., 2009b. Microarrays analysis of hippocampal genes
and pharmacological analysis. In: Rocha Arrieta, L.L., Granados-Soto, V. (Eds.), expression during memory explicit/implicit. In: 40th Society for Neuroscience
Models of Neuropharmacology. Transworld Research Network, Trivandrum-695 Meeting. Abstr 883.7/FF51. Chicago, IL, October 17e24.
023, Kerala, India, ISBN 978-81-7895-383-0, pp. 169e182. Reilly, S., Good, M., 1989. Hippocampal lesions and associative learning in the
Meneses, A., Manuel-Apolinar, L., Castillo, C., Castillo, E., 2007. Memory consolida- pigeon. Behav. Neurosci. 103, 731e742.
tion and amnesia modify 5-HT6 receptors expression in rat brain: an autora- Richmond, J., Colombo, M., 2002. Hippocampal lesions, contextual retrieval, and
diographic study. Behav Brain Res. 178 (1), 53e61. autoshaping in pigeons. Brain Res. 928, 60e68.
Meneses, A., Pérez-García, G., Ponce-Lopez, T., Castillo, C., 2011. 5-HT6 receptor Robbins, T.W., Roberts, A.C., 2007 Sep. Differential regulation of fronto-executive function
memory and amnesia: behavioral pharmacology - learning and memory by the monoamines and acetylcholine. Cereb. Cortex 17 (Suppl. 1), i151ei160.
processes. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 96, 27e47. Roberts, G.M., Garavan, H., 2010. Evidence of increased activation underlying
Meyer, J.S., Piper, B.J., Vancollie, V.E., 2008. Development and characterization of cognitive control in ecstasy and cannabis users. Neuroimage 52, 429e435.
a novel animal model of intermittent MDMA (“Ecstasy”) exposure during Rodriguez, J.S., Boctor, S.Y., Phelix, C.F., Martinez Jr., J.L., 2008. Differences in
adolescence. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1139, 151e163. performance between SpragueeDawley and Fischer 344 rats in positive rein-
Michelsen, K.A., Prickaerts, J., Steinbusch, H.W., 2008. The dorsal raphe nucleus and forcement tasks. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 89, 17e22.
serotonin: implications for neuroplasticity linked to major depression and Rose, E.J., Simonotto, E., Spencer, E.P., Ebmeier, K.P., 2006. The effects of escitalo-
Alzheimer’s disease. Prog. Brain Res. 172, 233e264. pram on working memory and brain activity in healthy adults during perfor-
Millan, M.J., Gobert, A., Roux, S., Porsolt, R., Meneses, A., Carli, M., et al., 2004. The mance of the n-back task. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 185, 339e347.
serotonin1A receptor partial agonist S15535 [4-(benzodioxan-5-yl)1-(indan-2- Savaskan, E., Müller, S.E., Böhringer, A., Schulz, A., Schächinger, H., 2008. Anti-
yl)piperazine] enhances cholinergic transmission and cognitive function in depressive therapy with escitalopram improves mood, cognitive symptoms,
rodents: a combined neurochemical and behavioral analysis. J. Pharmacol. Exp. and identity memory for angry faces in elderly depressed patients. Int.
Ther. 311, 190e203. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 11, 381e388.
Mitchell, E.S., Hoplight, B.J., Lear, S.P., Neumaier, J.F., 2006. BGC20-761, a novel Schenk, F., Morris, R.G., 1985. Dissociation between components of spatial memory
tryptamine analog, enhances memory consolidation and reverses scopolamine- in rats after recovery from the effects of retrohippocampal lesions. Exp. Brain
induced memory deficit in social and visuospatial memory tasks through a 5- Res. 58, 11e28.
HT6 receptor-mediated mechanism. Neuropharmacology 50, 412e420. Schilt, T., Koeter, M.W., Smal, J.P., Gouwetor, M.N., van den Brink, W., Schmand, B.,
Mitchell, E.S., Sexton, T., Neumaier, J.F., 2007. Increased expression of 5-HT6 2010. Long-term neuropsychological effects of ecstasy in middle-aged ecstasy/
receptors in the rat dorsomedial striatum impairs instrumental learning. Neu- polydrug users. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 207, 583e591.
ropsychopharmacology 32, 1520e1530. Schmitt, J.A., Wingen, M., Ramaekers, J.G., Evers, E.A., Riedel, W.J., 2006. Serotonin
Monleón, S., Vinader-Caerols, C., Arenas, M.C., Parra, A., 2008. Antidepressant drugs and human cognitive performance. Curr. Pharm. Des. 12, 2473e2486.
and memory: insights from animal studies. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 18, Schreiber, R., Vivian, J., Hedley, L., Szczepanski, K., Secchi, R.L., Zuzow, M., van
235e248. Laarhoven, S., Moreau, J.L., Martin, J.R., Sik, A., Blokland, A., 2007. Effects of the
Mossello, M., Boncinelli, V., Caleri, M.C., Cavallini, E., Palermo, M., Di Bari, S., Tilli, E., novel 5-HT6 receptor antagonist RO4368554 in rat models for cognition and
Sarcone, D., Simoni, C.A., Biagini, G., MasottiMarchionni, N., 2008. Is sensorimotor gating. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 17, 277e288.
A. Meneses et al. / Neuropharmacology 61 (2011) 355e363 363

Schreiber, S., Pick, C.G., 2006. From selective to highly selective SSRIs: a comparison Tellez, R., Rocha, L., Castillo, C., Meneses, A., 2010. Autoradiographic study of sero-
of the antinociceptive properties of fluoxetine, fluvoxamine, citalopram and tonin transporter during memory formation. Behav. Brain Res. 212 (1), 12e26.
escitalopram. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 16 (6), 464e468. Terry Jr., A.V., Buccafusco, J.J., Wilson, C., 2008. Cognitive dysfunction in neuro-
Schröder, N., O’Dell, S.J., Marshall, J.F., 2003. Neurotoxic methamphetamine regimen psychiatric disorders: selected serotonin receptor subtypes as therapeutic
severely impairs recognition memory in rats. Synapse 49, 89e96. targets. Behav. Brain Res. 195, 30e38.
Scott, J.C., Woods, S.P., Matt, G.E., Meyer, R.A., Heaton, R.K., Atkinson, J.H., Grant, I., Timár, J., Gyarmati, S., Szabó, A., Fürst, S., 2003. Behavioural changes in rats treated
2007. Neurocognitive effects of methamphetamine: a critical review and meta- with a neurotoxic dose regimen of dextrorotatory amphetamine derivatives.
analysis. Neuropsychol. Rev. 17, 275e297. Behav. Pharmacol. 14, 199e206.
Selvaraj, S., Hoshi, R., Bhagwagar, Z., Murthy, N.V., Hinz, R., Cowen, P., Curran, H.V., Thomasius, R., Zapletalova, P., Petersen, K., Buchert, R., Andresen, B., Wartberg, L.,
Grasby, P., 2009. Brain serotonin transporter binding in former users of MDMA Nebeling, B., Schmoldt, A., 2006. Mood, cognition and serotonin transporter
(‘ecstasy’). Br. J. Psychiatry 194, 355e359. availability in current and former ecstasy (MDMA) users: the longitudinal
Siegel, J.A., Craytor, M.J., Raber, J., 2010 Aug 19. Long-term effects of methamphet- perspective. J. Psychopharmacol. 20, 211e225.
amine exposure on cognitive function and muscarinic acetylcholine receptor Toda, N., Kaneko, T., Kogen, H., 2010. Development of an efficient therapeutic
levels in mice. Behav. Pharmacol. [Epub ahead of print]PMID: 20729719. agent for Alzheimer’s disease: design and synthesis of dual inhibitors of
Smith, J.E., Lakoski, J.M., 1997. Electrophysiological effects of fluoxetine and dulox- acetylcholinesterase and serotonin transporter. Chem. Pharm. Bull. (Tokyo) 58,
etine in the dorsal raphe nucleus and hippocampus. Eur J Pharmacol 323 (1), 273e287.
69e73. Tomie, A., Di Poce, J., Aguado, A., Janes, A., Benjamin, D., Pohorecky, L., 2003. Effects
Solodkin, A., van Hoesen, G.W., 1997. Neuropathology and functional anatomy of of autoshaping procedures on [3H]-8-OH-DPAT-labeled 5-HT1a binding and
Alzheimer’s disease. In: Brioni, J.D., Decker, M.W. (Eds.), Pharmacological [125I]-LSD-labeled 5-HT2a binding in rat brain. Brain Res. 975, 167e178.
Treatment of Alzheimer’s Disease. Wiley-Liss, New York, pp. 151e177. Tomie, A., Tirado, A.D., Yu, L., Pohorecky, L.A., 2004. Pavlovian autoshaping proce-
Sparber, S.B., 2001. Use of autoshaping with non-delayed and delayed reinforce- dures increase plasma corticosterone and levels of norepinephrine and sero-
ment for studying effects upon acquisition and consolidation of information. In: tonin in prefrontal cortex in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 153, 97e105.
Buccafusco, J.J. (Ed.), Methods of Behavior Analysis in Neuroscience. CRC Press, Valluzzi, J.A., Chan, K., 2007. Effects of fluoxetine on hippocampal-dependent and
Boca Raton, pp. 231e268. hippocampal-independent learning tasks. Behav. Pharmacol. 18, 507e513.
Stancampiano, R., Cocco, S., Cugusi, C., Sarais, L., Fadda, F., 1999. Serotonin and Vorhees, C.V., He, E., Skelton, M.R., Graham, D.L., Schaefer, T.L., Grace, C.E.,
acetylcholine release response in the rat hippocampus during a spatial memory Braun, A.A., Amos-Kroohs, R., Williams, M.T., 2010. Comparison of (þ)-meth-
task. Neuroscience 89, 1135e1143. amphetamine, þ/methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA), (þ)-amphet-
Steckler, T., Andrews, J.S., Marten, P., Turner, J.D., 1993. Effects of NBM lesions with amine and þ/fenfluramine in rats on egocentric learning in the Cincinnati
two neurotoxins on spatial memory and autoshaping. Pharmacol. Biochem. water maze. Synapse [Epub ahead of print] PMID: 20730798.
Behav. 44, 877e889. Woolley, M.L., Marsden, C.A., Sleight, A.J., Fone, K.C., 2003. Reversal of a cholinergic-
Steinbush, H.W.M., 1984. Serotonin-immunoreactive neurons and their projections induced deficit in a rodent model of recognition memory by the selective 5-HT6
in the CNS. In: Bjorklund, A., Hokfelt, T., Kuhar, M.J. (Eds.), Handbook of receptor antagonist, Ro 04-6790. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 170, 358e367.
Chemical Neuroanatomy. Classical Neurotransmitters and Transmitters Recep- Wuwongse, S., Chang, R.C., Law, A.C., 2010. The putative neurodegenerative links
tors in the CNS, Part III, vol. 3. Elsevier Sci Publishers, pp. 68e125. between depression and Alzheimer’s disease. Prog. Neurobiol. 91, 362e375.
Sulzer, D., Sonders, M.S., Poulsen, N.W., Galli, A., 2005. Mechanisms of neuro- Yamazaki, M., Matsuoka, N., Maeda, N., Kuratani, K., Ohkubo, Y., Yamaguchi, I., 1995
transmitter release by amphetamines: a review. Prog. Neurobiol. 75, 406e433. Jan. FR121196, a potential antidementia drug, ameliorates the impaired memory
Sunyer, B., Diao, W., Lubec, G., 2008. The role of post-translational modifications for of rat in the Morris water maze. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 272 (1), 256e263.
learning and memory formation. Electrophoresis 29, 2593e2602. Zahniser, N.R., Doolen, S., 2001. Chronic and acute regulation of Naþ/Cl-depen-
Talpos, J.C., McTighe, S.M., Dias, R., Saksida, L.M., Bussey, T.J., 2010. Trial-unique, dent neurotransmitter transporters: drugs, substrates, presynaptic receptors,
delayed nonmatching-to-location (TUNL): a novel, highly hippocampus- and signaling systems. Pharmacol. Ther. 92, 21e55.
dependent automated touchscreen test of location memory and pattern sepa- Zhao, Z., Zhang, H.T., Bootzin, E., Millan, M.J., O’Donnell, J.M., 2008 Oct 15. Associ-
ration. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem.. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2010.07.006. ation of changes in norepinephrine and serotonin transporter expression with
Tejani-Butt, S.M., Yang, J., Pawliyk, A.C., 1995. Altered serotonin transporter sites in the long-term behavioral effects of antidepressant drugs. Neuro-
Alzheimer’s disease raphe and hippocampus. Neuroreport 30, 6. psychopharmacology [Epub ahead of print] PMID: 18923402 [PubMed e as
Tellez, R., Rocha, L., Meneses. A., 2008. Influence of memory formation and amnesia supplied by publisher].
over serotonergic transporter in brain. Society for Neuroscience Meeting. Zola-Morgan, S., Squire, L.R., 1993. Neuroanatomy of memory. Ann. Rev. Neurosci.
Washington DC, November 15e19, 2008. Abstract 290.7/RR58. 16, 547e563.

Você também pode gostar