Você está na página 1de 15

©Eunice de Morais e Marcos B.

Carreira SUMÁRIO
Coordenação Editorial:
Texto e Contexto Editora CULTURAS, IDENTITADES E SUBJETIVIDADES:
Editora-chefe: LINGUAGENS EM lviOVIMENTO 5
Rosenéia Hauer Prof. Dr. a Eunite de Morais (UEPG) e Prof. Dr. iv1artos B. Carreira (UEPG)
Capa:
Imagem: Phellip Willian de Paula Gruber e Melissa Garabelli
CIEL 2015 E OS DESAFIOS CONTEMPORc-\NEOS
Arte final: André ALS
Revisão: Thariane Prochner DA EDUCAÇ\0 NO BRc\SIL
Diagramação: Rosenéia Hauer Eunia de Morais 16
Supervisão Editorial: Rosenéia Hauer e Silvana Oliveira
SELOS E SIGILOS:
CORRESPONDÊNCIA DE CLARICE LISPECTOR 26
Ficha Catalográfica Elaborada pelo Setor de Tratamento da Informação Nádia Battella Gotlib
BICEN/UEPG

Congresso Internacional de Estudos da Linguagem- CIEL, 2015


RETRATOS DO DELÍRIO
C749 Linguagens em movimento: culturas, identidades e suhietividades/ Eunice de (Ex-POSIÇÃO DE FoucAULT, ARTAUD E \1:\N GoGH) 53
{orais; Marcos B. Carreira (Org.). Ponta Grossa :Texto
Daniel de Oliveira Gomes
176 p.: il.

ISBN: 978-85-94441-00-3
A PALAVRA DEFINITIVA-
1. Linguagem - estudo. 2. Literatura. 3. Leitura- faces. ESCRITURA E MILITÂNCIA NA LITERATURA ARGENTINA
o Internacional de Estudos da Linguagem. II. Moraes, Eunice (Org.). III.
(CONTI, URONDO, WALSH) 77
AndréQtteiroz
CDD: 420.7

LA INESPERADA Y SUTIL HISTORIA DE AMOR


DE UNA MUJER FEA, CHUECA Y BIZCA 93
Presidente:
Cecilia Inês Luque
Dra. Silvana Oliveira (UEPG)

Membros: DA ARTE RUPESTRE AO HIPERTEXTO:


Dr. Fábio Augusto Steyer (UEPG) A PLURALIZAÇAO DAS LINGUAGENS SOB OS
Esp. Anderson Pedro Laurindo (FacPG) OLHARES DAS MÚLTIPLAS FACES D_A
Dra. Marly Catarina Soares (UEPG) LEITURA 106
Dra. Naira de Almeida Nascimento (UTFPR) Evanir Pavloski
Dra. Letícia Fraga (UEPG)
Dra. Anna Stegh Camari (UNIANDRADE)
Dr. Evanir Pavlovski (UEPG)
LITERATURA E QUADRINHOS:
Dr" Eunice de Morais (UEPG) O CISNE E O PATINHO FEIO
Dra. Joice Beatriz da Costa (UFFS) Paulo Ramos
Dra.Luana TeL'Ceira Porto (URI)
Dr. César Augusto Queirós(UFAM- Manaus)
LANGUAGE TEACHING:
Dr. Valdir Prigol (UFFS)
A LOOK WITH THE EYES OF CO:MPLEXITY 151
conteúdo dessa obra é de responsabilidade de seus autores. Elaine Ferreira do Vale Borges
'XTO E CONTEXTO EDITORA
>]28228473/00016-60
.osenéia do Rocio Prestes Hauer- l'vffii AGRADECIMENTOS DA COORDENAÇAO
de Sapucaí, FT 1113. Uvaranas- Ponta Grossa- Paraná
DO CIEL 2015 175
42) 32269464
extoecontexto.editora@gmail.com
LANGUAGE TEACHING:
A LOOK_ WITH THE EYES OF COMPLEXITY

Elaine Ferreira do Vale Borges, PhD- UEPG 1

INTRODUCTION

With the advent of complexity paradigm2 in applied linguistics


(AL), mainly by the -vvork of Larsen-Freeman (1997) and Larsen-
Freeman & Cameron (2008), new insights have arisen in language
studies along with the usage of a range of complexity terminology to
describe language phenomena as chaotic system.
In this work I argue in favor of a turning point in AL
perspectives into complexity / chaos theory - as also discussed in
Borges (2014b) - by briefly showing six examples o f complex
adaptive systems (CAS) that emerged the field, such as language
(BECI<NER et al, 2009), second language acquisition (SLA)
QUARTA PARTE 2005, 2012, 2014a, 2014b), identity (SADE, 2009), approach to
language teaching and learning (BORGES & 2011, 2014;
ENSINO
BORGES, 2015), language teaching syllabus (BORGES, 2014a),
and language teacher development (BORGES, 2016). Likewise,
these CAS (and others not focused here) have contributed to the
emergency of what I name complexity pedagogy in AL or additional
language complexity pedagogy which goes beyond the post-method
pedagogy, avoiding its claim for theorizers-practitioners ruoture in

1 Mestre em Linguística Aplicada pelo Instituto de Estudos da Linguagem (IEL) da UNI-


CAJ'viP (2003), doutora em Educação pela Faculdade de Educação da USP (2009) e pós-
-doutora em Estudos Linguíst:icos pela Faculdade de Letras da UFMG (2011). Fez estágio
doutoral na Universidade de Murcia (Espanha). Trabalhou como teacher assistent no The
Joseph H. Lauder Institute of Managment and International Studies da Universidade da
Pennsylvania (EUA). Tem experiência na área de Linguística Aplicada e Educação em en-
sino e aprendizagem/ aquisição de línguas adicionais (incluindo Libras como L2) e teoria
da complexidade, atuando principalmente nos seguintes temas: abordagens e pedagogia de
ensino de línguas adicionais, aquisição de segunda língua e formação pré e continuada de
professores. Atualmente é professora adjunta do Departamento de Estudos da Linguagem
(DEEL) da Universidade Estadual de Ponta Grossa (UEPG) e no Programa de Pós-Gra-
duação em Educação (Niestrado) da mesma Universidade.

2 As one can see in Capra (2006[1982]), and Morin (2010[1982]).


150 151
the practicali!J dimension (I<.UMARAVADIVELU, 2001, 2006). same way, complexity can be contemporaneously understood as
Also, by means of clarification, I highlight the importance a new paradigm in AL (BORGES, 2009; BORGES, 2014b) as it
of conceptual metaphor (LAI<.OFF & JOHNSON, 2001) and scientiftc "can push the field into a paradigm shift'' (LARSEN-FREMAN &
metaphor (BOYD, 1993) in advancing the AL theoretical framework CAMERON, 2008, p. 13).
into the paradigm o f complexity. This discussion is supported mainly According to Guba & Lincoln (1994, p. 107, author's
by the Wittgensteinian epistemological sense of paradigm in the italic), like I<.uhn, a paradigm is "viewed as a set o f basic beliefs (or
conception of knowledge and language-game in science; and by the metaphysics) that deals with ultimate o r first principles. I t represents
Lakoff-and-Johnsonian notion o f conceptual metaphor as a mental a worldview that defines, for its holder, the nature of the world (... )".
representation of human experience. Similarly, but sustaining a transdisciplinary approach (paradigm
of complexity) that goes beyond the fragmentation and hyper-
The shape of a paradigm specialization promoted by the paradigm of simplicity, Edgar Morin
understands the concept of paradigmas "a set of rules of which we
I<.uhn (1970[1962]) has discussed paradigm as a scientific are unaware but that govern ali the activities of thought of an age"
community consensus that evolves into a model recognized as a GOUANNET, 2012, p. 165).
scientific achievement. In other words, it "is the universally recognized Moreover, for Guba & Lincoln (1994), paradigms may also be
scientific achievements that for a time provide model problems and described in three ways: ontologically (nature of reality), epistemologicaljy
solutions to a community of practitioners" (p. viii). Paradigm shift (knowledge and knower relationship), and methodologicaljy (wqys of inquirer).
occurs when anomalies appear and they cannot be solved or explained These features bring a holistic view into "\vay we regard kno"\vledge
by the current paradigm. In this context, "the optical illusion o f some in research. Here, for the sake of the discussion in this paper, I have
images, like those of the Gestalt psychology, is a good example of special interest in the epistemological nature paradigm that can
paradigm shift, when the same information can be seen in an entirely connected with the thoughts o f Wittgenstein' s later philosophy.
different way" (BORGES, 2014b, p. 43). Newtonian mechanics According to Luckhardt (1978, p 245), the concept o f paradigm
and Einstein 's theories o f relativity are good examples o f I<.uhnian in Wittgenstein's language-game has three essencial characteJ istics:
paradigm in physics. In social science, we should use the sociological
"(1) they are standards by which particular cases are
meaning o f paradigmas "what the members o f a scientific co1munity judged, (2) agreement in them is necessary for engaging
share, and, conversely, a scientific com..._munity consists of men who in any practice which employs them, and (3) they can
never be said to be (or not to be) what particular cases
share a paradigm" (I<.UHN, 1970[1962], p. 176, author' s italic). In this judged according to them can be said to be (or not to
context, we must stand for the view of social science (AL included) be)."
as a mature science as happens with natural science. In this regarding,
I<.uhn (1970[1962], p. 179) has pointed out that "what changes with Still, the paradigm meter, Wittgenstein' s words, is "not
the transition to maturity is not the presence of a paradigm but something that is represented, but is a means of representation')
rather its nature". Schleicherian, Saussurean and Chomskyan theories, and "[w]e do not judge the pictures, "\Ve judge by means of picture.
for example, are considered paradigms in the history of linguistics We do not investigate them, we use them to investigate something"
evolution as a science (I<.OERNER, 1976; DASCAL, 1978). In the (LUCI<HARDT, 1978, p. 244). This is a condition for language-game

152 153
in any science, and so it is the case for AL when using ideas, concepts • 1970, chemist Ilya Prigogine (dissipative structures, self-
and words from complexity science. organization and irreversibility)
• 1970, biologists Humberto Maturana and Francisco Varela
Complexity, a complexus of theories (autopoietic system)
• 1970, psychologist Jean Piaget (transdisciplinarity)
As part o f the Wittgensteinian 's language-game in different • 1975, mathematician Benoit Mandelbrot (fractal geometry)
sciences we have the emergence of various terms with the same • 1980, neuroscientist J. A. Scott I<:.elso and mathematician
meaning. In the context of the complexity paradigm, for instance, Hermann Haken (synergetics)
we may encounter such interchangeable terminology as complexi!J, • 1980, philosopher Edgar Morin (paradigm of simplicity
complexi!J science, paradigm of complexi!J, complexi!J theory, complexi!J and paradigm of complexity)
revolution, complex thinking and so on (BORGES, 2014b; SILVA & • 1984, Santa Fe Institute (interdisciplinary study center of
BORGES, 2016). Nevertheless, the realmeaning of complexity is that complex adaptive center)
it is a tis sue o f inseparably associated heterogeneous constituents, that • 1990, cognitive scientists Timothy van Gelder and Robert
isto say, it is a complexus of theories (MORIN, 2007). In other words, F. Port (neural networks)
'"[c]omplexity science' is a term used to describe a set of concepts,
principies, propositions and ideas that have emerged and clustered being metaononcal
Just being or not
together over the course of the 20th century" (RAMALINGAM et
ai., 2008, p. 1). These concepts are used to portray and to understand Larsen-Freeman & Cameron (2008, p. 11, italic mine)
"dynamics and processes of change found in a range of physical emphasize that "complexity theory offers applied linguistics at least
and biological phenomena" (ibid, p. 1) -as well as in a number of an important new metaphor that brings it new ways of thinking
social and language phenomena, I might add. Larsen-Freeman & about issues in the field, and, maximally, may push the field towards
Cameron (2008, p. 2-4) provide us some of these theories as they radical theoretical change". Focusing on the importance of metaphor,
talk about 'disciplinary progenitors' to complexity theory, as follows 3 : the authors call our attention to the fact applied linguists has been
challenged as to whether they are 'just' being metaphorical when they
• 1880, mathematician Henri Poincaré (non-linear dynamics) talk or write about using ideas from complexity. As a matter of fact,
• 1940, biologist Conrad Waddington (embryology lay in this challenge does not make any sense, since metaphors (overtaking
genetics) its simplified concept of being a literary tool, a rhetorical flourish
• 1940, mathematicians John von Neumann and Norbert for ornamenting language) are a conceptual domain (conceptual
Wiener (cybernetics) metaphor) (LARSEN-FREEMAN & CAMERON, 2008). In other
• 1950, biologist von Bertalanffy (general systems theory) words, metaphors are a coherent organization human experience
• 1960, mathematician René Thom (catastrophe theory) that shape our communication, the way we see the world, the way
• 1960, meteorologist Edward Lorenz (chaos theory) we think and act. In Lakoff & Tohnson (2001, p. 124) own "\vords,

3 Some information has been added by me, like (approximate) dates and author/ s profes- "The concepts that govern our thought are not just
sional expertise. Also, I included Benoit Mandelbrot,Jean Piaget and Edgar Morin.
154 155
matters of the intellect. They also govern our every- dynamic (chaotic) systems, any particular research phenomena
day functioning, down to the most mundane details. in this context might be seen as a system that learns and changes
Our concepts structure what we perceive, how we get
around in the word, and how we relate to other people. over time. In other words, research phenomena of this kind can
Our conceptual system thus plays a central role in de- be defined as complex adaptive systems (CAS) (HOLLAND, 1995)
fining our everyday realities. If we are right in suggest
whose characteristics are: non-linearity, unpredictability, emergence,
ing that our conceptual system is largely metaphorical,
then the way we think, what we experience, and what openness, self-organization, adaptation, and dynamism. Also, they
we do every day is very much a matter o f metaphor." are highly sensitive to inicial conditions, and have a peculiar behavior
(strange or chaotic attractor4) that emerges (in fractaP shape)
From the conceptual metaphor point o f view, Larsen-Freeman
from the interactions of its various elements with no centralized
& Cameron (2008, p. 11) suggest that applied linguists should stand
control. These interactions are guided by simple rules that "tend
up for the importance of metaphor by rejecting the 'jus(. Also,
to set conditions for self-organization, and they also reinforce
applied linguists should discuss the meaning of saying "that the
those conditions as time goes on" (PATTERSON et al., 2012, p
comparison between systems in applied linguistics and complex
12). As a result, "the same system-or, at least, a system that seems
systemis more than metaphor". For the authors, "[c]omplexity theory
to have preserved its character and identity-can and will respond
will serve applied linguistics as more than metaphor if it works as a
very differendy to sets of conditions that appear identical
bridge that takes us [applied linguists] into a new way o f thinking "strict predictability and reliability of results are unreasonable
or theoretical framework, that is then rigorously developed within
criteria when dealingwith systems thatlearn" (DAVIS &
the field" (p, 15). From my perspective, in accordance with Larsen-
2006, p. 18). That is because "very similar systems under virtually
Freeman & Cameron, AL (as a mature science, in IZuhnian sense)
identical circumstances and subjected to virtually identical
uses a complexity terminology as more than metaphor because
can respond in dramatically different ways" (op. cit, p. 18)
this is part o f its language-game (in Wittgensteinian sense) to study
From now on, I am going to show and briefly discuss s1X
dynamic (chaotic) systems as language phenomena. In this context,
examples o f CAS that have emerged in (some o f them illustrated
we also have what Boyd (1993, p. 486) argues to be the case of
in a fractal shape): language, SLA, identity, approach to language
scientiftc metaphors: "those in v1hich metaphorical expression constitute,
teaching and learning, syllabus, and language teacher development.
at least for a time, an irreplaceable part of the linguistic machinery
of a scientific theory (... ) such metaphors are constitutive of the
1. LANGUAGE AS CAS
theories they express; rather than merely exegeticaf'. In addition,
Larsen-Freeman & Cameron (2008, p. 15) emphasize that "[t]he
Language as CAS has been discussed in works such as
metaphor serves 'a temporary aid thinking' (Baake 2003: 82) and is
Larsen-Freen1.an & Cameron (2008), Beckner et al. (2009), Ellis &
eventually literalized into field-specific theory, research, and practice."

4 "In the topological vocabulary of system landscapes, states, or particular modes of be-
Language teaching: a look with the eyes of complexity haviors, that the system 'prefers' are called attractorl' (L-\RSEN-FREEl'vLAN; CAJ'viERON,
2008, p. 49, authors' italic).
5 "In dynamic systems,jractals occur at the boundaries of attractor basins (... )"; "Fractal
organi::;ption endows a system with the flexibility to change and adapt to new circumstances"
As AL is contemporaneously concerned with studying
(op.cit, p. 63 and p. 110, italic mine).
156 157
Larsen-Freeman (2009), Nascimento (2009), Paiva (2002, 2005, language is a deterministic system. Rather, it is charac-
terized as a complex system, a system that continually
2014a, 2014b), and Borges (2016). First, Beckner et a/. state that changes without losing its identity."
language as a CAS implies the following fundamen:=al characteristics:
From ali these perspectives, Paiva (2014b, p. 121) has
"The system consists of multiple agents (the speakers
in the speech community) interacting with one anoth- been arguing since 20026 for a language complex model that
er. The system is adaptive; that is, speakers' behavior is "can accommodate opposing theories, because it can defend the
based on their past interactions, and current and past
interactions together feed forward into future behavior.
existence of innate mental structures and, at the same time, highlight
A speaker's behavior is the consequence of competing the importance of neural connections, language identity, social
factors ranging from perceptual constraints to social mediations, input, interaction, output, etc.", as well as linguistic
motivations. The structures of language emerge from
interrelated patterns of experience, social interaction, habit formations. In this matter, it is possible to understand
and cognitive mechanisms." (p. 1) that the concept of language as a CAS may nest many (if not
language definitions formulated by philosophers and linguists
Second, according to Larsen-Freeman & Cameron: over time, even those regarded as incompatible with one another.
'~ complexity theo:ry perspective views language us-
Here we have some o f these definitions: (1) Language is a
ing as a dynamic system that emerges and self-orga- mirror where "the order of syntax mirrors order of the world"
nizes from frequently occurring patterns of language (Aristotle) or a mirror that mediates "souls by mirroring minds"
use at different timescales from the milliseconds of
neural connections to the millennia of evolution, and (Plato) (NOLAN, 1990, p. 65); (2) language is ''both a social p.Lvuu\.-L
across a range of levels from the individual to inter- of the faculty of speech and a collection of necessary conventions
acting pairs to entire speech communities, rather than
that have been adopted by a social body to permit individuais to
as a fixed, autonomous, closed, and atemporal system.
Viewing language as a complex system makes us re- exercise that faculty" (SAUSSURE, 2011 [1915], p. 9); (3) language is
gard linguistic signs notas 'autonomous objects of any "a set of (finite or infinite) of sentences, each finite length
kind, either social or psychological', but as 'contextu-
alized products of the integration of various activities constructed out of a finite set of elements" (CHOMSI<Y, 2002[1957],
by [particular] individuais in particular communicative p. 2); (4) language is a combination of "function (language as a tool
situations'(Harris 1993: 311), whatwe have called their for communication) and structure (language as a lawful governed
language resources." (p. 111-112, autho:rs' bracket)
whole)" GAI<OBSON, 2002[1962] p. XXJl); (5) 1-ªJJ-_guage is "part of a
theory of action, simply because speaking is a :rule-gove:rned form o f
In cont:rast, Nas cimento (2009, p. 71, my t:ranslation) pointed out
behavior" (SEARLE, 1999[1969], p. 17); (6) language "is a continuous
thatrecursionis one o f the basicprinciples o faCAS, since self-organized
generative process implemented in social-verbal interaction
systems organize themselves by recursion. From this perspective,
of speakers" (VOLOSINOV, 2000[1973], p.98); (7) language is
the author bases his arguments on language as a CAS using, among
"more than just a semiotic system, a system of meanings; it is also
other references, Chomsky's Minimalist Program. He explains that
a system that n1akes meanings, a semogenic system; the sou:rce
"( ... ) to assume that the inte:rphase subsystem-the of this semogenic powe:r is grammar" (HALLIDAY, 2005, p. 59).
principie of :recu:rsion-acts as a facto:r that restricts,
delimits, the language 1andscape of possibilities ' in
the interface systems does not imply to assume that 6 See Paiva (2002; 2005) and Menezes (2013).
158 159
Borges (2016), in turn, advances in the attempt to systematize "(... ) there is a multitude of interacting factors that
the concept of language as CAS (Fig. 1; fractal in shape) by have been proposed to determine the degree to which
emphasizing its systems - as the three classical conceptions of the SLA process will be successful age, aptitude, so-
ciopsychological factors such as motivation and atti-
language: expression of thought, communication instrument/ tude, personality factors, cognitive style, hemisphenc-
code and interaction (GERALDI, 1995) -, and its subsystems, as ity, learning strategies, sex, birth order, interests, etc
suggested in Paiva (2014b): phonological, syntactic, morphological, (Larsen-Freeman and Long 1991). Perhaps no one of
these by itself is a determining factor, the interaction
lexical, pragmatic, semantic, dialects, registers, language varieties, etc. of them, however, has a very profound effect."

Along the same lines, Paiva (2005, 2012) argues in favor of a


SLA fractal model, a CAS in itself, composed of subsystems wherein
each one is also a dynamic system. These subsystems are actually
theories o f SLA (sometimes based on mental processes and sometimes
grounded on environmental perspectives) that try to explain how we
acquire an additional language. In this context, Paiva (2011, p.
explains that there is evidence to support the claim that SLA is a CAS
since it "exhibits an inherent ability to adapt to different conditions
present in both internai and externai environments". Thus, a complex
SLA perspective

"(... ) can simultaneously admit the existence of innate


mental structures, as proposed by generativists, and at the
same time sustain the notion that part of the language
is acquired by means of repetition and the creation of
automatic linguistic habits, as explained by the structur-
alists. In such a model, input, interaction and output are
also considered o f paramount importance for language
acquisition as they trigger both neural and sociocultural
connections. (op. cit., p. 61)

Fig. 1 - Language as CAS (BORGES, 2016, p. 372, my translation) Therefore, Paiva (2012, 2014a; 2014b) lists more expressive
SLA theories as subsystems (Fig. 2; fractal in shape) of SLA as
2. SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION AS CAS automatic habits, interaction, affiliation, sociocultural mediation,
input, innate mental structures, neural connections
Larsen-Freeman (1997, p. 151) is the first to claim t_hat second
language acquisition (SLA) can be understood as a complex nonlinear
system, i.e.,

160
161
title, an ethnic catego:ry, or a personality trait" (\XlEN G ER, 2000, p. 23 9)
that could be explained by simple (innate) causes and deterrninistic laws.
Contemporaneously, identity has been discussed in a Bakhtinian
perspective in the sense that discourse contributes to its continuing
construction. In this context, identity emerges as a result of social
formation (nesting different actors, contexts, linguistic and non-
linguistic behaviors) throughout the interaction amongst "several
communities of practice: family, school, club, and so on" (SADE,
2009, p. 519). As Giddens points out (1991, p. 190), "as
leaves one encounter and enters another, he sensitively adjusts
'presentation o f self' in relation to whatever is demanded o f a particular
situation". The result of this iterative social process over time is
has been called fragmentation oJ the se!f But L.1is process, as emphasized
by Giddens, is much more complex than just a disintegration of
individuais into multiple selves. Within this standpoint, Sade argues for
the replacement o f "the widely used term 'fragmented identities' to
'fractalized identities '", reminding us that "the process o f identity
Fig. 2- SLA as CAS (PANA, 2012, p. 22, my translation) (re)construction and emergence can be viewed as a complex system"
(SADE, 2009, p. 525). Regarding this, Sade explains
As it happens with theories in others CAS discussed nere, some "This new term evokes the properties of fractais and
o f these SLA theories are said to be incompatible with one "'-H'--' LL,_.._.._, can be ~ better metaphor to encompass ali the com-
but as a CAS they nest in the acquisition dynamic system as pJprnp;,-r.:, plexity which is involved in the process. The word
'fragmented' leads us to think of 'fragments', or 'bro-
that interact with others to emerge the behavior of the system as ken pieces' (... ) the idea that the process of identity
a whole. As highlighted by Paiva (2005, p. 23), SLA theories "are emergence causes the human to be 'broken into piec-
compatible with a fractal theory of language acquisition, since each es' as if the emerged identities were isolated construc-
tions o f one 's self (... ) . As in fractal, there are infinite
one o f them describes a certain aspect o f the complex system called possibilities of identity fractalization, as it is also in-
language acquisition''. finite the number of possible discourses we may have
access throughout our life span (... ) [and] no matter
the number of internal fractalizations, the parts are in-
3. T'\lC'l'.T'T'T'lr'V" AS CAS terconnected into a whole which is self-similar to the
parts." (p. 525).
For a long time, identity has been understood a Cartesian
framework. In other words, identity was perceived as a consciousness
o f self that remains identical throughout time, "a unified experience
belonging" (PANA, 2011, p. 62), "an abstract idea ora label, such as a
162 163
4. APPROACH TO LANGUAGE TEACHING AND LEARNING AS CAS

Larsen-Freeman & Cameron (2008) have suggested that a


complexity approach to language classroom should cover at least
four components, namely: connectivity across alllevels, dynamics of
language-usillg, co-adaptation, and dynamics of learnillg managed by
teachlng. From this perspective, Borges & Paiva (2011, 2014) have
developed a complexity approach to language teachlng and learnillg
(CALTL) (Fig. 3; fractalm shape) addlng other components to it, such
as language and SLA as CAS (Fig. 1 and 2), reconstructive dynamic
climension of teachlng and learnillg as a multifaceted process, focus
on learner 's performance (to promote competence) and on language
social practices, reception o f multiple identities or 'fractalized
identities' (SADE, 2009), eco-semiotic syllabus co-construction
(Fig. 4), affordance awareness, illdividual autopoietic organization. In
addition, Borges (2015) and Pereira & Borges (2016) have showed
how the CALTL can be used 1n English as additionallanguage (EAL)
classrooms with the help of the Brazilian Alive textbook series (for
Fig. 3 - CALTL as CAS - Produced by the author.
elementary and secondary school) that is equally based on complexity
theory. Also, by proposillg a chaotic mode! of reflective development of
Language as a CAS has been previously discussed 1n this secrion
language teachers 'professionality (CMRD), Borges (2016) and Borges et
(see topic 1, Fig. 1). Teaching and learning as a multifaceted process
a!. (2017) discuss the possibility of usmg the language conceptions
is related to co-adaptation in the classroom 's actions-and-responses.
as CAS (Fig. 1) as an initial conditions to get to the CALTL 1n initial
These actions-and-responses are correlated to the use of different
teacher education.
types of language teaching approaches and methods required for
As we (applied linguists 1n the language teachlng area) already
managing the dynamics of language social practices: teacher
know, Anthony (1963) has argued for two pillars that support an
manages the dynamics and the apprentices perform it in a co-adaptive
approach to language teachlng: one dealing with the nature of
process. This co-adaptation process (helped by the autopoieticindividual
language and other with the nature o f learnillg. So these two pillars
organization) may be accomplished with (a) the assistance of eco-
(CAS 1n themselves) are the CALTL's core (but they do not centralize
semiotic syllabus co-construction (discussed in the next topic; Fig.
the system control) - named, 1n the Fig. 3, as "language as CAS" and
4), (b) the affordance a-vvareness to learn an additionallanguage, (c)
"multifaceted teachlng & learnillg", respectively.
the reception of the students fractalized idenrities (topic 3). these
CALTL components are connected to each other across alllevels
a network of mteractions where each one changes in response to
other.
164 165
5. UNGUAGE TEACHING SYLLABUS AS CAS that give dynanúcity (dialogism) in language development, i. e.:
a) horizontal - linguistic normativity (habit and structure) as a
The severalltypes o f syllabuses (structural, situational, functional- language centripetal force which "forg[es] links between different
notional, procedural,_lexical etc.) found in language teaching literature subjects, explor[es] cross-curricular themes, and deal[s] -vvith global
can be divided into two large blocks: synthetic and/ or grammatical linguistic problems and issues"; b) vertical - linguistic creativity
syllabuses (subject-centered) and analytic and/ or communicative (variety and invention) as a language centrifugai force which
syllabuses (skills-based and process-oriented) (BORGES, 2009; "provid[es] deep and rich language experiences throughout the
BORGES, 2014a). They have given rise to very different n1.ethods and child's acadenúc career, and build[s] usable and lasting language
approaches to language teaching (grammar-translation, audiolingual, skills, both oral and written" (van LIER, 1996, p. 19, brackets mine).
communicative, language for special purposes etc.) (CLARl<:., 1987). As in ali syllabuses, teachers must have a starcing
Contemporaneously, Finney (201 0[2002]) proposes a mixed- to initiate their actions in classroom aiming at fostering teacher
focus curriculum whereupon we have a blend of grammatical and students interactions to promote language acquisicion. In
(content) and communicative (objective and process) syllabuses. In ESS context, this starting point is the syllabus' "inicial condicions"
this sense, Borges (2014a) suggests an eco-senúotic syllabus (ESS) as CAS which can be thought of as the students' needs and wants
(Fig. 4) based on van Lier' s (1996, 2000) Autonomy-Authenticity- (previously veriiied) as well as the contents and goals required by the
Awareness (AAA) curriculum and complexity theory. The ESS is one school curriculum. Along with the inicial conditions, teachers must
of the CALTL elements (see topic 4; Fig. 3) anda CAS in itself also have a sense of plausibili!J (PRABHU, 1987) to bring out their
methodology (BORGES, 2010) that will introduce the dynamics
required in the interaction and co-adaptation o f these tvvo classroom
autonomy-authenticity-awareness forces in the SLA: linguistic normativity and linguistic creativity.
~
linguistic creativity
(varíety and invention)
6. LA.NGUAGE TEACHER DEVELOPMENT AS

The teaching profession development in the field of language


.. "'
usage-meaning-use
Unguistic normativity
(habit and structure)
teacher education has evolved through the influence of classical
reflective training models such as those in Smyth (1989),
(1990), and Wallace (1991). From this base, Borges (2016) argues for
a chaotic model oj reflective development of language teachers' professionali!J
(CMRD) (Fig. 5) moving forvvard on the proposals already established
in the area by introducing elements and concepts derived from system
theory (block diagram). The CMRD focuses on language as CAS
Fig. 4- Language syllabus as CAS (the ESS) - Adapted from Borges
(Fig. 1) as an inicial condition for language teacher development in
(2014a, p. 22)
the complexity paradigm. The model emerges, as Borges explains,
The ESS is structured along two axes (vertical and horizontal)
from the evident difficulty of developing inicial teacher education
166 167
in accordance with the complexity perspective; and it provides Final remarks
important subsidies that may help comb:ing all CAS conceptions As well known, the language teaching field has progressed
(discussed in this paper) in the language teacher development. through what has been called 'eras': methods, approaches and post-
method. The post-method pedagogy - seen as a contemporary paradigm
shift in language teaching- rejected the predetermined set o f generic
principies underlying methods and approaches to language teaching.
This led to the abandonment of methods and approaches (already
developed in the area) encouraged by the theorizers-practitioners
language as CAS
rupture in the post-method pedagogy' s practicality dimension
(I<UMARAVADIVELU, 2001, 2006). Consequently, teachers were
asked ta construct their own theory if practice, adapting their classroom
Fig. 5- Language teacher deve1opment as CAS (the CMRD) (BORGES, methodologies in accordance with local and contextual factors.
2016, p. 379). With the advent of complexity paradigm in what I
coined. as additional language complexity pedagogy goes beyond the post-
method condition concerning its practicality. This is the case because
In the contex of pre-service language teacher education,
all CAS discussed in this work (language, SLA, identity, CALTL,
the CMRD begins by mapping (first stage) (using questionnaire,
ESS and CMRD), especially the CALTL and the CMRD, requires
encouraging discussions) the teacher students (TS) language
strong connectivity between teachers and theorizers. Therefore,
conceptions. These conceptions are believed to be an important
CAS (in language teaching area) are key parts for the emergence
chaotic attractor that can define/ show the cll.trent behavior/way o f
of a complexity pedagogy in AL. But for that matter, we (applied
thin.king on teaching and learning an additionallanguage. N ot ignoring
linguists) should look at this field with the eyes o f complexity.
the value of many other conceptions nested in this process, it is also
assumed that language conceptions will certainly set out (consciously
or unconsciously) actions in the classroom, including the methods REFERENCE
and approaches to language teaching to be used. If the TS do not yet ANTHONY, E. M. Approach, method, and technique . .L4.1.JL?;:;,JU.ou

possess the vision of language as CAS, the CMRD can initiate at any Teaching Forum, v. 3(1), p. 7-10, 1963.
conception that emerges among them7 (at the mapping stage) in order
BARTLE!T, L. Teacher dev~lopment through reflective teaching.
to achieve the notion of language as CAS. After the ffiapping stage, In. RICHARDS, J. C., NUNAN, D. (Eds.). Second Lan-
other processes might initiate such as iifonning (readings, counseling), guage Teacher Education. New York: Cambridge Univer-
interrogating (criticai reflection), evaluating (discussion, reports), and sity Press, 1990. p. 202-214
redirecting (connecting theory-practice). The disturbing stage may BECI<NER, C. et al. (2009). Language is a complex adaptive sys-
happen at any moment. The feedbacking stage is intended to energize tem: position paper. Language Learning, v. 59(1), p. 1-26.
the entire process by stabilizi.t"'1g the system whenever it is necessary. BORGES, E. F. V.; GABRE, B. S.; PUHL,J. Abordagem complexa
de ensino e de aprendizagem de línguas no estágio curricular
7 See Borges (2016) for further details.
supervisionado em língua inglesa. In: Práticas pedagógi-
168 169
cas exitosas na UEPG. NUTEAD. Ponta Grossa: Editora
UEPG, 2017. (in press) CHOMSI<:.Y, N. Syntactic structures. Berlim: Die Deutsche Bib-
liothek. (2002 [19 57])
BORGES, E. F. V. Um modelo caótico de desenvolvimento reflexi-
vo da profissionalidade de professores de línguas. ReVEL, CLARI<:., J. L. Curriculum renewal in school foreign language
v. 14(27), p. 364-388, 2016. learning. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1987.
_ _ _ _ _ _ _ . Complexity approach to language teaching and DASCAL, M. (Org.). Fundamentos metodológicos da linguísti-
learning: moving from theory to potencial practice. In: GIT- ca. Concepções gerais da teoria linguística. São Paulo: Glob-
SAI<:.I, C.; ALEXIOU, T. (Org.) Current Issues in Sec- al, v. i, 1978.
ond/Foreign Language Teaching and Teacher Educa-
tion: Research and Practice. upon UI<: DAVIS, B. & SUMARA, D. Complexity education: inquiries
Cambridge Scholars Publishing, 2015. into learning teaching, and research. N ew York: Rout-
ledge, 2006.
_ _ _ _ _ _ _ . Planejamento semiótico-ecológico de ensino de
línguas. Contexturas, n. 23, p. 39-61, 2014a. ELLIS, N. C.; LARSEN-FREEMAN, D. (Eds.).
complex adaptive system. Michigan:
_ _ _ _ _ _ _ . Paradigm shift in language teaching and lan- 2009.
guage teacher education. The ESPectalistJ v. 35(1), p. 42-
59, 2014b. FINNEY, D. The ELT curriculum: a flexible model for a chang-
ing world. In: J.C. RICHARDS; RENANDYA (Eds.)
_ _ _ _ _ _ . Metodologia, abordagem e pedagogias de ensi- Methodology in language teaching. anthology of
no de língua(s). Linguagem & Ensino, v. 13(2), p. 397- current practice, 2010[2002]. p. 69-79
414, 2010.
GERALDI,J. W Portos de passagem. 3. ed. São Paulo: Martins
BORGES, E. F. V. Uma reflexão filosófica sobre abordagens e Fontes, 1995.
paradigmas na constituição da subárea ensino-apren-
dizagem de LE/L2 na Linguística Aplicada. 298f. Tese GIDDENS, A. P. Modernity and self-identity: self society
de Doutorado Faculdade de Educação, Universidade de the late modern age. Stanford, CA: Stanford University
São Paulo, 2009. Press, 1991.

BORGES, E. F. V.; PAIVA, V. L. M. O. Towards a complexity ap- GUBA, E. G.; LINCOLN, Y S. Competing paradigms in qualita-
proach to language teaching and learning. 17th World tive research. In: DENZIN. N. I<:..; LINCOLN, Y S. (Eds.),
Congress of the International Association of Applied Lin- Handbook of qualitative research. Thousand Oaks, CA:
guistics (AILA), Brisbane, Austtalia, Abstract Book, 2014. Sage, 1994.p. 105-117

. Por uma abordagem complexa de ensino de lín- HALLIDAY, M. A. I<.. On matter and meaning:
guas: Linguagem & Ensino, v. 14(2), p. 337-56, 2011. human experience. Linguistics the
v. 1(1), p. 59-82, 2005.
BOYD, R. Metaphor and theory change: whatis "metaphor" a met-
aphor for? In: Andrew ORTONY (Ed.). Metaphor and HOLLAND, J. H. Hidden adaptation buHd.s com-
thought. 2nd ed. Austtalia: Cambridge University Press, plexity. New York: Basics Books: 1995.
1993. p. 481-532.
JAI<:.OBSON, R. Selected writings. New York: Mouton de Gruy-
CAPRA, F. O ponto de mutação. Trad. _Álvaro Cabral. São Paulo: ter, 2002[1962].
Culttix, 2006[1982], p. 47-48.
170 171
I<OERNER, E. F. I<. Paracligms in the 19th and 20th century his- p. 257-276.
tory o f linguistics: Schleicher, Saussure, Chomsky. In: HEI-
LMANN, L. (Ed.). Proceeding of the eleventh interna- NASCIMENTO, M. Linguagem como um sistema complexo: in-
tional congress o f linguistics. Bologna: II :l\1ulino, v. 1, terfases e interfaces. In: PAIVA, V. L. M. 0.; NASCIMEN-
1976. TO, M. Sistemas adaptativos complexos: lingua(gem)
e aprendizagem. Belo Horizonte: Faculdade de Letras da
I<UHN, T.S. The Structure of Scientifi.c Revolutions. 2nd ed., UFMG,2009.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1970[1962].
NOLAN, E. P. Now through a glass darkly: specular images
I<UMARAVADIVELU, B. TESOL methods: changing tracks, chal- being and knowing from Virgil to Chaucer. Michigan:
lenging trends. TESOL Quarterly, v. 40(1), 2006, p. 59-81. The University of Michigan Press, 1990.
_ _ _ _ _ _. Toward a postmethod pedagogy. TESOL PAIVA, V. L. M. O. Main second language acquisition theories: from
Quarterly, v. 35(4), 2001, p. 537-560. structuralism to complexity. Contexturas, n. 23, p. 112-124,
2014a.
JOUANNET, E. The liberal-welfarist law of nations: a histo-
ry of internationallaw. Cambridge: Cambridge University _ _ _ _ _ _ _. Aquisição de segunda São
Press, 2012. rábola Editorial, 2014b.

LAI<OFF, G.;JOHNSON, M. Metaphors we live by. In: O'BRIEN, _______ . Identity, motivation, autonomy from
].; I<OLLOCH, P. The production of reality. Essays and pe~spective ~f complex dynat?ical sy~te~s. In: MURRAY,
readings on social interaction. 3rd ed., London: Pine Forge G, GAR, X., LAMB, T. Identtty, mottvatton autono-
Press, 2001. p. 124-134 my in language learning. Bristol, Buffalo. Toronto:
lingual Matters, 2011. p. 57-72
LARSEN-FREEMAN, D.; CAMERON, L. Complex systems
and applied linguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press, _ _ _ _ _ _ _ . O modelo fractal de aquisição de línguas. In:
2008. BRUNO, F. C. (Org.). Reflexões e prática em ensino
aprendizagem de língua estrangeira. São Paulo: Clara
LARSEN-FREEMAN, D. Chaos/Complexity Science and second Luz,2005. p. 23-36
language acquisition. Applied Linguistics, v. 18(2), 141-
165, 1997. _______ . Ensino de inglesa no ensino
teoria e prática. São SM, 2012.
LUCI<HARDT, C. G. Beyond I<nowledge: Paradigms in Wittgen-
stein's Later Philosophy. Philosophy and Phenomenolog- _ _ _ _ _ _ _. Caleidoscópio: fractais uma 'U'JI.JI. ..... Ji.Ji.JL"""'

ical Research, v. 39(2), p. 240-252, 1978. ensino aprendizagem. Memorial para concurso de Pro-
fessor Titular. Faculdade de Letras. Universidade Federal de
MENEZES, V. Second Language Acquisition: Reconciling Theo- Minas Gerais, 2002.
ries. OpenJournal of Applied Science, v. 3(7), p. 393-403,
2013. PATTERSON et al. Radical rules schools: adanttve a\...uuu
for complex change. Circle Fines: u,.-v">.
MORIN, E. Introdução ao pensamento complexo. 3a. ed., Porto mies Institute, 2012.
Alegre: Sulina, 2007.
PEREIRA, L. S.; BORGES, E. F. Potencialidades da abordagem
_ _ _ _ _ _ _ . O sistema: paradigma ou/ e teoria. Ciência complexa no uso da coleção Alive! inglês Ensino Funda-
com consciência. Trad. de Maria Alexandre e Maria A. S. mental. In: SILVA, W ·BORGES, E. F. (Orgs.).
Dória. 13a ed., Rio de Janeiro: Bertrand Brasil, 2010[1982], plexidade em ambientes ensino e aprendizagem
172 173
de línguas adicionais. Curitiba: CRV, 2016. p. 93-114 AGRA.DECIMENTOS DA COORDENAÇAO DO CIEL 2015
PRABHU, N. S. Second language pedagogy. Oxford: Oxford ·
University Press, 1987.
Para alcançarmos os objetivos desta grande realização, o CIEL
RAMALNGAM, B. et al. Exploring the science of complexity:
ideas and implications for development and humani- UEPG contou com a colaboração imprescindível dos professores
tarian efforts. ODI Working Paper 285. London: Overseas Jhony Skeika, Eliane Raupp, Débora Sheidt, Rosita Bastos, Luisa
Development Institute, 2008. Cristina dos Santos Fontes, Marly Catarina Soares, Lucimar Braga,
SADE, A. S. Complexity and identity reconstruction in second lan- Larissa A. Ribeiro, Valeska G. Carlos, Miguel Sanches Neto, Sebastião
guage acquisition. RBLA, v. 9(2), p. 515-537, 2009. L. dos Santos, Rosana A. Harmuch, Isabel Volett Marson, J\!Iariza
Tulio, Jane I<.eli Oliveira, Elaine Borges, Fábio Augusto Stayer, Rúbia
SAUSSURE, F. Course in generallinguistics. Translated by Wade
I<.askin. London: McGraw-Hill Book Company, 2011 [1915]. Silva, Daniel de Oliveira Gomes, Diego Gomes do Vale, Rosângela
Schardong.
SEARLE, J. R. Speech acts: an essay in the philosophy of lan-
guage. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1999[1969]. Somos imensamente gratos ao corpo discente dos cursos de
graduação em Letras da Universidade Estadual de Ponta Grossa,
SILVA, W M.; BORGES, E. F. V. (Orgs.). Complexidade em am- bem como aos alunos do Mestrado em Linguagem, Identidade e
bientes de ensino e de aprendizagem de línguas adi-
cionais. Curitiba: CRV, 2016. Subjetividade, pelo apoio e pelo desejo de dar ao CIEL um rosto, uma
linguagem. O nosso rosto, a nossa linguagem sempre em movimento.
SMYTH, J. Develo_ping and sustaining critical reflection in teacher Agradecemos de modo específico à aluna do mestrado, Silvely Brandes,
education. Journal of Teacher Education, v. 40(2), p. 2-9,
1989. nossa fiel e corajosa assistente, que enfrentou com determinação e
paciência o processo de inscrição de nossos participantes. Nossos
van LIER, L. From input to affordance: social-interactive learning alunos foram a continuidade das nossas mãos, de nossa voz e de
from an ecological perspective. In: LANTOLF; J. P. (Ed.).
Sociocultural theory and second language learning. nossos ouvidos e buscaram com empenho a realização ideal oara o
Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2000. p. 245-259 nosso evento.
_ _ _ _ _ _ _. Interaction in the language curriculum: Queremos agradecer ainda o apoio financeiro da CAPES,
awareness, autonomy and authenticity. London: Long- da Prefeitura de Ponta Grossa e da Fundação Araucária, que
man, 1996. possibilitaram a presença de convidados importantes para movimentar
VOLOSINOV, V. N. Marxism and the Philosophy of Lan- nossas reflexões científicas e acadêmicas. Além destas instituições
guage. Trad. Ladislav Matejka and I.R. Titunik. Cambridge: foram essenciais para esta realização do CIEL, para a promoção
Harvard University Press, 2000[1973]. e valorização do ensino, da pesquisa e da extensão universitária,
WALLACE, M. Training foreign language teachers: a reflec- constituindo-se como parceiros da Educação Brasileira, as empresas:
tive approach. Cambridge: Cambridge Universlty Press, Heineken, Editora Estúdio Texto, luliane Pneus e Caixa Econômica
1991.
Federal.

174 175

Você também pode gostar